Abstract
Background and aims
Previous research into word learning in children with developmental language disorder (DLD) indicates that the learning of word forms and meanings, rather than form-referent links, is problematic. This difficulty appears to arise with impaired encoding, while retention of word knowledge remains intact. Evidence also suggests that word learning skills may be related to verbal working memory. We aimed to substantiate these findings in the current study by exploring word learning over a series of days.
Methods
Fifty children with DLD (mean age 6; 11, 72% male) and 54 age-matched typically developing (TD) children (mean age 6; 10, 56% male) were taught eight novel words across a four-day word learning protocol. Day 1 measured encoding, Days 2 and 3 measured re-encoding, and Day 4 assessed retention. At each day, word learning success was evaluated using Naming, Recognition, Description, and Identification tasks.
Results
Children with DLD showed comparable performance to the TD group on the Identification task, indicating an intact ability to learn the form-referent links. In contrast, children with DLD performed significantly worse for Naming and Recognition (signifying an impaired ability to learn novel word forms), and for Description, indicating problems establishing new word meanings. These deficits for the DLD group were apparent at Days 1, 2, and 3 of testing, indicating impairments with initial encoding and re-encoding; however, the DLD and TD groups demonstrated a similar rate of learning. All children found the retention assessments at Day 4 difficult, and there were no significant group differences. Finally, verbal working memory emerged as a significant moderator of performance on the Naming and Recognition tasks, such that children with DLD and poor verbal working memory had the lowest levels of accuracy.
Conclusions
This study demonstrates that children with DLD struggle with learning novel word forms and meanings, but are unimpaired in their ability to establish new form-referent links. The findings suggest that the word learning deficit may be attributed to problems with encoding, rather than with retention, of new word knowledge; however, further exploration is required given the poor performance of both groups for retention testing. Furthermore, we found evidence that an impaired ability to learn word forms may only be apparent in children who have DLD and low levels of verbal working memory.
Implications
When working with children with DLD, speech-language pathologists should assess word learning using tasks that evaluate the ability to learn word forms, meanings, and form-referent links to develop a profile of individual word learning strengths and weaknesses. Clinicians should also assess verbal working memory to identify children at particular risk of word learning deficits. Future research should explore the notion of optimal intervention intensity for facilitating word learning in children with poor language and verbal working memory.
Keywords: Word learning, vocabulary, verbal working memory, developmental language disorder
Introduction
Developmental language disorder (DLD) is a neurodevelopmental condition that affects approximately 1 in 14 children (Norbury et al., 2016; Tomblin et al., 1997). DLD is characterised by persistent language problems that cannot be attributed to a biomedical condition, such as autism spectrum disorder or intellectual disability; however, DLD may co-occur with conditions such as Attention Deficit-Hyperactive Disorder (ADHD; Bishop et al., 2017). DLD is heterogeneous, which results in varied profiles of impairment in oral language and cognitive skills (Bishop, 2006; Pennington, 2006). The study reported here focuses on word learning skills, which are often impaired in children with DLD and are shown to have a persistent and detrimental impact on academic, social-emotional, and vocational development (Kan & Windsor, 2010; Law et al., 2009; Spencer et al., 2017).
A problem with encoding or retention?
Word learning, or the ability to learn and establish new lexical items in vocabulary, is a critical component of language development (Beck et al., 2013). There are robust links between vocabulary and academic development, including literacy (Castles et al., 2018) and mathematics (Spencer et al., 2017), as well as social-emotional outcomes in adolescence and adulthood (Armstrong et al., 2017). Learning a novel word relies on the development of the word form (phonological representation), meaning (semantic representation), and the creation of an association between the two (form-referent link; Chiat, 2001). Deficits in the ability to establish these foundational representations during word learning can have an adverse impact on the ability to recognise, and therefore further refine, word knowledge in future instances (Gray et al., 2020). This can have a detrimental effect on the ability to establish syntactic, orthographic, and articulatory representations, all of which are required for effective use of words in spoken and written contexts (Castles et al., 2018).
The course of developing new word knowledge has been conceptualised as a process that centrally involves the establishment of new information in memory. Under this framework, word learning is described as involving encoding, re-encoding, and retention (McGregor et al., 2020; Storkel et al., 2019). Encoding involves several processes, including sensory perception of the novel word, recognition of the stimuli as novel, and the direction of attention to relevant environmental detail (Kane et al., 2001). Subsequently, an initial memory trace of the new form and referent is encoded (Suzuki, 2006). Following the initial encoding process, the memory trace may be forgotten (i.e., if insufficient information was stored) or retained in long-term memory (Wilhelm et al., 2012). Retention of new words relies on consolidation processes, whereby the encoded memory trace transfers to long-term memory without external input. Consolidation is driven by the passing of time, and is facilitated by sleep (Stickgold, 2005). Through consolidation, the retained memory of the novel word can become more stable, and integrates with existing information in vocabulary (Davis & Gaskell, 2009; McClelland et al., 1995). Retention of the word may be further strengthened (i.e., stored in more detail over a prolonged period of time) through re-encoding, whereby the word is encountered again and retrieved from long-term memory, making it susceptible to change (Desmottes et al., 2016). Through re-encoding, word knowledge can be refined in response to further input (McGregor et al., 2013a; Nader & Hardt, 2009).
It has been suggested that word learning deficits experienced by people with DLD may be attributed to impaired encoding (McGregor et al., 2013a). This likely results in problems with the retained word knowledge in long-term memory; however, the processes involved in retention itself may remain intact (i.e., in declarative memory; Bishop & Hsu, 2015). Evidence for this ‘encoding deficit hypothesis’ has been demonstrated in a series of studies with adolescents and adults with DLD (McGregor et al., 2013a, 2017a, 2017b). Across these studies, college students with DLD and their typically developing (TD) peers were trained on novel word forms and their associated referents. All students received an equal number of training exposures, and learning was tested via recall tasks immediately after training and one week later. McGregor et al. (2013a) found evidence of poor encoding of word forms and meanings for the DLD group, as indicated by poor performance on the immediate post-training assessment tasks. Retention of the word meanings at the one-week interval appeared intact; however, the DLD-TD gap widened over this time for word forms, indicating a potential problem with retaining phonological information. In their subsequent studies, McGregor et al. (2017a, 2017b, 2020) controlled for potential confounds on retention and demonstrated that word learning in the DLD group was characterised by poor encoding, yet intact retention. As a result, McGregor et al. (2020) concluded that “encoding of word forms is the primary bottleneck to word learning among people with DLD” (p. 14).
There is also evidence of an encoding deficit in children with DLD. Bishop and Hsu (2015) found that eight-year-olds with DLD were significantly worse than their TD peers at learning the names of novel animals. This deficit was observed immediately after training, yet the rate of learning over a two-week period was similar, supporting the notion of intact retention but impaired encoding. In Haebig et al. (2019) and Leonard et al. (2019), five-year-old children with and without DLD learned a set of new words through retrieval practice, whereby the new word and a definition were introduced, and learning was reinforced through repeated opportunities to retrieve the name and definition from memory. In comparison to the TD children, those with DLD demonstrated poor accuracy when naming the newly-learned words after a five-minute interval; however, their rate of learning after a one-week interval was similar to the TD group (Haebig et al., 2019; Leonard et al., 2019). While these findings point to an encoding deficit in word learning, this hypothesis requires further substantiation, as some research has failed to show a deficit for children with DLD across both encoding and retention.
For instance, Gray and colleagues measured encoding and retention of novel words that had either high or low phonotactic probability and neighbourhood density (Gray et al., 2012; Gray & Brinkley, 2011). The results of both studies showed no significant difference between the TD and DLD groups for either encoding or retention of word forms. It is possible that methodological differences contribute to the mixed findings from Gray and colleagues, and those from Haebig et al. (2019) and Leonard et al. (2019). In particular, in Gray and colleagues’ studies, participants engaged in four consecutive days of word learning through a supported learning context, followed by a fifth day of retention testing. In contrast, in Leonard and Haebig’s studies, the children were provided with novel word training over two consecutive days (during which time encoding was tested), followed by a post-test retention task one week later. It is possible that the more frequent learning sessions provided by Gray and colleagues allowed the children with DLD to effectively establish and refine their knowledge of the novel words (Gray et al., 2012; Gray & Brinkley, 2011). Furthermore, their administration of the post-tests at one day, instead of one week, after learning may have meant that novel word forms and meanings were subject to less decay and/or interference between each of the new memory traces (Haebig et al., 2019; Leonard et al., 2019; Mainela-Arnold et al., 2010).
