Abstract
Objective
Cognitive impairment is a common complication of chronic kidney disease (CKD). Caffeine intake has been reported to improve cognitive performance in several studies. However, whether the benefits of caffeine intake on cognitive function apply to patients with CKD remains unknown.
Methods
We performed a retrospective cross-sectional study based on the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES). The data of CKD subjects and non-CKD subjects from NHANES 2011−2014 were analyzed. Propensity score matching (PSM) was performed based on age, sex, diabetes, cancer, educational level, energy intake and protein intake to select subjects. The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease Word Learning Test (CERAD-WL), the CERAD Word List Recall Test (CERAD-DR), the Animal Fluency Test (AF) and the Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST) were used, whereby the occurrence of cognitive impairment was identified. Logistic regression models were performed to evaluate the association between caffeine intake and cognitive performance in CKD and non-CKD participants. Stratified analyses according to the stage of CKD and the urinary albumin/creatinine ratio levels were performed. Plot curves were then generalized to present a non-linear relationship, and the inflection point for each non-linear model was obtained by using a recursive algorithm.
Results
Cognitive impairment was more prevalent in CKD patients than in non-CKD subjects. For CKD patients, caffeine intake was associated with higher CERAD-WL, CERAD-DR, AF and DSST scores. For non-CKD subjects, caffeine intake was associated with higher DSST scores only. Subgroup analysis revealed that caffeine only benefited the cognitive function of patients with CKD stages 2 and 3. The analysis showed non-linear relationships of caffeine intake and cognitive function for both CKD and non-CKD subjects. The inflection point of caffeine intake for CKD patients was 279 mg/day.
Conclusion
The recommended dose of caffeine intake to improve the cognitive function of CKD patients is ≤279 mg/day.
Keywords: caffeine, chronic kidney disease, cognitive performance, the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, cognitive impairment
Introduction
Chronic kidney disease (CKD) poses a major challenge to public health due to its detrimental impact on individuals’ quality of life. Morbidity and mortality caused by CKD and CKD-related complications have increased drastically in recent years (1). Cognitive impairment is a common complication during the progression of CKD (2). It affects 27% to 62% of patients at CKD stages 1−4 (3). Various tests, including the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE), the Trail-making Test, and the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s disease (CERAD) are routinely used to assess the cognitive function of CKD patients (2, 4, 5). In developing countries, the morbidity of cognitive impairment in CKD patients reaches 50% or higher (6, 7). A decreased estimated glomerular filtration rate (eGFR) was found to be associated with cognitive dysfunction in several population-based cohorts (5, 8). Cognitive impairment remarkably decreases the living ability, quality of life and treatment compliance, which further increases the difficulty of treatment as well as the risk of mortality of CKD patients (9). Therefore, early identification and prevention of cognitive impairment in CKD patients are of great importance.
The exact mechanism for cognitive dysfunction in CKD remains unknown; however, multisystemic factors, including hypertension, anemia, acidosis, proteinuria, and uremic milieu, may conceivably exert a detrimental effect on the brains of patients with CKD (10).
Dietary patterns may positively affect cognitive performance, especially for the learning and memory abilities of individuals (11). Healthy dietary patterns such as the Mediterranean diet potentially reduce the risk of cognitive impairment such as in Alzheimer’s disease (12). However, nutrients or compounds implicated in healthy diets that benefit cognitive performance are still under investigation.
Caffeine (1,3,7-trimethylxanthine), a purine alkaloid detectable in various drinks, including coffee, cola, and tea, is probably the most commonly used psychoactive substance/psychostimulant worldwide. During the COVID-19 pandemic, the consumption of caffeine, especially coffee and tea, has increased, partially because these beverages could reduce the accumulated stress of dramatic changes in lifestyle (13). Caffeine has a similar structure to adenosine (Figure 1) (14). As a nervous system stimulant, caffeine has been revealed by numerous studies to have an effect on cognitive functions, including memory and attention (15). Caffeine consumption may enhance processing speed and relieve anxiety (16, 17). For adults, caffeine may also jeopardize oral health, including increasing the risk of tooth loss (18). Caffeine may have a greater impact on overall health than just mental and brain health.
Diet is a fundamental component of CKD integrated treatments. A proper diet reduces the complications of and improves the prognosis of CKD (19). Higher caffeine consumption was associated with a lower risk of incident CKD, with each additional cup consumed lowering the risk of CKD by 3% (20), especially for adults with metabolic syndrome (21). Higher coffee consumption is also associated with lower all-cause mortality in patients with CKD (22). Moderate caffeine intake through habitual coffee consumption is beneficial to cognitive function in patients receiving hemodialysis, possibly due to selective enhancement of attention and vigilance (10). Notwithstanding, whether caffeine benefits patients with CKD in terms of cognitive impairment is unknown.
Aim
In the present study, we aimed to examine the potential effect of caffeine intake on the cognitive performance of patients with CKD. We hypothesize that caffeine intake benefits patients with CKD in terms of alleviating cognitive impairment. While this hypothesis was being tested, we further sought to identify a beneficial dosage of caffeine. To this end, the relationships between caffeine intake and cognitive functions in CKD and non-CKD subjects were investigated based on a cross-sectional national study in the United States.
Materials and methods
Data source
The National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) is a cross-sectional national survey performed by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) of the United States every 2 years that aims to evaluate the health and nutritional status of the United States population1. Subjects of NHANES were sampled on the basis of a set of complicated, stratified and multistage study protocols. Then, demographic, dietary, body examination, laboratory, drug use, disease history and living habits data were collected either by interviews at home or health examinations in the mobile examination center (MEC). All NHNAES data are publicly available see text footnote 1. The protocols of NHANES were approved by the CDC of the United States, and written informed consent was obtained from all participants.
