Highlights
-
•
Anhedonia and amotivation are common symptoms across mood and psychotic disorders.
-
•
Value signals in the brain were measured using a delay discounting task during fMRI.
-
•
Anhedonia and amotivation were linked to dampened value-related signals.
-
•
This brain-symptom relationship was specific to anhedonia and amotivation.
Keywords: Anhedonia, Depression, Bipolar disorder, Schizophrenia, Ventromedial prefrontal cortex, fMRI
Abstract
Deficits in motivation and pleasure are common across many psychiatric disorders, and manifest as symptoms of amotivation and anhedonia, which are prominent features of both mood and psychotic disorders. Here we provide evidence for an association between neural value signals and symptoms of amotivation and anhedonia across adults with major depression, bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, or no psychiatric diagnosis. We found that value signals in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) during intertemporal decision-making were dampened in individuals with greater motivational and hedonic deficits, after accounting for primary diagnosis. This relationship remained significant while controlling for diagnosis-specific symptoms of mood and psychosis, such as depression as well as positive and negative symptoms. Our results demonstrate that dysfunction in the vmPFC during value-based decision-making is specifically linked to motivational and hedonic impairments. These findings provide a quantitative neural target for the potential development of novel treatments for amotivation and anhedonia.
1. Introduction
Reductions in motivation to pursue or ability to experience pleasure, clinically termed amotivation and anhedonia, are commonly seen across a wide range of neuropsychiatric disorders and are particularly prominent in mood and psychotic disorders. These motivational and hedonic symptoms are difficult to treat and have been linked to suicidal ideation (Ballard et al., 2017, Ducasse et al., 2018), cognitive dysfunction (Franke et al., 1993, McIntyre et al., 2016), and poor clinical prognosis (McMakin et al., 2012). A growing number of theoretical papers have proposed that these symptoms may arise from a shared disruption in the brain’s reward valuation processes that occurs across many diagnostic classes of disorders (Barch et al., 2016, Husain and Roiser, 2018, Lambert et al., 2018, Whitton et al., 2015, Winograd-Gurvich et al., 2006). However, although many existing studies have compared the neural processing of rewards in cases and controls, only recently have neuroimaging studies begun to investigate motivation and pleasure using a dimensional approach (Arrondo et al., 2015, Gradin et al., 2011, Park et al., 2017, Schilbach et al., 2016, Segarra et al., 2016, Sharma et al., 2017). Consistent with the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) and Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP) initiatives, moving beyond case-control comparisons to dimensional approaches based on biobehavioral and psychometric evidence would shed light on important targets for research and treatment (Insel, 2009, Kotov et al., 2017, Kotov et al., 2022). Both the RDoC and HiTOP frameworks suggest that amotivation and anhedonia are a transdiagnostic dimension that is distinguishable from other symptoms, mapping onto the positive valence system (RDoC) and the Detachment factor (HiTOP) (Khazanov et al., 2020, Kotov et al., 2017). Here we take such a dimensional framework to link amotivation and anhedonia across different conditions with disruptions in specific computational neural signals important for decision-making.
Specifically, we test the hypothesis that motivational and hedonic deficits are associated with dampened neural signals of the value of potential outcomes during decision making. This hypothesis builds on much research in decision neuroscience identifying reliable and replicable neural correlates of value during decision-making in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) and ventral striatum (VS) (Bartra et al., 2013). Intriguingly, both of these regions exhibit functional and morphological abnormalities in mood and psychotic disorders (Satterthwaite et al., 2015, Wolf et al., 2014, Zhang et al., 2016). A disruption of neural value signals in these regions could lead to indecisiveness or ambivalence when choosing between rewards or an unwillingness to expend effort for rewards that maps on to amotivation and anhedonia symptoms.
Our focus is on neural correlates of decision value, a signal about available rewards at the time a decision is made, in contrast to the focus in many previous studies on neural correlates of experienced value, a signal at the time a reward is received or anticipated (Platt & Plassmann, 2014). Many previous investigations of mood and psychotic disorders have measured neural activity at the time of receiving (e.g., gambling tasks (Delgado et al., 2000), guessing paradigms (Hajcak et al., 2006)) or anticipating reward feedback (monetary incentive delay task (Knutson et al., 2000)). Though many of these studies have observed reduced neural signals of experienced value in response to either positive stimuli (Epstein et al., 2006, Harvey et al., 2007, Keedwell et al., 2005) or anticipation of positive outcomes (Juckel et al., 2006a, Juckel et al., 2006b, Simon et al., 2010, Stoy et al., 2012, Wacker et al., 2009), there are also many conflicting findings (see review Zhang et al., 2016). Furthermore, accumulating evidence proposes distinguishing the in-the-moment, anticipation, and motivational aspects of reward and suggests that impairments in mood and psychotic disorders are primarily in the prospective consideration of reward, rather than in enjoyment during reward consumption (Kring and Barch, 2014, Treadway and Zald, 2011). That is, motivational and hedonic symptoms appear to primarily impact not experienced value – how people experience rewards in the moment – but rather decision value – how they evaluate potential rewards and decide which ones to pursue. Thus, considering this important distinction in decision neuroscience suggests that a disruption in decision value signals might better explain motivational and hedonic symptoms. Studies using computational approaches for reinforcement learning have indeed reported impaired representation of expected values during reinforcement learning in both mood and psychotic disorders, as well as in relation to motivational and hedonic symptoms (Gold et al., 2008, Gold et al., 2012, Gradin et al., 2011, Mukherjee et al., 2020, Strauss et al., 2011a, Waltz and Gold, 2016). However, because values are learned from experience in reinforcement learning tasks, one limitation of previous work is that deficits in valuation and learning can be confounded. Therefore, it is important to examine how disruptions in valuation in the absence of learning may be related to motivational and hedonic symptoms. Moreover, to our knowledge, this hypothesis has not been investigated across disorders.
To capture motivational and hedonic deficits across different conditions, we analyzed the data from 81 individuals with major depressive disorder (MDD = 17), bipolar disorder (BD = 21), schizophrenia (SCZ = 23), or no psychiatric history (HC = 20) based on the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-IV; First & Gibbon, 2004). Our primary measure of interest, symptoms of amotivation and anhedonia, was assessed using the Motivation and Pleasure (MAP) scale of the Clinical Assessment Interview for Negative Symptoms (CAINS; Kring et al., 2013). To rule out potential confounds, we also evaluated diagnosis-specific symptoms, such as depression, positive symptoms (e.g., hallucinations, delusions, disorganized speech), and negative symptoms (e.g., anhedonia, amotivation, alogia, flat affect, apathy) using the Calgary Depression Scale for Schizophrenia (CDSS; Addington et al., 1990), the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS; Andreasen, 1989a), and the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS; Andreasen, 1989b).
We examined neural representations of decision value in these individuals in a delay discounting task, which involves choices between a smaller proximal reward and a larger reward farther in the future (Kirby & Maraković, 1996). This paradigm was one of the first used to identify neural correlates of decision value in the vmPFC and vS (Kable & Glimcher, 2007) and a recent meta-analysis confirmed that both regions reliably track decision value during the task (Schüller et al., 2019). Because the tendency to devalue future rewards varies widely across individuals, this task dissociates objective values (in this case, monetary amounts) from decision values, which account for the monetary amount and delay, as well as individual differences in the subjective cost of waiting (see Equation (1) in Section 2.4). In addition, because this decision task specifically engages the ability to imagine the value of a future reward (i.e., no rewards are actually delivered during the task), its emphasis on the prospective evaluation of reward aligns with the clinical manifestations of amotivation and anhedonia. We expected that greater impairments in motivation and pleasure, as assessed by the CAINS MAP, would be associated with a reduced neural representation of decision values for future rewards.
2. Material and methods
2.1. Participants
Ninety participants who met clinical eligibility were recruited from other studies (Hershenberg et al., 2016, Wolf et al., 2014). Primary diagnosis of MDD, BD, or SCZ was ascertained using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID-IV; First & Gibbon, 2004), and all participants in the MDD and BD group were in a depressive episode at the time of the scan to minimize any variance due to manic symptoms. For BD, both type I and II were included in the study, with three participants meeting the diagnostic criteria for type II. Healthy control participants were excluded if they met criteria for any Axis I psychiatric disorder. Given the association between substance use and delay discounting, participants with a history of pathological gambling, substance abuse or dependence in the past six months (with the exception of nicotine), or a positive urine drug screen on the day of the study were excluded. Given the evidence for elevated discount rates in cigarette smokers, coupled with the higher prevalence of smoking in SCZ, all groups were matched on smoking status (Wing et al., 2012, Yu et al., 2017). Of the 90 adults who met clinical eligibility, four were excluded from the analyses due to excessive head motion during the scan, and five due to idiosyncratic responses on the task (see Quality control analysis). Therefore, our final sample consisted of 81 participants (Table 1). The excluded participants did not differ significantly on demographic and clinical variables (Supplementary Table S1). All study procedures were approved by the University of Pennsylvania’s Institutional Review Board, and all participants provided written informed consent.