Theoretically, encoding deficits in children with DLD may be underpinned by deficits within the working memory system. According to Baddeley (2003), working memory is a capacity-limited system of interacting components responsible for temporary storage and processing of verbal and visual information. This system is responsible for facilitating the transfer of new information into long-term memory, and as such is linked strongly to vocabulary development (Gathercole & Baddeley, 1989). Specifically, the encoding of novel words is thought to be supported by the components of working memory that are concerned with processing verbal information (for simplicity, we refer to this as ‘verbal working memory’; Archibald & Gathercole, 2006). Verbal working memory (WM) is likely to support encoding by directing and maintaining attention to the novel phonological stimulus and refreshing the echoic memory in the phonological loop, while processing other sensory input (such as contextual information about the word, e.g., physical features; Kane et al., 2001). Deficits with verbal WM are therefore proposed to contribute to problems with encoding, which would be expected to have a subsequent impact on the ability to store an accurate memory trace in long-term memory. However, it is likely that the long-term memory system itself (i.e., declarative memory) may be unimpaired (Bishop & Hsu, 2015; Lum et al., 2015). While verbal WM is thought to be primarily occupied with the initial stage of word learning (encoding), it may also be implicated with later retrieval and monitoring of word knowledge from long-term memory; however, the link between these processes and verbal WM has not been empirically tested (Cabeza et al., 2002; Lum et al., 2015).
A problem with word forms, meanings, or form-referent links?
Understanding the nature of the word learning deficit in children with DLD is complicated by the fact that word learning requires the development of various aspects of knowledge. We focus our investigation on the processes involved in learning nouns as they are typically concrete and non-relational, and are usually imageable (Skipp et al., 2002). At a minimum, learning nouns includes establishing the word form, word meaning (such as details about the physical features of the item), and a link between the two (i.e., the form-referent link; Gray et al., 2020). Difficulties with learning the word form have been well-evidenced in children with DLD (Kan & Windsor, 2010). The methods for evaluating word form learning are varied, and differ in their degree of linguistic demand. Frequently, knowledge of the word form is assessed using a naming task, which involves retrieving the word form for spoken production in response to seeing the item (Jackson et al., 2019a). Recognition tasks are also commonly used, requiring identification of the target from similar-sounding foils (Alt & Suddarth, 2012). Using such measures, studies have consistently shown that children with DLD have significant deficits learning novel word forms in comparison to control groups (Kan & Windsor, 2010), and there is strong theoretical support for the notion that verbal WM deficits underlie this impairment (Archibald, 2016; Baddeley, 2003). However, only a handful of studies have directly explored this relationship (Montgomery et al., 2010).
For instance, Alt and Plante (2006), Gray (2006), and Jackson et al. (2016) showed that nonword repetition performance significantly predicted naming accuracy at encoding. Additionally, Gray (2004) highlighted a significant link between nonword repetition performance and naming accuracy across several days of word learning. This suggests that verbal WM performance may also predict retention of novel word forms, and may be important in the process of retrieving word forms from long-term memory, and holding them active, in order to complete a naming task in the days following initial learning (Baddeley, 2003; Lum et al., 2015). However, further investigation is required to understand whether impairments in encoding, re-encoding, and retention may be ascribed to deficits within the working memory system. In contrast, Gray (2006) and Hansson et al. (2004) failed to find a link between verbal WM (measured using nonword repetition) and performance on a naming task. In these two studies, naming was measured following a single session of learning, and as such was largely a measure of encoding (McGregor et al., 2013a). The inconsistency in findings may reflect the fact that nonword repetition performance varies among children; some children with DLD show unimpaired nonword repetition skills, and some TD children are impaired at nonword repetition (Archibald & Joanisse, 2009).
Evidence suggests that children with DLD are also impaired at learning novel word meanings, as demonstrated by poor performance on tasks that are expressive (e.g., describing the appearance of the item; Alt & Plante, 2006) and receptive in nature (e.g., answering ‘Yes/No’ questions about elements of meaning; Nash & Donaldson, 2005). These findings provide support for the notion that the word learning deficit in children with DLD is multifactorial, affecting the ability to develop new word forms and meanings (Nation, 2014). It is possible that verbal WM deficits contribute to problems with learning word meanings in children with DLD (Alt & Plante, 2006); however, previous research into this relationship is limited and the findings equivocal. For instance, Alt and Plante (2006) found a significant relationship, whereas Storkel et al. (2019) did not.
Finally, there is mixed evidence regarding the ability of children with DLD to learn form-referent links for novel words (McGregor et al., 2020). This aspect of word learning is usually evaluated using an identification task, which involves hearing the novel word and selecting the target from an array of items (Jackson et al., 2019a). While Gray (2004, 2005) and Rice et al. (1990, 1992, 1994) found that children with DLD performed poorly on this task, Gray (2006) and Rice et al. (2000) found comparable performance with control groups. Gray et al. (2020) highlighted that children may be able to perform accurately on identification tasks even if they have encoded and retained weaker phonological and semantic representations, because only partial knowledge is required to create a link between the word form and its appearance. The findings of previous research indicate that verbal WM is not associated with the ability to learn form-referent links (e.g., see Gray, 2004), which may reflect the assumption that learning form-referent links places minimal demands on the working memory system (Ullman et al., 2020). However, this relationship has been the subject of little previous research, warranting further exploration.
The current study
Overall, the literature suggests that the initial encoding stage of the word learning process is impaired in children with DLD, with retention remaining intact. However, this pattern of impairment requires further substantiation. Further evidence is also required to determine whether learning the word forms, word meanings, and/or form-referent links is problematic. Word learning deficits – especially those impacting the learning of the word form – are likely to be driven by deficits in verbal WM; however, further investigation into the nature of this relationship is required. Thus, the current study had two primary aims:
- Using a four-day protocol, we aimed to identify whether a word learning deficit in children with DLD could be attributed to problems with initial encoding (Day 1), re-encoding (Days 2 and 3), or retention (Day 4). To determine whether learning the word forms, form-referent links, and/or word meanings is problematic for children with DLD, word learning was evaluated using four different outcome measures across the four-day protocol:
- Knowledge of word forms was tested using Naming (an expressive task) and Recognition (a receptive task);
- Knowledge of word meanings was evaluated using Description, and;
- Knowledge of the form-referent links was evaluated using Identification.
Based on previous literature, we predicted that the children with DLD would exhibit deficits at Naming, Recognition, and Description, indicating an impaired ability to establish novel word forms and meanings. In contrast, we predicted that these children would perform similarly to the TD group for Identification, indicating an intact ability to establish form-referent links. Furthermore, we hypothesised that deficits on the Naming, Recognition, and Description tasks would be apparent for the DLD group at Day 1, indicating deficits with initial encoding. Poor performance across Days 2 and 3 was also expected, which would reflect subsequent deficits in re-encoding as a result of poor initial encoding. We expected that the gap between the DLD and TD groups would not further widen at Day 4, which would indicate intact retention for the children with DLD (Bishop & Hsu, 2015; McGregor et al., 2020).
2. Verbal WM impairments may be a key factor contributing to the word learning deficits of children with DLD (Archibald, 2016; Baddeley et al., 1998). There is some evidence for this link with regards to the initial encoding of novel word forms, but the link between verbal WM to word meanings and form-referent links is unclear. Furthermore, to our knowledge very few studies have explored the influence of verbal WM across the stages of encoding, re-encoding, and retention (Gray, 2004; Montgomery et al., 2010). We predicted that verbal WM ability would moderate performance across the four days of word learning for the Naming and Recognition tasks, such that poor performance would only be observed in children who have impaired verbal WM; however, this was not expected for the Description and Identification tasks.
Method
Participants
The participants were involved in a broad programme of research that involved testing memory skills and word learning in TD children and those with DLD. The data regarding word learning are pertinent for this study and have not been previously reported. The data regarding the memory skills of this cohort are comprehensively reported in Jackson et al. (2020), and data regarding verbal WM skills are reported again in the current paper.
To qualify for this research programme, all children met the general criteria of speaking English as a primary language, with no significant history of hearing, articulation, or behavioural problems. No participant had a primary developmental condition that might account for their language disorder, such as Down syndrome or intellectual disability (Bishop et al., 2017). There were three children with DLD and one typically developing child who had a diagnosis of ADHD. All participants passed a hearing screen and the Diagnostic Evaluation of Articulation and Phonology (DEAP) Diagnostic Screen (Dodd et al., 2002). Children with DLD were recruited from two specialist language schools, and TD children attended three mainstream primary schools.
Children recruited from the specialist language schools had all been clinically diagnosed with DLD 12 to 18 months prior to recruitment, and so a full battery of oral language assessments was not required. Where the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals – 4th Edition, Australian and New Zealand standardisation (CELF-4AUZN) had been administered in the past 12 months, those scores were obtained with parental permission (Semel et al., 2006). Otherwise, general oral language skills were reassessed to confirm eligibility for the DLD group, using the Core Language Score subtests of the CELF-4AUZN. All participants with DLD obtained a Core Language Score of 85 or less (≤1SD below the mean). The children recruited from the mainstream schools for the TD group were also assessed using the Core Language subtests and achieved a standard score of 86 or higher (i.e., within or above the average range). Summary statistics describing demographic details and scores on the participant selection assessments are presented in Table 1.
Table 1.
