Abstract
Translational research in behavioral neuroscience seeks causes and remedies for human mental health problems in animals, following leads imposed by clinical research in psychiatry. This endeavor faces several problems because scientists must read and interpret animal movements to represent human perceptions, mood, and memory processes. Yet, it is still not known how mammalian brains bundle all these processes into a highly compressed motor output in the brain stem and spinal cord, but without that knowledge, translational research remains aimless. Based on some four decades of experience in the field, the article identifies sources of interpretation problems and illustrates typical translational pitfalls. (1) The sensory world of mice is different. Smell, hearing, and tactile whisker sensations dominate in rodents, while visual input is comparatively small. In humans, the relations are reversed. (2) Mouse and human brains are equated inappropriately: the association cortex makes up a large portion of the human neocortex, while it is relatively small in rodents. The predominant associative cortex in rodents is the hippocampus itself, orchestrating chiefly inputs from secondary sensorimotor areas and generating species-typical motor patterns that are not easily reconciled with putative human hippocampal functions. (3) Translational interpretation of studies of memory or emotionality often neglects the ecology of mice, an extremely small species surviving by freezing or flight reactions that do not need much cognitive processing. (4) Further misinterpretations arise from confounding neuronal properties with system properties, and from rigid mechanistic thinking unaware that many experimentally induced changes in the brain do partially reflect unpredictable compensatory plasticity. (5) Based on observing hippocampal lesion effects in mice indoors and outdoors, the article offers a simplistic general model of hippocampal functions in relation to hypothalamic input and output, placing hypothalamus and the supraspinal motor system at the top of a cerebral hierarchy. (6) Many translational problems could be avoided by inclusion of simple species-typical behaviors as end-points comparable to human cognitive or executive processing, and to rely more on artificial intelligence for recognizing patterns not classifiable by traditional psychological concepts.
Keywords: comparative, neuroanatomy, neuroethology, behavioral testing, hippocampal lesions, hypothalamus, memory models, motor priming
Introduction
The problem
Translational research extrapolates findings from basic research in animals to clinical fields. On the other side, clinical scientists are supposed to stimulate basic scientists to focus on research topics that may result faster in clinical and therapeutical solutions (Sampath and Ramchandran, 2014). Translational approaches in many fields of medicine are based on a solid body of biochemical, histological, and molecular knowledge of different organs in both standard laboratory animals and human tissue. Most of that knowledge reflects evolutionarily conserved features and processes that are unlikely to vary strongly between species. With respect to the brain, this also includes cellular and molecular processes in and between neurons and glial cells. Thus, research requests by clinicians focusing on such topics are often feasible and realization mostly depends on available resources and motivation. The situation is different in behavioral neuroscience. For one, there is a larger group of neurobehavioral scientists and animal psychologists without clinical goals but presenting their approaches as clinically meaningful, often by investigating genetically modified mice. On the clinical side, neuropsychologists, psychotherapists, and psychiatrists are requesting a focus on basic research eventually solving their problems but are facing an immense number of articles reporting that genetic targeting of mechanisms at the cellular level in mice and rats somehow produces behavioral changes that may or may not fit into clinical symptoms.
The main problem is simple. After some 200 years of brain research in mammalian species and humans, there is no commonly accepted view of how the brain operates as a whole in different species. There is no doubt that the structure of neurons, axons, synapses, and related molecular processes is vastly similar. But how to combine these components to reach an easy understanding of behavioral species differences–the condition sine qua non in translational research–has remained frustratingly elusive. The problem is compounded by the ill-defined term “behavior” which seems to have numerous connotations and interpretations even in neuroscience. However, in strict terms, the only output of the brain is the control of striate and smooth muscle fibers. Movements caused by striated muscles can be observed, while many effects on physiology are mediated by regulation of blood flow caused by the action of smooth muscle fibers on small arteries. Therefore, the sole and only purpose of the nervous system in all autonomously moving species is to produce patterned movements and homeostatic co-regulation of the milieu interieur. Evolution has shown an increasing investment in myelinated long fiber tracts enabling the brain to fine-tune motor acts (Mota et al., 2019) and everyone who has ever dissected the massive corticopontine fiber tracts in the human brain can only agree. However, the view of a brain shaped by its motor output structures is unpopular in the field of behavioral science but should be at least understood by translational scientists.
Two brands of neuroscientists
Neuroanatomists and neuroethologists are easier to convince since they have a rather straightforward view: above the level of the hypothalamus, mammalian brains have a rather uniform design. The thalamus and its ascending fibers distribute radially, like colored laser beams in a nightclub, various subcortical and sensory inputs to a dome-like screen organized in columns (Supplementary Figure S1). In contrast to a nightclub, that screen is heavily interconnected, permitting spreading and blending of patches of excitation yet primarily to neighbored columns. But the basic design of the neocortex is radial (Rakic, 2009) and not tangential, being driven by thalamic output that remains topographically arranged with little crosstalk between thalamic channels. The output of the mammalian neocortex is equally radially organized: myelinated fibers converge eventually to the mesencephalic midbrain and pyramidal tract handling together the precise activation and sequencing of striated muscle cells. Thus, the output of the entire brain is squeezed and twisted into a somatotopically arranged pattern of axons known as “homunculus”, “simiaculus”, or “musculus” depending on the species. Obviously, the contribution of the neocortex is shaped somehow by iteratively passing through thalamus, basal ganglia, and cerebellum in the form of aligned channels. But how the neocortex achieves the cognition-based selection and fine-tuning of output channels is beyond the neuroanatomist's experience and scope and thus left to others.
These others include a variety of scientists from fields, such as neuropsychology, psychiatry, and system theory, that have a penchant to perceive the cortex from its sensory side, preferably visual. Other neuroscientists focus on specific functional features resulting in tags and labels, such as “emotional brain”, “visceral brain”, “social brain”, “mind”, and “consciousness”, to name a few. Such approaches are mostly theory-driven and try to read the observed motor patterns in animals according to predictions made by psychological theories. But since all types of behaviors including complex patterns of movement, such as language and writing, are lastly motor endpoints, behavioral neuroscientists face the same problem as neuroanatomists in explaining the fine-tuning of the so-called executive functions. In principle, behavioral research in animals resembles interpreting the interiors of a moving black box and becomes increasingly tentative with evolutionary distance. At least in humans, advances in whole brain functional imaging and large-scale electrical patterns including EEG and event-related potentials (ERP) provide guidance, increasingly also in animals. However, for technical and conceptual reasons, the focus of these techniques is primarily on the cortex, while visualizing subcortical activity patterns is more difficult and demanding. Given this constellation, the task of translational neuroscientists is doubly difficult. They must cope with the situation of lacking fundamental knowledge of how cognition translates into motor pattern in mammals, but they are supposed to use animals, analyze their movements, and find critical brain mechanisms and processes that permit to realize clinically defined goals. Another difficulty is the flood of genetically modified mice in which genes theoretically meaningful for the proper functioning of memory and emotions were altered but the resulting changes in motor patterning of the transgenic animals appear frustratingly similar: either the mice move too fast, too slow, too early, or too late.
An overview of the article
Therefore, the goal of this article is to provide concepts and personal insights enabling scientists to tackle problems inherent in behavioral translational analysis and to avoid pitfalls in interpretation and experimental approaches. Concepts and insights are derived from 40 years of experience in various fields of behavioral neuroscience, often working with natural and genetically induced variations of brain and behavior in mice but also with several other mammalian and avian species. The article will thus review the older work of the authors but also present unpublished data bolstering some of the claims. Since both, reasonable knowledge of comparative neuroanatomy and neuroethology, are necessary for a translational approach, a condensed overview of brain architecture related to behavior and cognition will be given first. A discussion of hidden species differences in neuroanatomy of mice and humans follows, focusing in some detail on the hippocampus and its integration into the cortical functional architecture. The next section is devoted to key elements of mouse behavior and its role in daily life and evolution. As mechanistic explanations are often requested in translational research, examples from own research will show that unpredictable events in brain plasticity occur more frequently than thought and should be integrated conceptually. This is joined by a functional analysis of hippocampal lesions in mice as observed in the laboratory and under semi-naturalistic conditions. Finally, the article suggests that placing the basal forebrain structures and the supraspinal motor system (SMS) at the top of the brain hierarchy facilitates conceptually translational approaches and provides concrete suggestions and advice for experimental strategies in the field.
Neuroanatomical differences and similarities between mice and humans
Sensory systems
Species-differences in sensory capacities can easily be identified in Figure 1 showing the base of the brain with the main cranial nerves. In humans, the optic nerve and oculomotor nerve dominate in volume, while both olfactory tract and trigeminal nerve are comparatively small. The mouse, however, shows voluminous olfactory tracts yet small optic nerves. In comparison to humans, the mouse trigeminal nerve is also much larger. Whether the cochlear (auditory nerves) differ comparatively in size cannot be judged by such ventral views, but even the large ears of mice as compared to the ones of rats imply the importance of auditory cues for the mouse. With the exclusion of olfactory inputs, all these sensory modalities are channeled through the thalamus to the neocortex. There, the sizes of sensory cortical fields correspond to the sizes of the cranial nerves, specifically in the barrel cortex in mice that dominates auditory and visual fields (Froudarakis et al., 2019). Nonetheless, a literature search of mouse learning in relation to sensory abilities of specialized cortex regions reveals about 10 articles for vibrissae-based learning, and some 1,500 articles involving the visual cortex. Given the importance of whiskers for mice and their propensity of living in poorly illuminated environments, this represents an investigative mismatch. In fact, mice can learn vibrisso-tactile discrimination tasks quite easily (Warren et al., 2021), the problem is to analyze their learning strategies and type of errors (Lipp and Van der Loos, 1991). For interpreting mouse behavior correctly, scientists should try to imagine a world of smells (an “olfactorama” sensu Rekow et al., 2022) and to envision what it means to move around with a set of whiskers that extend farther than the reach of the hands. But the terms “imagine” and “envision” reveal the strong visual bias of humans. To be fair, the importance of olfaction for learning in mice is well documented but the translational value is difficult to estimate, not at least because the performance of human olfactory system is certainly inferior to one of many non-primate species.
Structural layout of the mammalian brain
The “old” brain and its connections
The structural layout of neuronal groups and fiber connections in basal forebrain and upper brainstem appears rather similar across vertebrate species, as it corresponds to the phylogenetically conserved “minimal brain” or “old brain” able to maintain sufficiently sophisticated motor outputs to ensure survival in various ecological niches. The basic layout and design of the vertebrate brain with a central ventricular system and a radial structure formed by glial and neuronal cells may have a history of 560 Mio years (Suryanarayana et al., 2020). The origin of the modern mammalian multilayered cortex interconnected by the corpus callosum dates back probably no further than 120 million years (Mihrshahi, 2006).
In terms of behavioral control, the most powerful structure is the phylogenetic old motor control system, the supraspinal motor system (SMS) in the reticular formation, and the rostral brain stem. An incredibly complex set of nuclei and axonal connections mediates species-specific behavior (ethological “elements”) and automated behavioral routines such as locomotion and integrates them with “pyramidal” input from the neocortex (Esposito and Arber, 2016; Josset et al., 2018). The SMS is bidirectionally connected with phylogenetic old sensory structures such as superior colliculus for visual inputs and inferior colliculus for auditory input. The other inputs to the SMS originate from the basal forebrain system (BFS) including anterior olfactory, septal nuclei, and the hypothalamic area by means of short reciprocal connections forming the medial forebrain bundle (MFB) and the dorsal longitudinal fascicle (DLF). The hypothalamic area is also extremely complex, its nine nuclei integrating physiological, humoral, motivational, and limbic inputs, often in set-points regulating homeostatic processes (Dougherty, 2020). From a behavioral point of view, the connections of the hypothalamus to the SMS orchestrate flexibly species-specific behaviors. For example, electric stimulation of the ventromedial hypothalamus (VMH) in marmoset monkeys elicits defensive vocal threat (chattering) and slashes toward a cage companion, but in presence of a fear-inducing observer the stimulated monkey switches to mobbing behavior, a species-specific behavior including lateral body swaying and sharp mobbing calls (Lipp, 1978; Lipp and Hunsperger, 1978). Such behavioral flexibility is regularly observed when stimulating hypothalamic areas in other species (Lipp, 1979; Halasz et al., 2002; Haller, 2013; Kruk, 2014; Wang et al., 2021; Bang et al., 2022) and appears to be a species-invariant feature.
