Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2022 Nov 11;17(11):e0277511. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0277511

Social, psychological and health characteristics associated with stability and change in adult alcohol consumption

Martin Ekholm Michelsen 1, Marie Grønkjær 1,2,3, Erik Lykke Mortensen 1,2, Cathrine Lawaetz Wimmelmann 1,2,*
Editor: Marc N Potenza4
PMCID: PMC9651548  PMID: 36367901

Abstract

Background

Many studies analyzing health effects of alcohol consumption have operationalized alcohol intake from a single baseline measure without further follow-up. Consequently, there is a lack of knowledge about stability and change in alcohol consumption over the life course and the social, psychological, lifestyle, and health characteristics associated with different alcohol consumption trajectories.

Objectives

The aims of the study were to describe the prevalence of different adult-life alcohol consumption trajectories among Danish men and to analyze social, psychological, lifestyle and health characteristics associated with these trajectories.

Methods

For 2510 Danish men, retrospective decade-based information on alcohol consumption during life period 26–60 years was obtained in late midlife and information on individual characteristics was obtained in young adulthood, late midlife and from national hospital registries. The men were allocated to one of six a priori defined alcohol consumption trajectories.

Results

About 65% of Danish men had a stable moderate consumption, drinking 1–21 units weekly while the five other consumption trajectories were comparatively rare: 3% stable abstainers, 4.7% stable high-risk drinkers, 10.9% with increasing and 12.7% with decreasing consumption. Moderate consumption over the adult life-course was associated with the most favorable social, psychological, lifestyle and health characteristics while the other trajectories were generally associated with less favorable characteristics to varying degrees–e. g. this was the case for the stable abstaining trajectory and in particular the trajectory with decreasing consumption.

Conclusion

The findings suggest that the majority of Danish men drink moderately in the life period from young adulthood to late midlife, and deviance from this ‘normal’ moderate consumption trajectory is associated with less favorable social, psychological, lifestyle and health characteristics. Some of these characteristics may influence alcohol consumption patterns, but for some of the trajectories, alcohol consumption may influence health as well as social and psychological functioning.

Introduction

Alcohol is a major contributor to the global burden of disease. Globally, excessive alcohol consumption is related to 5.3% of all mortality while alcohol is the cause of the loss of 5.1% of all disability adjusted life years according to WHO global status report [1]. Although alcohol is a leading risk factor for death and disability, its overall association with health is complex as several observational studies have observed the U- or J-shaped curve suggesting that light to moderate drinking can have beneficial health outcomes [2, 3]. Although this may be true in some cases, many other biological and environmental factors related to alcohol consumption and health may also differ among abstainers from alcohol, moderate drinkers, and high-risk drinkers. Such factors may include sex, age, social factors, psychological characteristics, lifestyle as well as physical and mental health status. Furthermore, most studies analyzing associations of alcohol consumption with health and other characteristics have operationalized alcohol intake according to a single baseline measure without further follow-up [4]. Consequently, the stability of alcohol consumption over the life course has been a concern, in particular in discussions of characteristics of people who report abstaining from alcohol because information on drinking habits in earlier life periods is missing [57]. In fact, it has been suggested that abstinence from alcohol may be a consequence of alcohol-related health problems [6], even though numerous other factors could influence changes in alcohol habits during the life course. Thus, a British study based on data from nine cohorts observed increasing mean consumption in adolescence, a relatively stable midlife level and decline around age 60 [8]. However, even after age 60 both decrease and increase in consumption can be observed [9].

Alcohol consumption habits develop during adolescence and young adulthood and are less stable in this life period. This was confirmed in a meta-analysis finding dramatic shifts in alcohol intake in adolescence, but only moderate changes later in life [10], suggesting a shift from irregular drinking and binge-drinking in adolescence to a more stable drinking pattern later in adulthood. More recent studies have observed different trajectories of consumption in adolescence, including patterns of increasing and decreasing intake [11]. These findings and an observed peak around age 25 [8] suggest that a distinction should be made between adolescence and young adulthood where alcohol habits are being established and later adulthood where changes in consumption level and pattern can be observed but may be less frequent. Consequently, the present study focuses on adult trajectories of consumption after adolescence and before age-related changes in consumption after age 60 [9].

Our study sample consists of men only, and this raises the issue of sex differences in trajectories of alcohol consumption. Although men have higher levels of alcohol consumption than women in most countries, including Denmark [12, 13], available evidence suggests that men and women have similar consumption trajectories across the life span [8]. However, there are relatively few relevant studies and more research comparing consumption trajectories in women and men is needed.

Some studies have focused on identifying consumption trajectories by analyses of empirical data. Thus, studies have used latent growth mixture modeling to identify different trajectories [11, 14] or used linear growth curve models to describe stability and change in consumption [4]. In contrast, we have defined consumption trajectories a priori to correspond to basic questions about adult alcohol consumption over the life course: What is the prevalence of abstaining during adult life, how prevalent and stable are moderate and high-risk consumption? Is increasing and decreasing consumption during adulthood common and how is change in consumption associated with social, psychological, lifestyle, and health characteristics? The aims of this explorative study are to describe the prevalence of different adult-life alcohol consumption trajectories among Danish men and to analyze social, psychological, lifestyle and health characteristics associated with these adult-life consumption trajectories. We expect that a trajectory reflecting moderate alcohol consumption during adult life will be the most prevalent among Danish men and that this trajectory will be associated with more favorable characteristics than other consumption trajectories. The latter assumption is corroborated by Danish studies showing that differences in alcohol consumption is associated with not only health-related characteristics, but also with a wide range of socio-demographic and psychological characteristics [12, 15, 16].

Materials and methods

Study design and participants

The study is based on participants from the Lifestyle and Cognition Follow-up study (LiKO-15) [17]. The men invited to LiKO-15 were selected from two existing databases [18, 19]. The sample comprises 2611 Danish men born between 1950 and 1961, draft board examined in 1968–89, and re-examined in 2015–17 [17]. The present study sample comprises 2510 men with draft board information as well as sufficient LiKO-15 follow-up information on adult-life alcohol consumption trajectories.

Information on alcohol consumption and individual characteristics was obtained from Draft Board registries (young adulthood) and LiKO-15 (late midlife). Additionally, hospital admission diagnoses were obtained from the Danish National Patient Registry and the Danish Psychiatric Central Research Register.

Measures

Adult-life alcohol consumption trajectories

At the LiKO-15 follow-up, the participants completed a computer-based questionnaire, which included self-reported information on the weekly number of units over the entire life course, including the age periods 26–30 years, 31–40 years, 41–50 years, and 51–60 years. Data were also obtained for age-periods below age 26, but to focus on relatively stable adult drinking patterns, the analysis was restricted to the age-period 26–60 years. In Denmark, a standard drink or unit corresponds to 12 g of pure alcohol, roughly equivalent to 1 beer, 1 glass of wine or 4 cl of 40% liquor. The weekly number of units was calculated based on self-reported information on typical consumption of beer, wine, and liquor. Since the focus was on adult drinking trajectories, six a priori consumption trajectories were derived from the weekly number of units in the age periods 26–30 years, 31–40 years, 41–50 years, and 51–60 years. Men belonging to the first three stable trajectories were first identified, and then the remaining men were allocated to one of the other three trajectories:

  • Abstaining: This trajectory included participants who throughout the age period from 26 to 60 consistently reported drinking less than 1 unit per week.

  • Moderate consumption: This trajectory included men who throughout the age period from 26 to 60 consistently reported drinking between 1 and 21 units per week.

  • High-risk consumption: This trajectory included men who throughout the age period from 26 to 60 consistently reported drinking more than 21 units per week.

  • Increasing consumption: This trajectory included men who reported higher weekly alcohol consumption in the age period 51–60 years than in the age period 26–30 years.

  • Decreasing consumption: This trajectory included men who reported lower weekly alcohol consumption in the age period 51–60 years than in the age period 26–30 years.

  • Fluctuating consumption: This trajectory included participants who reported consuming the same amount of alcohol in the age periods 26–30 years and 51–60 years but deviated in the interim periods.

Young adult data

All Danish men–except those with disqualifying disease such as intellectual disability, diabetes, and epilepsy–must appear before the draft board at age 18 or seek permission to appear later. The draft board assessment includes a health examination and administration of a test of intelligence. Computerized records include weight, height, information on education and the intelligence score. For most of the sample (n = 2170), education was coded on a 1–9-point scale with 1 corresponding to basic school education without formal exam and 9 to university education (a less detailed code was used for the remaining men). The intelligence test, Børge Priens Prøve (BPP), is a 45-minute group-administered test of intelligence, consisting of four subtests assessing verbal reasoning and fluid intelligence [20]. From the four subtests a standardized IQ score with a mean of 100 and a standard deviation (SD) of 15 in the study sample was derived. The participating men appeared before the draft board at a mean age of 20.29 (SD = 2.05) and at that time, the BPP was administered as a paper-and-pencil test.

Late midlife data

The late midlife follow-up consisted of a computerized version of the BPP intelligence test and a computerized questionnaire, which in addition to alcohol consumption included questions on socio-demographic factors, personality, lifestyle, and self-reported health. The mean age at follow-up was 61.62 years (SD = 3.27) and the average time since appearing before the draft board was 41.3 years (SD = 3.35).

Sociodemographic factors. A variable reflecting number of years of education was generated based on a combination of self-reported information on school education and post-school vocational training and education. Based on relevant questions, binary variables were derived indicating having a job, having a spouse or partner, and having children.

Intelligence. At the late midlife follow-up, the BPP was re-administered in a computerized version. Using the sample Draft Board BPP mean and SD, the follow-up BPP score was also transformed to a standardized IQ scale. Thus, the difference between the follow-up and the original draft board BPP score could be used as measure of age-related change in intelligence.

Personality. A 10-item short version of the Big Five Inventory (BFI-10) was integrated in the computerized questionnaire [21]. The 10 items are presented in a 1–5 Likert format, and two items are included for each of the Big Five personality dimensions (neuroticism, extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness). For each personality dimension, we calculated the mean of the two relevant items and transformed the scores to z-scores with a sample mean of 0 and a SD of 1. The BFI-10 has been validated against the BFI-44 and the NEO-PI-R [21].