Domain | DLD (n = 50) |
TD (n = 54) |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M (SD) | Range | M (SD) | Range | p | d | |
Age in months | 83.54 (7.59) | 71–104 | 82.04 (7.56) | 70–98 | .081 | 0.20 |
CLSa | 64.16 (11.47) | 40–85 | 101.26 (11.79) | 86–134 | <.001** | 3.19 |
Nonverbal IQa | 87.40 (15.20) | 70–141 | 102.93 (18.96) | 76–140 | <.001** | 1.08 |
Nonword Repetitionb | 72.45 (10.54) | 48.96–92.71 | 89.83 (5.05) | 80.21–99.31 | <.001** | 2.10 |
Digit Recalla | 84.26 (17.35) | 56–121 | 102.63 (14.90) | 73–137 | .018* | 1.14 |
Backwards Digit Recalla | 73.84 (13.04) | 56–104 | 95.61 (15.91) | 67–141 | .011* | 1.50 |
Verbal WM factor | -0.74 (0.81) | -2.42–0.76 | 0.69 (0.57) | -0.71–2.21 | <.001** | 2.04 |
CLS: Core Language Score on the CELF-4; Nonverbal IQ: standard score on the PTONI; Verbal WM factor: verbal working memory factor, calculated using principal component analysis of scores on the Nonword Repetition, Digit Recall, and Backwards Digit Recall tasks.
aScores are standardised to a mean of 100 and standard deviation of 15.
bScore is reported as percentage of phonemes correct (PPC).
*p < .05. **p < .01.
Additionally, all participants were assessed using the Primary Test of Nonverbal Intelligence (PTONI; Ehrler & McGhee, 2008) and achieved a standard score of above 70, ruling out the presence of an intellectual disability (Bishop et al., 2017). Participants were not excluded on the basis of low-range nonverbal intelligence (IQ) scores, in line with the CATALISE guidelines for the classification of DLD. Differences between groups for these variables were evaluated using independent samples t tests. As expected, the DLD group had significantly lower scores than the TD group on the measure of general oral language (Core Language Score). The DLD group was also significantly lower on the PTONI. In their meta-analysis, Kan and Windsor (2010) reported nonverbal IQ as a significant moderator of group differences in word learning performance. As such, we controlled for nonverbal IQ in our statistical analyses to ensure that any observed group differences on word learning were not the result of differences in nonverbal IQ.
Protocol
Data collection
Recruitment and testing followed procedures approved by the Jackson et al. (2020) ethics committees. All tests were completed individually in a quiet school room during normal school hours. As previously stated, participants in the current study completed a range of tests as part of a larger research protocol (details reported in Jackson et al., 2020). The tasks required for data collection reported here were administered across seven individual testing sessions distributed over two consecutive weeks. The first week involved administering the participant selection tasks and measures of verbal working memory in four 30-minute sessions, and the second week involved administration of the word learning task across four consecutive days (20-30 minute sessions).
Materials
The word learning stimuli were eight novel words (pseudowords) derived from English syllables. A large portion of English words are multisyllabic (Balota et al., 2007), and word learning success can differ depending on word length (Jackson et al., 2019b). Thus, to capture variation in performance the stimuli included two items at one, two, three, and four-syllable lengths (see Appendix 1). An additional two-syllable nonword was included as a practice item. Pseudowords were chosen to ensure there was no prior knowledge of the stimuli, and all had low phonotactic probability to reduce the possible bias of prior lexical knowledge on learning novel word forms (Gray et al., 2012). The stimuli did not include phonemes that the younger participants may not have developed by their chronological age, such as /x/,/θ/,/ð/,/v/, and /r/. There were no consonant clusters, to minimise articulatory complexity (Bowen, 2014). Each pseudoword was randomly assigned to a concrete referent (unfamiliar ‘alien’ creatures) made from coloured modelling clay (see Supplemental Materials). Thus, the stimuli were taught as proper nouns linked to the referent, which suited our aim of testing whether the word form, meaning, or form-referent link was problematic for children with DLD (McGregor et al., 2020). The referents were designed to be maximally different with regards to physical attributes: each item differed from the others in at least three physical features (body shape, body colour, leg colour, and number of eyes). Photos of each were integrated into an animated video using the Moovly online program (Moovly.com). A separate animation was created for each item and for each day of the protocol. The animations were narrated by a female Australian English speaker and administered individually via iPad. The procedures for training and administration of outcome measures are described below and detailed in the Supplemental Materials.
Word learning protocol
Encoding and re-encoding (days 1–3)
Day 1 (encoding) and Days 2 and 3 (re-encoding) all followed a similar procedure: the eight stimuli were presented in a training block, which was followed by the administration of four outcome measures (Naming, Recognition, Description, and Identification). The stimuli were presented in randomised order on each day for each participant. In the training block, the stimuli were presented one at a time. For each item, the word form was modelled four times in commenting phrases interleaved with semantic description about the body shape and colour of the legs. To ensure the participant was actively engaged, they were asked to imitate the word form, and regardless of their response, the examiner repeated the name to provide an additional exposure. Throughout the task, participants were not stopped from making extraneous comments, but were always redirected back to the task. At the start of Day 1, a practice item was presented to familiarise participants with the training procedure and outcome measures. A detailed description of the protocol is provided in Supplemental Materials.
Outcome measures
On Days 1, 2, and 3, the training task was followed by the administration of the four outcome measures. Each outcome measure inherently involves additional learning opportunities (i.e., through additional exposure and/or retrieval of the word); thus, the measures were administered in fixed order to ensure consistent learning opportunities prior to testing. The tasks were scored on-line within each session (using hard copy score forms) and were also voice recorded (using a Philips Audio Recorder) for later checking. Twenty percent of the tasks were later scored by an independent second scorer (an experienced speech-language pathologist) blind to groups. There was high inter-rater reliability for scoring of each outcome measure (Naming r = .91, Recognition r = .98, Identification r = .99, and Description r = .95).
For the practice item at the start of Day 1, specific feedback was provided to the child regarding the accuracy of their response on each of the four outcome measures (see script in Supplemental Materials). For the test items, only neutral feedback was provided (e.g., “You’re working well”), with no feedback on accuracy.
Naming
Participants were asked to say the name of each item to test their ability to produce the word form. If no response was given, participants were prompted to “try and say any of the sounds in the name”. Responses were phonetically transcribed, and accuracy was evaluated using the percentage of phonemes correct (PPC) method (Dollaghan & Campbell, 1998). To ensure that naming performance was not limited by restrictions in the child’s phonetic inventory, the child’s performance on the DEAP screener (Dodd et al., 2002) and observations of their spontaneous speech were considered to identify consistent errors in their speech. These errors were scored as correct in their Naming responses; any errors with producing phonemes that were in their phonetic inventories were marked as incorrect (Dollaghan & Campbell, 1998).
Recognition
A mispronunciation detection task involved identifying the correct word form from three phonologically-related foils. The foils differed from the target according to: 1) initial phoneme modification; 2) final phoneme modification; and, 3) syllable modification (i.e., transposition of syllables, or an added random syllable for one-syllable items). The target and foils were randomly ordered for each item and participants were not informed about the ratio of correct and incorrect choices. The participant was required to make a decision about each option as it was presented, and they pressed a green or red button if they judged the option to be correct or incorrect (Alt & Suddarth, 2012). No feedback was provided on each judgement made by the child. One point was allocated for each correct response, yielding a maximum score of 4 per item (and a total maximum score of 32), and a percentage of Recognition accuracy was calculated for use in the analyses.
Identification
Knowledge of the word-referent link was tested using a task in which participants saw all eight target items displayed on the screen and heard the target word in the phrase, “Point to ____”. Participants made their selection by pointing to the item on the screen. Responses were scored as correct (1 point) or incorrect (0 points), yielding a total maximum score of 8. A percentage of Identification accuracy was calculated.
Description
Knowledge of the word meaning was evaluated by asking the child to describe the item’s appearance (“What does ____ look like?”). During the practice task at the start of Day 1, participants were trained to provide four features in their descriptions. If an incomplete description was provided, the prompt “What else?” was given, until the description included four features or no further information could be provided. Responses were transcribed, and one point was allocated for each correct semantic feature, yielding a maximum of 4 for each item (maximum score of 32). A percentage of Description accuracy was calculated.
Retention (day 4)
Retention of the novel words was tested approximately 24 hours after the Day 3 session for each participant. The Naming, Recognition, and Identification tasks were administered, but not the Description task as retention testing of either the word form or meaning would require exposure to the target information, thus confounding assessment. Administration of the three outcome measures was counterbalanced for each item across participants. The same scoring procedures as above were used and no corrective feedback was provided.
Dosage
Previous studies have developed word learning protocols with the aim of training to mastery, or evaluating effective training intensities to facilitate comparable word learning among DLD and TD children (e.g., McGregor et al., 2020; Storkel et al., 2017). The purpose for the current study, however, was not intervention; we aimed to provide each participant with the same training opportunities in order to afford a controlled comparison of word learning abilities. On Days 1, 2, and 3 of the protocol, 8 exposures to the word form were provided (5 through training and 3 through outcome measure administration). Thus, prior to retention testing participants had received 24 verbal models for each item.
