Skip to main content
Autism in Adulthood: Challenges and Management logoLink to Autism in Adulthood: Challenges and Management
. 2022 Jun 9;4(2):141–150. doi: 10.1089/aut.2021.0035

“This Was Just How This Friendship Worked”: Experiences of Interpersonal Victimization Among Autistic Adults

Amy Pearson 1,, Jon Rees 1, Samantha Forster 2
PMCID: PMC9645672  PMID: 36605970

Abstract

Background:

The victimization of autistic people by familiar others (interpersonal victimization) is an understudied phenomenon despite suggestions that prevalence rates may be disproportionately high. We know very little about the way autistic people perceive these experiences, and how to support them. The aim of the current study was to explore experiences of interpersonal victimization among autistic adults from their own perspective.

Methods:

We recruited 43 autistic adults to take part in a qualitative online study, and asked about their experiences of being victimized or taken advantage of by people they know in the past. We analyzed their comments at the semantic level using inductive thematic analysis, from a critical realist perspective.

Results:

We identified two key themes in the data. The first theme, “cycles of victimization” highlighted the occurrence of polyvictimization in the sample. The second (“perceptions of victimization”) focused on how these experiences were related to difficulties with trust (of both self and others), the recognition of victimization, and heightened compliance. The participants expressed difficulty with saying no to people, and found it difficult to identify when someone had negative or manipulative intentions.

Conclusions:

Our findings suggest that autistic adults experience victimization from a range of close others, and may find it difficult to recognize when someone is acting in an abusive manner. Many participants had experienced heightened compliance in response to unreasonable requests from others, however, reasons for this were varied (e.g., fear and desire to avoid confrontation) and require further investigation. These findings have implications for developing supports that enable autistic adults to recognize their own boundaries and advocate for themselves, in addition to helping them to recognize what a healthy relationship looks like.

Keywords: autism, friendships, relationships, victimization, interpersonal victimization

Community brief

Why was this study done?

We think that many autistic people experience being hurt by people they know. This can include both physical harm such as hitting, and emotional harm such as being called horrible things. We currently know very little about these experiences, even though we think it happens a lot.

What was the purpose of this study?

The purpose of this study was to find out more about the experiences of autistic people who have been hurt by someone they know, from their own point of view.

What did the researchers do?

Using an online survey, we invited autistic adults who had been hurt by someone they know to write about what had happened to them from their own point of view. Forty-three autistic people wrote about their experiences and we analyzed these data by reading what they had said and looking for patterns (themes) across people.

What were the results of this study?

Many of the participants told us that they had been hurt more than once, by different people (e.g., their parents, and people they thought were friends). Some participants said that they found it difficult to tell whether someone is treating them badly, and to trust their own judgment about other people's behavior. Some people said that they felt like they had do what other people told them to do. Some did this to avoid getting other people into trouble, whereas others did it because they were scared of what would happen if they said no.

What do these findings add to what was already known?

Previous studies have shown that lots of autistic people have been hurt by people they know, and that this has a negative impact on their mental health. Researchers have also asked autistic people what might put them at risk of being hurt by people they know, and how to prevent it. To our knowledge, this is the first study to directly ask autistic people about their own experiences of being hurt by people they know, and highlights what they feel is important for us to know about it.

What are potential weaknesses in this study?

We do not know very much about the people who took part aside from their age and gender (e.g., what their race or ethnicity is), and we do not know much about the people who victimized them (e.g., whether they were neurotypical or autistic themselves), or exactly when it happened.

How will these findings help autistic people now or in the future?

The findings from this study could be used to think about how we can support autistic people in saying no if someone tells them to do something they do not want to do. They can also be used to find ways to help people to recognize when other people are being hurtful. Finally, we hope that reading this study will make other researchers and practitioners interested in learning how to support autistic people who have been hurt by people they know.

Introduction

Interpersonal victimization refers to violence and abuse that occur within close personal relationships, committed by people such as friends, family members, or carers.1–3 Interpersonal victimization can involve sexual and financial exploitation (e.g., forcing someone into unwanted sexual activity with oneself or others, forcing someone to give you their money), physical, psychological, or emotional abuse, and humiliation/cruelty.4 To date, there is a limited body of research on the experience of interpersonal victimization among autistic adults5–8 despite suggestions that between 49% and 80% of autistic adults have been victimized by someone they know.9–13

There is a growing body of literature7,8 examining the experiences of interpersonal violence in disabled adults more broadly, some of which has included autistic adults. While these studies provide important insight into the impact of victimization on disabled adults, it is important to develop a more focused knowledge base on the experience of autistic adults, specifically given the high prevalence of interpersonal victimization in this population.

It is important to establish the nuance between interpersonal victimization and other forms of peer victimization such as bullying despite some overlap between the two, as there is an extant body of work on bullying in autistic people.14 Both interpersonal victimization and bullying/peer victimization can include physical, emotional, and psychological harm against another, and have a negative impact on well-being outcomes for autistic people.8,13,14 However, unlike bullying, interpersonal victimization is defined by the presence of an interpersonal relationship between the perpetrator and victim.5 Acknowledgment of the role that social relationships can play in providing the context for interpersonal victimization to occur is crucial, given the fallacious yet pervasive belief that autistic people neither desire nor value social relationships.15

