Abstract
Background
To investigate environmental and social risk factors for myopia in children and adolescents in Germany.
Methods
1437 children aged between 3 and 18 inclusive were examined as part of the LIFE Child study based in Leipzig, Germany. Information about leisure time activities and social status was ascertained by parents and children in a questionnaire. Refractive status was attained by measuring noncycloplegic autorefraction. Myopia was defined as spherical equivalent (SE) ≤ − 0.75 D. Risk factors were identified using multiple logistic regression analysis.
Results
In multiple logistic regression analysis, myopia was significantly associated with less frequent outdoor activity (“once a week” vs. “twice a week or more”: odds ratio (OR) 4.35, 95% confidence interval (CI) 1.89–9.98, p<0.01) and longer near work sessions (1–2 h vs. < 1 h: OR 1.83, CI 1.10–3.04, p=0.02; > 3 h vs. < 1 h: OR 3.71, CI 1.43–9.61, p<0.01) after adjustment for age, sex and socioeconomic status (SES). Duration of outdoor activity, near work frequency and SES showed no significant association with myopia (p > 0.05). Children with a lower SES were involved in longer periods of outdoor and near work activities but on fewer occasions over the course of the week, although this connection was not significant.
Conclusion
Myopia is associated with environmental factors. The present findings suggest that daily exposure to sunlight and a restriction of long-duration near work activities might protect against pathological eye growth. Prevention strategies should be implemented for children at all ages.
Keywords: Myopia, Children, Refractive error, Near work, Time outdoors, Socioeconomic status
Background
There remains an underappreciation for the seriousness of myopia amongst the general public. In 2010, uncorrected refractive error (difference between vision impairment at presentation and best-corrected sight) was responsible for one in five cases of blindness and the leading cause of moderate or severe visual impairment [1]. From an economic perspective, according to an estimation by Fricke et al. [2], distance vision impairment caused by uncorrected refractive error, either directly or indirectly, leads to global productivity losses equivalent to USD 202 billion annually. At the same time, the global prevalence of myopia has increased notably in recent decades. A meta-analysis by Holden et al. included 145 studies published since 1995 and found a worldwide increase in the prevalence of myopia from 25.6% in 2000 to 32.3% in 2010 [3]. This trend has been most pronounced in East Asian populations, and particularly in countries with ‘high-pressure educational systems’ such as South Korea, China, Taiwan and Singapore [3], leading to children spending more time indoors out of the sunlight and engaged in near work as part of their studies. A remarkable study from Taiwan estimated an increase in the prevalence of myopia in 12-year-old children from 36.7% in 1983 to 60.7% in 2000, while for young people aged 16–18 in 2000, prevalence had reached 84% [4]. Globally, myopia prevalence is projected to reach approximately 49.8% in 2050 with as many as 5 billion people affected [3].
At birth, children tend to be hyperopic with positive spherical equivalents (SE) [5, 6]. The human eye grows and modifications of the cornea, lens and the axial length of the eye determine refractive status in adulthood. In most cases, these modifications lead to good visual acuity. However, if the process of axial elongation is accelerated, children are more likely to develop myopia.
Why do some eyes grow too fast? The rapid increase in the prevalence [3] and national differences in the progression of myopia [7, 8] suggest that this type of growth cannot be explained by genetic factors. Based on animal experiments finding that intraocular dopamine antagonizes stimuli for eye growth [9] and Jones et al. finding an association of time outdoors with myopia [10], Rose et al. developed the hypothesis that the protective effect of time outdoors is caused by a light-triggered dopamine release in brighter light [11]. Following experiments with vertebrates such as chickens [12] and rhesus monkeys [13] have shown that ambient illuminance has a significant influence on eye growth, although the level of brightness required to have an impact usually only originates in natural sunlight. As such, numerous studies have analyzed the association of sunlight exposure – measured as time spent outdoors – with the prevalence of myopia [10, 11, 14–19]. The suspected association of near work activities with myopia originates from studies from the 19th century, e.g., by Hermann Cohn in 1867. Cohn found that there was a greater prevalence of myopia in children who worked in dark classrooms, and an association between myopia and higher levels of schooling. As students in dark classrooms are required to work at closer distances, and a higher school level demands more studying and reading, he concluded that near work activities were responsible for myopia onset [20]. To date, this hypothesis has been tested in numerous studies examining the correlation of self-reported quantities of near work and myopia [16, 19, 21, 22].
What characteristics tend to be present in children who are at equal risk of becoming myopic? One way to define social class is with the socioeconomic status (SES). SES, or the three associated components education, income and occupational status have been found to be significantly associated with myopia in various studies [15, 23–26].
Myopia prevalence has been reported to be 41.3% among German adults aged 18–35 [5]. However, as this study assessed myopia based on self-reported noncycloplegic measurements, it might overestimate the real value. Although this prevalence remains lower than in some Asian countries, understanding the factors involved in myopia onset is crucial if we hope to appropriately respond to future shifts in prevalence or even reduce the current incidence of myopia. The KiGGS survey, a multicenter study in Germany, explored the relationship of leisure activities with myopia in a large German cohort based on questionnaires in which parents reported whether their children were myopic or not [27]. The research discussed in this paper is intended to supplement this data analysis by determining the relationship between child myopia (identified using autorefraction), time spent outdoors and in near work as well as the role of SES.
