Summary
The hippocampus occupies a central role in mammalian navigation and memory. Yet an understanding of the rules that govern the statistics and granularity of the spatial code, as well as its interactions with perceptual stimuli, is lacking. We analyzed CA1 place cell activity recorded while rats foraged in different large-scale environments. We found that place cell activity was subject to an unexpected but precise homeostasis—the distribution of activity in the population as a whole being constant at all locations within and between environments. Using a virtual reconstruction of the largest environment, we showed that the rate of transition through this statistically stable population matches the rate of change in the animals’ visual scene. Thus, place fields near boundaries were small but numerous, while in the environment’s interior, they were larger but more dispersed. These results indicate that hippocampal spatial activity is governed by a small number of simple laws and, in particular, suggest the presence of an information-theoretic bound imposed by perception on the fidelity of the spatial memory system.
Keywords: spatial memory, hippocampus, dorsal CA1, place cells, single-unit, homeostasis, perception, information theory, Weber’s law, large environment
Graphical abstract
Highlights
-
•
Neural activity in rodent CA1 place cell populations is homeostatically balanced
-
•
Hippocampal place field size and frequency are governed by proximity to boundaries
-
•
Transition rate through place cell population matches rate of change in visual scene
New work by Tanni, de Cothi, and Barry finds that place cell activity in the mammalian hippocampus is homeostatically controlled—the distribution of firing rates in the population is stable. During motion, the rate of transition through these cells matches the rate of change in the visual scene, yielding smaller, more numerous fields near walls.
Introduction
Hippocampal place cells, pyramidal neurons in regions CA1 and CA3, are distinguished by their spatially constrained firing fields.1 The activity of these cells, as a population, provides a sparse representation of self-location that is relatively independent of other variables—such as head direction and velocity2,3—and is believed to provide the neural basis of a cognitive map.4 Fifty years of research have contributed extensively to our knowledge of this system, and today, place cells are understood to be common to mammals,5,6 have been shown to be engaged as a temporal and abstract code,7,8 and are known to be reactivated during periods of quiescence.9,10
Despite these achievements, our understanding of the dynamics that control the statistics and distribution of place cell representations has been slower to advance. In part, this is due to technical barriers that make it difficult to collect long-duration, high-yield recordings in animals as they explore large spaces. Despite these constraints, a small number of groups have conducted work in extended environments, showing that individual place cells can develop multiple fields11 and exhibit distinct propensities to be recruited on long linear tracks.12,13 Similarly, investigations along the hippocampal axis identified a gradient of spatial scales, with ventral cells having considerably larger place fields than dorsal cells.14 Nevertheless, an understanding of the hippocampal population code on the implementational level, in Marr’s terms,15 is lacking. Effectively, we know little about the rules that govern activity in the place cell population, how it evolves across space, and how it is influenced by sensory information. Put simply, we do not know how place field size and density interact with each other and the environment. So, for example, although place fields are known to be smaller and more numerous in visually rich environments,16 it is not clear whether these changes are linked and how they affect activity at the level of the entire population and, thus, the implication for information transfer to downstream structures.
One practical outcome of this situation is that we do not have sufficient empirical data to arbitrate between classes of computational models. For example, geometric cue-based models describe place field locations by integrating distance and direction from environmental features such as boundaries.17,18 These models predict that the place fields near to boundaries are generally smaller, specifically being more compact perpendicular to the adjacent boundary, while more distant fields are expected to be diffuse. Notably, if fields simply became larger at locations more distant from boundaries, without some form of compensation, we would expect to observe a net increase in population firing rate toward the interior of environments. To offset this, the boundary-tuned precursors to place cells—boundary vector cells—are typically assumed to be more densely distributed adjacent to walls than at longer distances.17,19 In contrast, models based on attractor dynamics, presumed to be instantiated in region CA3, tend to ignore any systematic variance in place field size and density across environments, emphasizing even coverage and carefully balanced activity.20,21 These two classes of model, as well as others,22, 23, 24 provide competing but not incompatible explanations of hippocampal dynamics, yet the evidence needed to generate a synthesis is lacking.
Here we analyze large populations of place cells recorded while rats foraged in different-sized, equally proportioned environments of up to 8.75 m2. We find that place fields and cells are recruited in proportion to environmental area but with a strong influence of location on field frequency and size—fields are smaller and more numerous near boundaries, whereas being larger and less numerous toward the environment’s center. Surprisingly, these two effects counter each other exactly, resulting in stable population-level firing within and between environments. Thus, the proportion of co-active cells, mean firing rate, and distribution of activity across the population were preserved at all locations, suggesting the presence of a strong homeostatic mechanism governing place cell firing. Using a virtual reality (VR) replica of the recording environment, we show that the rate of change in the activity of the place cell population is strongly correlated with the rate of change in the animals’ visual scenes. Thus, although the statistics describing the distribution of place cell activity were stable across time and space, the rate at which the components of this distribution varied corresponded with the amount of perceivable change experienced by the animal. Taken together, these results suggest that the size and extent of individual place fields are well described by geometric cue-based models, whereas the population as a whole conforms to the expectations of attractor-based models. More generally, as predicted by theory,25,26 it appears that the effective scale of representations within the spatial memory system are limited by the perceptible information afforded by the environment.
Results
Using extracellular electrodes (128 channels per animal), we recorded 629 CA1 place cells (89–172 cells/rat) from five rats while they foraged for randomly dispensed rewards (20 mg pellets) in four familiar, differently sized environments. The environments—designated A–D—were identically proportioned, with each being double the area of the previous one to a maximum of 8.75 m2 (Figures 1A and S1A). The order of environments B, C, and D was randomized for each animal, with the smallest environment, A, being used at the beginning and end of the recording session. Recording duration was scaled proportional to environment area—15 to 120 min—and a single recording session consisting of 5 trials was analyzed from each animal (Figure 1B). Place cells were isolated based on their waveforms (Figure 1C) and temporal firing rate statistics (Figure 1D), with many cells being active in multiple environments (Figure 1E). The spatial rate maps of these cells were stable in all environments, with high intra-trial (1st versus 2nd half) spatial correlation across place cells (mean correlation by environment, range 0.58–0.65; Figure S1C; Data S1A). Individual place fields were detected iteratively as contiguous regions of stable firing rates continuously increasing toward a peak (STAR Methods) (Figure S1B). There was no correlation between the number of place fields detected in the largest environment and the intra-trial (1st and 2nd half) spatial correlation (r = −0.071, p = 0.100, n = 537), indicating that the observed multiple fields were not caused by intra-trial remapping.
As is typical of the hippocampal system, the spatially modulated activity of neurons in environment A was stable both within and between recordings (Figure S2A; intra-trial [A ½ versus ½] spatial correlation, 0.61; inter-trial [A versus A′], 0.51; inter-trial with shuffle [A versus shuffled A′], −0.03), while activity in the four different environments was highly separable, being sufficient to assign 94.5% of activity vectors (1 s duration) to the correct environment (STAR Methods) (Figure S2B).