Table 1.
HC | MDD | BD* | SCZ | |
---|---|---|---|---|
N | 20 | 17 | 21 | 23 |
Age in years | 41.7 (10.6) | 35.0 (12.6) | 37.7 (11.6) | 41.2 (10.0) |
Sex: male | 35 % | 47 % | 67 % | 39 % |
Race | ||||
White | 55 % | 59 % | 67 % | 30 % |
African American | 45 % | 29 % | 29 % | 61 % |
Asian | 0 % | 6 % | 5 % | 4 % |
Mixed | 0 % | 6 % | 0 % | 4 % |
Ethnicity | ||||
Non Hispanic | 100 % | 94 % | 86 % | 96 % |
Hispanic | 0 % | 6 % | 14 % | 4 % |
Years of education | 15.0 (2.2) | 14.8 (2.6) | 15.5 (2.3) | 13.7 (1.8) |
Clinical symptoms | ||||
Smoking | 25 % | 18 % | 29 % | 30 % |
CAINS MAP | 0.82 (0.40) | 1.82 (0.71) | 1.90 (0.67) | 1.46 (0.67) |
CDSS† | 0.32 (1.38) | 11.71 (3.14) | 11.05 (4.42) | 2.95 (2.70) |
SAPS | 0.11 (0.47) | 0.53 (1.33) | 1.52 (2.34) | 4.45 (4.15) |
SANS | 1.11 (2.32) | 6.18 (2.27) | 6.86 (2.50) | 7.91 (4.31) |
Delay discounting task | ||||
log10(k) | −1.81 (0.67) | −1.81 (0.43) | −1.79 (0.74) | −1.67 (0.44) |
Tjur’s D | 0.71 (0.18) | 0.65 (0.18) | 0.64 (0.17) | 0.57 (0.14) |
% predicted | 90.98 (6.12) | 85.53 (8.10) | 89.78 (9.73) | 87.49 (5.98) |
Cognitive performance | ||||
CNB z-score | 0.38 (0.47) | −0.04 (0.52) | −0.04 (0.53) | −0.18 (0.62) |
Medication | ||||
CPZ equivalent | 0 (0.0) | 0 (0.0) | 250.5 (424.1) | 730.0 (850.4) |
Typical antipsychotics | 0 % | 0 % | 0 % | 22 % |
Atypical antipsychotics | 0 % | 0 % | 43 % | 83 % |
Benzodiazepines | 0 % | 18 % | 24 % | 22 % |
Lithium | 0 % | 6 % | 52 % | 0 % |
Mood stabilizing anticonvulsants |
0 % | 0 % | 33 % | 9 % |
Other anticonvulsants | 0 % | 6 % | 5 % | 4 % |
Antidepressants | 0 % | 59 % | 24 % | 48 % |
Stimulants | 0 % | 12 % | 5 % | 9 % |
Anticholinergic | 0 % | 6 % | 0 % | 9 % |
Other | 0 % | 41 % | 29 % | 13 % |
Note: All values in X (Y) format are mean (standard deviation). HC = healthy control, MDD = major depressive disorder, BD = bipolar disorder, SCZ = schizophrenia, CNB = average z-score on the Penn Computerized Neurocognitive Battery, log10(k) = subject-specific parameter for discount rate, Tjur’s D = Tjur’s coefficient of discrimination, % predicted = percentage of correct model predictions of choice, CAINS MAP = motivation and pleasure, CDSS = Calgary Depression Scale for Schizophrenia, SAPS = Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms, SANS = Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms, CPZ equivalent = chlorpromazine equivalent for antipsychotic drugs (in mg).
*Three participants met the diagnostic criteria for bipolar disorder type II.
† Data available on a subsample (n = 77).
2.2. Clinical and cognitive measures
The Clinical Assessment Interview for Negative Symptoms - Beta (CAINS) was used as the primary measure of amotivation/anhedonia (Kring et al., 2013). The CAINS assesses motivation and pleasure in the social, recreational, and vocational domains, and therefore is less susceptible to environmental or external constraints than other existing scales (Strauss and Gold, 2016, Wolf et al., 2014). Given the two-factor structure of the CAINS (motivation and pleasure; expression), we calculated an aggregate motivation and pleasure (CAINS MAP) score by taking the mean of all relevant items. Though originally developed in a schizophrenia sample, the CAINS MAP has been used in other diagnostic groups, including healthy controls, and shown to measure motivational and hedonic impairments similarly as it does in schizophrenia (Prettyman et al., 2021, Wolf et al., 2014). Of the 81 participants included in the analysis, 77 were also administered the Calgary Depression Scale for Schizophrenia (CDSS; Addington et al., 1990), the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS; Andreasen, 1989a), and the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS; Andreasen, 1989b). For all clinical measures, higher scores indicate increasing levels of severity. Additionally, given the association between discount rates and cognitive abilities (Bickel et al., 2011, Heerey et al., 2011, Hinson et al., 2003), cognitive abilities were assessed using a subset of tasks from the Penn Computerized Neurocognitive Battery (CNB; Moore et al., 2015), which included (neurobehavioral function; domain): Penn Face Memory (episodic memory; face memory), Short Penn Continuous Performance Test (executive control; attention), Penn Emotion Recognition Test (social cognition; emotion identification), Penn Word Memory (episodic memory; verbal memory), Short Letter N-back (executive control; working memory), Short Penn Line Orientation Test (complex cognition; spatial ability), Short Penn Conditional Exclusion Task (executive control; mental flexibility), Short Penn Logical Reasoning Test (complex cognition, language reasoning). An aggregate cognitive functioning score was calculated by taking the mean of standardized accuracy scores (z-scores) on individual tasks.
2.3. Delay discounting task
Participants performed a delay discounting task in the scanner (Fig. 1A; Kable & Glimcher, 2010). The task consisted of 200 choices (4 runs of 50 trials) between two options: $20 now and $X in Y days, in which X ranged from $20.50 to $50 and Y ranged from 1 to 178 days. Some versions of this task vary the amount of both immediate and delayed options (Yu et al., 2017). In our task, the value of the immediate option is fixed at $20 across all trials. This means that the decision value of the delayed option (DVdelayed), the sum of the decision values of the two options (DVdelayed + $20), and the signed difference between the decision values of the two options (DVdelayed - $20) are perfectly collinear. This feature of the design allows all value-related activity to be identified with a single contrast (Kable and Glimcher, 2007, Kable and Glimcher, 2010). For each trial, participants had 4 s to make a response by pressing the right or left button, and the locations of the immediate and delayed options were pseudorandomized across trials. Trials in which the participant did not make a choice in 4 s were coded as missing. Each trial was followed by an inter-trial interval (ITI) that ranged from 2 to 20 s (mean ITI = 6 s). Participants were informed that one of the trials would be randomly selected for payment at the end of the experiment; payment was provided on a debit card, available immediately or after a delay according to the participant’s choice on the randomly selected trial. EPrime (https://pstnet.com) 2.0 was used for task presentation.
2.4. Parameter estimation
Individual discount curves were fitted using a hyperbolic function (Kirby and Herrnstein, 1995, Lempert and Pizzagalli, 2010, Myerson and Green, 1995, Rachlin et al., 1991, Richards et al., 1999), which assumes that the decision value (DV) of the delayed reward is:
(1) |
where A is the amount of the reward, D is the delay to receiving the reward, and k is the subject-specific free parameter for their discount rate. Participants' individual choice data was fit with the following logistic function using maximum likelihood estimation with function minimization routines in MATLAB:
(2) |
(3) |
where σ is the scaling parameter in the logistic function. Because k was not normally distributed (Shapiro-Wilk’s W = 0.69, p < 0.001), individual k was transformed using log10.
2.5. Quality control analysis
Runs that were missing more than 10 % of the trials (5 out of 50 trials per run), or that were characterized by excessive head motion (see Image processing), were excluded from the analyses. Four participants were excluded because there were fewer than two runs of usable data, due to missing trials or excessive head motion. Additionally, participants were excluded if the fit of the logistic model was poor, as assessed by a Tjur’s coefficient of discrimination (Tjur’s D) <0.20. One participant was excluded due to poor model fit. Lastly, participants who chose a single option (either the immediate or delayed reward) more than 99 % of the time were excluded. Three participants were excluded because they only chose the immediate option, and one participant was excluded because they only chose the delayed option. Therefore, our imaging and behavioral quality control analyses excluded 4 and 5 participants, respectively. For the subjects included for analyses (N = 81), the percentage of choices of the immediate option ranged from 2.5 % to 98.5 % (M = 58.99, SD = 26.66).