Lifestyle. For smoking, total pack years was derived based on the question ‘How many cigarettes did you smoke per day (on the days you smoke) in each age period <15, 15–18, 19–25, 26–30, 31–40, 41–50, 51–60, and >60. Number of pack years was calculated among ever smokers using the following tobacco equivalents: 1 cigarette = 1 unit, 1 cigarillo = 3 units, 1 cigar = 5 units and 1 pipe = 3 units. A question was also asked about extreme binge drinking (more than 10 units on one occasion) during the age periods from 26 to 60. Based on this information, the number of years with weekly extreme binge drinking was counted. Moreover, questions were asked about the weekly number of hours spent on physical activity on a 1–7-point scale and on the intensity of physical activity on a 1–4-point scale. These two questions were submitted to a principal component analysis and as the first principal component explained 73% of the variance, it was used as a measure of physical activity with a mean of 0 and a SD of 1. Height and weight were self-reported at the follow-up examination. Body mass index (BMI) was calculated and compared with the draft board BMI to evaluate change in BMI.

Self-reported health. Physical and mental health was self-reported on a five-point Likert scale, but transformed to binary measures of good vs. bad physical and mental health, respectively. The Major Depression Inventory (MDI) comprising 10 items in a 0–5 point Likert format was used to assess self-reported depression with a total score range from 0 to 50 [22]. Several questions were also asked about current and previous health conditions and diseases. These questions were coded in a binary yes/no format: High cholesterol, high blood pressure, diabetes, heart attack, stroke, and concussion. Finally, a self-reported question on previous treatment for alcohol problems was also coded in a yes/no format.

Hospital diagnoses

Information on hospital admission diagnoses from the Danish National Patient Registry was used to calculate the Charlson Comorbidity Index [23]. The index includes 19 different comorbidities that are weighted by their potential influence on the risk of death. In the present study sample, the number of comorbidities had a range from 0 to 16.

Information on hospital diagnoses from psychiatric wards obtained from the National Patient Registry and the Psychiatric Central Research Registry was used to assess mental disorders over the life course. ICD-8 and ICD-10 diagnoses were derived from the registry information that was updated in January 2018 when the mean age of the participants was 62.62 (SD = 3.22). The following ICD-8 and ICD-10 diagnoses were registered:

  1. Alcohol-related disorders: 303.x diagnoses and F10.x diagnoses.

  2. Substance Abuse: 304.x and F11.x –F19.x diagnoses.

  3. Psychotic disorders: 295.x, 297.x, 298.x diagnoses and F20.x diagnoses.

  4. Mood disorders: 296.x diagnoses and F30–F39 diagnoses.

  5. Neuroses/Anxiety disorders: 300.x diagnoses, F40.x–F42.x and F44.x–F49.x diagnoses.

  6. Adjustment disorders: 307.x diagnoses and F43.x diagnoses.

  7. Personality disorders: 301.x diagnoses and F60.x–F61.x or F69.x diagnoses.

Data analysis

Differences among the six consumption trajectories were analyzed using regression models that in addition to dummy variables coding the consumption trajectories included adjustment for either birthdate or follow-up age. Late midlife follow-up data were analyzed in regression models adjusting for age at follow up while young adulthood data and health registry data were analyzed in models adjusting for date of birth. The moderate consumption trajectory (1–21 weekly units) was used as reference in the analyses. Continuous variables were analyzed using linear regression, binary variables using logistic regression and count variables using zero-inflated negative binomial regression. All analyses were conducted with Stata version 17 and using Stata’s vce option to obtain robust standard errors.

For most variables, there were either no missing data or less than 1 percent was missing. However, due to a technical error, data were missing for 45 men on the personality variables.

While the draft board data generally can be assumed to be unaffected by adult-life alcohol consumption, this is not the case for late midlife follow-up data and health registry data. Consequently, we have not conducted analyses that include the latter variables as covariates (e.g. adjusting for personality traits or psychiatric diagnoses).

Ethics

According to current Danish law all health science projects must be reported to and approved by the local scientific ethics committee (§ 14 in the law). However, the law specifies several exceptions to this general rule, such as 14.2, which states that projects based on questionnaires and/or register data should only be reported and approved if the project also involves human biological material. Since the analyses do not involve biological material, this study does not require approval by the Danish scientific ethical committee system, but the study was approved by the Danish Data Protection Agency. When the participants were invited to participate, they received written information about the purpose of the study and the study procedures. Based on this information, they decided whether to participate or not.

Results

The characteristics of the full sample are shown in the first column of Tables 14. The average weekly number of units in the full sample was relatively stable across adult-life age periods (range 11.6–12.9 units per week) (Table 1). Most men reported to have good physical (85.9%) and mental health (91.2%). The sample included 10.3% who reported that they had been in treatment for alcohol problems (Table 3), and 7.8% with alcohol-related hospital diagnoses (Table 4).

Table 1. Average number of units per week for each age period in six adult-life trajectories of alcohol consumption.

Full sample Abstaining Moderate High-risk Increasing Decreasing Fluctuating
N = 2510 men n = 76 n = 1635 n = 119 n = 274 n = 320 n = 86
3.0% 65.1% 4.7% 10.9% 12.7% 3.4%
Age periods
26–30 11.56 0.00 7.87 35.50 9.22 25.78 13.38
31–40 12.86 0.00 8.22 39.91 14.23 26.49 20.13
41–50 12.75 0.00 8.75 45.28 19.35 16.77 18.93
51–60 11.85 0.00 9.08 46.73 25.26 3.98 13.49
Mean (unweighted) a 12.23 0.00 8.48 41.86 17.02 18.25 16.48

a Across age periods, 26–60

Table 4. Birthdate-adjusted analyses of differences in comorbidity index and prevalence of hospital admission diagnoses (registry data) among six adult-life trajectories of alcohol consumption.

Full sample n = 2510 men Abstaining n = 76 Moderate n = 1635 High-risk n = 119 Increasing n = 274 Decreasing n = 320 Fluctuating n = 86 P-valuea
Registry Data (binary)
Charlson Comorbidity Index (count) 0.87 (1.62) 1.07 0.73 1.58** 0.83 1.23*** 1.33** <0.001
Alcohol-related disorders 8.17% 0.03 0.04 0.26*** 0.07* 0.25*** 0.15** <0.001
Substance abuse 2.11% 0.05 0.01 0.03 0.02 0.07*** 0.02 <0.001
Psychotic disorders 4.70% 0.10* 0.03 0.03 0.06 0.08** 0.10* 0.001
Mood disorders 10.04% 0.15 0.08 0.17* 0.09 0.15** 0.12 0.003
Neurosis/Anxiety disorders 6.45% 0.16** 0.05 0.09 0.08 0.09** 0.11 0.002
Adjustment disorders 12.43% 0.24** 0.10 0.17* 0.11 0.17** 0.22* <0.001
Personality disorders 8.76% 0.20** 0.06 0.14* 0.10* 0.17*** 0.11 <0.001

a P-value for overall test of differences among the six trajectories

* P < 0.05

** P < 0.01

*** P < 0.001

Table 3. Age-adjusted analyses of differences in late midlife (follow-up) among six adult-life trajectories of alcohol consumption.

Full sample n = 2510 men Abstaining n = 76 Moderate n = 1635 High-risk n = 119 Increasing n = 274 Decreasing n = 320 Fluctuating n = 86 P-valuea
Late Midlife Follow-up Data
Sociodemographic factors
Age at follow-up (continuous) 61.62 (3.27) 61.60 61.63 62.37* 62.03 61.13* 61.11 0.001
Years of Education (continuous) 13.58 (2.52) 13.03* 13.79 13.36 13.61 12.73*** 13.31 <0.001
Job (binary) 65.30% 0.59 0.69 0.49*** 0.62* 0.55*** 0.63 <0.001
Partner (binary) 81.79% 0.76 0.85 0.68*** 0.83 0.71*** 0.73* <0.001
Children (binary) 82.11% 0.76 0.86 0.75** 0.77** 0.74*** 0.77 <0.001
Intelligence (continuous)
Intelligence (IQ) 94.91 (14.64) 91.50* 96.06 93.43 93.75* 91.69*** 93.73 <0.001
IQ change -5.09 (9.33) -4.33 -4.93 -6.75 -5.69 -4.94 -5.29 0.490
Personality (continuous)
Neuroticism z-score 0.08 -0.04 -0.01 0.07 0.10* 0.05 0.222
Extraversion z-score -0.23* 0.03 0.12 0.02 -0.14*** -0.12 0.017
Openness z-score -0.01 -0.00 0.13 -0.07 0.05 -0.08 0.551
Agreeableness z-score -0.10 0.04 -0.03 0.05 -0.16** -0.16 0.026
Conscientiousness z-score -0.07 0.05 -0.04 -0.01 -0.19*** -0.17* 0.006
Lifestyle
Smoking: Pack Years (count) 19.51 (26.90) 15.42 15.67 34.64*** 21.89** 29.58*** 30.54*** <0.001
Years with weekly Extreme Binge Drinking (count) 3.93 (9.49) 0.06*** 1.78 17.51*** 5.79*** 8.46*** 6.51*** <0.001
Physical activity (continuous) z-score -0.30** 0.08 -0.16* -0.10** -0.15*** -0.19* <0.001
BMI (continuous) 26.59 (4.05) 27.69** 26.28 28.10*** 26.83* 27.19*** 26.56 <0.001
BMI change (continuous) 5.17 (3.67) 6.27** 4.91 6.35*** 5.34 5.64** 5.29 <0.001
Self-reported Health (binary)
Good Physical health 85.98% 0.82 0.90 0.76** 0.83** 0.76*** 0.79* <0.001
Good Mental health 91.21% 0.87 0.93 0.84** 0.88** 0.87** 0.89 <0.001
Major Depression Inventory (continuous) 5.47 (6.34) 6.99 4.73 7.91*** 6.37*** 6.93*** 6.75** <0.001
High Blood Pressure 37.22% 0.30 0.36 0.46* 0.38 0.41 0.44 0.088
High Cholesterol 32.19% 0.26 0.30 0.39 0.36 0.35 0.37 0.083
Diabetes 8.79% 0.12 0.07 0.18** 0.08 0.13** 0.17* <0.001
Heart attack 5.67% 0.12 0.05 0.04 0.05 0.09* 0.06 0.108
Stroke 4.35% 0.09 0.03 0.07 0.06 0.08** 0.07 0.001
Concussion 35.84% 0.30 0.34 0.45* 0.32 0.45** 0.37 0.002
Treatment for alcohol problems 10.32% No data 0.04 0.35*** 0.10** 0.32*** 0.23*** <0.001

a P-value for overall test of differences among the six trajectories

* P < 0.05

** P < 0.01

*** P < 0.001

Table 1 shows the prevalence of the six adult-life consumption trajectories. Among the participants, 3.0% reported to abstain from alcohol from age 26 to 60 years, while the majority (65.1%) reported a trajectory of stable moderate consumption, defined as drinking 1–21 units per week. Stable high-risk drinkers comprised 4.7% of the sample while 10.9% reported increasing consumption during adult life and 12.7% reported decreasing consumption. Only 3.4% of the men reported a fluctuating trajectory characterized by periods with change in consumption patterns between young adulthood and late midlife.