Subject selection
Datasets of the 2011−2012 cycle and the 2013−2014 cycle were extracted. The total number of participants in these two cycles was 19931. A 24-h dietary recall interview was performed in each survey cycle. Because individuals aged 80 and over are top coded to ensure their anonymity, the specific ages of these individuals are not available2. Adults younger than 80 years old who participated in the cognitive questionnaires and kidney function evaluation were included. We excluded participants with diets that differed from their usual dietary habits on the day before the interview. Subjects who were prescribed a special diet with energy intakes of <500 or >8000 kcal/day for men and <500 or >5000 kcal/day for women were excluded (23). CKD was defined as an estimated glomerular filtration rate (eGFR) < 60 mL/min/1.73 m2 and/or a urinary albumin creatinine ratio (ACR) >30 mg/g (24). eGFR was calculated by the Chronic Kidney Disease Epidemiology Collaboration equation (25). Participants without weight or ACR data were excluded.
Collection of caffeine intake and dietary data
Based on the 24-h dietary recall interview in each survey cycle, caffeine consumption was estimated from the foods and beverages that contain caffeine by the survey called What We Eat in America (26). Dietary status was collected using an in-person 24-h recall by a computerized 5-step method and a second non-consecutive 24-h recall via telephone. Data for each nutrient were calculated (27). We extracted the total energy, protein, carbohydrate, sugar and fiber intake data from the database.
Cognitive function evaluation
Four scale assessments were performed in NHANES, including the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease Word Learning Test (CERAD-WL), the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease Word List Recall Test (CERAD-DR), the Animal Fluency test (AF), and the Digit Symbol Substitution test (DSST) (28). The CERAD-WL and CERAD-DR were used to evaluate immediate and delayed verbal memory, respectively (29). The AF tested language ability and executive function (30). The DSST was used to assess cognitive executive function and processing speed (31). The detailed process of each test was described in the NHANES protocol3 as well as in previous studies (32). The cutoff values of the tests for low cognitive performance were set as CERAD-WL < 17, CERAD-DR < 5, AF < 14, or DSST < 40 (33). The relationship between caffeine intake and the test scores was investigated.
Propensity score matching and the matching factors
To identify the confounding factors, we adjusted the logistic regression model by covariates. Accounting for the baseline differences, propensity score matching (PSM) was performed to investigate the effects of caffeine intake on cognitive function. The matching conditions included age, BMI, sex, diabetes, cardiovascular disease, cancer, educational level, energy intake and protein intake. Demographic data, including age, sex and education status, were extracted from the NHANES database. The BMI value was calculated by dividing the weight (kg) by the height squared (m2). The medical history of the patients (diabetes, cardiovascular disease and cancer) and daily nutrient intakes (total energy and protein) were collected by questionnaires.
Statistics
Statistical analysis was performed by Empower Stats4. In NHANES, all data were weighted according to the analytical guidelines of the NHANES5 for better representation of the total. For continuous variables, medians are expressed and compared by Student’s t-test. For categorical variables, data are expressed as percentages and analyzed by the chi-squared test. Logistic regression analysis was performed to evaluate the association between caffeine intake and cognitive performance in non-CKD and CKD participants. Subgroup analysis was performed according to the different CKD stages and ACR levels. CKD patients were divided into a CKD stage 1 group, CKD stage 2 and 3 groups and CKD stage 4 and 5 groups. According to the ACR levels, CKD patients were grouped into ACR < 30 mg/mmol, 30 mg/mmol ≤ ACR < 300 mg/mmol and ACR ≥ 300 mg/mmol groups. Logistic regressions of caffeine intake and cognitive function were performed in each subgroup. Graphics were generate to show the non-linear relationship. The inflection point was calculated for each non-linear model using a recursive algorithm in which a two-piecewise linear regression model was adopted on both sides (34).
Results
Demographic data of the studied subjects
Among the 19931 subjects in NHANES 2011−2014, 1570 subjects were enrolled after excluding 18361 subjects based on the exclusion criteria (Figure 2). PSM selected 442 non-CKD subjects and 442 CKD patients from the matching criteria (Figure 2). Only subjects aged over 60 years old were tested for cognitive performance in NHANES, and therefore, the mean age of the enrolled subjects was 68.68 (SD = 5.63) years old. Men accounted for 53.39% of all subjects. Whites were the major ethnicity, accounting for 47.06%. The mean age of the CKD patients was 69.21 (SD = 5.63) years old, which was higher than that of the non-CKD subjects with a mean age of 68.15 (SD = 5.60) years old (Table 1, P = 0.005). No ethnic difference was found between the non-CKD and CKD subgroups (Table 1, P = 0.261 for ethnicities); thus, the intergroup comparisons were conducted without regard to ethnicity.
TABLE 1.
