Abstract
We propose that older adults’ ability to retrieve episodic autobiographical events, while often viewed through a lens of decline, reveals much about what is preserved and prioritized in cognitive aging. Central to our proposal is the idea that the so-called “gist” of an autobiographical event is not only spared with normal aging but also well adapted to serve memory-guided behavior in older age. To support our proposal, we review neurocognitive evidence indicating an age-related shift towards gist memory. We then discuss why this shift likely arises from more than age-related decline, and instead reflects a natural, arguably adaptive, effect of experience, motivational, and mode-of-thinking factors. Our proposal reveals an upside of age-related memory changes and identifies important research questions.
Keywords: event cognition, autobiographical memory, episodic memory, semantic memory, aging, naturalistic memory
Aging and autobiographical event memory
Autobiographical event memories (see Glossary), “episodic” memories of personal moments situated in time and place, are a defining feature of human consciousness and serve a multitude of identity, social, and decision-making functions [1,2]. Constructing autobiographical event memories requires integrating a variety of details, including perceptual and narrative information, into coherent representations of personal moments in the life story [3,4]. To date, research on normal (i.e., cognitively typical) aging has focused on showing how an age-related decline in episodic memory translates to older adults describing autobiographical events with less specific detail, such as forgetting exactly where an event took place and the color of someone’s jacket. This reduction in specific detail has been attributed to age-related deficits in several learning and memory mechanisms (among others) [5–7] and may reflect multiple age-related changes in brain functioning [8–11].
However, there is a growing body of lab-based research suggesting that older adults can still describe details from complex events at a general level, such as what type of experience a memory refers to (e.g. a holiday dinner) and the type of jacket worn (a parka) [12–15] (but also see [16]). In fact, as illustrated in Figure 1, older adults tend to add more general details to their autobiographical event memories relative to young adults. While this tendency toward representing events at a general level has the potential to invite irrelevant or distracting information [7], it also can capture an event’s broader meaning (how this event is like other family gatherings) [3,17–20], which may be critical for functions of memory, like generalization and integration [21]. In this Opinion, we draw attention to understanding how and why general details of autobiographical event memories have such a prominent role among older adults. We propose that at the heart of this stronghold for autobiographical event memory is the preservation and prioritization of an event’s “gist”, which we view as an outcome of cognitive, brain, and motivation-driven factors that change with age.
Gist and specific representations of memory
Cognitive aging experiments of lab-derived episodic memory, largely testing memory for recently encoded non-personal stimuli (e.g., words and pictures paired together), have long been clear on one point: among older adults, gist representations, meaning general categorical (superordinate and basic category level) or prototypical information (e.g., I was shown animal words; they were superimposed on outdoor scenes) about the learned stimuli, are better preserved relative to specific representations, that is the concrete, high-fidelity details about these stimuli (e.g., I saw a Boston terrier paired with a beach scene) [22–24]. Several lines of work have built on these findings, including the influential Fuzzy Trace Theory [24]. Here, we take a closer look at the connection between gist representations and normal age-related changes in the way autobiographical events are formed and remembered. In doing so, our Opinion goes beyond prior work on aging and gist memory in two important ways. First, much of the research on cognitive aging has viewed gist representations in a largely negative light, assuming gist is a primary source of memory errors that are more common in older adults. In contrast, we cast a positive light on gist representations as not only a bastion for older adults’ capacity to remember autobiographical events in the face of declining specifics, but also increasingly relevant, and arguably adapted to, age-related changes in motivated remembering. Second, whereas prior work has drawn a connection between gist representations and semantic memory, we, alongside others [19,20,25–28], are proposing that gist representations are situated between the boundaries of semantic and episodic memory, particularly in the context of autobiographical events (Box 1). To begin to explore our proposal, we review evidence showing that gist representations for events are intact in typically-aging older adults, and we discuss how this is reflected in the brain.
Box 1. Gist contributes to the debate surrounding episodic and semantic memory.
The possibility that episodic memory (i.e., memory for unique events) and semantic memory (i.e., knowledge extracted from experiences) may depend on dissociable brain systems has inspired decades of human memory research [104,105]. Recent work, however, suggests that the neural and cognitive boundaries between episodic memory and semantic memory are less striking when we consider methodological and theoretical issues [25–27,56,106]. These findings bring to the forefront an earlier framework suggesting that episodic and semantic memory may fall on a single continuum, such that semantic knowledge is stored at various levels of abstraction, with many forms of knowledge approximating the qualities of episodic memory [23].
If we entertain the idea of a continuum, we believe it is best to view gist representations as a type of memory taking an intermediate position. On the one hand, a gist representation includes the main elements (i.e., items) and setting (i.e., scene) of a unique event, and therefore maintains a semblance of episodicisity. On the other hand, these elements are represented in terms of their generalities, which pushes gist representations to some extent toward the abstract end of the continuum. As such, gist representations cannot be firmly situated within the camp of either episodic memory or semantic memory, based on their traditional definitions.
This way of framing gist representations aligns with recent neuropsychological findings [56,106,107] suggesting that the hippocampus/medial temporal lobe contributes to the retrieval of personal knowledge, but only if the knowledge has qualities typically ascribed to episodic memory. A continuum framework also might help explain different profiles of event memory impairment in age-related neurodegenerative diseases. For example, while progressively worsening categorical knowledge loss is the defining feature of semantic dementia, the retrieval of autobiographical event memories also is affected. Interestingly, several studies have shown that in the early stages of semantic dementia, when atrophy is most pronounced in anterolateral temporal lobe regions connected to the anterior hippocampus, it is remote, gist-like autobiographical event memories that are primarily affected, with recent, highly specific autobiographical event memories being relatively spared until posterior cortical thinning advances in later stages [108,109]. In contrast to semantic dementia, recent work involving individuals with amnestic mild cognitive impairment raises the possibility that Alzheimer’s disease may affect both gist and specific event memory representations early on [15], which is consistent with evidence of widespread pathology affecting both anterior and posterior hippocampal-cortical circuity in early to intermediate stages of the disease [110].