In all animals, the number of active place cells—neurons with at least one spatial field—was greater in larger environments (Figures 2A and 2D), as was the total number of place fields (Figure 2B). The number of fields per cell was not correlated with the cells’ clustering quality, and only one animal exhibited a correlation between the L-ratio and field count (Table S1), indicating that the observed place cells with multiple fields were not a result of multiple single-field cells being assigned to the same cluster. Place field recruitment increased more rapidly than the number of active cells, such that the average number of fields per cell was higher in larger environments (1.28 fields/cell in A to 2.28 fields/cell in D; Figure 2C). The same place cells were recorded in all environments, allowing for the consideration of the total spatial representation of the place cell population. When all recording environments were considered collectively (total area 16.4 m2), an approach used previously in the context of hippocampal spatial memory capacity,13 it was more common for cells to have 2 fields than 1, and 84.8% of all cells had multiple fields either within (Data S1B) or across environments (Figure 2C). Furthermore, consistent with previous studies,12,13 we found that individual cells had different propensities to form place fields, and that this proclivity was maintained across environments (Figure S3C), meaning that cells with numerous fields in one environment were more likely to have numerous fields in another. Thus, the number of fields per cells were better fit with a gamma-Poisson model—which allows for cells to have different rates of field formation—than an equal-Poisson model13—which assumes the same frequency of field formation for all place cells (log-likelihoods, −1,538 versus −1,679; Bayesian information criterion, 3,089 versus 3,365). If all cells formed fields at the same rate, then the number of fields per cell would follow a Poisson distribution, with its mean determined by the environment area and field formation rate.12,13 In our data, this simpler equal-Poisson model consistently overestimates the proportion of cells with 4–6 fields across the collective environment (Figures S2D–S2F). In contrast, the gamma-Poisson model accurately fits the rate at which place cells were recruited (≥1 field) as a function of environment area (Figure 2D), predicting that 99% of CA1 place cells will have at least one place field in environments greater than 51.8 m2 (Figure 2D, inset).
To quantify the relationship between the number of place fields and environment size, we examined how field recruitment varied with area—normalizing place field counts by total number of fields detected in each animal. We found a remarkably robust linear regression with a positive intercept (r = 0.983, p = 10−14, slope = 0.038, intercept = 0.093, n = 20; Figure 2E). The r value for the fitted line can be viewed as being particularly high because each datapoint—the proportion of place fields detected per animal, per environment—is the average of a large number of samples (each animal has several hundred place fields) and therefore is less susceptible to fluctuations. Notably, this linear fit indicates that the proportion of place fields per unit area is lower in larger spaces (Figure 2E, inset; Kruskal-Wallis: H = 17.9, p = 4.71 × 10−4, n = 5).
Next, to understand why place fields were less numerous per unit area in large environments, we examined how fields were distributed within environments. To this end, we segmented the space into concentric bands according to distance to the nearest boundary (each band 25 cm wide). In environments B–D, for which at least two bands could be defined, we calculated the density of place field peaks and found that it was greater near to the walls, reducing toward the environment center (Kruskal-Wallis tests: env. B, H = 4.81, p = 0.028; env. C, H = 12.5, p = 0.002; env. D, H = 16.1, p = 0.001; n = 5; Figure 3A). Equally, considering only the band closest to the wall (<25 cm), field peak density was generally higher in smaller environments (Figure 3A; Kruskal-Wallis: H = 12.4, p = 6.02 × 10−3, n = 5). These effects could not be explained by the difference in dwell time between locations (Figure S3A). In direct contrast, the average size of place fields increased with distance from the wall (Kruskal-Wallis tests: env. B, H = 6.82, p = 0.009; env. C, H = 10.2, p = 0.006; env. D, H = 10.5, p = 0.015; n = 5; Figure 3B) and were smaller in environment A (Kruskal-Wallis: H = 15.9, p = 0.001, n = 5; Figure 3B). In particular, it appeared that the predominant factor contributing to this effect was that the field width in a given axis was proportional to the nearest wall distance along that axis (Figure S3B) and not to wall distance orthogonal to that axis (Figures 3C and 3D; Kruskal-Wallis: H = 12.1, p = 0.007, n = 5; H = 0.39, p = 0.94, n = 5, respectively). The average field size of a cell also did not correlate with its clustering quality (Table S1). Therefore, compared with locations near the walls of an enclosure, there were on average fewer individual place fields further from the walls, but those fields that were present tended to be larger.
Considered alone, the observed decrease in the density of field peaks away from the boundary would be expected to result in fewer active place cells, yielding a lower mean firing rate. Conversely, the increase in field size would lead to a greater overlap between place fields, resulting in the opposite outcome. Remarkably, we found that these two effects were exactly balanced, meaning that neither the proportion of co-active place cells (firing rate > 1 Hz) nor the mean firing rate of the population varied with distance from the wall (Figures 4A and 4B; Kruskal-Wallis: H = 1.2, p = 0.77 n = 5; H = 0.61, p = 0.89, n = 5, respectively). This was the case within and across all environments (Figures S4A and S4B). Similarly, interneuron firing rates were also constant at different distances to the bounding walls (Figure S4C; Kruskal-Wallis: H = 0.23, p = 0.97, n = 5). Furthermore, the distribution of firing rates in the place cell population was also stable across different distances to the wall, as there was no difference in how variable the firing rate distributions were in different recording halves at the same location or at different locations (see STAR Methods for more details; comparison of distribution divergence within and between locations with Mann-Whitney: U = 45, p = 0.43, n = 4 and 18; Figure 4C). These results are underlined by a very high correlation between the environment size and the total place field area in that environment, expressed as a proportion of the total area of all fields recorded from an animal (linear regression: r = 0.996, p = 10−20, slope = 0.06, intercept = 0.006, n = 20) (Figure 4D). The r value for this correlation can be viewed as being particularly high because each datapoint is aggregating across a large number of place fields (between 50 and 300) and so is less susceptible to fluctuations.
Thus, taken together, there appear to be several consistent and related features of the place cell code for space. First, the total area of place fields active in an environment is a near perfect linear function of the environment’s area. Second, population activity is homeostatically balanced, maintaining a constant proportion of active cells (15%) and mean firing rate (0.52 Hz/place cell)—despite field size growing with distance to walls. Finally, and more generally, the distribution of activity in the place cell population is also stable across space.
The distribution of firing rates in the place cell population is constant, but the size of place fields and the density of their peaks varies systematically; this implies that hippocampal activity must change at different rates depending on the location of the animal and the direction in which it is moving (Figures S5E–S5J). Specifically, because place field width in a given axis is strongly determined by distance to the nearest wall orthogonal to that axis, the activity vector (see STAR Methods on population activity change) should change fastest when the animal is near a wall and moving directly toward or away from it. Thus, analyzing only trajectories running orthogonal to the short wall of the largest environment (D), we found that the Euclidean distance between activity vectors (1-cm intervals) was the greatest when animals were close to the wall (Kruskal-Wallis: H = 12.2, p = 0.007, n = 5; Figure 5A), an effect that was not found for parallel runs (Kruskal-Wallis: H = 3.78, p = 0.29, n = 5; difference between orthogonal and parallel, Mann-Whitney: U = 2, p = 0.018, n = 5; Figure 5B). Similar effects were confirmed in the other environments (Figures S5A and S5B). There was no correlation between this measure and theta frequency, while accounting for running speed in partial correlation analysis (Figure S5C), despite prior work having shown that theta oscillations organize place cell firing and field sizes.27 Note that by analyzing the instantaneous rate of change in the population as a whole, we avoid the difficulty of estimating field size adjacent to boundaries, where it is difficult to determine how much of a field would exist outside the walls of the environment. This same analysis also eliminates the need to derive measures from individual rate maps, which represent average spatial firing over time and can be distorted when neural activity is modulated by factors such as heading direction.28 By replicating the analysis of Keinath et al.,28 we did not observe a systematic offset in the cross-correlograms between opposing boundary-tethered rate maps (t(99) = 1.1, p = 0.27), which may be of concern given the imperfect overlap of fields between runs (Data S1A); however, using instantaneous rate of change further maximizes the fidelity of our analysis against such errors.
