Abstract
Empathy and altruistic behavior are more crucial abilities for pre-service teachers to possess when compared with other study fields. The relationship between empathy and altruistic behavior in Chinese pre-service teachers and their underlying mechanisms, however, has received relatively little attention in the literature. Therefore, the goal of the current study was to examine the links between study fields (i.e., pre-service teachers whose study field is pedagogy and non-pre-service teachers whose study field is non-pedagogy), self-control, emotional empathy (i.e., empathic concern), and altruistic preferences among undergraduates and graduates in five Chinese universities (the age range of participants is 18–20 years; 58.4% women) with the Interpersonal Reactivity Index-C Questionnaire, the Self-Control Scale, and the Chinese Self-Report Altruism Scale tests. The results showed a significant difference between pre-service and non-pre-service teachers in empathic concern and self-control. Furthermore, empathic concern and altruistic behavior tendency of pre-service teachers were significantly higher than those of non-pre-service teachers. Moreover, mediation analyses indicated that empathic concern partially mediated the relationship between study fields and altruistic tendency. Moderated mediation analysis further revealed that self-control buffered the relation between empathic concern and altruistic behavior tendency. These results demonstrate that altruistic tendency of pre-service teachers is influenced by empathic concern and self-control.
Keywords: pre-service teachers, non-pre-service teachers, altruistic tendency, self-control, empathic concern
Introduction
InChina, a saying goes, “it takes ten years to grow a tree, whereas a hundred years to cultivate a good man.” Teachers play an essential role in students’ growth and development. Teachers’ altruistic behaviors will affect students, which has an impact on the future development of the country and society. In the current Chinese education system, the primary source of teachers is pre-service students with professional education knowledge in a normal university. In China, normal universities are universities that prioritize teacher education (Wang and Zhao, 2021). Pre-service teachers refer to undergraduate or graduate students whose study field is pedagogy and who undertake a teacher education curriculum to qualify for a degree in education (National Council for Accreditation for Accreditation of Teacher Education [NCATE], 2002). Some studies found that altruistic behavior among youth groups in contemporary society has been weakened (Tettegah and Anderson, 2007; Haddara and Lingard, 2017), which will undoubtedly bring a series of adverse effects on the construction of a harmonious society. Pre-service teachers are a part of the youth group who take the responsibility of educating students, so it is of paramount significance to evaluate the altruistic behavior of pre-service teachers.
Existing studies found that it is more conducive for teachers to become the promoters of students’ learning if teachers can actively perceive students’ emotions and feelings (Viadero, 2004) and even influence students’ later careers and interpersonal communication (Myrick, 2003). Therefore, society has more positive expectations of teachers, requiring teachers to have more altruistic behaviors.
Altruistic behavior refers to a kind of behavior in which an individual would rather sacrifice his interests to meet the needs of others (Fehr and Fischbacher, 2003; Fehr and Rockenbach, 2004). According to previous research, motivations for altruistic behavior include negative emotions (Blair and Mitchell, 2009; Kimonis et al., 2019; Thielmann et al., 2020), a desire for fairness (Fehr et al., 2008; Engelmann and Tomasello, 2019), and moral emotions (de Oliveira-Souza et al., 2016). Altruistic behavior, however, is a decision behavior that demands rational analysis by the brain rather than just being a simple, intuitive behavior. According to neuroscientific studies, the anterior cingulate cortex and the anterior insula are activated when people see other people in pain. The degree of activation in these brain regions significantly correlates with a person’s capacity for empathy. More importantly, the response of the anterior insula and anterior cingulate cortex can accurately predict individuals’ later helping behavior (Hein et al., 2010). In addition, the arousal-cost-reward model proposed by Penner et al. (2005) suggests that when someone’s distress induces the individual’s empathy arousal, empathy arousal will lead to an individual’s negative emotions, which motivates the individual to take some actions to alleviate these negative emotions. Altruistic behavior is one of the ways to achieve this goal. Thus, empathy, an individual’s ability to perceive and understand others’ emotions and respond appropriately (Decety and Svetlova, 2012; Decety et al., 2016), is an essential mediator between individuals and altruistic behaviors (Davis and Kraus, 1997).
Empathy includes both affective empathy and cognitive empathy (Gladstein, 1983). Cognitive empathy is a top-down ability that allows people to think about problems from the perspective of others, primarily including perspective taking, whereas affective empathy is a bottom-up ability that will enable people to perceive the emotions of others, principally including empathic concern (Decety and Meyer, 2008; Heyes, 2018).
Previous studies examined the prediction of affective empathy on altruism (Batson and Coke, 1981; Batson and Toi, 1982; Batson, 1997; Cialdini et al., 1997; Klimecki et al., 2016). Batson et al. (2007) proposed that empathic concern will arise when (a) another person’s welfare is valued terminally, not as an instrumental means to self-benefit, and (b) that person is perceived to be in need. As teachers, when we desire to help students actively, the awakening of empathic concern will allow us to perceive students’ emotions and feelings to better help students. Cialdini et al. (1997) also suggest that empathic concern for others results in selflessness and true altruism. Notably, empathic concern affects helping primarily as an emotional signal of oneness. Namely, teachers are also “students” who have experienced the learning difficulties their students are experiencing. Consequently, they can perceive more of themselves in the other (i.e., their students). In addition, studies on altruistic punishment found that, when subjects had both the choice of “helping the victim” and “punishing the perpetrator,” there was a significant positive correlation between empathic concern and helping the victim (Leliveld et al., 2012; Hu et al., 2015). Studies on teachers’ empathy found that teachers’ ability to empathize is beneficial for addressing school bullying (Tettegah and Anderson, 2007) and is positively correlated with students’ prosocial behavior in self-report (Raskauskas et al., 2010). Moreover, as students grow through life, they will not always take the initiative to tell teachers their current emotional feelings. Therefore, teachers must be able to perceive students’ emotions and feelings accurately. Affective empathy plays a significant role in this process. Consequently, affective empathy is regarded as a necessary trait and competency for normal students relative to students in other study fields, contributing to students’ overall development and teachers’ professional growth (Peck et al., 2014).