Methods
LIFE child and study population
The LIFE Child study (clinical trial no. NCT02550236) is part of LIFE, a large-scale research program implemented by the Leipzig University, Germany. LIFE Child is a prospective cohort study carried out at the Leipzig Research Center for Civilization Diseases, which seeks to assess the factors involved in the development of health and non-communicable diseases in young people from the prenatal stage to adulthood. The study launched in July 2011; participants are invited to attend annual follow-ups over a period of 10 years. The population is drawn from the city of Leipzig and its immediate surroundings. Recruitment is conducted in a range of settings, such as schools, kindergartens, local clinics, primary care practices, public health centers and partner studies. The data are collected by a trained multidisciplinary team following standard operating procedures. The majority of participating children have no migratory background (95% of both parents and 92% of all grandparents were born in Germany).
Eye examination
The data used in this paper were produced in the eye examinations carried out on study participants between the ages 3 and 18 years inclusive. For each subject, three refraction measurements were carried out for both eyes (3 mm pupil diameter and 12 mm vertex distance) using noncycloplegic autorefraction (i.Profiler® plus, Carl Zeiss Vision GmbH, Aalen, Germany [28]). The use of cycloplegics in the measurement of refractive status was not approved by the local Ethics Committee.
For further analysis, each participant was classified for the eye’s refractive error. The refractive error was defined as follows: First the SE was calculated as SE = sphere + cylinder/2; out of the three SE values calculated for each participant, the median value was selected. This practice is suggested by Rauscher et al. who showed that the true refractive error can be approximated by employing the median of repeated measurement in case measurement under cycloplegia is not an option [28]. Myopia was defined as ≤ − 0.75 D.
Questionnaire data
As part of the basic examination and eye examination, parents and children were asked to answer age-dependent questionnaires. To assess SES, parents reported their highest occupational and educational status and their total household income. Each of these pieces of data was subsequently converted to a score between 1 and 7 inclusive, with the sum of these scores used as a total SES index score for the family. The SES index scores therefore ranged from 3 to 21, with a higher score indicating higher SES. To explore differences in children’s leisure behavior between social classes, we defined three SES groups (low, medium, high) using the SES index as suggested by Lampert et al. in the context of the KiGGS study [29]. Questions about the child’s leisure activities were designed by researchers of the LIFE Child study team. They were answered by parents or, if the child was 10 years old or older, by children themselves. These items produce information about frequency per week and duration per day of reading/writing/drawing tasks (near work) and outdoor playtime. With regard to reading/writing/drawing, the activity with the highest frequency or duration was recorded. Possible responses for the frequency question were “once per week”, “two days a week”, “every two days” and “every day”, while the answer options for duration were “< 1 h”, “1–2 h”, “3–4 h” and “> 4 h”.
Data analysis
For all data analysis, we utilized the open source application R, version 3.5.3 (R Core Team 2019-03-11). From the 4820 eye examinations conducted to date, each participant’s most recent visit was filtered. This produced a more equal age distribution. Siblings and twins were excluded to prevent genetic bias in our results. After these steps, a total of 1416 examined children remained. A population characteristics table was subdivided into two age groups for comparison purposes as suggested by Schuster et al. [27]. Group differences in the SE of both eyes were tested using t-tests.
Using logistic regression models with myopia as the dependent variable, the associations between questionnaire parameters (outdoor play frequency and duration, near work frequency and duration) and myopia were analyzed. These models were then adjusted for age, sex and SES as continuous measure. Finally, we examined whether children from different SES groups showed significantly different leisure time activity patterns with a chi-squared test. The level of significance was set to α = 0.05.
Results
Study population
1416 children and adolescents between the ages of 3 and 18 inclusive were included in this study. Table 1 shows the population characteristics.
Table 1.
Population Characteristics | 3–10 years | 11–18 years |
---|---|---|
N = 620 | N = 796 | |
Mean age (years): | 7.3 | 14.9 |
Sex: | ||
Female | 296 (47.7%) | 390 (49.0%) |
Male | 324 (52.3%) | 406 (51.0%) |
Myopia: | ||
Myopic | 24 (3.9%) | 171 (21.5%) |
Non-myopic | 596 (96.1%) | 625 (78.5%) |
Socioeconomic status: | ||
Low | 65 (10.8%) | 105 (15.2%) |
Medium | 272 (45.3%) | 366 (52.8%) |
High | 263 (43.8%) | 222 (32.0%) |
Urban environment: | ||
No | 125 (20.4%) | 178 (26.1%) |
Yes | 488 (79.6%) | 505 (73.9%) |
Time outdoors frequency: | ||
Every day | 461 (79.2%) | 264 (45.4%) |
Every two days | 68 (11.7%) | 147 (25.3%) |
Two days per week | 39 (6.7%) | 132 (22.7%) |
Once per week | 14 (2.4%) | 38 (6.5%) |
Time outdoors duration: | ||
< 1 h | 58 (10.0%) | 88 (15.1%) |
1–2 h | 296 (50.9%) | 327 (56.1%) |
3–4 h | 169 (29.0%) | 140 (24.0%) |
> 4 h | 59 (10.1%) | 28 (4.8%) |
Near work frequency: | ||
Every day | 364 (65.9%) | 194 (40.2%) |
Every two days | 110 (19.9%) | 120 (24.9%) |
Two days per week | 53 (9.6%) | 88 (18.3%) |
Once per week | 25 (4.5%) | 80 (16.6%) |
Near work duration: | ||
< 1 h | 373 (67.6%) | 237 (49.6%) |
1–2 h | 164 (29.7%) | 219 (45.8%) |
3–4 h | 14 (2.5%) | 18 (3.7%) |
> 4 h | 1 (0.2%) | 4 (0.8%) |
Based on the median SE per eye, the mean SE of all children was − 0.12 ± 0.07 D in the right eye and − 0.11 ± 0.07 D in the left; the median SE values were 0.025 D and 0.018 D, respectively. The t-test revealed no significant difference in the refraction values between left and right eyes (p = 0.66). Therefore, the following analysis will refer to refraction data for the right eye only.