Verbal working memory
Three tasks were administered to evaluate verbal WM skills. Two subtests from the Working Memory Test Battery for Children (WMTB-C; Gathercole & Pickering, 2001) were administered: (1) Digit Recall, which requires hearing, temporarily storing, and recalling strings of digits; and, (2) Backwards Digit Recall, which requires hearing and temporarily storing strings of digits, and then recalling these in reverse order. Together, both tasks give a good indication of a child’s verbal WM capabilities (Archibald & Gathercole, 2006). These subtests are standardised to a mean of 100 and SD of 15. The Nonword Repetition Test (Dollaghan & Campbell, 1998) was also administered, which requires the oral repetition of nonwords that increase in length. Guidelines for pronunciation and scoring outlined by Dollaghan and Campbell (1998) were followed. As with scoring of the word learning Naming task, responses were scored using the PPC method while taking into account each child’s phonetic inventory. Responses on this task were audio recorded and 20% of the tasks were later scored by the independent second scorer, with high inter-rater reliability (r = .93).
Verbal WM for this cohort of children was reported as part of a large battery of memory measures in Jackson et al. (2020). The results showed that the DLD group scored significantly lower than the TD group on all three measures. Furthermore, scores were significantly correlated with each other (rs ranging from .52 to .69), indicating that these measures evaluated a similar construct. As such, principal components analysis was conducted to achieve data reduction and obtain a Verbal WM Factor (full details are provided in Jackson et al., 2020).
Results
Our first aim was to examine whether word learning impairments in children with DLD may be attributed to deficits in encoding or retention. As such, word learning was evaluated across a four-day protocol (Day 1: encoding, Days 2 and 3: re-encoding, Day 4: retention). Across the four days, we evaluated the word learning process using four outcome measures (Naming, Recognition, Identification, and Description) in order to identify whether children with DLD have deficits with learning the word form, meaning, or form-referent link. A series of four Generalised Linear Mixed Model (GLMM) analyses were run using IBM SPSS Statistics (Version 26; IBM Corp., 2019). For the Naming, Recognition, and Identification analyses, participant was included as a random effect (random intercept). Time (Day 1, Day 2, Day 3, and Day 4), Group (DLD, TD), and the interaction between Time and Group were included as fixed effects. As described in the Method, we noted significant group differences for nonverbal IQ (PTONI) abilities, so we adjusted for this. The fixed effects for the nonverbal IQ factor for each analysis are reported in Supplemental Materials. The Description analysis involved the same random and fixed effects; however, there were only three time points (Day 4 retention testing was not conducted for this outcome measure, as described in the Method).
The second study aim was to explore whether verbal WM abilities moderated performance on Naming, Recognition, Identification, and Description across the four-day protocol. To explore this aim, the same GLMM analyses as above were run, with the inclusion of the Verbal WM factor as an additional fixed effect, and the three-way interaction for Group × Verbal WM × Time.
There were low rates of missing data (<2% on all variables), and these data were missing completely at random, χ2(52) = 40.91, p = .866. Missing data were imputed using expectation maximisation. There were two univariate outliers but these were deemed genuine scores rather than errors in data entry (and were not more than 3SD away from their group mean). These data points were dropped and the analyses were re-run, but they did not have an impact on the results; therefore, the reported analyses used the full data set. Descriptive statistics for all analyses (disaggregated by group) are summarised in Table 2. Statistics for all follow-up contrasts are presented in Supplemental Materials.
Table 2.
Measure | Group | M a | M b | SE a | SE b | 95% CIa | 95% CIb |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Day 1 (encoding) | |||||||
Naming | DLDTD | 49.5975.05 | 60.8069.47 | 2.812.29 | 2.283.57 | 44.06, 55.1270.55, 79.54 | 56.32, 65.2862.45, 76.50 |
Recognition | DLDTD | 72.2788.41 | 77.8086.13 | 1.841.08 | 1.911.95 | 69.65, 76.9086.28, 90.54 | 74.05, 81.5682.29, 89.96 |
Identification | DLDTD | 94.2098.00 | 93.3197.40 | 1.980.70 | 2.271.05 | 90.31, 98.0996.62, 99.39 | 88.85, 97.9695.34, 99.46 |
Description | DLDTD | 69.9980.38 | 72.5379.91 | 2.441.70 | 2.762.41 | 65.19, 74.7977.03, 83.72 | 67.09, 77.9675.17, 84.64 |
Day 2 (re-encoding) | |||||||
Naming | DLDTD | 56.7584.72 | 66.0379.71 | 2.812.22 | 3.093.08 | 51.24, 62.2780.36, 89.08 | 59.96, 72.1073.66, 85.77 |
Recognition | DLDTD | 75.7893.79 | 81.3592.54 | 1.860.91 | 1.921.30 | 71.93, 79.2392.01, 95.57 | 77.57, 85.1389.98, 95.10 |
Identification | DLDTD | 95.7897.99 | 95.3197.06 | 1.100.82 | 1.221.46 | 93.62, 97.9496.16, 99.39 | 92.91, 97.7194.19, 99.93 |
Description | DLDTD | 70.3279.57 | 75.1281.25 | 2.161.92 | 2.222.52 | 66.07, 74.5675.78, 83.35 | 70.75, 79.4976.30, 86.21 |
Day 3 (re-encoding) | |||||||
Naming | DLDTD | 67.2888.53 | 78.1384.61 | 3.061.94 | 2.712.81 | 61.26, 73.3084.71, 92.34 | 72.81, 83.4679.09, 90.13 |
Recognition | DLDTD | 80.4096.28 | 86.9095.76 | 2.210.69 | 2.880.86 | 76.06, 84.7494.92, 97.64 | 91.23, 92.5794.07, 97.45 |
Identification | DLDTD | 97.9198.28 | 97.8897.84 | 0.920.59 | 1.131.10 | 96.10, 99.7197.13, 99.44 | 95.30, 100.4595.65, 99.98 |
Description | DLDTD | 74.1984.23 | 78.9784.35 | 2.101.41 | 3.121.82 | 70.06, 78.3281.47, 87.00 | 76.82, 85.1180.77, 87.94 |
Day 4 (retention) | |||||||
Naming | DLDTD | 34.7136.00 | 39.1531.95 | 2.872.11 | 2.813.87 | 29.07, 40.3531.83, 40.14 | 31.54, 46.7525.42, 38.47 |
Recognition | DLDTD | 73.9692.81 | 81.1692.15 | 2.510.89 | 3.051.44 | 69.02, 78.8991.05, 94.57 | 75.17, 87.1589.32, 94.98 |
Identification | DLDTD | 43.4151.34 | 45.9846.87 | 3.882.99 | 5.504.47 | 35.78, 51.0445.45, 57.22 | 35.17, 56.7938.10, 55.65 |
95% CI: 95% confidence interval (upper, lower). All values indicate percentage of accuracy on the tasks.
aValues are from the first set of GLMM analyses, which included a fixed effect to control for nonverbal IQ.
bValues are from the second set of GLMM analyses, which included fixed effects to control for nonverbal IQ and verbal WM.
Aim 1: Encoding, re-encoding, and retention of word forms, form-referent links, and meanings
Word forms: Naming
The main effect of time was significant, F(3, 407) = 140.85, p < .001, partial ŋ2 = 0.51. Follow-up contrasts for the DLD group revealed a significant increase in naming accuracy from Days 1 to 2, and from Days 2 to 3. This was followed by a significant decrease from Day 3 to Day 4. The same pattern of results was observed for the TD group. Examination of the group averages at each day showed that by Day 3, the DLD group on average attained 67% accuracy and the TD children obtained 89%. At Day 4, both groups demonstrated evidence of some retention, with accuracy levels of 35% and 36% for the DLD and TD groups, respectively. It is noteworthy that there is no chance-level performance on this task given that naming requires retrieval of the word form from memory.
The main effect of group was significant, F(1, 407) = 42.15, p < .001, partial ŋ2 = 0.09. To determine whether the group effect could be attributed to problems with initial encoding (Day 1), re-encoding (Days 2 and 3), or retention (Day 4), LSD contrasts between the groups were examined at each time point. The DLD group had significantly lower naming accuracy than the TD group at Day 1 (initial encoding) and at Days 2 and 3 (re-encoding). At Day 4 (retention), there was no significant difference between any of the three groups. The Group × Time interaction was significant, F(3, 407) = 12.40, p < .001, partial ŋ2 = 0.08 (see Figure 1(a)); however, it appears that this interaction was driven by the accuracy score for the TD group significantly dropping at Day 4, resulting in no group difference at this point.
Word forms: Recognition
The main effect for time was significant, F(3, 407) = 19.46, p < .001, partial ŋ2 = 0.13. For the DLD group, there was a non-significant increase in average recognition accuracy from Days 1 to 2, and a significant increase from Days 2 to 3. However, accuracy declined significantly from Days 3 to 4. For TD, recognition accuracy increased significantly from Days 1 to 2, Days 2 to 3, and then declined significantly from Days 3 to 4. By Day 3, the DLD and TD groups obtained average accuracy levels of 80% and 96%, respectively. At Day 4, while there was a significant drop in accuracy (to 74% for the DLD group and 93% for the TD group), the groups were still performing above chance level (i.e., 50%, given that on each trial, the child was able to answer ‘yes’ or ‘no’). As with the Naming task, both groups showed a similar pattern of linear increase in Recognition accuracy across the encoding and re-encoding phases; however, they had difficulty maintaining that level of accuracy at the retention test.