Table 1 also shows the average weekly alcohol intake in the different adult-life periods for the six alcohol consumption trajectories. The overall mean for the moderate trajectory was 8.48 units per week with a range from 7.87 to 9.08 units per week, indicating a slight increase during adulthood. For the high-risk trajectory, the consumption means suggest a substantial increase during adulthood from 35.50 units per week in young adulthood to 46.73 units in late midlife. However, the distributions were quite skewed, and the corresponding medians suggest an increase from 30 units to 35 units. For the increasing trajectory, the mean consumption levels indicated moderate consumption in young adulthood (9.2 units per week), but gradually increasing to high-risk consumption in late midlife (25.26 units per week). For the decreasing trajectory, the pattern was reversed with high-risk consumption in the first two age periods and a very low consumption level in late midlife (3.98 units per week). Men with a fluctuating alcohol consumption trajectory showed relatively moderate consumption levels in both young adulthood and late midlife (about 13 units per week), but an increase in the age periods 31–40 and 41–50 years (about 19–20 units per week).

Table 2 shows young adult characteristics in relation to consumption trajectories. The highest intelligence score and educational level were observed among moderate drinkers, while participants who were abstaining from alcohol had a mean intelligence score of 95.81. The decreasing alcohol consumption trajectory was associated with significantly lower intelligence, educational level and height compared with moderate drinkers.

Table 2. Birthdate-adjusted analyses of differences in young adulthood (draft board data) among six adult-life trajectories of alcohol consumption.

Full sample n = 2510 men Abstaining n = 76 Moderate n = 1635 High-risk n = 119 Increasing n = 274 Decreasing n = 320 Fluctuating n = 86 P-valuea
Draft Board Data (continuous)
Intelligence (IQ) 100.0 (15.0) 95.81** 100.99 100.19 99.43 96.61*** 99.01 <0.001
Education (1–9 scale) 6.07 (1.92) 5.73 6.22 6.07 6.12 5.44*** 5.67* <0.001
Weight 69.04 (8.87) 68.80 69.09 69.85 69.36 68.58 67.97 0.581
Height 179.48 (6.60) 179.09 179.69 179.88 179.74 178.32** 178.67 0.019
BMI 21.48 (3.14) 21.44 21.38 21.63 21.75 21.76 21.26 0.489

a P-value for overall test of differences among the six trajectories

* P < 0.05

** P < 0.01

*** P < 0.001

Table 3 presents late midlife characteristics in relation to consumption trajectories. Compared with moderate drinkers, participants who were abstaining or showed a decreasing consumption trajectory had significantly lower mean number of years of education. The percentages with a partner, children and with a job were highest among moderate drinkers and significantly lower among high-risk drinkers and participants with a decreasing consumption trajectory.

Compared with the moderate consumption trajectory the abstaining, the increasing and the decreasing consumption trajectories obtained significantly lower intelligence scores in late midlife (Table 3). The high-risk consumption trajectory showed the largest decline in intelligence scores, but there were no significant differences in decline among the six consumption trajectories.

Significant overall p-values on the personality traits of extraversion, agreeableness, and conscientiousness were found (Table 3). Compared with moderate drinkers, men with abstaining and decreasing consumption trajectories scored significantly lower on extraversion, and this was also the case for men with decreasing trajectory on agreeableness. Both the fluctuating and the decreasing trajectories were associated with significantly lower scores on conscientiousness compared with the moderate consumption trajectory. The overall p-values were not significant for neuroticism, but the decreasing consumption trajectory was associated with significantly higher scores than the moderate consumption trajectory.

The smoking level was similar for the abstaining and moderate consumption trajectories, while all the other consumption trajectories differed substantially from these two trajectories (Table 3). Higher average alcohol consumption was associated with larger number of years with weekly extreme binge drinking, and only the abstaining trajectory did not differ significantly from the moderate consumption trajectory. Moderate drinkers had the highest level of physical activity, and in fact, all other consumption trajectories were associated with significantly lower levels. The lowest level of physical activity was observed among participants with abstaining trajectory. The moderate drinkers had the lowest BMI and the smallest increase in BMI from young adulthood to late midlife, while the fluctuating trajectory was the only trajectory that did not deviate significantly from the moderate consumption trajectory. The largest increase in BMI was observed among participants with abstaining and high-risk consumption trajectories.

The moderate consumption trajectory was associated with the best self-reported physical and mental health, and most of the other consumption trajectories had significantly poorer self-reported health (Table 3). The scores on the Major Depression Inventory followed the trajectory of scores on self-reported health and showed a similar pattern of significant differences. On the remaining health variables, participants with abstaining and moderate consumption trajectories did not differ, while high blood pressure, diabetes, and concussions were significantly more prevalent among high-risk drinkers. For diabetes, this was also the case for the decreasing and the fluctuating consumption trajectories and for concussions the decreasing consumption trajectory only. Compared with moderate drinkers, heart attack and stroke were also more prevalent among participants with a decreasing alcohol consumption trajectory. Finally, apart from the abstaining trajectory, all other consumption trajectories were associated with higher prevalence of previous treatment for alcohol problems compared with the moderate drinkers. The highest prevalence was observed for high-risk drinkers and the decreasing consumption trajectories, while the lowest prevalence was observed for participants with increasing alcohol consumption trajectory.

Table 4 shows the prevalence of hospital diagnoses in relation to consumption trajectories. Moderate drinkers had the lowest score on the Charlson Comorbidity Index and lowest prevalence of most mental disorders. Compared with moderate drinkers, significantly higher scores on the Charlson Comorbidity Index (indicating worse health) and higher prevalence of alcohol-related diagnoses were observed in participants with high-risk, decreasing and fluctuating consumption trajectories. The prevalence of substance abuse diagnoses was about 2%, and only the decreasing consumption trajectory differed significantly from the moderate trajectory with respect to this diagnosis. Compared with moderate drinkers, significantly higher prevalence of psychotic disorders was observed for the abstaining, decreasing and the fluctuating consumption trajectories, while the increasing trajectory was marginally significant. Significantly higher prevalence of mood disorder was observed for the high-risk and decreasing consumption trajectories, and this was also the case for the abstaining and the decreasing trajectories with respect to neurosis or anxiety disorders.

Adjustment disorders were quite common (12.4%) and compared with the moderate consumption trajectory, the prevalence was significantly higher for the abstaining, high-risk, decreasing and fluctuating consumption trajectories. Almost 9% of the men were diagnosed with personality disorders and compared with the moderate consumption trajectory, the prevalence of personality disorders was significantly higher for the abstaining, high-risk, increasing and decreasing trajectories.

Discussion

Most of the study sample belonged to the moderate consumption trajectory while the five other consumption trajectories were comparatively rare. The abstaining trajectory included the smallest number of men (n = 76), but this trajectory is important in relation to the large literature on abstaining, alcohol, and health. The analyses of sociodemographic factors, psychological characteristics, lifestyle, and health revealed several significant differences among men in the different consumption trajectories.

Moderate consumption trajectory

About two out of three men showed a stable trajectory of moderate drinking within 1–21 units per week. This “normal” consumption trajectory was associated with favorable characteristics on most of the investigated variables. Thus, stable moderate alcohol consumption was associated with better social adjustment, desirable mental characteristics, healthy lifestyle, and good health. Even though the range of weekly units was quite broad for the moderate trajectory, the large number of men with this trajectory indicates considerable stability in alcohol habits for the majority of the study sample, and this is in line with similar British findings [4]. The average consumption levels in Table 1 also suggest substantial stability.

Abstaining trajectory

The relatively few stable abstainers during adulthood deviate negatively on several social, psychological, lifestyle and mental health characteristics. Among the 76 abstainers, 31 individuals reported weekly moderate consumption before age 26. However, none of them reported high-risk drinking, and consequently, alcohol consumption in adolescence is unlikely to explain the associations with health. These findings are important since it has been suggested that negative deviation for abstainers primarily reflects the inclusion of former drinkers in the abstainer category [6, 24, 25]. In line with our findings, a previous study of young adult Danes observed low education, low intelligence, and personality deviance in abstainers [15] while a broad range of cardiovascular risk factors have shown to be more prevalent among nondrinkers than among drinkers after adjustment for age and sex [26].

High-risk consumption trajectory

This trajectory was observed in 119 men, indicating stable consumption of more than 21 units per week over the adult life span. An increasing and very high average level of consumption in late midlife was observed. However, only 72 (60.5%) men had higher consumption in late midlife (about 58 units) than in young adulthood (about 36 units per week) while the remaining 47 men showed a decrease in average consumption of about 4 units from young adulthood to late midlife. For the high-risk trajectory, it may be particularly informative to compare draft board data and late midlife follow-up data because the high consumption level may have influenced functioning and health over the life course. Compared with the moderate drinkers, the high-risk drinkers did not differ significantly with respect to young adult intelligence (in contrast to a previous Danish study [15]) and BMI, but in late midlife they showed the largest decrease in intelligence score and the largest increase in BMI of all consumption trajectories. The decrease in intelligence did not differ significantly from moderate drinkers, but a previous study using a different methodology observed significantly more decline in intelligence for adult-life consumption of more than 28 units per week [17]. The high-risk consumption trajectory was associated with the highest prevalence of several health problems, which may not only reflect the consistently high level of alcohol consumption, but also the high levels of extreme binge drinking and smoking (in line with [27]) as well as a generally unhealthy lifestyle [28]. The social functioning of high-risk drinking men has perhaps also been influenced by the high level of alcohol consumption as a significantly smaller percentage had a job, a partner, and children. A previous study of young adult Danes observed that high-risk drinking was associated with higher scores on neuroticism and a number of other personality traits [15], and a recent Danish study found that neuroticism prospectively predicts alcohol and substance abuse [29]. In line with these results, we observed significantly higher prevalence of adjustment and personality disorders among men with high-risk consumption trajectory.