Clinical variables | Total (N = 884) | Non-CKD (N = 442) | CKD (N = 442) | P | Effect size |
Age (year) | 68.68 ± 5.63 | 68.15 ± 5.60 | 69.21 ± 5.63 | 0.005 | 0.189 |
Male (%) | 53.39 | 54.75% | 52.04% | 0.418 | 0.055 |
Race | 0.261 | 0.014 | |||
White (%) | 47.06 | 45.48 | 48.64 | ||
Black (%) | 24.43 | 23.08 | 25.79 | ||
Other races (%) | 28.51 | 31.44 | 25.75 | ||
Education | 0.700 | −0.053 | |||
Less than 9th grade (%) | 11.54 | 11.76 | 11.31 | ||
9−11th grade (%) | 14.37 | 13.57 | 15.16 | ||
High school graduate (%) | 21.83 | 21.27 | 22.40 | ||
College or AA degree (%) | 29.75 | 28.96 | 30.54 | ||
College graduate or above (%) | 22.51 | 24.43 | 20.59 | ||
Body Mass Index (kg/m2) | 29.84 ± 6.78 | 29.51 ± 6.82 | 30.17 ± 6.73 | 0.148 | 0.097 |
Systolic blood pressure (mmHg) | 132.47 ± 18.76 | 130.41 ± 17.57 | 134.52 ± 19.69 | 0.001 | 0.226 |
Diastolic blood pressure (mmHg) | 67.88 ± 14.43 | 67.96 ± 13.68 | 67.81 ± 15.15 | 0.740 | −0.011 |
Pulse (bpm) | 70.66 ± 11.96 | 70.40 ± 11.44 | 70.92 ± 12.46 | 0.522 | 0.044 |
Alanine transaminase (U/L) | 22.26 ± 9.83 | 22.78 ± 9.22 | 21.74 ± 10.38 | 0.115 | −0.106 |
Aspartate aminotransferase (U/L) | 25.12 ± 9.66 | 24.98 ± 9.48 | 25.25 ± 9.85 | 0.679 | 0.028 |
Alkaline phosphatase (U/L) | 69.65 ± 22.52 | 67.52 ± 18.81 | 71.79 ± 25.55 | 0.005 | 0.190 |
γ-glutamyl transferase (U/L) | 27.31 ± 28.78 | 26.29 ± 26.19 | 28.33 ± 31.15 | 0.294 | 0.071 |
Blood urea nitrogen (mmol/L) | 5.87 ± 2.38 | 5.01 ± 1.52 | 6.73 ± 2.75 | <0.001 | 0.774 |
Total cholesterol (mmol/L) | 4.90 ± 1.14 | 4.98 ± 1.08 | 4.82 ± 1.18 | 0.028 | −0.142 |
Lactate dehydrogenase (U/L) | 132.49 ± 27.42 | 128.76 ± 22.42 | 136.22 ± 31.22 | <0.001 | 0.275 |
Creatine phosphokinase (IU/L) | 134.61 ± 99.01 | 134.66 ± 92.21 | 134.57 ± 105.48 | 0.989 | −0.001 |
Total protein (g/L) | 71.07 ± 4.98 | 70.79 ± 4.84 | 71.34 ± 5.10 | 0.100 | 0.111 |
Uric acid (μmol/L) | 351.19 ± 88.02 | 326.48 ± 75.22 | 375.91 ± 92.89 | <0.001 | 0.585 |
Blood glucose (mmol/L) | 6.49 ± 2.90 | 6.23 ± 2.55 | 6.74 ± 3.19 | 0.009 | 0.177 |
Serum phosphorus (mmol/L) | 1.21 ± 0.18 | 1.20 ± 0.17 | 1.23 ± 0.19 | 0.008 | 0.166 |
Serum sodium (mmol/L) | 139.51 ± 2.51 | 139.63 ± 2.51 | 139.40 ± 2.51 | 0.172 | −0.092 |
Serum potassium (mmol/L) | 4.06 ± 0.41 | 4.02 ± 0.37 | 4.10 ± 0.44 | 0.003 | 0.197 |
Serum iron (μmol/L) | 14.84 ± 5.55 | 15.31 ± 5.56 | 14.36 ± 5.49 | 0.011 | −0.011 |
Serum calcium (mmol/L) | 2.37 ± 0.09 | 2.36 ± 0.09 | 2.38 ± 0.10 | 0.007 | 0.210 |
Hemoglobin (g/dL) | 13.85 ± 1.43 | 13.99 ± 1.30 | 13.71 ± 1.55 | 0.003 | −0.196 |
Caffeine intake (mg/day) | 153.54 ± 183.38 | 154.40 ± 190.42 | 152.69 ± 176.28 | 0.890 | −0.009 |
Energy intake (kcal/day) | 1858.78 ± 782.73 | 1939.11 ± 827.57 | 1778.44 ± 727.27 | 0.002 | −0.206 |
Protein intake (g/day) | 74.77 ± 36.64 | 78.21 ± 40.31 | 71.34 ± 32.25 | 0.005 | −0.188 |
Carbohydrate intake (g/day) | 224.03 ± 99.71 | 236.16 ± 104.80 | 211.91 ± 92.90 | <0.001 | −0.245 |
Sugar intake (g/day) | 93.90 ± 59.22 | 98.77 ± 60.73 | 89.03 ± 57.32 | 0.014 | −0.165 |
Fiber intake (g/day) | 17.25 ± 10.88 | 19.09 ± 12.30 | 15.42 ± 8.89 | <0.001 | −0.342 |
Coronary heart disease (%) | 8.82 | 6.79 | 10.86 | 0.036 | 0.141 |
Stroke (%) | 7.13 | 5.66 | 8.60 | 0.145 | 0.098 |
Cancer (%) | 18.33 | 18.78 | 17.87 | 0.941 | 0.005 |
Diabetes (%) | 29.41 | 23.76 | 35.07 | 0.001 | 0.167 |
CERAD-WL < 17 | 25.97 | 23.45 | 28.47 | 0.090 | 0.114 |
CERAD-DR < 5 | 22.36 | 19.63 | 25.06 | 0.055 | 0.129 |
AF < 14 | 25.55 | 22.63 | 28.44 | 0.050 | 0.132 |
DSST < 40 | 32.59 | 28.14 | 37.12 | 0.005 | 0.190 |
AF, Animal Fluency test; CERAD-WL, Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease Word Learning test; CERAD-DR, Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease Delayed Recall test; DSST, Digit Symbol Substitution test.