2.6. fMRI acquisition
All images were acquired using a Siemens Tim Trio 3 T and a 32-channel head coil. For functional images, 3 mm interleaved axial slices were acquired with echo-planar T2* weighting (repetition time [TR] = 3000 ms, echo time [TE] = 30 ms, flip angle = 90°, field of view [FOV] = 192 × 192 mm, matrix = 64 × 64, slice thickness = 3 mm). Slice orientation was −30° from the anterior commissure-posterior commissure (ACPC) plane to minimize signal drop out in the orbitofrontal cortex (Weiskopf et al., 2006). Each run consisted of 168 images, and the first 6 volumes (18 s) were discarded to compensate for T1 saturation effects. High-resolution T1-weighted MPRAGE anatomical images were acquired for spatial registration to a standard coordinate system (slice thickness = 1 mm, TR = 1810 ms, TE = 3.51 ms, inversion time [TI] = 1100 ms, flip angle = 9°, FOV = 1192 × 256 mm, matrix = 256 × 192, 160 slices). Additionally, for distortion correction, a B0 field map was acquired using a double-echo gradient recall echo (GRE) sequence (TR = 1000 ms, TE1 2.69 ms, TE2 5.27 ms, flip angle = 60°, FOV = 240 mm, slice thickness = 4 mm).
2.7. Image processing
Image processing and statistical analyses were performed using FSL 5.0.9 (http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). All volumes were corrected for differences in slice acquisition using Fourier-space time-series phase-shifting and corrected for small head movements using MCFLIRT (Jenkinson et al., 2002). Runs with mean relative displacement (MRD) greater than 0.30 mm were excluded. Data were smoothed using a Gaussian kernel of FWHM 6.0 mm and filtered in the temporal domain using a nonlinear high-pass filter (Gaussian-weighted least-squares straight line fitting with sigma = 50.0 s). To account for anatomical differences across subjects and to allow for statistical inference at the group level, functional images were registered to the anatomical image and spatially normalized to standard MNI space (MNI152, T1 2 mm) using linear registration with FMRIB's Linear Image Registration Tool (FLIRT) and further refined using FNIRT nonlinear registration (Andersson et al., 2007, Jenkinson et al., 2002, Jenkinson and Smith, 2001).
2.8. fMRI analysis
Using FSL’s FMRI Expert Analysis Tool Version 6.0, we fit a general linear model (GLM) that estimated (1) averaged activity for all decisions versus rest (trial regressor) and (2) activity that was correlated across trials with the decision value of the delayed option (DV regressor, calculated using Equation (1) above with the subject-specific k). As noted above, this regressor would detect activity varying with the decision value of the delayed option, the sum of the decision values of both options, or the signed difference in decision values between the two options. For secondary analyses, an additional GLM replaced the DV regressor with the absolute difference in the decision values of the two options (|DV - $20|). The first six volumes of each run were discarded prior to analysis. All other events were modeled with a fixed duration of 4 s following the stimulus presentation, and convolved with a canonical double-gamma HRF. Temporal derivatives of these two regressors, as well as the six motion parameters, were included as covariates of no interest. Missed trials, in which the participant failed to make a response in 4 s, were modeled separately.
Subsequently, all eligible runs from each participant were combined using a fixed effect model. Group-level analyses were performed using FMRIB Local Analysis of Mixed Effects module (Beckmann et al., 2003). For region-of-interest (ROI) analyses, we used vmPFC and vS masks from a meta-analysis of 206 published studies examining the neural correlates of decision value (See Bartra et al., 2013). To test for any differences in activations across groups, an F-test was performed. To test for the main effect of CAINS MAP across the whole brain, individual MAP scores were demeaned and included in the GLM as an explanatory variable. Using FMRIB Software Library, thresholded Z statistic images were prepared by using a threshold of Z greater than 3.1 and a corrected extent threshold of p < 0.05, familywise error-corrected using Gaussian Random Field Theory (Poline et al., 1997). In all multisubject statistics, outliers were de-weighted using mixture modeling (Woolrich, 2008).
2.9. Statistical analyses
Behavioral analyses were performed in MATLAB R2016b (Mathworks). All imaging analyses were performed in FSL 5.0.9. All correlation and regression analyses were performed in R 3.5.2 (CRAN). All pairwise t-tests were corrected for multiple comparisons using Holm’s method (Holm, 1979). To minimize the influence of potential higher leverage points, we fit linear models by robust regression using M−estimation (Huber, 2004). All multiple linear regression models were performed to statistically control for sociodemographic variables (sex, age, education, race, and ethnicity). All categorical variables were binarized (0 or 1) and continuous variables in the regression models were z-scored to standardize effects. Data and study materials are available upon request. This study was not preregistered.
3. Results
3.1. Clinical characteristics of the transdiagnostic sample
All clinical groups, regardless of the primary diagnosis, scored higher on the MAP than healthy participants (tMDD>HC = 5.16, tBD>HC = 6.34, tSCZ>HC = 3.80, all p’s < 0.01), but did not significantly differ from one another (F(2,58) = 2.68, p = 0.08). Similarly, all clinical groups reported higher levels of overall negative symptoms than healthy participants (tMDD>HC = 6.52, tBD>HC = 7.44, tSCZ>HC = 6.36, all p’s < 0.01), which did not differ across diagnosis (F(2,57) = 1.43, p = 0.25). Pairwise t-tests for group differences in cognitive and clinical measures are included in Supplementary Figure S1. Participants with mood disorders (MDD and BD) reported the highest general symptoms of depression, although those with SCZ were more depressed than healthy participants (tMDD>HC = 13.82, tBD>HC = 10.34, tSCZ>HC = 4.02, all p’s < 0.05). Those with SCZ had more positive symptoms than the other groups (tSCZ>HC = 4.87, tSCZ>MDD = 4.17, tSCZ>BD = 2.87, all p’s < 0.01). Additionally, SCZ also had significantly worse overall cognitive functioning than healthy participants (t = 3.35, p = 0.007). Not surprisingly, MAP was significantly correlated with total SANS negative symptoms (r = 0.51) and depressive symptoms (r = 0.54) across the entire sample. MAP was also significantly correlated with performance on the neurocognitive battery (r = -0.34), smoking status (t = 2.06) and antipsychotic medications (r = 0.28, all p’s < 0.05, Supplementary Table S2). Given these associations between MAP and other measures, we report statistical analyses evaluating these potential confounds below.
3.2. Discount rates were not related to psychiatric diagnosis or symptoms
Discount rates, or the tendency to prefer immediate rewards, varied widely across individuals, but were not associated with primary diagnosis or MAP. Participants completed a delay discounting task that consisted of choices between $20 available now and a larger amount ($20.50–50) available in the future (1–178 days). Individual discount rates (raw k values) ranged from 0.0006 (preference for future rewards) to 0.27 (preference for immediate rewards), with a geometric mean of 0.017. Log-transformed discount rates (log10(k)) did not differ significantly across groups (F(3,77) = 0.28, p = 0.84) (Fig. 1B) or by smoking status (t = 0.81, p = 0.42), and were not correlated with MAP (r = 0.034, p = 0.78) (Fig. 1C). For the model estimating discount rates, we assessed two measures of model fit, Tjur’s D (range = 0.22–0.94, mean [SD] = 0.64 [0.17]) and the percentage of choices predicted by the model (range = 53.6 %-98.7 %, mean [SD] = 88.5 [7.75]). Neither measure significantly differed across groups (Tjur’s D: F(3,77) = 2.38, p = 0.08; % correct prediction: F(3,77) = 2.09, p = 0.11). Similarly, model fit was not significantly correlated with MAP (Tjur’s D: r = -0.12, p = 0.29; % correct prediction: r = -0.07, p = 0.54). The lack of significant differences in discount rates and model fit by diagnosis or as a function of MAP assures that any observed neural differences were not simply due to behavioral differences on the task.
3.3. Motivational and hedonic impairments were associated with dampened value signals during decision making
Motivational and hedonic deficits were associated with dampened decision value signals in the vmPFC during the delay discounting task. Consistent with previous reports (Kable and Glimcher, 2007, Kable and Glimcher, 2010), decision value was correlated with activity in widespread regions, including the vmPFC, vS and posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), across all participants (Supplementary Table S3 and Supplementary Figure S2). We defined our a priori regions of interest as the vmPFC and vS based on a meta-analysis of published studies examining the neural correlates of decision value (Bartra et al., 2013; Fig. 2A and B). As expected, we saw robust effects of DV in both ROIs (vmPFC: t = 6.53, 95 % CI [0.33, 0.62], p < 0.001; VS: t = 8.32, 95 % CI [0.34, 0.55], p < 0.001). There were no significant group differences in DV-related activity in the vmPFC (F(3,77) = 1.84, p = 0.15) or vS (F(3,77) = 1.92, p = 0.13; Fig. 2C and D). However, DV-related activity in the vmPFC was inversely correlated with MAP, such that individuals with increasing levels of motivational and hedonic deficits exhibited weaker value signals in the vmPFC (r = -0.27, p = 0.01; Fig. 2E). The relationship between MAP and DV-related activity was not significant in the vS (r = -0.10, p = 0.36; Fig. 2F), though it was in the same direction and the difference between the correlations in vmPFC and vS was not significant (Fisher r-to-z = -1.1, two-tailed p = 0.27).