Out of 1416 children, 195 (13.8%) children were myopic with a SE ≤ − 0.75 D, and 1221 (86.2%) had a SE > − 0.75 D.
Myopia and leisure time activities
The results of the univariate logistic regression analysis are displayed in Table 2. It shows significant associations of myopia with low outdoor activity frequency, high near work duration and low near work frequency. Outdoor activity duration was not significantly associated with myopia.
Table 2.
Univariate logistic regression analysis |
Odds ratio | 95% confidence interval for Odds ratios |
p-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Socioeconomic status: | 0.93 | 0.89 | 0.97 | < 0.01 |
Time outdoors duration: | ||||
< 1 h | Reference | |||
1–2 h | 0.90 | 0.53 | 1.53 | 0.70 |
3–4 h | 0.71 | 0.39 | 1.29 | 0.25 |
> 4 h | 0.73 | 0.32 | 1.68 | 0.46 |
Time outdoors frequency: | ||||
Every day | Reference | |||
Every two days | 1.92 | 1.22 | 3.05 | 0.01 |
Two days per week | 2.55 | 1.60 | 4.06 | < 0.01 |
Once per week | 4.08 | 2.09 | 7.95 | < 0.01 |
Near work duration: | ||||
< 1 h | Reference | |||
1–2 h | 2.08 | 1.39 | 3.12 | < 0.01 |
3–4 h | 4.48 | 1.97 | 10.21 | < 0.01 |
> 4 h | 7.63 | 1.25 | 46.77 | 0.03 |
Near work frequency: | ||||
Every day | Reference | |||
Every two days | 1.37 | 0.84 | 2.24 | 0.20 |
Two days per week | 1.45 | 0.82 | 2.56 | 0.20 |
Once per week | 2.63 | 1.52 | 4.56 | < 0.01 |
Table 3 shows the results of the multiple logistic regression analysis. The models from the univariate analysis were complemented by the parameters age, sex and SES of the study participants. Outdoor activity frequency and near work duration maintained a significant association with myopia after this adjustment while near work frequency was no longer significantly associated with myopia. Outdoor activity duration continued to display no significant association with myopia.
Table 3.
Multiple logistic regression analysis |
Odds ratio | 95% confidence interval for Odds ratios |
p-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Socioeconomic status: | 0.95 | 0.91 | 1.00 | 0.06 |
Time outdoors duration: | ||||
< 1 h | Reference | |||
1–2 h | 1.03 | 0.57 | 1.87 | 0.91 |
3–4 h | 1.02 | 0.52 | 2.00 | 0.94 |
> 4 h | 1.25 | 0.48 | 3.25 | 0.65 |
Time outdoors frequency: | ||||
Every day | Reference | |||
Every two days | 1.34 | 0.81 | 2.23 | 0.25 |
Two days per week | 1.33 | 0.78 | 2.24 | 0.29 |
Once per week | 3.05 | 1.49 | 6.25 | 0.01 |
Near work duration: | ||||
< 1 h | Reference | |||
1–2 h | 1.78 | 1.13 | 2.80 | 0.01 |
3–4 h | 3.86 | 1.56 | 9.54 | 0.01 |
> 4 h | 3.15 | 0.44 | 22.55 | 0.25 |
Near work frequency: | ||||
Every day | Reference | |||
Every two days | 0.95 | 0.55 | 1.65 | 0.85 |
Two days per week | 1.06 | 0.58 | 1.95 | 0.84 |
Once per week | 1.18 | 0.62 | 2.24 | 0.61 |
Finally, the two parameters outdoor activity frequency and near work duration were included simultaneously in the same model. Both remained significantly associated with myopia after adjustment. Since outdoor activity frequencies of twice a week or more showed no significant association with myopia (see Table 3), subjects who were reported to spend time outdoors “every day”, “every two days” and “two days a week” were grouped in one group, which was designated “twice a week or more”. This was performed in the same manner with near work durations of “3–4 hours” and “> 4 hours”, merging these response groups into one designated “> 3 h“. The relationships between myopia and outdoor activity frequency and near work duration in this final model are illustrated in Figures 1 and 2, respectively. These show that both low outdoor activity frequency and long near work duration are significantly associated with myopia, even after adjusting for each other as well as for age, sex and SES.
Expressed in terms of probability, children who spend time outdoors once a week were more than four times as likely to be myopic than children who go outside twice a week or more (OR 4.35, 95% CI 1.89–9.98, p < 0.01). Children who engage in near work for longer periods each day were two to four times as likely to be myopic as those who engage in near work for shorter lengths of time (1–2 h vs. < 1 h: OR 1.82, CI 1.10–3.02, p = 0.02; > 3 h vs. < 1 h: OR 3.70, CI 1.43–9.60, p = 0.01).
It was then checked whether the associations between myopia and the various leisure time activity parameters varied with the age of the participant, but interactions of age with outdoor activity frequency and near work duration all failed to reach significance (all p > 0.22). This was the case both when the subjects were divided into age groups – preschool (3–5 years), elementary school (6–10 years) and high school (11–18 years) – and when the interactions with age were tested as a continuous variable. Interactions of sex (all p > 0.20) and SES (all p > 0.08) with outdoor activity frequency and near work duration were not significant either.
Myopia and socioeconomic status
The distribution of children between SES groups is displayed in Table 1. In univariate logistic regression analysis, higher SES was significantly associated with a lower likelihood of myopia (see Table 2). However, after correcting for age and sex (see Table 3), this association was no longer significant. Figure 3 illustrates the relationship between the SES index and myopia in the afore mentioned final model with adjustment for age, sex, outdoor time frequency and near work duration. The rate of myopia decreased with higher SES values, although this association did not reach significance (OR 0.94, CI 0.88–1.01, per + 1 at the SES index scale, which corresponds to an OR of 0.73 per + 5 on the SES index scale, p = 0.08).
Children with a low SES played outside on fewer days per week (every day: 59.8% with low SES vs. 67.7% with high SES; once a week: 7.2% vs. 4.3%; p = 0.04, chi-squared test), but for longer time periods (1–2 h: 44.3% vs. 59.6%; > 4 h: 12.4% vs. 5.0%; p = 0.04) than children with a high SES. Furthermore, children with a low SES were engaged in near work activities on fewer days per week (every day: 42.9% vs. 60.3%; once a week: 14.3% vs. 7.8%; p = 0.09), but for greater amounts of time (< 1 h: 54.9% vs. 63.3%; > 4 h: 1.2% vs. 0.4%; p = 0.57) than children with a high SES. Although these relationships are comprehensible, the differences between the SES groups were not significant (p > 0.05) for the parameters “near work frequency” and “near work duration”.
Discussion
The LIFE Child study is the first study in Germany to work with refraction data in a cohort of children and adolescents aged 3–18 years. A detailed analysis of the refractive status of children in the LIFE Child cohort is described elsewhere [30]. The prevalence of myopia in this cohort (3.84% in the 3- to 10-year-old age group and 21.5% in 11- to 18-year-olds) is similar to the findings of Schuster et al. in another German cohort, which estimated that 5.4% of 3- to 10-year-olds and 21.1% of 11- to 17-year-old children were myopic [27]. Compared to Schuster et al., children with a high SES are overrepresented in our population (our population vs. Schuster et al.: 43.8% vs. 27.1% in the younger cohort and 32.1% vs. 24.7% in the older cohort), and children with a low SES are underrepresented (10.6% vs. 27.0% in the younger cohort and 15.3% vs. 26.2% in the older cohort) [27]. However, we were still able to include more than 120 children with a low SES for robust data analysis.
Myopia prevalence was higher in older than in younger children. Less frequent time outdoors, shorter time outdoors durations and longer near work activities in older children, probably due to a growing amount of school hours and homework as well as a change in the way older children spend their remaining leisure time, might be possible reasons for this effect. While young children like to play outside, adolescents tend to be more involved in (social) media. The findings that outdoor time becomes shorter and less frequent in older children is in line with several other studies, e.g. Gao et al. [31], Shah et al. [32] and Enthoven et al. [19]. A higher near work frequency of younger children is rather puzzling as we defined near work as reading, writing and drawing and children only learn to read at the age of six years. A social desirability bias will presumably have a substantial influence as questionnaire data was ascertained by parents for children younger than 10 years.
With regard to time spent outdoors or engaged in near work, we distinguished between the length per day and the frequency per week of leisure time activity, which enabled us to investigate more precisely which aspect of leisure time behavior is more important with respect to myopia.
Time outdoors
Since Jones et al. found that an increased amount of time spent outdoors was negatively associated with myopia [10], many other studies followed, mostly detecting the same relationship of myopia and time outdoors, regardless of whether a cross-sectional [11] or longitudinal [14] study design was chosen. The results are consistent for different populations, e.g., Dutch [19], Spanish [17] or Chinese [15, 16]. A global meta-analysis by Xiong et al. in 2017 included 25 studies with 34420 subjects and found negative associations between the amount of time spent outdoors and the incidence and prevalence of myopia [18].