Studies on self-control suggest that self-control also plays a vital role in altruistic behavior. Self-control is the ability of individuals to control their consciousness (including thoughts) and behaviors in a goal-oriented fashion (Bowers et al., 2011). As has been argued, negative emotions are part of the motivation for altruistic behavior (Cialdini et al., 1981; Cialdini and Trost, 1998; Jordan et al., 2016b; Kimonis et al., 2019; Thielmann et al., 2020). When the individual as an observer finds that others are in distress, empathic arousal will induce the individual’s negative emotions and lead to altruistic behavior (Cialdini et al., 1987; Nelissen and Zeelenberg, 2009). However, altruistic behavior requires individuals to sacrifice their interests (Jordan et al., 2016a). In this case, it is necessary to restrain selfishness through self-control to act in altruistic behaviors (Müller-Leinß et al., 2018). In addition, there is a need when it comes to self-control to modulate the relationship between negative emotions and altruistic behavior in this process of restraining selfishness through self-control to act in altruistic behaviors (Eisenberg et al., 1998; Grecucci et al., 2013). Therefore, self-control is another factor that mediates the relationship between negative emotions generated by empathy and altruistic behavior (Krueger and Hoffman, 2016; Bellucci et al., 2017). Other studies found moral emotions are also the motivation for altruistic behavior (de Oliveira-Souza et al., 2016). Moral emotions will surface when a person, acting as an observer, notices that others are being treated unfairly. At this moment, individuals need to adjust their moral emotions through self-control to decide whether to make altruistic behaviors or not. In lesion studies employing economic games as surrogates of moral emotions like guilt and envy, participants’ decisions to punish or donate reflected moralistic punishment and generous response inclinations. Unquestionably, their decisions to punish or donate are regulated by self-control (Krajbich et al., 2009). That is to say, the moral emotions aroused by the violations of others will predict the following punishment behavior. If the individual does not want to be retaliated against by others or does not want to lose profits, in that case, it is paramount to control themself to not punish the violator. In addition, from the perspective of the viewer (or witness), the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex integrates representations of intent with the agent’s actual behavior (completed harm vs. no harm at all) to come up with a final condemning or exculpating judgment (Young and Saxe, 2008). It is universally acknowledged that the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is a crucial brain area for self-control (Miller and Cohen, 2001; Delgado et al., 2005; Knoch et al., 2006), which indicates that self-control modulates an individual’s moral emotions to determine whether to act altruistically. As empathy is one of the moral emotions, self-control should also play a moderating role between empathic concern and altruistic behavior in theory.
Based on the literature review, we propose the following hypotheses: First, compared with non-pre-service teachers, pre-service teachers exemplify more altruistic tendencies and empathic concern; second, empathic concern mediates the relationship between study fields and altruistic tendency; and third, self-control mediates the relationship between empathic concern and altruistic tendency (Figure 1).
FIGURE 1.
The proposed theoretical model.
Materials and methods
Participants
A total of 841 undergraduate and graduate students whose study fields include pedagogy, psychology, literature, sociology, engineering, and science from five universities in China participated in this study. The 4-year undergraduate education system provides sufficient time for pre-service teachers to systematically learn professional theories and cultivate their practical technical ability (Hu et al., 2021). Therefore, these undergraduates whose study field is pedagogy and who are students that are willing to become teachers in future were grouped as pre-service teachers. Participation in the present study was entirely voluntary, and no compensation was given to the participants for their participation. To abide by local government policies, the study questionnaire was distributed to potential participants electronically via Wen Juan Xing. This platform provides functions equivalent to Amazon Mechanical Turk (Changsha Ranxing Science and Technology, Changsha, China), and no face-to-face contact was made. All participants consented to participation, and data were anonymized. People younger than 18 years or older than 26 years who did not completely fill out the demographic section or whose answers were all the same were exempted from the analyses. To ensure the validity of the data, we also eliminated the scores of each questionnaire according to three standard deviations. Of the remaining 741 participants, 58.4% were women (Mage = 21.44, SD = 2.06, range = 18–26). Descriptive statistics of specific demographic variables are given in Table 1.
TABLE 1.
Descriptive statistics of specific demographic variables.
Variable | Options | Frequency | Percentage |
Gender | Men | 308 | 41.6% |
Women | 433 | 58.4% | |
Grade | Freshman | 134 | 18.1% |
Sophomore | 133 | 17.9% | |
Junior | 120 | 16.2% | |
Senior | 91 | 12.3% | |
1 Master | 181 | 24.4% | |
2 Master | 50 | 6.7% | |
3 Master | 32 | 4.3% | |
Study fields | Pedagogy | 309 | 41.7% |
Non-pedagogy | 432 | 58.3% |
Questionnaire
Interpersonal reactivity index-C questionnaire
The Interpersonal Reactivity Index-C (IRI-C) Questionnaire, which was revised by Zhang et al. (2010) based on IRI (Davis, 1980), was used to measure the empathic ability of Chinese participants. The revised questionnaire consisted of 22 questions divided into four subscales: perspective taking (PT, the tendency to adopt the point of view of other people), empathic concern (EC, the tendency to experience feelings of warmth, compassion, and concern for other people), personal distress (PD, one’s own feelings of personal unease and discomfort in reaction to the emotions of others), and fantasy (FS, an exciting and unusual experience or situation you imagine happening, but which will probably never happen). Individuals rated each item on a five-point Likert scale, with 0 indicating “very inappropriate” and 4 indicating “very appropriate,” with higher scores indicating higher empathy. Cronbach’s alpha of IRI-C was 0.80.
Self-control scale
The present study adopted SCS to measure the self-control ability of Chinese participants, which is based on Tangney et al. (2004) Self-Control Scale. Nineteen items were preserved in view of cultural differences and reliability (Tan and Guo, 2008). The scale still was divided into five subscales: controlling impulses (six items, such as “I am too prone to lose my temper”), keeping healthy habits (three items, such as “I am lazy”), resisting temptation (four items, such as “I can resist the temptation”), focusing on work (three items, such as “I can’t concentrate”), and controlling entertainment (three items, such as “I do something that will give me pleasure but do harm to myself”). All items were measured on a five-point Likert scale, with 1 indicating “strongly disagree” and 5 indicating “strongly agree,” with higher scores indicating greater self-control. Cronbach’s alpha of SCS was 0.87.