Higher prevalence of cognitive impairment in chronic kidney disease patients than in non-chronic kidney disease subjects
We compared the demographic data, laboratory tests, disease history, dietary intakes and cognitive performance of CKD and non-CKD subjects. The gender composition (P = 0.418), education status (P = 0.700) and caffeine consumption (P = 0.890) showed insignificant differences between the two groups (Table 1). Compared with non-CKD subjects, the systolic blood pressure (SBP, P < 0.001) of CKD patients was much higher (Table 1). The diastolic blood pressure showed no difference between the CKD and non-CKD patients (P = 0.740). The variances in the kidney function (blood urea nitrogen and uric acid), liver function (alkaline phosphatase and lactate dehydrogenase) and ion levels (serum phosphorus, potassium, iron and calcium) coincided with the disease characteristics of CKD. The comparison of dietary intake showed that CKD patients consumed diets with lower levels of energy (P = 0.002), protein (P = 0.005), carbohydrate (P < 0.001), sugar (P = 0.014) and fiber (P < 0.001). Regarding complications, the comorbidity rates of coronary heart disease (P = 0.036) and diabetes (P = 0.001) were higher in CKD patients than in non-CKD subjects. For CKD patients, ratios of cognitive impairment in DSST (P = 0.005) were higher than in non-CKD subjects, which means CKD patients were more predisposed to cognitive executive function and processing speed impairment. Other cognitive parameters, including CERAD-WL (P = 0.090), CERAD-DR (P = 0.055) and AF (P = 0.050), showed no difference between CKD and non-CKD patients.
Promoting effect of caffeine on the cognitive performance of chronic kidney disease patients and non-chronic kidney disease subjects
Logistic regression analysis was used to investigate the relationship between each cognitive test and caffeine consumption. Caffeine was confirmed to be associated with an improved cognitive performance in all enrolled subjects (Table 2). Subgroup analysis was performed for the CKD and non-CKD subjects. For the CKD patients, CERAD-WL, AF and DSST were significantly associated with caffeine consumption in the unadjusted model (Table 2, P < 0.050). However, caffeine only improved the performance on the DSST for non-CKD patients (Table 2, P = 0.022). For the CERAD-DR test, caffeine consumption did not affect the cognitive function of either non-CKD or CKD subjects.
TABLE 2.
Non-CKD (N = 442) |
CKD (N = 442) |
Total (N = 884) |
||||
OR (95%CI) | P-value | OR (95%CI) | P-value | OR (95%CI) | P-value | |
CERAD-WL | 0.001 (−0.001, 0.003) | 0.311 | 0.003 (0.001, 0.005) | 0.010 | 0.002 (0.000, 0.003) | 0.013 |
CERAD-DR | 0.001 (−0.000, 0.002) | 0.083 | 0.001 (−0.000, 0.002) | 0.073 | 0.001 (0.000, 0.002) | 0.013 |
AF | 0.002 (−0.001, 0.004) | 0.183 | 0.004 (0.001, 0.007) | 0.003 | 0.003 (0.001, 0.005) | 0.003 |
DSST | 0.009 (0.001, 0.016) | 0.022 | 0.014 (0.006, 0.022) | <0.001 | 0.011 (0.006, 0.017) | <0.001 |
AF, Animal Fluency test; CERAD-WL, Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease Word Learning test; CERAD-DR, Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease Delayed Recall test; DSST, Digit Symbol Substitution test.
Caffeine intake specifically benefits the cognitive function of patients with stage 2 and 3 chronic kidney disease
Subgroup analysis was performed for each test among the CKD patients to explore whether caffeine intake would benefit cognitive function in different CKD stages and ACR levels. The results revealed that caffeine consumption reduced the risk of cognitive impairment in patients with CKD stages 2 and 3 for all tests (CERAD-WL, OR = 0.003, 95% CI: 0.000−0.006, P = 0.032, Figure 3A; CERAD-DR, OR = 0.002, 95% CI: 0.000−0.003, P = 0.046, Figure 4A; AF, OR = 0.005, 95% CI: 0.001−0.008, P = 0.007, Figure 5A; DSST, OR = 0.017, 95% CI: 0.007−0.027, P = 0.001, Figure 6A). For ACR, caffeine consumption only influenced the risk of cognitive impairment in the ACR < 30 mg/mmol group of AF (OR = 0.005, 95% CI: 0.000−0.010, P = 0.049, Figure 5B) and all ACR groups of DSST (ACR < 30 mg/mmol, OR = 0.019, 95% CI: 0.004−0.033, P = 0.010; 30 mg/mmol ≤ ACR < 300 mg/mmol, OR = 0.011, 95% CI: 0.002−0.021, P = 0.023; ACR ≥ 300 mg/mmol, OR = 0.027, 95% CI: 0.002−0.052, P = 0.045, Figure 6B). Caffeine intake would not influcence cognitive performance in all ACR groups of CERAD-WL and CERAD-DR (Figures 3B, 4B).
Moderate caffeine intake improved cognitive performance for both the non-chronic kidney disease and chronic kidney disease subjects
Smooth curve fittings were generalized for non-CKD and CKD subjects to intuitively show the association between each cognitive test and caffeine consumption (Figure 7 and Table 3). A non-linear relationship was found for all cognitive tests for the two groups. The inflection points were determined with a two-piecewise linear regression model (Table 3). For CKD subjects, positive associations were shown when caffeine intake ≤ inflection points (P < 0.050), which means moderate caffeine intake would improve the cognitive performance of CKD patients. For non-CKD subjects, positive associations were observed in CERAD-WL and DSST when caffeine intake ≤ inflection points (P < 0.050). Thus, a moderate amount of caffeine consumption would improve immediate verbal memory, cognitive executive function and processing speed. When caffeine intake was above the inflection point, negative associations were found in the DSST (P = 0.027) of CKD patients. CKD patients with caffeine intake ≤ 279 mg/day benefited in terms of cognitive function (Table 3).
TABLE 3.