The neocortical sensorimotor loop system
The thalamus distributes afferent sensory inputs in diverging bundles creating somatotopic, retinotopic, or cochleotopic maps in primary sensory and motor cortex areas that show, except for auditory input, few, if any, callosal projections (Figure 2). Their activity patterns blend with the adjacent association cortex and transform eventually into the marginal limbic cortex and hippocampus. The motor cortex gives origin to the well-known somatotopic corticospinal (pyramidal tract)—the only efferent system of the neocortex passing to the spinal cord—which mediates fine-tuned movements. The output activity of the motor and premotor cortex is constantly refined by two loop systems: a topographically mapped (but only partially somatotopically organized) feedback loop from all cortical areas descends as corticopontine tract to the pontine nuclei, from where cerebellar mossy fibers ascend to the contralateral cerebellum. The ponto-cerebellum integrates polymodal neocortical activity patterns with proprioceptive somatotopic inputs from the vestibulo- and spinocerebellum, and bundles converging fibers to the deep cerebellar nuclei (DN). These send somatotopically arranged fibers chiefly to the motor thalamic areas. The other topographically ordered loops include polymodal fibers from most cortical regions converging on the striatum (STR) from where inhibitory fibers of the pallidum (PL) reach the motor thalamic nuclei where they control the ascending throughput of the cortico-ponto-cerebello-thalamo-cortical loop. Note the distinction between somatotopic (just mapping a motor “homunculus”) and polymodal yet topographically organized projections (keeping fibers from specific cortex regions together) influencing the refinement of the motor output. In simple terms, the total activity pattern of the neocortex is being blended, twisted, and forced to drive the motor “homunculus” in spinal cord and brain stem, or, for that matter, a corresponding “simiaculus” in monkeys or “musculus” in mice.
The “cognitive” loop system
The other loops as shown in the left half of Figure 2 depict the arrangement of the “thinking brain” (Cook, 1986). As with the posteriorly located somatosensory inputs, ascending fiber tracts from the thalamus diverge radially to reach a variety of more frontally located cortical areas including various types of associative cortex fields, not unlike a bouquet of flowers in the form of neocortical columns. However, these remain connected with proximal ipsilateral columns but also with near-symmetrically located contralateral columns on the other side. The main difference to the sensorimotor cortex is that the thalamic input originates from the midbrain and from a chain of interconnected structures of the basal forebrain, such as the anterior olfactory nuclei, the nucleus accumbens septi, the septum, and from the various divisions of the hypothalamus. Thus, the associative cortex is not only concerned with handling the interactions with cortical regions but is driven substantially by motivationally relevant structures. Given that the thalamocortical fibers of the limbic and “non-specific” thalamus ascend bilaterally, their impact on a given neocortical column appears even stronger than the input from neighbored ipsilateral columns alone, because of the corpus callosum connecting near-homotopic cortical fields. Part of the output of the association cortex is fed into the motor fine-tuning loops. Another portion reaches via polysynaptic chains the (marginal) limbic cortex and the hippocampal formation, but this pathway for propagation narrows considerably (see also Figure 4). The cingular and entorhinal cortex also innervate the ventralmost basal ganglia, including the nucleus accumbens and the ventral pallidum, the later inhibiting the anterior thalamic nuclei and possibly the intralaminar system of the thalamus. Other fibers from hippocampus and amygdala reach the hypothalamus and reticular formation, from where fibers ascend again to the associative thalamus, closing a cortico-limbic-hypothalamo-thalamo-cortical loop. Thus, the general layout of the associative cortex and its subcortical loops resembles the sensorimotor portions of the neocortex.
Taken together, the integrative activity of the entire cortex is structured by the thalamus (Shine, 2021): non-specific nuclei drive associative and limbic cortex, the strength of driving being regulated by intrathalamic inhibition of throughput channels. For one, tonically active inhibitory fibers from the limbic pallidum regulate the transmission of subcortical input to the anterior thalamic nuclei that are connected to prefrontal and limbic areas. In addition, the second GABAergic intrathalamic system, the reticular nuclei (Crabtree, 2018), appears to receive input from the rostral reticular formation and the SMSs that could throttle or amplify any sensory and associative input to the neocortex. Finally, the thalamus harbors the intralaminar system (a powerful input amplifier of widespread cortical regions). Further ascending control systems in the brain stem are the aminergic projections (dopaminergic, noradrenergic, serotoninergic) which innervate the neocortex in a less precise fashion than topographically ordered projections. Any alteration in these comparatively minor structures, be it genetic, developmental, or pathological, has thus a potentially powerful impact on neocortical activity.
Focus on the hippocampus
In evolutionary terms, the hippocampus is a relative newcomer as the classic trisynaptic loop with mossy fibers is found in mammals only (Witter et al., 2017). In less encephalized species, it appears that the hippocampus is occasionally the largest associative cortical structure as nicely observed in the African elephant shrew (Elephantulus myurus) whose hemispheres appear to consist primarily of hippocampi yet have only small neocortical areas (Slomianka et al., 2013). Nonetheless, it is perfectly adapted to its habitat and its rodent family is found widely across Subsaharan Africa.
Hippocampal functions: History of concepts underlying translational research
Functionally, the hippocampus proper was considered for a long time a somewhat enigmatic structure. Early lesion studies listed by O'Keefe and Nadel (1978) revealed a bewildering variety of behavioral effects including deficits in some species-typical behaviors (Kim et al., 1970, 1971) yet often contradictory results with respect to acquisition and performance in learning paradigms used in experimental psychology. Likewise, electrical stimulation of the hippocampus provoked chiefly brief attentional responses, much in contrast to the spectacular behaviors elicited by electrical brain stimulation in the hypothalamus and the amygdala. Since many lesion studies entailed hyperactivity, the consensus was that the hippocampus appeared to have a generally inhibiting role for spontaneous locomotor behaviors (Douglas, 1967; Simonov, 1974), but why and how remained unclear.
Significant progress resulted from an in-depth neurophysiological analysis of hippocampal circuitry by the Andersen group. They described the now classical “trisynaptic” feed-forward loop (Andersen, 1959; Andersen et al., 1973), the lamellar organization of the hippocampus (Andersen et al., 1969) as shown in Figures 3, 15, the phenomenon of long-term potentiation, LTP (Bliss and Lomo, 1973) and the in-vitro preparation of hippocampal slices (Skrede and Westgaard, 1971). Taken together, this permitted a vision of the hippocampus in which various inputs could be held temporarily in functionally separate channels in CA3 yet permitted to interact by means of Schaffer collaterals. This vision has been criticized, e.g., by Buszaki (Lisman et al., 2017) but remains essential for most theories relying on functional mapping of cortical and subcortical connections in the hippocampus (Andersen et al., 2000; Sloviter and Lomo, 2012). Eventually, two main lines of interpretation of hippocampal function emerged, one considering the hippocampus as coordinating ongoing motor behavior, the other postulating hippocampo–neocortical interactions based on theoretical concepts.
The most straightforward interpretation was offered by Vanderwolf who discovered the septohippocampal theta rhythm (Vanderwolf, 1969) and perceived the hippocampus as a machinery adapting olfactory inputs to movement-related proprioception (Vanderwolf, 2001). Most animals rely on smell to set goals that must be either approached or avoided, and the hippocampus is continually balancing such approach–avoidance conflicts. While this notion was deemed too simplistic by many, one should note that even recent MRI studies in human hippocampi have visualized approach–avoidance conflicts not explainable by cognitive theories (O'Neil et al., 2015). Thus, reading a summary of Vanderwolf (2007) view might be instructive for students of behavioral neuroscience, including his (unsuccessful) cautioning against mixing empirical neuroscience with antique philosophical ideas. The second theory of the hippocampus as modulator of conflicting behavioral tendencies was established by Gray and McNaughton (1982) who argued that this primordial hippocampal function is overshadowing potential memory and cognitive processes, whose significance for ongoing adaptation of behavior must be demonstrated experimentally using the sophisticated toolbox of animal learning psychology (Davidson and Jarrard, 2004). Yet, as explained before, the way how ongoing behavior is shaped by “cognitive” structures is still not known, and many of the behavioral results reviewed and reported here still fit the views of Vanderwolf and Gray better than other theories based on hippocampo–neocortical interactions.
The theoretical foundations of the main “cognitive” explanations of hippocampal function can be tracked back to Marr (1971) and O'Keefe and Nadel (1978). In essence, they rely heavily on single-cell neurophysiological ideas and techniques. Marr's computational approach perceived the hippocampal circuitry as holding temporarily a reduced assembly of daily neocortical activity and feeding it back during the night for permanent memory storage (Willshaw et al., 2015). His ideas also promoted the view of the hippocampus as an auto-associative network digesting neocortical input by completing or separating patterns of hippocampal activity thought to result from recurrent collaterals of CA3 and CA1 neurons (Yassa and Stark, 2011). The second and conceptually most influential approach was the monumental effort of O'Keefe and Nadel (1978) to overturn earlier views of hippocampal functions by successfully creating the notion that the hippocampus was sort of a neuronal map handling pieces and bits of ongoing neocortical activity to create a virtual Euclidean space supporting memory and guiding future actions. The theory gained support from neurophysiological studies showing the presence of rodent “place cells” firing in given locations but needed lengthy and not always convincing argumentation to refute interpretation of hippocampal lesion studies in terms of behavioral inhibition. Likewise, the cognitive map theory as presented initially barely tried to explain deficits in species-typical behaviors such as hoarding or circadian activity, a problem recognized by Nadel himself (Nadel, 1995).
Rodent and human hippocampus: Similarities and differences
Macroscopically, rodents show smaller association cortices than more encephalized species, but the in-folded hippocampi represent still a substantial portion of the hemispheres (Figures 3A,B). This is in contrast to the human brain in which a dissected hippocampus has about the size of a thumb, weighs about 10 g (Patzke et al., 2014), and occupies only part of the temporal lobe. Morphologically, there are other clear differences. For one, the rodent hippocampus has extensive contralateral connections passing through the ventral hippocampal commissure, also known as associational/commissural pathway. In contrast, the human hippocampus proper has few commissural fibers. If any, they pass through the dorsal hippocampal commissure (the psalterium dorsale located below the corpus callosum) and play a minor role as judged by the volume of visible fibers. Thus, the human hippocampus appears to communicate chiefly with the ipsilateral hemisphere, being strongly lateralized itself as commissural interactions must take place via the commissural fibers of entorhinal and cingular cortex. Therefore, translational research in rodents must expect immediate contralateral responses to manipulations in one of the hippocampi that will be less or not observable in humans.
Species similarities in hippocampal architecture and hypothalamic projections
The general architecture of the hippocampus appears comparable in many species and is summarized in Figures 3C–F showing the well-known trisynaptic loop, labeled as “lamellar”, loop because it seems debatable how many stations that loop might show in different species (Witter et al., 2017). There is widespread agreement that the subiculum is the output gateway of the hippocampus. From there, neuronal activity is fed back to the entorhinal cortex, but many fibers leaving the hippocampal formation converge to the fornix and reach the anterior olfactory nuclei, the septal region, rostral portions of the hypothalamus, and fan out to the VMH to reach eventually the mammillary bodies and anterior (limbic) nuclei of the thalamus (Poletti and Creswell, 1977; Saunders and Aggleton, 2007). The trajectory of the postcommissural fornix is paralleled by the stria terminals pathway from the amygdala. Thus, the basal forebrain and hypothalamus are targeted in mammals by many efferent fibers from both hippocampus and amygdala, thought to exert downstream control over the motivational systems (Wang et al., 2021; Bang et al., 2022). This notion should be modified, however, as the septal nuclei and the hypothalamus contains many fibers that reach the hippocampus and limbic cortex parts upstreams (see Section A closer look at ascending hypothalamo–hippocampal connections).
Species differences in hippocampal inputs: Different neocortical partners
When comparing mice and humans, it is often overlooked that their neocortical input patterns to the hippocampal formation are different, while the connections between hypothalamus and hippocampus appear to be largely similar. Figure 4 shows a simplistic concept of information processing from neocortex to hippocampus. The salient feature is that the widespread activity patterns of the cortical columns must be reduced when approaching the entorhinal cortex by a process resembling digital JPG image reduction (Mulligan, 1997), from where they are fed into a stack of lamellar loops. This reduction process is governed by existing or newly formed synaptic tags (Frey and Morris, 1997; Redondo and Morris, 2011). In this view, the hippocampus receives a much-reduced copy of the neocortical activity pattern which can be fed back to the cortex but is also communicated to the basal forebrain and hypothalamus. Depending on the signals evoking tag activity, primitive memories can thus be stored even at the hypothalamic level (Krzywkowski et al., 2020). The concept is debatable, but it is obvious that the rodent hippocampus receives information that is mostly derived from the rather small associative cortex regions dealing with sensation and movements yet not cognition. In contrast, the human hippocampus receives inputs that were preprocessed through complex multimodal association cortex in the frontal, parietal, and temporal lobes, and is thus getting “cognitive” input. Yet, this view also implies that the observable output of “hippocampal” tests in rodents may not show the cognitive causation expected by the translational scientist.