Chinese self-report altruism scale
The Chinese Self-Report Altruism Scale (Li, 2008), which researchers in mainland China frequently use, was adopted in the current study to accurately describe the altruistic behavior of young people in mainland China. There were 22 questions in the Chinese Self-Report Altruism Scale, broken up into five subscales: Responsible Altruistic Behavior, Respect and Care for Others, Care and Focus on Yourself, Altruistic Behavior Performance and Fantasy, and Egoistic Behaviors and Perceptions. Higher scores indicated greater altruism. Participants rated each item on a seven-point Likert scale, with 1 denoting “very inappropriate” and 7 denoting “very appropriate.” Cronbach’s alpha of the Chinese Self-Report Altruism Scale was 0.87.
Common method biases test
The current study’s data were gathered using Interpersonal Response Index Inventory (IRI-C), the Self-Control Scale, and the Chinese Self-Report Altruism Scale (Li, 2008). As a result, there might be widespread method biases at play here. To achieve this, the questionnaire design and the response procedure must be strictly controlled, and the data must also undergo a single-factor test for statistical analysis. Specifically, the results of the unrotated principal component analysis were examined by conducting exploratory factor analysis on all items, and a serious common method bias was determined if only one factor or a common factor had particularly high explanatory power (Eby and Dobbins, 1997; Livingstone et al., 1997; Podsakoff et al., 2003); if multiple factors with eigenvalues greater than 1 are obtained and the amount of variation explained by the first factor does not exceed 40%, then the common method bias is not severe (Ashford and Tsui, 1991). According to the test results, there are 15 factors with eigenvalues greater than 1 in unrotated principal component analysis, and the first factor accounts for 18.48% of the variance. Therefore, the current study’s common method bias issue is not major.
Data analysis
The objectives of this research were to see whether empathic concern played a mediating role between study field and altruistic tendency in undergraduate and graduate students and, if so, whether self-control played a moderating role in the latter path between empathic concern and altruistic tendency. These research questions were tested in three steps. First, the descriptive statistics and bivariate Pearson’s correlations were calculated. Second, the mediating effect of empathic concern was examined by using PROCESS macro for SPSS (Model 4) (Hayes, 2017). Third, the analyses of the moderating effect of self-control on the latter links between empathy and altruistic tendency were constructed by applying the PROCESS macro (Model 14). All study continuous variables were standardized, and the models utilized 5,000 resamples through bootstrapping confidence intervals (CIs) to determine whether the effects in PROCESS Model 4 and Model 14 were significant (Hayes, 2017).
Results
Correlation between study field, altruistic tendency, empathic concern, and self-control
The Pearson correlations are presented in Table 2. The study field was positively correlated with altruistic tendency and empathic concern. Altruistic tendency was positively associated with empathic concern and self-control. Empathic concern was also positively correlated with altruistic tendency.
TABLE 2.
Pearson’s correlation coefficient.
Variables | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 |
1. Study field | — | |||
2. Altruistic tendency | 0.14** | — | ||
3. Empathic concern | 0.12** | 0.62** | — | |
4. Self-control | 0.03 | 0.32** | 0.17** | — |
Study field was dummy coded as 0 = non-pedagogy; 1 = pedagogy.
**p < 0.01.
Analysis of empathic concern as a mediator
To test the mediating effect of empathic concern, we used Model 4 of the SPSS macro PROCESS complied by Hayes (2017). The regression results for testing mediation are reported in Table 3. After controlling for gender and age, the results indicated that study field was positively related to empathic concern (b = 0.16, p < 0.05); in addition, study field and empathic concern were positively associated with altruistic tendency (b = 0.14, p < 0.05; b = 0.61, p < 0.001). As the direct predictive effect of the study field on altruistic behavioral tendency was significant (b = 0.24, p < 0.01), empathic concern partially mediated the association between study field and altruistic tendency. The bias-corrected percentile bootstrap analyses further showed that the mediation effect accounted for 42% of the total effect of the study field on altruistic tendency; the mediating effect was 0.10, with a 95% CI of [0.0148, 0.1894].
TABLE 3.
Analysis of empathic concern as a mediator.
Regression equation |
Fit index |
Regression coefficient |
|||||
Dependent variable | Independent variable | R | R 2 | F | B | SE | t |
Empathic concern | Study field | 0.24 | 0.06 | 15.35 | 0.16 | 2.19 | 7.43* |
Altruistic tendency | Study field | 0.62 | 0.39 | 116.83 | 0.14 | 0.06 | −2.28* |
Empathic concern | 0.61 | 0.03 | 20.41*** |
*p < 0.05 and ***p < 0.001.
Analysis of self-control as a moderator
To examine whether the latter indirect relationships between study field and altruistic tendency via empathic concern would be moderated by self-control, we used Model 14 of PROCESS macro developed by Hayes (2017). The regression results for testing the moderator are reported in Table 4. The results showed that self-control positively interacted with empathic concern in predicting altruistic tendency. The interaction effect is visually plotted in Figure 2. Simple slope tests revealed that empathic concern had a significant positive effect on altruistic tendency in high- and low-level self-control. The effect of empathic concern on altruistic tendency was weaker for college students with high levels of self-control (b simple = 0.50, t = 12.53, p < 0.001) than for those with low levels of self-control (b simple = 0.62, t = 18.34, p < 0.001), and the mediating effects of self-control at different levels are reported in Table 5.
TABLE 4.
Analysis of self-control as a moderator.
Regression equation | Fit index |
Regression coefficient |
||||
Dependent variable | Independent variable | R | R 2 | F(df) | B | t |
Altruistic tendency | 0.66 | 0.44 | 96.07*** | |||
Empathic concern | 0.56 | 19.02*** | ||||
Self-control | 0.22 | 7.82*** | ||||
EC × SC | –0.06 | −2.61** |
EC is empathic concern and SC is self-control.