Non-CKD (N = 442) |
CKD (N = 442) |
|||
OR (95%CI) | P-value | OR (95%CI) | P-value | |
CERAD-WL | ||||
Inflection point (K) | 304 | 288 | ||
Caffeine intake < K | 0.006 (0.002, 0.009) | 0.003 | 0.007 (0.003, 0.011) | 0.014 |
Caffeine intake > K | −0.003 (−0.006, 0.000) | 0.085 | −0.001 (−0.005, 0.003) | 0.687 |
Log likelihood ratio | 0.005 | 0.029 | ||
CERAD-DR | ||||
Inflection point (K) | 67 | 306 | ||
Caffeine intake < K | 0.008 (−0.001, 0.017) | 0.074 | 0.003 (0.001, 0.005) | 0.004 |
Caffeine intake > K | 0.000 (−0.001, 0.002) | 0.509 | −0.001 (−0.003, 0.001) | 0.332 |
Log likelihood ratio | 0.106 | 0.023 | ||
AF | ||||
Inflection point (K) | 463 | 279 | ||
Caffeine intake < K | 0.003 (−0.000, 0.007) | 0.085 | 0.013 (0.008, 0.019) | <0.001 |
Caffeine intake > K | −0.001 (−0.007, 0.004) | 0.674 | −0.004 (−0.009, 0.001) | 0.089 |
Log likelihood ratio | 0.261 | <0.001 | ||
DSST | ||||
Inflection point (K) | 463 | 294 | ||
Caffeine intake < K | 0.020 (0.009, 0.030) | <0.001 | 0.046 (0.032, 0.061) | <0.001 |
Caffeine intake > K | −0.012 (−0.029, 0.005) | 0.163 | −0.016 (−0.030, −0.002) | 0.027 |
Log likelihood ratio | 0.007 | <0.001 |
AF, Animal Fluency test; CERAD-WL, Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease Word Learning test; CERAD-DR, Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease Delayed Recall test; DSST, Digit Symbol Substitution test.
Discussion
In this study, we investigated the association between caffeine intake and cognitive function in CKD patients. Our results show that higher levels of caffeine consumption benefited CKD patients on all cognitive tests except for the CERAD-DR and benefited the non-CKD subjects only on the DSST. When caffeine intake ≤ 279 mg/day for CKD patients, a linear dose–response association was detected between caffeine intake and cognitive performance.
In NHANES 2011−2014, the ratios of cognitive impairment in four cognitive function tests were all higher in CKD patients. However, the basis of cognitive impairment in patients with CKD is not clear. The possible mechanisms, however, are discussed as follows. First, CKD and cognitive impairment always share the same risk factors, including hypertension, diabetes, and smoking (35), which may result in the coexistence of these two diseases. Another possibility is that vascular changes in CKD (36), such as vascular calcification, epithelial dysfunction and arteriosclerosis, contribute to the insufficient blood supply to the brain and neuronal cell damage (37, 38). Likewise, the decreased clearance function of CKD leads to the accumulation of uremic neurotoxins, which may act on the kidney-brain axis and cause cognitive dysfunction (39). As such, patients with CKD may face a higher risk of cognitive impairment, and CKD has been identified as a risk factor for cognitive impairment, especially for vascular dementia.
Previous studies have explored the association between caffeine consumption and cognitive function in community-dwelling people. In the Prevencion con Dieta Mediterranea plus (PREDIMED-plus) study, researchers observed that dietary caffeine consumption was related to better cognitive function in a Mediterranean elderly population (40). Another study based on the NHANES 2011−2014 database also indicated that caffeine intake would benefit cognitive performance (23). Our findings that caffeine intake significantly improved the cognitive function of community-dwelling adults are consistent with previous studies.
The mechanism of action of caffeine on cognitive function is complex. As a central nervous system stimulant, caffeine could benefit cognitive function by three mechanisms (14). The first mechanism of caffeine could be described as a non-selective antagonist of adenosine receptors, which triggers the release of neurotransmitters, such as dopamine, norepinephrine, acetylcholine, gamma-aminobutyric acid, and glutamate (41). All of the abovementioned neurotransmitters can influence cognitive function. Methylxanthine, which is an organic molecule chemically similar to caffeine, can also improve the mobilization of intercellular calcium and inhibit phosphodiesterases, thereby enhancing cognitive function (42). Furthermore, caffeine could suppress neuroinflammation and act as a neuroprotectant by regulating the levels of protein kinase A and the blood supply in the nervous system (43).
We found that dietary caffeine intake improved scores on 3 of 4 cognitive tests for CKD patients. Caffeine may prevent afferent arteriolar constriction and increase renal plasma flow, which would accelerate the metabolism of uremic toxin (21). Although few studies have focused on the effects of caffeine on the cognitive function of CKD patients, caffeine was confirmed to be beneficial to CKD patients in decreasing the risk of CKD and improving their prognosis. Daily coffee intake was associated with a decreased risk of developing CKD (44). Similarly, another NHANES study also revealed an inverse association between caffeine consumption and all-cause mortality based on data from 4863 non-institutionalized American adults with CKD (45). Caffeine intake was dose-dependently associated with a lower incidence of CKD and albuminuria (22). The potential mechanism might be the adenosine antagonism of caffeine, which may prevent the constriction of glomerulus afferent arterioles (21), increase renal tissue oxygenation (46) and prevent renal fibrosis (47). However, the mechanism conferring a positive effect on kidney function needs further investigation.
In regard to the significantly positive effect on the cognitive function of CKD patients, direct evidence is not well documented in the literature. One possible explanation is that caffeine may reduce the incidence of depression, which is more common in patients with CKD and is closely associated with cognitive impairment (48, 49). Recent studies revealed that caffeine-derived metabolites could modulate the gut microbiota and improve depression (50). Collectively, caffeine may indirectly improve cognitive function by ameliorating renal function as well as directly stimulating the nervous system.