A neglected feature for translational research: Mossy fiber distribution and behavior
The trait showing maximal species differences and being thus of potential importance for translational research is the mossy fiber distribution within CA3. In fact, individual and genetic variation of the mossy fiber projection originating from dentate granule cells correlates with a variety of rodent behaviors, specifically the extent of the intra/infrapyramidal projection (IIP-MF), synapsing on basal dendrites or pyramidal cell bodies in the CA3 region (Figure 3D), for reviews see the study by Lipp et al. (1989) and Lipp and Wolfer (1999). In brief, variations of IIP-MF responded to selective breeding for two-way avoidance and remained correlated with this behavior in various studies including mouse strains, individual mice, and after postnatal manipulation. Likewise, genetic studies showed that mice with small IIP-MF projections were exploring less (Crusio et al., 1989) and correlations with radial maze learning were frequent (Schwegler et al., 1991; Crusio and Schwegler, 2005). Later studies revealed correlations with other tasks sensitive to hippocampal lesions, such as water maze learning (Schöpke et al., 1991), but variations of the IIP-MF were also found to correlate with different non-cognitive tasks including strength and asymmetry of paw preference (Gruber et al., 1991) and latency to attack intruders (Guillot et al., 1994; Sluyter et al., 1994). These findings did not easily fit into cognitive theories of hippocampal function. Qualitatively, it appeared to us that the extent of the IIP-MF was correlated with predictability of ongoing behavior: mice with larger IIP-MF were less distracted by noises and external stimuli and thus more successful in rather complex tasks, while individuals or strains with small IIP-MF appeared to react stronger when facing distracting or fear-inducing stimuli. Nonetheless, as argued by Crusio and Schwegler (2005), the multitude of observed correlations strongly suggests a causal physiological effect of mossy fiber variations on behavior.
By (non-systematically) inspecting many species, we observed enormous variation of mossy fiber projections across species (Supplementary Figure S2) suggesting that the mossy fiber input was larger in predatory or encephalized species, appearing mostly dependent on the size of the dentate gyrus. Figure 5A compares the mossy fiber patterns in a human hippocampus with that of a mouse taken at a mid-septotemporal level (Figure 5C). In the human hippocampus, the mossy fibers pervade the pyramidal cell layer from the beginning till the end of CA3 (Figure 5B, while the pyramidal cells in mouse CA3 are only partially covered by mossy fiber boutons (Figure 5D). Given that simple measures of the IIP-MF projection had often correlated well with many behaviors of mice, one might infer that the massive mossy fiber input in the human CA3 region might reflect a much stronger control of the pyramidal cells by mossy fiber input. But explanations must remain speculative, while a translational approach might be feasible (see Section The hippocampus as a sensor of malaise and well-being).
Experimental natural selection of mossy fiber distributions and associate behaviors
The long series of mossy fiber studies abutting into more and more apparently mossy fiber-dependent behaviors led to the question whether variations of this trait were truly functional or became just visible by cherry-picking of standard behavioral tests. Therefore, four mouse strains with large or small IIP-MF projections were crossed systematically to obtain a founder population with equal proportions of genetic variance (a so-called diallel cross) and the F1-animals were transported to a Russian field station and released in two outdoor pens (Lipp and Wolfer, 1999, 2013) while a control population obtained by interbreeding the same founders was kept in a standard mouse facility. The results were instructive (Figure 6). After 2 years (5–6 generations of outdoor life), the mice living in the outdoor pens had developed significantly reduced IIP-MF projection and this persisted up to the fourth year. The shift in IIP-MF was not due to environmental factors, because mice from the three groups underwent embryo transfer and were maintained as ordinary mouse lines in the same animal facility in Switzerland. They also underwent a series of behavioral tests. One of them conducted in an automated home-cage testing system (Kiryk et al., 2020) proved instructive because the mice were tested without handling, and members of all three lines were together in an IntelliCage (Figure 6C). This study revealed that the naturally selected mice were reluctant in approaching a harmless novel object (a nut) placed in one of the corners while the non-selected mice approached it frequently but lost interest rapidly. However, after 24 h, the naturally selected mice were visiting the object more frequently, implying that natural selection had shifted a genetically dependent balance in exploratory tendencies, perhaps by altering a hypothalamic set-point (Figure 6D). A further test assessed the behavior of the mouse lines after the introduction of randomly occurring noise in the corners (generated by air-puffs), and delivery of an air-puff when a mouse had finally entered a corner. All mice hesitated before visiting a corner, but the naturally selected lines took 5 h more (Figure 6E). Re-entering a punished corner was dramatically delayed in these mice as well (Figure 6F). Thus, smaller IIP-MF on basal dendrites remained correlated with an altered response pattern in animals facing threatening or stressing situations as it was predicted by the very first studies focusing on IIP-MF variation and exploratory behavior (Crusio et al., 1989). It can be added here that small IIP-MF projections were most often correlated with superior two-way avoidance learning but poor performance in radial maze learning (Crusio and Schwegler, 2005), while apparently superior cognitive abilities of mice with larger IIP-MF projections, as judged by observing them in radial and water mazes, were less useful for survival under real-life conditions. Whatever the interpretation, the study proved that natural selection could act surprisingly fast in changing hippocampal circuitry and behavior, and that variation of the IIP-MF appeared to be truly associated with meaningful variations of mouse behavior outdoors and indoors.
Anthropomorphizing mice entails logical fallacies and wrong conclusions
A major cognitive problem pervading translational approaches is illogical shortcuts, shown graphically in Figures 7A,B. The main point is that functional deficits in motivational systems or cognitive processing as observed in humans are extrapolated to mice. The social part of the problem is that behavioral testing in a translational context is often done collaboratively, specifically for genetically modified mice. Molecular biologists wishing to have their mice tested for translational potential are often dissatisfied with complex explanations of behavior. Tests such as fear conditioning or water maze learning offer much face validity because most lay people quickly grasp that a probe test in the water maze proves the existence of spatial memory or freezing in a chamber of previous shock experience a memory engram. However, they are mostly unaware that these measures are generally quantitative and verified by statistics only. But memory is not easily quantified in behavioral tests and performance variation supposed to reflect memory or lack thereof is often based on coping strategies or species-specific priorities of situation-dependent action as explained below.
Ethological priorities of mice: Stop or go!
In classifying behavior of mice and men, it is often useful to distinguish between behavioral traits used in daily or monthly life from these important during a lifetime or evolution. Figure 8 shows a somewhat arbitrary hierarchy of behavioral processes potentially influenced by rather minor experimental interference, such as drugs or many targeted mutations. Most important for the daily life of mice is a very rapid decision to freeze or rush when dealing with predators or aggressive conspecifics (Kondrakiewicz et al., 2019). Large animals and humans have the freedom of mulling when facing conflicting cues and they may choose to remain passive or active, revising the decision if required. As mice face rapid predators or conspecifics, they are likely to enter immediately a state of immobility or rushing. Humans are inclined to believe that such behavior is caused by or at least associated with an emotion, chiefly fear and anxiety. But it could also happen reflexively without emotions that may form later. This ethological antagonism facilitates the detection of comparatively minor experimental effects in many brain systems not necessarily related to memory or fear. It seems reasonable to assume that these antagonistic innate behavioral responses are under some form of inhibitory control, probably by tonically active GABA-ergic axons from the reticular part of the substantia nigra. A sudden rush is easier initiated by disinhibition of structures such as the mesencephalic locomotor region (Esposito and Arber, 2016) than by building up increasing pressure to move. Likewise, behavioral arrest can be produced rapidly by GABAergic systems. This constellation may hinder translational research, specifically testing of drugs aimed at reducing anxiety and improving mood. These may result in lowering anxiety of mice, but if they interfere with GABAergic mechanisms, some results may be unexpected. In humans, paradoxical effects of diazepam such as hyperactivity, hostility, and aggression have been observed, but the incidence appears to be low (Paton, 2002). The situation may be different in mice. For example, keeping adult male mice isolated for more than 4 weeks alters the subunit expression of GABA type A receptors (GABAA-R) and increases their locomotor activity following exposure to diazepam or imidazenil (Pinna et al., 2006). Since the pharmaceutic industry has a preference to work with male mice and these often need separate housing, other types of drugs supposed to act via GABA receptors may elicit no or paradoxical responses as well. This might at least partially explain why 17 out of 19 mouse tests designed to measure anxiolytic effects were found to be highly unreliable (Rosso et al., 2021).
Ethological priorities of mice: Moving alone or together?
The second-most important behavior in the daily life of a mouse is foraging and feeding, driven by energetic needs. However, it also means that mice have a strong drive for moving. This may conflict with measures of memory assessing the duration of freezing after shock-exposure: the assumption that mice that start moving in the shock-box have a poor memory might simply reflect a stronger intention to get out of it. But there is little doubt that mice like to move. When given a running wheel, they may run some kilometers per day, although there are large interindividual differences even among inbred mice. But their penchant for moving poses a problem in rewarded learning tasks (e.g., radial mazes) as they seem not much concerned by moving as long as there is a partial reward. They also tend to use win-stay strategies by visiting repeatedly arms with consumed bait, while rats, crows, and juvenile rabbits rely on win-shift strategies (never visit a baited arm again) as shown by a study using a giant outdoor radial maze (Lipp et al., 2001). Unsurprisingly, hedgehogs and chickens, species relying on random walk strategies for foraging, were poor radial maze learners while guinea pigs remained immobile and could not be tested.
Social interactions during a day occur less frequently. Rodents often huddle together to save energy or to maintain social contact. A less known feature is that foraging in mice often occurs in social groups. Studies with transponder-tagged female mice in an outdoor pen with eight feeder boxes emulating a radial maze situation have shown that the animals established group territories, each group visiting only half of the boxes, thus thwarting the expectations of the experimenters (Dell'Omo et al., 2000).
While these three behavioral classes are dominating the daily life of mice, cognitive abilities such as spatial or olfactory memory probably play a minor role because the environment and smells of a mouse colony do not change quickly. Such abilities are doubtlessly important for long-term survival and reproduction, but in mice they have not be tuned for rapid adaptation and behavioral flexibility as required for predators.
Bothersome unpredictable results in neurobehavioral brain research
Mechanistic thinking in the clockwork sense is still deeply rooted in brain research, specifically in molecular and genetic fields and synapse physiology. They are mostly appropriate there, but much less so in the domains of neuropsychology and behavioral brain research investigating behavior only. Leaving brain imaging and large-scale electrophysiological mapping aside, there seems to be a strong penchant to a mental short-cut in experimenters: they anticipate that their interaction with a given brain system results in a behavioral outcome related directly to the target structure (Figure 7C). One can suspect that most observers know that the observed behavioral reaction is a response either from the remaining brain, remaining genome, or being caused by compensatory homeostatic processes (Figure 7D) but that knowledge is rarely communicated. Part of it is simply peer pressure arising from cooperating molecular biologists or clinicians: they do not like to learn that their efforts might be marred or blurred by unpredictable processes. The usual excuse of the behavioral specialist is then mostly statistical. There must be a specific mechanistic effect but the tools for measuring are not good enough so that unpleasant variability is evident. Essentially, this is an engineer's view in which the terms standard deviation and standard error reflect imprecision of measurement. But the possibility of truly stochastic (statistically unpredictable) processes masking phenotypes after whatever treatment should not be ruled out. Therefore, this section deals with one example of unpredictable events following gene deletions.
Figure 9 shows a re-analysis of earlier data obtained by characterizing electrophysiologically and behaviorally mice lacking the gene for tissue plasminogen activator, tPA (Huang et al., 1996). The mice had been tested by us in a standard water maze procedure, and the same animals subsequently for two-way avoidance (shuttlebox) learning (Figures 9A–D), while long-term potentiation (LTP) in hippocampal slices was investigated in the United States. Given that, we observed significant group differences in both tests, we had assumed that the tPA deficiency was affecting a common underlying factor, possibly LTP, accounting for the behavioral phenotypes. However, when presenting the results as individual data scores (Figures 9A,B) we were asked why half of the knockout mice were showing similar scores as the controls and why the “outliers” were apparently responsible for a significant behavioral phenotype. Having no good answer, we then analyzed correlations between the individual data scores. If there would be a common underlying factor causing covariance of the scores in the two tests, the worst shuttlebox learners should also be the worst watermaze learners. This assumption held only for the controls that showed a moderate correlation between the scores (Figure 9E) but not at all for the knockout mice in which we found a zero correlation (Figure 9F). This implied that the tPA knockouts were compensating problems in the two tasks in different yet unpredictable ways and that the overlap between controls and knockouts showed that a substantial fraction of the tPA mutants were apparently able to compensate their deficit fully. Since then, all studies conducted by our lab always checked for such interactions.