**p < 0.01 and ***p < 0.001.
FIGURE 2.
Interaction graphs.
TABLE 5.
Mediating effects of self-control at different levels.
Self-control | Effect | Boot SE | Boot LLCI | Boot ULCI | |
Level of self-control | 47.79 (M-1SD) | 0.62 | 0.034 | 0.55 | 0.68 |
58.03 (M) | 0.56 | 0.030 | 0.50 | 0.61 | |
68.27 (M + 1SD) | 0.50 | 0.040 | 0.42 | 0.58 |
Discussion
The current study aimed to investigate the relationship between study field and altruistic tendency. In addition, it is of paramount importance to check whether empathic concern mediates the relationship between study fields and altruistic tendency and whether self-control moderates the relationship between empathic concern and altruistic tendency. The results revealed that study fields were negatively correlated with empathic concern and altruistic tendency, while empathic concern was positively correlated with altruistic tendency. Pre-service teachers show more altruistic tendencies and empathic concern than non-pre-service teachers. More importantly, empathic concern mediates between study fields and altruistic tendencies. Moreover, we found that self-control moderates empathic concern and altruistic tendency.
In terms of study field differences in altruistic tendency, pre-service teachers have a higher altruistic tendency than non-pre-service teachers, which is consistent with previous studies (Bostic, 2014). First, some studies found that teachers have a high sense of professional identity (Zhang et al., 2016; Wang et al., 2017; Weinberg et al., 2021). According to social identity theory, professionals will identify with both their occupation and their organization at the same time (Dutton et al., 1994; Tajfel and Turner, 2004), and further studies found that employees’ professional identity can promote the generation of their organizational identity (Meixner and Bline, 1989). The term “organizational identity” refers to an individual’s perceptual cognition and emotional sense of belonging to the organization to which they belong. This psychological foundation underscores their preference for altruistic behaviors, such as upholding the organization and assisting its members (Ashforth et al., 2008). Gaziel (1995) found that teachers’ professional identity was significantly negatively associated with their willingness to leave the workplace and their jobs. So teachers’ professional identity will affect their organizational identity. In addition, individuals’ organizational identity is positively related to good interpersonal relationships between individuals and their colleagues (Morgan, 1986). Because of the good internal relationship among pre-service teachers, they are more willing to regard their classmates or colleagues as in-group individuals. According to the in-group favoritism theory (Tajfel et al., 1971; Vermue et al., 2019), people are more likely to assist those who share their identity. Hence, pre-service teachers have a more significant concern for empathy than non-teachers. Second, social desirability refers to the internal psychological tendency of individuals to try their best to make their behaviors and ideas meet the needs of society and the masses, expect to be recognized by society, and maintain their self-image (Perinelli and Gremigni, 2016). From ancient times, Chinese society has expected teachers to be proactive and to go out of their way to help students. According to the social desirability theory, excellent interpersonal desirability will promote individuals’ careers to shape the corresponding self-belief and self-requirement (Carlo et al., 1991; Braun et al., 2001; Lalwani et al., 2006; Wanat et al., 2020; Lanz et al., 2022). Consequently, the expectation of Chinese society that teachers should actively assist students will motivate teachers to meet and maintain this social expectation throughout their academic and professional careers (Xuan, 2006; Li, 2017).
In terms of study field differences in empathic concern, pre-service teachers have higher empathic concern than non-pre-service teachers. Certain emotions always accompany students’ learning. Teachers with high empathic concern can timely pay attention to students, empathize with students’ emotional changes, and adjust their teaching methods simultaneously (McAllister and Irvine, 2002). Coffman (1981) found that teachers with a high empathic concern could communicate with their students in a way that made them feel understood and that their emotions were felt. Students were inspired to change their attitudes toward learning, encouraging them to take the initiative to learn and facilitating their academic success. When teachers pay good emotional and empathic attention to their students during the teaching process, it enables students to develop in the long run. This is another way that teachers’ empathy can enable the students to socialize (Peart and Campbell, 1999). Moreover, to meet pre-service teacher’s own developmental needs, empathic skills are also trained in their daily courses that include experiential training (Kolb et al., 2001), skills training (Redman, 1977), video training (Barone et al., 2005; Liu, 2012; Shin, 2017; Innamorati et al., 2019; Liu and Ren, 2019), and practice mindfulness (Kabat-Zinn, 1990). Pre-service teachers are trained in these daily courses during their academic career, so their empathic attention skills are high compared with other study fields.
Regarding the mediating role of empathic concern, the results showed that empathic concern mediated the relation between study field and altruistic tendency. Previous studies have looked into the effect that affective empathy has on altruism by making predictions about it (Batson and Coke, 1981; Batson and Toi, 1982; Batson, 1997; Cialdini et al., 1997; Lahvis, 2017; Crockett and Lockwood, 2018; Canevello et al., 2021; Miyazono and Inarimori, 2021). In the group of pre-service teachers, empathic concern is a desirable trait for their professional development. They can be trained in empathy through courses and by watching empathy videos (Liu, 2012; Shin, 2017; Innamorati et al., 2019; Liu and Ren, 2019). Eventually, they are easily motivated to perceive others’ emotions and feelings, which makes them more willing to help others and thus demonstrates a stronger altruistic tendency than students of other study fields. Cialdini et al. (1997) ever suggested empathic concern for another result in selflessness and true altruism. Batson et al. (2007) also proposed that empathic concern arises when (a) another person’s welfare is valued terminally, not as an instrumental means to self-benefit and (b) that person is perceived to be in need. Accordingly, the emergence of empathic concern will enable teachers to perceive students’ emotions and feelings to assist students more effectively when they want to help them actively. Compared with pre-service teachers, non-pre-service teachers are not professionally trained in empathy, and empathy is not a necessary trait and ability for them. Consequently, their empathic attention ability is relatively low, and it is not easy for them to empathize with others’ unfortunate circumstances, so their altruistic tendency is low. Previous studies showed that empathy is a crucial mediating variable between individual’s negative emotions and altruistic behavior (Davis and Kraus, 1997). Individuals with high empathic concern are more likely to feel the negative emotions of others and thus are more inclined to help others (Cialdini et al., 1981; Cialdini et al., 1987; Cialdini and Trost, 1998; de Waal, 2008; Hu et al., 2015; Decety et al., 2016). Therefore, pre-service teachers not only have higher altruistic tendencies but also understand and enter into others’ feelings through empathic concern, thereby enhancing their altruistic tendency.