Finally, to investigate whether visual information contributed to this effect, we developed a VR replica of environment D (Figure S5D). A 300° field of view was used to reconstruct each animals’ visual scene at 1-cm increments along its trajectory (Figure 5C), and the visual change between consecutive frames was calculated during motion (speed >10 cm/s; STAR Methods). We found a tight coupling between the change in visual scene and change in place cell population activity (Figures 5D and 5G; time series correlation collapsed across all animals: Pearson’s r = 0.60, p < 0.001, n = 510,871), which is a stronger relationship than that was found for other behavioral variables (Figure 5G) and which persisted when they were accounted for (partial correlation collapsed across all animals: r = 0.51, p < 0.001, n = 510,871). As expected, when mapped out in space, both the activity vector change and visual change showed a general increase with proximity to the walls, corners, and the two wall-mounted cues (Figures 5E and 5F; no smoothing applied), as well as a strong correlation between both maps (correlation between maps collapsed across all animals: Pearson’s r = 0.72, p < 0.001, n = 5,524). Importantly, filtering the behavior by heading direction (Figure 5H) emphasizes how this variation depends not only on position but also on movement direction, with walls and cues orthogonal to the direction of motion being key factors in driving the visual change—a pattern that closely mirrored the direction-filtered activity vector change (direction-filtered map correlations collapsed across all animals: Pearson’s r > 0.66, all p < 0.001, N > 5,306 for all 4 cardinal heading directions).
Discussion
Using high-yield recordings from rodents foraging in large environments, we have shown that although the distribution and extent of individual place fields are governed by proximity to environmental boundaries, the statistics of the population activity as a whole are effectively constant. Specifically, the widths of place fields displayed a Weber’s law-like29 increase with distance to the orthogonal wall, which was exactly opposed by a commensurate decrease in field count. Thus, on average, small place fields were densely distributed near the boundaries of the environments, while broader fields were found more sparsely toward the middle. Practically, this means that the combined field area of all active place cells scales linearly with environment area because the reduced size of place fields in small environments is offset by their increased number and vice versa. Therefore, while the instantaneous activity in the population was stable, the rate of change through the population depends on both boundary proximity and movement direction, in a manner that correlates strongly with the rate of change in the animals’ reconstructed visual scenes.
Place cells have often been conceptualized as pure allocentric representations of space fully abstracted from sensory stimuli,4 and at first glance, this seems difficult to reconcile with the results reported here. However, our findings encapsulate a large body of prior work, suggesting that perceptual experience plays a larger than expected role in the formation of spatial representations. Thus, results showing that individual cells have more and larger fields in bigger spaces11,13 can be understood in terms of a balance between homeostasis at the population level and the amount of perceptual change, which is naturally elevated near boundaries and cues, as well as other locations where the visual scene changes rapidly as the animal moves. Equally, this same relationship accounts for the clustering of place fields near visual cues in a 1D VR,16 as well as a similar clustering of fields close to the walls,30 doorways,31 and other environmental features19 of physical spaces. It is also consistent with the observation that place cell firing in a virtual environment predominantly reflects visual inputs following a manipulation of the relationship between motion inputs and visual gain32 and can be sustained solely on the basis of visual cues when an animal is moved passively.33,34 However, given that place cell responses can be oriented by sounds and smells35,36 and persist in darkness,37 as well as in congenitally blind rodents,38 it seems highly likely that information from other modalities also contributes to the fidelity of spatial representations. Hence, more generally, we propose that the resolution of the hippocampal code for space is determined by the rate of change in the cross-modal perceptual environment. Recent observations that rodent place fields concentrate at the interfaces between different textures,39 points where the tactile milieu changes rapidly, support this view.
Our work resonates with early geometric cue-based models of place cell activity that sought to describe place fields as the threshold sum of putative boundary responsive neurons, evidence for which has been found in the subiculum and entorhinal cortex.17, 18, 19 Notably, these models focused on the description of individual fields but were agnostic of population-level interactions.20,21 Even so, geometric models generally assumed that boundary responses would be more diffuse for cells tuned to distant walls and that long-range boundary responses would be less common than short-range tuning.17,19 Interestingly, early formulations of these geometric models explicitly linked the tuning width of boundary responses to rate of change in the visual scene—specifically rate of change in the angle subtended to the top of boundary40—foreshadowing our results. Although these components yield spatial responses that broadly match our observations, it is unclear whether preconfigured boundary responses alone would be sufficient to generate a precise homeostasis. Thus, our current results likely indicate that a synthesis of both cell-level and population-level approaches is important for understanding how the hippocampus represents large-scale spaces—yoking the evolution of statistically stable population-level activity to movement through visual states.24,41
The precise mechanism that maintains the observed population-level statistics is unclear but might plausibly result from metabolic homeostasis imposed by limitations on blood flow, or the energetic limitations of the neurons themselves,42 and could be modeled as competitive learning between hippocampal inputs for these limited resources.43 Equally, it may well be a natural limitation of the highly recurrent CA3 network and the need to balance excitation with inhibition to avoid a runaway increase in activity.44
Recent publications have investigated the hippocampal map in larger environments,45,46 with a focus on describing the variability of place field size.11, 12, 13,47,48 A notable exception from these was a study describing how and why this distribution changes between locations, even claiming that there is no variability in place field size for small environments (i.e., no multiscale representation).45 By recording across more than two differently sized environments, our results corroborate another study,46 showing that there is a broad distribution of place field sizes in all environments and that larger environments allow for the mean of this distribution to increase, making the variability more prominent. Importantly, we extend this prior work, showing that it is precisely this change in the field size that can be explained by a simple mechanism—the rate of perceptual change afforded by the environment. We also corroborate findings from previous work that compared gamma-Poisson and equal-Poisson models in describing place cell field formation propensities on linear track and VR environments,12,13 extending their findings to an open field environment.
Importantly, the relationship between visual change and place cell fidelity can be understood in terms of information theory. The rate of change of perceptual states is tightly linked to the amount of Fisher information they transmit about an unknown parameter49—in this case, position. Regions where the visual scene changes rapidly with respect to position—near the walls, for example—convey more information about self-location than regions where visual stimuli change more slowly. Indeed, previous work has shown that the spatial information conveyed by place cell activity is higher in the presence of cues,30 with place fields closest to visual cues being the most informative.50 In turn, for simple neural codes in one and two dimensions, the tuning width of a given neuron is inversely proportional to the Fisher information it carries.25,26 If we make the reasonable assumption that the place code maximizes information transmitted up to the limit imposed by vision, then this directly predicts that place field width will be inversely proportional to the rate of change in the visual scene. Alternatively stated, the rate of change in place cell population activity is expected to be proportional to visual change—the result we observe. Notably, this relationship is not necessarily specific to vision or place cells. Thus, it seems plausible that the scale and fidelity of other neural representations of self-location must be subject to the same information-theoretic limits. Indeed, the increase in entorhinal grid cell scale noted toward the center of large environments,51 and potentially other spatial distortions,52,53 can be seen through the same lens.