The group main effect was also significant, F(1, 407) = 69.53, p < .001, partial ŋ2 = 0.15, and follow-up contrasts showed a significant difference between the groups at all four time points. This indicates that the DLD group performed more poorly than the TD group across the tests of encoding, re-encoding, and retention. The Group × Time interaction was also significant, F(3, 407) = 3.12, p = .026, partial ŋ2 = 0.02 (see Figure 1(b)), which appeared to be driven by the DLD group dropping further in accuracy at Day 4 in comparison to the TD group.
Form-referent link: Identification
The main effect of time was significant, F(3, 407) = 139.09, p < .001, partial ŋ2 = 0.51. Follow-up contrasts for the DLD group showed no changes in accuracy across the first three days. At Day 4, however, there was a significant drop in accuracy. The TD group demonstrated the same pattern. This decline in performance at Day 4 for both groups appeared to drive the time effect. While the groups exhibited a significant drop in accuracy for the retention test, both still performed above chance level (which was 12.5%) by attaining accuracy levels of 43% and 51% (DLD and TD, respectively), indicating some degree of successful retention after the 24-hour delay. The main effect of group was significant, F(1, 407) = 5.72, p = .017, partial ŋ2 = 0.01. However, follow-up contrasts showed no significant differences between the groups at any of the four time points, suggesting comparable encoding, re-encoding and retention for the groups. The Group × Time interaction was also non-significant (see Figure 1(c)).
Word meanings: Description
There was a significant main effect for time, F(2, 305) = 6.12, p = .002, partial ŋ2 = 0.04. Follow-up contrasts for the DLD group showed a non-significant increase in description accuracy from Days 1 to 2, and a significant increase from Days 2 to 3. The TD group demonstrated the same pattern of growth. The group effect was significant, F(1, 305) = 17.26, p < .001, partial ŋ2 = 0.05. Follow-up contrasts revealed that the DLD group was significantly less accurate than the TD group across Days 1, 2, and 3, indicating deficits in encoding and re-encoding for semantic details. The Group X Time interaction was non-significant (see Figure 1(d)).
Aim 2: The relationship between word learning and verbal WM
Each of the four GLMM analyses were re-run with the addition of the Verbal WM factor as a fixed effect. Notable results are described below, and the findings for the fixed effects and interactions for each GLMM are presented in Tables 3, 4, 5, and 6.
Word forms: Naming
After controlling for verbal WM, there was a significant main effect of time (see Table 3). The main effect of group was no longer statistically significant. The interactions for Time × Verbal WM and Group × Verbal WM × Time were also non-significant. However, the main effect of verbal WM was significant, as was the Time × Group interaction. The Group × Verbal WM interaction was also significant, indicating that verbal WM had differential effects on naming in the two groups.
Table 3.
Fixed effect | F | Degrees of freedom | p | Partial ŋ2 |
---|---|---|---|---|
Group | 3.72 | 1, 399 | .055 | 0.01 |
Time | 63.02 | 3, 399 | <.001** | 0.32 |
Nonverbal IQ | 0.40 | 1, 399 | .526 | 0.001 |
Verbal WM | 63.69 | 1, 399 | <.001** | 0.14 |
Group × Time | 3.97 | 3, 399 | .008** | 0.03 |
Group × Verbal WM | 4.41 | 1, 399 | .036* | 0.01 |
Time × Verbal WM | 1.04 | 3, 399 | .377 | 0.01 |
Group × Time × Verbal WM | 0.74 | 3, 399 | .526 | 0.01 |
Nonverbal IQ: nonverbal IQ, as measured on the PTONI; Verbal WM: verbal working memory factor.
*p < .05. **p < .01.
The interaction between group and verbal WM was probed by plotting the estimated values for the DLD and TD groups when verbal WM was at the mean level and at 1 SD below the mean. Figure 2(a) displays these estimated values for children with DLD and low verbal WM (‘DLDlow’), children with DLD and verbal WM at the mean (‘DLDav’), TD children with low verbal WM (‘TDlow’), and TD children with average verbal WM (‘TDav’). Examination of the confidence intervals indicates that children with DLD and low verbal WM performed significantly worse than TD children (with average and low verbal WM), as well as those with DLD and average verbal WM, across the first three time points. However, performance was similar between all groups at Day 4. None of the other comparisons were significant, except for the DLDav group being significantly lower than the TDav group at Day 2 (confidence intervals are reported in Supplemental Materials).
Word forms: Recognition
There was a significant main effect of time, group, and verbal WM (see Table 4). The two-way interactions for Time × Group and Time × Verbal WM were non-significant, as was the three-way interaction for Group × Verbal WM × Time. However, the Group ×Verbal WM interaction was significant, indicating differential effects of verbal WM on recognition accuracy in the two groups. As with the Naming analysis, to probe the Group × Verbal WM interaction, we plotted the estimated values for the DLD and TD children according to average and below-average verbal WM (i.e., DLDlow, DLDav, TDlow, TDav; see Figure 2(b)). Examination of the confidence intervals (reported in Supplemental Materials) showed that, across Days 1, 2, and 3, children with DLD and low verbal WM had significantly poorer recognition accuracy in comparison to the DLDav, TDlow, and TDav children. At Day 4, children with DLDlow were significantly lower than the TDlow and TDav children. Finally, the children with DLD and average verbal WM were significantly lower than TD children with average verbal WM across all four days.
Table 4.
Fixed effect | F | Degrees of freedom | p | Partial ŋ2 |
---|---|---|---|---|
Group | 17.48 | 1, 399 | <.001** | 0.04 |
Time | 12.65 | 3, 399 | <.001** | 0.09 |
Nonverbal IQ | 1.30 | 1, 399 | .256 | 0.003 |
Verbal WM | 20.74 | 1, 399 | <.001** | 0.05 |
Group × Time | 0.88 | 3, 399 | .451 | 0.007 |
Group × Verbal WM | 8.30 | 1, 399 | .004** | 0.02 |
Time × Verbal WM | 0.10 | 3, 399 | .958 | 0.001 |
Group × Time × Verbal WM | 1.05 | 3, 399 | .370 | 0.007 |
Nonverbal IQ: nonverbal IQ, as measured on the PTONI; Verbal WM: verbal working memory factor.
**p < .01.
Form-referent link: Identification
The main effect for time was significant, which was likely driven by the significant drop in accuracy from Day 3 to 4 (see Table 5). The remaining main effects and interactions were all non-significant. The results suggest that verbal WM does not moderate performance for Identification.
Table 5.
Fixed effect | F | Degrees of freedom | p | Partial ŋ2 |
---|---|---|---|---|
Group | 0.67 | 1, 399 | .413 | 0.002 |
Time | 66.21 | 3, 399 | <.001** | 0.33 |
Nonverbal IQ | 3.78 | 1, 399 | .052 | 0.009 |
Verbal WM | 2.37 | 1, 399 | .125 | 0.006 |
Group × Time | 0.76 | 3, 399 | .518 | 0.006 |
Group × Verbal WM | 1.01 | 1, 399 | .316 | 0.003 |
Time × Verbal WM | 0.72 | 3, 399 | .542 | 0.005 |
Group × Time ×Verbal WM | 0.10 | 3, 399 | .958 | 0.001 |
Nonverbal IQ: nonverbal IQ, as measured on the PTONI; Verbal WM: verbal working memory factor.
**p < .01.
Word meanings: Description
The main effects of time and group were significant; however, the remaining main effects and interactions were all non-significant (see Table 6). The results suggest that verbal WM does not moderate performance for Description accuracy.
Table 6.
Fixed effect | F | Degrees of freedom | p | Partial ŋ2 |
---|---|---|---|---|
Group | 4.97 | 1, 299 | <.001** | 0.01 |
Time | 3.14 | 1, 299 | .027* | 0.01 |
Nonverbal IQ | 0.39 | 2, 299 | .532 | 0.002 |
Verbal WM | 1.90 | 1, 299 | .169 | 0.01 |
Group × Time | 0.11 | 2, 299 | .897 | 0.001 |
Group × Verbal WM | 2.98 | 1, 299 | .086 | 0.01 |
Time × Verbal WM | 0.15 | 2, 299 | .862 | 0.001 |
Group × Time ×Verbal WM | 1.91 | 2, 299 | .149 | 0.01 |
Nonverbal IQ: nonverbal IQ, as measured on the PTONI; Verbal WM: verbal working memory factor.
*p < .05; **p < .01.
Discussion
In this study, we explored whether word learning in children with DLD is characterised by deficits in encoding, re-encoding, or retention. Across these stages, word learning was evaluated using four outcome measures that tested how well the children learned the novel word forms, form-referent links, and word meanings. Additionally, we explored verbal WM as a moderator of word learning performance. Overall, the children with DLD presented with an impaired ability to learn the word forms (Naming and Recognition) and meanings (Description) compared to their TD peers. These deficits were apparent across the stages of encoding and re-encoding. In contrast, children with DLD exhibited an intact ability to learn the form-referent links (Identification). Notably, both groups exhibited problems at the retention stage for all aspects of word learning. Verbal WM significantly moderated the ability to learn new word forms, such that poor performance on the Naming and Recognition tasks was only identified in children who had DLD and low verbal WM.