Regarding the moderating role of self-control for empathic concern, the results demonstrate that, for individuals with high empathic concern, the lower the self-control, the greater the altruistic tendency, whereas for individuals with low empathic concern, the higher the self-control, the greater the altruistic tendency. According to previous studies, the factors influencing altruistic behavior include negative emotions (Cialdini et al., 1981; Cialdini et al., 1987; Cialdini and Trost, 1998; Darley and Pittman, 2003; Blair and Mitchell, 2009; Kimonis et al., 2019; Thielmann et al., 2020) and selfishness (McAuliffe et al., 2015; Yang et al., 2018). In the process of altruistic decision making, individuals face a cognitive conflict. That is, the negative emotions that individuals generate when they observe others in unfavorable situations will drive them to make altruistic behaviors, but altruistic behavior requires sacrificing their interests simultaneously. Therefore, individuals need self-control to resolve the conflict between self-interested motives and emotional impulses (McAuliffe et al., 2015; Yang et al., 2018). According to previous studies, negative emotions and motivations to help others aroused by empathy will surpass selfish motivation, so individuals with higher empathy are more inclined to help others (de Waal, 2008; Hu et al., 2015). Therefore, for individuals with a high empathic concern, the lower their self-control ability, the less they are capable of repressing the negative emotions they experience when witnessing the suffering of others and the greater their willingness to engage in altruistic behavior. However, individuals with strong self-control can control their negative emotions and consider their interests rationally when deciding whether or not to engage in altruistic behavior. In contrast, for individuals with low empathic concern, others’ misfortune does not evoke strong negative emotions, and whether or not to help others at that time is primarily influenced by selfish motivations; that is, individuals with low self-control ability are unable to control their selfish motivations, so they are hesitant to engage in altruistic behavior. Individuals with high self-control can control their selfishness, make rational cognitive decisions, and engage in more altruistic behavior. Other studies suggested that moral emotions are also predictors of individual altruistic behaviors (de Oliveira-Souza et al., 2016). Moral emotions motivate individuals to behave altruistically when they notice others being treated unfairly. Nevertheless, whether they will help the victim next is modulated by self-control (Young and Saxe, 2008; Krajbich et al., 2009). If the individual does not want to be retaliated against by the offender or does not want to lose his interest, then he needs to control himself and not punish the offender.
It is essential to be aware of some restrictions on this study. First, in conjunction with previous research, perspective taking, personal distress, and fantasy are also predictors of altruistic behavior (Oswald, 1996; Tusche et al., 2016; Pan et al., 2022). However, in the present study, we did not find a significant role for perspective taking, personal distress, and fantasy in the altruistic behavior of the pre-service teachers. Therefore, it is well worth further exploring the role of perspective taking, personal distress, and fantasy in different groups of study fields in future. Second, in the training courses for normal students in a normal Chinese university, pre-service teachers will be taught how to accurately perceive students’ emotions and feelings to take appropriate measures to help students overcome difficulties. Notwithstanding, this can easily lead to a question: Does learning a particular study field affect empathic concern? The present study did not provide a satisfying response to this question, but future research might examine changes in teachers’ empathic concerns. Third, the cross-sectional design of this study failed to confirm causal relationships between study fields, altruistic tendency, and empathic concern. Therefore, future longitudinal research is required to establish the causal relationship. Fourth, the current study collected data through questionnaires, which may have reduced the results’ reliability. Accordingly, future research could recruit students from different study fields to explore the relationship between study fields, empathic attention, and altruistic behavior through altruistic punishment games. Finally, the sample was limited to college students from a university in the Midwest. Therefore, it is conceivable that the sample does not adequately represent the majority of pre-service teachers in China.
Meanwhile, it should be noted that the current study allows us to discover the relationship and mechanism between empathic concern and altruistic preference in this particular sample. Hopefully, it can also provide some guidance for the future curriculum setting of teacher education.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Ethics Committee of the Institute of Brain and Psychological Sciences, Sichuan Normal University. The protocol was reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Institute of Brain and Psychological Sciences, Sichuan Normal University (approval no. SCNU-210602). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants for their participation in this study.
Author contributions
ML was responsible for the current study’s design, data collection, interpretation of data for the work, manuscript writing, and revising. WL contributed to the data collection and data analysis. QY and LH contributed to the revision of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
This work was supported by the Educational Commission of Sichuan province of China (No. JG2018-654) and Foundation of Sichuan Normal University (No. 340856008).