Gist representations are spared in typical aging
For older adults to effectively support the type of general detail preserved and often elevated in their autobiographical event memories, gist representations cannot simply offer a vague theme of the past. Rather, older adults must be able to call on gist representations to capture a basic degree of fidelity for several defining qualities of a singular event, including the items, scene, time, and narrative storyline behind their experiences. In recent years, laboratory-based research has begun to reveal evidence in favor of this proposition. For instance, several studies have found that while older adults forget specific details about singular items (e.g., an item’s exact cost), they nonetheless can recall as well as young adults basic or superordinate category-level features about particular items (e.g., whether the item was given a fair market value or was over-priced) [29],[30]. Similar results of spared gist level memory for singular elements of event memory come from a recent study [31] in which older adults were comparable to young adults in their ability to learn cause-effect associations for basic category-level knowledge (i.e., what types of food would cause an allergic reaction), despite older adults showing a disadvantage for remembering which items were used in the learning phase.
Emerging evidence from laboratory studies also shows that older adults remember general time and location detail about event memories. For instance, one study [32] asked whether, relative to young adults, older adults could learn the weather of an upcoming week. While older adults had more difficulty recalling specific details (e.g., exact temperatures on certain days), they retained as well as young adults a gist memory for the weather forecast, such as noting which days prompted carrying an umbrella. Another study [33] drew a similar conclusion regarding location detail. Here, older adults could extract the gist of people-location events as well as young adults, which was examined by pairing images of faces with scenes. True and false memory performance revealed that the ability to retrieve gist memories of these associations was unaffected in older adults (e.g., older adults remembered that older men, not younger men, were paired with park scenes, as opposed to with another scene, such as an indoor mall), whereas memory for more specific details was reduced in older age.
In line with the findings described above, recent work on narrative memory for verbal stories indicates that older adults can retain the gist of extended events learned in a laboratory as well as young adults. For example, by auditorily presenting short stories containing both perceptual (referred to as peripheral) and gist (i.e., core to the plot or story’s meaning) details, one recent study [14] showed that while older adults retain fewer perceptually specific details, they effectively remember the central content making up a story’s narrative similar to young adults. In fact, older adults retain narrative gist for a wide range of content in a storyline, including detail about where and when an event occurred [34]. Several additional studies bolster the idea that gist representations for narratives are effectively formed by older adults [13,15,35,36].
Bringing these findings together, there is growing evidence that older adults appear to be able to store and retrieve general detail about unique event elements at a gist level, that is the features about singular items [29],[30], location [33] and time [32], and they seem to be capable of integrating this gist detail into the basic multi-element narrative of an event. We conclude, therefore, that the reviewed behavioral studies, and others [37,38], provide evidence that the type of detail spared and enhanced in typical-aging older adults’ autobiographical event memories can be plausibly attributed to the preservation of gist representations.
Neural evidence for a specific-to-gist shift with aging
The retrieval of autobiographical event memories is supported by a widespread set of brain regions that centers upon the hippocampus. This network of brain regions has also been implicated in studies of non-personal episodic memory tasks and has been referred to by many terms: the default network, autobiographical memory network, the posteromedial anterotemporal network, and the recollection network [1,39–42]. Regardless of the term used to describe this “core” network, many contemporary neurocognitive models (for in-depth reviews, see [1,27,28,39,43,44]) have proposed that brain regions within this network specialize in cognitive processes that appear to be relevant to the gist versus specific distinction. For instance, several posterior cortical regions, including the parahippocampal cortex, precuneus, angular gyrus, and retrosplenial cortex, have been implicated in memory processes that are associated with reinstating specific details of an event, either in a single or multi-modal/content manner [43,45]. These memory processes include so-called episodic detail retrieval [46,47], self-reported memory vividness [48,49], retrieving spatial features of autobiographical events [50], and constructing precise item and spatial location detail [51,52]. In comparison, other cortical regions, like the anterolateral temporal lobe and dorsal and ventral medial prefrontal cortex, are believed to support functions that may have a prominent role in forming and retrieving the generalities of event details. These generalities include themes of autobiographical events [50], social knowledge [53], conceptual representations of personal identity [54], and abstract autobiographical facts [55,56]. Similar distinctions in memory representations have been made in the hippocampus. That is, while there is broad agreement that the hippocampus has a central role in the reconstruction of event memories, a few models propose that the anterior hippocampus may support general features of events (so-called conceptual, global, or coarse features), whereas the posterior hippocampus may be crucial for remembering specific features of events (so-called fine-grained, sensory-perceptual, and contextual features) [1,28,57].
Based on these neurocognitive models [1,27,28,39,42–44], in Figure 2 we illustrate a basic schematic of how the selective sparing of gist representations among older adults may alter the engagement of the neural network underlying autobiographical event memory. That is, if gist representations are selectively spared at encoding and retrieval, older adults might show relatively intact, or greater use of, the anterior hippocampus and cortical regions implicated in processing and retrieving an autobiographical event memory’s generalities. Equally important, older adults might show underuse of the posterior hippocampus and cortical regions essential to processing and later reconstructing the specifics of event elements.