The subgroup analysis according to the stage of CKD showed that the beneficial effects of caffeine on cognitive function were only present in the CKD stage 2 and 3 groups. For patients with CKD stages 4 and 5, the clearance capacity of the kidney is severely damaged, which may cause the accumulation of caffeine metabolites in the blood. Higher levels of metabolites, which are equivalent to consuming a large amount of caffeine, are harmful to the nervous system. According to our results, the levels of ACR did not appear to act as an influencing factor on caffeine consumption and cognitive function in CKD patients. Thus, CKD stage 1 patients who have high ACR levels and a normal eGFR are not affected by caffeine.
Usually, a low caffeine intake refers to below 200 mg/day. Moderate caffeine intake is considered to be between 200 and 400 mg/day and high for above 400 mg/day (51). The recommended dose of caffeine intake for the general population is no more than 400 mg/day according to the European Food Safety Authority (52) and U.S. Food and Drug Administration (53). A daily dose of caffeine intake >500–600 mg may affect the cardiovascular system and nervous system by positive inotropic and anxiogenic-like effects leading to tachycardia, anxiety and so on, which increases the risk of health problems (53, 54). A dose of caffeine for cognitive protection or renal protection has not been recommended. A paucity of studies revealed that caffeine intake of 300−400 mg/day may enhance cognitive performance (23, 55). The morbidity and mortality of CKD patients would significantly decrease for populations with a caffeine intake of 200−400 mg/day. Caffeine may act as a methylxanthine compound to promote the movement of intercellular calcium (14). The proper concentrations of methylxanthine increase the uptake of calcium and neurotransmitters to affect the nervous system (14). However, higher concentrations of methylxanthine can inhibit the uptake of calcium. Our research further supports the efficacy of an appropriate dose of caffeine to protect cognitive function in CKD patients. When the dose was ≤ 279 mg/day, more caffeine intake was associated with better cognitive function. The recommended dose of caffeine intake was ≤ 279 mg/day for better cognitive function in CKD patients.
We acknowledge the limitations of this study. First, NHANES is not representative of all populations because of the ethnic composition of the United States, which limits the extrapolation of our findings to a more general population. Second, basic kidney function and cognitive status before caffeine consumption were unavailable. As a consequence, it remains unclear whether there was a direct cause-and-effect relationship between caffeine intake and kidney function. Further longitudinal studies are warranted to demonstrate the causal relationship between caffeine intake and cognitive function in CKD patients. Third, retrospective studies inevitably cause some bias. For example, the 24-h recall dietary interview by telephone may result in recall bias. Another important indicator, tobacco habits, was not included in this study because the smoking data of two-thirds of the subjects were missing from the NHANES datasets. A prospective study is expected in the future. Finally, only four cognitive tests were used in the NHANES database. Other commonly used cognitive tests, such as the Mini-mental State Examination and Montreal Cognitive Assessment, should be included in future studies for a more comprehensive assessment of cognitive function.
Conclusion
Better cognitive performance is associated with daily caffeine consumption among community-dwelling people. Caffeine consumption confers a remarkable mitigating effect on the cognitive dysfunction of CKD patients. The recommended dose of caffeine intake should be no more than 279 mg/day for these patients.
Data availability statement
The original contributions presented in this study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.
Author contributions
LJ, RJ, and H-LZ designed the study. LJ, HZ, and LH extracted the data. LJ, HZ, and RJ performed the statistical analysis and interpreted the data. LJ, HZ, L-HJ, and RJ drafted the manuscript. H-LZ critically revised the manuscript. All authors approved the final version for submission and publication.
Footnotes
Funding
The study was supported by grants from National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 81900655), General Projects of Science and Technology Plan of Beijing Municipal Commission of Education (No. KM202010025022), Beijing Hospitals Authority Youth Programme (No. QML20200807), and Wu Jieping Medical Foundation Clinical Research Funding (No. 320.6750.16050).
Conflict of interest
H-LZ was affiliated with the National Natural Science Foundation of China. The views expressed are his own and do not necessarily represent the views of the National Natural Science Foundation of China or the Chinese government. The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
- 1.Xie Y, Bowe B, Mokdad AH, Xian H, Yan Y, Li T, et al. Analysis of the global burden of disease study highlights the global, regional, and national trends of chronic kidney disease epidemiology from 1990 to 2016. Kidney Int. (2018) 94:567–81. 10.1016/j.kint.2018.04.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Paterson EN, Maxwell AP, Kee F, Cruise S, Young IS, McGuinness B, et al. Association of renal impairment with cognitive dysfunction in the Northern Ireland Cohort for the Longitudinal Study of Ageing (NICOLA). Nephrol Dial Transplant. (2021) 36:1492–9. 10.1093/ndt/gfab182 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Chen JQA, Scheltens P, Groot C, Ossenkoppele R. Associations between caffeine consumption, cognitive decline, and dementia: a systematic review. J Alzheimers Dis. (2020) 78:1519–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kong JY, Kim JS, Kang MH, Hwang HS, Won CW, Jeong KH. Renal dysfunction is associated with decline of cognitive function in community-dwelling older adults: Korean frailty and aging cohort study. BMC Geriatr. (2020) 20:462. 10.1186/s12877-020-01862-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Tollitt J, Odudu A, Montaldi D, Kalra PA. Cognitive impairment in patients with moderate to severe chronic kidney disease: the Salford kidney cohort study. Clin Kidney J. (2021) 14:1639–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Luo Y, Murray AM, Guo YD, Tian R, Ye PP, Li X, et al. Cognitive impairment and associated risk factors in older adult hemodialysis patients: a cross-sectional survey. Sci Rep. (2020) 10:12542. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Viji PCSK, Sreelatha M. Chronic kidney disease and cognitive status. Int J Pharm Sci Res. (2017) 8:2194–9. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Gela YY, Getu AA, Adane A, Ayal BM, Akalu Y, Ambelu A, et al. Cognitive impairment and associated factors among chronic kidney disease patients: a comparative cross-sectional study. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2021) 17:1483–92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.O’Lone E, Connors M, Masson P, Wu S, Kelly PJ, Gillespie D, et al. Cognition in people with end-stage kidney disease treated with hemodialysis: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Kidney Dis. (2016) 67:925–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Steinbach EJ, Harshman LA. Impact of chronic kidney disease on brain structure and function. Front Neurol. (2022) 13:797503. 10.3389/fneur.2022.797503 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Nishi SK, Babio N, Gómez-Martínez C, Martínez-González M, Ros E, Corella D, et al. Mediterranean, DASH, and MIND dietary patterns and cognitive function: the 2-year longitudinal changes in an older Spanish cohort. Front Aging Neurosci. (2021) 13:782067. 10.3389/fnagi.2021.782067 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Berendsen AAM, Kang JH, van de Rest O, Feskens EJM, de Groot L, Grodstein F. The dietary approaches to stop hypertension diet, cognitive function, and cognitive decline in American older women. J Am Med Dir Assoc. (2017) 18:427–32. 10.1016/j.jamda.2016.11.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Castellana F, De Nucci S, De Pergola G, Di Chito M, Lisco G, Triggiani V, et al. Trends in coffee and tea consumption during the COVID-19 pandemic. Foods. (2021) 10:2458. 10.3390/foods10102458 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Fiani B, Zhu L, Musch BL, Briceno S, Andel R, Sadeq N, et al. The neurophysiology of caffeine as a central nervous system stimulant and the resultant effects on cognitive function. Cureus. (2021) 13:e15032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Araújo LF, Giatti L, Reis RC, Goulart AC, Schmidt MI, Duncan BB, et al. Inconsistency of association between coffee consumption and cognitive function in adults and elderly in a cross-sectional study (ELSA-Brasil). Nutrients. (2015) 7:9590–601. 10.3390/nu7115487 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Nehlig A. Interindividual differences in caffeine metabolism and factors driving caffeine consumption. Pharmacol Rev. (2018) 70:384–411. 10.1124/pr.117.014407 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Dietz C, Dekker M, Piqueras-Fiszman B. An intervention study on the effect of matcha tea, in drink and snack bar formats, on mood and cognitive performance. Food Res Int. (2017) 99(Pt 1):72–83. 10.1016/j.foodres.2017.05.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Zupo R, Castellana F, De Nucci S, Dibello V, Lozupone M, Giannelli G, et al. Beverages consumption and oral health in the aging population: a systematic review. Front Nutr. (2021) 8:762383. 10.3389/fnut.2021.762383 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Pace RC, Kirk J. Academy of nutrition and dietetics and national kidney foundation: revised 2020 standards of practice and standards of professional performance for registered dietitian nutritionists (competent, proficient, and expert) in nephrology nutrition. J Acad Nutr Diet. (2021) 121:529–44.e56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Hu EA, Selvin E, Grams ME, Steffen LM, Coresh J, Rebholz CM. Coffee consumption and incident kidney disease: results from the atherosclerosis risk in communities (ARIC) study. Am J Kidney Dis. (2018) 72:214–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Díaz-López A, Paz-Graniel I, Ruiz V, Toledo E, Becerra-Tomás N, Corella D, et al. Consumption of caffeinated beverages and kidney function decline in an elderly Mediterranean population with metabolic syndrome. Sci Rep. (2021) 11:8719. 10.1038/s41598-021-88028-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Kanbay M, Siriopol D, Copur S, Tapoi L, Benchea L, Kuwabara M, et al. Effect of coffee consumption on renal outcome: a systematic review and meta-analysis of clinical studies. J Ren Nutr. (2021) 31:5–20. 10.1053/j.jrn.2020.08.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Dong X, Li S, Sun J, Li Y, Zhang D. Association of coffee, decaffeinated coffee and caffeine intake from coffee with cognitive performance in older adults: National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) 2011-2014. Nutrients. (2020) 12:840. 10.3390/nu12030840 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Ketteler M, Block GA, Evenepoel P, Fukagawa M, Herzog CA, McCann L, et al. Diagnosis, evaluation, prevention, and treatment of chronic kidney disease-mineral and bone disorder: synopsis of the kidney disease: improving global outcomes 2017 clinical practice guideline update. Ann Intern Med. (2018) 168:422–30. 10.7326/M17-2640 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Levey AS, Stevens LA, Schmid CH, Zhang YL, Castro AF, III, Feldman HI, et al. A new equation to estimate glomerular filtration rate. Ann Intern Med. (2009) 150:604–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Hoy MK, Murayi T, Moshfegh AJ. Diet quality of frequent fast-food consumers on non-fast food intake day is similar to a day with fast food. What we eat in America, NHANES 2013-2016. J Acad Nutr Diet. (2022) 122:1317–25. 10.1016/j.jand.2022.02.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Dwyer J, Picciano MF, Raiten DJ. Collection of food and dietary supplement intake data: what we eat in America-NHANES. J Nutr. (2003) 133:590s–600s. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Larvie DY, Armah SM. Estimated phytate intake is associated with improved cognitive function in the elderly, NHANES 2013-2014. Antioxidants. (2021) 10:1104. 10.3390/antiox10071104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Morris JC, Heyman A, Mohs RC, Hughes JP, van Belle G, Fillenbaum G, et al. The consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer’s disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. (1989) 39:1159–65. 10.1212/wnl.39.9.1159 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Wei J, Wang L, Kulshreshtha A, Xu H. Adherence to life’s simple 7 and cognitive function among older adults: the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey 2011 to 2014. J Am Heart Assoc. (2022) 11:e022959. 