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
- Ashforth B. E., Harrison S. H., Corley K. G. (2008). Identification in organizations: an examination of four fundamental questions. J. Manag. 34 325–374. [Google Scholar]
- Ashford S. J., Tsui A. S. (1991). Self-regulation for managerial effectiveness: the role of active feedback seeking. Acad. Manag. J. 34 251–280. 10.5465/256442 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Barone D. F., Hutchings P. S., Kimmel H., Traub H. L., Cooper J. T., Marshall C. M. (2005). Increasing empathic accuracy through practice and feedback in a clinical interviewing course. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 24 156–171. 10.1521/jscp.24.2.156.62275 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Batson C. D. (1997). Self-other merging and the empathy-altruism hypothesis : reply to Neuberg (1997). J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 73 517–522. 10.1037/0022-3514.73.3.517 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Batson C. D., Coke J. S. (1981). “Empathy: A source of altruistic motivation for helping?,” in Altruism and Helping Behavior: Social, Personality, and Developmental Perspectives, eds Rushton J. P., Sorrentino R. M. (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; ), 167–211. [Google Scholar]
- Batson C. D., Toi M. (1982). More evidence that empathy is a source of altruistic motivation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 43 281–292. 10.1037/0022-3514.43.2.281 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Batson C. D., Eklund J. H., Chermok V. L., Hoyt J. L., Ortiz B. G. (2007). An additional antecedent of empathic concern: valuing the welfare of the person in need. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 93 65–74. 10.1037/0022-3514.93.1.65 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bellucci G., Chernyak S., Hoffman M., Deshpande G., Dal Monte O., Knutson K. M., et al. (2017). Effective connectivity of brain regions underlying third-party punishment: functional MRI and Granger causality evidence. Soc. Neurosci. 12 124–134. 10.1080/17470919.2016.1153518 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blair R. J., Mitchell D. G. (2009). Psychopathy, attention and emotion. Psychol. Med. 39 543–555. 10.1017/S0033291708003991 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bostic T. B. (2014). Teacher empathy and its relationship to the standardized test scores of diverse secondary english students. J. Res. Educ. 24 3–16. [Google Scholar]
- Bowers E. P., Gestsdottir S., Geldhof G. J., Nikitin J., von Eye A., Lerner R. M. (2011). Developmental trajectories of intentional self-regulation in adolescence: the role of parenting and implications for positive and problematic outcomes among diverse youth. J. Adol. 34 1193–206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braun H. I., Jackson D. N., Wiley D. E. (2001). “Socially desirable responding: the evolution of a construct,”in The Role of Constructs in Psychological and Educational Measurement, eds Braun H. I., Jackson D. N., Wiley D. E. (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; ), 49–69. 10.4324/9781410607454 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Canevello A., Hall J., Walsh J. I. (2021). Empathy-mediated altruism in intergroup contexts: the roles of posttraumatic stress and posttraumatic growth. Emotion. 10.1037/emo0000803 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlo G., Eisenberg N., Troyer D., Switzer G., Speer A. L. (1991). The altruistic personality: in what contexts is it apparent? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 61 450–458. 10.1037//0022-3514.61.3.450 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cialdini R. B., Trost M. R. (1998). “Social influence: social norms, conformity and compliance,” in The Handbook of Social Psychology, eds Gilbert D. T., Fiske S. T., Lindzey G. (New York, NY: McGraw-Hill; ), 151–192. [Google Scholar]
- Cialdini R. B., Baumann D. J., Kenrick D. T. (1981). Insights from sadness: a three-step model of the development of altruism as hedonism. Dev. Rev. 1 207–223. 10.1016/0273-2297(81)90018-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cialdini R. B., Brown S. L., Lewis B. P., Luce C., Neuberg S. L. (1997). Reinterpreting the empathy-altruism relationship: when one into one equals oneness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 73 481–494. 10.1037/0022-3514.73.3.481 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cialdini R. B., Schaller M., Houlihan D., Arps K., Fultz J., Beaman A. L. (1987). Empathy-based helping: is it selflessly or selfishly motivated? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 52 749–758. 10.1037//0022-3514.52.4.749 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coffman S. L. (1981). Empathy as a relevant instructor variable in the experiential classroom. Group Organ. Manag. 6 114–120. 10.1177/105960118100600111 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Crockett M. J., Lockwood P. L. (2018). Extraordinary altruism and transcending the self. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22 1071–1073. 10.1016/j.tics.2018.09.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Darley J. M., Pittman T. S. (2003). The psychology of compensatory and retributive justice. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 7 324–336. 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0704_05 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 10:85. [Google Scholar]
- Davis M. H., Kraus L. A. (1997). “Personality and empathic accuracy,” in Empathic Accuracy, ed. Ickes W. (New York, NY: Guilford; ), 144–168. [Google Scholar]
- de Oliveira-Souza R., Zahn R., Moll J. (2016). “The neuropsychiatry of social behavior and the cerebral organization of morality,” in Moral Brains. The Neuroscience of Morality, ed. Liao S. M. (New York, NY: Oxford University Press; ), 203–236. 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199357666.003.0010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- de Waal F. B. (2008). Putting the altruism back into altruism: the evolution of empathy. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 59 279–300. 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Decety J., Meyer M. (2008). From emotion resonance to empathic understanding: a social developmental neuroscience account. Dev. Psychopathol. 20 1053–1080. 10.1017/S0954579408000503 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Decety J., Svetlova M. (2012). Putting together phylogenetic and ontogenetic perspectives on empathy. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2 1–24. 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.05.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Decety J., Bartal I. B., Uzefovsky F., Knafo-Noam A. (2016). Empathy as a driver of prosocial behaviour: highly conserved neurobehavioural mechanisms across species. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 371:20150077. 10.1098/rstb.2015.0077 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delgado M. R., Frank R. H., Phelps E. A. (2005). Perceptions of moral character modulate the neural systems of reward during the trust game. Nat. Neurosci. 8 1611–1618. 10.1038/nn1575 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dutton J. E., Dukerich J. M., Harquail C. V. (1994). Organizational images and member identification. Admin. Sci. Quart. 39 239–263. 10.2307/2393235 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Eby L. T., Dobbins G. H. (1997). Collectivistic orientation in teams: an individual and group-level analysis. J. Organ. Behav. 18 275–295. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Eisenberg N., Wentzel N., Harris J. D. (1998). The role of emotionality and regulation in empathy-related responding. Sch. Psychol. Rev. 27 506–521. 10.1080/02796015.1998.12085934 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Engelmann J. M., Tomasello M. (2019). Children’s sense of fairness as equal respect. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23 454–463. 10.1016/j.tics.2019.03.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fehr E., Fischbacher U. (2003). The nature of human altruism. Nature 425, 785–791. 10.1038/nature02043 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fehr E., Rockenbach B. (2004). Human altruism: economic, neural, and evolutionary perspectives. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 14, 784–790. 10.1016/j.conb.2004.10.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fehr E., Bernhard H., Rockenbach B. (2008). Egalitarianism in young children. Nature 454 1079–1083. 10.1038/nature07155 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaziel H. H. (1995). Sabbatical leave, job burnout and turnover intentions among teachers. Int. J. Life. Edu. 14 331–338. [Google Scholar]