STAR★Methods
Key resources table
REAGENT or RESOURCE | SOURCE | IDENTIFIER |
---|---|---|
Chemicals, peptides, and recombinant proteins | ||
Cresyl violet | Sigma Aldrich | Product code: C5042, http://www.sigmaaldrich.com/catalog/product/sigma/c5042?lang=en®ion=US |
Histoclear | National Diagnostics | Product code: HS-202, https://www.nationaldiagnostics.com/histology/product/histo-clear-ii |
Deposited data | ||
Supporting data for: State transitions in the statistically stable place cell population are determined by rate of perceptual change | figshare | Figshare: https://doi.org/10.5522/04/18128891.v1 |
Experimental models: Organisms/strains | ||
Lister Hooded rats | Charles River | http://www.criver.com/products-services/basic-research/find-a-model/lister-hooded?loc=GB |
Software and algorithms | ||
Custom Python code | Zenodo | Zenodo: https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.5842287 |
Unity3D | https://unity3d.com/get-unity/download | 2019 LTS |
Tint Spike sorting software | Axona | Product code: COMP/TINT01, http://axona.com/products |
Other | ||
Single-screw mouse microdrive | Axona | Product code: MDMR-01M1, http://axona.com/products |
NanoZ plating equipment | Multichannel Systems | nanoZ, http://www.multichannelsystems.com/products/nanoz |
Open EPhys Acquisition Board | Open Ephys Organisation | https://open-ephys.org/acquisition-system/eux9baf6a5s8tid06hk1mw5aafjdz1 |
Intan Standard SPI interface cable | Intan Technologies | RHD2000 3-ft (1.8 m), https://intantech.com/products_RHD2000.html |
Intan amplifier/accelerometer board with 64 unipolar inputs | Intan Technologies | RHD2164, https://intantech.com/products_RHD2000.html |
32 channel connector, Omnetics | Genalog | A79621-001, https://genalog.com/ |
12.7 μm HM-L coated Stablohm 650 | California Fine Wire Company | 100187, http://www.calfinewire.com |
17 μm H HL coated platinum-iridium | California Fine Wire Company | 100167, http://www.calfinewire.com |
Resource availability
Lead contact
Further information and requests for resources and reagents should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the lead contact, Caswell Barry (caswell.barry@ucl.ac.uk).
Materials availability
This study did not generate new unique reagents.
Experimental model and subject details
Animals and tetrode implantation
Five male Lister Hooded rats were used for this study. All procedures were approved by the UK Home Office, subject to the restrictions and provisions contained in the Animals Scientific Procedures Act of 1986. All rats (333-386 g/13-17 weeks old at implantation) were implanted with two microdrives targeted to the right and left CA1 (ML: 2.5 mm, AP: 3.8 mm posterior to bregma, DV: 1.6 mm from dura) following a standard surgery and recovery procedure.54 After surgery, rats were housed individually in Perspex cages (70 cm long x 45 cm wide x 30 cm high) on a 12 hr light/dark cycle. Screening and experiments took place during the dark phase of the cycle. After one week of recovery, rats were maintained at 90-95% of free-feeding weight with ad libitum access to water.
The hair around the incision site was removed, and the skin was sterilized with Betadine. The animal was placed on a heating pad for the duration of the surgery to maintain body temperature. Viscotears Liquid Gel was used to protect the animal’s eyes. General anesthesia during the operation was maintained with an isoflurane-oxygen mix of 1.5-3% at 3 l/min. Carprieve (1:10) and Baytril were injected subcutaneously (0.1 ml/100 g) before the surgery for analgesia and to minimize chances of infection, respectively. Baytril was also included in post-operative treatment in their water for one week. An analgesic, Metacam Oral Suspension suspended in jelly, was administered for three days post-surgery. The tetrodes were implanted through ∼1 mm trephine craniotomies over target sites, and they were fixed to the exposed skull with dental cement (Super-Bond C&B) and six bone screws. A gold pin used as ground and reference was soldered to one of the orbital bone screws before its implantation. The craniotomies and elements of the microdrives were protected from dental cement using Vaseline.
Method details
Electrophysiological and behavioral recordings
Each single-screw microdrive (Axona) was assembled with two 32 channel Omnetics connectors (A79026-001), 16 tetrodes of twisted wires (either 17 μm H HL coated platinum-iridium, 90% and 10% respectively, or 12.7 μm HM-L coated Stablohm 650; California Fine Wire), and platinum-plated to reduce impedance to below 150 kΩ at 1 kHz (NanoZ).
Electrophysiological recordings were acquired using Open Ephys recording system55 and a 64-channel amplifier board per drive (Intan RHD2164). The recorded signal was referenced to an orbital bone screw - also the ground for the amplifier boards. The Open Ephys Acquisition Board was grounded to an aluminum foil sheet positioned underneath the vinyl flooring throughout the entire extent of the experimental room. Electrophysiological signals were recorded from 128 channels at 30 kHz. Spikes were detected as negative threshold crossings of more than 50 μV in the 30 kHz signal after bandpass filtering between 600 and 6000 Hz. For each spike, waveforms were stored at 30 kHz for the 1.2 ms window surrounding the threshold crossing. The waveforms are displayed and discussed in their inverted form, where the largest deflection from baseline is a positive peak.
Positional tracking was performed with an open-source multi-camera tracking system SpatialAutoDACQ.56 The output position data from SpatialAutoDACQ was the spatial coordinate of an infra-red LED positioned above the animal’s ears, sampled at 30 Hz.
Histology
Anatomical locations of recordings were verified using histology. Rats were anesthetized with isoflurane and given intraperitoneal injection of Euthatal (sodium pentobarbital) overdose (0.5 ml / 100 g) after which they were transcardially perfused with saline, followed by a 10% Formalin solution. Brains were removed and stored in 10% Formalin and 30% sucrose solution for 3-4 days before sectioning. Subsequently, 50 μm frozen coronal sections were cut using a cryostat, mounted on gelatin coated or positively charged glass slides, stained with cresyl violet and cleared with clearing agent (Histo-Clear II), before covering with DPX and coverslips. Sections were then inspected using an Olympus microscope, and tetrode tracks reaching into CA1 pyramidal cell layer were verified.
Experimental paradigm
Screenings for a suitable place cell yield were performed from one week after surgery in a 1.4 x 1.4 m environment, different from those used in any of the experiments. Tetrodes were gradually advanced in 62.5 μm steps until ripple oscillations could be observed, and pyramidal cells with stable firing fields could be identified.
Screenings, training, and experiments all took place in the same experimental room, using environments constructed of the same materials. Environments had black vinyl flooring; were constructed of 60 cm high modular boundaries (MDF) colored matt black, surrounded by black curtains on the sides and above. Each environment was illuminated by an elevated (2 m) diffuse daylight lamp from each corner of the environment that was adjacent to a corner of the experimental room (multiple lights in larger environments), with each lamp producing between 30-50 Lux/m. All experiments involved scattered 20 mg chocolate-flavored pellets (Dustless Precision Pellets Rodent, Purified, Bio-Serv, USA) dropped into the environment by an automated system in SpatialAutoDACQ to encourage foraging. The automated system scattered the pellets randomly with greater preference for areas least visited by the animal.
Place cells were recorded as each animal foraged in four environments of different size (Figures 1A and S1A). The environment sizes were 87.5 x 125 cm (environment A), 175 x 125 cm (environment B), 175 x 250 cm (environment C) and 350 x 250 cm (environment D). All environments were rectangles with close to identical shape (axes ratio 1.40). Environment A was the smallest, and the other sequentially larger environments – B, C and D – each doubled in size by doubling the length of the shortest axis (Figure 1A). There were two sets of cues in the environments. The most prominent cue elevated above the wall of the enclosure was different in all environments, varying in size and the type of pattern, but always black and white. Two different secondary smaller cues were used, an A4 sheet (11 × 16 cm) with a dot pattern and a set of three adjacent A4 pages, placed on the wall at a height the animal could not reach. The number of secondary cues and the size of the primary cues varied slightly between environments to scale with their size (Figure 1A).