Forster and Pearson5 asked autistic adults about their experiences of relationships and understanding of mate crime, which is a form of interpersonal victimization perpetrated by those considered friends (“mate” is a British slang term for friend).1,2 Participants outlined the challenges that they faced building genuine, reciprocal relationships and their experiences of victimization. They also spoke about their difficulties identifying unreasonable behavior from others, and heightened social compliance. These findings were consistent with previous research into relationships16–20 and peer victimization among autistic adults.6,21

Participants in this study also explicitly suggested that a personal relationship between the perpetrator and victim was more insidious than bullying alone, as it could make it harder to spot disingenuous behavior. This aligns with the suggestion made by disabled scholars that terming interpersonal victimization as “bullying” can make it appear “low level” in nature.22–24

The difficulty in identifying manipulative and implicit social intentions can be explained through the lens of the double empathy problem.25 The double empathy problem posits that differences in communication style can lead to mutual difficulties in understanding between interlocutors. Autistic adults have self-identified how these bidirectional breakdowns can lead to problems with reading below surface-level social intentions in other people, and have shared concerns over their potential for being manipulated.20,26 The double empathy problem helps to frame these concerns as an interaction between the person and context, as opposed to an innate vulnerability.27

The perception of autistic people as innately vulnerable is perpetuated by the stereotype of autistic people as “child-like.”28 Research does suggest that autistic people are more likely to experience situational vulnerabilities (e.g., underemployment, poverty, isolation, and lack of social support)12 that might put them at a higher risk of victimization. However, situational vulnerabilities are often underpinned by stigma and marginalization29 that autistic people experience routinely30–33 in both the academic34 and public sphere.28,35–37

Perceptions of vulnerability, in addition to mislabeling interpersonal victimization as bullying, can lead to interpersonal victimization being taken less seriously by the criminal justice system.22 It can also lead to the overlooking of experiential insights, due to the assumption that risk of victimization is an innate characteristic, rather than an interaction between the person and environment.6 Fardella et al.6 asked autistic adults about factors that might result in increased risk of interpersonal violence for autistic adults, and what could be done to prevent victimization. They used the ecological model38 to examine how different factors such as environment, individual differences, and social context could account for increased vulnerability. Participants in their study highlighted the importance of interpersonal skills, self and supported advocacy, and the need to foster inclusion and acceptance.

These findings provide important advancements in knowledge about risk factors and support. However, it is important we find out firsthand from autistic adults who have experienced interpersonal victimization firsthand how they perceive these experiences.

Thus, more research is needed to understand the interpersonal victimization experiences of autistic people from their perspective, taking into account the context of social relationships and acknowledging the complexity of why autistic people may be at heightened risk.6 This research can help us to begin to ascertain what can be done to (a) minimize victimization and (b) provide support.

The aim of the current study was to build upon previous research, exploring the lived experience of interpersonal victimization among autistic adults from a phenomenological perspective.

Methods

Research question

The research question that we aimed to address in this study was: “What are the experiences of autistic adults who have been victimized by people close to them?”

To address this question, we conducted an online survey. In this survey, we asked participants whether they had ever:

  • a) been the victim of a crime

  • b) been victimized by someone close to them

  • c) been taken advantage of by someone close to them

  • d) heard the term “mate crime” before?

For each question, we provided an open-text box for people to write about their experiences. For the purpose of this study, we were interested in responses from people who answered “yes” to questions b and/or c, and wrote about their experiences. We left interpretation of the terms used in these questions up to the participant, as we did not want them to feel restricted by our definitions of these concepts. We sought additional feedback from an autistic colleague not involved in the project about the accessibility of the study and wording of the study information and questions before recruitment.

Participants

Sixty-four autistic adults responded to an advertisement that we shared on social media (e.g., Twitter, Facebook) stating that we were seeking autistic adults (18+ only) to take part in research about perceptions of friendship and mate crime. The #AskingAutistics hashtag was used to promote reach. Of those who responded, our final sample consistent of 43 people who reported having been victimized or taken advantage of by someone close to them and provided open-ended responses describing these experiences.

This sample comprised 27 women, 13 men, 2 nonbinary people, and one genderqueer person. Participants reported whether they were clinically diagnosed (n = 36) or self-identified (n = 7). Both were welcome, as the researchers acknowledge that a clinical diagnosis is not always an accessible process.39–42 The average age of diagnosis was 29 years (range = 4–52). We asked whether participants had any additional diagnoses* alongside autism. Thirty-nine participants reported having multiple diagnoses (e.g., anxiety and autism). The most frequently reported diagnoses were depression (n = 15), anxiety (n = 10), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (n = 9), and dyspraxia (n = 5). In addition, several participants reported physical disabilities, including Ehlers–Danlos syndrome (n = 3), myalgic encephalomyelitis (n = 2), and fibromyalgia (n = 3).

We asked participants about whether they communicated mainly through speaking (n = 42) or nonspeaking (n = 1) means. We also asked how many close friends participants had (M = 3.6), and how many people they socialized with online or in person on a weekly basis (M = 10.6). We did not ask for any further demographic data (e.g., socioeconomic status, race/ethnicity). The study received ethical approval from the University of Sunderland Research Ethics Committee. This study was not funded and no financial inducements were offered to participants.

Methodological approach

We chose to use a reflexive thematic analysis for the open-text data, following the six-step method outlined by Braun and Clarke43 situated within a critical realist44 paradigm. We selected thematic analysis as it provides a flexible framework for considering aspects of a whole data set and is suitable for larger samples in qualitative analysis. We used a critical realist approach as it allows for consideration that autistic people are both the experts of their own experiences, and that experiences of being autistic will differ across people. We used an inductive, data-driven approach as research into the experience of interpersonal victimization among autistic adults is sparse.