However, the problem of spurious correlations between endophenotypes and behavior can be found in many studies claiming to find correlations without checking individual correlations. For example, there have been numerous studies linking experimentally manipulated rates of hippocampal neurogenesis with parallel behavioral changes, but out of some 900 studies, only eleven studies were measuring both the degree of adult neurogenesis and the behavioral scores in the same animals (Lazic et al., 2014). Thus, many claims of functional correlates between hippocampal neurogenesis and cognitive abilities might reflect spurious correlations (Lipp and Bonfanti, 2016).
Back to hippocampus: Behavioral lesion effects not predicted by current theories of hippocampal function in mice
The following sections deal with the effects of complete bilateral and excitotoxic hippocampal lesions on mouse behavior. Obviously, complete removal of the hippocampus is a crude approach for assessing its role, but the seminal water maze experiment of Morris et al. (1982) was crucial in implementing the belief that also the rat hippocampus was necessary for cognitive performance, such as spatial mapping. Therefore, we initiated a series of studies to document the effects of complete hippocampal lesions in mice tested in the laboratory and under naturalistic conditions. For both sets of (largely unpublished) experiments, the hippocampi of mice were destroyed bilaterally by infusion of excitotoxic substances (Deacon et al., 2002), and mice with sham operations or removed hippocampi were observed over prolonged periods.
Acute and chronic effects of hippocampal lesions in standardized water maze testing
Figure 10 summarizes these studies. The standardized water maze protocol included 3 days of learning to find a hidden submerged platform, each mouse having six trials of 120 s duration per day to assess improvement within and across days. At the beginning of the fourth day, the platform position was reversed, and this trial was taken as a probe trial for measuring approach preferences to the former platform position. However, the trials were continued for further 11 trials to assess navigational flexibility. Ten days after the operation, the controls showed rapid learning but the lesioned ones showed no acquisition, reflecting excessive thigmotaxis in an unknown environment. The probe trial showed clear spatial preference scores in the controls while the lesioned ones performed at chance levels. Various samples of mice including a total of 45 mice were tested 2–8 months after the lesion. There was again a significant lesion effect in learning: the controls were faster in learning, but now the lesioned mice also showed a clear learning curve, the difference due to stronger thigmotaxis (wall-hugging). Interestingly, the probe trial showed significant spatial preference scores in both groups, indicating that hippocampectomized mice could remember well the platform position but were unable to learn a new one. Thus, the long-term effect of removing large segments of the mouse brain was relatively subtle, a loss in behavioral flexibility in water maze learning. This insight was not new as it was shown that partial and selective recovery of navigational learning could occur after retrohippocampal lesions (Schenk and Morris, 1985), but this recovery was barely noticed by the neuroscience community. Likewise, studies showing that rats with complete hippocampal lesions could use alternative yet less-efficient systems for learning contextual fear (Wiltgen et al., 2006) did not overcome the dominant narrative of the hippocampus as the main substrate for memory in humans and animals.
Hippocampal lesions, species-specific behavior, and circadian activities outdoors and in the laboratory: Some own data and explanatory hypotheses
Surprising findings
Inspired by the development of tests assessing species-specific behavior following hippocampal lesions in mice (Deacon et al., 2002; Deacon and Rawlins, 2005), outdoor studies in a Russian field station were initiated to assess known and unknown lesion effects. Most of the data are not published yet. In one study, after about 20 days of postlesional recovery time, 40 female mice (20 hippocampal, 20 sham controls) were shipped to Russia and brought to the field station. They tolerated the trip surprisingly well and underwent adaptation procedures to one-way gates enabling them to pass later through gates leading into computer-controlled feeder boxes placed in outdoor pens. They were all placed together in a circular arena that contained two mouse cages with attached one-way gates (Figure 11A). The cages were removed after 20 mice had entered them, and the group identity was verified by transponder reading. Figure 11B shows the results of this superbly fast collective test of hippocampal malfunction: among the first batch entering the cages, there were only three with hippocampal lesions. The gates were then modified by adding a plastic diaphragm. Because it took more time for all mice to enter the cages, the cut-off point was set to 15 mice, out of which there were only two lesioned mice. Overnight testing of mice using the Deacons pellet removal test (Figures 11C–F) confirmed the sensitivity of this simple test in revealing hippocampal lesions (Figure 11G).
Despite the precaution in pre-adapting the mice, about half of the lesioned ones died within 1–2 days after transfer to the pen (Figure 12A) for unknown reasons. The others adapted and the losses of them or of the control mice remained minimal over a period of 45 days. Taken together, the behavior of the lesioned mice was characterized by poorly structured nocturnal activity and by repeated visiting of the same box even though they would no longer receive food. Figure 12C shows an observed nocturnal activity pattern of mice in an outdoor pen: a marked activity peak at the begin of darkness, and a less-pronounced second peak in the mid of the night. In contrast, the hippocampally lesioned mice became very active soon and maintained a high activity level throughout the night (Figure 12D). The observed deficit in activity timing was analyzed later in more detail in the laboratory by using IntelliCages® housing female mice with sham lesions, prefrontal, and hippocampal lesions together. Their lesions were chronic, and they lived in these home cages for prolonged periods (Voikar et al., 2018). To check their timing abilities, they had an external zeitgeber by delivering water during corner visits for 1 h in the night only. Figures 12E,F shows that the controls and the prefrontally lesioned mice kept relatively low activity at the begin of the night but increased it by visiting corners and nose-poking for water specifically in the hour before water delivery. In contrast, the hippocampus-lesioned mice started with high activity levels that became reinforced before drinking time (probably because of the behavior of their cage mates). Thereafter, all mice became rapidly inactive.
How to interpret such observations?
One factor is the huge behavioral variability observed in the hippocampally lesioned mice, specifically in complex situations, such as water maze and IntelliCage®. Partially, this may reflect incomplete lesions as there was no possibility to check the lesion size in the field station. However, most lesions were done by experienced persons, so this factor might not be decisive. Rather, it appears that the variation in many behavioral scores reflects a genuine inability to stabilize behavior in complex situations, so that a variability index as used in stock markets might be a better descriptor. On the other hand, the predictability of behavioral changes following lesions in seemingly non-cognitive species-typical behaviors is astounding and may indicate the involvement of hippocampus-dependent processes evading explanations in terms of experimental neuropsychology. For example, the unwillingness of lesioned mice to enter a one-way gate may reflect vibrissal hypersensitivity, a fear reaction, or something else. In any case, it seems to be a reliable and predictable trait that has been described in detail (Deacon and Rawlins, 2005). It appears that passing narrow doors may have a significance for mice that eludes the human observers.
Nobody knows what drives a normal C57BL/6 mouse to clean frantically a tube from food pellets or other kinds of pebbles, while a lesioned mouse of the same strain is not handicapped in sensorimotor abilities but unresponsive to the situation. Possibly, such hippocampal deficits in natural behaviors reflect a broken interaction between an ethological releasing stimulus and a corresponding species-specific behavioral pattern. But this (old-fashioned) view would imply that the interplay of the hippocampus and the basal forebrain structures (inclusive hypothalamus) had evolved under the demands of the basal forebrain to have computational extensions.
Primordial functions of the hypothalamo–hippocampal loops?
Evolutionarily, it is conceivable that the hippocampal formation has developed as an associative structure straddling hypothalamus and basal forebrain and was then marginalized by the development of the neocortex and its direct sensory inputs as visualized in Figures 13A,B. Given the enormous density of intrahypothalamic connections needed to orchestrate physiology and behavior, it appears that during evolution the hypothalami of different vertebrate lines have added various types of gray matter to deal with sensory and proprioceptive information. In fishes, a lateral hypothalamic lobe is handling multisensory inputs and behavior (Schmidt, 2020), while in mammals, the rostral end of the telencephalon formed a vesicle whose walls formed a monolayered archicortex (the primordial limbic system). Its rostral parts (becoming prefrontal and entorhinal cortex) were handling olfactory input, its caudal parts (becoming the amygdaloid complex) were receiving chemical and auditory inputs, while the central portions (becoming the later hippocampus) had to balance these various inputs with proprioceptive (movement-related) signals generated by the SMS. However, in simplistic terms, this would imply that the hippocampus and its connected cortical structures would represent a kind of fine-tuning “slave processor” of basal forebrain and hypothalamus (Figures 13C–F), like the cerebellum for the motor system. This hypothesis conflicts with the usual view of the hypothalamus as downstream recipient of cognitive hippocampal processes and would require more upstream channels than usually assumed. The following section will thus focus on the hypothalamic projections to the hippocampus and limbic structures yet omitting upstream fibers in the septo-hippocampal system (Niewiadomska et al., 2009) and in the pre-commissural fornix (fibers rostral of the anterior commissure).
A closer look at ascending hypothalamo–hippocampal connections
The presence of hypothalamic fibers ascending directly to the hippocampus has been repeatedly reported for squirrel and rhesus monkeys (DeVito and White, 1966; Poletti and Creswell, 1977; Senova et al., 2020), because it is relatively easy to cut or inject the fornix in primates as it is located under the corpus callosum. Labeling all fibers leaving or entering the rodent hippocampus through the fornix is more difficult because it would require filling the entire structure with neuronal tracer. Partial injections of horseradish peroxidase (HRP) into the hippocampus had revealed rather small numbers of retrogradely labeled neurons, chiefly in the dorsal supramammillary nucleus, SUM (Pasquier and Reinososuarez, 1978). Further studies of this region have shown that it is part of a widespread reciprocally connected network involving hippocampus and limbic cortices that appear topographically mapped into the hypothalamus (Pan and McNaughton, 2004). The SUM region is comparatively small but an old pilot study (Lipp and Nauta, 1984) had shown, by labeling the fimbria stump after suction of all overlying structures, that this projection appears massive (Figure 14 and legend for details). Preliminary quantification revealed about 20'000 ipsilateral labeled neurons that were sending ascending fibers to the hippocampus (Figure 14B). Given that the postcommissural fornix of rats contains some 40 to 50'000 fibers (Powell et al., 1957), this would imply that nearly half of the postcommissural fornix fibers are ascending from the SUM. Their endings in the hippocampus could not be determined with certainty (Figure 14A) but recent studies have shown that the area projects monosynaptically to the dentate gyrus, potentiating its output (Hashimotodani et al., 2018) and is possibly sending novelty signals (Chen et al., 2020). One may note that the dorsal SUM belongs to a hypothalamic cell group controlling central gray structures modulating species-specific defense behavior both to innate and conditioned threats (Wang et al., 2021; Bang et al., 2022) and so the concomitant alerting of the hippocampus by activation these nuclei is not surprising. Another candidate structure for showing links between hippocampus and species-specific behaviors is the VMH, whose electrical stimulation evokes aggressive-defensive behavior in many species (see The “old” brain and its connections). Recent studies have even identified “territorial” place cells in the mouse VMH, possibly established via a hippocampal loop (Krzywkowski et al., 2020).
An exclusively hypothalamic “powerhouse” is formed by the orexinergic neurons innervating diffusively the entire brain and co-regulating sleep–wakefulness, vigilance, feeding, energy, and neuroendocrine homeostasis, yet also partially motor behavior and reward mechanisms. Many of these fibers ascend through fornix and stria terminalis (Zhang et al., 2013). A similar cluster of activating histaminergic neurons with widespread efferent axons is the tuberomammillary nucleus (Blandina et al., 2012). Both nuclei are generally viewed as relay structures used by various other systems to tune specific brain functions, but the idea that they are a main tool of the hypothalamus to orchestrate the brain according to basic needs and motivation seems to be rare. A telling example is the role of the massive projection from the mammillary bodies (mammillo-thalamic tract, MTT, Figure 14B) to the anterior thalamus (AT), itself a hub innervating limbic cortex and the subiculum whose destruction causes cognitive, emotional, and memory deficiencies (Witter et al., 2017). Since the subiculum sends fiber through the fornix reaching the AT directly or indirectly through the mammillary bodies and the MTT, this so-called Papez loop is mostly viewed as a control system of the hippocampus by which navigational and memory-related information is passed through the hypothalamus for integrating basic motivational signals that will change the thalamo-cortical activation patterns of the AT (Aggleton et al., 2022). But the reverse view, namely that mammillary bodies send commands to the AT and limbic system, using the hippocampus for feedback information through the fornix, is apparently not much discussed, even though the mammillary bodies are known to drive head-direction cells and theta rhythms.