- Gladstein G. A. (1983). Understanding empathy: integrating counseling, developmental, and social psychology perspectives. J. Couns. Psychol. 30 467–482. 10.1037/0022-0167.30.4.467 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Grecucci A., Brambilla P., Siugzdaite R., Londero D., Fabbro F., Rumiati R. I. (2013). Emotional resonance deficits in autistic children. J. Aut. Dev. Dis. 43 616–628. 10.1007/s10803-012-1603-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haddara W., Lingard L. (2017). Exploring the premise of lost altruism: content analysis of two codes of ethics. Adv. Health Sci. Educ. Theory Pract. 22 839–852. 10.1007/s10459-016-9713-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes A. F. (2017). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. New York, NY: Guilford Publications. [Google Scholar]
- Hein G., Silani G., Preuschoff K., Batson C. D., Singer T. (2010). Neural responses to in-group and outgroup members’ suffering predict individual differences in costly helping. Neuron 68 149–160. 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heyes C. (2018). Empathy is not in our genes. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 95 499–507. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.11.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hu C., Liao C., Luo. (2021). The study on the optimal path of pre-service training for secondary vocational school teachers. J. Jimei Univ. 22 7–13. [Google Scholar]
- Hu Y., Strang S., Weber B. (2015). Helping or punishing strangers: neural correlates of altruistic decisions as third-party and of its relation to empathic concern. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:24. 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Innamorati M., Ebisch S., Gallese V., Saggino A. (2019). A bidimensional measure of empathy: empathic experience scale. PLoS One 14:e0216164. 10.1371/journal.pone.0216164 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jordan J. J., McAuliffe K., Rand D. G. (2016b). The effects of endowment size and strategy method on third party punishment. Exp. Econom. 19 741–763. 10.1007/s10683-015-9466-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Jordan J. J., Hoffman M., Bloom P., Rand D. G. (2016a). Third-party punishment as a costly signal of trustworthiness. Nature 530 473–476. 10.1038/nature16981 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kabat-Zinn J. (1990). Full Catastrophe Living: Using the Wisdom of Your Body and Mind to Face Stress, Pain, and Illness by Jon Kabat-Zinn. New York, NY: Delta Trade Paperback. [Google Scholar]
- Kimonis E. R., Kidd J., Most S. B., Krynen A., Liu C. (2019). An elusive deficit: psychopathic personality traits and aberrant attention to emotional stimuli. Emotion 20:951–964. 10.1037/emo0000601 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klimecki O. M., Mayer S. V., Jusyte A., Scheeff J., Schönenberg M. (2016). Empathy promotes altruistic behavior in economic interactions. Sci. Rep. 6:31961. 10.1038/srep31961 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knoch D., Pascual-Leone A., Meyer K., Treyer V., Fehr E. (2006). Diminishing reciprocal fairness by disrupting the right prefrontal cortex. Science 314 829–832. 10.1126/science.1129156 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kolb D. A., Boyatzis R. E., Mainemelis C. (2001). “Experiential learning theory: previous research and new directions,” in Perspectives on Thinking, Learning, and Cognitive Styles, eds Sternberg R. J., Zhang L.-f. (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; ), 227–247. [Google Scholar]
- Krajbich I., Adolphs R., Tranel D., Denburg N. L., Camerer C. F. (2009). Economic games quantify diminished sense of guilt in patients with damage to the prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 29 2188–2192. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5086-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krueger F., Hoffman M. (2016). The emerging neuroscience of third-party punishment. Trends Neurosci. 39 499–501. 10.1016/j.tins.2016.06.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lahvis G. P. (2017). Social reward and empathy as proximal contributions to altruism: the camaraderie effect. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 30 127–157. 10.1007/7854_2016_449 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lalwani A. K., Shavitt S., Johnson T. (2006). What is the relation between cultural orientation and socially desirable responding? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 90 165–178. 10.1037/0022-3514.90.1.165 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lanz L., Thielmann I., Gerpott F. H. (2022). Are social desirability scales desirable? A meta-analytic test of the validity of social desirability scales in the context of prosocial behavior. J. Pers. 90 203–221. 10.1111/jopy.12662 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leliveld M. C., Dijk E. V., Vanbeest I. (2012). Punishing and compensating others at your own expense: the role of empathic concern on reactions to distributive injustice. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 42 135–140. 10.1002/ejsp.872 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Li L. (2017). Traditional connotation and contemporary reflection of the teachers’ sense of responsibility. Univ. Educ. Sci. 3 76–80. [Google Scholar]
- Li Y. F. (2008). The Research on the Relation between Moral Value, Self-concept and Altruistic Behavior of College Students. Postgraduate Academic Thesis, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan. [Google Scholar]
- Liu R., Ren Y. (2019). Research on effects of flow experience and empathy in immersive virtual environment. Educ. Res. 4 99–105. [Google Scholar]
- Liu Y. (2012). The Development and Use of Primary School Teachers’ Empathy Training Video, Master thesis. College of education, Zhejiang Normal University, Jinhua. [Google Scholar]
- Livingstone L. P., Nelson D. L., Barr S. H. (1997). Person-environment fit and creativity: an examination of supply-value and demand-ability versions of fit. J. Manag. 23 119–146. 10.1177/014920639702300202 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McAllister G. F., Irvine J. J. (2002). The role of empathy in teaching culturally diverse students. J. Teach. Educ. 53 433–443. 10.1177/002248702237397 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McAuliffe K., Jordan J. J., Warneken F. (2015). Costly third-party punishment in young children. Cognition 134 1–10. 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meixner W. F., Bline D. M. (1989). Professional and job-related attitudes and the behaviors they influence among governmental accountants. Acc. Audit. Account. J. 2 8–20. [Google Scholar]