The first author (an autistic woman) read the open-text data multiple times to gain familiarity, and then coded the data based on semantic content (Table 1). This coding process was organic and open,45 and involved identifying key or salient information in the data set.

Table 1.

Results of the Thematic Analysis Including Themes, Subthemes, and Example Codes

Theme Subtheme Example codes
1. Cycles of victimization   Resilient
Sustained
Escape
2. Perceptions of victimization 2.1. Problems with trust Trust/trusting
Self-blame/distrust
Helpful/polite
Suspicious
  1.2. “This was just how this friendship worked”: recognizing victimization Overt/covert abuse
One-way relationships
Outer perceptions
Blame from others
Gaslighting
Used
Normalized
Later realizations
  1.3. “I had to”: the role of compliance “To please”
Compliant
Nonconfrontational
Forced compliance
Desire for comfort

The first author developed the themes by grouping the codes together based on shared content or meaning (e.g., nonconfrontational, compliant), and then refined these themes. We collected the data reported in this article in early 2019, and throughout the article revision process we have reorganized and renamed the themes. Throughout this process, the first author maintained reflexivity by engaging in internal reflexive rumination and having team discussions about the data with the remaining authors, who provided feedback on the analysis throughout. These discussions centered on the interpretation of the data, power relationships, and the representation of the experiences and thoughts of the participants.

Procedure

We hosted the study on the online survey platform Qualtrics. Participants took part by clicking a link, which directed them to an information sheet detailing the aims of the study. We told participants that the topic was of a sensitive and potentially upsetting nature, that all data provided would be anonymous, and that they were free to withdraw at any point during the study by closing the browser. We provided contact details for the lead researcher, along with contact details for related charitable support services. Participants provided consent by clicking to confirm that they were (a) older than 18 years, (b) had read and understood the information sheet, and (c) consented to taking part in the study.

We presented participants with demographic questions first, followed by questions about their experiences. At the end, we thanked participants for their time and gave them the opportunity to provide comments and feedback before they finally submitted their data.

Findings

We conducted the final analysis using the data combined from both questions b and c (“victimized by someone you know” and “taken advantage of”). We do not report the data from questions a and d in this article. The responses that the participants provided varied in length substantially, from a few words to multiple sentences. We identified two main themes from the data set (Table 1) to address our research questions. We discuss each theme in turn, providing quotes to support. Each quote is attributed to a participant by the participant number, gender, and age.

Cycles of victimization

It was clear from the comments provided by the participants that many of them had experienced repeated (polyvictimization) or sustained acts of victimization, for example:

“Bullied at school, ganged up on, bullied at work, stolen from, my natural generosity exploited.” (P.34, woman, 54)

“Taken advantage off by male mates when incapacitated. Stalked by ex. Abused by friend I wouldn't date. Suspect friend stole from me.” (P.24, woman, 40)

Some people reported incidences during a specific time point in their life, for example, “I was bullied a lot growing up and victimised during my teenage years by people I thought of then as friends” (P.37, woman, 46). However, others recounted multiple instances across the life span, often spanning multiple relationships:

“A male friend raped me and sexually assaulted me for over four hours and he kept trying to pull my hearing aids…My mam has mentally emotionally and financially abused me throughout the years…My brother regularly took his anger out on me in many ways such as mentally emotionally and physically… I have been bullied throughout growing up and was even cyber bullied at college.” (P.9, woman, 36)

The most commonly reported forms of interpersonal victimization were intimate partner violence and familial abuse (i.e., sister, mother), however, participants also wrote about experiences with (ex) friends and colleagues. Some participants wrote about how they had gone on to form good relationships after multiple experiences of abuse, “Most of my relationships with men have involved some level of emotional abuse…thankfully I have finally found a partner who seems to get me and is kind.” (P.24, woman, 54).

However, this was not the case for everyone, and the experience of polyvictimization had led others to see themselves as the problem: “I believe there is something wrong in the way I introduce myself to friendship which makes people feel I am worth nothing beyond physical resources” (P.14, woman, 30). Here it was clear that polyvictimization was a shared experience for multiple participants, however, the way that the participants reflected on it differed from person to person.

Perceptions of victimization

Theme 2 drew together three subthemes, centered around how the participants perceived what had happened to them. They focused on their difficulty in trusting their instincts around what is acceptable social behavior, often giving others the benefit of the doubt at a detriment to their own needs. They also highlighted how difficult it could be to recognize victimization, and how they felt when they did not recognize it. Finally, the third theme focused on the notion of compliance and how it had pervaded their situation.

Problems with trust

Many of the participants identified personal difficulties around trust, which was not surprising given the range of negative experiences in interpersonal relationships. However, problems with trust manifested in several different ways. Some participants worried about how being too trusting could be taken advantage of by perpetrators, while still wanting to see the best in others: “having my trust misused and abused where I am actually giving people the benefit of the doubt” (P.11, man, 54). Others felt like they could not trust themselves or their judgment about other people's behavior. Some of these comments acknowledged difficulties picking up on manipulation: “I take things literally and miss so much of some people's manipulative behaviour, I'm oblivious” (P.24, woman, 54).

However, for others the problem was not the identification itself, but trusting their own judgment: “I end up rationalising their behaviour when they themselves are vulnerable, which leads to me excusing thing I probably shouldn't” (P.10, woman, 30). This comment was also indicative of the complexities involved in identifying victimization when the perpetrator also experiences vulnerability.