The encoding of word forms and meanings is problematic for children with DLD
Consistent with our predictions, the group of children with DLD appeared to have intact skills for learning form-referent links of novel words (McGregor et al., 2020). To create a link between the form and referent, the child needs to store only a partial representation of the word form in order to be able to identify the correct item upon hearing its label (Gray et al., 2020). Therefore, it seems that the children with DLD were able to store a sufficient level of phonological detail in order to create an association with the physical referent. This finding highlights the importance of evaluating word learning skills in children with DLD using a range of outcome measures, as Identification in isolation may fail to identify a deficit. It is also important to note that, if additional novel words were included, or if fewer exposures were presented, a group difference might become apparent. This may be further explored in the future to build an understanding of the nature of form-referent link learning.
Also in line with our hypothesis, the DLD group exhibited significant difficulties with learning novel word forms. These group differences were observed on both the Naming and Recognition tasks, indicating the deficit is apparent when assessing knowledge of the word form expressively and receptively (Jackson et al., 2019a). Furthermore, examination of Naming and Recognition performance showed a significant deficit for the DLD group at Day 1. Across Days 2 and 3, both groups showed a linear increase in accuracy on the tasks, with the between-groups gap remaining relatively stable. These findings indicate that difficulties with learning novel word forms originate at the initial encoding stage for children with DLD (Bishop & Hsu, 2015; McGregor et al., 2020). Following encoding, both groups appeared to improve their knowledge of word forms through re-encoding, whereby the initially-encoded memory trace of each phonological representation was retrieved from long-term memory and further refined throughout the training task (McGregor et al., 2013a; Nader & Hardt, 2009). However, while the children with DLD clearly improved their word form knowledge through re-encoding, they did not catch up to their TD peers at this level of training intensity (Cepeda et al., 2006; Leonard et al., 2019). Difficulty encoding novel word forms has also been documented in a body of literature concerning children, adolescents, and adults with DLD (e.g., see Bishop & Hsu, 2015; Haebig et al., 2019; McGregor et al., 2013a, 2017a, 2017b, 2020).
Notably, performance on the measures of Naming, Recognition, and Identification dropped significantly for both groups at retention testing (i.e., Day 4), and group differences disappeared. On each task, however, accuracy for each group was above chance level, indicating the retention of some new word knowledge in long-term memory (Dumay & Gaskell, 2007). It may be possible to interpret the low Day 4 scores as evidence that children with DLD have comparable retention skills to TD children. That is, while the children with DLD struggled to encode effectively, they managed to retain a similar level of knowledge about word forms and form-referent links as the TD children (Lukacs et al., 2017). However, it is also important to consider why performance of the TD group dropped dramatically at Day 4, to the point where the DLD-TD gap closed. We can infer that the TD children were able to most effectively take advantage of within-session opportunities to encode and re-encode across the three days of training. It is likely that stronger oral language skills facilitated their novel word learning within the training tasks, with previous evidence showing that children with typical language more effectively take advantage of strategies such as syntactic and semantic bootstrapping to learn novel words (Chiat, 2001; Eyer et al., 2002). Despite the effectiveness of within-session learning, the TD children had not retained as much information at Day 4 as training suggested. Perhaps what we observed was that training was advantageous for TD children; however, this did not necessarily translate into consolidated gains. That is, it seems that the children with and without DLD were similarly subject to memory decay after approximately 24-hours at this level of intensity (Lukacs et al., 2017).
While our findings may suggest ineffective retention across the groups, a higher degree of training intensity would likely have bolstered retention (Kan & Windsor, 2010). Recent research, such as Leonard et al. (2019) and Storkel et al. (2017) – published after the design of our word learning protocol – showed that children with and without DLD demonstrated high levels of retention accuracy when provided with a higher degree of exposures than here. Leonard et al. (2019) provided 48 exposures over four sessions and focused on training to mastery. It is perhaps unsurprising, therefore, that the children in our study did not demonstrate effective retention in response to the provision of 24 exposures over 3 training sessions. As noted in the Methods section, however, the intention of the word learning protocol in this research was not to provide intervention, nor to ensure training to mastery. Instead, we aimed to explore patterns of word learning across different tasks (and the relationship with verbal WM). Future research may adapt our word learning protocol to facilitate more effective retention. It would also be useful to evaluate retention after various interval periods (e.g. 24 hours, one week, one month) in order to further develop effective intervention strategies to ensure long-term retention of new vocabulary knowledge for children with DLD (McGregor et al., 2020).
Finally, in addition to problems learning novel word forms, we predicted that children with DLD would struggle with establishing word meanings. Performance on the Description task highlighted a significant deficit for the DLD group; however, the small to medium effect size (partial η2 = 0.05) indicates that the deficit was more severe for learning novel word forms. This is consistent with a body of previous research (Alt & Plante, 2006; Nash & Donaldson, 2005) and supports the notion of multifactorial word learning deficits in children with DLD (Gray et al., 2020). These findings also align with those of McGregor et al. (2013b), who found that children with DLD exhibit deficits in their vocabulary depth, such that their definitions of known words are sparser than those of children with typical language. While McGregor et al. (2013b) investigated semantic knowledge of already-established vocabulary, the findings from the present study highlight that these deficits in vocabulary depth likely originate with poor encoding of semantic knowledge in children with DLD. As a result of poor encoding, we found that there remained a significant gap in Description abilities between groups across Days 2 and 3, indicating that the children with DLD did not close the gap in semantic knowledge with this degree of training intensity (Storkel et al., 2019).
A key limitation of our study was the lack of inclusion of a retention test for Description, and so we are unable to comment on retention for either group with regards to learning word meanings. This should be the focus of future research. Additionally, the nature of the novel items were limited in range in that they were all proper nouns that differed by a few visual features. Future research should further explore a range of word types and use stimuli and outcomes measures that allow exploration into how well the children make connections in their semantic networks (Mainela-Arnold et al., 2010). These findings should also be substantiated in research tasks that more closely resemble everyday word learning situations, such as interactive book reading tasks (Storkel et al., 2019).
The influence of verbal working memory
The second aim of our study was to explore whether verbal WM moderated performance across the four measures of word learning. As predicted, verbal WM significantly moderated performance on the tasks measuring word-form learning (i.e., Naming and Recognition). Notably, poor performance was observed specifically for children with DLD who also had low verbal WM; this effect was magnified especially on the Naming task. These findings indicate that verbal WM is a key factor that drives the ability to learn novel phonological representations, supporting the theoretical claim that verbal WM acts as a ‘gateway’ for vocabulary development (Baddeley et al., 1998; Lum et al., 2015). Specifically, the pattern of moderation across the four-day protocol suggests that verbal WM facilitates the initial encoding of word forms (Gathercole & Baddeley, 1989), and may also play a role in supporting the retrieval and monitoring of phonological information from long-term memory, as required in re-encoding and retention (Lum et al., 2012). Despite there being a theoretical link between verbal WM and learning novel word forms, only a handful of past studies have provided empirical evidence in support of this relationship (e.g., see Gray, 2004; Jackson et al., 2016). Furthermore, Gray (2006) and Horohov and Oetting (2004) failed to find a relationship between verbal WM and word-form learning; however, this may have been the result of floor-level performances on naming tasks, resulting in a lack of variability among scores to yield a relationship (Kan & Windsor, 2010).
As predicted, verbal WM did not moderate performance on the Identification task. It seems that the act of establishing form-referent links bypasses weaknesses in verbal WM by placing minimal demands on phonological storage and retrieval (Gray et al., 2020). Notably, however, there was a lack of variability in performance across Days 1 to 3 that may have masked a potential association, which was due to both groups performing close to ceiling. Finally, verbal WM did not moderate learning of word meanings, which was consistent with our understanding of the theoretical contribution that verbal WM plays in the word learning process (Baddeley, 2003). Our findings indicate that stronger oral language skills (i.e., as in the TD group) facilitated better performance on the Description task. It is likely that existing vocabulary was used to establish new collections of semantic details for each item, and grammatical skills were drawn on to weave these details into descriptive sentences (Carr & Johnston, 2001).
Conclusions
The results of our study provide further evidence that word learning is a problematic aspect of language development for children with DLD, especially for those who have comorbid deficits in verbal WM. Our findings indicate that children with DLD are able to effectively establish novel form-referent associations. However, they exhibit significant difficulties with developing accurate, detailed knowledge of the word forms and meanings. The problem with establishing novel word forms is compounded by poor verbal WM skills in children with DLD. Additionally, our findings support the notion that “encoding is the bottleneck that limits word learning” (McGregor et al., 2020, p. 14) in this population. While our results suggest that retention may be a relative strength, this requires further substantiation using a task that overcomes the limitations of our word learning protocol. The relationship between nonverbal IQ and word learning should also be explored in future research, as Kan and Windsor (2010) found this to be a significant moderator of word learning performance in their meta-analysis. While we controlled for nonverbal IQ, we did not have sufficient power to include an additional interaction and therefore did not explore the nature of this relationship.
The findings of the current study are relevant for the assessment and treatment practices of children with DLD. Four specific implications emerge:
Word learning is a multifaceted process, and given the heterogeneous nature of DLD it is important for speech-language pathologists (SLPs) to consider that clients will likely present with an individual profile of word learning ability. SLPs should evaluate word learning using a range of outcome measures, and avoid using a single outcome measure such as Identification, which may give a false sense of word learning abilities. Understanding a child’s word learning strengths and weaknesses would support the provision of individually-tailored intervention strategies (Storkel et al., 2019).