The association between motivational and hedonic deficits and dampened decision value signals was specific to regions tracking decision value. When we examined DV-related activity in a region where it is not expected, the primary motor cortex (BA4a from Juelich Histological Atlas), we found no significant effects of DV (left: t = -0.40, 95 % CI [-0.15, 0.10], p = 0.69; right: t = 0.92, 95 % CI [-0.07, 0.20], p = 0.36). Moreover, we found no significant effect of group (left: F(3,77) = 1.45, p = 0.23; right: F(3,77) = 0.64, p = 0.43) and no correlation with MAP (left: r = 0.13, p = 0.23; right: r = 0.09, p = 0.43). The difference in correlations between MAP-vmPFC and MAP-motor cortex was statistically significant (left: Fisher r-to-z = 2.55, p = 0.005; right: Fisher r-to-z = 2.29, p = 0.01).
3.4. The brain-symptom relationship was specific to motivational and hedonic impairments
Though motivational and hedonic impairments were correlated with several other characteristics of the sample, these other characteristics did not explain the relationship between motivational and hedonic symptoms and reduced decision value signals in the vmPFC. To determine the specificity of the association between MAP and value-related activity in the vmPFC, we performed a series of sensitivity analyses. We first demonstrated a significant link between MAP and vmPFC activity while controlling for demographic variables such as age, sex, education, race, and ethnicity (Table 2, Model 1). We then added primary diagnosis (MDD, BD, and SCZ) to this model to rule out the possibility that the relationship is better explained by the diagnostic criteria (Table 2, Model 2). To further rule out other potential confounds, we also examined a model (Table 2, Model 3) that includes symptoms of depression (CDSS), positive and negative symptoms (SAPS and SANS), antipsychotic medications (chlorpromazine [CPZ]-equivalent dose), performance on the neurocognitive battery (CNB), current smoking, discount rate (log(k)), and model fit (Tjur’s D) (see Supplementary Table S4 for the individual associations between these confounds and DV-related activity). In multiple linear regressions that included primary diagnoses alone (Model 2) or all potential confounds (Model 3), MAP remained a significant predictor of DV-related activity, and notably, the inclusion of these covariates did not reduce the magnitude of its standardized coefficient. Although our sample is not powered to detect within-group effects, we repeated the analyses in each of the separate groups, which consistently revealed negative correlations between MAP and DV-related activity. The within-group correlation was strongest amont non-psychiatric controls, intermediate in depression groups and weakest in schizophrenia (Supplementary Figure S3). To determine whether the association between MAP and vmPFC varied across groups, we examined additional regression models that include the interaction of MAP and group. Including the interaction term (MAP*group) did not meaningfully change the model fit as measured by residual standard error (RSEModel 2 = 0.79; RSEModel 2 with interaction = 0.75; RSEModel 3 = 0.52; RSEModel 3 with interaction = 0.52). Because CPZ-equivalent dose primarily controls antipsychotics, we attempted to capture potential effects of other types of medications by excluding participants on any one class of medication. This series of analyses also did not significantly change the relationship between MAP and vmPFC activity (Supplementary Table S5).
Table 2.
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | |
---|---|---|---|
MAP | −0.34 [-0.58,-0.10]* | −0.46 [-0.75,-0.17]* | −0.58 [-0.90,-0.26]** |
Primary diagnosis | |||
MDD | – | 0.44 [-0.38,1.25] | −0.32 [-1.66,1.02] |
BD | – | 0.31 [-0.49,1.12] | −0.23 [-1.43,0.97] |
SCZ | – | −0.40 [-1.03,0.23] | −0.30 [-1.11,0.52] |
Dignosis-specific symptoms | |||
CDSS | – | – | 0.40 [-0.10,0.89] |
SAPS | – | – | 0.15 [-0.13,0.42] |
SANS | – | – | 0.11 [-0.22,0.45] |
Covariates of interest | |||
CPZ equivalent | – | – | −0.28 [-0.56,0.01] |
CNB | – | – | 0.16 [-0.09,0.40] |
Smoking | – | – | 0.16 [-0.36,0.67] |
Task performance log10(k) Tjur’s D |
– – |
– – |
−0.44 [-0.70,-0.18]* −0.09 [-0.32,0.15] |
Note: MAP = motivation and pleasure, MDD = major depressive disorder, BD = bipolar disorder, SCZ = schizophrenia, CDSS = Calgary Depression Scale for Schizophrenia, SAPS = Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms, SANS = Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms, CPZ equivalent = chlorpromazine-equivalent dose for current antipsychotic medications (in mg), CNB = mean accuracy z-score on the Penn Computerized Neurocognitive Battery, Smoking = 1 for smoker, 0 for non-smoker, log10(k) = subject-specific parameter for discount rate, Tjur’s D = Tjur’s coefficient of discrimination.
* p < 0.01.
** p < 0.001.
3.5. Additional brain regions tracking motivational and hedonic impairments
Beyond our a priori ROIs, we also conducted an exploratory analysis of the correlation between MAP and decision value signals across the whole brain. We found that higher MAP scores were associated with increased DV signals in the left PCC (Z greater than 3.1, p < 0.05, FWE corrected; Fig. 3).
3.6. Motivational and hedonic impairments were not associated with choice difficulty signals during decision making
The association with motivational and hedonic deficits was also specific to value signals and was not found for choice difficulty. To examine neural activity related to choice difficulty, we examined the effects of the absolute (i.e., unsigned) difference in decision values, which is related to the inverse of choice difficulty. We found the expected neural effects (Supplementary Table S6 and Supplementary Figure S4), including a positive effect of absolute value difference in the vmPFC (t = -2.48, 95 % CI [-0.45, −0.05], p = 0.02) but not the vS (t = 0-0.35, 95 % CI [-0.14, 0.20], p = 0.73). However, difficulty-related activity in the vmPFC did not differ by the primary diagnosis (F(3,77) = 0.64, p = 0.59) or vary with MAP (r = -0.07, p = 0.54). In an exploratory whole brain analysis, we found no regions in which absolute value difference signals were correlated with MAP.
4. Discussion
Our findings link dimensional impairments in motivation and pleasure to disruptions in specific computational neural signals during intertemporal decision-making in a transdiagnostic sample including individuals with mood disorders, psychosis, or no psychiatric diagnoses. Dozens of previous studies have identified neural correlates of decision value, the value of potential outcomes during decision-making, in the vmPFC (see meta-analysis Bartra et al., 2013). Here we find that individuals with more severe deficits in motivation and pleasure exhibited a blunting of the neural response in the vmPFC to the decision value of future rewards. Moreover, these symptoms predicted decision value signals in the vmPFC above and beyond the effects of primary diagnosis, illness-specific symptoms of depression and schizophrenia, medications, cognitive functioning, and nicotine use. These results demonstrate the specificity of the link between motivational and hedonic impairments and vmPFC function in the context of evaluating future rewards. Our findings further suggest that the vmPFC may be an important therapeutic target for amotivation and anhedonia across disorders, and demonstrate how quantitative models in decision neuroscience can help to elucidate the common pathophysiology underlying dimensional clinical deficits.
Our findings are novel and consistent with other evidence pointing to functional and morphological abnormalities in the vmPFC associated with motivational and hedonic impairments (Ward et al., 2019). Both preclinical and clinical findings suggest that vmPFC dysfunction may be normalized using direct brain stimulation, resulting in increased reward-seeking behavior in rodents and reductions of anhedonia in patients with major depression (Hamani et al., 2012, Mayberg et al., 2005). Although directly stimulating the vmPFC is not possible without surgical implants, using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) or transcranial direct-current stimulation (tDCS) to indirectly stimulate the vmPFC via inter-regional functional connectivity is an emerging area of research in decision neuroscience (Baumgartner et al., 2011, Hiser and Koenigs, 2018, Lev-Ran et al., 2012, Zheng et al., 2016), and has the potential to contribute to a novel intervention that targets motivational and hedonic symptoms across disorders.
Several prior studies have examined amotivation using effort discounting, which measures the willingness to expend cognitive or physical effort for rewards (Culbreth et al., 2018). Individuals with mood and psychotic disorders tend to avoid effort expenditure (Hershenberg et al., 2016, Wolf et al., 2014), and neuroimaging studies of effort discounting have found blunted activations at the time of decision in the striatum in major depression (Yang et al., 2016) and schizophrenia (Huang et al., 2016) and in the vmPFC in adolescents at risk for depression (Rzepa et al., 2017). An unwillingness to expend effort for rewards could be due to reduced valuation of rewards, heightened registration of effort costs, or both. An important aspect of our contribution is a test of whether amotivation is associated with dampened reward value signals during decision making, in a paradigm where there are no effort costs and amotivation is not associated with different decisions.