Taken together, an analysis of hypothalamic upstream connections strongly implies that the basic drivers controlling “higher-order” brain functions, including memory, motivations, emotions, and goal-directed behavior, are located along the longitudinal axis of the basal forebrain and interconnected by the MBF and DLF (see Figures 2, 13). These systems correct ongoing behavior by crude upstream signals that are then refined by cognitive and motor processing in associative cortices and hippocampus and fed back in condensed projections to these subcortical structures. Three simple arguments support such a view. (i) Evolutionarily, the basal forebrain structures appeared first. (ii) Destroying basal forebrain structures has mostly detrimental consequences for behavioral control, while loss of hippocampus and associative neocortex can be compensated over time, and (iii) the neuroanatomical analysis of cortico-subcortical loops shows that the focal control or set-points are subcortical.
Obviously, the bidirectional connections of the hippocampus with the BFS (together with the amygdala inputs and loops) are of special interest to translational research, specifically in the domain of deep brain stimulation (Senova et al., 2020), but the local density of subcortical target structures will pose problems due to co-stimulation of other loops.
Viewing cognition through the lens of the motor system and hypothalamus
The conventional scenario
The neocortex is traditionally viewed as the superior structure guiding behavior, a belief formulated in detail more than 200 years ago (Gall, 1822). For example, a widespread notion holds that the visual cortex perceives an interesting object and transmits somehow his discovery to the navigational brain parts, including hippocampus, that in turn tells the prefrontal cortex to make decisions and to take action by sending information to the midbrain and spinal cord instructing them what to do. The execution is then thought to be a largely automated process by the two control loops refining motor actions originating in the motor cortex as shown in Figure 2. In addition, the hemispheres appear to contain circuits labeled “social brain” or “visceral brain” or “computational brain” to name a few. Their common denominator is a top-to-bottom view as in Figures 2, 4, the various cortical systems busy in executing specific cognitive tasks pushing the motor system to act. Here, we present another perspective that appears helpful (at least to us) and might even resolve old riddles in the field.
Mice think with their feet–the neocortex as a (dispensable) slave processor of the motor system
As formulated by one of the authors (D.P.W.) “mice think with their feet”. In analogy, there seem also many humans capable of thinking only while talking, as evidenced by countless hours spent in meetings with people arriving ill-prepared and grasping the topics for discussion only while speaking (Nauta W.H.J., oral communication, 1983). Thus, it appears that the activity of the neocortex is primed markedly by ongoing neuronal activity in midbrain and the basal forebrain. In computer terms, a small CPU can be equipped with two big graphic cards permitting complex computer games executed by simple inputs from a keyboard or a game console, ending with simple final output, “you lost”, so why not. The idea of the neocortex as a subordinate processor fed by mesencephalon, hypothalamus, and basal forebrain nuclei is not new, but apart from its logical appeal, it has received limited support from neuroscience. This is changing now by the advent of visualizing whole brain activity in various species (Kaplan and Zimmer, 2020) that seem to convey a similar message: ongoing motor activity is priming the activity of even sensory brain parts in different species, such as flies (Aimon et al., 2019), mice (Stringer et al., 2019; Parker et al., 2020), nematodes (Kaplan et al., 2020), and lampreys (Grillner et al., 2008).
Turning the brain upside down
By turning Figure 4 by 180°, Figure 15 shows the same components, but now the hierarchy is topped by the midbrain motor system (SMS), superior colliculus, hypothalamus, and the nuclei of the basal forebrain, forming together the very old core complex governing behavior in so many species. Simplistically, it illustrates that the information flow from midbrain reaches sensory areas through non-specific thalamic nuclei, while the hippocampus is now suddenly a structure that primarily receives patterned motivational information from the hypothalamus and sends back simple correcting signals, such as position and movement status, not unlike the cerebellum. In theory, it could be possible that the hypothalamus and basal forebrain are mapped topographically into the hippocampus, most rostrally the olfactory nuclei. But in parallel, the hippocampal loops also communicate with the entorhinal cortex from which the patterns are distributed tangentially to form increasingly complex configurations, perhaps generating complex memories, visions, or dreams, at least in humans. However, the visions will be structured by the parallel input from the SMS through non-specific thalamic nuclei ensuring that the patterns will develop along and align with motor signals generated in the midbrain. Three interesting points emerge from this concept.
1) The pyramidal tract in the human brain (Figure 15A) appears as a feedback loop rather than a commander. The relevant areas governing behavior and cognition, the phylogenetically old brain, are framed in red: midbrain motor areas, the basal forebrain nuclei, the rostral reticular formation, and the limbic and associative thalamus. The neocortex remains as a neuronal playground for free-floating patterns of excitation and various structured sensory inputs yet are much smaller in rodents (Figure 15B). An inhibitory clutch in the midbrain blocks unwanted selection of motor channels (Fraigne et al., 2015) in both species even though the “old brain” is feeding in motivational and ongoing motor activity patterns. The latter may be virtual while dreaming.
2) Downstream from thalamus, much of the neocortex is dispensable, at least for daily life and after some adaptation. The phylogenetically old brain structures are capable of learning, as shown for example by a rarely cited study by Huston and Borbely (1974) demonstrating that “thalamic” rats (with most forebrain structures removed) can be conditioned by rewarding hypothalamic stimulation when they adopted a particular posture. But there was no extinction of the response and they had to be primed by rewarding stimulation to adopt another one—hinting at a neocortical role of information selection and suppression. More recent work has shown that a two-year-old rat with massive hydrocephalus performed decently on a battery of behavioral tests, including spatial learning (Ferris et al., 2019). Later neuroanatomical analysis showed that it had a malformed hippocampus that was compressed into the lower hindbrain, together with hypothalamus, midbrain, and pons. Likewise, mice without cortex and hippocampus master many behavioral tasks, including predator avoidance (Turan, 2021).
3) Finally, it is well known but generally blanked out (or hotly disputed) by psychologists (Branwen, 2021) that there are recurrent observations of people living well without a cortex visible on brain scans (Lewin, 1980; Feuillet et al., 2007; Persad et al., 2021). The general cause is juvenile hydrocephalus wherein the growth of the ventricles is flattening and eventually suppressing the cortex and its fiber tracts. But what remains appear to be sufficient for enabling an existence as white-collar worker and family father in France (Feuillet et al., 2007), albeit handicapped by an IQ of 75, which, however, is still within the normal range and shared by some 12% of contemporaries with an intact brain. Notably, at the age of 44, his verbal IQ was 84, perhaps disguising his performance IQ of 70.
Obviously, mammals without a cortex and hippocampus can master their daily routines, but in the long run, they will be victims of natural selection. Whether the costly evolutionary addition of gray matter and comparatively more long fiber tracts in the human brain accounts for an average increase of 25% IQ is an intriguing pending question. For translational research, however, focusing on simple daily life and the minimal cerebral structures supporting it should be a guiding principle.
Concluding perspectives
Simple or complex tasks?
The main message of this article is to keep things simple (“mice think with their feet”) and to avoid explanations of rodent behavioral data in terms of human psychological concepts or theories. Given that the endpoint of any animal behavioral test is simple movements, translational researchers are on the safe side when opting for simple tests involving species-specific features. Usually, such tests are learned far more rapidly than observed in standard conditioning procedures (Rosenberg et al., 2021; Meister, 2022). Whenever possible, an initial focus on impaired naturalistic behaviors or even simple motor acts is justified, because restoring them by treatments will be closer to the causal roots. For example, if a drug aimed at curing hippocampal functions can re-establish nest-building and burrowing (Jirkof, 2014), it is translationally more valuable than a drug that reinstates water maze learning, because the latter is sensitive to many confounding mechanisms. Therefore, the treatment might cure (as so often in medicine) symptoms rather than causes. In any case, checking naturalistic behaviors is more efficient and faster than, for example, testing mice for visual pattern discrimination (Dickson and Mittleman, 2022).
Conversely, if the expected behavioral profile resulting from a mutation or a candidate drug is not clear, it helps to unravel the presence of deficits by subjecting the animals to tests limiting the degree of freedom in displaying behaviors, for example, avoidance learning, or to conduct forced tests like the water maze that challenge many brain systems beyond the hippocampus. An additional dissection of the symptoms is then mandatory, however. Whenever possible, animals should undergo different tests permitting analysis of individual scores.
A particular problem is the development of new tests and systems supposed to improve analysis. Few tests were accepted so rapidly as the water maze test already after its first presentation by Morris et al. (1982). Generally, new variants, procedures, and apparatus face problems: one is slow acceptance because established experience and data collections are jeopardized, especially in the pharmaceutical industry. Another one is the unwillingness of researchers to cross-validate their findings with other apparatus or protocols. A last and thorny one is standardization of tests, partly because of disagreement which ones to include or abandon, partly because the value of standardization itself is questioned (Würbel, 2002; Richter et al., 2009; Völkl et al., 2021). From the data presented here, it seems clear that standardization is overvalued and should not be sought in translational research. The lab of the authors has tested several thousand mice in highly standardized procedures, such as water maze learning, but the results obtained in totally non-standardized semi-naturalistic environments were identical, at least for hippocampal lesions and genetic malfunction, namely severe problems in reversal learning.
Working with other species?
Inclusion of other species for behavioral testing can provide useful insights, for example, how wild species behave in the water maze task (Pleskacheva et al., 2000). After all, rats and mice represent only two cases out of about 5000 rodent species, and mice themselves are special because of their immediate motor reactions to threats. In terms of behavioral testing, this might require additional efforts, however. Yet, in some species such as dogs and primates, plain observation of social interactions and depressive behavior might provide important insights. For example, studies in dogs have shown that interconnected parts of amygdala and hypothalamus might form two tonically active yet antagonistic loop systems regulating socio-positive and socio-negative emotions and behaviors whose balance can be regulated by lesions of different amygdala structures (Fonberg, 1973). Such patterns can be easily observed in dogs but are difficult to recognize in rodents. Likewise, Patas monkeys (Erythrocebus patas) are gregarious during the daytime but spend the night on separate trees whenever possible (Hall, 1966; Gron, 2006). Such circadian oscillation between socio-positive and socio-negative behavior might deserve telemetric investigation of electrical activity in amygdala and hypothalamus.
Which brain systems to investigate?
Translational behavioral analysis should not confound system properties of networks with physiological properties of neurons
Given our obvious penchant toward neuroethological and observational approaches, we recommend checking a review by Vanderwolf and Cain (1994). It disentangles the neurobiological meanings of memory and learning, arguing that research in learning and memory should be pursued by biological studies of animal behavior, combined with a cellular/molecular approach in neuronal function yet without equating the two approaches. The last two sections are of special interest to translational behavioral research: (1) learning in real-life situations is likely to involve the participation of multiple brain structures and their connectivity. Thus, investigations should not single out structures of actual interest, and behavioral assessment should not only use conventional maze learning and sensory discrimination tasks, but also natural behaviors related to parental care, dominance, or territoriality. (2) The translational study of neurodegenerative diseases such as Alzheimer's disease (AD) appears heavily biased by studying hippocampal memory deficits yet paying less attention to socially and clinically more relevant behavioral changes, such as irritability, phases of locomotor hyperactivity, and showing dangerous behaviors. We agree that such commonalities are likely to be shared by mice and men and should move into the focus of neurobehavioral translational research but suggest being open for new hypotheses.
Neocortex or subcortical structures?
In terms of specific brain systems for translational research in the domain of psychiatry and neuropsychology, concentrating on phylogenetically old systems such as the hypothalamus appear appropriate when emotions or depression are involved. The wealth of data from hypothalamic stimulation in different species, including rats and mice, suggests that this brain region might be a preferable target for deep brain stimulation (DBS) involving fornix and amygdala projections (Senova et al., 2020), and possibly also vagal afferent and efferent fibers (Breit et al., 2018). It should be considered, however, that the mode of action of DBS for the treatment of depressions is poorly known (Ramasubbu et al., 2018). Given that the well-known improving effects of DBS in Parkinson's patients can also be obtained by MRI-guided ultrasound surgery (Gallay et al., 2018), one might consider whether a lesion approach in laboratory animals might be a cheaper strategy for studying and evaluating psychosurgery.
If the problems appear to be autism or cortical malfunction, the theoretically most efficient focus would be the inhibitory systems of the thalamus that tune ascending channels to the cortex. However, they are not easily targeted. The approach by Simmons et al. (2021) to focus on simple sensory channels for discovering intrathalamic malfunction could thus be helpful.
The hippocampus as a sensor of malaise and wellbeing?