Children with DLD will require intervention that explicitly targets their ability to learn new word forms and meanings. Furthermore, they will likely benefit from a high degree of training intensity compared to their peers as a way of mitigating an encoding deficit (McGregor et al., 2013a), and long-term retention of word knowledge may be supported through the use of periodic review (McGregor et al., 2020). Further research should explore whether there is an ideal treatment intensity that benefits children with DLD who have comorbid deficits in verbal WM (Archibald & Gathercole, 2006). Various strategies and cues, such as the presence of orthography and gestural cues, may also prove useful for these children (Ricketts et al., 2015; Vogt & Kauschke, 2017).
A theoretically-informed assessment battery for children with DLD should include measures of verbal WM (such as nonword repetition and digit span tasks). This information may allow SLPs to identify which children are at greatest risk of word learning impairments.
The finding that within-session learning may not transfer to retention of learned information has important implications for teaching new vocabulary in the context of intervention and classroom teaching. SLPs and teachers should monitor how effectively children have learned new words within sessions but also after a delay (e.g., a day, week, and month) to determine whether effective retention has occurred.
Supplemental Material
Supplemental material, sj-pdf-1-dli-10.1177_23969415211004109 for Word learning and verbal working memory in children with developmental language disorder by Emily Jackson, Suze Leitão, Mary Claessen and Mark Boyes in Autism & Developmental Language Impairments
Supplemental material, sj-pdf-2-dli-10.1177_23969415211004109 for Word learning and verbal working memory in children with developmental language disorder by Emily Jackson, Suze Leitão, Mary Claessen and Mark Boyes in Autism & Developmental Language Impairments
Acknowledgements
We would like to sincerely thank the schools and students who contributed greatly to this research through their participation.
Appendix 1
Table A1.
Word length | Training item | Experimental items |
---|---|---|
1-syllable | – | /nɔb/ /lɜz/ |
2-syllable | /poʊdɔd/a | /doʊnug/ /jugɔɪn/ |
3-syllable | – | /gɔnəpɛk/ /gɪtəmoʊk/ |
4-syllable | – | /hɒʃətæjɪk/ /gufəʃɜgʊs/ |
Note. Pseudowords were taken from Jackson et al. (2016) and Jusczyk et al. (1994). Two- and three-syllable stimuli were pronounced with stress on the first syllable, and the four-syllable items were pronounced with emphasis on the third syllable.
aOnly one item (two-syllable nonword) was used for training, as pilot testing showed that training one item was sufficient for participants to understand the parameters of the task.
Footnotes
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Funding: The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: The first author was supported by an Australian Government Research Training Program Scholarship, and the fourth author was supported by the National Health and Medical Research Council, Australia (Investigator Grant 1173043).
ORCID iDs: Emily Jackson https://orcid.org/0000-0002-2537-7387
Mark Boyes https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5420-8606
Supplemental material: Supplemental material for this article is available online.
References
- Alt M., Plante E. (2006). Factors that influence lexical and semantic fast mapping of young children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 49, 941–954. https://doi.org/1092-4388/06/4905-0941 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alt M., Suddarth R. (2012). Learning novel words: Detail and vulnerability of initial representations for children with specific language impairment and typically developing peers. Journal of Communication Disorders, 45, 84–97. 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2011.12.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Archibald L. M. (2016). Working memory and language learning: A review. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 33, 5–17. 10.1177/0265659016654206 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Archibald L. M., Gathercole S. E. (2006). Short-term and working memory in specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 41, 675–693. 10.1080/13682820500442602 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Archibald L. M. D., Joanisse M. F. (2009). On the sensitivity and specificity of nonword repetition and sentence recall to language and memory impairments in children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 52, 899–914. https://doi.org/1092-4388/09/5204-0899 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Armstrong R., Arnott W., Copland D. A., McMahon K., Khan A., Najman J. M., Scott J. G. (2017). Change in receptive vocabulary from childhood to adulthood: Associated mental health, education and employment outcomes. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 52, 561–572. 10.1111/1460-6984.12301 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley A. D. (2003). Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36, 189–208. 10.1016/S0021-9924(03)00019-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley A. D., Gathercole S., Papagno C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158–173. https://doi.org/0033-295X/98/$3.00 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balota D. A., Yap M. J., Hutchison K. A., Cortese M. J., Kessler B., Loftis B., Neely J. H., Nelson D. L., Simpson G. B., Treiman R. (2007). The English lexicon project. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 445–459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck I. L., McKeown M. G., Kucan L. (2013). Bringing words to life: Robust vocabulary instruction (2nd ed.). The Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Bishop D. V. (2006). What causes specific language impairment in children? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 217–221. https://www.jstor.org/stable/20183118?origin=JSTOR-pdf&seq=1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop D. V., Snowling M. J., Thomson P. A., Greenhalgh T., CATALISE-2 Consortium. (2017). Phase 2 of CATALISE: A multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development: Terminology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 58, 1068–1080. 10.1111/jcpp.12721 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop D. V. M., Hsu H. J. (2015). The declarative system in children with specific language impairment: A comparison of meaningful and meaningless auditory-visual paired associate learning. BMC Psychology, 3(3), 1–12. 10.1186/s40359-015-0062-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowen C. (2014). Children's speech sound disorders (2nd ed.). John Wiley & Sons Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Cabeza R., Dolcos F., Graham R., Nyberg L. (2002). Similarities and differences in the neural correlates of episodic memory retrieval and working memory. NeuroImage, 16, 317–330. 10.1006/nimg.2002.1063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carr L., Johnston J. (2001). Morphological cues to verb meaning. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 601–618. [Google Scholar]
- Castles A., Rastle K., Nation K. (2018). Ending the reading wars: Reading acquisition from novice to expert. Psychological Science in the Public Interest: A Journal of the American Psychological Society, 19, 5–51. doi: 10.1177/1529100618772271 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cepeda N. J., Pashler H., Vul E., Wixted J. T., Rohrer D. (2006). Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: A review and quantitative synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 354–380. 10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.354 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiat S. (2001). Mapping theories of developmental language impairment: Premises, predictions and evidence. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 113–142. 10.1080/01690960042000012 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Davis M. H., Gaskell M. G. (2009). A complementary systems account of word learning: Neural and behavioural evidence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 364, 3773–3800. 10.1098/rstb.2009.0111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desmottes L., Maillart C., Meulemans T. (2016). Memory consolidation in children with specific language impairment: Delayed gains and susceptibility to interference in implicit sequence learning. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 39, 265–221. 10.1080/13803395.2016.1223279 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dodd B., Hua Z., Crosbie S., Holm A., Ozanne A. (2002). Diagnostic evaluation of articulation and phonology (DEAP). Pearson.
- Dollaghan C., Campbell T. F. (1998). Nonword repetition and child language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 41, 1136–1146. https://doi.org/1092-4388/98/4105-1136 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dumay N., Gaskell M. G. (2007). Sleep-associated changes in the mental representation of spoken words. Psychological Science, 18, 35–39. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01845.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ehrler D., McGhee R. (2008). Primary Test of Nonverbal Intelligence (PTONI). Pro Ed.
- Eyer J. A., Leonard L. B., Bedore L. M., McGregor K. K., Anderson B., Viescas R. (2002). Fast mapping of verbs by children with specific language impairment. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 16, 59–77. 10.1080/02699200110102269 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gathercole S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 513–543. https://doi.org/10.1017.S0142716406060383 [Google Scholar]
- Gathercole S. E., Baddeley A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 200–213. [Google Scholar]
- Gathercole S. E., Pickering S. J. (2001). Working memory test battery for children. Pearson.
- Gray S. (2004). Word learning by preschoolers with specific language impairment: Predictors and poor learners. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 47, 1117–1132. https://doi.org/1092-4388/04/4705-1117 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray S. (2005). Word learning by preschoolers with specific language impairment: Effect of phonological or semantic cues. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 48, 1452–1467. https://doi.org/1092-4388/05/4806-1452 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray S. (2006). The relationship between phonological memory, receptive vocabulary, and fast mapping in young children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 49, 955–969. https://doi.org/1092-4388/06/4905-0955 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray S., Brinkley S. (2011). Fast mapping and word learning by preschoolers with specific language impairment in a supported learning context: Effect of encoding cues, phonotactic probability, and object familiarity. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 54, 870–884. 10.1044/1092-4388(2010/09-0285) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray S., Brinkley S., Svetina D. (2012). Word learning by preschoolers with SLI: Effect of phonotactic probability and object familiarity. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 55, 1289–1300. 10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0095) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray S., Lancaster H. S., Alt M., Hogan T., Green S., Levy R., Cowan N. (2020). The structure of word learning in young school-age children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 63, 1446–1466. 10.1044/2020_JSLHR-19-00186 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haebig E., Leonard L. B., Deevy P., Karpicke J., Christ S. L., Usler E., Kueser J. B., Souto S., Krok W., Weber C. (2019). Retrieval-based word learning in young typically developing children and children with development language disorder II: A comparison of retrieval schedules. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 62, 944–964. 10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-18-0071 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hansson K., Forsberg J., Lofqvist A., Maki-Torkko E., Sahlen B. (2004). Working memory and novel word learning in children with hearing impairment and children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 39, 401–422. 10.1080/13682820410001669887 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horohov, J. E., & Oetting, J. B. (2004). Effects of input manipulations on the word learning abilities of children with and without specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 43--65. https://doi.org/10.1017.S0142716404001031
- IBM Corp. (2019). IBM SPSS statistics for Windows (Version 26.0) [Computer software]. IBM Corp.