For other participants, previous abuse had made them distrustful, particularly of people in authority, for example: “My parents were very verbally abusive and manipulative…I felt I couldn't trust adult figures which was further compounded…I didn't feel safe with authority figures, nor did I trust they actually meant it when they said I could reach out to them for help” (P.35, non-binary, 25). Thus, although issues with trust were shared across the participants, the experience and associated impact of these issues also demonstrated several nuances.

“This was just how this friendship worked”: recognizing victimization

Recognizing that a relationship or situation was nefarious was not always easy. Some participants outlined how hard it could be to tell when someone was taking advantage: “Some of these incidents have been overt—such as a friend underhandedly stealing my jewellery…and then denying all knowledge, which I have tended to believe at the time. Other examples have been less clear, for example though coercion, elaborate lies, guilt tripping…” (P.10, woman, 30).

Both overt and covert signals can rely on a perpetrator's belief that a victim either would not realize what was going on, or if they did, would feel too awkward to say anything. This comment also echoes the self-doubt that can occur while in these situations, even when signs might seem retrospectively “obvious” (i.e., P.10 suspecting that the perpetrator had indeed taken the jewelry, but then believing the perpetrator enough to question their own initial suspicions), aligning with concerns expressed in theme 2.1.

Recognizing abuse could also be made more difficult by the actions of the perpetrator, for example, the specific mention of “guilt tripping” was a shared experience among some participants. Participant 15 recounted:

“…both of them essentially lecturing me about how I was ‘too sensitive’ and that I was being selfish and that this was just how this friendship worked…‘being honest’ with me ‘for my own good’. This lead to me thinking I was a horrible person for questioning them…they gaslit me several times” (P.15, man, 26).

The comment from the participant here also highlights the use of gaslighting (attempts by the perpetrator to convince the victim that their own knowledge/mental state is compromised, which was also mentioned by several other participants). This had an impact on the way the participant perceived their own role in the situation and their ability to recognize what was happening. Questioning their own input had meant it had taken them time and support from others to process a situation, only recognizing later what had happened to them: “otherwise having peace and time to focus on myself and my other friends (as well as support from a few close friends), I soon recognised that both ‘friends' had been abusive towards me from nearly the beginning of our friendship” (P.15, man, 26).

The experience of uncertainty around one's own perception of a situation can make it harder to recognize when behavior is unreasonable or abusive. This can also be compounded by a lack of validation from close others, for example, one participant when recalling familial abuse wrote “my mam said that you always hurt the ones you love” (P.9, woman, 36).

It was not the case that recognition was delayed for all participants, for some the recognition of what had happened to them had occurred during the situation, and they commented on how this made them feel in the moment: “Then one of them said something that made it clear they'd planned the whole thing in advance without telling me, which made me feel very naive and used” (P.32, woman, 37).

“I had to”: the role of compliance

Compliance was highlighted by many participants as playing an important role in their experiences. Some of the participants spoke of a need to please others, or “go along” with their manipulations: “(I) have been pressured into doing things that I wasn't comfortable with to try to please others who I thought were friends” (P.21 woman, 45). However, reasons for compliance were not uniform across participants. Some desired to avoid confrontation: “Friends would ask me to buy them stuff when I was in town and then would just not mention paying me when coming to collect and knew I would find the conversation too awkward to directly ask for the money” (P.36, man, 36). Others wanted to avoid being perceived negatively by others: “desperately wanted to appease” (P.41 woman, 45).

Some recognized the danger of the situation that they were in and compliance was described as self-preservation: “I knew I had to do what they told me and be their ‘friend’ or else they would make my life hell” (P.26, woman, 28). Others complied out of a desire not to get others “into trouble” particularly in situations where an uneven power dynamic with the perpetrator was present, for example, the perpetrator was a family member: “I either had to report my mam for fraud where she'd be arrested etc. or keep my mouth shut and pay it myself. I'm still paying it off to this day” (P.7, woman, 36).

Many of the participants described the toll that compliance had taken on them emotionally or financially, leaving them to face long-term ramifications alone: “I was like their personal bank account, and I got into serious debt as a result. I managed to pay it off, but it took nearly 10 years to do so” (P.30, woman, 33).

Discussion

The aim of the current study was to explore the experience of interpersonal victimization among autistic adults from their own perspective. Many participants reported experiencing polyvictimization and repeated cycles of abuse, and difficulties with trust, either in terms of trusting their own judgment and intuition, or with being overly trusting of other people. In addition, our participants self-identified difficulty with ascertaining what “normal” boundaries and behavior might look like in social relationships. Consistent with previous research in interpersonal victimization, we also found that autistic adults had difficulty with increased compliance.6,21 Here, we discuss these findings in more detail.

It was clear from theme 1 that many participants had experienced repeated acts of victimization, and while some had gone on to form good relationships, others had internalized the view that they were the problem. The experience of polyvictimization within the sample is consistent with previous research3 and reinforces the importance of understanding (a) why interpersonal victimization is so prevalent in the autistic population, and (b) the impact of interpersonal violence on autistic people.13 There is very little research into factors that help to buffer against adversity in autistic adults,46,47 and the discussion of resilience in regard to victimization is complex.

There is an obvious survivorship bias in the collection of data from those who have survived victimization and feel able to share their experiences. There are likely many people who have experienced interpersonal victimization who are unable to share their experiences with others due to trauma. We also cannot assume from the comments made here that participants have “recovered” from abuse.