Findings from several recent neuroimaging studies using lab-based stimuli can be taken as support for Figure 2’s proposed signature of selective sparing of gist representations among older adults. One such fMRI study [58] analyzed brain activity of over 500 participants as they viewed a complex movie, a proxy for personal events. By focusing on activation around event boundaries, which are believed to be crucial for event memory formation, they found that older participants a) showed spared anterior hippocampal activation, but reduced posterior hippocampal activation, b) exhibited decreased activation among several posterior cortical regions, and c) engaged the medial prefrontal cortex more than young adults. Collectively, these findings support the possibility of older adults showing an enhanced reliance on gist representations during the formation of an event memory. Related findings emerged from another recent fMRI study that involved scanning young and older adults as they encoded and retrieved visual scenes [59]. The experimenters used deep convolutional neural networks to code presumed neural representations for the perceptual features of a scene in early visual cortex (bilateral calcarine, cuneus, and lingual gyrus) and the categorical (i.e., conceptual or gist) features of scenes in the anterior temporal lobe (left dorsal temporal pole and ventral temporal pole), and they estimated the amount of item-specific neural encoding-retrieval similarity within these brain regions. While older age was associated with a reduction in item-specific encoding-retrieval similarity within early visual cortex, older adults had enhanced item-specific encoding-retrieval similarity relative to young adults in the anterior temporal lobe, with the latter associated with better recognition memory in only the older adults. The described neuroimaging results align with other recent findings of encoding and retrieval using lab-based stimuli, showing older age-related spared or increased involvement of anterior cortical regions implicated in gist-aligned cognitive processes, and decreased posterior hippocampal and posterior cortical regions relevant to event specificity [60],[49,61–63].
Findings from neuroimaging studies of autobiographical event memory also generally support the proposal illustrated in Figure 2. A recent study examined the associations between hippocampal and temporal lobe volumes to autobiographical event retrieval in young and older adults. The ability of older adults to generate detailed autobiographical event memories was positively associated with temporal pole volume, and in a sex-specific manner, the anterior hippocampus [64]. Along the same lines, a recent meta-analysis that examined the neural substrates of autobiographical event memory retrieval in young and healthy older adults found that both age cohorts commonly activated several left-lateralized regions (left hippocampus, medial parietal cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, angular gyrus) and that the older adult group had additional activation of regions including the medial and lateral temporal gyrus and the right anterior hippocampus [65]. Neuroimaging studies using both lab-based stimuli and autobiographical events, therefore, suggest that older adults tend to show a specific-to-general shift in several brain regions underlying remembering.
In closing this section, we note again that the proposal in Figure 2 is viewed as a basic schematic that can be refined with additional research. For instance, according to a few models [27,39,43,44], it could be that some neural regions underused by older adults reflect areas specialized in certain types of specific detail or cognitive operations. A reduced ability to combine these specific details, on the other hand, may be most strongly evident in the hippocampus, the posterior hippocampus in particular according to some models [1,28]. Along the same lines, some brain regions spared and used more by older adults may specialize in forming and retrieving certain types of general content of an event, whereas the spared integration of this content with other memories may be most strongly reflected in the anterior hippocampus in particular. A second consideration is that while the proposal in Figure 2 highlights key brain regions in which we might expect to see a reliance on gist representations shaping autobiographical event memory, other areas involved in coding detail from an autobiographical event, such as the auditory cortex [66], may show specific-to-general shifts as well. A final consideration is that neural regions that may have a more central role in integrating both general and specific content together into a coherent memory, such as the posterior cingulate cortex [43,46], may show local shifts within their neuroanatomical boundaries that reflect specific to gist representational formation. Indeed, what constitutes event-specific versus gist regions, networks, or functional patterns is an interesting avenue for future work.
Older adults’ reliance on gist: a multifactorial outcome
So far, we have covered behavioral data indicating that the gist of autobiographical event memories is selectively spared in older adulthood, and we have suggested how this may be reflected in the brain. A common interpretation of a shift to gist in older adults is it is strictly a compensatory, automatic reaction to cognitive and brain decline. However, in this final section, we bring forward evidence that the specific-to-gist shift, in addition to being partly driven by age-related decline, may also reflect a natural outcome of older adult cognitive development [67]. That is, while specific memory may be optimal for young adults (Box 2), older adults may rely on gist representations because of natural changes to the availability of memories that occur with time and experience, normative motivation-based changes in older adults, and the momentum of a “gist mode of thinking” that naturally occurs with aging.
Box 2. Considering the value of detailed remembering in young adulthood.
In this article, we propose that there is a shift toward representing memories at a gist level in older age that is not exclusively driven by decline but rather is also partly motivated. This position implies that young adults may focus on specific memory representations for several reasons. As we do not view cognitive aging from a purely decline-oriented perspective, we do not view young adults’ focus on specific details as simply an outcome of inexperience or lack of the ability to consider gist aspects of memory. The contexts and goals driving the use of autobiographical event memory in young adulthood dictate a prioritization of specific details.
Recent findings suggest that remembering specific details has many adaptive (directive, social, and identity) functions that young adults may be inclined to capitalize on. For instance, focusing on specifics may be critical to mapping out a thorough, effective plan in novel scenarios. In comparison to older adults, young adults may be motivated to engage specific processing because they are more likely to face unfamiliar decisions and because they are generally more likely to be information-seeking [111,112]. Specific memory representations also may have a vital role in updating, and perhaps maintaining, a sense of identity [107,113,114]. Given that young adults are in their formidable years of identity formation, detailed remembering may be particularly influential for this function of memory retrieval. Relatedly, specific details may provide valuable context for distinguishing social concepts of individuals. Given that the size and complexity of our social networks tend to peak in young adulthood and then steadily decline with age [115], specific details may be relied on by young adults to outline large, complex social networks spanning multiple environmental contexts (e.g., work, school, home). However, much like a focus on gist memory in older age comes with some negative side effects, so too might a focus on specific details in young adulthood. For example, an over-reliance on specific detail could limit the applicability of a memory to other relevant, but different, contexts, thus limiting transfer of learning and generalization [21]. It also seems more likely that if there is a hyper-focus on specifics, a person may miss the meaning of an event (i.e., not seeing the forest for the trees), which could translate to decisions that stray from personal values and goals.
Aging naturally changes what memories are available
Our first non-decline-oriented point acknowledges that older adults, by the mere fact of being older, have vastly more memories of remote experiences available to them than young adults. This is relevant in part because remoteness and rehearsal, which come with older memories, naturally reduce the availability of specific details more than gist details of events [68–70]. In addition, autobiographical memories are believed to be stored in a connected fashion according to some higher order theme, like general event categories (e.g., holidays, surprising events) [71]. It follows, then, that older adults typically have more related memories to draw on during remembering (i.e., those from the same theme). This idea closely aligns with the “lifetime experience” hypothesis [9], which suggests that older age comes with a natural increase in familiarity with categorical information, and older adults are arguably more likely than young adults to activate and draw on connections between members of a category (in this case, autobiographical event memories from the same theme). These points highlight that older adults’ inclination to retrieve gist representations may partly reflect that with more years lived comes natural changes to what memories are available, leading to an inevitable drift toward gist compared to specific representations. While this “years lived” factor may be viewed as a relatively passive outcome of typical aging, next we consider what are arguably more active, motivation-based factors that may contribute to an increased reliance on gist-based remembering.
Aging leads to motivated familiarity-seeking behavior
There is growing interest in the idea that older adults are goal driven to focus on what is familiar, “exploiting” tried-and-true experiences over “exploring” new ones [72–74]. Relative to young adults, older adults spend more time in familiar environments before seeking novelty, as demonstrated by foraging paradigms [75,76]. Older adults also spend more time in comparison to young adults viewing information in their environment that is consistent with prior knowledge, and therefore familiar [77]. This drive for familiarity may even translate to basic personality traits, with large cross-sectional and longitudinal studies finding that trait-level openness is lower in older age, hinting at a shift toward less exploration [78,79]. Interestingly, recent work [80] has shown that familiar spatial environments tend to spontaneously cue semantic memories more than highly specific event memories, suggesting that older adults’ preference for familiar experiences may naturally spark gist-based remembering. More broadly, a focus on processing what is familiar may disproportionately block specific details, consistent with cue overload theory [81,82]. Familiarity seeking may further increase the relevance of schemas to learning and remembering, which capture category typical or general features of an event situation (e.g., what is found in a doctor’s office) [83] and are thought to be supported by cortical regions implicated in gist [84]. Finally, work on expertise and cognitive processing further suggests that when dealing with familiar scenarios and environments, often the most relevant information can be gleaned from the gist of an experience [85–87]. In sum, with older age may come an increased motivation to pursue what is familiar, opening multiple paths through which encoding and retrieving shifts toward the gist.
Aging leads to social goals for remembering
A large body of research indicates that older age is associated with another relevant motivational shift: self-transcendence, a state of mind that promotes reflecting on world or societal knowledge [88–90]. This raises the possibility that older adults are motivated by different socially-oriented goals than young adults when they retrieve and share past events [67], and we propose the goals of older adults align with gist-oriented thinking. This proposal follows several studies suggesting that in social situations, older adults may be motivated by goals that promote sharing meaning more than young adults. For instance, self-report data indicate that while older age is associated with a decline in seeking out social relationships that involve new topics (i.e., “information seeking”), the desire to find social interactions that will involve reflecting on values, mutual understanding, and life meaning subtly increases with older age [91]. Relatedly, researchers [92], using a mobile device to unobtrusively capture the acoustics of daily interactions, found that older adults often share memories to teach and reflect on their identity, and to connect with or entertain others. Importantly, these are social and self functions linked to a prioritization of the conceptual meaning or story-telling quality of memories, and not typically the specific details [18,93]. Putting these findings together, the fact that older adults are inherently motivated by social goals that promote meaning may influence how events in the lab, and everyday autobiographical episodes, are encoded and retrieved.
Aging promotes a gist frame of mind
The above described ideas suggest that older adults are processing and remembering their experiences through something akin to a gist frame of mind. This is intriguing, as recent research has shown that how a memory is retrieved is strongly linked to a person’s current mode of thinking [94–97]. According to this induction-based research, retrieval prompts a feedback loop in which constructing specific memories of any event temporarily primes a person to engage an “episodic detail mode” of retrieval on a range of cognitive tasks that call on memory. While not the focus of this induction-based work (although see [95]), it stands to reason that retrieving gist-based representations of memories similarly primes a “gist mode” of thinking during direct and indirect attempts at memory retrieval. To this point, work across sub-fields of memory, decision-making, and perception has shown that older adults do use cognitive strategies guided by generalities and prior knowledge more than younger adults, which may partly reflect that older adults were primed to process the gist [77,98,99]. Relatedly, and as alluded to by the results of [58], a gist mode of thinking may operate at encoding as well as retrieval. While encoding is challenging to study in autobiographical memory, recent work using lab events in young adults suggests that a gist mode may in fact alter encoding [100]. In other words, some aftereffects of a gist mode of thinking may not be temporary, but rather, they could have a permanent shaping of new memory formation. Thus, although older adults’ reliance on gist might initially arise for multiple reasons, both decline and non-decline in manner, natural retrieval aftereffects may come to contribute to a long-term exacerbation of a gist focus.
Concluding remarks
Researchers are beginning to ask how cognitive functions that are preserved with older age [72] and age-related differences in motivation [101] influence the way older adults remember. The perspective that we have outlined in this Opinion extends this movement in scientific thinking and casts a spotlight on gist memories as a key factor. In sum, there is growing evidence that autobiographical event memories include gist and specific details, and cognitive aging brings on decline and preservation with respect to these representations. More so, emerging work indicates that, in older adulthood, the gist of an event may be increasingly important to older adults’ use of memory in daily life, and possibly adaptive to rely on in light of age-related changes in motivated behavior. The prioritization and increased reliance on gist representations for event memories may explain why recent studies using so-called “naturalistic” stimuli, examining memory in more realistic contexts, or asking individuals to remember in a more gist-like way, are either not finding pronounced age effects [12,102], or are reporting older adult advantages [17,103]. This casts a new light on how we interpret normative age differences in event remembering and raises several avenues for future work (see Outstanding Questions).
Outstanding Questions.
How can we measure the gist of autobiographical event memories? While most research directly examining gist has focused on non-personal stimuli, methods could be adapted to directly assess the gist of autobiographical events as well.
What are the roles of encoding and retrieval in a shift toward gist? There is reason to think both are involved, but their relative contributions remain uncertain.
When in the adult lifespan does gist reliance emerge? Because cognitive aging research typically compares young and older adults, we have a limited understanding of when in the adult lifespan a dependence and preference for gist memory begins to take shape, or how it evolves in typical aging.
Does a preference for gist, once it emerges, equally affect remote and recent memories? Does this shift develop over time for a memory (i.e., with (re)consolidation), or is it evident, to a degree, upon initial formation/re-retrieval?
Is a preference for gist only evident in older adults with declining episodic memory and associated neural mechanisms? The answer to this question is a critical test of the idea that a shift toward gist is not entirely driven by cognitive and brain decline.
What is the neural signature of a specific-to-gist shift? In the text, we consider several ways in which this shift may be evident within regions and networks.
Are non-decline causes of a shift toward gist automatic, strategic, or both? The degree to which a shift toward gist is under the control of older adults is unclear.
Under what circumstances is a shift toward gist adaptive? Relying on the gist likely aids and hinders the functionality of memory. The adaptive consequences, and whether they vary by age group, is an important direction for future work.
Do youth-favored experimental settings bias older adults to gist memory? Future work should determine how the typical setting in which university-based research is conducted affects how age cohorts describe autobiographical events.
Highlights.
Cognitive aging is often viewed through the lens of loss of function. Research examining the way older adults remember autobiographical event memories, therefore, has focused on how aging reduces the ability to form and retrieve specific details, with less emphasis on understanding the aspects of event memories that are preserved among older adults.
We propose that at the heart of what is preserved in older adults’ autobiographical event memories is the selective sparing of the ability to store and retrieve the gist.
We discuss why older adults’ reliance on gist may be driven by multiple factors beyond cognitive decline, including changes in goals that come with age.
Acknowledgments
M Grilli was supported by the NIH/NIA R56 AG068098 and R03 AG060271. We thank Austin Deffner for illustrating Figure 1. We have no conflicts of interest to declare.
Glossary
- Autobiographical event memory
Mental representations of complex, autobiographical events that are consciously re-experienced and situated in a particular time and place. At a conceptual level, autobiographical event memory is definitionally synonymous with episodic memory, although there are important methodological differences (see episodic memory). Relatedly, autobiographical event memories are believed to typically include general or non-specific detail that is often described as semantic memory (see semantic memory)
- Episodic memory
Conceptually akin to autobiographical event memory in that both are believed to reflect consciously retrieved unique events bound to a time and place. However, methodologically episodic memory is often tested with non-personal stimuli. The number of event-defining elements also is often much more limited relative to autobiographical event memories, and generalities are often controlled (e.g., all pictures of outdoor scenes)
- Gist representations
A general memory for the superordinate, basic-category, and prototype features of elements making up a singular event. Applied to autobiographical events, gist representations capture the basics about who, what, and where, as well as an event’s general storyline and emotion. Gist representations serve as a solid conceptual base from which to draw meaning, and to make connections with related experiences
- Semantic memory
Knowledge of autobiographical and non-personal concepts or “facts”. Semantic memories are derived from experience but are believed to be retrievable without reconstructing the specifics around the time and place from which they were extracted
- Specific representations
Memory for concrete features of elements that constitute a singular event. Applied to autobiographical events, specific representations capture visual, auditory, and other sensory modalities, and are defined as any detailed information that adds to an event’s uniqueness, including an element’s physical features and location, the length of time and sequencing of actions within an event, and a verbatim-like record of dialogue
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.Sheldon S et al. (2019) A Neurocognitive Perspective on the Forms and Functions of Autobiographical Memory Retrieval. Front. Syst. Neurosci 13, 4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Vranić A et al. (2018) Functions of Autobiographical Memory in Younger and Older Adults. Front. Psychol 9, 219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Levine B et al. (2002) Aging and autobiographical memory: Dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval. Psychol. Aging 17, 677–689 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Conway MA (2009) Episodic memories. Neuropsychologia 47, 2305–2313 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Piolino P et al. (2010) Reduced specificity of autobiographical memory and aging: Do the executive and feature binding functions of working memory have a role? Neuropsychologia 48, 429–440 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Naveh-Benjamin M et al. (2007) The associative memory deficit of older adults: The role of strategy utilization. Psychol. Aging 22, 202–208 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Amer T et al. (2022) Cluttered memory representations shape cognition in old age. Trends Cogn. Sci 26, 255–267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Burke SN et al. (2018) Shared Functions of Perirhinal and Parahippocampal Cortices: Implications for Cognitive Aging. Trends Neurosci. 41, 349–359 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Koen JD and Rugg MD (2019) Neural Dedifferentiation in the Aging Brain. Trends Cogn. Sci 23, 547–559 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Yassa MA and Stark CEL (2011) Pattern separation in the hippocampus. Trends Neurosci. 34, 515–525 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Andrews-Hanna JR et al. (2019) A Review and Reappraisal of the Default Network in Normal Aging and Dementia. In Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology Oxford University Press [Google Scholar]
- 12.Davis EE et al. (2021) Looking the same, but remembering differently: Preserved eye-movement synchrony with age during movie watching. Psychol. Aging 36, 604–615 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Delarazan AI et al. (2022) Aging impairs memory for perceptual, but not narrative event details, PsyArXiv. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Sacripante R et al. (2019) Benefit of wakeful resting on gist and peripheral memory retrieval in healthy younger and older adults. Neurosci. Lett 705, 27–32 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Taler V et al. (2021) A discourse-theoretic approach to story recall in aging and mild cognitive impairment. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn 28, 762–780 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Koutstaal W (2006) Flexible remembering. Psychon. Bull. Rev 13, 84–91 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Acevedo-Molina MC et al. (2020) Beyond episodic remembering: elaborative retrieval of lifetime periods in young and older adults. Memory 28, 83–93 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Bluck S et al. (2016) Story asides as a useful construct in examining adults’ story recall. Psychol. Aging 31, 42–57 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Renoult L et al. (2020) Classification of general and personal semantic details in the Autobiographical Interview. Neuropsychologia 144, 107501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Strikwerda-Brown C et al. (2019) External details revisited – A new taxonomy for coding ‘non-episodic’ content during autobiographical memory retrieval. J. Neuropsychol 13, 371–397 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Richards BA and Frankland PW (2017) The Persistence and Transience of Memory. Neuron 94, 1071–1084 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Devitt AL and Schacter DL (2016) False memories with age: Neural and cognitive underpinnings. Neuropsychologia 91, 346–359 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Craik FIM (2002) Levels of processing: Past, present… and future? Memory 10, 305–318 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Brainerd CJ and Reyna VF (2015) Fuzzy-trace theory and lifespan cognitive development. Dev. Rev 38, 89–121 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Renoult L et al. (2012) Personal semantics: at the crossroads of semantic and episodic memory. Trends Cogn. Sci 16, 550–558 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Renoult L et al. (2019) From Knowing to Remembering: The Semantic–Episodic Distinction. Trends Cogn. Sci 23, 1041–1057 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Irish M and Vatansever D (2020) Rethinking the episodic-semantic distinction from a gradient perspective. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci 32, 43–49 [Google Scholar]
- 28.Robin J and Moscovitch M (2017) Details, gist and schema: hippocampal–neocortical interactions underlying recent and remote episodic and spatial memory. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci 17, 114–123 [Google Scholar]
- 29.Flores CC et al. (2017) Gist-based memory for prices and “better buys” in younger and older adults. Memory 25, 565–573 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Whatley MC and Castel AD (2022) The role of metacognition and schematic support in younger and older adults’ episodic memory. Mem. Cognit 50, 601–616 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Mutter SA and Asriel MW (2016) Gist and Generalization in Young and Older Adults’ Causal Learning. J. Gerontol. B. Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci DOI: 10.1093/geronb/gbw026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Gallo HB et al. (2019) Memory for Weather Information in Younger and Older Adults: Tests of Verbatim and Gist Memory. Exp. Aging Res 45, 252–265 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Greene NR and Naveh-Benjamin M (2022) The effects of divided attention at encoding on specific and gist-based associative episodic memory. Mem. Cognit 50, 59–76 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Davis D et al. (2015) The Difference between Right and Wrong: Accuracy of Older and Younger Adults’ Story Recall. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public. Health 12, 10861–10885 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Adams C et al. (1997) Adult Age-Group Differences in Recall for the Literal and Interpretive Meanings of Narrative Text. J. Gerontol. B. Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci 52B, P187–P195 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Martin C-O et al. (2018) Narrative Discourse in Young and Older Adults: Behavioral and NIRS Analyses. Front. Aging Neurosci 10, 69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Abadie M et al. (2021) Verbatim and gist memory in aging. Psychol. Aging 36, 891–901 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Greene NR and Naveh-Benjamin M (2022) Adult age differences in specific and gist associative episodic memory across short- and long-term retention intervals. Psychol. Aging DOI: 10.1037/pag0000701 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Andrews-Hanna JR and Grilli MD (2021) Mapping the Imaginative Mind: Charting New Paths Forward. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci 30, 82–89 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Svoboda E et al. (2006) The functional neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 44, 2189–2208 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Rugg MD and Vilberg KL (2013) Brain networks underlying episodic memory retrieval. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol 23, 255–260 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Ranganath C and Ritchey M (2012) Two cortical systems for memory-guided behaviour. Nat. Rev. Neurosci 13, 713–726 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Ritchey M and Cooper RA (2020) Deconstructing the Posterior Medial Episodic Network. Trends Cogn. Sci 24, 451–465 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Barnett AJ et al. (2021) Intrinsic connectivity reveals functionally distinct cortico-hippocampal networks in the human brain. PLOS Biol. 19, e3001275. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Thakral PP et al. (2017) Decoding the content of recollection within the core recollection network and beyond. Cortex J. Devoted Study Nerv. Syst. Behav 91, 101–113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Bird CM et al. (2015) Consolidation of Complex Events via Reinstatement in Posterior Cingulate Cortex. J. Neurosci 35, 14426–14434 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Thakral PP et al. (2017) A Role for the Left Angular Gyrus in Episodic Simulation and Memory. J. Neurosci 37, 8142–8149 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Lee H et al. (2019) Decomposing Parietal Memory Reactivation to Predict Consequences of Remembering. Cereb. Cortex 29, 3305–3318 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Folville A et al. (2020) Age-related differences in the neural correlates of vivid remembering. NeuroImage 206, 116336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Gurguryan L and Sheldon S (2019) Retrieval orientation alters neural activity during autobiographical memory recollection. NeuroImage 199, 534–544 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Korkki SM et al. (2021) Hippocampal–Cortical Encoding Activity Predicts the Precision of Episodic Memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci 33, 2328–2341 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Robin J et al. (2018) The Primacy of Spatial Context in the Neural Representation of Events. J. Neurosci 38, 2755–2765 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Raykov PP et al. (2021) Activation of Person Knowledge in Medial Prefrontal Cortex during the Encoding of New Lifelike Events. Cereb. Cortex 31, 3494–3505 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Brietzke S and Meyer ML (2021) Temporal self-compression: Behavioral and neural evidence that past and future selves are compressed as they move away from the present. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci 118, e2101403118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Martinelli P et al. (2013) Neural substrates of the self-memory system: New insights from a meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp 34, 1515–1529 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Grilli MD and Verfaellie M (2016) Experience-near but not experience-far autobiographical facts depend on the medial temporal lobe for retrieval: Evidence from amnesia. Neuropsychologia 81, 180–185 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Sheldon S and Levine B (2016) The role of the hippocampus in memory and mental construction: Memory and mental construction. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci 1369, 76–92 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Reagh ZM et al. (2020) Aging alters neural activity at event boundaries in the hippocampus and Posterior Medial network. Nat. Commun 11, 3980. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Deng L et al. (2021) Age-related dedifferentiation and hyperdifferentiation of perceptual and mnemonic representations. Neurobiol. Aging 106, 55–67 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Chamberlain JD et al. (2022) Age-related differences in encoding-retrieval similarity and their relationship to false memory. Neurobiol. Aging 113, 15–27 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Matijevic S et al. (2022) Individual differences in the relationship between episodic detail generation and resting state functional connectivity vary with age. Neuropsychologia 166, 108138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Srokova S et al. (2022) The Retrieval-Related Anterior Shift Is Moderated by Age and Correlates with Memory Performance. J. Neurosci 42, 1765–1776 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.St-Laurent M et al. (2014) Memory Reactivation in Healthy Aging: Evidence of Stimulus-Specific Dedifferentiation. J. Neurosci 34, 4175–4186 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Setton R et al. (2022) Temporal pole volume is associated with episodic autobiographical memory in healthy older adults. Hippocampus 32, 373–385 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Fenerci C et al. (2022) Comparing neural activity during autobiographical memory retrieval between younger and older adults: An ALE meta-analysis. Neurobiol. Aging DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2022.06.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Cohn-Sheehy BI et al. (2021) The hippocampus constructs narrative memories across distant events. Curr. Biol 31, 4935–4945.e7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Hess TM (2005) Memory and Aging in Context. Psychol. Bull 131, 383–406 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.St-Laurent M et al. (2014) The perceptual richness of complex memory episodes is compromised by medial temporal lobe damage: M tle and Perceptual Richness of Complex Memories. Hippocampus 24, 560–576 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Sekeres MJ et al. (2016) Recovering and preventing loss of detailed memory: differential rates of forgetting for detail types in episodic memory. Learn. Mem 23, 72–82 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Radvansky GA et al. (2022) A new look at memory retention and forgetting. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001110 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Conway MA (2005) Memory and the self☆. J. Mem. Lang 53, 594–628 [Google Scholar]
- 72.Spreng RN and Turner GR (2019) The Shifting Architecture of Cognition and Brain Function in Older Adulthood. Perspect. Psychol. Sci 14, 523–542 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Spreng RN and Turner GR (2021) From exploration to exploitation: a shifting mental mode in late life development. Trends Cogn. Sci 25, 1058–1071 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Wilson RC et al. (2021) Balancing exploration and exploitation with information and randomization. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci 38, 49–56 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Mata R et al. (2013) Foraging across the life span: is there a reduction in exploration with aging? Front. Neurosci 7, [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Wiegand I et al. (2019) Hybrid foraging search in younger and older age. Psychol. Aging 34, 805–820 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Wynn JS et al. (2020) Effects of prior knowledge on active vision and memory in younger and older adults. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen 149, 518–529 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Donnellan MB and Lucas RE (2008) Age differences in the big five across the life span: Evidence from two national samples. Psychol. Aging 23, 558–566 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Graham EK et al. (2020) Trajectories of Big Five Personality Traits: A Coordinated Analysis of 16 Longitudinal Samples. Eur. J. Personal 34, 301–321 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Robin J et al. (2019) Spontaneous memory retrieval varies based on familiarity with a spatial context. Cognition 190, 81–92 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Berntsen D et al. (2013) Why am I remembering this now? Predicting the occurrence of involuntary (spontaneous) episodic memories. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen 142, 426–444 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Moscovitch M and Craik FIM (1976) Depth of processing, retrieval cues, and uniqueness of encoding as factors in recall. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav 15, 447–458 [Google Scholar]
- 83.Ghosh VE and Gilboa A (2014) What is a memory schema? A historical perspective on current neuroscience literature. Neuropsychologia 53, 104–114 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Gilboa A and Marlatte H (2017) Neurobiology of Schemas and Schema-Mediated Memory. Trends Cogn. Sci 21, 618–631 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Corbin JC et al. (2015) How reasoning, judgment, and decision making are colored by gist-based intuition: A fuzzy-trace theory approach. J. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn 4, 344–355 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Brennan PC et al. (2018) Radiologists can detect the ‘gist’ of breast cancer before any overt signs of cancer appear. Sci. Rep 8, 8717. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Robson SG et al. (2021) The effect of expertise, target usefulness and image structure on visual search. Cogn. Res. Princ. Implic 6, 16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Freund AM and Blanchard-Fields F (2014) Age-related differences in altruism across adulthood: Making personal financial gain versus contributing to the public good. Dev. Psychol 50, 1125–1136 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Isaacowitz DM et al. (2021) Age-Related Changes in the Role of Social Motivation: Implications for Healthy Aging. J. Gerontol. Ser. B 76, S115–S124 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Shane J et al. (2021) Prosociality across adulthood: A developmental and motivational perspective. Psychol. Aging 36, 22–35 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Gong X et al. (2019) Development and Validation of Social Motivation Questionnaire. The Gerontologist 59, e664–e673 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Demiray B et al. (2019) Reminiscence in Everyday Conversations: A Naturalistic Observation Study of Older Adults. J. Gerontol. Ser. B 74, 745–755 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Grilli MD and Verfaellie M (2015) Supporting the self-concept with memory: insight from amnesia. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci 10, 1684–1692 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Madore KP et al. (2014) Constructive episodic simulation: Dissociable effects of a specificity induction on remembering, imagining, and describing in young and older adults. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn 40, 609–622 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Rudoy JD et al. (2009) Recall of remote episodic memories can appear deficient because of a gist-based retrieval orientation. Neuropsychologia 47, 938–941 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Sheldon S et al. (2019) Constructing autobiographical events within a spatial or temporal context: a comparison of two targeted episodic induction techniques. Memory 27, 881–893 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Peters SL et al. (2019) Episodic memory contributions to autobiographical memory and open-ended problem-solving specificity in younger and older adults. Mem. Cognit 47, 1592–1605 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Blanco NJ et al. (2016) Exploratory decision-making as a function of lifelong experience, not cognitive decline. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen 145, 284–297 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Smith ME et al. (2020) Differential effects of knowledge and aging on the encoding and retrieval of everyday activities. Cognition 196, 104159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Grilli MD et al. (2019) Evidence that an episodic mode of thinking facilitates encoding of perceptually rich memories for naturalistic events relative to a gist-based mode of thinking. Memory 27, 1468–1474 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Ryan AD and Campbell KL (2021) The ironic effect of older adults’ increased task motivation: Implications for neurocognitive aging. Psychon. Bull. Rev 28, 1743–1754 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Strikwerda-Brown C et al. (2021) What are your thoughts? Exploring age-related changes in episodic and semantic autobiographical content on an open-ended retrieval task. Memory 29, 1375–1383 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Badham SP et al. (2022) An older adult advantage in autobiographical recall. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn DOI: 10.1080/13825585.2022.2063789 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Renoult L and Rugg MD (2020) An historical perspective on Endel Tulving’s episodic-semantic distinction. Neuropsychologia 139, 107366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Tulving E (1972) Episodic and semantic memory. In Organization of memory Academic Press [Google Scholar]
- 106.Grilli MD and Verfaellie M (2014) Personal semantic memory: Insights from neuropsychological research on amnesia. Neuropsychologia 61, 56–64 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Wank AA et al. (2022) Autobiographical memory unknown: Pervasive autobiographical memory loss encompassing personality trait knowledge in an individual with medial temporal lobe amnesia. Cortex 147, 41–57 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Irish M et al. (2011) Profiles of recent autobiographical memory retrieval in semantic dementia, behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia, and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia 49, 2694–2702 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Irish M et al. (2018) Evolution of autobiographical memory impairments in Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia – A longitudinal neuroimaging study. Neuropsychologia 110, 14–25 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Berron D et al. (2020) Medial temporal lobe connectivity and its associations with cognition in early Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 143, 1233–1248 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Beaman A et al. (2007) Autobiographical Memory Specificity Predicts Social Problem-Solving Ability in Old and Young Adults. Q. J. Exp. Psychol 60, 1275–1288 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Vandermorris S et al. (2013) Differential Contributions of Executive and Episodic Memory Functions to Problem Solving in Younger and Older Adults. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc 19, 1087–1096 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Garland MM et al. (2021) Who Are You? The Study of Personality in Patients With Anterograde Amnesia. Psychol. Sci 32, 1649–1661 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Sawczak C et al. (2022) I remember therefore I am: Episodic memory retrieval and self-reported trait empathy judgments in young and older adults and individuals with medial temporal lobe excisions. Cognition 225, 105124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Wrzus C et al. (2013) Social network changes and life events across the life span: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull 139, 53–80 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]