While the role of the hippocampus in regulating the stress axis through mineralo- and glucocorticoid receptors is well established, it should also be considered that the hippocampal formation harbors the highest density of some 250 murine endocrine receptors for blood-borne ligands, and that these receptors show a spatially segregated expression in the hippocampal formation (Lathe, 2001; Lathe et al., 2020). These findings hint at hitherto unrecognized organizing functions of the hippocampus that could be of relevance to translational research. To our knowledge, it is poorly understood why being sick shuts down activity levels permitting better recovery but also reducing cognitive abilities. Lathe's hypothesis implies that an evolutionarily primeval chemosensory function of the hippocampus was enteroceptive, permitting this structure to coordinate body physiology, hormone levels, emotions, and synaptic memory processes in spatially separated channels.
A more concrete translational approach might be malfunctions of the hippocampal mossy fiber system (see section A neglected feature for translational research: mossy fiber distribution and behavior). Pathological sprouting within the mossy fiber distribution was observed after castration in rats (Skucas et al., 2013; Scharfman and MacLusky, 2014; Mendell et al., 2017). It might also occur after prostatectomy in men because this is often followed by “chemical castration” such as androgen deprivation therapy (ADT) lowering testosterone levels for inhibiting the growth of cancer cells. Interestingly, the human hippocampus shows about the same density of androgen receptor mRNA as the prostate tissue itself (Beyenburg et al., 2000), raising the question of whether ADT in men might also entail mossy fiber sprouting and hippocampal malfunction. Problems with short-term memory are frequent after ADT (Alibhai et al., 2010; Kluger et al., 2020). For example, a patient intends to go to the garage but ends up in front of the refrigerator having forgotten that he was hungry. In the scenario given in Figure 14, the initial impulse would originate in the hypothalamus, then being fed to the (impaired) hippocampus for propagation to the limbic cortex but will not be integrated with the ongoing motor activity, faking a cognitive short-term memory problem.
Classic behavioral test batteries or home-cage tests?
For translational research, assessment of behavior in animals, TV-controlled and electronic surveillance by implanted RFID chips in home cages is preferable as this fits the ARRIVE guidelines for sustainable research data much better (du Sert et al., 2020), while the use of large test batteries, complex operant conditioning or complex mazes would seem less important because they lower reproducibility of behavioral studies—a most important point for acceptance in science (Ioannidis, 2014). This holds specifically for mice. For one, an anxiety-inducing experimenter effect is always present (Nigri et al., 2022), unless the mice climb on the hand of the scientist to undergo testing willingly. The second reason is that systems such as Phenotyper® or IntelliCage® permit monitoring behavior over days or weeks, and free humans for other tasks. In doing so, they can detect many mutations or dysfunctions just by patterns of spontaneous activity (Dell'Omo et al., 2002; Vannoni et al., 2014) and can measure effortlessly behavioral flexibility and even rule-learning (Endo et al., 2011). Another ethologically oriented multi-individual home-cage system is the EcoHab (Puscian et al., 2016), which is particularly well-suited to study automatically social preferences and olfactory discrimination of familiar and unfamiliar conspecifics—behaviors essential for studying autism-like traits in mice.
Lastly, focusing on simple behaviors and responses will also facilitate the use of machine learning recognizing behavior patterns eluding human observation. However, this should occur in varied environments to avoid machine-learning of apparatus- or situation-specific properties and must be cross-validated. Admittedly, this perspective is intellectually less challenging than traditional behavioral research, but translational approaches must focus on a given human problem regardless of the preferences of the investigator.
Author's note
Translational research in behavioral and psychiatric neuroscience faces difficult problems. While neurobehavioral scientists in basic research are free to use species, concepts, methods, and approaches, translational scientists are constrained as they must read and interpret animal movements in terms of human psychological processes. This article identifies and illustrates practical problems, different theoretical views, and hidden pitfalls in interpreting animal behavior in human psychological terms. The main translational problems are inadequate knowledge in comparative neuroanatomy and ecology, anthropomorphizing animals (especially mice), and conceptual flaws in interpreting behavioral changes after treatments. This article suggests a neuroethological approach emphasizing that neocortical selection of observable motor patterns is primed primarily by ongoing activity of both hypothalamus and the midbrain supraspinal motor system. In practice, this means that translational research should focus on analysis and opportunities for intervention in these structures, assessing behavioral changes by complementary automated supervision and machine learning.
Author contributions
H-PL wrote the major review part. DW contributed graphs and statistical analysis. Both authors have contributed equally to many of the reviewed publications.
Funding
The review part was supported by intramural funds of the University of Zürich to H-PL (R-42900-01-01) while the unpublished data parts were obtained with the support of the following grants: Swiss National Science Foundation SNFS 037497 (Genetical variability of brain and behavior in near-natural settings), SNFS 046691 Brain, behavior and ecology: experimental natural selection of brain traits, and SNFS 057139 Micro- and macro-evolution of the hippocampal mossy fiber system to H-PL and DW. Further support was provided by SNFS 054184 (The role of neuroserpin and extracellular proteolysis in learning and memory) to DW, National Competence Center for Neural Plasticity and Repair CTE2 to H-PL and DW, FP6 STREP/SME 037965 (Intellimaze) to H-PL and DW, European Consortium on Synaptic Protein Networks in Neurological and Psychiatric seases, HEALTH-F2-2009-241498 (EUROSPIN) to H-PL and DW, EMDO Foundation Zürich, (The role of neuroserpin in the regulation of learning and anxiety related behavior) and Zürich Center for Integrative Human Physiology (Enhanced cognitive functioning and memory by hypoxia and erythropoietin) to DW, and PSRP-2010-2010 (Polish-Swiss-Research Programme) to H-PL.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Acknowledgments
The authors appreciate the organizational support of Frank Rühli, head of the Institute of Evolutionary Medicine, and thank Sven Krackow for critical reading and Irina Lipp for continuous administrative support.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnbeh.2022.958067/full#supplementary-material
References
- Aggleton J. P., Nelson A. J. D., O'Mara S. M. (2022). Time to retire the serial Papez circuit: implications for space, memory, and attention. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 140, 104813. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104813 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aimon S., Katsuki T., Jia T., Grosenick L., Broxton M., Deisseroth K., et al. (2019). Fast near-whole-brain imaging in adult Drosophila during responses to stimuli and behavior. PLoS Biol. 17, e2006732. 10.1371/journal.pbio.2006732 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alibhai S. M., Breunis H., Timilshina N., Marzouk S., Stewart D., Tannock I., et al. (2010). Impact of androgen-deprivation therapy on cognitive function in men with nonmetastatic prostate cancer. J. Clin. Oncol. 28, 5030–5037. 10.1200/JCO.2010.30.8742 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersen P. (1959). Interhippocampal impulses. I. Origin, course and distribution in cat, rabbit and rat. Acta Physiol Scand 47, 63–90. 10.1111/j.1748-1716.1960.tb01821.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersen P., Bland B. H., Dudar J. D. (1973). Organization of the hippocampal output. Exp. Brain Res. 17, 152–168. 10.1007/BF00235025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersen P., Bliss T. V., Lomo T., Olsen L. I., Skrede K. K. (1969). Lamellar organization of hippocampal excitatory pathways. Acta Physiol Scand 76, 4A-5A. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersen P., Soleng A. F., Raastad M. (2000). The hippocampal lamella hypothesis revisited. Brain Res. 886, 165–171. 10.1016/S0006-8993(00)02991-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bang J. Y., Sunstrum J. K., Garand D., Parfitt G. M., Woodin M., Inoue W., et al. (2022). Hippocampal-hypothalamic circuit controls context-dependent innate defensive responses. Elife 11. 10.7554/eLife.74736.sa2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beyenburg S., Watzka M., Clusmann H., Blumcke I., Bidlingmaier F., Elger C. E., et al. (2000). Androgen receptor mRNA expression in the human hippocampus. Neurosci. Lett. 294, 25–28. 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)01542-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blandina P., Munari L., Provensi G., Passani M. B. (2012). Histamine neurons in the tuberomamillary nucleus: a whole center or distinct subpopulations? Front. Syst. Neurosci. 6, 33. 10.3389/fnsys.2012.00033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bliss T. V., Lomo T. (1973). Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. J. Physiol. 232, 331–356. 10.1113/jphysiol.1973.sp010273 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Branwen G. (2021). Hydrocephalus and Intelligence: the hollow men. Available online at: https://www.gwern.net/Hydrocephalus (accessed on 2015).
- Breit S., Kupferberg A., Rogler G., Hasler G. (2018). Vagus Nerve as Modulator of the Brain–Gut Axis in Psychiatric and Inflammatory Disorders. Frontiers in Psychiatry 9. 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00044 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen S., He L., Huang A. J. Y., Boehringer R., Robert V., Wintzer M. E., et al. (2020). A hypothalamic novelty signal modulates hippocampal memory. Nature 586, 270–274. 10.1038/s41586-020-2771-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coenen V. A., Panksepp J., Hurwitz T. A., Urbach H., Madler B. (2012). Human medial forebrain bundle (MFB) and anterior thalamic radiation (ATR): imaging of two major subcortical pathways and the dynamic balance of opposite affects in understanding depression. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 24, 223–236. 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.11080180 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cook N. D. (1986). The Brain Code. Mechanisms of Information Transfer and the Role of the Corpus Callosum. London: Routledge and CRC Press. [Google Scholar]
- Crabtree J. W. (2018). Functional diversity of thalamic reticular subnetworks. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 12, 41. 10.3389/fnsys.2018.00041 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crusio W. E., Schwegler H. (2005). Learning spatial orientation tasks in the radial-maze and structural variation in the hippocampus in inbred mice. Behav. Brain Funct. 1, 3. 10.1186/1744-9081-1-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crusio W. E., Schwegler H., van Abeelen J. H. (1989). Behavioral responses to novelty and structural variation of the hippocampus in mice. II. Multivariate genetic analysis. Behav Brain Res. 32, 81–88. 10.1016/S0166-4328(89)80075-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davidson T. L., Jarrard L. E. (2004). The hippocampus and inhibitory learning: a 'Gray' area? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 28, 261–271. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.02.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deacon R. M., Croucher A., Rawlins J. N. (2002). Hippocampal cytotoxic lesion effects on species-typical behaviours in mice. Behav. Brain Res. 132, 203–213. 10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00401-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deacon R. M., Rawlins J. N. (2005). Hippocampal lesions, species-typical behaviours and anxiety in mice. Behav. Brain Res. 156, 241–249. 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.05.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dell'Omo G., Ricceri L., Wolfer D. P., Poletaeva I. I., Lipp H. P. (2000). Temporal and spatial adaptation to food restriction in mice under naturalistic conditions. Behav. Brain Res. 115, 1–8. 10.1016/S0166-4328(00)00234-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dell'Omo G., Vannoni E., Vyssotski A. L., Di Bari M. A., Nonno R., Agrimi U., et al. (2002). Automated home cage monitoring of mice infected with BSE and scrapie differentiates early behavioural changes according to prion strain. Eur. J. Neurosci. 16, 735–742. 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02128.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeVito J. L., White L. E., Jr. (1966). Projections from the fornix to the hippocampal formation in the squirrel monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 127, 389–398. 10.1002/cne.901270307 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickson P. E., Mittleman G. (2022). Working memory and pattern separation in founder strains of the BXD recombinant inbred mouse panel. Sci. Rep. 12, 69. 10.1038/s41598-021-03850-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dougherty P. (2020). Chapter 1: Hypothalamus: Structural Organization [Online]. Houston: University of Texas. Available onlin at: https://nba.uth.tmc.edu/neuroscience/m/s4/chapter01.html (accessed 2020). [Google Scholar]