Research suggests that complex post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is prevalent among autistic people and is often related to interpersonal experiences.48 It is important that we understand the kinds of factors that contribute toward recovery from victimization, to assist in developing better supports for those who have been victimized. However, it is also important that research into protective factors30 is not centered on “developing resilience.”49 This shifts responsibility from the perpetrator to the victim, suggesting that people need to be better at living with continued and sustained victimization, instead of targeting the source of victimization itself. As such, we need continuing work to reduce societal stigmas and the subsequent dehumanization that affects autistic people. This will ensure both first- and second-order change that will address the needs of this generation and future generations to come.

Theme 2.1 highlighted that problems with trust were common among our participants. Some self-identified difficulty with being “overly trusting” of others, or trusting their instincts around other people. While trusting people who have not “earned” it might be risky in interpersonal relationships, it should be noted that being trusting of others does not necessarily reflect naivety or difficulty with reading social intentions. Having one's trust repeatedly broken can, however, lead to internalizing the belief that trusting others is “naive” as we look back on experiences with hindsight. This experience is not limited to autistic people, and a feature of victimization more widely.50

The experience of broken trust had also led some of our participants to find it incredibly difficult to trust again in new relationships, and to trust those in positions of authority. This finding has implications for support systems and services for those who have been victimized, as we may need to recognize that trust may be in short supply, and might make it difficult for a person to seek formal support.

Some participants said that they struggled to spot negative social intentions and identify abusive behavior (theme 2.2), or trust their own judgment about other people (theme 2.1), which is consistent with some previous research.26,51 Some of the participants seemed to blame themselves for not “spotting” the abuse while it occurred, labeling themselves as oblivious, however, a participant who did recognize that they were being manipulated during the situation itself also said it made them feel naive. It is worth noting that the ability to retrospectively identify abuse and the ability to spot abuse “in situ” are not the same, but that they may lead to the same emotional response.

There are also multiple factors that can affect both identifying abuse and knowing how to deal with it. First, the ability to pick up on often subtle signals exhibited by abusers, particularly in situations where coercion is used, requires knowledge about what a good healthy relationship looks like. Comments from some of our participants were indicative of victimization beginning early in childhood, perpetrated by parents and caregivers. Abuse from those who are meant to care for us and “know better” can impact on the ability to recognize unacceptable behavior in others later in life.52

Spotting these signals can also rely on not taking people at “face value,” and engaging in continuing reflection on what someone has said or done. For an autistic person who tends to be straightforward in their communication style and who says what they mean, it might not occur that someone they are interacting with is being disingenuous. This can be explained through the lens of the “double empathy problem.”25 The double empathy problem recognizes that difficulties in inferring the intentions of others do not need to be labeled as a social “deficit” or “one sided” to recognize its impact. It is important that we draw upon the double empathy problem to find ways of supporting people who feel they struggle with understanding social intentions without pathologizing this difficulty.

Second, identifying abuse can also be impacted by our perceptions of our own contribution to the situation. Several participants highlighted the experience of gaslighting and invalidation from perpetrators, and this had led some people to question their perception of the situation and blame themselves. This made it harder for them to recognize that what was happening was abusive, and to put a stop to it or leave the situation. Our findings did suggest that a good support network and time/guidance to introspect could be helpful in recognizing abuse.53 This is consistent with research showing the importance of peer support systems47,54 and suggests that it is important to support autistic people in developing good-quality relationships.18

Third, even if someone can identify abusive behavior, this does not always result in knowing what to do about it, or feeling like you have a choice to change the situation, which our findings around compliance (theme 2.3) highlighted. Some participants discussed feeling like they needed to appease perpetrators, or avoid confrontation, which is consistent with previous literature.5,21 However, this was not the only reason for compliance, and the circumstances that contributed toward it were often complex. Some participants complied out of recognition that it was the only way to maintain a semblance of safety within risky situations, for example, having to comply with the demands of others to avoid physical harm. For other participants, power dynamics were present that made the situation they were in more complicated, for example, the perpetrator was a family member.

Thus, there are two key issues that we need to focus on in future research. First, we need to understand more fully why some autistic people might find it particularly difficult to dissent or set personal boundaries in the face of unreasonable requests from others. This is particularly important given that many behavioral interventions used with autistic people focus on compliance training.55,56 Such behavioral interventions may further reinforce to children and adults that their right to say “no” is not valued, and to accept uncomfortable or unreasonable requests, which may in turn lead to increased risk of victimization. There is very little empirical research into the negative effect of behavioral interventions,57,58 despite accounts from advocates of the long-term negative impact.59–61 However, it is essential that further research focuses on both compliance and how compliance may become entrenched in autistic lives (i.e., through intervention or relationship power dynamics).

Second, we need to understand and reduce structural barriers that interact with compliance, which is relevant for a range of marginalized people beyond the autistic population. For example, for some people, their circumstances might mean that a safe exit is not immediately possible, or that there are barriers to accessing support. It is important that future research examines how we can increase access to support or resources that enable people to safely exit or diffuse a particular situation, and what current barriers exist.

Finally, it is important to recognize that the experiences reported here are certainly not unique to autistic people, and show similarities to the wider abuse that many people experience from familiar others.50,62 We are not suggesting that all of our participants were victimized because they are autistic (i.e., many may have been targeted due to perceived differences, whether knowingly labeled or not). We are suggesting that the victimization of autistic people is an issue that goes beyond the scope of “bullying,” and that we need to consider the context of interpersonal relationships.

A recent study examined the experience of trauma and PTSD symptoms in autistic adults, finding that the experience of “bullying” was one of the common traumatic life events reported by autistic people who had increased PTSD symptoms.48 The authors argue that autistic people may be more likely to experience trauma from events outside of current PTSD diagnostic criteria, which is not unlikely given the sustained stigma that many autistic people experience.63,64 Some of the acts that our participants described as “bullying” and being “taken advantage of” included domestic and sexual abuse, and financial exploitation.

There may be considerations to be made here about the way in which we ask questions about negative life experiences, and how different questions may elicit different responses. The line between abuse, bullying, and more nebulous concepts such as “being taken advantage of” is not particularly clear, and we know that bullying can have incredibly negative effects on an individual.14 However, it is important that future research acknowledges that despite autistic people being labeled as “literal” in their communication, they may downplay their experiences through the terminology they use.

Overall, our findings elucidate the importance of considering the role of heightened compliance and autistic social style in understanding experiences of interpersonal victimization among autistic adults. More research is needed to examine how heightened compliance develops, and what can be done to support autistic people in recognizing abusive behavior. There may be particular recovery needs that autistic people have that are not considered in the wider scope of literature on abuse and victimization. This mirrors research into sexual victimization, where there is very little investigation specifically into the experience of intimate partner violence among autistic people3,13,65 and how best to support them.66 These issues warrant further investigation in the future.

Limitations

One major limitation of the study was the nature of static data collection in a qualitative study. Collecting an online sample allowed us to address the issues around anonymity presented in previous research,6 and recruit participants from a broad age range. However, it also meant that the amount of details provided varied from person to person, and that we were not able to prompt for additional details or clarifications during or after the responses. Static surveys with open-ended questions can allow participants additional time to process the questions, however, it does limit the more interactive aspects of using different asynchronous methods (e.g., discussion boards).

A further limitation is that our sample consisted largely of women, many of whom were diagnosed later in life. While women have historically been underrepresented in autism research, this does not appear to be the case in online samples where women are often overrepresented.67 The large female sample in the current study may speak more broadly to considerations around gender and experiences of interpersonal violence. However, without further data on the perpetrator (e.g., their gender), it is difficult to comment on specific implications here, particularly given that autistic people are more likely to report being part of a sexual minority.68

We did not collect any data on other demographic factors such as location, socioeconomic status, or race and ethnicity that would provide further insight into intersectional issues. We did, however, collect data on co-occurring diagnoses/disabilities. One participant made specific mention of nonautism, disability-related abuse in their comments, which highlights a need to consider more broadly the experiences of autistic people with multiple disabilities. Future research should aim to recruit as diverse a sample of autistic adults as possible, to account for intersectional69 factors that may relate to increased prevalence of interpersonal victimization. It is likely that people who are marginalized on multiple axes are at heightened likelihood of victimization, due to increased likelihood of social isolation and structural inequalities.62

Previous research has suggested that autistic masking might be an important factor to consider when examining interpersonal relationships and victimization among autistic people.5 The current study did not take any quantitative or qualitative measure of masking, and thus, we cannot comment on the interaction between masking and interpersonal victimization, however, it is important that future research takes this into account, particularly in relation to compliance.

Finally, there are several questions that future research needs to address, including gathering further information around the time period in which these events occurred (e.g., adolescence, adulthood), and who committed them (e.g., friends, family). Not all victimization autistic people experience is cross-neurotype (e.g., neurotypical on autistic victimization). Thus, more research is needed to understand the role of the perpetrator in the victimization of autistic people, and autistic–autistic violence. Unfortunately, we did not gather specifics on these factors in the current study, but we hope that future research in this area can address these issues.

Conclusion

The current study aimed to investigate experiences of interpersonal victimization among autistic adults. The findings highlighted the experience of polyvictimization within the sample, with many participants having experienced multiple forms of interpersonal violence across the life span. Findings also suggest that some autistic adults may find it difficult to recognize when someone is acting in an abusive manner. Heightened compliance was a common experience among our participants, however, reasons for this heightened compliance were complex and would benefit from further investigation. This study adds to our understanding of the factors present in the experiences of interpersonal victimization among autistic adults, and areas we should focus on to address this issue.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Dr. Monique Botha for the invaluable comments on a draft of this article, Colin Wilson for his assistance with the wording of the study, and Kieran Rose for his helpful discussions. They also thank everyone who took part in this research for sharing their experiences with them. The authors hope that you find justice. A.P. also thanks Andrew Brown for his unwavering support, and for providing a sounding board for personal reflection and growth throughout the research process.

Authorship Confirmation Statement

A.P. designed this study, collected and analyzed the data and wrote the article. S.F. contributed toward the design of the study, discussion of analysis, and writing of the article. J.R. contributed toward the discussion of analysis and writing of the article. All authors have approved this submission.

Author Disclosure Statement

None of the authors has a conflict of interest to declare.

Funding Information

This study did not receive funding.

*

While we have used the term “diagnoses” here, we did not ask whether these were clinically “verified.” We also left the term “diagnoses” up to the interpretation of the participants, to choose whether they wanted to list mental health, additional forms of neurodivergence, and/or physical health.