Increasing consumption trajectory

The increasing consumption trajectory was characterized by moderate drinking in young adulthood, but on average increasing about five weekly units for each age decade and reaching a high-risk average in the life-period 51–60 years. Participants with this trajectory were comparable on many characteristics to participants with a moderate consumption trajectory. Increased statistical power due to the relatively large group of men with this trajectory may partly explain some of the statistically significant differences. This seems to be the case for the significantly lower intelligence score and higher BMI at the late midlife follow-up, while the higher levels of smoking, extreme binge drinking, treatment for alcohol problems and alcohol-related diagnoses may reflect the drinking trajectory. The increasing trajectory also differed significantly from the moderate trajectory regarding self-reported health, self-reported depression and personality disorders. However, on several health variables, the men in the increasing consumption trajectory showed the second most favorable health status, only surpassed by participants with the moderate consumption trajectory, suggesting that the ability to increase alcohol consumption over the life span may require relatively good physical health.

Decreasing consumption trajectory

The decreasing consumption trajectory was associated with low intelligence and educational level both in young adulthood and in late midlife. Moreover, suboptimal social functioning, personality deviance and a less healthy lifestyle were observed in late midlife. Generally, the decreasing consumption trajectory was associated with remarkably poor physical and mental health characteristics, and this was confirmed by higher prevalence of all categories of mental disorder diagnoses. Particularly important was the high prevalence of alcohol-related diagnoses comparable to the prevalence for the high-risk trajectory as this suggests that the decreasing consumption trajectory includes a substantial number of men with previous high-risk consumption. This was also supported by the high mean consumption levels in the age period 26–40 years and the remarkably low consumption level in the age period 51–60. In fact, among the men with decreasing consumption trajectory, 70% reported abstaining from alcohol in the latter age period. Thus, a study defining abstainers by self-report in late midlife will tend to mix the relatively few life-long abstainers with a larger number of former high-risk drinkers, confounding social, psychological, lifestyle and health characteristics associated with abstaining [5, 6, 25]. A related issue is why previous high-risk drinkers are motivated and able to limit or abstain from alcohol intake in late midlife. Compared with the high-risk trajectory, the men with the decreasing trajectory seemed to have more health problems, and the substantial health problems may be a crucial factor both directly and indirectly influencing the experience and health consequences of alcohol consumption [9, 30]. Finally, low intelligence and educational level in young adulthood have been observed to predict both physical and mental health problems [15, 31, 32], and thus, the health problems associated with the decreasing consumption trajectory are not necessarily a consequence of excessive alcohol consumption in early life periods.

Fluctuating consumption trajectory

This trajectory (comprising the second smallest part of the sample) is characterized by fluctuations in level of consumption over the adult life span even though men with this trajectory had the same level of consumption in young adulthood (26–30 years) and in late midlife (51–60 years). Among the 86 men, only six were abstainers in the two defining age periods, while eight men were high-risk drinkers in both periods, and closer analysis of consumption trajectories showed that most of the men had been high-risk drinkers at some point during the age period 31–50 years. Thus, the fluctuating trajectory includes a very heterogeneous group of men, and it may be difficult to interpret the observed associations. However, the trajectory was associated with the second highest smoking level, with several years of extreme binge drinking, and high levels of alcohol-related diagnoses and self-reported treatment for alcohol problems. The trajectory was also associated with significantly poorer physical and mental health. Most of the men had a decrease in consumption from age period 31–50 years to age period 51–60 years, and some of the findings are comparable to the results for the decreasing consumption trajectory.

Strengths and limitations

There are several strengths in the present study, including the large study sample of men with detailed information on alcohol consumption trajectories and many different social, psychological, lifestyle and health characteristics, which both include self-reported information and information from national registries. The adult-life alcohol consumption information made it possible to describe different categories of abstainers and to analyze six groups with different consumption trajectories.

A major limitation is the fact that alcohol consumption during adult life was self-reported and therefore potentially inaccurate due to recall errors and bias. However, the associations between alcohol consumption trajectories and alcohol-related hospital diagnosis corroborate the validity of the self-reported assessments. Moreover, the self-reported information was reliable enough to identify distinct social, psychological, lifestyle and health characteristics for six a priori defined consumption trajectories. Furthermore, the self-report method is supported by a previous British study using the retest method to evaluate the reliability of decade-based life-course self-report questions on alcohol consumption [33]. The study demonstrated not only reasonable reliability, but also correlations with prospective data on alcohol consumption.

Another limitation is the selection of the study sample. First, the LiKO-15 study only invited individuals living in a specific area in Copenhagen who had completed the BPP at draft board examinations, thus excluding men with disqualifying diseases and all women. Second, only 13% of the invited men participated in the study and these participants comprised a selected group with relatively high education and high intelligence in young adulthood [17]. Third, the invited population included a relatively large proportion of men with previous psychiatric admission and diagnosis, including alcohol-related hospital diagnoses [34]. This has obviously influenced the prevalence of mental disorders in the full sample and the prevalence of the six consumption trajectories as well as the social, psychological and health characteristics associated with the trajectories. Thus, it is likely that a more representative Danish sample would have a larger prevalence of both the abstaining and the moderate consumption trajectories as well as a lower prevalence of high-risk consumption. However, irrespective of the selected sample, the prevalence of the different consumption categories obviously reflects the Danish alcohol culture and may not be generalizable to other countries and cultures. In fact, some findings are dramatically different even when differences in sample and methodology are considered. Thus, a study of American military veterans observed about 65% to be abstinent or rare alcohol drinkers, while about 30% were moderate drinkers [14].

While our sample included a large number of men with a moderate consumption trajectory, the number of men with the other trajectories was relatively small, resulting in large standard errors and low statistical power. In particular, this was the case for the abstaining, the high-risk and the fluctuating trajectories, but in spite of potential power problems, we observed a number of significant differences between these trajectories and the moderate consumption trajectory.

Finally, we did not analyze the whole adult life span, but only the age period 26–60 years. Adolescence and young adulthood were excluded on purpose while the age of the participants prohibited analysis of consumption in old adulthood. However, average consumption seems to decline after age 60 [8], and changes in consumption in this life period may reflect both health status and social factors [9].

Conclusions

The findings of this explorative study suggest that the majority of Danish men drink moderately in the life period from young adulthood to late midlife, and in a statistical sense stable moderate consumption is “normal” in Denmark. The study also suggests that moderate consumption over the adult life-course is associated with favorable social, psychological, lifestyle and health characteristics. Deviance from stable “normal” consumption includes trajectories of abstaining, high-risk consumption, increasing and decreasing consumption and generally, deviant trajectories are associated with less favorable characteristics to varying degrees depending on the alcohol consumption trajectory across the adult life span. Stable abstaining is rare in adulthood, but in late midlife, a relatively large number of former drinkers become abstainers. Decreasing consumption during adult life tends to be associated with substantial health problems as well as social and psychological deviance while this to a lesser extent is the case for men who increase consumption or are stable high-risk drinkers.

Data Availability

The study includes data of a sensitive nature which, due to the low frequencies of some patterns of information, could compromise participant privacy even when anonymized. The study was approved by the Danish Data Protection Agency, and in accordance with the Act on Processing of Personal Data (Act No. 429 of 31 May 2000) of the Danish Data Protection Agency, data thus cannot be made publicly available due to considerations for privacy and anonymity of the participants. However, an anonymized version of the full data set can be available to researchers who are qualified to handle confidential information in accordance with the Danish Data Protection Agency act. Contact Department of Public Health, Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences University of Copenhagen, Øster Farimagsgade 5, 1353 København K (https://ifsv.ku.dk; phone +45 35 32 76 23).

Funding Statement

The author(s) received no specific funding for this work.