- Douglas R. J. (1967). The hippocampus and behavior. Psychol. Bull. 67, 416–442. 10.1037/h0024599 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- du Sert N. P., Hurst V., Ahluwalia A., Alam S., Avey M. T., Baker M., et al. (2020). The ARRIVE guidelines 2.0: updated guidelines for reporting animal research. J. Physiol.-London 598, 3793–3801. 10.1113/JP280389 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Endo T., Maekawa F., Voikar V., Haijima A., Uemura Y., Zhang Y., et al. (2011). Automated test of behavioral flexibility in mice using a behavioral sequencing task in IntelliCage. Behav. Brain Res. 221, 172–181. 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.02.037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Esposito M. S., Arber S. (2016). Motor control: illuminating an enigmatic midbrain locomotor center. Curr. Biol. 26, R291–R293. 10.1016/j.cub.2016.02.043 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferris C. F., Cai X., Qiao J., Switzer B., Baun J., Morrison T., et al. (2019). Life without a brain: Neuroradiological and behavioral evidence of neuroplasticity necessary to sustain brain function in the face of severe hydrocephalus. Sci. Rep. 9, 16479. 10.1038/s41598-019-53042-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feuillet L., Dufour H., Pelletier J. (2007). Brain of a white-collar worker. Lancet. 370, 262. 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)61127-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fonberg E. (1973). The normalizing effect of lateral amygdalar lesions upon the dorsomedial amygdalar syndrome in dogs. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 33, 449–466. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fraigne J. J., Torontali Z. A., Snow M. B., Peever J. H. (2015). REM sleep at its Core - Circuits, Neurotransmitters, And Pathophysiology. Front. Neurol. 6, 123. 10.3389/fneur.2015.00123 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frey U., Morris R. G. M. (1997). Synaptic tagging and long-term potentiation. Nature. 385, 533–536. 10.1038/385533a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Froudarakis E., Fahey P. G., Reimer J., Smirnakis S. M., Tehovnik E. J., Tolias A. S. (2019). The visual cortex in context. Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci. 5, 317–339. 10.1146/annurev-vision-091517-034407 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gall F. J. (1822). Sur les Fonctions du Cerveau et sur Celles de chacune de ses Parties. Paris: J.-B. Baillière. [Google Scholar]
- Gallay M. N., Moser D., Jeanmonod D. (2018). Safety and accuracy of incisionless transcranial MR-guided focused ultrasound functional neurosurgery: single-center experience with 253 targets in 180 treatments. J. Neurosurg. 1–10. 10.3171/2017.12.JNS172054 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray J. A., McNaughton N. (1982). The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: An Enquiry into the Functions of the Septo-Hippocampal System. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Grillner S., Wallen P., Saitoh K., Kozlov A., Robertson B. (2008). Neural bases of goal-directed locomotion in vertebrates–an overview. Brain Res. Rev. 57, 2–12. 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.06.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gron K. J. (2006). Primate Factsheets: Patas monkey (Erythrocebus patas) Taxonomy, Morphology, and Ecology. Available online at: http://pin.primate.wisc.edu/factsheets/entry/patas_monkey/taxon
- Gruber D., Waanders R., Collins R. L., Wolfer D. P., Lipp H.-P. (1991). Weak or missing paw preference in a mouse strain (I/LnJ) with congenital absence of the corpus callosum. Behav. Brain Res. 45, 9–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guillot P. V., Roubertoux P. L., Crusio W. E. (1994). Hippocampal mossy fiber distributions and intermale aggression in seven inbred mouse strains. Brain Res. 660, 167–169. 10.1016/0006-8993(94)90852-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halasz J., Liposits Z., Meelis W., Kruk M. R., Haller J. (2002). Hypothalamic attack area-mediated activation of the forebrain in aggression. Neuroreport. 13, 1267–1270. 10.1097/00001756-200207190-00010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall K. R. L. (1966). Behaviour and ecology of the wild Patas monkey, Erythrocebus patas, in Uganda. J. Zool. 148, 15–87. 10.1111/j.1469-7998.1966.tb02942.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Haller J. (2013). The neurobiology of abnormal manifestations of aggression–a review of hypothalamic mechanisms in cats, rodents, and humans. Brain Res. Bull. 93, 97–109. 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2012.10.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hashimotodani Y., Karube F., Yanagawa Y., Fujiyama F., Kano M. (2018). Supramammillary Nucleus Afferents To The Dentate Gyrus Co-Release Glutamate And Gaba And Potentiate Granule Cell Output. Cell Rep. 25, 2704–2715 e2704. 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.11.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang Y.-Y., Bach M. E., Lipp H.-P., Zhuo M., Wolfer D. P., Hawkins R. D., et al. (1996). Mice lacking the gene encoding tissue-type plasminogen activator show a selective interference with late phase LTP in both Schaffer collateral and mossy fiber pathways. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 93, 8699–8704. 10.1073/pnas.93.16.8699 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huston J. P., Borbely A. A. (1974). The thalamic rat: general behavior, operant learning with rewarding hypothalamic stimulation, and effects of amphetamine. Physiol. Behav. 12, 433–448. 10.1016/0031-9384(74)90121-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ioannidis J. P. A. (2014). How to Make More Published Research True. PLoS Med. 11. 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001747 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jirkof P. (2014). Burrowing and nest building behavior as indicators of well-being in mice. J. Neurosci. Methods. 234, 139–146. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2014.02.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Josset N., Roussel M., Lemieux M., Lafrance-Zoubga D., Rastqar A., Bretzner F. (2018). Distinct contributions of mesencephalic locomotor region nuclei to locomotor control in the freely behaving mouse. Curr. Biol 28, 884–901. 10.1016/j.cub.2018.02.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaplan H. S., Salazar-Thula O., Khoss N., Zimmer M. (2020). Nested Neuronal Dynamics Orchestrate a Behavioral Hierarchy across Timescales. Neuron. 105, 562–576. 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.10.037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaplan H. S., Zimmer M. (2020). Brain-wide representations of ongoing behavior: a universal principle? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 64, 60–69. 10.1016/j.conb.2020.02.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim C., Choi H., Kim J. K., Chang H. K., Park R. S., Kang I. Y. (1970). General behavioral activity and its component patterns in hippocampectomized rats. Brain Res. 19, 379–394. 10.1016/0006-8993(70)90381-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim C., Kim C. C., Kim J. K., Kim M. S., Chang H. K., Kim J. Y., et al. (1971). Fear response and aggressive behavior of hippocampectomized house rats. Brain Res. 29, 237–251. 10.1016/0006-8993(71)90031-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiryk A., Janusz A., Zglinicki B., Turkes E., Knapska E., Konopka W., et al. (2020). IntelliCage as a tool for measuring mouse behavior-20 years perspective. Behav. Brain Res. 388. 10.1016/j.bbr.2020.112620 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kluger J., Roy A., Chao H. H. (2020). Androgen deprivation therapy and cognitive function in prostate cancer. Curr. Oncol. Rep. 22, 24. 10.1007/s11912-020-0884-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kondrakiewicz K., Kostecki M., Szadzinska W., Knapska E. (2019). Ecological validity of social interaction tests in rats and mice. Genes Brain Behav. 18, e12525. 10.1111/gbb.12525 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kruk M. R. (2014). Hypothalamic attack: a wonderful artifact or a useful perspective on escalation and pathology in aggression? A viewpoint. Curr. Top Behav. Neurosci. 17, 143–188. 10.1007/7854_2014_313 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krzywkowski P., Penna B., Gross C. T. (2020). Dynamic encoding of social threat and spatial context in the hypothalamus. Elife. 9. 10.7554/eLife.57148.sa2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lathe R. (2001). Hormones and the hippocampus. J. Endocrinol. 169, 205–231. 10.1677/joe.0.1690205 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lathe R., Singadia S., Jordan C., Riedel G. (2020). The interoceptive hippocampus: Mouse brain endocrine receptor expression highlights a dentate gyrus (DG)-cornu ammonis (CA) challenge-sufficiency axis. PLoS ONE. 15, e0227575. 10.1371/journal.pone.0227575 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lazic S. E., Fuss J., Gass P. (2014). Quantifying the behavioural relevance of hippocampal neurogenesis. PLoS ONE. 9, e113855. 10.1371/journal.pone.0113855 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewin R. (1980). Is your brain really necessary. Science. 210, 1232–1234. 10.1126/science.7434023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipp H.-P. (1978). Aggression and flight behaviour of the marmoset monkey Callithrix jacchus: an ethogram for brain stimulation studies. Brain Behav. Evol. 15, 241–259. 10.1159/000123781 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipp H.-P. (1979). Differential hypothalamic self-stimulation behaviour in Roman high- avoidance and low-avoidance rats. Brain Res. Bull. 4, 553–559. 10.1016/0361-9230(79)90041-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipp H.-P. (2015). Über die Organisation des Gedächtnisses: ein neurobiologisches Modell zum Verständnis der unbewussten Erinnerung. Psychotherapiewissenschaft. 5, 24–32. [Google Scholar]
- Lipp H.-P. (2017). Evolutionary shaping of adult hippocampal neurogenesis in mammals-cognitive gain or developmental priming of personality traits? Front. Neurosci. 11, 420. 10.3389/fnins.2017.00420 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipp H.-P., Bonfanti L. (2016). Adult neurogenesis in mammals: variations and confusions. Brain Behav. Evol. 87, 205–221. 10.1159/000446905 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipp H.-P., Hunsperger R. W. (1978). Threat, attack and flight elicited by electrical stimulation of the ventromedial hypothalamus of the marmoset monkey Callithrix jacchus. Brain Behav. Evol. 15, 260–293. 10.1159/000123782 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipp H.-P., Nauta W. J. H. (1984). Massive Ascending Hypothalamo-Hippocampal Projections in the Rat. Boston, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Unpublished work. [Google Scholar]
- Lipp H.-P., Pleskacheva M. G., Gossweiler H., Ricceri L., Smirnova A. A., Garin N. N., et al. (2001). A large outdoor radial maze for comparative studies in birds and mammals. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 25, 83–99. 10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00050-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipp H.-P., Schwegler H., Crusio W. E., Wolfer D. P., Leisinger-Trigona M. C., Heimrich B., et al. (1989). Using genetically-defined rodent strains for the identification of hippocampal traits relevant for two-way avoidance behavior: a non-invasive approach. Experientia. 45, 845–859. 10.1007/BF01954059 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipp H.-P., Van der Loos H. (1991). A computer-controlled Y-maze for testing vibrisso-tactile discrimination learning in mice. Behav. Brain Res. 45, 135–145. 10.1016/S0166-4328(05)80079-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipp H.-P., Wolfer D. P. (1998). Genetically modified mice and cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 8, 272–280. 10.1016/S0959-4388(98)80151-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipp H.-P., Wolfer D. P. (1999). Natural genetic variation of hippocampal structures and behavior, in Cellular and Quantitative Methods in Neurogenetics. Methods in Life Sciences - Cellular and Molecular Neuropharmacology (Lakoski), Jones B.C., Mormède P. (eds). Boca Raton, Florida: CRC Press. p. 217–235. 10.1201/9781420048384.ch16 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lipp H.-P., Wolfer D. P. (2013). Natural neurobiology and behavior of the mouse: relevance for behavioral studies in the laboratory, in Behavioral Genetics of the Mouse. Vol. 1 Genetics of Behavioral Phenotypes, Crusio W. E., Sluyter F., Gerlai R. T., Pietropaola S. (New York: Cambridge University Press; ) p. 5–16. 10.1017/CBO9781139541022.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lisman J., Buzsaki G., Eichenbaum H., Nadel L., Ranganath C., Redish A. D. (2017). Viewpoints: how the hippocampus contributes to memory, navigation and cognition. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 1434–1447. 10.1038/nn.4661 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marr D. (1971). Simple memory: a theory for archicortex. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond,. B,. Biol. Sci. 262, 23–81. 10.1098/rstb.1971.0078 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meister M. (2022). Learning, fast and slow. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 75, 102555. 10.1016/j.conb.2022.102555 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mendell A. L., Atwi S., Bailey C. D., McCloskey D., Scharfman H. E., MacLusky N. J. (2017). Expansion of mossy fibers and CA3 apical dendritic length accompanies the fall in dendritic spine density after gonadectomy in male, but not female, rats. Brain Struct. Funct. 222, 587–601. 10.1007/s00429-016-1237-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mihrshahi R. (2006). The corpus callosum as an evolutionary innovation. J. Exp. Zool. B Mol. Dev. Evol. 306b, 8–17. 10.1002/jez.b.21067 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris R. G., Garrud P., Rawlins J. N., O'Keefe J. (1982). Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature. 297, 681–683. 10.1038/297681a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mota B., Dos Santos S. E., Ventura-Antunes L., Jardim-Messeder D., Neves K., Kazu R. S., et al. (2019). White matter volume and white/gray matter ratio in mammalian species as a consequence of the universal scaling of cortical folding. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 116, 15253–15261. 10.1073/pnas.1716956116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mulligan J. B. (1997). Application of M-JPEG compression hardware to dynamic stimulus production. Spat. Vis. 11, 19–32. 10.1163/156856897X00032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nadel L. (1995). The psychobiology of spatial behavior: the hippocampal formation and spatial mapping, in Behavioural Brain Research in in Naturalistic and Semi-Naturalistic Settings, Alleva, E., Fasolo, A., Lipp, H.-P., Nadel, L., and Ricceri, L. Dordrecht Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers; p. 245–258. 10.1007/978-94-011-0091-5_13 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Nauta W. J. H. (1983). RE: Thinking While Speaking. Type to H.-P. Lipp. [Google Scholar]