Several prior studies relating anhedonia to neural responses to rewards or reward cues have primarily reported blunted reward signals in the vS and often did not find a significant effect in the vmPFC (Juckel et al., 2006a, Juckel et al., 2006b, Simon et al., 2010, Stoy et al., 2012, Wacker et al., 2009). In contrast, we found that amotivation and anhedonia were associated with reduced value-related activity in the vmPFC, whereas in the vS this same relationship was a non-significant trend. This discrepancy may be partly due to the focus on experienced value signals in previous studies, versus the focus on decision value signals in the current study. meta-analyses report stronger experienced value-related activity in the vS and stronger decision value-related activity in the vmPFC, though both kinds of signals are present in both regions (Bartra et al., 2013, Oldham et al., 2018).
Beyond these a priori regions of interest, individuals with amotivation and anhedonia recruited the PCC to a greater extent in encoding the decision value of future rewards. Though exploratory, this finding is broadly consistent with a prior study that found greater PCC activation in individuals with SCZ during a delay discounting task (Avsar et al., 2013). One speculation is that this increased representation of decision value in the PCC might reflect compensation for weaker decision value signals in the vmPFC. Regardless of the interpretation, this result provides further evidence that the representation of decision values is altered in individuals with motivational and hedonic deficits.
In this study, neither diagnosis nor motivational and hedonic symptoms were associated with discount rate. Prior behavioral studies have reported elevated discount rates in SCZ (Ahn et al., 2011, Brown et al., 2018, Heerey et al., 2007, Heerey et al., 2011, Weller et al., 2014), although results are mixed in mood disorders (Ahn et al., 2011, Brown et al., 2018, Imhoff et al., 2014, Mason et al., 2012, Pulcu et al., 2014, Takahashi et al., 2008, Urošević et al., 2016). Although not statistically significant in our sample, we observed a trend in individuals with SCZ to discount more than others. Our observed effect size was indeed smaller (Cohen’s d = 0.24) than the meta-analytic estimate of medium effect size (Hedges g = 0.46; Amlung et al., 2019), though in the same direction. The absence of significant behavioral differences in our sample aids interpretation of the imaging findings, though, as the observed functional differences are not secondary to behavioral differences on the task. Specifically, differences in performance in clinical groups can confound the interpretation of neural differences, a limitation that is commonly addressed by matching for behavioral performance (Avsar et al., 2013, Barch et al., 2001, Cannon et al., 2005, Lee et al., 2008), although this approach often poses yet another confound as participants would be presented with different task stimuli by clinical status.
We hypothesize that dampened decision value signals would lead to an increase in the variability of choices, rather than a change in discount rates. This hypothesis derives from Equations (1), (2). If the disruption is to the DVs in Equation (1), the primary behavioral effect would be an increase in inconsistent choices (assuming σ in Equation (2) does not change), though there could also be some bias towards sooner or later choices depending on the person’s choice tendencies and the choice set. Lesion studies are consistent with this speculation and have found that vmPFC damage leads to more variable choices, rather than to overall shifts in preference (Fellows & Farah, 2007). Similarly, variable choice behavior has been reported in both mood and psychotic disorders (Pulcu et al., 2014, Strauss et al., 2011b, Tsypes et al., 2022, Weller et al., 2014). The delay discounting task used in our study, however, was not sensitive to detecting changes in choice variability because we presented a large range of decision values to elicit robust neural effects (i.e., the majority of the decisions were easy). Therefore, we did not find any significant correlation between choice variability and MAP, although we observed a trend that all clinical groups tended to make less consistent choices compared to those without a psychiatric history. Because vmPFC signals also predict confidence during value judgments (De Martino et al., 2013), we speculate that reduced confidence in one’s decision would be another possible effect of blunted decision value signals in this region. Future studies tailoring the decision values near the participant’s indifference point (i.e., including more difficult decisions) and including trial-by-trial measures of confidence would be able to empirically test these hypotheses.
Our study has several limitations. First, although our findings remained significant after accounting for the effect of primary diagnoses, suggesting a dimensional link between MAP and disruptions in value-related signals, there was also some evidence pointing towards categorical effects. Specifically, the SCZ group showed reduced activity in the vmPFC despite intermediate MAP and weaker vmPFC-MAP correlation within group. Furthermore, the dimensional finding was strongest in controls despite overall lower MAP, suggesting that there may be other factors in the clinical groups that impact MAP and/or vmPFC activity, perhaps partly obscuring the relationship that is present in the controls who may have fewer confounds such as medications and comorbidities. Second, although the overall sample size was relatively large, the sample sizes for individual diagnoses were small, and therefore we did not have the sufficient statistical power to examine any within-group effect. In addition, the neuroimaging and task parameters may not have been maximally sensitive to weaker effects of value-related signals. Third, the MAP subscale of the CAINS constitutes a single scale not suited for distinguishing motivation and pleasure. Anticipatory pleasure is commonly associated with motivation and goal-directed behavior, and motivation and pleasure often load on a single factor. Although psychometric, correlational, and preclinical studies suggest that clinical deficits in the two domains are interrelated, others, including the DSM, separate these into two distinct domains. We used a measure that combines motivation and pleasure because the two are clinically interrelated and our task engages both aspects of reward processing. Moreover, both the CAINS and the CDSS were developed and validated in schizophrenia-only samples, and their psychometric properties remain unknown in healthy, unipolar and bipolar depression samples; the lack of symptom measures validated across diagnostic groups is a limitation for much transdiagnostic work. Fourth, because the immediate reward was fixed at $20 across all trials, our study does not discriminate between neural activity correlated with the decision value of the delayed option, the sum of the decision values of the two options, or the signed difference between the decision values of the two options. Future studies that systematically vary the immediate reward amount may reveal links between MAP and these more specific aspects of value-related activity. Fifth, given the cross-sectional and correlational nature of our study, no causal association between the vmPFC value signals and MAP can be inferred. Lastly, we did not evaluate whether vmPFC hypofunction is a general feature of decision-making, or rather specific to delay discounting. Despite these limitations, these findings demonstrate that perturbed value signals in this region during the evaluation of future rewards were specifically linked to impairments in motivation and pleasure, after accounting for the effects of diagnosis and general symptoms. Demonstrating such symptom-specific alterations helps elucidate the pathophysiological underpinnings of amotivation and anhedonia and can inform future treatment development.
5. Conclusion
Taken together, our results link impairments in motivation and pleasure to disruptions in specific computational neural signals during decision-making. In particular, more severe deficits in motivation and pleasure were related to blunted decision value-related responses in the vmPFC during a prospective decision-making task. Furthermore, motivational and hedonic symptoms predicted decision value signals in this region above and beyond the effects of primary diagnosis, illness-specific symptoms of depression and schizophrenia, medications, cognitive functioning, and nicotine use. Though the causal mechanisms remain to be understood, our results suggest the therapeutic potential of interventions targeting symptom-specific changes in neural signals across disorders.
Author Note
Data collection and analysis were sponsored by the AE Foundation, National Institute of Mental Health (R01MH113565 and R01MH101111 to DHW; R01MH113550, R01MH107703, and K23MH098130 to TDS), NARSAD/BBRF, University of Pennsylvania (Penn Translational Neuroscience Center [to DHW, TDS, and JWK], and Penn-CHOP Lifespan Brain Institute [to TDS]). Portions of these findings were presented as a poster at the 2019 Society for Neuroeconomics, Dublin, Ireland. A preliminary draft of the manuscript was posted on bioRxiv: https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2020.12.01.407197v2. All authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose. All study procedures were approved by the University of Pennsylvania Institutional Review Board in compliance with the APA Ethical Principles (Protocol #810211). Data and study materials are available upon request. This study was not preregistered.
CRediT authorship contribution statement
Min K. Souther: Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Visualization. Daniel H. Wolf: Conceptualization, Methodology, Resources, Writing – review & editing, Supervision, Funding acquisition. Rebecca Kazinka: Formal analysis, Data curation, Writing – review & editing. Sangil Lee: Formal analysis, Resources, Writing – review & editing. Kosha Ruparel: Data curation, Writing – review & editing. Mark A. Elliott: Methodology, Writing – review & editing. Anna Xu: Data curation, Writing – review & editing. Matthew Cieslak: Methodology, Writing – review & editing. Greer Prettyman: Writing – review & editing. Theodore D. Satterthwaite: Conceptualization, Methodology, Resources, Writing – review & editing, Supervision, Funding acquisition. Joseph W. Kable: Conceptualization, Methodology, Resources, Writing – review & editing, Supervision, Funding acquisition.
Declaration of Competing Interest
The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.
Footnotes
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2022.103227.
Appendix A. Supplementary data
The following are the Supplementary data to this article:
Data availability
Data will be made available on request.