References

  • 1.WHO. Global status report on alcohol and health 2018. Geneva: World Health Organization, 2018.
  • 2.GBD 2016 Alcohol Collaborators. Alcohol use and burden for 195 countries and territories, 1990–2016: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet. 2018;392(10152):1015–1035. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)31310-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Wood AM, Kaptoge S, Butterworth AS, Willeit P, Warnakula S, Bolton T. Risk thresholds for alcohol consumption: combined analysis of individual-participant data for 599 912 current drinkers in 83 prospective studies. Lancet. 2018; 391(10129):1513–1523. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)30134-X [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Knott CS, Bell S, Britton A, The stability of baseline-defined categories of alcohol consumption during the adult life-course: a 28-year prospective cohort study. Addiction. 2018;113(1):34–43. doi: 10.1111/add.13949 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Goldman E, J M Najman JM. Lifetime abstainers, current abstainers and imbibers: a methodological note. Br J Addict. 1984. Sep;79(3):309–14. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1984.tb00279.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Shaper AG, Wannamethee G, Walker M. Alcohol and mortality in British men: explaining the U-shaped curve. Lancet. 1988;2(8623):1267–73. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(88)92890-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Rehm J, Kehoe T, Gmel G, Stinson F, Grant B, Gmel G. Statistical modeling of volume of alcohol exposure for epidemiological studies of population health: the US example. Popul Health Metr. 2010; 8:3. doi: 10.1186/1478-7954-8-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Britton A, Ben-Shlomo Y, Benzeval M, Kuh D, Bell S. Life course trajectories of alcohol consumption in the United Kingdom using longitudinal data from nine cohort studies. BMC. 2015;13:47. doi: 10.1186/s12916-015-0273-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Britton A, Bell S. Reasons why people change their alcohol consumption in later life: findings from the Whitehall II Cohort Study. PLoS One. 2015;10(3):e0119421. eCollection 2015. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0119421 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Fillmore KM, Hartka E, Johnstone BM, Leino EV, Motoyoshi M, Temple MT. A meta-analysis of life course variation in drinking. Br J Addict.;86(10):1221–67. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1991.tb01702.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Wiesner M, Weichold K, Silbereisen RK. Trajectories of alcohol use among adolescent boys and girls: Identification, validation and sociodemographic characteristics. Psychol Addict Behav. 2007;21(1):62–75. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.21.1.62 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Bloomfield K, Grittner U, Rasmussen HB, Petersen HC. Socio-demographic correlates of alcohol consumption in the Danish general population. Scand J Public Health. 2008;36(6):580–8. doi: 10.1177/1403494808089648 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Wilsnack RW, Wilsnack SC, Kristjanson AF, Vogeltanz-Holm ND, Gmel G. Gender and alcohol consumption: patterns from the multinational GENACIS project. Addiction. 2009;104(9):1487–500. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02696.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Fuehrlein BS, Kachadourian LK, DeVylder EK, Trevisan LA, Potenza MN, Krystal JH et al. Trajectories of alcohol consumption in U.S. military veterans: Results from the National Health and Resilience in Veterans Study. Am J Addict. 2018; 27, 383–390. doi: 10.1111/ajad.12731 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Mortensen EL, Jensen HH, Sanders SA, Reinisch JM. Associations between volume of alcohol consumption and social status, intelligence, and personality in a sample of young adult Danes. Scand J Psychol. 2006;47(5):387–98. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2006.00520.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Mortensen EL, Jensen HH, Sanders SA, Reinisch JM. Better psychological functioning and higher social status may largely explain the apparent health benefits of wine: a study of wine and beer drinking in young Danish adults.Arch Intern Med. 2001;161(15):1844–8. doi: 10.1001/archinte.161.15.1844 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Grønkjær M, Flensborg-Madsen T, Osler M, Sørensen HJ, Becker U, Mortensen EL. Adult-life alcohol consumption and age-related cognitive decline from early adulthood to late midlife. Alcohol Alcohol. 2019;54(4):446–454. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agz038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Christensen GT, Molbo D, Ängquist LH, Mortensen EL, Christensen K, Sørensen TIA et al. Cohort Profile: The Danish Conscription Database (DCD): A cohort of 728 160 men born from 1939 through 1959. Int J Epidemiol. 2015;44(2):432–40. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyu114 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Urfer-Parnas A, Mortensen EL, Saebye D, Parnas J. Pre-morbid IQ in mental disorders: a Danish draft-board study of 7486 psychiatric patients. Psychol Med. 2010;40(4):547–56. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709990754 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Teasdale TW. The Danish draft board’s intelligence test, Børge Priens Prøve: psychometric properties and research applications through 50 years. Scand J Psychol. 2009;50(6):633–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00789.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Rammstedt B, John OP. Measuring personality in one minute or less: A 10-item short version of the Big Five Inventory in English and German. J. Res. Personal. 2007, 41, 203–212. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2006.02.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Olsen LR, Mortensen EL, Bech P. Prevalence of major depression and stress indicators in the Danish general population. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2004;109(2):96–103. doi: 10.1046/j.0001-690x.2003.00231.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Charlson ME, Pompei P, Ales KL, MacKenzie CR. A new method of classifying prognostic comorbidity in longitudinal studies: development and validation. J Chronic Dis. 1987;40(5):373–83. doi: 10.1016/0021-9681(87)90171-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Fillmore K.M., Stockwell T., Chikritzhs T., Bostrom A., Kerr W., 2007. Moderate alcohol use and reduced mortality risk: systematic error in prospective studies and new hypotheses. Ann Epidemiol. 2007;17(5 Suppl):S16–23. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2007.01.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Rehm J, Irving H, Ye Y, Kerr WC, Bond J, Greenfield TK. Are lifetime abstainers the best control group in alcohol epidemiology? On the stability and validity of reported lifetime abstention. Am J Epidemiol. 2008. Oct 15;168(8):866–71. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwn093 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Naimi TS, Brown DW, Brewer RD, Giles WH, Mensah G, Serdula MK et al. Cardiovascular risk factors and confounders among nondrinking and moderate-drinking US adults. Am J Prev Med. 2005;28(4):369–73. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2005.01.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Ferrari P, Licaj I, Muller DC, Andersen PK, Johansson M, Boeing H. Lifetime alcohol use and overall and cause-specific mortality in the European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and nutrition (EPIC) study. BMJ Open. 2014;4(7):e005245. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2014-005245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Rosal MC, Ockene JK, Hurley TG, Reiff S.Prevalence and co-occurrence of health risk behaviors among high-risk drinkers in a primary care population. Prev Med. 2000;31(2 Pt 1):140–7. doi: 10.1006/pmed.2000.0678 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Christensen DS, Flensborg-Madsen T, Andersen NE, Rosenkær IB, Mortensen EL Personality and risk of hospital diagnosed mental disorder: a 35 years’ prospective study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2021. doi: 10.1007/s00127-020-02001-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Liang W, Chikritzhs T. Reduction in alcohol consumption and health status. Addiction. 2011;106(1):75–81. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03164.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Christensen GT, Mortensen EL, Christensen K & Osler M. Intelligence in young adulthood and cause-specific mortality in the Danish Conscription Database–A cohort study of 728160 men. Intelligence, 59(11): 64–71, 2016. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2016.08.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Gottfredson LS. Intelligence: is it the epidemiologists’ elusive "fundamental cause" of social class inequalities in health? J Pers Soc Psychol. 2004. Jan;86(1):174–99. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.174 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Bell S, Britton A. Reliability of a retrospective decade-based life-course alcohol consumption questionnaire administered in later life. Addiction. 2015;110(10):1563–73. doi: 10.1111/add.13012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Grønkjær M, Flensborg-Madsen T, Osler M, Sørensen HJ, Becker U, Mortensen EL. Intelligence test scores before and after alcohol-related disorders—a longitudinal study of Danish male conscripts. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2019. Oct;43(10):2187–2195. doi: 10.1111/acer.14174 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Marc N Potenza

22 Apr 2022

PONE-D-22-04437Social, psychological and health characteristics associated with stability and change in adult alcohol consumptionPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Mortensen,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Jun 06 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Marc Potenza

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. In your Data Availability statement, you have not specified where the minimal data set underlying the results described in your manuscript can be found. PLOS defines a study's minimal data set as the underlying data used to reach the conclusions drawn in the manuscript and any additional data required to replicate the reported study findings in their entirety. All PLOS journals require that the minimal data set be made fully available. For more information about our data policy, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability.

Upon re-submitting your revised manuscript, please upload your study’s minimal underlying data set as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and include the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers within your revised cover letter. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories. Any potentially identifying patient information must be fully anonymized.

Important: If there are ethical or legal restrictions to sharing your data publicly, please explain these restrictions in detail. Please see our guidelines for more information on what we consider unacceptable restrictions to publicly sharing data: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions. Note that it is not acceptable for the authors to be the sole named individuals responsible for ensuring data access.

We will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide in your cover letter.

3. We note that you have included the phrase “data not shown” in your manuscript. Unfortunately, this does not meet our data sharing requirements. PLOS does not permit references to inaccessible data. We require that authors provide all relevant data within the paper, Supporting Information files, or in an acceptable, public repository. Please add a citation to support this phrase or upload the data that corresponds with these findings to a stable repository (such as Figshare or Dryad) and provide and URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers that may be used to access these data. Or, if the data are not a core part of the research being presented in your study, we ask that you remove the phrase that refers to these data.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: General Comments

Thank you for the opportunity to review this manuscript. The authors examined the prevalence and changes of alcohol consumption at two time points, as well as demographic and psychosocial correlates associated with alcohol use, among Danish men ages 26-60 years old. The current study has several strengths, including a large sample size and rich dataset. However, I have some concerns about the study rationale, analysis, and interpretation. I have provided several comments below, which I hope the authors will find helpful.

Major Comments

1. This study examines alcohol consumption over time among Danish men; however, the introduction currently indicates examining changes in alcohol use over time but does not specify why it should exclusively be studied among men. The authors should revise portions of the introduction to improve the framing of this study regarding their sample: Danish men. For example, the authors may wish to cite previous research that is more relevant to the current sample: Danish men or men in general.

2. In addition to the aims, what were the authors’ hypotheses for this study? There are many variables assessed in the current study (e.g., sociodemographic variables, IQ, personality, cigarette smoking, physical activity, BMI), and I am curious what the authors’ rationale was for including these variables. Please include hypotheses in the last paragraph of the introduction.

3. How were the drinking consumption trajectories derived? It is unclear from the method whether these were derived statistically (e.g. growth mixture modeling), based on prior literature and/or a priori hypotheses, or another way. The authors should elaborate on this point. After further reading, I see this is mentioned in the abstract, but not reported in the method or other main sections of the manuscript.

4. The sub-sample cell sizes for each drinking trajectory are quite variable; i.e., ranging from n=76 for abstainers to n=1,635 for moderate drinkers who represented over half of the total sample, as presented in Table 2. Given the authors’ findings of most favorable health outcomes associated with moderate drinkers who are also over-represented in this sample, is it possible that the large cell size drove these differences? In other words, what was done statistically to account for such different cell sizes?

5. Why were regression models used to analyze differences in consumption trajectories? In other words, what was the outcome/dependent variable across analyses? What was the referent category across analyses? It seems that a different type of analysis would be better suited to answer these research questions (e.g., ANOVA). If I am misinterpreting, addressing my second comment regarding indicating study hypotheses/research questions may also help to address the statistical tests chosen.

Minor Comments

6. The objectives should be changed from “adult-life” regarding alcohol consumption to “men” or “male” alcohol consumption to accurately reflect the current sample. Similarly, the focus on “adult” alcohol use mentioned should be changed to alcohol use among “men” or “males” throughout the manuscript.

7. What is the legal age of alcohol consumption in Denmark?

8. What was the exclusion criteria for the current study? Specifically, please talk more about what constituted “disqualifying disease.”

9. In the method, please clarify what “basic school education” indicates. The 1-9 point coding scale to measure education seems potentially problematic if 1 = basic school education. What about those who dropped out/did not complete school?

10. My next comments/questions involve the inclusion of an intelligence measure for this study, as assessed by the Børge Priens Prøve (BPP). Why was IQ assessed in this study? How is it relevant to alcohol consumption? What was the mean and SD of the IQ from the BPP among this sample? Lastly, it seems problematic to use changes in IQ score, as some research indicates that IQ is determined by a certain age and does not typically vary (excluding brain injuries and normative cognitive decline due to aging, for example). Assessing changes in academic performance or achievement over time would be a better indicator.

11. It seems problematic that the Major Depression Inventory, which was seemingly the only measure of mental health, was included as one of the key measures in the “self-reported health” section. I suggest adding depression as its own section with a header to clearly indicate it.

12. I recommend that the authors provide the 19 comorbidities assessed in the Charlson Comorbidity Index, or provide several examples.

13. The following sentence in the result section could be potentially harmful and should be tempered given that this mean (i.e., M = 95.81) still falls within the normal/average range for IQ, “…participants who were abstaining from alcohol had the lowest intelligence score.”

14. The authors should clarify the following sentence in the results section, “The scores on the Major Depression Inventory followed the trajectory of scores on self-reported health and showed a similar pattern of significant differences.” What does this mean for levels of depression among this sample?

Reviewer #2: I found the abstract well written and the sample size/scope impressive. I found the rationale in the introduction strong and easy to follow.

In the measures section, it was unclear to me whether the weekly number of units consumed over the entire life course was collected via timeline follow back or some other standardized tool. Perhaps it was similar to the cigarette pack years? Clarifying this would strengthen the paper. Moreover, it was unclear why the bottom age period was 26-30 instead of 21-30. I assume this was a function of the way the questionnaire was programmed. Finally, it was unclear why between 1 and 21 drinks per week was categorized as moderate consumption—this is a very large group with a wide range of consumption habits. More information here would be useful, world-count permitting. The rationale for transforming this measure to a binary measure of good vs. bad health was also unclear.