10.1161/JAHA.121.022959 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Jaeger J. Digit symbol substitution test: the case for sensitivity over specificity in neuropsychological testing. J Clin Psychopharmacol. (2018) 38:513–9. 10.1097/JCP.0000000000000941 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Iranpour S, Saadati HM, Koohi F, Sabour S. Association between caffeine intake and cognitive function in adults; effect modification by sex: data from National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) 2013-2014. Clin Nutr. (2020) 39:2158–68. 10.1016/j.clnu.2019.09.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Bailey RL, Jun S, Murphy L, Green R, Gahche JJ, Dwyer JT, et al. High folic acid or folate combined with low vitamin B-12 status: potential but inconsistent association with cognitive function in a nationally representative cross-sectional sample of US older adults participating in the NHANES. Am J Clin Nutr. (2020) 112:1547–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Yao X, Chen L, Xu H, Zhu Z. The association between serum uric acid and bone mineral density in older adults. Int J Endocrinol. (2020) 2020:3082318. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Kurella Tamura M, Xie D, Yaffe K, Cohen DL, Teal V, Kasner SE, et al. Vascular risk factors and cognitive impairment in chronic kidney disease: the chronic renal insufficiency cohort (CRIC) study. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol. (2011) 6:248–56. 10.2215/CJN.02660310 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Tsuchiya K, Akihisa T. The importance of phosphate control in chronic kidney disease. Nutrients. (2021) 13:1670. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Song X, Li J, Hua Y, Wang C, Liu B, Liu C, et al. Chronic kidney disease is associated with intracranial artery stenosis distribution in the middle-aged and elderly population. J Atheroscler Thromb. (2020) 27:245–54. 10.5551/jat.49569 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Godino Mdel C, Romera VG, Sánchez-Tomero JA, Pacheco J, Canals S, Lerma J, et al. Amelioration of ischemic brain damage by peritoneal dialysis. J Clin Invest. (2013) 123:4359–63. 10.1172/JCI67284 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Bugnicourt JM, Godefroy O, Chillon JM, Choukroun G, Massy ZA. Cognitive disorders and dementia in CKD: the neglected kidney-brain axis. J Am Soc Nephrol. (2013) 24:353–63. 10.1681/ASN.2012050536 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Paz-Graniel I, Babio N, Becerra-Tomás N, Toledo E, Camacho-Barcia L, Corella D, et al. Association between coffee consumption and total dietary caffeine intake with cognitive functioning: cross-sectional assessment in an elderly Mediterranean population. Eur J Nutr. (2020) 60:2381–96. 10.1007/s00394-020-02415-w [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Ribeiro JA, Sebastião AM. Caffeine and adenosine. J Alzheimers Dis. (2010) 20(Suppl. 1):S3–15. 10.1089/caff.2019.0001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Nehlig A, Daval JL, Debry G. Caffeine and the central nervous system: mechanisms of action, biochemical, metabolic and psychostimulant effects. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. (1992) 17:139–70. 10.1016/0165-0173(92)90012-b [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Kolahdouzan M, Hamadeh MJ. The neuroprotective effects of caffeine in neurodegenerative diseases. CNS Neurosci Ther. (2017) 23:272–90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Jhee JH, Nam KH, An SY, Cha MU, Lee M, Park S, et al. Effects of coffee intake on incident chronic kidney disease: a community-based prospective cohort study. Am J Med. (2018) 131:1482–90.e3. 10.1016/j.amjmed.2018.05.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Bigotte Vieira M, Magriço R, Viegas Dias C, Leitão L, Neves JS. Caffeine consumption and mortality in chronic kidney disease: a nationally representative analysis. Nephrol Dial Transplant. (2019) 34:974–80. 10.1093/ndt/gfy234 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Harer MW, Rothwell AC, Richard LJ, Adegboro CO, McAdams RM. Renal tissue oxygenation after caffeine administration in preterm neonates. Pediatr Res. (2021) 90:1171–6. 10.1038/s41390-021-01579-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Nilnumkhum A, Kanlaya R, Yoodee S, Thongboonkerd V. Caffeine inhibits hypoxia-induced renal fibroblast activation by antioxidant mechanism. Cell Adh Migr. (2019) 13:260–72. 10.1080/19336918.2019.1638691 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Tateishi H, Mizoguchi Y, Monji A. Is the therapeutic mechanism of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in cognitive dysfunctions of depression related to the neuroinflammatory processes in depression? Front Psychiatry. (2022) 13:834425. 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.834425 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Cogley C, Carswell C, Bramham K, Chilcot J. Chronic kidney disease and severe mental illness: addressing disparities in access to health care and health outcomes. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol. (2022) 17:1413–7. 10.2215/CJN.15691221 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Cornelis MC, Bennett DA, Weintraub S, Schneider JA, Morris MC. Caffeine consumption and dementia: are lewy bodies the link? Ann Neurol. (2022) 91:834–46. 10.1002/ana.26349 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Londzin P, Zamora M, Kąkol B, Taborek A, Folwarczna J. Potential of caffeine in Alzheimer’s disease-a review of experimental studies. Nutrients. (2021) 13:537. 10.3390/nu13020537 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.EFSA Panel on Dietetic Products, Nutrition and Allergies. Scientific opinion on the safety of caffeine. EFSA J. (2015) 13:4102. [Google Scholar]
- 53.Sanchez JM. Methylxanthine content in commonly consumed foods in Spain and determination of its intake during consumption. Foods. (2017) 6:109. 10.3390/foods6120109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Zhou X, Zhang L. The neuroprotective effects of moderate and regular caffeine consumption in Alzheimer’s disease. Oxid Med Cell Longev. (2021) 2021:5568011. 10.1155/2021/5568011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Ritchie K, Carrière I, de Mendonca A, Portet F, Dartigues JF, Rouaud O, et al. The neuroprotective effects of caffeine: a prospective population study (the three city study). Neurology. (2007) 69:536–45. 10.1212/01.wnl.0000266670.35219.0c [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The original contributions presented in this study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.