References
- Addington D., Addington J., Schissel B. A depression rating scale for schizophrenics. Schizophr. Res. 1990;3(4):247–251. doi: 10.1016/0920-9964(90)90005-r. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahn W.-Y., Rass O., Fridberg D.J., Bishara A.J., Forsyth J.K., Breier A., Busemeyer J.R., Hetrick W.P., Bolbecker A.R., O’Donnell B.F. Temporal discounting of rewards in patients with bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2011;120(4):911–921. doi: 10.1037/a0023333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amlung M., Marsden E., Holshausen K., Morris V., Patel H., Vedelago L., Naish K.R., Reed D.D., McCabe R.E. Delay Discounting as a Transdiagnostic Process in Psychiatric Disorders: A Meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry. 2019;76(11):1176. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.2102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersson J.L., Jenkinson M., Smith S. the University of Oxford; 2007. Non-linear registration, aka spatial normalisation FMRIB Technial Report TR07JA2. FMRIB Analysis Group of. [Google Scholar]
- Andreasen N.C. Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS) The British J. Psychiatry. 1989;155(Suppl 7):53–58. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andreasen N.C. The Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS): Conceptual and Theoretical Foundations. The British J. Psychiatry. 1989;155(S7):49–52. doi: 10.1192/S0007125000291496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arrondo G., Segarra N., Metastasio A., Ziauddeen H., Spencer J., Reinders N.R., Dudas R.B., Robbins T.W., Fletcher P.C., Murray G.K. Reduction in ventral striatal activity when anticipating a reward in depression and schizophrenia: A replicated cross-diagnostic finding. Front. Psychol. 2015;6 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Avsar K.B., Weller R.E., Cox J.E., Reid M.A., White D.M., Lahti A.C. An fMRI investigation of delay discounting in patients with schizophrenia. Brain and Behavior. 2013;3(4):384–401. doi: 10.1002/brb3.135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ballard E.D., Wills K., Lally N., Richards E.M., Luckenbaugh D.A., Walls T., Ameli R., Niciu M.J., Brutsche N.E., Park L., Zarate J.C. Anhedonia as a clinical correlate of suicidal thoughts in clinical ketamine trials. J. Affect. Disord. 2017;218:195–200. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.04.057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barch D.M., Carter C.S., Braver T.S., Sabb F.W., MacDonald A., Noll D.C., Cohen J.D. Selective Deficits in Prefrontal Cortex Function in Medication-Naive Patients With Schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2001;58(3):280–288. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.3.280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barch D.M., Pagliaccio D., Luking K. In: Behavioral Neuroscience of Motivation. Simpson E.H., Balsam P.D., editors. Springer International Publishing; 2016. Mechanisms Underlying Motivational Deficits in Psychopathology: Similarities and Differences in Depression and Schizophrenia; pp. 411–449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bartra O., McGuire J.T., Kable J.W. The valuation system: A coordinate-based meta-analysis of BOLD fMRI experiments examining neural correlates of subjective value. NeuroImage. 2013;76:412–427. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baumgartner T., Knoch D., Hotz P., Eisenegger C., Fehr E. Dorsolateral and ventromedial prefrontal cortex orchestrate normative choice. Nat. Neurosci. 2011;14(11):1468–1474. doi: 10.1038/nn.2933. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beckmann C.F., Jenkinson M., Smith S.M. General multilevel linear modeling for group analysis in FMRI. NeuroImage. 2003;20(2):1052–1063. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00435-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bickel W.K., Yi R., Landes R.D., Hill P.F., Baxter C. Remember the Future: Working Memory Training Decreases Delay Discounting Among Stimulant Addicts. Biol. Psychiatry. 2011;69(3):260–265. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.08.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown H.E., Hart K.L., Snapper L.A., Roffman J.L., Perlis R.H. Impairment in delay discounting in schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder but not primary mood disorders. npj Schizophr. 2018;4(1):9. doi: 10.1038/s41537-018-0050-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cannon T.D., Glahn D.C., Kim J., Erp T.G.M.V., Karlsgodt K., Cohen M.S., Nuechterlein K.H., Bava S., Shirinyan D. Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex Activity During Maintenance and Manipulation of Information in Working Memory in Patients With Schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2005;62(10):1071–1080. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.10.1071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Culbreth A.J., Moran E.K., Barch D.M. Effort-cost decision-making in psychosis and depression: Could a similar behavioral deficit arise from disparate psychological and neural mechanisms? Psychol. Med. 2018;48(6):889–904. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717002525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Martino B., Fleming S.M., Garrett N., Dolan R. Confidence in value-based choice. Nat. Neurosci. 2013;16(1):105–110. doi: 10.1038/nn.3279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delgado M.R., Nystrom L.E., Fissell C., Noll D.C., Fiez J.A. Tracking the hemodynamic responses to reward and punishment in the striatum. J. Neurophysiol. 2000;84(6):3072–3077. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.84.6.3072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ducasse D., Loas G., Dassa D., Gramaglia C., Zeppegno P., Guillaume S., Olié E., Courtet P. Anhedonia is associated with suicidal ideation independently of depression: A meta-analysis. Depression and Anxiety. 2018;35(5):382–392. doi: 10.1002/da.22709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Epstein J., Pan H., Kocsis J.H., Yang Y., Butler T., Chusid J., Hochberg H., Murrough J., Strohmayer E., Stern E., Silbersweig D.A. Lack of ventral striatal response to positive stimuli in depressed versus normal subjects. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 2006;163(10):1784–1790. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.10.1784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fellows L.K., Farah M.J. The Role of Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex in Decision Making: Judgment under Uncertainty or Judgment Per Se? Cereb. Cortex. 2007;17(11):2669–2674. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- First, M. B., & Gibbon, M. (2004). The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID-I) and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Disorders (SCID-II). In Comprehensive handbook of psychological assessment, Vol. 2: Personality assessment (pp. 134–143). John Wiley & Sons Inc.
- Franke P., Maier W., Hardt J., Hain C. Cognitive functioning and anhedonia in subjects at risk for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 1993;10(1):77–84. doi: 10.1016/0920-9964(93)90079-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gold J.M., Waltz J.A., Prentice K.J., Morris S.E., Heerey E.A. Reward Processing in Schizophrenia: A Deficit in the Representation of Value. Schizophr. Bull. 2008;34(5):835–847. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gold J.M., Waltz J.A., Matveeva T.M., Kasanova Z., Strauss G.P., Herbener E.S., Collins A.G.E., Frank M.J. Negative Symptoms and the Failure to Represent the Expected Reward Value of Actions: Behavioral and Computational Modeling Evidence. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2012;69(2):129–138. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gradin V.B., Kumar P., Waiter G., Ahearn T., Stickle C., Milders M., Reid I., Hall J., Steele J.D. Expected value and prediction error abnormalities in depression and schizophrenia. Brain: A. J. Neurol. 2011;134(Pt 6):1751–1764. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hajcak G., Moser J.S., Holroyd C.B., Simons R.F. The feedback-related negativity reflects the binary evaluation of good versus bad outcomes. Biol. Psychol. 2006;71(2):148–154. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamani C., Machado D.C., Hipólide D.C., Dubiela F.P., Suchecki D., Macedo C.E., Tescarollo F., Martins U., Covolan L., Nobrega J.N. Deep Brain Stimulation Reverses Anhedonic-Like Behavior in a Chronic Model of Depression: Role of Serotonin and Brain Derived Neurotrophic Factor. Biol. Psychiatry. 2012;71(1):30–35. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.08.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harvey P.-O., Pruessner J., Czechowska Y., Lepage M. Individual differences in trait anhedonia: A structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging study in non-clinical subjects. Mol. Psychiatry. 2007;12(8), 703:767–775. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heerey E.A., Robinson B.M., McMahon R.P., Gold J.M. Delay discounting in schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry. 2007;12(3):213–221. doi: 10.1080/13546800601005900. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heerey E.A., Matveeva T.M., Gold J.M. Imagining the future: Degraded representations of future rewards and events in schizophrenia. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2011;120(2):483–489. doi: 10.1037/a0021810. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hershenberg R., Satterthwaite T.D., Daldal A., Katchmar N., Moore T.M., Kable J.W., Wolf D.H. Diminished effort on a progressive ratio task in both unipolar and bipolar depression. J. Affect. Disord. 2016;196:97–100. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.02.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hinson J.M., Jameson T.L., Whitney P. Impulsive decision making and working memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2003;29(2):298–306. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.29.2.298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hiser J., Koenigs M. The multifaceted role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in emotion, decision-making, social cognition, and psychopathology. Biol. Psychiatry. 2018;83(8):638–647. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.10.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holm, S. (1979). A Simple Sequentially Rejective Multiple Test Procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 6(2), 65–70. JSTOR.