- Miller E. K., Cohen J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24 167–202. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyazono K., Inarimori K. (2021). Empathy, altruism, and group identification. Front. Psychol. 12:749315. 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.749315 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morgan G. (1986). Images of Organization. Am. J. Nurs. 88: 395. [Google Scholar]
- Müller-Leinß J. M., Enzi B., Flasbeck V., Brüne M. (2018). Retaliation or selfishness? An rTMS investigation of the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in prosocial motives. Soc. Neurosci. 13 701–709. 10.1080/17470919.2017.1411828 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myrick R. D. (2003). Developmental Guidance and Counseling: A Practical Approach, 4th Edn. Minneapolis, MN: Educational Media Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- National Council for Accreditation for Accreditation of Teacher Education [NCATE] (2002). Handbook for Accreditation Visits. Washington, DC: NCATE. [Google Scholar]
- Nelissen R. M. A., Zeelenberg M. (2009). When guilt evokes self-punishment: evidence for the existence of a Dobby Effect. Emotion 9 118–122. 10.1037/a0014540 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oswald P. A. (1996). The effects of cognitive and affective perspective taking on empathic concern and altruistic helping. J. Soc. Psychol. 136 613–623. 10.1080/00224545.1996.9714045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pan Y., Liang S., Shek D. (2022). Attachment insecurity and altruistic behavior among chinese adolescents: mediating effect of different dimensions of empathy. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:10371. 10.3390/ijerph191610371 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peart N. A., Campbell F. A. (1999). At-Risk students’ perceptions of teacher effectiveness. J. Just Caring Educ. 5 269–284. [Google Scholar]
- Peck N. F., Maude S. P., Brotherson M. J. (2014). Understanding preschool teachers’ perspectives on empathy: a qualitative inquiry. Early Child. Educ. J. 43:169. 10.1007/s10643-014-0648-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Penner L. A., Dovidio J. F., Piliavin J. A., Schroeder D. A. (2005). Prosocial behavior: multilevel perspectives. Annual Rev Psychol. 56 365–392. 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070141 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perinelli E., Gremigni P. (2016). Use of social desirability scales in clinical psychology: a systematic review. J. Clin. Psychol. 72 534–551. 10.1002/jclp.22284 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Podsakoff P. M., MacKenzie S. B., Lee J. Y., Podsakoff N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 88 879–903. 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raskauskas J., Gregory J. E., Harvey S. T., Rifshana F., Evans I. M. (2010). Bullying among primary school children in New Zealand: relationships with prosocial behaviour and classroom climate. Edu. Res. 52 1–13. [Google Scholar]
- Redman G. L. (1977). Study of the relationship of teacher empathy for minority persons and inservice human relations training. J. Educ. Res. 70 205–210. [Google Scholar]
- Shin D. D. (2017). The role of affordance in the experience of virtual reality learning: technological and affective affordances in virtual reality. Telemat. Inform. 34 1826–1836. 10.1016/j.tele.2017.05.013 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Tajfel H., Turner J. C. (2004). “The social identity theory of intergroup behavior,” in Political Psychology: Key Readings, eds Jost J. T., Sidanius J. (London: Routledge; ), 276–293. 10.4324/9780203505984-16 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Tajfel H., Billig M. S., Bundy R. P., Flament C. (1971). Social categorization and intergroup behaviour. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 1 149–178. 10.1002/ejsp.2420010202 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Tan S., Guo Y. (2008). Revision of self-control scale for Chinese college students. Chin. J. Clin. Psychol. 16 3. [Google Scholar]
- Tangney J. P., Baumeister R. F., Boone A. L. (2004). High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. J. Pers. 72 271–324. 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tettegah S., Anderson C. J. (2007). Pre-Service teachers’ empathy and cognitions: statistical analysis of text data by graphical models. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 32 48–82. 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2006.10.010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Thielmann I., Spadaro G., Balliet D. (2020). Personality and prosocial behavior: a theoretical framework and meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 146 30–90. 10.1037/bul0000217 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tusche A., Böckler A., Kanske P., Trautwein F. M., Singer T. (2016). Decoding the charitable brain: empathy, perspective taking, and attention shifts differentially predict altruistic giving. J. Neurosci. 36 4719–4732. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3392-15.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vermue M., Meleady R., Seger C. R. (2019). Member-to-member generalisation in trust behaviour: how do prior experiences inform prosocial behaviour towards novel in-group and outgroup members? Curr. Psychol. 38 1003–1020. 10.1007/s12144-019-00289-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Viadero D. (2004). Declaration calls for more caring environment inschool. Educ. Week 8:10. [Google Scholar]
- Wanat M. A., Varkey D. A., Sulaica E. M., Thornton K. A., Thornton J. D. (2020). Does social desirability influence preceptors’ completion of student experiential evaluations? Am. J. Pharm. Educ. 84:aje7949. 10.5688/ajpe7949 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Q., Zhao G. (2021). ICT self-efficacy mediates most effects of university ICT support on preservice teachers’ TPACK: evidence from three normal universities in China. Br. J. Educ. Technol. 52 2319–2339. 10.1111/bjet.13141 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wang X. Q., Zhu J. C., Liu L., Chen X. Y. (2017). Cognitive-processing bias in chinese student teachers with strong and weak professional identity. Front. Psychol. 8:784. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00784 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weinberg A. E., Balgopal M. M., Sample McMeeking L. B. (2021). Professional Growth and Identity Development of STEM Teacher Educators in a Community of Practice. Int. J. Sci. Math. Edu., 19 99–120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xuan Z. (2006). The impact of social expectations on teachers’ professional development: facilitators and inhibitors. Contemp. Educ. Sci. 6 36–38. [Google Scholar]
- Yang F., Choi Y. J., Misch A., Yang X., Dunham Y. (2018). In defense of the commons: young children negatively evaluate and sanction free riders. Psychol. Sci. 29 1598–1611. 10.1177/0956797618779061 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young L., Saxe R. (2008). The neural basis of belief encoding and integration in moral judgment. Neuroimage 40 1912–1920. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.057 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang F. F., Dong Y., Wang K., Zhan Z. Y., Xie L. F. (2010). Reliability and Validity of the Chinese Version of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index-C. Chinese J. Clin. Psychol. 2 155–157. 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2010.02.019 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang Y., Hawk S. T., Zhang X., Zhao H. (2016). Chinese preservice teachers’ professional identity links with education program performance: the roles of task value belief and learning motivations. Front. Psychol. 7:573. 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00573 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.