- Niewiadomska G., Baksalerska-Pazera M., Riedel G. (2009). The septo-hippocampal system, learning and recovery of function. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 33, 791–805. 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.03.039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nigri M., Ahlgren J., Wolfer D. P., Voikar V. (2022). Role of environment and experimenter in reproducibility of behavioral studies with laboratory mice. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16, 835444. 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.835444 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Keefe J., Nadel L. (1978). The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford: Clarendon Press. [Google Scholar]
- O'Neil E. B., Newsome R. N., Li I. H., Thavabalasingam S., Ito R., Lee A. C. (2015). Examining the role of the human hippocampus in approach-avoidance decision making using a novel conflict paradigm and multivariate functional magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurosci. 35, 15039–15049. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1915-15.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pan W. X., McNaughton N. (2004). The supramammillary area: its organization, functions and relationship to the hippocampus. Prog. Neurobiol. 74, 127–166. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2004.09.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parker P. R. L., Brown M. A., Smear M. C., Niell C. M. (2020). Movement-related signals in sensory areas: roles in natural behavior. Trends Neurosci. 43, 581–595. 10.1016/j.tins.2020.05.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pasquier D. A., Reinososuarez F. (1978). Topographic organization of hypothalamic and brain-stem projections to hippocampus. Brain Res. Bull. 3, 373–389. 10.1016/0361-9230(78)90106-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paton C. (2002). Benzodiazepines and disinhibition: a review. Psychiat. Bullet. 26, 460–462. 10.1192/pb.26.12.460 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Patzke N., Olaleye O., Haagensen M., Hof P. R., Ihunwo A. O., Manger P. R. (2014). Organization and chemical neuroanatomy of the African elephant (Loxodonta africana) hippocampus. Brain Struct. Funct. 219, 1587–1601. 10.1007/s00429-013-0587-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Persad A. R., Bass V., Meguro K. (2021). Hydrocéphalie asymptomatique. Can. Med. Assoc. J. 193. 10.1503/cmaj.200889-f [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinna G., Agis-Balboa R. C., Zhubi A., Matsumoto K., Grayson D. R., Costa E., et al. (2006). Imidazenil and diazepam increase locomotor activity in mice exposed to protracted social isolation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 103, 4275–4280. 10.1073/pnas.0600329103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pleskacheva M. G., Wolfer D. P., Kupriyanova I. F., Nikolenko D. L., Scheffrahn H., Dell'Omo G., et al. (2000). Hippocampal mossy fibers and swimming navigation learning in two vole species occupying different habitats. Hippocampus 10, 17–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poletti C. E., Creswell G. (1977). Fornix system efferent projections in the squirrel monkey: an experimental degeneration study. J. Comp. Neurol. 175, 101–128. 10.1002/cne.901750107 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Powell T. P. S., Guillery R. W., Cowan W. M. (1957). A quantitative study of the fornix-mamillo-thalamic system. J. Anat. 91, 419–437. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puscian A., Leski S., Kasprowicz G., Winiarski M., Borowska J., Nikolaev T., et al. (2016). Eco-HAB as a fully automated and ecologically relevant assessment of social impairments in mouse models of autism. Elife. 5. 10.7554/eLife.19532.040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rakic P. (2009). Evolution of the neocortex: a perspective from developmental biology. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 724–735. 10.1038/nrn2719 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramasubbu R., Lang S., Kiss Z. H. T. (2018). Dosing of electrical parameters in deep brain stimulation (dbs) for intractable depression: a review of clinical studies. Front. Psychiat. 9. 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00302 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Redondo R. L., Morris R. G. (2011). Making memories last: the synaptic tagging and capture hypothesis. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 17–30. 10.1038/nrn2963 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rekow D., Baudouin J. Y., Durand K., Leleu A. (2022). Smell what you hardly see: Odors assist visual categorization in the human brain. Neuroimage. 255, 119181. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119181 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richter S. H., Garner J. P., Würbel H. (2009). Environmental standardization: cure or cause of poor reproducibility in animal experiments? Nat. Methods 6, 257–261. 10.1038/nmeth.1312 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenberg M., Zhang T., Perona P., Meister M. (2021). Mice in a labyrinth show rapid learning, sudden insight, and efficient exploration. Elife. 10. 10.7554/eLife.66175.sa2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosso M., Wirz R., Loretan A. V., Sutter N. A., Pereira da Cunha C. T., Jaric I., et al. (2021). Reliability of mouse behavioural tests of anxiety: a systematic review and meta-analysis on the effects of anxiolytics. bioRxiv. 2021.2007.2028.454267. 10.1101/2021.07.28.454267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sampath V., Ramchandran R. (2014). Translational research - it takes two to tango. MOJ Cell Sci Report 1, 1–3. 10.15406/mojcsr.2014.01.00003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Saunders R. C., Aggleton J. P. (2007). Origin and topography of fibers contributing to the fornix in macaque monkeys. Hippocampus 17, 396–411. 10.1002/hipo.20276 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scharfman H. E., MacLusky N. J. (2014). Differential regulation of BDNF, synaptic plasticity and sprouting in the hippocampal mossy fiber pathway of male and female rats. Neuropharmacology. 76, 696–708. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.04.029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schenk F., Morris R. G. M. (1985). Dissociation between components of spatial memory in rats after recovery from the effects of retrohippocampal lesions. Exp. Brain Res. 58, 11–28. 10.1007/BF00238949 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt M. (2020). Evolution of the hypothalamus and inferior lobe in ray-finned fishes. Brain Behav. Evol. 95, 302–316. 10.1159/000505898 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schöpke R., Wolfer D. P., Lipp H.-P., Leisinger-Trigona M.-C. (1991). Swimming navigation and structural variations of the infrapyramidal mossy fibers in the hippocampus of the mouse. Hippocampus. 1, 315–328. 10.1002/hipo.450010322 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schröder H., Moser N., Huggenberger S. (2020). Neuroanatomy of the Mouse. An introduction. Springer Nature Switzerland. 10.1007/978-3-030-19898-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Schwegler H., Crusio W. E., Lipp H.-P., Brust I., Mueller G. G. (1991). Early postnatal hyperthyroidism alters hippocampal circuitry and improves radial-maze learning in adult mice. J. Neurosci. 11, 2102–2106. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-07-02102.1991 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Senova S., Fomenko A., Gondard E., Lozano A. M. (2020). Anatomy and function of the fornix in the context of its potential as a therapeutic target. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 91, 547–559. 10.1136/jnnp-2019-322375 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shine J. M. (2021). The thalamus integrates the macrosystems of the brain to facilitate complex, adaptive brain network dynamics. Prog. Neurobiol. 199, 101951. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2020.101951 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simmons D. H., Titley H. K., Hansel C., Mason P. (2021). Behavioral tests for mouse models of autism: an argument for the inclusion of cerebellum-controlled motor behaviors. Neuroscience 462, 303–319. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2020.05.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simonov P. V. (1974). On the role of the hippocampus in the integrative activity of the brain. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars) 34, 33–41. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skrede K. K., Westgaard R. H. (1971). The transverse hippocampal slice: a well-defined cortical structure maintained in vitro. Brain Res. 35, 589–593. 10.1016/0006-8993(71)90508-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skucas V. A., Duffy A. M., Harte-Hargrove L. C., Magagna-Poveda A., Radman T., Chakraborty G., et al. (2013). Testosterone depletion in adult male rats increases mossy fiber transmission, LTP, and sprouting in area CA3 of hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 33, 2338–2355. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3857-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slomianka L., Drenth T., Cavegn N., Menges D., Lazic S. E., Phalanndwa M., et al. (2013). The hippocampus of the eastern rock sengi. cytoarchitecture, markers of neuronal function, principal cell numbers, and adult neurogenesis. Front. Neuroanat. 7. 10.3389/fnana.2013.00034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sloviter R. S., Lomo T. (2012). Updating the lamellar hypothesis of hippocampal organization. Front. Neural Circuits. 6, 102. 10.3389/fncir.2012.00102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sluyter F., Jamot L., van Oortmerssen G. A., Crusio W. E. (1994). Hippocampal mossy fiber distributions in mice selected for aggression. Brain Res. 646, 145–148. 10.1016/0006-8993(94)90068-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sonne J., Lopez-Ojeda W. (2022). Neuroanatomy, Cranial Nerve. Treasure Island (FL): StatPearls. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stringer C., Pachitariu M., Steinmetz N., Reddy C. B., Carandini M., Harris K. D. (2019). Spontaneous behaviors drive multidimensional, brainwide activity. Science. 364, 255. 10.1126/science.aav7893 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suryanarayana S. M., Perez-Fernandez J., Robertson B., Grillner S. (2020). The evolutionary origin of visual and somatosensory representation in the vertebrate pallium. Nat Ecol. Evol. 4, 639–651. 10.1038/s41559-020-1137-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turan Z. (2021). Life Without Cortex: Subcortical Circuits in Naturalistic behaviors. California: California Institute of Technology. [Google Scholar]
- Vanderwolf C. H. (1969). Hippocampal electrical activity and voluntary movement in the rat. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 26, 407–418. 10.1016/0013-4694(69)90092-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vanderwolf C. H. (2001). The hippocampus as an olfacto-motor mechanism: were the classical anatomists right after all? Behav. Brain Res. 127, 25–47. 10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00354-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vanderwolf C. H. (2007). The Evolving Brain. The Mind and the Neural Control of Behavior. New York: Springer Science. [Google Scholar]
- Vanderwolf C. H., Cain D. P. (1994). The behavioral neurobiology of learning and memory: a conceptual reorientation. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 19, 264–297. 10.1016/0165-0173(94)90015-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vannoni E., Voikar V., Colacicco G., Sanchez M. A., Lipp H.-P., Wolfer D. P. (2014). Spontaneous behavior in the social homecage discriminates strains, lesions and mutations in mice. J. Neurosci. Methods 234, 26–37. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2014.04.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voikar V., Krackow S., Lipp H.-P., Rau A., Colacicco G., Wolfer D. P. (2018). Automated dissection of permanent effects of hippocampal or prefrontal lesions on performance at spatial, working memory and circadian timing tasks of C57BL/6 mice in IntelliCage. Behav. Brain Res. 352, 8–22. 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.08.048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Völkl B., Würbel H., Krzywinski M., Altman N. (2021). The standardization fallacy. Nat. Methods 18, 5–7. 10.1038/s41592-020-01036-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vyssotski A. L., Dell'Omo G., Poletaeva I. I., Vyssotski D. L., Minichiello L., Klein R., et al. (2002). Long-term monitoring of hippocampus-dependent behavior in naturalistic settings: mutant mice lacking the neurotrophin receptor TrkB in the forebrain show spatial learning but impaired behavioral flexibility. Hippocampus. 12, 27–38. 10.1002/hipo.10002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang W., Schuette P. J., La-Vu M. Q., Torossian A., Tobias B. C., Ceko M., et al. (2021). Dorsal premammillary projection to periaqueductal gray controls escape vigor from innate and conditioned threats. Elife. 10, e69178. 10.1101/2021.05.02.442354 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Warren R. A., Zhang Q., Hoffman J. R., Li E. Y., Hong Y. K., Bruno R. M., et al. (2021). A rapid whisker-based decision underlying skilled locomotion in mice. Elife. 10. 10.7554/eLife.63596 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Willshaw D. J., Dayan P., Morris R. G. (2015). Memory, modelling and marr: a commentary on Marr (1971) 'Simple memory: a theory of archicortex'. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond,. B,. Biol. Sci. 370. 10.1098/rstb.2014.0383 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiltgen B. J., Sanders M. J., Anagnostaras S. G., Sage J. R., Fanselow M. S. (2006). Context fear learning in the absence of the hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 26, 5484–5491. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2685-05.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Witter M. P., Kleven H., Flatmoen A. K. (2017). Comparative contemplations on the hippocampus. Brain Behavior and Evolution 90, 15–24. 10.1159/000475703 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Würbel H. (2002). Behavioral phenotyping enhanced–beyond (environmental) standardization. Genes Brain Behav. 1, 3–8. 10.1046/j.1601-1848.2001.00006.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yassa M. A., Stark C. E. (2011). Pattern separation in the hippocampus. Trends Neurosci. 34, 515–525. 10.1016/j.tins.2011.06.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang X. Y., Yu L., Zhuang Q. X., Zhu J. N., Wang J. J. (2013). Central functions of the orexinergic system. Neurosci. Bull. 29, 355–365. 10.1007/s12264-012-1297-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.