- Huang, J., Yang, X., Lan, Y., Zhu, C., Liu, X., Wang, Y., Cheung, E. F. C., Xie, G., & Chan, R. C. K. (20160407). Neural substrates of the impaired effort expenditure decision making in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology, 30(6), 685. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000284. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Huber P.J. John Wiley & Sons; 2004. Robust Statistics. [Google Scholar]
- Husain M., Roiser J.P. Neuroscience of apathy and anhedonia: A transdiagnostic approach. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2018;19(8):470–484. doi: 10.1038/s41583-018-0029-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Imhoff S., Harris M., Weiser J., Reynolds B. Delay discounting by depressed and non-depressed adolescent smokers and non-smokers. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2014;135:152–155. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.11.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Insel T.R. Translating Scientific Opportunity Into Public Health Impact: A Strategic Plan for Research on Mental Illness. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2009;66(2):128–133. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2008.540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkinson M., Bannister P., Brady M., Smith S. Improved Optimization for the Robust and Accurate Linear Registration and Motion Correction of Brain Images. NeuroImage. 2002;17(2):825–841. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkinson M., Smith S. A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Med. Image Anal. 2001;5(2):143–156. doi: 10.1016/s1361-8415(01)00036-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Juckel G., Schlagenhauf F., Koslowski M., Filonov D., Wüstenberg T., Villringer A., Knutson B., Kienast T., Gallinat J., Wrase J., Heinz A. Dysfunction of ventral striatal reward prediction in schizophrenic patients treated with typical, not atypical, neuroleptics. Psychopharmacology. 2006;187(2):222–228. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0405-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Juckel G., Schlagenhauf F., Koslowski M., Wüstenberg T., Villringer A., Knutson B., Wrase J., Heinz A. Dysfunction of ventral striatal reward prediction in schizophrenia. NeuroImage. 2006;29(2):409–416. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kable J.W., Glimcher P.W. The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice. Nat. Neurosci. 2007;10(12):1625–1633. doi: 10.1038/nn2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kable J.W., Glimcher P.W. An “as soon as possible” effect in human intertemporal decision making: Behavioral evidence and neural mechanisms. J. Neurophysiol. 2010;103(5):2513–2531. doi: 10.1152/jn.00177.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keedwell P.A., Andrew C., Williams S.C.R., Brammer M.J., Phillips M.L. The neural correlates of anhedonia in major depressive disorder. Biol. Psychiatry. 2005;58(11):843–853. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.05.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khazanov G.K., Ruscio A.M., Forbes C.N. The positive valence systems scale: development and validation. Assessment. 2020;27(5):1045–1069. doi: 10.1177/1073191119869836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirby K.N., Herrnstein R.J. Preference reversals due to myopic discounting of delayed reward. Psychol. Sci. 1995;6(2):83–89. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00311.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kirby K.N., MarakoviĆ N.N. Delay-discounting probabilistic rewards: Rates decrease as amounts increase. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 1996;3(1):100–104. doi: 10.3758/BF03210748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knutson B., Westdorp A., Kaiser E., Hommer D. FMRI visualization of brain activity during a monetary incentive delay task. NeuroImage. 2000;12(1):20–27. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kotov R., Krueger R.F., Watson D., Achenbach T.M., Althoff R.R., Bagby R.M., Brown T.A., Carpenter W.T., Caspi A., Clark L.A., Eaton N.R., Forbes M.K., Forbush K.T., Goldberg D., Hasin D., Hyman S.E., Ivanova M.Y., Lynam D.R., Markon K., Miller J.D., Moffitt T.E., Morey L.C., Mullins-Sweatt S.N., Ormel J., Patrick C.J., Regier D.A., Rescorla L., Ruggero C.J., Samuel D.B., Sellbom M., Simms L.J., Skodol A.E., Slade T., South S.C., Tackett J.L., Waldman I.D., Waszczuk M.A., Widiger T.A., Wright A.G.C., Zimmerman M. The hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology (HiTOP): A dimensional alternative to traditional nosologies. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2017;126(4):454–477. doi: 10.1037/abn0000258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kotov R., Cicero D.C., Conway C.C., DeYoung C.G., Dombrovski A., Eaton N.R., First M.B., Forbes M.K., Hyman S.E., Jonas K.G., Krueger R.F., Latzman R.D., Li J.J., Nelson B.D., Regier D.A., Rodriguez-Seijas C., Ruggero C.J., Simms L.J., Skodol A.E., Waldman I.D., Waszczuk M.A., Watson D., Widiger T.A., Wilson S., Wright A.G.C. The Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP) in psychiatric practice and research. Psychol. Med. 2022;52(9):1666–1678. doi: 10.1017/S0033291722001301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kring A.M., Barch D.M. The motivation and pleasure dimension of negative symptoms: Neural substrates and behavioral outputs. European Neuropsychopharmacology: The Journal of the European College of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;24(5):725–736. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2013.06.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kring A.M., Gur R.E., Blanchard J.J., Horan W.P., Reise S.P. The Clinical Assessment Interview for Negative Symptoms (CAINS): Final Development and Validation. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2013;170(2):165–172. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12010109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lambert C., Da Silva S., Ceniti A.K., Rizvi S.J., Foussias G., Kennedy S.H. Anhedonia in depression and schizophrenia: A transdiagnostic challenge. CNS Neurosci Ther. 2018;24:615–623. doi: 10.1111/cns.12854. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee J., Folley B.S., Gore J., Park S., Zak P. Origins of Spatial Working Memory Deficits in Schizophrenia: An Event-Related fMRI and Near-Infrared Spectroscopy Study. PLoS ONE. 2008;3(3):e1760. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001760. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lempert K.M., Pizzagalli D.A. Delay Discounting and Future-directed Thinking in Anhedonic Individuals. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry. 2010;41(3):258–264. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.02.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lev-Ran S., Shamay-Tsoory S.G., Zangen A., Levkovitz Y. Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex impairs theory of mind learning. European Psychiatry: The Journal of the Association of European Psychiatrists. 2012;27(4):285–289. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2010.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mason L., O’Sullivan N., Blackburn M., Bentall R., El-Deredy W. I want it now! Neural correlates of hypersensitivity to immediate reward in hypomania. Biol. Psychiatry. 2012;71(6):530–537. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.10.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayberg H.S., Lozano A.M., Voon V., McNeely H.E., Seminowicz D., Hamani C., Schwalb J.M., Kennedy S.H. Deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant depression. Neuron. 2005;45(5):651–660. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McIntyre R.S., Woldeyohannes H.O., Soczynska J.K., Maruschak N.A., Wium-Andersen I.K., Vinberg M., Cha D.S., Lee Y., Xiao H.X., Gallaugher L.A., Dale R.M., Alsuwaidan M.T., Mansur R.B., Muzina D.J., Carvalho A.F., Jerrell J.M., Kennedy S.H. Anhedonia and cognitive function in adults with MDD: Results from the International Mood Disorders Collaborative Project. CNS Spectr. 2016;21(5):362–366. doi: 10.1017/S1092852915000747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McMakin D.L., Olino T.M., Porta G., Dietz L.J., Emslie G., Clarke G., Wagner K.D., Asarnow J.R., Ryan N.D., Birmaher B., Shamseddeen W., Mayes T., Kennard B., Spirito A., Keller M., Lynch F.L., Dickerson J.F., Brent D.A. Anhedonia predicts poorer recovery among youth with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor treatment-resistant depression. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2012;51(4):404–411. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.01.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore T.M., Reise S.P., Gur R.E., Hakonarson H., Gur R.C. Psychometric properties of the Penn Computerized Neurocognitive Battery. Neuropsychology. 2015;29(2):235–246. doi: 10.1037/neu0000093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mukherjee D., Filipowicz A.L.S., Vo K., Satterthwaite T.D., Kable J.W. Reward and punishment reversal-learning in major depressive disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2020;129(8):810–823. doi: 10.1037/abn0000641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myerson J., Green L. Discounting of Delayed Rewards: Models of Individual Choice. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 1995;64(3):263–276. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1995.64-263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oldham S., Murawski C., Fornito A., Youssef G., Yücel M., Lorenzetti V. The anticipation and outcome phases of reward and loss processing: A neuroimaging meta-analysis of the monetary incentive delay task. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2018;39(8):3398–3418. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park I.H., Lee B.C., Kim J.-J., Kim J.I., Koo M.-S. Effort-based reinforcement processing and functional connectivity underlying amotivation in medicated patients with depression and schizophrenia. J. Neurosci. 2017;37(16):4370–4380. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2524-16.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Platt M.L., Plassmann H. In: Neuroeconomics. Second Edition. Glimcher P.W., Fehr E., editors. Academic Press; 2014. Chapter 13—Multistage Valuation Signals and Common Neural Currencies; pp. 237–258. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Poline J.B., Worsley K.J., Evans A.C., Friston K.J. Combining spatial extent and peak intensity to test for activations in functional imaging. NeuroImage. 1997;5(2):83–96. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1996.0248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prettyman G.E., Kable J.W., Didier P., Shankar S., Satterthwaite T.D., Davatzikos C., Bilker W.B., Elliott M.A., Ruparel K., Wolf D.H. Relationship of ventral striatum activation during effort discounting to clinical amotivation severity in schizophrenia. npj Schizophr. 2021;7:48. doi: 10.1038/s41537-021-00178-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pulcu E., Trotter P.D., Thomas E.J., McFarquhar M., Juhasz G., Sahakian B.J., Deakin J.F.W., Zahn R., Anderson I.M., Elliott R. Temporal discounting in major depressive disorder. Psychol. Med. 2014;44(9):1825–1834. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713002584. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rachlin H., Raineri A., Cross D. Subjective probability and delay. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 1991;55(2):233–244. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1991.55-233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richards J.B., Zhang L., Mitchell S.H., de Wit H. Delay or probability discounting in a model of impulsive behavior: effect of alcohol. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 1999;71(2):121–143. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1999.71-121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rzepa E., Fisk J., McCabe C. Blunted neural response to anticipation, effort and consummation of reward and aversion in adolescents with depression symptomatology. Journal of Psychopharmacology (Oxford, England) 2017;31(3):303–311. doi: 10.1177/0269881116681416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Satterthwaite T.D., Kable J.W., Vandekar L., Katchmar N., Bassett D.S., Baldassano C.F., Ruparel K., Elliott M.A., Sheline Y.I., Gur R.C., Gur R.E., Davatzikos C., Leibenluft E., Thase M.E., Wolf D.H. Common and dissociable dysfunction of the reward system in bipolar and unipolar depression. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2015;40(9):2258–2268. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schilbach L., Hoffstaedter F., Müller V., Cieslik E.C., Goya-Maldonado R., Trost S., Sorg C., Riedl V., Jardri R., Sommer I., Kogler L., Derntl B., Gruber O., Eickhoff S.B. Transdiagnostic commonalities and differences in resting state functional connectivity of the default mode network in schizophrenia and major depression. NeuroImage: Clinical. 2016;10:326–335. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.11.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schüller C.B., Kuhn J., Jessen F., Hu X. Neuronal correlates of delay discounting in healthy subjects and its implication for addiction: An ALE meta-analysis study. Am. J. Drug and Alcohol Abuse. 2019;45(1):51–66. doi: 10.1080/00952990.2018.1557675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Segarra N., Metastasio A., Ziauddeen H., Spencer J., Reinders N.R., Dudas R.B., Arrondo G., Robbins T.W., Clark L., Fletcher P.C., Murray G.K. Abnormal Frontostriatal Activity During Unexpected Reward Receipt in Depression and Schizophrenia: Relationship to Anhedonia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;41(8):2001–2010. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sharma A., Wolf D.H., Ciric R., Kable J.W., Moore T.M., Vandekar S.N., Katchmar N., Daldal A., Ruparel K., Davatzikos C., Elliott M.A., Calkins M.E., Shinohara R.T., Bassett D.S., Satterthwaite T.D. Connectome-wide analysis reveals common dimensional reward deficits across mood and psychotic disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2017;174(7):657–666. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16070774. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simon J.J., Biller A., Walther S., Roesch-Ely D., Stippich C., Weisbrod M., Kaiser S. Neural correlates of reward processing in schizophrenia—Relationship to apathy and depression. Schizophr. Res. 2010;118(1):154–161. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoy M., Schlagenhauf F., Sterzer P., Bermpohl F., Hägele C., Suchotzki K., Schmack K., Wrase J., Ricken R., Knutson B., Adli M., Bauer M., Heinz A., Ströhle A. Hyporeactivity of ventral striatum towards incentive stimuli in unmedicated depressed patients normalizes after treatment with escitalopram. J. Psychopharmacol. 2012;26(5):677–688. doi: 10.1177/0269881111416686. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strauss G.P., Frank M.J., Waltz J.A., Kasanova Z., Herbener E.S., Gold J.M. Deficits in positive reinforcement learning and uncertainty-driven exploration are associated with distinct aspects of negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry. 2011;69(5):424–431. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.10.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strauss G.P., Gold J.M. A psychometric comparison of the clinical assessment interview for negative symptoms and the brief negative symptom scale. Schizophr. Bull. 2016;42(6):1384–1394. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbw046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strauss G.P., Robinson B.M., Waltz J.A., Frank M.J., Kasanova Z., Herbener E.S., Gold J.M. Patients with schizophrenia demonstrate inconsistent preference judgments for affective and nonaffective stimuli. Schizophr. Bull. 2011;37(6):1295–1304. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takahashi T., Oono H., Inoue T., Boku S., Kako Y., Kitaichi Y., Kusumi I., Masui T., Nakagawa S., Suzuki K., Tanaka T., Koyama T., Radford M.H.B. Depressive patients are more impulsive and inconsistent in intertemporal choice behavior for monetary gain and loss than healthy subjects—An analysis based on Tsallis’ statistics. Neuro Endocrinology Letters. 2008;29(3):351–358. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Treadway M.T., Zald D.H. Reconsidering anhedonia in depression: Lessons from translational neuroscience. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2011;35(3):537–555. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.06.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsypes A., Szanto K., Bridge J.A., Brown V.M., Keilp J.G., Dombrovski A.Y. Delay discounting in suicidal behavior: Myopic preference or inconsistent valuation? J. Psychopathology and Clinical Science. 2022;131(1):34–44. doi: 10.1037/abn0000717. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Urošević S., Youngstrom E.A., Collins P., Jensen J.B., Luciana M. Associations of age with reward delay discounting and response inhibition in adolescents with bipolar disorders. J. Affect. Disord. 2016;190:649–656. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wacker J., Dillon D.G., Pizzagalli D.A. The role of the nucleus accumbens and rostral anterior cingulate cortex in anhedonia: Integration of resting EEG, fMRI, and volumetric techniques. NeuroImage. 2009;46(1):327–337. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waltz J.A., Gold J.M. Motivational deficits in schizophrenia and the representation of expected value. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences. 2016;27:375–410. doi: 10.1007/7854_2015_385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ward J., Lyall L.M., Bethlehem R.A.I., Ferguson A., Strawbridge R.J., Lyall D.M., Cullen B., Graham N., Johnston K.J.A., Bailey M.E.S., Murray G.K., Smith D.J. Novel genome-wide associations for anhedonia, genetic correlation with psychiatric disorders, and polygenic association with brain structure. Transl. Psychiatry. 2019;9(1):327. doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-0635-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiskopf N., Hutton C., Josephs O., Deichmann R. Optimal EPI parameters for reduction of susceptibility-induced BOLD sensitivity losses: A whole-brain analysis at 3 T and 1.5 T. NeuroImage. 2006;33(2):493–504. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weller R.E., Avsar K.B., Cox J.E., Reid M.A., White D.M., Lahti A.C. Delay discounting and task performance consistency in patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2014;215(2):286–293. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.11.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitton A., Treadway M., Pizzagalli D. Reward processing dysfunction in major depression, bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Current Opinion in Psychiatry. 2015;28(1):7–12. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wing V.C., Moss T.G., Rabin R.A., George T.P. Effects of cigarette smoking status on delay discounting in schizophrenia and healthy controls. Addict. Behav. 2012;37(1):67–72. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.08.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winograd-Gurvich C., Fitzgerald P.B., Georgiou-Karistianis N., Bradshaw J.L., White O.B. Negative symptoms: A review of schizophrenia, melancholic depression and Parkinson’s disease. Brain Res. Bull. 2006;70(4):312–321. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2006.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolf D.H., Satterthwaite T.D., Kantrowitz J.J., Katchmar N., Vandekar L., Elliott M.A., Ruparel K. Amotivation in schizophrenia: integrated assessment with behavioral, clinical, and imaging measures. Schizophr. Bull. 2014;40(6):1328–1337. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbu026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woolrich M. Robust group analysis using outlier inference. NeuroImage. 2008;41(2):286–301. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang X., Huang J., Lan Y., Zhu C., Liu X., Wang Y., Cheung E.F.C., Xie G., Chan R.C.K. Diminished caudate and superior temporal gyrus responses to effort-based decision making in patients with first-episode major depressive disorder. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry. 2016;64:52–59. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2015.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yu L.Q., Lee S., Katchmar N., Satterthwaite T.D., Kable J.W., Wolf D.H. Steeper discounting of delayed rewards in schizophrenia but not first-degree Relatives. Psychiatry Res. 2017;252:303–309. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.02.062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang B., Lin P., Shi H., Öngür D., Auerbach R.P., Wang X., Yao S., Wang X. Mapping anhedonia-specific dysfunction in a transdiagnostic approach: An ALE meta-analysis. Brain Imaging and Behavior. 2016;10(3):920–939. doi: 10.1007/s11682-015-9457-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zheng H., Huang D., Chen S., Wang S., Guo W., Luo J., Ye H., Chen Y. Modulating the activity of ventromedial prefrontal cortex by anodal tDCS enhances the trustee’s repayment through altruism. Front. Psychol. 2016;7:1437. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Data will be made available on request.