In the data analysis section, it wasn’t stated why moderate consumption trajectory (through presumably because this was the largest group) was used as the reference, but otherwise the analyses seemed reasonable. Some of the descriptive statistics in the beginning of the results are repetitive as these are already described in the prior sections. The discussion nicely highlights some key findings including that other studies “defining abstainers by self-report in late midlife will tend to mix the relatively few life-long abstainers with a larger number of former high-risk drinkers.”

My overall impressions of the manuscript are favorable.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2022 Nov 11;17(11):e0277511. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0277511.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


17 May 2022

PONE-D-22-04437

Social, psychological and health characteristics associated with stability and change in adult alcohol consumption

PLOS ONE

Author response to journal requirements and reviews

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

Response: We have carefully checked that the manuscript meets the journal’s format and style requirements.

2. In your Data Availability statement, you have not specified where the minimal data set underlying the results described in your manuscript can be found. PLOS defines a study's minimal data set as the underlying data used to reach the conclusions drawn in the manuscript and any additional data required to replicate the reported study findings in their entirety. All PLOS journals require that the minimal data set be made fully available. For more information about our data policy, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability.

Upon re-submitting your revised manuscript, please upload your study’s minimal underlying data set as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and include the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers within your revised cover letter. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories. Any potentially identifying patient information must be fully anonymized.

Important: If there are ethical or legal restrictions to sharing your data publicly, please explain these restrictions in detail. Please see our guidelines for more information on what we consider unacceptable restrictions to publicly sharing data: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions. Note that it is not acceptable for the authors to be the sole named individuals responsible for ensuring data access.

We will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide in your cover letter.

Response: We have revised the data availability statement to refer to the Department of Public Health, including full address and phone number.

The study includes data of a sensitive nature which, due to the low frequencies of some patterns of information, could compromise participant privacy even when anonymized. In accordance with the Act on Processing of Personal Data (Act No. 429 of 31 May 2000) of the Danish Data Protection Agency, data thus cannot be made publicly available due to considerations for privacy and anonymity of the participants. However, an anonymized version of the full data set can be available to researchers who are qualified to handle confidential information in accordance with the Danish Data Protection Agency act. Contact Department of Public Health, Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences University of Copenhagen, Øster Farimagsgade 5, 1353 København K (https://ifsv.ku.dk; phone +45 35 32 76 23)

3. We note that you have included the phrase “data not shown” in your manuscript. Unfortunately, this does not meet our data sharing requirements. PLOS does not permit references to inaccessible data. We require that authors provide all relevant data within the paper, Supporting Information files, or in an acceptable, public repository. Please add a citation to support this phrase or upload the data that corresponds with these findings to a stable repository (such as Figshare or Dryad) and provide and URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers that may be used to access these data. Or, if the data are not a core part of the research being presented in your study, we ask that you remove the phrase that refers to these data.

Response: “Data not shown” has been a standard terminology to indicate a certain analysis is not presented in detail in a table. We used this terminology several places in the discussion and understand this has led to the misunderstanding that we describe another dataset, which is not available. Actually, we refer to specific analysis of our main database, which is available according to the data availability statement:

“However, the distributions were quite skewed, and the corresponding medians suggest an increase from 30 units to 35 units (data not shown)”. (page 12 in the original manuscript).

“Among the 76 abstainers, 31 individuals reported weekly moderate consumption before age 26 (data not shown)”. (page 19 in the original manuscript).

“In fact, among the men with decreasing consumption trajectory, 70% reported abstaining from alcohol in the latter age period (data not shown)”. (page 21 in the original manuscript)

“Among the 86 men, only six were abstainers in the two defining age periods, while eight men were high-risk drinkers in both periods, and closer analysis of consumption trajectories showed that most of the men had been high-risk drinkers at some point during the age period 31-50 years (data not shown)”. (page 22 in the original manuscript)

“Most of the men had a decrease in consumption from age period 31-50 years to age period 51-60 years (data not shown)”. (page 22 in the original manuscript)

We believe that it is rather clear from the context that we in all cases refer to the database that is the basis for all analyses in the manuscript. However, since the terminology “data not shown” can cause confusion, we have deleted the statements from the manuscript.

We find that this the “data not shown” statement is unnecessary in all cases and assume that the editor will agree.

Reviewers' comments:

5. Review Comments to the Author

Reviewer #1: General Comments

Thank you for the opportunity to review this manuscript. The authors examined the prevalence and changes of alcohol consumption at two time points, as well as demographic and psychosocial correlates associated with alcohol use, among Danish men ages 26-60 years old. The current study has several strengths, including a large sample size and rich dataset. However, I have some concerns about the study rationale, analysis, and interpretation. I have provided several comments below, which I hope the authors will find helpful.

Major Comments

1. This study examines alcohol consumption over time among Danish men; however, the introduction currently indicates examining changes in alcohol use over time but does not specify why it should exclusively be studied among men. The authors should revise portions of the introduction to improve the framing of this study regarding their sample: Danish men. For example, the authors may wish to cite previous research that is more relevant to the current sample: Danish men or men in general.

Response: The reviewer rightly points out that this issue should have been discussed in the introduction. We don’t think that changes in alcohol use over time should exclusively be studied among men, but we focus on men because the available study sample only included men. In the strengths and limitations section the exclusion of all women is mentioned as a limitation, but we have added the following in a new paragraph in the introduction:

“Our study sample consists of men only, and this raises the issue of sex differences in trajectories of alcohol consumption. Although men have higher levels of alcohol consumption than women in most countries, including Denmark [12, 13], available evidence suggests that men and women have similar consumption trajectories across the life span [8]. However, there are relatively few relevant studies and more research comparing consumption trajectories in women and men is needed”. (revised manuscript page 5, line 92).

2. In addition to the aims, what were the authors’ hypotheses for this study? There are many variables assessed in the current study (e.g., sociodemographic variables, IQ, personality, cigarette smoking, physical activity, BMI), and I am curious what the authors’ rationale was for including these variables. Please include hypotheses in the last paragraph of the introduction.

Response: Our study is essentially explorative, and this is the reason that we have not formulated hypotheses. However, to accommodate the reviewers concern, we have added the following to the paragraph describing the aims of the study:

“We expect that a trajectory reflecting moderate alcohol consumption during adult life will be the most prevalent among Danish men and that this trajectory will be associated with more favorable characteristics than other consumption trajectories. The latter assumption is corroborated by Danish studies showing that differences in alcohol consumption is associated not only with health-related characteristics, but also with a wide range of socio-demographic and psychological characteristics [12, 15, 16].” (revised manuscript page 6, line 109).

3. How were the drinking consumption trajectories derived? It is unclear from the method whether these were derived statistically (e.g. growth mixture modeling), based on prior literature and/or a priori hypotheses, or another way. The authors should elaborate on this point. After further reading, I see this is mentioned in the abstract, but not reported in the method or other main sections of the manuscript.

Response: It is true, that the method section does not explicitly use the term “a priori trajectories”, but the last paragraph in the introduction includes the following text “Some studies have focused on identifying consumption trajectories by analyses of empirical data. Thus, studies have used latent growth mixture modeling to identify different trajectories [11, 12] or used linear growth curve models to describe stability and change in consumption [4]. In contrast, we have defined consumption trajectories a priori to correspond to basic questions about adult alcohol consumption over the life course” (page 5, line 92 in the original manuscript). This is further elaborated in the method section “Since the focus was on adult drinking trajectories, six consumption trajectories were derived from the weekly number of units in the age periods 26–30 years, 31–40 years, 41–50 years, and 51–60 years. Men belonging to the first three stable trajectories were first identified, and then the remaining men were allocated to one of the other three trajectories” (original manuscript page 6, line 121).

To accommodate the reviewer’s concerns, we have now inserted “a priori defined” in the method section “Since the focus was on adult drinking trajectories, six a priori defined consumption trajectories were derived from the weekly number of units in the age periods 26–30 years, 31–40 years, 41–50 years, and 51–60 years” (revised manuscript page 7, line 134).

4. The sub-sample cell sizes for each drinking trajectory are quite variable; i.e., ranging from n=76 for abstainers to n=1,635 for moderate drinkers who represented over half of the total sample, as presented in Table 2. Given the authors’ findings of most favorable health outcomes associated with moderate drinkers who are also over-represented in this sample, is it possible that the large cell size drove these differences? In other words, what was done statistically to account for such different cell sizes?

Response: For several reasons we compared the other 5 trajectories with the moderate consumption trajectory, which would have small standard errors and associated confidence intervals due to the large cell size. However, the significance of comparison with this trajectory would also depend on the cell size of each of the other 5 trajectories. Thus, on page 20, line 415 in the original manuscript we suggest that “Increased statistical power due to the relatively large group of men with this trajectory [the increasing consumption trajectory] may partly explain some of the statistically significant differences” and suggest that this may be the case for midlife intelligence and BMI. Table 3 shows that the intelligence level was 96.06, 93.75, and 93.73 for the moderate, increasing and fluctuating trajectories (with cell sizes of 1635, 274, and 86). Due to variance in cell size the standard errors of the estimated means were 0.35, 0.89, and 1.55 respectively, and the standard errors of the contrast comparing the increasing and fluctuating trajectory means with that of the moderate consumption trajectory were 0.95 and 1.59 respectively. Thus, the statistical power of these comparisons to a large extent depends on the cell size of the trajectory with the small cell size. We find this reasonable because the mean with the moderate trajectory are the most healthy and the cell size is large enough to serve as a normative reference.

To accommodate the reviewer’s concerns we discuss this problem in the strengths and limitations section of the revised manuscript:

“While our sample included a large number of men with a moderate consumption trajectory, the number of men with the other trajectories was relatively small, resulting in large standard errors and low statistical power. In particular, this was the case for the abstaining, the high-risk and the fluctuating trajectories, but in spite of potential power problems, we observed a number of significant differences between these trajectories and the moderate consumption trajectory”. (revised manuscript page 24, line 541).

5. Why were regression models used to analyze differences in consumption trajectories? In other words, what was the outcome/dependent variable across analyses? What was the referent category across analyses? It seems that a different type of analysis would be better suited to answer these research questions (e.g., ANOVA). If I am misinterpreting, addressing my second comment regarding indicating study hypotheses/research questions may also help to address the statistical tests chosen.

Response: The first paragraph in the data analysis section provides most of this information:

“Differences among the six consumption trajectories were analyzed using regression models.