During a single session consisting of 5 trials, the place cells of an animal were recorded in all four environments and twice in environment A (Figure 1B). The second recording in environment A is referred to as a recording in environment A’. The duration of the recording in the smallest environment (A) was 15 minutes. The recording duration doubled along with the size of each environment, reaching 120 minutes in the largest environment (D). Each animal was recorded on three or four sessions, the data analyzed here comes from the first session in which an animal achieved good spatial sampling in all environments (sessions 3, 2, 2, 3, and 4, for the 5 animals).
After a recording in each environment, the flooring was wiped with an unscented soap solution to clear any potential olfactory cues. The animal was kept in a familiar rest box (with water provided) between each recording for 15 to 30 minutes, while the preceding and following environments were disassembled and reassembled, respectively.
Cell identification
Spikes were assigned unit identities with automated clustering software (KlustaKwik)57 based on spike waveforms. The results from the automated clustering were curated using an offline data analysis suite (Tint, Axona, St. Albans, UK) to further separate under-clustered units and merge over-clustered units.
All recordings from a given animal that were performed on the same session were clustered simultaneously, concatenating the spike waveform data. Therefore, the same set of units were identified across all such recordings. This approach made it possible to analyze the properties of the same place cell population in multiple conditions.
L-ratio58 and Isolation Distance59 were calculated through Mahalanobis distance to verify that sorting quality has not affected the results. The features used for this analysis were the same as those used for automated and manual clustering: amplitude, time-to-peak, time-to-trough, peak-to-trough, half-width, trough-ratio, and the first three PCA components of waveforms. These measures were computed on waveforms combining all clusters, including noise, and pooling across all recordings – the same way as was done for spike sorting.
Place cells were identified computationally after the clustering procedure. The following criteria were used to identify place cells:
-
•
Waveform peak-to-trough duration of over 0.45 ms.
-
•
Waveform peak half-width of over 0.1 ms.
-
•
The ratio between amplitude and trough voltage values (trough-ratio) of over 0.175.
-
•
Spatial correlation of odd and even minute ratemaps of over 0.5 in at least one recording.
-
•
Spatial correlation of first and last half ratemaps of over 0.25 in at least one recording.
-
•
At least one field in one of the recordings (field detection method described below).
-
•
Mean firing rate across all recordings lower than 4 Hz.
Place cells were further filtered for duplicates recorded on separate tetrodes. Duplicate units were considered to be unit pairs that passed the following criteria mostly based on cross-correlograms with 2 ms bins and a maximum lag of 25 ms:
-
•
At least 200 spikes at 0-lag.
-
•
Lower than 0.5 ms sigma of a gaussian fitted to the cross-correlogram.
-
•
Mean spatial correlation of ratemaps higher than 0.5 across recordings where both units have at least 200 spikes.
If duplicate units were detected, the one with more total spikes was set as noise, to maximize signal to noise ratio. This approach was based on the observation that the unit in the duplicated pair that had more spikes was usually less well isolated from noise or other units.
Interneurons were identified based on the following criteria:
-
•
Minimum mean firing rate of 4 Hz across all recordings.
-
•
Maximum waveform half-width of 150 μs.
-
•
Maximum trough-ratio of 0.4.
-
•
Maximum spatial correlation of 0.75 in any environment.
Computing ratemaps
To calculate a ratemap for each unit, the position data was binned into 4 cm square bins, and the number of position samples in each bin was divided by the sampling rate, producing the dwell time for each spatial bin. Spike timestamps were paired with simultaneous position samples and assigned to corresponding spatial bins, thereby producing spike counts for each spatial bin. Only the position samples and spike timestamps from periods where the animal was moving at more than 10 cm/s were used to produce these dwell time and spike count maps. Both dwell times and spike counts were smoothed with a Gaussian kernel (standard deviation of 2 spatial bins) while setting unsampled bin values and those outside the environment to 0. The resulting smoothed spike counts were divided by smoothed dwell times, producing spatial ratemaps.
Place field detection
Place fields were detected in spatial ratemaps to analyze place cell properties at the level of individual place fields. Here, a place field is defined as a contiguous area in a ratemap, where the firing rate decays continuously from a single prominent peak, and the observed firing rates are well correlated across multiple visits to the same location. Often individual place fields are so close to each other that the firing rate threshold traditionally used for place field detection (1 Hz) would not be able to detect them as separate place fields. This effect is exacerbated by the spatial smoothing step in computing spatial ratemaps. However, based on the definition above, these areas should be considered as separate place fields.
An iterative thresholding method was used to find spatial bins that constituted a single place field in a ratemap (Figure S1B). As a first step, the ratemap was thresholded at 1 Hz, and contiguous groups of bins (ignoring diagonal connections) were identified as candidate fields. The regions including at least 10 spatial bins and a peak value of at least 2 Hz were considered as valid candidate fields. The ratemap of each valid candidate field was then thresholded again with a 0.05 Hz higher threshold (1.05 Hz), and the same method of finding contiguous regions and their validation was applied. This was done iteratively, resulting in continuously smaller regions, each with a higher threshold and associated with their parent field candidates with a lower threshold, some having more than one child field candidate.
The resulting lists were then parsed in reverse order, starting with the smallest candidate fields with highest thresholds. Candidate fields that were too large (greater than half the bins of the ratemap) or not sufficiently stable over repeated visits to the region (spatial correlation of odd and even minute ratemaps below 0.25) were ignored. As the increasingly lower threshold candidate fields overlapping with each other were assessed, the lowest threshold valid candidate field in a sequence of overlapping candidate fields was detected as a place field. The overlapping candidate fields with a higher threshold were ignored. If more than one child candidate field of a lower threshold candidate field was valid, the large single candidate field was ignored, and the smaller valid candidate fields were detected as separate place fields. In this manner, multiple place fields were detected in individual ratemaps of single units, as illustrated in Figure S1B.
Position decoding
Position decoding was used to estimate the location encoded in the activity of a place cell population at specific timepoints. The probability of the animal being at each location in the environment is computed based on the similarity between the ongoing firing rates of place cells in a time-window (e.g. 1 second) and their spatial ratemaps. The decoded location is then identified as the one with the highest likelihood. The spatial ratemaps for this purpose were computed using periods where the animal was moving faster than 10 cm/s, excluding the time-point that was being decoded – cross-validation with a 3-minute window. The method of matching the ongoing population activity to spatial ratemaps has been used previously60,61 and is based on the original formulation by Zhang et al.62
Specifically, the population activity of units was computed, where is the spike rate of the i-th unit in a temporal bin (e.g. 1 second). Expected population activity at location , belonging to the set of all spatial ratemap bin centres , was based on the values of all units in the spatial ratemap corresponding to that location bin , such that , refers to the value in the spatial ratemap of unit at location . These representations of neural activity were used to compute the conditional probability of observing , at location as:
(Equation 1) |
This method allows assessing the probability of any spatial bin being decoded independently of the number of bins considered and their spatial arrangement. It is agnostic to the animal’s real location and past decoded locations, as it considers all locations to have equal prior probability – it has a flat prior. The location encoded in the population activity was then computed as the centre of the spatial bin with the highest conditional probability:
(Equation 2) |
To decode the environment from the place cell population activity (Figure S2B), the posterior probability distribution (Equation 1) was calculated over all visited bins in all environments. The environment pertaining to the most probable spatial bin (Equation 2) was then identified as the decoded environment.
Spatial correlation
Spatial correlation was used to quantify the similarity between spatial ratemaps of pairs of cells or spatial ratemaps of the same cell that were constructed using data from different parts of the same recording. Spatial correlation was the Pearson correlation coefficient for pairs of values from spatial bins with matching locations in two ratemaps. For a spatial bin to be included, it must have had a firing rate above 0.01 Hz in at least one of the ratemaps to avoid high correlations between 0 Hz bins. Unvisited bins were ignored. At least 6 such valid spatial bins were required for spatial correlation to be computed, which was always the case for place cells.