In multivariate analyses, our data suggest that the frequency of daylight exposure is more important than the duration concerning myopia onset. The total time spent outdoors throughout a day was not significantly associated with myopia. Based on our data, we cannot tell whether time outdoors was spent in sunlight or not. Especially in winter, children might spend time outdoors without being exposed to sunlight. At the same time, children who played outside multiple times per week were less likely to be myopic than children who only played outside once a week. This finding indicates that, when achieved on a daily basis, even a short stimulus of sunlight might inhibit eye growth and go along with a reduced myopic shift. Based on animal experiments [33, 34], Wallman and Winawer reported in a review that short, but intermittent eye-growth-inhibiting signals in between lasting periods of growth-inducing signals have a disproportionately high protective effect with regard to myopic shift [35]. A frequent daylight exposure operates as this intermittent growth-inhibiting signal. An experiment on chicks by Lan et al. supports this hypothesis, finding that low frequency cycles of bright light provide a stronger inhibiting effect against deprivation myopia than exposures to bright light for 5 h or more [36]. The dominant association of outdoor frequency compared to outdoor duration with myopia in our study does not contradict but rather complement current literature as it offers a more distinguished insight into the underlying mechanisms responsible for the significant association of time outdoors with myopia in the studies mentioned above [10, 11, 14–19].
Based on evidence from observational studies like these, He et al. explored the effect of prevention measures in 6-year-old Chinese primary school children in a cluster-randomized intervention-controlled trial. The intervention group (952 children) was assigned one additional 40-minute class each school day and parents were encouraged to engage their children in more outdoor activities after school. The control group (951 children) followed their usual activity pattern. After three years, the intervention group showed a significantly lower cumulative incidence rate of myopia (30,4%) and change in the SE refraction (− 1.42 D) than the control group (39.7% and − 1.59 D) [37], leading to the conclusion that onset and progression of myopia can be controlled through such interventions. Comparable intervention studies from Taiwan also found a protective effect of increased outdoor activity time against myopia onset [38, 39].
Near work
An inconsistent definition of near work among authors complicates the process of comparing results and drawing a final conclusion on the association between near work and myopia. Even if we exclude studies that include mid-distance activities as part of their near work investigated, such as watching TV or computer time, and those that examined preschool children, there is still no clear picture. A longitudinal study from the United Kingdom [40] and a cross-sectional study from the Netherlands [19] both defined near work as “reading” and found a positive association between near work and myopia. A cross-sectional study from Beijing, on the other hand, could not find an association between near work (activities < 50 cm distance) and myopia [16]. A study by Ip et al. is notable for the examination of intensive near work: Longer time periods (> 30 min) spent on reading for pleasure and a close reading distance (< 30 cm) were significantly associated with a more myopic refraction [21].
We defined near work as “reading, drawing or writing”. In multivariate analyses, a greater amount of near work per day was positively associated with myopia, but a greater frequency of near work activity per week was not. As suggested by Ip et al., the intensity of near work with constant accommodation seems to have a larger effect on myopia genesis than the frequency with which such near work takes place. This is concordant with the assertion by Wallman and Winawer, as mentioned above [35]. The interruption of near work activities is a signal that slows down eye growth. In our data, this is demonstrated by significant differences in the effect, in terms of myopic shift, of longer near work periods compared to shorter periods. Huang et al. from Taiwan reported similar results, finding that taking breaks to interrupt near work activities lasting 30 min or more led to significantly less myopia after 6 months than was the case with children who did not interrupt long sessions (> 30 min) of near work [22]. Intervention-controlled studies with school classes of varying durations could be used to confirm whether there is a causal effect. As such, these insights may result in the development of measures to prevent myopia in children.
Socioeconomic status
In the present study, the SES measure is composed of three variables: household income, parental education and parental occupation. Irrespective of what combinations of these variables have been used in previous studies, a high SES has usually been associated with a higher prevalence of myopia. In a study with 1st- and 4th-grade children from Beijing, those with a higher SES had a significantly higher risk of being myopic [15]. Saw et al. share similar results in 7- to 9-year-old children from Singapore [23]. One of the only exceptions to this association was found in 5711 6-year-old children from Rotterdam. Tideman et al. found that a low family income and a low educational level on the part of the mother was significantly associated with a higher prevalence of myopia in children [24]. In all three mentioned studies, the associations did not remain significant after adjusting for outdoor activity and near work time [15, 23, 24], indicating that the effect of SES might be explained by differences in near work behavior and outdoor activity in different SES groups. Many other studies, which were unable to adjust for near work and outdoor time, retained a significant association in multivariate analyses [25, 26].
Our analyses indicate the same relationship between SES and myopia as did the findings of Tideman et al.: the lower the parents’ SES index score, the greater the risk that their children will be myopic. As in previous studies [15, 23, 24], the association was no longer significant after adjusting for the frequency of outdoor activity and the duration of near work.
In our cohort, children with a higher SES tended to play outside more often and to spend shorter periods of time engaged in near work than children with a lower SES. These relationships are indicative of healthier leisure time habits, with regard to eye health, among children from a higher social class. A greater awareness of lifestyle risks among families with a higher SES may be responsible for this. Tideman et al. reported – in reference to the Rotterdam-based cohort – that children with a low SES spend more time engaged in indoor activities with less compensation provided by outdoor exposure and that lifestyle factors explained 71% of the increased risk for myopia in children with a lower SES [24]. Most other studies indicate that children with a higher SES spend less time playing outside and more time in near work activities, explaining the comparatively inverse association of myopia with SES.
Limitations
The major limitation of this study is that we were unable to use cycloplegics during eye examination. To reduce an overestimation of the prevalence of myopia, we set the cutoff for myopia as − 0.75 D and used the median value out of three measurements per eye. Although myopia prevalence is comparable to the German KiGGS study cohort [27], it is unclear whether these measures exclude some genuine myopic children and whether they effectively eliminate the number of pseudo-myopes in our cohort, so the true cycloplegic value remains unknown.
Another parameter reflecting near work activity is smartphone use, which was included as a separate question in our assessment. However, to date, we do not have a sufficient amount of data to investigate smartphone use, and regrettably, this shift in near work behavior in children in the past 10–15 years is not represented in the present results.
A further limitation is the large age range of the included children. Even if all associations were adjusted by child age, confounding cannot be completely excluded.
Although recruitment of study participants was conducted with diverse methods, a recruitment bias remains an issue. Citizens who do not speak German as well as families with a low SES are underrepresented in our study cohort.
The questionnaires used to assess near work and time outdoors were designed by our researchers and were not validated. Furthermore, depending on the age of the participant, questionnaires were completed either by the parent or, for young people aged 10 and upwards, by children themselves. Parents and the young participants may have different perspectives and estimate leisure time activity differently. In addition to questionnaire bias in general, we should consider the potential for a recall bias as well as a social desirability bias [41], especially concerning questions about outdoor time.
Conclusion
Given the rapid increase in myopia prevalence in East Asian countries, it is likely that environmental factors contribute to the genesis of myopia to a large degree. Our findings suggest that a daily exposure to sunlight and restricting the duration of uninterrupted near work might protect against pathological eye growth. Furthermore, in our cohort, children from a higher social class tended to be less myopic. Considering the vast economic burden caused by myopia, and the apparent cost-effectiveness of intervention, prevention strategies should be established for children at all ages.
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to all the families who participated in this study. We also wish to thank the LIFE Child team for their contribution to this research.
Abbreviations
- SE
spherical equivalent
- SES
socioeconomic status
- OR
odds ratio
- CI
confidence interval
Authors’ contributions
Substantial contribution to conception and design, acquisition of data or analysis and interpretation of data was provided by every listed author. Contribution to drafting the article was provided by D.P., critical revision was provided by D.P., W.K., T.P., M.V. and F.R. Final approval of the version to be published was given by W.K.
Funding
This publication is supported by LIFE – Leipzig Research Center for Civilization Diseases, University of Leipzig. LIFE is funded by means of the European Union, by means of the European Social Fund (ESF), by the European Regional Development Fund (ERDF), and by means of the Free State of Saxony, as per the budget approved by the state parliament.
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
Data availability
The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are not publicly available due to ethical restrictions. The LIFE Child study is a study collecting potentially sensitive information. Publishing data sets is not covered by the informed consent provided by the study participants. Furthermore, the data protection concept of LIFE requests that all (external as well as internal) researchers interested in accessing data sign a project agreement. Researchers that are interested in accessing and analyzing data collected in the LIFE Child study may contact the data use and access committee (forschungsdaten@medizin.uni-leipzig.de).
Declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
The LIFE Child study was designed in accordance with the declaration of Helsinki and approved by the local Ethics Committee of the University of Leipzig (Reg. No. 264-10-ek). Fully informed and written consent is obtained each year from the parents of all participants, and – in the case of participants over 11 years in age – also from participants themselves. All data are protected by German data protection law and are pseudonymized accordingly [42, 43].
Consent for publication
Consent for publication is not applicable for the data used in this manuscript.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Dariusch Philipp, Email: dariusch.philipp@gmail.com.
Mandy Vogel, Email: mandy.vogel@medizin.uni-leipzig.de.
Manuela Brandt, Email: brandt.manuela@gmx.net.
Franziska G. Rauscher, Email: franziska.rauscher@medizin.uni-leipzig.de
Andreas Hiemisch, Email: andreas.hiemisch@medizin.uni-leipzig.de.
Siegfried Wahl, Email: siegfried.wahl@zeiss.com.
Wieland Kiess, Email: wieland.kiess@medizin.uni-leipzig.de.
Tanja Poulain, Email: tanja.poulain@medizin.uni-leipzig.de.
References
- 1.Bourne RRA, Stevens GA, White RA, Smith JL, Flaxman SR, Price H, et al. Causes of vision loss worldwide, 1990–2010: A systematic analysis. The Lancet Global Health. 2013;1(6):e339–49. doi: 10.1016/S2214-109X(13)70113-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Fricke TR, Holden BA, Wilson DA, Schlenther G, Naidoo KS, Resnikoff S, et al. Global cost of correcting vision impairment from uncorrected refractive error. Bull World Health Organ. 2012;90(10):728–38. doi: 10.2471/BLT.12.104034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Holden BA, Fricke TR, Wilson DA, Jong M, Naidoo KS, Sankaridurg P, et al. Global Prevalence of Myopia and High Myopia and Temporal Trends from 2000 through 2050. Ophthalmology. 2016;123(5):1036–42. doi: 10.1016/j.ophtha.2016.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Lin LLK, Shih YF, Hsiao CK, Chen CJ. Prevalence of myopia in Taiwanese schoolchildren: 1983 to 2000. Ann Acad Med Singap. 2004;33(1):27–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Jobke S, Kasten E, Vorwerk C. The prevalence rates of refractive errors among children, adolescents, and adults in Germany. OPTH 2008:601–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 6.Mayer DL, Hansen RM, Moore BD, Kim S, Fulton A. Cycloplegic Refractions in Healthy Children Aged 1 Through 48 Months. Arch Ophthalmol. 2001;119(11):1625–8. doi: 10.1001/archopht.119.11.1625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Truckenbrod C, Meigen C, Brandt M, Vogel M, Wahl S, Jurkutat A, et al. Reference curves for refraction in a German cohort of healthy children and adolescents. PLoS ONE. 2020;15(3):e0230291. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0230291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Rudnicka AR, Kapetanakis VV, Wathern AK, Logan NS, Gilmartin B, Whincup PH, et al. Global variations and time trends in the prevalence of childhood myopia, a systematic review and quantitative meta-analysis: Implications for aetiology and early prevention. Br J Ophthalmol. 2016;100(7):882–90. doi: 10.1136/bjophthalmol-2015-307724. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.McCarthy CS, Megaw P, Devadas M, Morgan IG. Dopaminergic agents affect the ability of brief periods of normal vision to prevent form-deprivation myopia. Exp Eye Res. 2007;84(1):100–7. doi: 10.1016/j.exer.2006.09.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Jones LA, Sinnott LT, Mutti DO, Mitchell GL, Moeschberger ML, Zadnik K. Parental history of myopia, sports and outdoor activities, and future myopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2007;48(8):3524–32. doi: 10.1167/iovs.06-1118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Rose KA, Morgan IG, Ip J, Kifley A, Huynh S, Smith W, et al. Outdoor activity reduces the prevalence of myopia in children. Ophthalmology. 2008;115(8):1279–85. doi: 10.1016/j.ophtha.2007.12.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Ashby RS, Schaeffel F. The effect of bright light on lens compensation in chicks. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2010;51(10):5247–53. doi: 10.1167/iovs.09-4689. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Smith EL, Hung L-F, Huang J. Protective effects of high ambient lighting on the development of form-deprivation myopia in rhesus monkeys. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2012;53(1):421–8. doi: 10.1167/iovs.11-8652. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Guggenheim JA, Northstone K, McMahon G, Ness AR, Deere K, Mattocks C, et al. Time outdoors and physical activity as predictors of incident myopia in childhood: A prospective cohort study. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2012;53(6):2856–65. doi: 10.1167/iovs.11-9091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Guo Y, Liu LJ, Xu L, Lv YY, Tang P, Feng Y, et al. Outdoor activity and myopia among primary students in rural and urban regions of Beijing. Ophthalmology. 2013;120(2):277–83. doi: 10.1016/j.ophtha.2012.07.086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Lin Z, Vasudevan B, Jhanji V, Mao GY, Gao TY, Wang FH, et al. Near work, outdoor activity, and their association with refractive error. Optom Vis Sci. 2014;91(4):376–82. doi: 10.1097/OPX.0000000000000219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Sánchez-Tocino H, Villanueva Gómez A, Gordon Bolaños C, Alonso Alonso I, Vallelado Alvarez A, García Zamora M, et al. The effect of light and outdoor activity in natural lighting on the progression of myopia in children. J Fr Ophtalmol. 2019;42(1):2–10. doi: 10.1016/j.jfo.2018.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Xiong S, Sankaridurg P, Naduvilath T, Zang J, Zou H, Zhu J, et al. Time spent in outdoor activities in relation to myopia prevention and control: A meta-analysis and systematic review. Acta Ophthalmol (Copenh) 2017;95(6):551–66. doi: 10.1111/aos.13403. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Enthoven CA, Tideman JWL, Polling JR, Yang-Huang J, Raat H, Klaver CCW. The impact of computer use on myopia development in childhood: The Generation R study. Prev Med. 2020;132:105988. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2020.105988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Cohn HL. Untersuchungen der Augen von 10060 Schulkindern, nebst Vorschlägen zur Verbesserung der den Augen nachtheiligen Schuleinrichtungen. Verlag von Friedrich Fleischer; 1867.