References

  • 1. Landman RA. “A counterfeit friendship”: Mate crime and people with learning disabilities. J Adult Prot. 2014;16(6):355. [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Thomas P. ‘Mate crime’: Ridicule, hostility and targeted attacks against disabled people. Disabil Soc. 2011;26(1):107–111. [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Weiss JA, Fardella MA. Victimization and perpetration experiences of adults with autism. Front Psychiatry. 2018;9:203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Quarmby K. Scapegoat: How We Are Failing Disabled People. London: Portobello; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Forster S, Pearson A. “Bullies tend to be obvious”: Autistic adults perceptions of friendship and the concept of ‘mate crime’. Disabil Soc. 2020;35(7):1103–1123. [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Fardella MA, Burnham Riosa P, Weiss JA. A qualitative investigation of risk and protective factors for interpersonal violence in adults on the autism spectrum. Disabil Soc. 2018;33(9):1460–1481. [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Hughes RB, Lund EM, Gabrielli J, Powers LE, Curry MA. Prevalence of interpersonal violence against community-living adults with disabilities: A literature review. Rehabil Psychol. 2011;56(4):302–319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Hughes RB, Robinson-Whelen S, Raymaker D, et al. . The relation of abuse to physical and psychological health in adults with developmental disabilities. Disabil Health J. 2019;12(2):227–234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. National Autistic Society. Careless Campaign Report. 2014. National Autistic Society. [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Wirral Autistic Society. Mate Crime in Merseyside. 2014. Wirral Autistic Society. [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Papadopoulos C. Autism stigma and the role of ethnicity and culture. Netw Autism. 2016.
  • 12. Griffiths S, Allison C, Kenny R, Holt R, Smith P, Baron-Cohen S. The vulnerability experiences quotient (VEQ): A study of vulnerability, mental health and life satisfaction in autistic adults. Autism Res. 2019;12(10):1516–1528. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Reuben KE, Stanzione CM, Singleton JL. Interpersonal trauma and posttraumatic stress in autistic adults. Autism Adulthood. 2021;3(3):247–256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Cappadocia MC, Weiss JA, Pepler D. Bullying experiences among children and youth with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42(2):266–277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Chevallier C, Kohls G, Troiani V, Brodkin ES, Schultz RT. The social motivation theory of autism. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16(4):231–239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Bishop-Fitzpatrick L, Mazefsky CA, Eack SM. The combined impact of social support and perceived stress on quality of life in adults with autism spectrum disorder and without intellectual disability. Autism. 2018;22(6):703–711. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Müller E, Schuler A, Yates GB. Social challenges and supports from the perspective of individuals with Asperger syndrome and other autism spectrum disabilities. Autism. 2008;12(2):173–190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Sosnowy C, Silverman C, Shattuck P, Garfield T. Setbacks and successes: How young adults on the autism spectrum seek friendship. Autism Adulthood. 2019;1(1):44–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Sedgewick F, Hill V, Pellicano E. ‘It's different for girls’: Gender differences in the friendships and conflict of autistic and neurotypical adolescents. Autism. 2018;23(5):1119–1132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Sedgewick F, Crane L, Hill V, Pellicano E. Friends and lovers: The relationships of autistic and neurotypical women. Autism Adulthood. 2019;1(2):112–123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Chandler RJ, Russell A, Maras KL. Compliance in autism: Self-report in action. Autism. 2019;23(4):1005–1017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Shakespeare T. Blaming the victim: Disability hate crime. Lancet. 2012;380(9845):P878. [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Callanan C. Taking action on hate crime. Learn Disabil Pract (through 2013). 2010;13(6):17. [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Balderston S. Victimized again? Intersectionality and injustice in disabled women's lives after hate crime and rape. In: Gendered Perspectives on Conflict and Violence: Part A. Bingley: Emerald Group Publishing Limited; 2013:17–51. [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Milton DEM. On the ontological status of autism: The ‘double empathy problem.’ Disabil Soc. 2012;27(6):883–887. [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Portway SM, Johnson B. Do you know I have Asperger's syndrome? Risks of a non-obvious disability. Health Risk Soc. 2005;7(1):73–83. [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Mathews I. Representations of vulnerability, innocence and evil in the murder of a disabled person. Disabil Soc. 2018;33(10):1620–1638. [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Cage E, Di Monaco J, Newell V. Understanding, attitudes and dehumanisation towards autistic people. Autism. 2019;23(6):1373–1383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Frost DM. Social stigma and its consequences for the socially stigmatized. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2011;5(11):824–839. [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Botha M, Frost DM. Extending the minority stress model to understand mental health problems experienced by the autistic population. Soc Ment Health. 2020;10(1):20–34. [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Milton D, Moon L. The normalisation agenda and the psycho-emotional disablement of autistic people. Auton Crit J Interdiscip Autism Stud. 2012;1(1). [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Goffman E. Stigma: Notes on the Management of Spoiled Identity. London: Penguin Books; 1963. [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Gerstenfeld PB. Hate Crimes: Causes, Controls, and Controversies. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Botha M. Academic, activist, or advocate? Angry, entangled, and emerging: A critical reflection on autism knowledge production. Front Psychol. 2021;12:4196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Gernsbacher MA. On not being human. APS Obs. 2007;20(2):5. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Jones SC, Harwood V. Representations of autism in Australian print media. Disabil Soc. 2009;24(1):5–18. [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Holton AE, Farrell LC, Fudge JL. A threatening space? Stigmatization and the framing of autism in the news. Commun Stud. 2014;65(2):189–207. [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Bronfenbrenner U. Toward an experimental ecology of human development. Am Psychol. 1977;32(7):513. [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Samuel T, Verity C, Eli G, et al. . Autism identification across ethnic groups: A narrative review. Adv Autism. 2020;7(3):241–255. [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Bargiela S, Steward R, Mandy W. The experiences of late-diagnosed women with autism spectrum conditions: An investigation of the female autism phenotype. J Autism Dev Disord. 2016;46:3281–3294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Leedham A, Thompson AR, Smith R, Freeth M. ‘I was exhausted trying to figure it out’: The experiences of females receiving an autism diagnosis in middle to late adulthood. Autism. 2020;24(1):135–146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Giwa Onaiwu M. “They don't know, don't show, or don't care”: Autism's white privilege problem. Autism in Adulthood. 2020;2(4):270–272. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Braun V, Clarke V. Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qual Res Psychol. 2006;3(2):77–101. [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Bhaskar R. Reclaiming Reality: A Critical Introduction to Contemporary Philosophy. London: Taylor & Francis; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Braun V, Clarke V. One size fits all? What counts as quality practice in (reflexive) thematic analysis? Qual Res Psychol. 2020;18(3):328–352. [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Lai M-C, Szatmari P. Resilience in autism: Research and practice prospects. 2019;23(3):539–541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Botha M. Autistic community connectedness as a buffer against the effects of minority stress. 2020. http://epubs.surrey.ac.uk/854098/7/Autistic Community Connectedness as a Buffer Against Minority Stress - M Botha.pdf (last accessed July 13, 2020).
  • 48. Rumball F, Happé F, Grey N. Experience of trauma and PTSD symptoms in autistic adults: Risk of PTSD development following DSM-5 and non-DSM-5 traumatic life events. Autism Res. 2020;13(12):2122–2132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Meyer IH. Resilience in the study of minority stress and health of sexual and gender minorities. Psychol Sex Orientat Gend Divers. 2015;2(3):209. [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Simmons J, Brüggemann AJ, Swahnberg K. Disclosing victimisation to healthcare professionals in Sweden: a constructivist grounded theory study of experiences among men exposed to interpersonal violence. BMJ Open. 2016;6(6):e010847. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Morrison KE, Pinkham AE, Kelsven S, Ludwig K, Penn DL, Sasson NJ. Psychometric evaluation of social cognitive measures for adults with autism. Autism Res. 2019;12(5):766–778. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Liem JH, Boudewyn AC. Contextualizing the effects of childhood sexual abuse on adult self- and social functioning: An attachment theory perspective. Child Abuse Negl. 1999;23(11):1141–1157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Wright MO, Crawford E, Sebastian K. Positive resolution of childhood sexual abuse experiences: The role of coping, benefit-finding and meaning-making. J Fam Violence. 2007;22(7):597–608. [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Bertilsdotter Rosqvist H. Knowing what to do: Exploring meanings of development and peer support aimed at people with autism. Int J Incl Educ. 2019;23(2):174–187. [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Dufour M, Lanovaz MJ. Increasing compliance with wearing a medical device in children with autism. J Appl Behav Anal. 2020;53(2):1089–1096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Diorio R, Bray M, Sanetti L, Kehle T. Using video self-modeling to increase compliance to classroom requests in students with autism spectrum disorder. Int J Sch Educ Psychol. 2019;7(Suppl. 1):145–157. [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Bottema-Beutel K, Crowley S, Sandbank M, Woynaroski TG. Adverse event reporting in intervention research for young autistic children. Autism. 2021;25(2):322–335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Sandoval-Norton AH, Shkedy G, Shkedy D. Long-term ABA therapy is abusive: A response to Gorycki, Ruppel, and Zane. Adv Neurodev Disord. 2021;5(2):126–134. [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Davison S. Does ABA Harm Autistic People? 2018. https://autisticuk.org/does-aba-harm-autistic-people/ (last accessed November 18, 2020).
  • 60. Sequenzia A. My thoughts on ABA. 2015. https://awnnetwork.org/my-thoughts-on-aba/ (last accessed November 18, 2020).
  • 61. Bascombe J. Quiet Hands. 2014. http://neuroqueer.blogspot.com/2014/03/quiet-hands-by-julia-bascom.html (last accessed November 18, 2020).
  • 62. Macdonald SJ, Donovan C, Clayton J. ‘I may be left with no choice but to end my torment’: Disability and intersectionalities of hate crime. Disabil Soc. 2021:1–21. [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Botha M, Dibb B, Frost DM. “Autism is me”: An investigation of how autistic individuals make sense of autism and stigma. 2020. [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Milton D, Sims T. How is a sense of well-being and belonging constructed in the accounts of autistic adults? Disabil Soc. 2016;31(4):520–534. [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Brown-Lavoie SM, Viecili MA, Weiss JA. Sexual knowledge and victimization in adults with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44(9):2185–2196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Ridout S. Establishing neurodivergent authorship in the sexual violence debate. In: Milton D, Ridout S, Martin N, Mills R, Murray D, eds. The Neurodiversity Reader. Shoreham by Sea: Pavilion; 2020:275–287. [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Gillespie-Lynch K, Kapp SK, Brooks PJ, Pickens J, Schwartzman B. Whose expertise is it? Evidence for autistic adults as critical autism experts. Front Psychol. 2017;8:438. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Dewinter J, De Graaf H, Begeer S. Sexual orientation, gender identity, and romantic relationships in adolescents and adults with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2017;47(9):2927–2934. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Crenshaw K. Mapping the margins: Intersectionality, identity politics, and violence against women of color. Stan L Rev. 1990;43:1241. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Autism in Adulthood: Challenges and Management are provided here courtesy of Mary Ann Liebert, Inc.

RESOURCES