- Jackson E., Leitao S., Claessen M. (2016). The relationship between phonological short-term memory, receptive vocabulary and fast mapping in children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 51, 61–73. 10.1111/1460-6984.12185 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jackson E., Leitao S., Claessen M., Boyes M. E. (2019. a). The evaluation of word-learning abilities in people with developmental language disorder: A scoping review. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 54, 742–755. 10.1111/1460-6984.12490 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jackson E., Leitao S., Claessen M., Boyes M. E. (2019. b). Fast mapping short and long words: Examining the influence of phonological short-term memory and receptive vocabulary in children with developmental language disorder. Journal of Communication Disorders, 79, 11–23. 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2019.02.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jackson, E., Leitão, S., Claessen, M., & Boyes, M. E. (2020). Working, declarative, and procedural memory in children with developmental language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 63, 4162--4178. https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_JSLHR-20-00135 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jusczyk P. W., Luce P. A., Charles-Luce J. (1994). Infants' sensitivity to phonotactic patterns in the native language. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 630–645. 10.1006/jmla.1994.1030 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kan P. F., Windsor J. (2010). Word learning in children with primary language impairment: A .mMeta-analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 53, 739–756. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kane M. J., Bleckley M. K., Conway A. R., Engle R. W. (2001). A controlled-attention view of working-memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 169–183. 10.1037//0096-3445.130.2.169 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Law J., Rush R., Schoon I., Parsons S. (2009). Modeling developmental language difficulties from school entry into adulthood: Literacy, mental health, and employment outcomes. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 52, 1401–1416. 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0142) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leonard L. B., Karpicke J., Deevy P., Weber C., Christ S. L., Haebig E., Souto S., Kueser J. B., Krok W. (2019). Retrieval-based word learning in young typically developing children and children with developmental language disorder I: The benefits of repeated retrieval. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 62, 932–943. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukacs A., Kemeny F., Lum J. A., Ullman M. T. (2017). Learning and overnight retention in declarative memory in specific language impairment. PLoS One, 12, e0169474. 10.1371/journal.pone.0169474 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lum J. A., Conti-Ramsden G., Page D., Ullman M. T. (2012). Working, declarative and procedural memory in specific language impairment. Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 48, 1138–1154. 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.06.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lum J. A., Ullman M. T., Conti-Ramsden G. (2015). Verbal declarative memory impairments in specific language impairment are related to working memory deficits. Brain and Language, 142, 76–85. 10.1016/j.bandl.2015.01.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McClelland J. L., McNaughton B. L., O'Reilly R. C. (1995). Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychological Review, 102, 419–457. 10.1037/0033-295X.102.3.419 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGregor K., Arbisi-Kelm T., Eden N. (2017. a). The encoding of word forms into memory may be challenging for college students with developmental language impairment. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 19, 43–57. 10.3109/17549507.2016.1159337 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGregor K., Arbisi-Kelm T., Eden N., Oleson J. (2020). The word learning profile of adults with developmental language disorder. Autism & Developmental Language Impairments, 5, 239694151989931–239694151989919. 10.1177/2396941519899311 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGregor K., Gordon K., Eden N., Arbisi-Kelm T., Oleson J. (2017. b). Encoding deficits impede word learning and memory in adults with developmental language disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 60, 2891–2905. 10.1044/2017_JSLHR-L-17-0031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGregor K., Licandro U., Arenas R., Eden N., Stiles D., Bean A., Walker E. (2013. a). Why words are hard for adults with developmental language impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 56, 1845–1856. http://dx.doi.org.dbgw.lis.curtin.edu.au/1092-4388(2013/12-0233) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGregor K. K., Oleson J., Bahnsen A., Duff D. (2013. b). Children with developmental language impairment have vocabulary deficits characterized by limited breadth and depth. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 48, 307–319. 10.1111/1460-6984.12008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mainela-Arnold E., Evans J. L., Coady J. A. (2010). Explaining lexical-semantic deficits in specific language impairment: The role of phonological similarity, phonological working memory, and lexical competition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 53, 1742–1756. 10.1044/1092-4388(2010/08-0198) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Montgomery J. W., Magimairaj B. M., Finney M. C. (2010). Working memory and specific language impairment: An update on the relation and perspectives on assessment and treatment. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 19, 78–94. 10.1044/1058-0360(2009/09-0028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nader K., Hardt O. (2009). A single standard for memory: The case for reconsolidation. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 10, 224–234. 10.1038/nrn2590 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nash M., Donaldson M. L. (2005). Word learning in children with vocabulary deficits. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 48, 439–458. https://doi.org/1092-4388/05/4802-0439 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nation K. (2014). Lexical learning and lexical processing in children with developmental language impairments. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 369, 1–10. 10.1098/rstb.2012.0387 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norbury C. F., Gooch D., Wray C., Baird G., Charman T., Simonoff E., Vamvakas G., Pickles A. (2016). The impact of nonverbal ability on prevalence and clinical presentation of language disorder: Evidence from a population study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied disciplines , 57, 1247–1257. 10.1111/jcpp.12573 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pennington B. F. (2006). From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition, 101, 385–413. 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.04.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rice M. L., Buhr J. C., Nemeth M. (1990). Fast mapping word-learning abilities of language-delayed preschoolers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55, 33–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rice M. L., Buhr J. C., Oetting J. B. (1992). Specific-language-impaired children's quick incidental learning of words: The effect of a pause. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 35, 1040–1048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rice M. L., Cleave P. L., Oetting J. B. (2000). The use of syntactic cues in lexical acquisition by children with SLI. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 43, 582–594. https://doi.org/1092-4388/00/4303-0582 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rice M. L., Oetting J. B., Marquis J., Bode J., Pae S. (1994). Frequency of input effects on word comprehension of children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 37, 106–122. https://doi.org/0022-4685943701-0106$0 1.00/0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ricketts J., Dockrell J. E., Patel N., Charman T., Lindsay G. (2015). Do children with specific language impairment and autism spectrum disorders benefit from the presence of orthography when learning new spoken words? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 134, 43–61. 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.01.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Semel E., Wiig E. H., Secord W. A. (2006). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals, fourth edition (CELF-4 Australian). Pearson.
- Skipp A., Windfuhr K. L., Conti-Ramsden G. (2002). Children's grammatical categories of verb and noun: A comparative look at children with specific language impairment (SLI) and normal language (NL). International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 37, 253–271. 10.1080/13682820110119214 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spencer S., Clegg J., Stackhouse J., Rush R. (2017). Contribution of spoken language and socio-economic background to adolescents' educational achievement at age 16 years. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 52, 184–196. 10.1111/1460-6984.12264 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stickgold R. (2005). Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature, 437, 1272–1278. 10.1038/nature04286 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Storkel H. L., Komesidou R., Pezold M. J., Pitt A. R., Fleming K. K., Swinburne Romine R. (2019). The impact of dose and dose frequency on word learning by kindergarten children with developmental language disorder during interactive book reading. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 50, 518–539. 10.1044/2019_LSHSS-VOIA-18-0131 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Storkel H. L., Voelmle K., Fierro V., Flake K., Fleming K. K., Swinburne Romine R. (2017). Interactive book reading to accelerate word learning by kindergarten children with specific language impairment: Identifying an adequate intensity and variation in treatment response. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 48, 16–30. 10.1044/2016_LSHSS-16-0014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suzuki W. A. (2006). Encoding new episodes and making them stick. Neuron, 50, 19–21. 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tomblin B. J., Records N. L., Buckwalter P., Zhang X., Smith E., O'Brien M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 40, 1245–1260. 10.1044/jslhr.4006.1245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ullman M. T., Earle F. S., Walenski M., Janacsek K. (2020). The neurocognition of developmental disorders of language. Annual Review of Psychology, 71, 389–417. 10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011555 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vogt S., Kauschke C. (2017). Observing iconic gestures enhances word learning in typically developing children and children with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Language, 44, 1458–1484. 10.1017/S0305000916000647 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilhelm I., Prehn-Kristensen A., Born J. (2012). Sleep-dependent memory consolidation – What can be learnt from children? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 1718–1728. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.03.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Supplemental material, sj-pdf-1-dli-10.1177_23969415211004109 for Word learning and verbal working memory in children with developmental language disorder by Emily Jackson, Suze Leitão, Mary Claessen and Mark Boyes in Autism & Developmental Language Impairments
Supplemental material, sj-pdf-2-dli-10.1177_23969415211004109 for Word learning and verbal working memory in children with developmental language disorder by Emily Jackson, Suze Leitão, Mary Claessen and Mark Boyes in Autism & Developmental Language Impairments