Late midlife follow-up data were analyzed in regression models adjusting for age at follow

up while young adulthood data and health registry data were analyzed in models adjusting for

date of birth. The moderate consumption trajectory (1–21 weekly units) was used as

reference in the analyses. Continuous variables were analyzed using linear regression, binary

variables using logistic regression and count variables using zero-inflated negative binomial 0 regression.” (original manuscript page 19, line 214)

In other words, the characteristics listed in tables 2, 3, and 4 were outcome variables in regression analyses testing differences between the six trajectories and using the moderate consumption trajectory as reference. We could have used ANOVA to analyze the continuous variables, but this was not possible for binary and count variables. However, since the publication of Cohen’s book, behavioral scientists have been aware that ANOVA and multiple linear regression are both applications of the same general linear model (Cohen & Cohen, 1975 p. 186) and that results are identical for identical ANOVA and regression models. In our case, the models included a categorical variable (the 6 trajectories) and one continuous variable (either birthdate or age), and thus the regressions models correspond to analysis of covariance. STATA’s regression procedure was preferred over STATA’s ANOVA/ANCOVA procedure because it can provide robust standard errors. The use of robust standard errors was not mentioned in the original manuscript, but is now included in the slightly expanded data analysis paragraph:

“Differences among the six consumption trajectories were analyzed using regression models that in addition to dummy variables coding the consumption trajectories included adjustment for either birthdate or follow-up age. Late midlife follow-up data were analyzed in regression models adjusting for age at follow up while young adulthood data and health registry data were analyzed in models adjusting for date of birth. The moderate consumption trajectory (1–21 weekly drinks) was used as reference in the analyses. Continuous variables were analyzed using linear regression, binary variables using logistic regression and count variables using zero-inflated negative binomial 0 regression. All analyses were conducted with Stata version 17 and using Stata’s vce option to obtain robust standard errors” (revised manuscript page 11, line 226).

Minor Comments

6. The objectives should be changed from “adult-life” regarding alcohol consumption to “men” or “male” alcohol consumption to accurately reflect the current sample. Similarly, the focus on “adult” alcohol use mentioned should be changed to alcohol use among “men” or “males” throughout the manuscript.

Response: We understand the reviewer’s point, but also find it important to stress that we analyze consumption in adult life. Consequently, we have stressed “men” and “male” throughout the manuscript, but have also kept “adult life” where relevant.

7. What is the legal age of alcohol consumption in Denmark?

Response: There is no legal age of alcohol consumption in Denmark, but currently you need to be at least 16 years to be allowed to buy beer etc. (alcohol percentage below 16.5%) and 18 years to be allowed to buy liquor (alcohol percentage above 16.5%).

8. What was the exclusion criteria for the current study? Specifically, please talk more about what constituted “disqualifying disease.”

Disqualifying diseases are conditions that make men unfit for military service, such as mental retardation, diabetes or epilepsy. It is estimated that about 5-10% of the men born between 1950 and 1961 were exempted from appearing before a draft board (Gunhild et al., 2015).

We have provided examples of these diseases in the revised manuscript: “All Danish men – except those with disqualifying disease such as mental retardation, diabetes, and epilepsy – must appear before the draft board at age 18 or seek permission to appear later.” (Revised manuscript page 7, line 152)

9. In the method, please clarify what “basic school education” indicates. The 1-9 point coding scale to measure education seems potentially problematic if 1 = basic school education. What about those who dropped out/did not complete school?

Response: The 1-9 point scale used by the Danish military has the following steps:

1 = 7 years of school education without formal exam

2 = 7 years of school education supplemented by courses of general or practical education or 8 years of school education without formal exam

3 =8 years of school education supplemented by courses of general or practical education or ≥9 years of school education without formal exam

4 = trade apprenticeship

5 = 9 years of advanced school education with formal exam and diploma

6 = 10 years of advanced school education with formal exam and diploma, commercial or technical education, or high school without diploma

7 = non-academic professional training (e.g. teaching or engineering)

8 = high school formal exam and diploma

9 = university degree

This scale has been found very useful in research. In our sample it correlates 0.68 with BPP scores in young adulthood and 0.59 with middle age scores. However, due to changes in the Danish educational system, the Danish Military has introduced a more simple scale. This scale is much less informative, and this is the reason we decided to only include analyses of the 1-9 point scale.

Those who dropped out or did not complete school were scored 1 on this scale.

The original manuscript includes the following information:

“For most of the sample (n =2170), education was coded on a 1–9-point scale with 1 corresponding to basic school education and 9 to university education (a less detailed code was used for the remaining men)” (page..)

In the revised manuscript we have added “without formal exam” to the paragraph:

“For most of the sample (n =2170), education was coded on a 1–9-point scale with 1 corresponding to basic school education without formal exam and 9 to university education (a less detailed code was used for the remaining men)” (revised manuscript page 8, line 156).

Since education is only one of many variables, we find that this description is sufficient, but should the editor disagree and prefer to include the detailed description of the 1-9 point scale, it can of course be included (or submitted as additional online material).

10. My next comments/questions involve the inclusion of an intelligence measure for this study, as assessed by the Børge Priens Prøve (BPP). Why was IQ assessed in this study? How is it relevant to alcohol consumption? What was the mean and SD of the IQ from the BPP among this sample? Lastly, it seems problematic to use changes in IQ score, as some research indicates that IQ is determined by a certain age and does not typically vary (excluding brain injuries and normative cognitive decline due to aging, for example). Assessing changes in academic performance or achievement over time would be a better indicator.

Response:

Draft board information was a requirement for the inclusion in the LIKO-15 sample, and consequently the BPP was available in our study sample. We report on the BPP because Danish studies have found associations of alcohol consumption and AUD diagnoses with low intelligence (Mortensen et al., 2006; Grønkjær et al,2019a; Mortensen et al, 2005; Grønkjær et al., 2019b).

The relevance to alcohol consumption is demonstrated in the references above.

The young adult mean and SD were by definition 100 and 15 (table 2), while the midlife mean and SD were 94.91 and 14.64 (table 3). Tables 2 and 3 shows the IQ mean for the 6 alcohol consumption trajectories and confirm associations between intelligence and alcohol consumption patterns.

Concerning the stability of intelligence: It is important to distinguish between stability of individual differences (rank-order consistency) and stability of mean level of performance. The correlation between young adult and midlife IQ is 0.80 in our sample and thus demonstrates considerable rank-order stability. However, this does not mean that the mean level of performance in IQ tests is constant across the life span. For the present sample, table 3 shows a mean decline of 5 IQ points corresponding to one third of a standard deviation. Furthermore, previous publications based on the LIKO-15 sample have demonstrated associations of lifetime alcohol consumption and alcohol diagnoses with decline on the BPP (Grønkjær et al,2019a; Grønkjær et al., 2019b), indicating that the consumption trajectories may be associated with cognitive decline. In our discussion of the high-risk consumption trajectory, we refer to the findings of the Grønkjær study (revised manuscript page 20, line 413).

11. It seems problematic that the Major Depression Inventory, which was seemingly the only measure of mental health, was included as one of the key measures in the “self-reported health” section. I suggest adding depression as its own section with a header to clearly indicate it.

Response: The MDI was the only multi-item measure of mental health, but a single item measure indicating the general level of mental health on a 1-5 point scale was also included (and so was a question on treatment for alcohol problems). However, we find the detailed information on diagnoses from admission to psychiatric hospital departments in table 4 perhaps more important than the self-report measures, and we don’t find it necessary to include a separate “mental health section” in table 3. Should the editor disagree, table 3 can of course be adjusted.

12. I recommend that the authors provide the 19 comorbidities assessed in the Charlson Comorbidity Index, or provide several examples.

Response: The comorbidity index of Charlson et al. (1987) includes the following diseases: Myocardial infarct, congestive heart failure, peripheral vascular disease, cerebrovascular disease, dementia, chronic pulmonary disease, connective tissue disease, ulcer disease, mild liver disease, diabetes, hemiplegia, moderate or severe renal disease, diabetes with end organ damage, any tumor, leukemia, lymphoma, moderate or severe liver disease, metastatic solid tumor, AIDS. This list can of course be included in the manuscript, but this comorbidity index is widely used, and we leave to the editor to decide whether it should be included (the index is rarely described in detail when used as a covariate).

13. The following sentence in the result section could be potentially harmful and should be tempered given that this mean (i.e., M = 95.81) still falls within the normal/average range for IQ, “…participants who were abstaining from alcohol had the lowest intelligence score.”

Response: The sentence that the reviewer is referring to only points to the fact that men with the abstaining trajectory had the lowest mean intelligence score in young adulthood. In other words, the sentence describes what the table presents. We are sure that the readers of Plos One will read this sentence appropriately: IQ is a continuous variable approximating a normal distribution with a mean of 100 an SD of 15, and a mean IQ of 95.81 implies that a substantial number of abstainers will in fact have an IQ above 100. Thus, the sentence in no way implies a direct relationship between abstaining and IQ below the normal range.

14. The authors should clarify the following sentence in the results section, “The scores on the Major Depression Inventory followed the trajectory of scores on self-reported health and showed a similar pattern of significant differences.” What does this mean for levels of depression among this sample?

Response: Table 3 shows similar patterns of significant differences between the moderate consumption trajectory and the remaining 5 trajectories for self-reported MDI, self-reported physical and mental health. The table also shows that the average score on the MDI was 5.47 with an SD of 6.34. Since the 10 items in the scale are scored on a 0 – 5 point scale, a mean of 5.47 is very low (Bech et al., 2001, report of a mean of 38.6 for 21 patients with major depression).

Reviewer #2: I found the abstract well written and the sample size/scope impressive. I found the rationale in the introduction strong and easy to follow.

In the measures section, it was unclear to me whether the weekly number of units consumed over the entire life course was collected via timeline follow back or some other standardized tool. Perhaps it was similar to the cigarette pack years? Clarifying this would strengthen the paper. Moreover, it was unclear why the bottom age pa prioroeriod was 26-30 instead of 21-30. I assume this was a function of the way the questionnaire was programmed.