- 21.Ip JM, Saw S-M, Rose KA, Morgan IG, Kifley A, Wang JJ, et al. Role of near work in myopia: Findings in a sample of Australian school children. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2008;49(7):2903–10. doi: 10.1167/iovs.07-0804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Huang P-C, Hsiao Y-C, Tsai C-Y, Tsai D-C, Chen C-W, Hsu C-C, et al. Protective behaviours of near work and time outdoors in myopia prevalence and progression in myopic children: a 2-year prospective population study. Br J Ophthalmol 2019. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 23.Saw S-M, Chua W-H, Hong C-Y, Wu H-M, Chan W-Y, Chia K-S, et al. Nearwork in early-onset myopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2002;43(2):332–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Tideman JWL, Polling JR, Hofman A, Jaddoe VW, Mackenbach JP, Klaver CC. Environmental factors explain socioeconomic prevalence differences in myopia in 6-year-old children. Br J Ophthalmol. 2018;102(2):243–7. doi: 10.1136/bjophthalmol-2017-310292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Williams KM, Kraphol E, Yonova-Doing E, Hysi PG, Plomin R, Hammond CJ. Early life factors for myopia in the British Twins Early Development Study. Br J Ophthalmol. 2019;103(8):1078–84. doi: 10.1136/bjophthalmol-2018-312439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Lim HT, Yoon JS, Hwang S-S, Lee SY. Prevalence and associated sociodemographic factors of myopia in Korean children: The 2005 third Korea National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (KNHANES III) Jpn J Ophthalmol. 2012;56(1):76–81. doi: 10.1007/s10384-011-0090-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Schuster AK, Elflein HM, Pokora R, Urschitz MS. Prävalenz und Risikofaktoren der Kurzsichtigkeit bei Kindern und Jugendlichen in Deutschland – Ergebnisse der KiGGS-Studie. Klin Padiatr. 2017;229(4):234–40. doi: 10.1055/s-0043-102938. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Rauscher FG, Lange H, Yahiaoui-Doktor M, Tegetmeyer H, Sterker I, Hinz A, et al. Agreement and Repeatability of Noncycloplegic and Cycloplegic Wavefront-based Autorefraction in Children. Optom Vis Sci. 2019;96(11):879–89. doi: 10.1097/OPX.0000000000001444. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Lampert T, Müters S, Stolzenberg H, Kroll LE. Messung des sozioökonomischen Status in der KiGGS-Studie: Erste Folgebefragung (KiGGS Welle 1) Bundesgesundheitsblatt Gesundheitsforschung Gesundheitsschutz. 2014;57(7):762–70. doi: 10.1007/s00103-014-1974-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Brandt M, Meigen C, Truckenbrod C, Vogel M, Poulain T, Jurkutat A, et al. Refractive status in a German pediatric cohort: A cross-sectional Analysis of the LIFE Child data. Optometry & Contact Lenses. 2021;1(1):6–13. doi: 10.54352/dozv.HISM2127. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Gao F, Guo Q, Wang B, Cao S, Qin N, Zhao L, et al. Distributions and determinants of time spent outdoors among school-age children in China. Journal of Exposure Science & Environmental Epidemiology 2022; 32(2):223–31. Available from: URL: 10.1038/s41370-021-00401-w. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 32.Shah RL, Huang Y, Guggenheim JA, Williams C. Time Outdoors at Specific Ages During Early Childhood and the Risk of Incident Myopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2017;58(2):1158–66. doi: 10.1167/iovs.16-20894. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Zhu X, Winawer JA, Wallman J. Potency of Myopic Defocus in Spectacle Lens Compensation. Invest Opthalmology Visual Sci. 2003;44(7):2818–27. doi: 10.1167/iovs.02-0606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Ashby R, Ohlendorf A, Schaeffel F. The effect of ambient illuminance on the development of deprivation myopia in chicks. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2009;50(11):5348–54. doi: 10.1167/iovs.09-3419. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Wallman J, Winawer J. Homeostasis of eye growth and the question of myopia. Neuron. 2004;43(4):447–68. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Lan W, Feldkaemper M, Schaeffel F. Intermittent episodes of bright light suppress myopia in the chicken more than continuous bright light. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(10):e110906. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0110906. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.He M, Xiang F, Zeng Y, Mai J, Chen Q, Zhang J, et al. Effect of Time Spent Outdoors at School on the Development of Myopia Among Children in China: A Randomized Clinical Trial. JAMA. 2015;314(11):1142–8. doi: 10.1001/jama.2015.10803. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Wu P-C, Tsai C-L, Wu H-L, Yang Y-H, Kuo H-K. Outdoor activity during class recess reduces myopia onset and progression in school children. Ophthalmology. 2013;120(5):1080–5. doi: 10.1016/j.ophtha.2012.11.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Wu P-C, Chen C-T, Lin K-K, Sun C-C, Kuo C-N, Huang H-M, et al. Myopia Prevention and Outdoor Light Intensity in a School-Based Cluster Randomized Trial. Ophthalmology. 2018;125(8):1239–50. doi: 10.1016/j.ophtha.2017.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Williams C, Miller LL, Gazzard G, Saw SM. A comparison of measures of reading and intelligence as risk factors for the development of myopia in a UK cohort of children. Br J Ophthalmol. 2008;92(8):1117–21. doi: 10.1136/bjo.2007.128256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Rah MJ, Mitchell GL, Mutti DO, Zadnik K. Levels of agreement between parents’ and children’s reports of near work. Ophthalmic Epidemiol. 2002;9(3):191–203. doi: 10.1076/opep.9.3.191.1514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Quante M, Hesse M, Döhnert M, Fuchs M, Hirsch C, Sergeyev E, et al. The LIFE child study: A life course approach to disease and health. BMC Public Health. 2012;12:1021. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-12-1021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Poulain T, Baber R, Vogel M, Pietzner D, Kirsten T, Jurkutat A, et al. The LIFE Child study: a population-based perinatal and pediatric cohort in Germany. Eur J Epidemiol. 2017;32(2):145–58. doi: 10.1007/s10654-016-0216-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are not publicly available due to ethical restrictions. The LIFE Child study is a study collecting potentially sensitive information. Publishing data sets is not covered by the informed consent provided by the study participants. Furthermore, the data protection concept of LIFE requests that all (external as well as internal) researchers interested in accessing data sign a project agreement. Researchers that are interested in accessing and analyzing data collected in the LIFE Child study may contact the data use and access committee (forschungsdaten@medizin.uni-leipzig.de).