Field formation models
Two models were used to estimate place field count per cell as a function of environment size: equal-Poisson and gamma-Poisson models. The equal-Poisson model has one parameter, the average field formation propensity , which is constant for all cells, and the model predicts the field counts per cell as a function of and environment area in m2
(Equation 3) |
The gamma-Poisson model estimates the field formation propensities in the place cell population based on a shape and scale parameters
(Equation 4) |
These field formation propensities are then used to estimate the place field counts
(Equation 5) |
Gamma-Poisson can then be defined using a negative binomial
(Equation 6) |
Using the change of variables =,
(Equation 7) |
giving the gamma-Poisson probability mass function
(Equation 8) |
The parameters for both models were optimized using maximum likelihood estimation using the field counts of all place cells (N = 627) in the combined environment - counting fields per cell across all four environments. The parameter optimization was performed with L-BFGS-B solver implemented in the Scipy Python package.63 The two models were compared using the Bayesian information criterion.
The gamma-Poisson model was fit as described above, and then evaluated on prediction of proportion of cell recruitment with the physical environment size values and also with environment sizes adjusted based on field density. The latter always performed better, therefore, all the reported results were computed using environment sizes adjusted based on field density. The proportion of fields in each environment was modeled with linear regression , where is the area of an environment (Figure 2E). Therefore, using the same parameters and the field density in an environment of size can be computed as
(Equation 9) |
The field density adjusted environment size for computing the gamma-Poisson probability mass function in an environment with size was computed as
(Equation 10) |
Proportion of place cells recruited to form at least one field in the four environments (A, B, C and D) based on the gamma-Poisson model was computed by modelling a population of 100,000 cells. Each cell had a field formation propensity (drawn from the gamma distribution defined by and fit already previously to field formation propensities) that was used to compute the number of fields using a Poisson process with rate or . Modelled cells with no field in any of the environments were ignored to match the conditions applied to the experimental data.
Field size measures
The place field areas were computed by summing the area of all spatial bins (16 cm2) covered by each detected field. The place field widths in the two axes were computed as the length of the field’s projection onto a given axis (Figure S3B). These values were used to calculate the mean field area and width in each animal at every spatial bin by averaging the values of all fields overlapping a particular spatial bin. To estimate the average field area and width at different distances to the wall, the values for spatial bins in a particular range of distance from the wall (e.g. 0-25 cm) were averaged separately for each animal. Where further spatial selection is indicated in the cartoons above figures (e.g. only including data from the middle third of the environment), the averaged spatial bins were selected in such manner to minimize the effects from orthogonal walls.
Population activity statistics
The proportion of co-active place cells and the mean firing rate of place cells at different distances to the wall were computed by averaging the spatial ratemap values in all spatial bins that were in that range of distances from the wall. The proportion of co-active cells (firing rate ≥1 Hz) and the mean firing rate was computed using the spatial ratemap values at a given location, including all place cells detected in a given animal.
The population firing rate distributions were computed by aggregating the Gaussian smoothed (1 second sigma) firing rates aligned to position samples (30 Hz), across animals, where the animals’ location was within a particular range of distance from the wall. At each timepoint, only the activity of neurons with firing rate ≥1 Hz were included to facilitate measuring firing rate distributions. Only samples when the animal was moving faster than 10 cm/s were included. The samples assigned to each range of distances from the wall were further split temporally into the first and second half. The Jensen-Shannon divergence, a measure of similarity between two distributions, was then computed between each pair of temporal halves, to quantify inherent variation, and also between all halves at different distances to the wall, to quantify variation in the firing rate distributions arising from difference in the animals’ position.
Population activity change
Position data smoothed with Savitzky–Golay filter (166 ms window and polynomial order 5) and place cell firing rates computed at 33 ms bins and smoothed with a Gaussian (166 ms sigma) were used to construct population activity vectors for computing the population activity change. The position data was reduced to one dimension by computing the Euclidean distance cumulatively over consecutive samples. It was then used to linearly interpolate cell firing rates to position samples 1 cm apart. The population activity change was then computed as the Euclidean distance between consecutive samples of place cell firing rates - population activity vectors measured at 1 cm intervals. All place cells detected in an animal were included and periods where the animal was moving slower than 10 cm/s were excluded.
Population activity change was used in place of a direct rate map analysis as it is less susceptible to issues caused by ‘cut-off’ place fields at boundaries (Data S1B) and intra-trial remapping (Data S1A). To explain why we performed a series of simulations. First, we simulated N=1271 equal-sized Gaussian place fields distributed to evenly span the largest space used in the study. Thus the centers of some place fields lay outside of the walls, and so were not accessible to the ‘rat’, giving rise to ‘cut off’ place fields (Figure S5E). In these cases the effective peak of the field lay against the wall. Despite this, and in accordance with our results, the total population activity of the simulated place cells is the same at every measurable position in the environment (Figure S5F, top). This also means that the measurable rate map peaks, confined by the enclosure, are more densely distributed at the boundaries. Most importantly, however, the rate of change in the place cell population vector is constant everywhere (Figure S5F, bottom) due to the uniform level of overlap between fields in the measurable space - a result that holds true irrespective of heading direction (Figure S5G). Conversely, if instead of using population measures we were to just examine the size of individual place fields, then these would necessarily appear to be smaller near to the walls because of the cut off portions. For these reasons we prefer the population-level measures.
Next, we adapted our simulation to capture the main elements of our experimental findings (Figure S5H): 1) Place field centers are more densely distributed closer to boundaries (here we use a 1/x2 scaling for field density, abstracted from the derivative of the angle to the top of a boundary wall that is distance x away) 2) Field width orthogonal to the boundary follows the inverse scaling (i.e. scales with x2) such that 3) The total population firing is the same everywhere (Figure S5I, top). Here we see that now the rate of change in the population vector is greater near the boundaries of the environment (Figure S5I, bottom), and in particular this is mainly the case when travelling orthogonally to those boundaries (Figure S5J). Note, the predictions of this model are directly in line with our main results (Figure 5) - although this modelling does not take into account the position of the visual cues in the environment which can be seen to also have a strong effect on the rate of change in the population vector (Figures 5G and 5H - high rate of change is visible at the edges of the visual cue on the North wall).
Visual change
The virtual environment was created in Unity3D with the same proportions as the physical environment. Animal trajectories were speed filtered (>10 cm/s) and interpolated so that consecutive samples were 1 cm apart (equivalent to method in population activity change). The visual scene from each sample point was then captured by three greyscale cameras, raised the equivalent of 5 cm from the floor and angled 35° above the horizontal axis. These cameras were oriented 100° apart in the horizontal plane, and each rendered a 64x64 pixel image with a field of view of 100° to give a total field of view of 300°. The absolute difference between consecutively sampled greyscale images was used to yield the pixel-by-pixel change at each sampling point. This pixel-by-pixel change was then z-scored per pixel and averaged across pixels to generate a single value for visual change at each sample point.
Visual change and population activity change maps represent the average value of samples across the environment using a 4x4 cm bin size and no smoothing applied. For illustration purposes, the time series presented in Figure 5D was smoothed with a 1D boxcar filter of width 3, but no smoothing was applied when calculating the reported correlations between time series.
Path Integration
Points of contact with a particular wall were determined as the animal being within 12cm of it.64 The path integration variable for a given sampling point in the trajectory was then taken as the cumulative distance travelled along the trajectory since the last point of contact with a wall.
Quantification and statistical analysis
The details of statistical analysis - test statistics, p value, and sample size (N) - can be found in figure legends or the relevant parts of the results section. Any exclusion of data is detailed in the results section and figures.
In most cases Kruskal-Wallis test was used to test for differences between groups due to small sample sizes. Positive Kruskal-Wallis tests of more than two samples were followed by two-sided Mann-Whitney U test for individual pairwise comparisons, and Benjamini/Hochberg (non-negative) correction65 for multiple comparisons. Benjamini/Hochberg (non-negative) correction was implemented in Statsmodels Python package66 and Kruskal-Wallis, Mann-Whitney U, Linear regression, Pearson coefficients, Poisson and Gamma distributions were computed using Python statistics package Scipy.63
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Tim Behrens, Dan Bush, Julie Lefort, and Romain Bourboulou for useful comments on the manuscript. S.T., W.d.C., and C.B. thank the Wellcome Trust for supporting this work through the Senior Research Fellowship awarded to C.B. (212281/Z/18/Z) and the Medical Research Council UK for the PhD studentship awarded to S.T. This research was funded in whole, or in part, by the Wellcome Trust (212281/Z/18/Z).
Author contributions
S.T. and C.B. conceptualized the experiment. S.T. collected and spike-sorted the data. S.T. and C.B. analyzed the electrophysiology and behavioral data. W.d.C. created the virtual replica, performed the simulations, and related analysis. S.T., W.d.C., and C.B. wrote the manuscript.
Declaration of interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Published: July 13, 2022
Footnotes
Supplemental information can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2022.06.046.
Supplemental information
Data and code availability
-
•
All data have been deposited at figshare (UCL Research Data Repository) and are publicly available as of the date of publication. DOIs are listed in the key resources table.
-
•
All original code has been deposited at Zenodo and is publicly available as of the date of publication. DOIs are listed in the key resources table.
-
•
Any additional information required to reanalyze the data reported in this paper is available from the lead contact upon request.
References
- 1.O’Keefe J., Dostrovsky J. The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Res. 1971;34:171–175. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(71)90358-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Muller R.U., Bostock E., Taube J.S., Kubie J.L. On the directional firing properties of hippocampal place cells. J. Neurosci. 1994;14:7235–7251. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-12-07235.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Skaggs W.E., McNaughton B.L. Spatial firing properties of hippocampal CA1 populations in an environment containing two visually identical regions. J. Neurosci. 1998;18:8455–8466. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-20-08455.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.O’Keefe J., Nadel L. Clarendon Press; 1978. The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Ekstrom A.D., Kahana M.J., Caplan J.B., Fields T.A., Isham E.A., Newman E.L., Fried I. Cellular networks underlying human spatial navigation. Nature. 2003;425:184–188. doi: 10.1038/nature01964. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Yartsev M.M., Ulanovsky N. Representation of three-dimensional space in the hippocampus of flying bats. Science. 2013;340:367–372. doi: 10.1126/science.1235338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Grosmark A.D., Buzsáki G. Diversity in neural firing dynamics supports both rigid and learned hippocampal sequences. Science. 2016;351:1440–1443. doi: 10.1126/science.aad1935. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Kraus B.J., Robinson R.J., White J.A., Eichenbaum H., Hasselmo M.E. Hippocampal “time cells”: time versus path integration. Neuron. 2013;78:1090–1101. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.04.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Ólafsdóttir H.F., Carpenter F., Barry C. Task demands predict a dynamic switch in the content of awake hippocampal replay. Neuron. 2017;96:925–935.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.09.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Wilson M.A., McNaughton B.L. Dynamics of the hippocampal ensemble code for space. Science. 1993;261:1055–1058. doi: 10.1126/science.8351520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Fenton A.A., Kao H.-Y., Neymotin S.A., Olypher A., Vayntrub Y., Lytton W.W., Ludvig N. Unmasking the CA1 ensemble place code by exposures to small and large environments: more place cells and multiple, irregularly arranged, and expanded place fields in the larger space. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:11250–11262. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2862-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Lee J.S., Briguglio J.J., Cohen J.D., Romani S., Lee A.K. The statistical structure of the hippocampal code for space as a function of time, context, and value. Cell. 2020;183:620–635.e22. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2020.09.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Rich P.D., Liaw H.-P., Lee A.K. Large environments reveal the statistical structure governing hippocampal representations. Science. 2014;345:814–817. doi: 10.1126/science.1255635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Kjelstrup K.B., Solstad T., Brun V.H., Hafting T., Leutgeb S., Witter M.P., Moser E.I., Moser M.-B. Finite scale of spatial representation in the hippocampus. Science. 2008;321:140–143. doi: 10.1126/science.1157086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Marr D. W.H. Freeman and Company; 1982. Vision: A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Bourboulou R., Marti G., Michon F.-X., El Feghaly E., Nouguier M., Robbe D., Koenig J., Epsztein J. Dynamic control of hippocampal spatial coding resolution by local visual cues. eLife. 2019;8 doi: 10.7554/eLife.44487. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Hartley T., Burgess N., Lever C., Cacucci F., O’Keefe J. Modeling place fields in terms of the cortical inputs to the hippocampus. Hippocampus. 2000;10:369–379. doi: 10.1002/1098-1063(2000)10:4<369::AID-HIPO3>3.0.CO;2-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.O’Keefe J., Burgess N. Geometric determinants of the place fields of hippocampal neurons. Nature. 1996;381:425–428. doi: 10.1038/381425a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Barry C., Lever C., Hayman R., Hartley T., Burton S., O’Keefe J., Jeffery K., Burgess N. The boundary vector cell model of place cell firing and spatial memory. Rev. Neurosci. 2006;17:71–97. doi: 10.1515/revneuro.2006.17.1-2.71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Káli S., Dayan P. The involvement of recurrent connections in area CA3 in establishing the properties of place fields: a model. J. Neurosci. 2000;20:7463–7477. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-19-07463.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Samsonovich A., McNaughton B.L. Path integration and cognitive mapping in a continuous attractor neural network model. J. Neurosci. 1997;17:5900–5920. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-15-05900.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.de Cothi W., Barry C. Neurobiological successor features for spatial navigation. Hippocampus. 2020;30:1347–1355. doi: 10.1002/hipo.23246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Stachenfeld K.L., Botvinick M.M., Gershman S.J. The hippocampus as a predictive map. Nat. Neurosci. 2017;20:1643–1653. doi: 10.1038/nn.4650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Uria B., Ibarz B., Banino A., Zambaldi V., Kumaran D., Hassabis D., Barry C., Blundell C. The spatial memory pipeline: a model of egocentric to allocentric understanding in mammalian brains. bioRxiv. 2020 doi: 10.1101/2020.11.11.378141. Preprint at. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Zhang K., Sejnowski T.J. Neuronal tuning: to sharpen or broaden? Neural Comput. 1999;11:75–84. doi: 10.1162/089976699300016809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Brown W.M., Bäcker A. Optimal neuronal tuning for finite stimulus spaces. Neural Comput. 2006;18:1511–1526. doi: 10.1162/neco.2006.18.7.1511. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Maurer A.P., VanRhoads S.R., Sutherland G.R., Lipa P., McNaughton B.L. Self-motion and the origin of differential spatial scaling along the septo-temporal axis of the hippocampus. Hippocampus. 2005;15:841–852. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Keinath A.T., Epstein R.A., Balasubramanian V. Environmental deformations dynamically shift the grid cell spatial metric. eLife. 2018;7 doi: 10.7554/eLife.38169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Fechner G.T. Breitkopf u. Härtel; 1860. Elemente der Psychophysik. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Hetherington P.A., Shapiro M.L. Hippocampal place fields are altered by the removal of single visual cues in a distance-dependent manner. Behav. Neurosci. 1997;111:20–34. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.111.1.20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Spiers H.J., Hayman R.M.A., Jovalekic A., Marozzi E., Jeffery K.J. Place field repetition and purely local remapping in a multicompartment environment. Cereb. Cortex. 2015;25:10–25. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Chen G., Lu Y., King J.A., Cacucci F., Burgess N. Differential influences of environment and self-motion on place and grid cell firing. Nat. Commun. 2019;10:630. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-08550-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Purandare C.S., Dhingra S., Rios R., Vuong C., To T., Hachisuka A., Choudhary K., Mehta M.R. Moving bar of light evokes vectorial spatial selectivity in the immobile rat hippocampus. Nature. 2022;602:461–467. doi: 10.1038/s41586-022-04404-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Chen G., King J.A., Burgess N., O’Keefe J. How vision and movement combine in the hippocampal place code. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2013;110:378–383. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1215834110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Jeffery K.J., Donnett J.G., Burgess N., O’Keefe J.M. Directional control of hippocampal place fields. Exp. Brain Res. 1997;117:131–142. doi: 10.1007/s002210050206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Anderson M.I., Jeffery K.J. Heterogeneous modulation of place cell firing by changes in context. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:8827–8835. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-26-08827.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Quirk G.J., Muller R.U., Kubie J.L. The firing of hippocampal place cells in the dark depends on the rat’s recent experience. J. Neurosci. 1990;10:2008–2017. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-06-02008.1990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Save E., Cressant A., Thinus-Blanc C., Poucet B. Spatial firing of hippocampal place cells in blind rats. J. Neurosci. 1998;18:1818–1826. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-05-01818.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Wang C.H., Monaco J.D., Knierim J.J. Hippocampal place cells encode local surface-texture boundaries. Curr. Biol. 2020;30:1397–1409.e7. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.01.083. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Burgess N., Donnett J.G., Jeffery K.J., O’Keefe J. Robotic and neuronal simulation of the hippocampus and rat navigation. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 1997;352:1535–1543. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1997.0140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Hedrick K.R., Zhang K. Megamap: flexible representation of a large space embedded with nonspatial information by a hippocampal attractor network. J. Neurophysiol. 2016;116:868–891. doi: 10.1152/jn.00856.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Laughlin S.B., de Ruyter van Steveninck R.R., Anderson J.C. The metabolic cost of neural information. Nat. Neurosci. 1998;1:36–41. doi: 10.1038/236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Barry C., Burgess N. Learning in a geometric model of place cell firing. Hippocampus. 2007;17:786–800. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Hasselmo M.E., Schnell E., Barkai E. Dynamics of learning and recall at excitatory recurrent synapses and cholinergic modulation in rat hippocampal region CA3. J. Neurosci. 1995;15:5249–5262. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-07-05249.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Eliav T., Maimon S.R., Aljadeff J., Tsodyks M., Ginosar G., Las L., Ulanovsky N. Multiscale representation of very large environments in the hippocampus of flying bats. Science. 2021;372 doi: 10.1126/science.abg4020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Harland B., Contreras M., Souder M., Fellous J.-M. Dorsal CA1 hippocampal place cells form a multi-scale representation of megaspace. Curr. Biol. 2021;31:2178–2190.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2021.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Alme C.B., Miao C., Jezek K., Treves A., Moser E.I., Moser M.-B. Place cells in the hippocampus: eleven maps for eleven rooms. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2014;111:18428–18435. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1421056111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Park E., Dvorak D., Fenton A.A. Ensemble place codes in hippocampus: CA1, CA3, and dentate gyrus place cells have multiple place fields in large environments. PLoS ONE. 2011;6 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0022349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Wei X.-X., Stocker A.A. Lawful relation between perceptual bias and discriminability. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2017;114:10244–10249. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1619153114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Olypher A.V., Lánský P., Muller R.U., Fenton A.A. Quantifying location-specific information in the discharge of rat hippocampal place cells. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2003;127:123–135. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0270(03)00123-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Hägglund M., Mørreaunet M., Moser M.-B., Moser E.I. Grid-cell distortion along geometric borders. Curr. Biol. 2019;29:1047–1054.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.01.074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Krupic J., Bauza M., Burton S., Barry C., O’Keefe J. Grid cell symmetry is shaped by environmental geometry. Nature. 2015;518:232–235. doi: 10.1038/nature14153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Stensola T., Stensola H., Moser M.-B., Moser E.I. Shearing-induced asymmetry in entorhinal grid cells. Nature. 2015;518:207–212. doi: 10.1038/nature14151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Barry C., Hayman R., Burgess N., Jeffery K.J. Experience-dependent rescaling of entorhinal grids. Nat. Neurosci. 2007;10:682–684. doi: 10.1038/nn1905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Siegle J.H., López A.C., Patel Y.A., Abramov K., Ohayon S., Voigts J. Open Ephys: an open-source, plugin-based platform for multichannel electrophysiology. J. Neural Eng. 2017;14 doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/aa5eea. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Tanni S., Barry C. Zenodo; 2021. SpatialAutoDACQ—Automated System for Scalable Data Acquisition in Spatial Tasks. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Kadir S.N., Goodman D.F.M., Harris K.D. High-dimensional cluster analysis with the masked EM algorithm. Neural Comput. 2014;26:2379–2394. doi: 10.1162/NECO_a_00661. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Schmitzer-Torbert N., Redish A.D. Neuronal activity in the rodent dorsal striatum in sequential navigation: separation of spatial and reward responses on the multiple T task. J. Neurophysiol. 2004;91:2259–2272. doi: 10.1152/jn.00687.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Harris K.D., Hirase H., Leinekugel X., Henze D.A., Buzsáki G. Temporal interaction between single spikes and complex spike bursts in hippocampal pyramidal cells. Neuron. 2001;32:141–149. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00447-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Mathis A., Herz A.V.M., Stemmler M. Optimal population codes for space: grid cells outperform place cells. Neural Comput. 2012;24:2280–2317. doi: 10.1162/NECO_a_00319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Towse B.W., Barry C., Bush D., Burgess N. Optimal configurations of spatial scale for grid cell firing under noise and uncertainty. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2014;369 doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Zhang K., Ginzburg I., McNaughton B.L., Sejnowski T.J. Interpreting neuronal population activity by reconstruction: unified framework with application to hippocampal place cells. J. Neurophysiol. 1998;79:1017–1044. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.2.1017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Virtanen P., Gommers R., Oliphant T.E., Haberland M., Reddy T., Cournapeau D., Burovski E., Peterson P., Weckesser W., Bright J., et al. SciPy 1.0: fundamental algorithms for scientific computing in Python. Nat. Methods. 2020;17:261–272. doi: 10.1038/s41592-019-0686-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Hardcastle K., Ganguli S., Giocomo L.M. Environmental boundaries as an error correction mechanism for grid cells. Neuron. 2015;86:827–839. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Benjamini Y., Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. B. 1995;57:289–300. [Google Scholar]
- 66.Seabold S., Perktold J. 9th Python in Science Conference. 2010. Statsmodels: econometric and statistical modeling with Python. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
-
•
All data have been deposited at figshare (UCL Research Data Repository) and are publicly available as of the date of publication. DOIs are listed in the key resources table.
-
•
All original code has been deposited at Zenodo and is publicly available as of the date of publication. DOIs are listed in the key resources table.
-
•
Any additional information required to reanalyze the data reported in this paper is available from the lead contact upon request.