Response: We agree with the reviewer that this part of the methods section is too short and have revised paragraph:

“At the LiKO-15 follow-up, the participants completed a computer-based questionnaire, which included self-reported information on the weekly number of units over the entire life course, including the age periods 26–30 years, 31–40 years, 41–50 years, and 51–60 years. Data were also obtained for age-periods below age 26, but to focus on relatively stable adult drinking patterns, the analysis was restricted to the age-period 26-60 years. In Denmark, a standard drink or unit corresponds to 12 g of pure alcohol, roughly equivalent to 1 beer, 1 glass of wine or 4 cl of 40% liquor. The weekly number of units was calculated based on self-reported information on typical consumption of beer, wine, and liquor. Since the focus was on adult drinking trajectories, six a priori consumption trajectories were derived from the weekly number of units in the age periods 26–30 years, 31–40 years, 41–50 years, and 51–60 years. Men belonging to the first three stable trajectories were first identified, and then the remaining men were allocated to one of the other three trajectories” (revised manuscript page 6, line 129).

The LiKO-15 questionnaire also included the age periods below 15, 15-18, and 19-25. Thus, it was not possible to analyze 21-30 years, but we could have analyzed 19-30 years. However, adolescent alcohol consumption is likely to be more variable, and consequently we preferred to analyze the age period 26-60 years. This argument is expanded in the section paragraph of the introduction:

“Alcohol consumption habits develop during adolescence and young adulthood and are less

stable in this life period. This was confirmed in a meta-analysis finding dramatic shifts in

alcohol intake in adolescence, but only moderate changes later in life [10], suggesting a shift from irregular drinking and binge-drinking in adolescence to a more stable drinking pattern later in adulthood. More recent studies have observed different trajectories of consumption in adolescence, including patterns of increasing and decreasing intake [11]. These findings and an observed peak around age 25 [8] suggest that a distinction should be made between adolescence and young adulthood where alcohol habits are being established and later adulthood where changes in consumption level and pattern can be observed but may be less frequent. Consequently, the present study focuses on adult trajectories of consumption after adolescence and before age-related changes in consumption after age 60 [9]”. (revised manuscript page. 4, line 81).

Reviewer #2: Finally, it was unclear why between 1 and 21 drinks per week was categorized as moderate consumption—this is a very large group with a wide range of consumption habits. More information here would be useful, world-count permitting.

Response: An important background for the broad 1-21 drinks per week trajectory is the fact that Danish health authorities has recommended 21 drinks per week as an upper limit for sensible drinking for men since 1990 (there has been a recent change in these recommendations after our data were collected). Ignoring the few abstainers, the moderate consumption trajectory thus consists of men with a consumption level within recommended sensible limits. We were aware that 1-21 drink per week is a quite broad range, but a previous Danish study showed few differences between men drinking 1-9 drinks and 10-21 drinks per week (Mortensen et al., 2006).

Reviewer #2: The rationale for transforming this measure to a binary measure of good vs. bad health was also unclear.

Response: This was mainly to simplify presentation, analysis and discussion of the results. The results in table 3 suggest that the binary measure was sensitive enough to show several significant differences among the trajectories.

Reviewer #2: In the data analysis section, it wasn’t stated why moderate consumption trajectory (through presumably because this was the largest group) was used as the reference, but otherwise the analyses seemed reasonable.

Response: Since abstaining during adult life is relatively rare among Danish men, and not only this study, but also other Danish studies suggest that abstaining may be associated with less favorable characteristics (Mortensen et al., 2006), we did not use the abstaining trajectory as a reference. Among the remaining trajectories, the moderate and high-risk trajectories reflected a stable consumption level, but for several reasons, including the small cell size, it would be problematic to use the high-risk trajectory as reference. In contrast, the moderate trajectory included about two thirds of all men and had the most favorable characteristics on many variables. Consequently, it was used as reference.

Reviewer #2: Some of the descriptive statistics in the beginning of the results are repetitive as these are already described in the prior sections.

Response: In the revised manuscript we have tried to avoid unnecessary repetitions.

Reviewer #2: The discussion nicely highlights some key findings including that other studies “defining abstainers by self-report in late midlife will tend to mix the relatively few life-long abstainers with a larger number of former high-risk drinkers.”

My overall impressions of the manuscript are favorable.

Response: Thanks, for these positive comments.

References:

Bech P, Rasmussen NA, Raabæk Olsen L, Noerholm V, Abildgaard W. The sensitivity and specificity of the Major Depression Inventory using the Present Stata Examination as the Index of diagnostic validity. Journal of Affective Disorders, 66: 159-164, 2001.

Christensen GT; Molbo D; Angquist LH; Mortensen EL; Christensen K; Sørensen T I A. & Osler M. Cohort Profile: The Danish Conscription Database (DCD): A cohort of 728 160 men born from 1939 through 1959. International Journal of Epidemiology, 44 (2), 423-440, 2015.

Cohen J & Cohen P (1975), Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hills, New Jersey,

Grønkjær M, Flensborg-Madsen T, Osler M, Sørensen HJ, Becker U, Mortensen EL

Adult-Life Alcohol Consumption and Age-Related Cognitive Decline from Early Adulthood to Late Midlife.Alcohol Alcohol. 2019a. pii: agz038. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agz038.

Grønkjær M, Flensborg-Madsen T, Osler M, Sørensen HJ, Becker U, Mortensen EL.

Intelligence test scores before and after alcohol-related disorders - a longitudinal study of Danish male conscripts. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2019b. doi: 10.1111/acer.

Mortensen EL; Sørensen HJ; Jensen HH; Reinisch JM. & Mednick SA. IQ and mental disorder in young men.British Journal of Psychiatry, 187, 407-415, 2005.

Mortensen EL; Jensen HH; Sanders SA & Reinisch JM. .Associations between volume of alcohol consumption and social status, intelligence, and personality in a sample of young adult Danes. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 47, 387-398, 2006.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviews.docx

Decision Letter 1

Marc N Potenza

8 Sep 2022

PONE-D-22-04437R1Social, psychological and health characteristics associated with stability and change in adult alcohol consumptionPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Mortensen,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Oct 23 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Marc N. Potenza

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #2: Thank you for the opportunity to re-review this manuscript.

I found the authors responses to the requirements of the journal adequate. I appreciate the authors’ response to the issue of studying sex differences raised by Reviewer 1. With regard to the lack of hypotheses, can the authors briefly state somewhere in the manuscript that these analyses were exploratory? I agree with R1 on point 13. The sentence “…participants who were abstaining from alcohol had the lowest intelligence score…” could be reworded to just state “…participants who were abstaining from alcohol had an intelligence score of M = 95.81…” to avoid any confusion. While the readership of PLOS One might not misinterpret this sentence, I think erring on the side of caution can’t hurt. This is a sensitive topic and if there is a chance that readers could infer that abstainers have an IQ below the normal range (even if the authors do not imply this direct relationship), that could be problematic.

I found the response to my concern about the methods section appropriate, especially the reference to the meta-analyses of alcohol intake in adolescents. I also found the response to my concern about the 1-21 drinks per week categorization acceptable given Danish standards for sensible drinking and few differences between subgroups who drink 1-9 vs. 10-21. In general, all of my concerns were addressed. A minor note: In response to R1’s request for more info on exclusion criteria, the authors mention “mental retardation” which I believe is now called “intellectual disability.”

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #2: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2022 Nov 11;17(11):e0277511. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0277511.r004

Author response to Decision Letter 1


13 Sep 2022

PONE-D-22-04437R1

Social, psychological and health characteristics associated with stability and change in adult alcohol consumption

PLOS ONE

Author response to journal requirements and reviews

Journal Requirements:

To our knowledge we have not cited retracted papers

Reviewer 2:

I found the authors responses to the requirements of the journal adequate. I appreciate the authors’ response to the issue of studying sex differences raised by Reviewer 1.

Response: we appreciate the reviewer’s positive evaluation of our response to the previous reviews.

With regard to the lack of hypotheses, can the authors briefly state somewhere in the manuscript that these analyses were exploratory?

Response: Thank you for this suggestion. We have stressed the explorative nature of the study in both the introduction and the conclusion.

Line 106: The aims of this explorative study are to describe the prevalence of different adult-life alcohol consumption trajectories among Danish men and to analyze social, psychological, lifestyle and health characteristics associated with these adult-life consumption trajectories.

Line 539: The findings of this explorative study suggest that the majority of Danish men drink moderately in the life period from young adulthood to late midlife, and in a statistical sense stable moderate consumption is “normal” in Denmark.

I agree with R1 on point 13. The sentence “…participants who were abstaining from alcohol had the lowest intelligence score…” could be reworded to just state “…participants who were abstaining from alcohol had an intelligence score of M = 95.81…” to avoid any confusion. While the readership of PLOS One might not misinterpret this sentence, I think erring on the side of caution can’t hurt. This is a sensitive topic and if there is a chance that readers could infer that abstainers have an IQ below the normal range (even if the authors do not imply this direct relationship), that could be problematic.

Response: We have followed the reviewer’s suggestion in the revised manuscript:

Line 297: Table 2 shows young adult characteristics in relation to consumption trajectories. The highest intelligence score and educational level were observed among moderate drinkers, while participants who were abstaining from alcohol had a mean intelligence score of 95.81.

I found the response to my concern about the methods section appropriate, especially the reference to the meta-analyses of alcohol intake in adolescents. I also found the response to my concern about the 1-21 drinks per week categorization acceptable given Danish standards for sensible drinking and few differences between subgroups who drink 1-9 vs. 10-21. In general, all of my concerns were addressed.

Response: Thanks again for the positive comments.

A minor note: In response to R1’s request for more info on exclusion criteria, the authors mention “mental retardation” which I believe is now called “intellectual disability.”

Response: This is correct and line 156 has been revised accordingly: All Danish men – except those with disqualifying disease such as intellectual disability, diabetes, and epilepsy – must appear before the draft board at age 18 or seek permission to appear later.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 2

Marc N Potenza

31 Oct 2022

Social, psychological and health characteristics associated with stability and change in adult alcohol consumption

PONE-D-22-04437R2

Dear Dr. Mortensen,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Marc N. Potenza

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Marc N Potenza

3 Nov 2022

PONE-D-22-04437R2

Social, psychological and health characteristics associated with stability and change in adult alcohol consumption

Dear Dr. Mortensen:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Marc N. Potenza

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviews.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    The study includes data of a sensitive nature which, due to the low frequencies of some patterns of information, could compromise participant privacy even when anonymized. The study was approved by the Danish Data Protection Agency, and in accordance with the Act on Processing of Personal Data (Act No. 429 of 31 May 2000) of the Danish Data Protection Agency, data thus cannot be made publicly available due to considerations for privacy and anonymity of the participants. However, an anonymized version of the full data set can be available to researchers who are qualified to handle confidential information in accordance with the Danish Data Protection Agency act. Contact Department of Public Health, Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences University of Copenhagen, Øster Farimagsgade 5, 1353 København K (https://ifsv.ku.dk; phone +45 35 32 76 23).


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES