Abstract
A major unanswered question in the dementia field is whether cognitively unimpaired individuals who harbor both Alzheimer’s disease neuropathological hallmarks (that is, amyloid-β plaques and tau neurofibrillary tangles) can preserve their cognition over time or are destined to decline. In this large multicenter amyloid and tau positron emission tomography (PET) study (n = 1,325), we examined the risk for future progression to mild cognitive impairment and the rate of cognitive decline over time among cognitively unimpaired individuals who were amyloid PET-positive (A+) and tau PET-positive (T+) in the medial temporal lobe (A+TMTL+) and/or in the temporal neocortex (A+TNEO-T+) and compared them with A+T− and A−T− groups. Cox proportional-hazards models showed a substantially increased risk for progression to mild cognitive impairment in the A+TNEO-T+ (hazard ratio (HR) = 19.2, 95% confidence interval (CI) = 10.9–33.7), A+TMTL+ (HR = 14.6, 95% CI = 8.1–26.4) and A+T− (HR = 2.4, 95% CI = 1.4–4.3) groups versus the A−T− (reference) group. Both A+TMTL+ (HR = 6.0, 95% CI = 3.4–10.6) and A+TNEO-T+ (HR = 7.9, 95% CI = 4.7–13.5) groups also showed faster clinical progression to mild cognitive impairment than the A+T− group. Linear mixed-effect models indicated that the A+TNEO-T+ (β = −0.056 ± 0.005, T = −11.55, P < 0.001), A+TMTL+ (β = −0.024 ± 0.005, T = −4.72, P < 0.001) and A+T− (β = −0.008 ± 0.002, T = −3.46, P < 0.001) groups showed significantly faster longitudinal global cognitive decline compared to the A−T− (reference) group (all P < 0.001). Both A+TNEO-T+ (P < 0.001) and A+TMTL+ (P = 0.002) groups also progressed faster than the A+T− group. In summary, evidence of advanced Alzheimer’s disease pathological changes provided by a combination of abnormal amyloid and tau PET examinations is strongly associated with short-term (that is, 3–5 years) cognitive decline in cognitively unimpaired individuals and is therefore of high clinical relevance.
Subject terms: Prognostic markers, Alzheimer's disease
Abnormal amyloid and tau PET in cognitively unimpaired individuals is strongly associated with short-term cognitive decline and subsequent development of dementia.
Main
Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is neuropathologically characterized by the presence of amyloid-β (Αβ) plaques and tau neurofibrillary tangles. Although the two most well-established diagnostic criteria for AD both acknowledge the importance of Αβ and tau pathology in AD pathogenesis1, an important distinction is that the National Institute on Aging and Alzheimer’s Association (NIA-AA) criteria2 define AD by its biological features (that is, the presence of Αβ and tau pathology) irrespective of the clinical syndrome, whereas the International Working Group (IWG) criteria3 require the presence of objective cognitive impairment (mild cognitive impairment (MCI) or dementia) in conjunction with positive AD biomarkers. Consequently, there is fundamental disagreement between the criteria about the nomenclature for cognitively unimpaired individuals who harbor one or both AD hallmark neuropathological features. For example, a cognitively unimpaired individual with positive Αβ (Α+) and tau (T+) biomarkers is classified as ‘preclinical AD’ by the NIA-AA criteria2, while the IWG criteria3 would label such an individual ‘at risk for progression to AD’ (Fig. 1).
Aside from philosophical differences (for example, according to the IWG criteria the term AD should be restricted to symptomatic individuals), the discrepancy between the NIA-AA and IWG can be explained by at least two factors. First, according to the IWG criteria, currently available Αβ and tau biomarkers show ‘low predictive accuracy’ for development of cognitive symptoms. For Αβ biomarkers alone this may indeed be the case4,5, although PET studies with long follow-ups (approximately 10 years) indicate substantial Αβ-associated risk for cognitive decline6, dementia7 and death7. For tau biomarkers, however, the clinicopathological correlates are much stronger than for Αβ biomarkers8,9, even in asymptomatic individuals10,11. Second, most prognostic studies to date are based on cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) biomarkers of soluble phospho-tau levels, which is an early marker of AD pathology12,13. In contrast, the more recently introduced tau PET technique measures more advanced pathological changes since it captures insoluble tau aggregates14,15. Although tau PET positivity in the neocortex is relatively rare among cognitively unimpaired individuals (approximately 5–10%16–18), its presence is strongly associated with worse cross-sectional10,19–22 and longitudinal11,23–32 cognitive outcomes. However, large-scale longitudinal studies with combinations of amyloid PET and tau PET biomarker profiles as predictors of clinical progression among cognitively unimpaired individuals are lacking33.
In addition to addressing the differences between the NIA-AA and IWG criteria, early detection of AD pathological changes may be key for future interventions with disease-modifying treatments since these will most likely be cost-effective and show the most favorable benefit versus risk ratio when specifically targeting preclinical populations with AD that are most likely to experience substantial cognitive deterioration in the short term (that is, 3–5 years). Therefore, the aim of the current multicenter study was to examine clinical progression to MCI or dementia and assess cognitive decline in cognitively unimpaired individuals with different Αβ (A) and tau (T) biomarker profiles as defined by PET at baseline. We divided A+T+ individuals into medial temporal lobe (MTL) only (A+TMTL+) and temporal neocortical (A+TNEO-T+) T+ groups to additionally investigate the impact of more advanced tau pathological changes on clinical progression.
Results
Participants
We included 1,325 cognitively unimpaired participants from 7 cohorts, of whom 843 (63.6%) were A−T−, 328 (24.8%) A+T−, 55 (4.2%) A+TMTL+ and 65 (4.9%) A+TNEO-T+ (see Table 1 and Supplementary Table 1 for a breakdown by cohort). All biomarker-positive groups were older and had lower baseline Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) scores compared to the A−T− group (all P < 0.001). There were no sex differences between groups. The average follow-up duration was 41.8 ± 18.9 months. The A−T+ group was considerably smaller than the other groups (n = 34; Extended Data Table 1), hence their results are only reported in Extended Data Fig. 1.
Table 1.
A−T− | A+T− | A+TMTL+ | A+TNEO-T+ | P | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
n | 843 | 328 | 55 | 65 | |
Age, years | 68.6 ± 9.6 | 75.5 ± 8.2 | 75.6 ± 6.6 | 76.4 ± 6.8 | <0.001a |
Sex, n (%) male | 424 (50.3) | 164 (50.0) | 24 (43.6) | 31 (47.7) | 0.822 |
Education, years | 14.7 ± 3.1 | 14.6 ± 3.1 | 13.7 ± 3.8 | 13.9 ± 3.5 | 0.02b |
Follow-up duration, months | 43.0 ± 18.9 | 40.1 ± 17.6 | 39.0 ± 16.5 | 36.4 ± 14.9 | 0.004c |
Follow-up visits, number | 4.1 ± 1.5 | 4.1 ± 1.4 | 4.0 ± 1.3 | 3.6 ± 1.2 | 0.07 |
MMSE, baseline score | 29.0 ± 1.0 | 28.7 ± 1.3 | 28.3 ± 1.5 | 28.2 ± 1.3 | <0.001d |
Progression to MCI, n (%) | 26 (8.9%) | 26 (3.3%) | 25 (49.0%) | 32 (53.3%) | <0.001 |
Progression to all-cause dementia, n (%) | 4 (0.5%) | 3 (1.0%) | 2 (3.9%) | 12 (20.0%) | <0.001 |
P values from two-sided statistical tests were reported. ANOVAs were used for continuous variables and chi-squared tests were used for categorical variables. Post-hoc tests were adjusted using Bonferroni correction.
aA+TNEO-T+ and A+TMTL+ and A+T− > A−T− b post-hoc tests revealed no significant group differences, cA−T− > A+TNEO-T+, dA+TNEO-T+, A+TMTL+ and A+T− < A−T− and A+TNEO-T+ < A+ T-.
Extended Data Table 1.
Clinical progression to MCI
During the clinical follow-up, 26 out of 781 (3.3%) of A−T−, 26 out of 292 (8.9%) of A+T−, 25 out of 51 (49.0%) of A+TMTL+ and 32 out of 60 (53.3%) of A+TNEO-T+ participants progressed to MCI or dementia. Among A+TNEO-T+ individuals, the progressors (27.8 ± 1.5) had worse baseline MMSE scores compared to stable individuals (28.6 ± 1.0, P = 0.02; Extended Data Table 2) and tended to have higher tau PET retention at baseline (Supplementary Fig. 1), suggesting that progressors were already in a slightly more advanced disease stage at the start of this study. Cox proportional-hazards models, adjusted for age, sex, education and cohort, showed an increased risk for future progression to MCI in the A+TNEO-T+ (HR = 19.2, 95% CI = 10.9–33.7, P < 0.001), A+TMTL+ (HR = 14.6, 95% CI = 8.1–26.4, P < 0.001) and A+T− (HR = 2.4, 95% CI = 1.4–4.3, P = 0.002) groups compared to the A−T− (reference) group (Fig. 2a,b). Both A+TMTL+ (HR = 6.0, 95% CI = 3.4–10.6, P < 0.001) and A+TNEO-T+ (HR = 7.9, 95% CI = 4.7–13.5, P < 0.001) groups also showed faster clinical progression to MCI than the A+T− group (Fig. 2c). Pairwise log-rank tests showed that the A+TMTL+ and A+TNEO-T+ groups did not differ from each other (P = 0.19). Fifty percent of the A+TNEO-T+ and A+TMTL+ groups had progressed to MCI after 42.8 and 43.6 months, respectively.
Extended Data Table 2.
Clinical progression to all-cause dementia
During clinical follow-up, 21 participants progressed to all-cause dementia: 4 out of 781 (0.5%) in A−T−, 3 out of 292 (1.0%) in A+T−, 2 out of 51 (3.9%) in A+TMTL+ and 12 out of 60 (20%) in A+TNEO-T+. Of those, 14 progressed to clinically defined AD-type dementia and 7 to non-AD dementias (see Extended Data Table 3 for dementia type). Cox proportional-hazards models, adjusted for age, sex, education and cohort, demonstrated an increased risk for future progression to all-cause dementia in the A+TNEO-T+ (HR = 41.3, 95% CI = 16.7–101.9, P < 0.001) and A+TMTL+ (HR = 6.2, 95% CI = 1.4–26.8, P = 0.01) groups compared to the A−T− (reference) group (Fig. 2d,e). There was no difference between the A+T− and the A−T− group (HR = 1.6, 95% CI = 0.5–5.4, P = 0.53). The A+TNEO-T+ (HR = 26.7, 95% CI = 10.4–68.1, P < 0.001) group showed faster clinical progression to all-cause dementia than the A+T− group, while there was no significant difference between the A+TMTL+ (HR = 3.8, 95% CI = 0.9–16.2, P = 0.08) and the A+T− group (Fig. 2f). Pairwise log-rank tests showed that the A+TNEO-T+ group progressed significantly faster to all-cause dementia than the A+TMTL+ group (P = 0.01). Similar results were found when using progression to AD-type dementia (11 A+TNEO-T+, 2 A+TMTL+ and 1 A+T−) as outcome instead of all-cause dementia (Extended Data Fig. 2).
Extended Data Table 3.
Cognitive trajectories
Linear mixed-effect models adjusting for age, sex, education and cohort indicated that the A+TNEO-T+ (standardized β (stdβ) of interaction with time in months ± s.e. = −0.020 ± 0.002, T = −10.14, P < 0.001), A+TMTL+ (stdβ = −0.017 ± 0.002, T = −8.84, P < 0.001) and A+T− (stdβ = −0.005 ± 0.001, T = −5.26, P < 0.001) groups showed faster decline over time on the modified preclinical Alzheimer cognitive composite 5 (mPACC5 (ref.34)) compared to the A−T− (reference) group (Fig. 3a). Additionally, the A+TNEO-T+ (stdβ = −0.16 ± 0.002, T = −7.53, P < 0.001) and A+TMTL+ (stdβ = −0.13 ± 0.002, T = −6.21, P < 0.001) groups progressed faster than the A+T− group but there was no difference between the A+TNEO-T+ and A+TMTL+ groups (stdβ = −0.003 ± 0.002, T = −1.13, P = 0.26). Exploratory study of the mPACC5 subcomponents showed that A+TNEO-T+ and A+TMTL+ groups did not differ on delayed episodic memory (stdβ = −0.002 ± 0.003, P = 0.47; Fig. 3c) but the A+TNEO-T+ group showed faster decline on timed executive function (stdβ = −0.007 ± 0.002, P = 0.003; Fig. 3d) and semantic memory (stdβ = −0.009 ± 0.003, P = 0.007; Fig. 3e).
On the MMSE, the A+TNEO-T+ (β = −0.056 ± 0.005, T = −11.55, P < 0.001), A+TMTL+ (β = −0.024 ± 0.005, T = −4.72, P < 0.001) and A+T− (β = −0.008 ± 0.002, T = −3.46, P < 0.001) groups showed faster decline over time compared to the A−T− (reference) group (Fig. 3b). The A+TNEO-T+ (stdβ = −0.49 ± 0.005, T = −9.51, P < 0.001) and A+TMTL+ (stdβ = −0.16 ± 0.005, T = −3.04, P = 0.002) groups progressed faster than the A+T− group and the A+TNEO-T+ group declined faster than the A+TMTL+ group (stdβ = −0.033 ± 0.007, T = −4.82, P < 0.001). Cognitive trajectories on the MMSE and mPACC5 for each cohort are displayed in Extended Data Fig. 3.
Replication across different age groups
Based on the lower age of the A−T− group compared to the biomarker-positive groups (Table 1), we performed the Cox proportional-hazards models and linear mixed models (1) stratified by different age groups (that is, 50–69, 70–79 and 80+ years; Tables 2 and 3) and (2) restricting the A−T− group to individuals >65 years so that groups were age-matched (Extended Data Tables 4 and 5). All analyses yielded highly similar results as the primary analyses, suggesting that age did not explain the observed differences in clinical progression rates between AT groups and that the findings can be generalized across different age groups.
Table 2.
All ages A−T−: n = 781 A+T−: n = 292 A+TMTL+: n = 51 A+TNEO-T+: n = 60 |
50–69 years A−T−: n = 433 A+T−: n = 75 A+TMTL+: n = 12 A+TNEO-T+: n = 10 |
70–79 years A−T−: n = 256 A+T−: n = 123 A+TMTL+: n = 25 A+TNEO-T+: n = 29 |
80+ years A−T−: n = 92 A+T−: n = 94 A+TMTL+: n = 14 A+TNEO-T+: n = 21 |
|
---|---|---|---|---|
HR (95% CI), P | HR (95% CI), P | HR (95% CI), P | HR (95% CI), P | |
Progression to MCI | ||||
A+T− | 2.43 (1.38–4.26), P = 0.002 | 0.94 (0.25–3.59), P = 0.93 | 2.93 (1.16–7.39), P = 0.02 | 2.41 (0.90–6.42), P = 0.08 |
A+TMTL+ | 14.60 (8.06–26.41), P < 0.001 | 17.19 (5.98–49.42), P < 0.001 | 21.94 (8.51–56.56), P < 0.001 | 5.14 (1.39–18.94), P = 0.01 |
A+TNEO-T+ | 19.19 (10.93–33.71), P < 0.001 | 17.21 (4.54–65.19), P < 0.001 | 26.07 (10.79–62.99), P < 0.001 | 11.28 (3.91–32.57), P < 0.001 |
Progression to all-cause dementia | ||||
A+T− | 1.58 (0.46–5.41), P = 0.47 | 2.50 (0.27–23.00), P = 0.42 | 1.33 (0.16–10.84), P = 0.78 | 0.90 (0.50–15.98), P = 0.94 |
A+TMTL+ | 6.22 (1.44–26.79), P = 0.01 | 18.09 (1.99–165), P = 0.01 | NA | 6.26 (0.34–116), P = 0.22 |
A+TNEO-T+ | 41.26 (16.70–101.93), P < 0.001 | 28.17 (4.47–177), P < 0.001 | 47.62 (9.55–237), P < 0.001 | 27.69 (2.49–307.56), P = 0.007 |
The HR are derived from Cox proportional-hazards models with clinical progression (progression to MCI or all-cause dementia) as outcome, age, sex, education and cohort as covariates and A−T− serving as the reference group.
NA, there were no progressors to dementia in the A+TMTL+ group for this particular age bin.
Table 3.
All ages A−T−: n = 843 A+T−: n = 328 A+TMTL+: n = 55 A+TNEO-T+: n = 65 |
50–69 years A−T−: n = 442 A+T−: n = 77 A+TMTL+: n = 12 A+TNEO-T+: n = 10 |
70–79 years A−T−: n = 284 A+T−: n = 148 A+TMTL+: n = 27 A+TNEO-T+: n = 33 |
80+ years A−T−: n = 117 A+T−: n = 103 A+TMTL+: n = 16 A+TNEO-T+: n = 22 |
|
---|---|---|---|---|
Stdβ (s.e.), P | Stdβ (s.e.) P | Stdβ (s.e.) P | Stdβ (s.e.) P | |
mPACC5 | ||||
A+T− | −0.005 (0.001), P < 0.001 | −0.002 (0.001), P = 0.11 | −0.004 (0.001), P = 0.005 | −0.003 (0.003), P = 0.33 |
A+TMTL+ | −0.017 (0.002), P < 0.001 | −0.015 (0.003), P < 0.001 | −0.016 (0.003), P < 0.001 | −0.015 (0.005), P = 0.004 |
A+TNEO-T+ | −0.020 (0.002), P < 0.001 | −0.025 (0.004), P < 0.001 | −0.019 (0.003), P < 0.001 | −0.012 (0.005), P = 0.02 |
MMSE | ||||
A+T− | −0.008 (0.002), P < 0.001 | −0.001 (0.003), P = 0.73) | −0.007 (0.004), P = 0.06 | −0.013 (0.007), P = 0.06 |
A+TMTL+ | −0.024 (0.005), P < 0.001 | −0.039 (0.008), P < 0.001) | −0.006 (0.008), P = 0.43 | −0.032 (0.014), P = 0.02 |
A+TNEO-T+ | −0.056 (0.005), P < 0.001 | −0.050 (0.011), P < 0.001 | −0.049 (0.007), P < 0.001 | −0.063 (0.013), P < 0.001 |
The stdβ values are derived from the linear mixed models adjusted for age, sex, education and cohort and represent the group × time interaction with the A−T− serving as the reference group. P values from two-sided statistical tests without adjustment for multiple comparisons are reported.
Extended Data Table 4.
Extended Data Table 5.
Findings are independent of white matter lesions
To account for the potential effect of white matter lesions on clinical progression, we additionally adjusted the Cox proportional-hazards models and linear mixed models for white matter hypointensity volumes (Extended Data Tables 6 and 7). These analyses yielded highly similar results as the primary analyses, suggesting that the observed differences in clinical progression rates between AT groups were independent of white matter pathology.
Extended Data Table 6.
Extended Data Table 7.
Discussion
To examine whether amyloid and tau PET-positive cognitively unimpaired individuals are destined to decline, we performed a multicenter study in 1,325 participants with, on average, approximately 3.5 years of clinical follow-up data available. We found that A+TNEO-T+ and A+TMTL+ cognitively unimpaired individuals had clearly increased risk for future development of MCI and all-cause dementia and showed steep trajectories of cognitive decline. Hence, evidence of advanced AD pathology provided by amyloid and tau PET is strongly associated with short-term clinical progression in initially cognitively unimpaired individuals. This supports the NIA-AA criteria-based classification of A+T+ cognitively unimpaired individuals as ‘preclinical AD’, especially when ‘T’ is defined by PET. To consider A+T+ merely as a risk factor, and not manifest disease, may be an underestimation of its malignancy.
Although the A+TNEO-T+ group was at increased risk for progression to all-cause dementia compared to the A+TMTL+ group, there were no differences in risk for progression to MCI. A potential explanation is that tau pathological changes in the MTL can cause severe-enough memory loss leading to an individual being classified as MCI (but not dementia), while widespread tau pathology into the neocortex might be needed to produce a dementia syndrome16,17,35. Supporting this hypothesis, we found that the A+TNEO-T+ group exhibited faster decline in global cognition, semantic memory and timed executive function but not in delayed episodic memory function compared to the A+TMTL+ group (Fig. 3c–e).
Another important finding was the higher clinical progression rate for both A+T+ groups compared to the A+T− group, with HRs up to 7.9 and 26.7 for progression to MCI and all-cause dementia, respectively. This indicates that among A+ cognitively unimpaired individuals, who are presumably already on the AD pathological continuum, the coexistence of tau pathological changes in the MTL and/or the neocortex represents a ssubstantial additional relative risk for short-term cognitive decline. However, a proportion of the A+T+ group remained cognitively intact after approximately 3.5 years of follow-up, highlighting the variable rates of cognitive decline even among individuals who are at the highest risk of deterioration based on their AD biomarker profile5–7,23,24,36. This possibly represents highly resilient individuals due to a favorable genetic makeup and/or a healthy lifestyle37–39. Alternatively, these nonprogressors may harbor fewer additional factors on top of Aβ and tau pathology, for example, synaptic loss, copathologies or neuroinflammation, resulting in an attenuation of their cognitive decline40–42. Hence, research into both resilience and risk factors is necessary to optimize future prediction models.
The main strengths of this study include the large-scale multicenter dataset with available amyloid PET, tau PET and longitudinal clinical and cognitive data, which allowed us to accurately estimate the relative risk of the different AT groups in terms of clinical progression and cognitive decline. This study also has several limitations. First, there are inherent challenges due to the multicenter study design, such as data pooling and harmonization, different amyloid and tau PET tracers and dissimilarities in PET acquisition protocols. Also, different neuropsychological tests were used as mPACC5 subcomponents across cohorts and these differences could impact the overall mPACC5 score. Relatedly, the varying rates of clinical progression in the A+TNEO-T+ and A+TMTL+ groups across cohorts (Supplementary Table 1) could be due to chance given the relatively small number of T+ participants but might also be a result of different ascertainment and recruitment methods. Second, the number of events (that is, progression to MCI and especially dementia) was relatively low. Third, we may have underestimated the actual risk of A+TNEO-T+ and A+TMTL+ cognitively unimpaired individuals due to consent or volunteer bias (lower study participation among individuals at risk) and informative censoring (the tendency of people to drop out when experiencing onset or worsening of symptoms)43. Fourth, we acknowledge that our design only allowed establishing relative risk and not lifetime risk and we did not control for the competing risk of death in our survival analyses.
Future studies should test whether our findings are generalizable to more diverse populations in terms of ethnicity, socioeconomic status and medical comorbidities. Furthermore, studies with longer follow-ups and larger samples of A+TNEO-T+ and A+TMTL+ cognitively unimpaired individuals will help refine the current findings. This may, in turn, aid in enriching clinical trials for fast progressors and developing algorithms for a personalized prognosis that reliably estimates the risk for future cognitive decline at an individual level. Of particular interest will be the performance of head-to-head studies between tau PET and high performing plasma p-tau assays44–47 to investigate the potential added value and cost-effectiveness of tau PET as a prognostic tool.
Methods
Participants
We included 1,325 participants from the Mayo Clinic Olmsted Study of Aging48 (MCSA, n = 680), the Swedish BioFINDER-1 (n = 56) and BioFINDER-2 (n = 228) studies at Lund University10,16, the Berkeley Aging Cohort Study49 (BACS, n = 109), the Harvard Aging Brain Study50 (HABS, n = 162, data obtained in March 2022 from data release 2.0 via https://habs.mgh.harvard.edu), the Australian Imaging, Biomarker & Lifestyle Flagship Study of Ageing51 (AIBL, n = 48) and the SCIENCe project52, which is part of the Amsterdam Dementia Cohort (ADC, n = 42). A brief description of each cohort is provided in Supplementary Table 2. All participants were (1) cognitively unimpaired at baseline defined by neuropsychological test scores within the normative range given an individual’s age, sex and educational background, (2) had amyloid PET available to determine Αβ status, (3) underwent a tau PET scan before 1 January 2019, to allow for sufficiently long follow-up and (4) had at least one clinical follow-up visit available. Follow-up data were collected until 1 April 2022. Amyloid and tau PET scans included in the study were acquired at the same time point in most cases and always within a maximum of 1 year of each other. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants and local institutional review boards for human research approved the study. This includes the Mayo Clinic and Olmsted Medical Center institutional review boards for MSCA, regional ethics committee at Lund University for BioFINDER-1 and BioFINDER-2, institutional review boards at Lawrence Berkeley National Laboratory and the University of California, Berkeley, for BACS, institutional human research ethics committees of Austin Health, St. Vincent’s Health, Hollywood Private Hospital and Edith Cowan University for AIBL, partners human research committee for HABS and the medical ethics review committee of the Amsterdam University Medical Center for ADC.
Amyloid PET status
Amyloid PET scans were performed and analyzed at each respective cohort site. Αβ status was determined using center-specific cutoffs or visually read metrics using [18F]flutemetamol PET for BioFINDER-1 and BioFINDER-2, [11C]Pittsburgh compound-B PET for MCSA, BACS and HABS, [18F]florbetapir PET for ADC and AIBL (n = 47 out of 48) and [18F]NAV4694 for AIBL (n = 1 out of 48). Each cohort provided the Αβ status for their participants (see Supplementary Table 3 for details).
Tau PET status
Tau PET was performed using [18F]flortaucipir across all cohorts, except BioFINDER-2 where [18F]RO948 was used, and data were processed according to previously described procedures (Supplementary Table 4). BioFINDER-1, BioFINDER-2 and BACS (part of a previous multicenter study23) tau PET scans were analyzed at Lund University. For the other cohorts, tau PET scans were processed at the respective sites; standardized uptake value ratios and region-of-interest (ROI) volumes were sent to the statistical analysis team (R.O. and A.P.B.) at Lund University. Based on these data, we computed the tau PET status for an MTL (an unweighted average of bilateral entorhinal cortex and amygdala) and an NEO-T (a weighted average of bilateral middle temporal and inferior temporal gyri) ROI. We used an unweighted MTL ROI because we intended the entorhinal cortex to have a relatively higher contribution since this region is involved in the earliest stages of tau accumulation yet it is somewhat smaller compared to the amygdala. The MTL and NEO-T ROIs were modified from a previously described temporal meta-ROI17,53 based on a well-established stereotypical progression of tau pathology from the MTL into the lateral temporal cortex19,54,55. The threshold was determined for each cohort separately, based on the mean + 2 × s.d. across all Αβ-negative participants in each cohort (see cohort-specific cutoffs in Supplementary Table 4). Based on amyloid and tau PET status, we generated four different biomarker groups: A−T−; A+T−; A+TMTL+ (defined as tau PET-positive in the MTL but not in the neocortex); and A+TNEO-T+ (defined as tau PET-positive in the NEO-T and/or MTL; 49 out of 65 were also TMTL+). The A−T+ group was considerably smaller than the other groups (n = 34; Extended Data Table 1), hence their results are only reported in Extended Data Fig. 1.
Clinical outcome measures
We used both binary and continuous measures of clinical progression. First, we examined progression from cognitively unimpaired to MCI (Fig. 2a), all-cause dementia (Fig. 2d) or AD-type dementia (Extended Data Fig. 2). MCI was established using the Petersen criteria56 and is defined as significant cognitive symptoms as assessed by a physician, in combination with cognitive impairment on one or multiple domains (for example, memory, executive functioning, attention, language) that is below the normative range given an individual’s age, sex and educational background but not sufficiently severe to meet the diagnostic criteria for dementia. A large systematic review assessing 11,000 studies showed convergence across practices when validated diagnostic tools were used, such as in the current study57. AD-type dementia was diagnosed using established criteria58. Both MCI and dementia diagnoses were made by clinicians who were blinded for any PET or CSF outcome. For BACS, no formal diagnosis of MCI or dementia was made during the study; hence, the cohort was excluded from this analysis. Second, we examined cognitive trajectories using a sensitive composite measure specifically developed to detect cognitive changes in preclinical stages of AD (that is, the mPACC5 (refs. 34,59); Fig. 3a) and a screening tests of global cognition (that is, the MMSE, which is frequently used in clinical practice and in trials; Fig. 3b). The mPACC5 consists of tests capturing episodic memory, executive function, semantic memory and global cognition34. Individual tests were z-transformed using the baseline test scores of Αβ-negative participants in each cohort as the reference group and then averaged to obtain a composite z-score. The composition of mPACC5 is described for each cohort in Supplementary Table 5. Note that we used a modified version of the mPACC5 because, although we measured the same cognitive domains, the specific tests used in this study are not consistent with the original mPACC5 and they differ by cohort. However, a direct comparison between the original PACC5 and the mPACC5 in HABS (where the original PACC5 was developed) showed a strong correspondence between the 2 versions (r = 0.89, P < 0.001; Supplementary Fig. 2).
Statistical analyses
All statistical analyses were performed in R v.4.0.5. Differences in baseline characteristics between groups were assessed using analysis of variance (ANOVA) with post-hoc t-tests with Bonferroni correction for continuous variables and chi-squared and Kruskal–Wallis with post-hoc Mann–Whitney U-tests for categorical or ordinal variables. First, we examined progression from cognitively unimpaired to MCI (Fig. 2a), all-cause dementia (Fig. 2d) or AD-type dementia (Extended Data Fig. 2) using Cox proportional-hazards models, adjusting for age, sex, education and cohort using A−T− as the reference group. We additionally repeated the Cox proportional-hazards model analysis while using the A+T− group as the reference group. Furthermore, we compared the A+TMTL+ and A+TNEO-T+ groups using pairwise log-rank tests with false discovery rate correction. For individuals who progressed to MCI and subsequent dementia, we used the respective times at conversion to MCI and dementia for the analyses presented in Fig. 2a,d. Second, we examined differences in cognitive trajectories between groups on the mPACC5 (Fig. 3a) and on global cognition (that is, the MMSE; Fig. 3b) using linear mixed-effect models with random intercepts and slopes, adjusting for age, sex, education and cohort. Finally, we examined whether our findings would generalize across different age groups and whether our results would be consistent when accounting for white matter pathology. Therefore, we performed two sets of analyses. First, we performed the main analyses when (1) stratifying the participants into different age groups (that is, 50–69, 70–79 and 80+ years old) and (2) restricting the A−T− group to individuals older than 65 years, so that all groups were age-matched. Second, we additionally adjusted the statistical models for a measure of white matter pathology, that is, white matter hypointensity volumes derived from T1-weighted magnetic resonance imaging scans using the standard FreeSurfer pipeline60. Statistical significance for all models was set at two-sided P < 0.05.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Online content
Any methods, additional references, Nature Research reporting summaries, source data, extended data, supplementary information, acknowledgements, peer review information; details of author contributions and competing interests; and statements of data and code availability are available at 10.1038/s41591-022-02049-x.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
We thank B. Dubois for advice on the nomenclature used for the IWG criteria in Fig. 1a. The data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the Harvard Aging Brain Study (HABS) (P01AG036694; https://habs.mgh.harvard.edu). The HABS study was launched in 2010, funded by the National Institute on Aging, and is led by principal investigators R. A. Sperling and K. A. Johnson at Massachusetts General Hospital/Harvard Medical School in Boston. Data were also obtained from the Mayo Clinic Study of Aging (U01 AG006786) and the Mayo Clinic Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center (P30 AG067677) supported by the National Institute on Aging, Alzheimer’s Association and GHR Foundation (principal investigator R. C. Petersen). The research of Alzheimer Center Amsterdam is part of the neurodegeneration research program of Amsterdam Neuroscience. Alzheimer Center Amsterdam is supported by the Alzheimer Nederland and VUmc Foundations. The chair of Wiesje van der Flier is supported by the Pasman Foundation. The SCIENCe project receives funding from the Gieskes-Strijbis Fund and the Dioraphte Foundation. V. Venkatraghavan kindly provided the white matter hypointensity data for the ADC. This project received funding from the European Research Council under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme (grant no. 949570; principal investigator R.O.). Work at Lund University was supported by the Swedish Research Council (2016-00906, O.H.), the Knut and Alice Wallenberg Foundation (2017-0383, O.H.), the Marianne and Marcus Wallenberg Foundation (2015.0125, O.H.), the Strategic Research Area MultiPark (Multidisciplinary Research in Parkinson’s disease) at Lund University, the Swedish Alzheimer Foundation (AF-939932, O.H.), the Swedish Brain Foundation (FO2021-0293, O.H.), The Parkinson Foundation of Sweden (1280/20, O.H.), the King Gustaf V’s and Queen Victoria’s Masonic Foundation, the Skåne University Hospital Foundation (2020-O000028, O.H.), Regional Research Support (2020-0314, O.H.) and the Swedish federal government under the ALF agreement (2018-Projekt0279, O.H.). We also thank the donors of the Alzheimer’s Disease Research, a program of the BrightFocus Foundation, for support of this research (A2021013F, O.H. and A.P.B.). A.P.B. is supported by a postdoctoral fellowship from the Fonds de Recherche en Santé Québec (298314). C.G. is supported by a Dementia Fellowship grant from ZonMW (10510022110010). The precursor of 18F-flortaucipir was provided by AVID Radiopharmaceuticals and the precursor of 18F-RO948 was provided by Roche. The precursor of 18F-flutemetamol was sponsored by GE Healthcare. Research of Alzheimer Center Amsterdam has been funded by ZonMW, NWO, EU-FP7, EU-JPND, Alzheimer Nederland, Hersenstichting CardioVascular Onderzoek Nederland, Health~Holland, Top Sector Life Sciences & Health, the Dioraphte Foundation, Gieskes-Strijbis Fund, Equilibrio Foundation, Edwin Bouw Fund, Pasman Foundation, Alzheimer & Neuropsychiatry Foundation, Philips, Biogen, Novartis-NL, Life Molecular Imaging, AVID Radiopharmaceuticals, Roche, Fujifilm and Combinostics. W.F. is the recipient of ABOARD, a public–private partnership receiving funding from ZonMW (no. 73305095007) and Health~Holland and Top Sector Life Sciences & Health (PPP allowance; no. LSHM20106). The funding sources had no role in the design and conduct of the study, in the collection, analysis, interpretation of the data or in the preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript.
Extended data
Author contributions
R.O., C.R.J. and O.H. designed the study. R.O., A.P.B. and C.G. had full access to the raw data and carried out the final statistical analyses. R.O., A.P.B., C.R.J. and O.H. wrote the manuscript and had the final responsibility to submit for publication. All other authors contributed demographic, clinical, biomarker and neuroimaging data, contributed to the interpretation of the results and critically reviewed the manuscript.
Peer review
Peer review information
Nature Medicine thanks Harald Hampel and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Primary Handling Editor: Jerome Staal, in collaboration with the Nature Medicine team.
Funding
Open access funding provided by Lund University.
Data availability
Due to the multicenter design of the study, access to individual participant data from each cohort would need to be made available through the principal investigators or project websites of the respective cohorts. For the MCSA, raw and analyzed de-identified data can be requested at https://ras-rdrs.mayo.edu/Request/IndexRequest. The request will be reviewed by the MCSA investigators and Mayo Clinic to verify whether it is subject to any intellectual property or confidentiality obligations. A data sharing agreement must be obtained before release. For BioFINDER-1 and BioFINDER-2, anonymized data will be shared by request to O.H. from a qualified academic investigator for the sole purpose of replicating the procedures and results presented in the article and as long as data transfer is in agreement with European Union legislation on the general data protection regulation and decisions by the Swedish Ethical Review Authority and Region Skåne, which should be regulated in a material transfer agreement. For BACS, data are available on request to W.J.J. Requests for data from the open access part of HABS can be submitted to https://habs.mgh.harvard.edu. Requests for access to the AIBL data can submitted via an online form available at https://aibl.csiro.au/adni/index.html. For the ADC, the dataset used for the present study is available from R.O. and/or W.F. upon reasonable request.
Code availability
The codes used for the data analyses in our study can be requested from the corresponding authors (R.O., O.H.).
Competing interests
O.H. has acquired research support (for the institution) from ADx, AVID Radiopharmaceuticals, Biogen, Eli Lilly, Eisai, Fujirebio, GE Healthcare, Pfizer and Roche. In the past 2 years, he received consultancy/speaker fees from AC Immune, Amylyx Pharmaceuticals, ALZpath, BioArctic, Biogen, Cerveau Technologies, Fujirebio, Genentech, Novartis, Roche and Siemens. R.O. gave a lecture in a symposium sponsored by GE Healthcare (fee paid to the institution). S.P. has served on scientific advisory boards and/or given lectures in symposia sponsored by Biogen, Eli Lilly, Geras Solutions and Roche. W.J.J. consults for Eli Lilly, Eisai, Bioclinica and Prothena. C.R. is a scientific advisor to Cerveau Technologies, Prothena, Roche and has received research grants (paid to the institution) from Biogen, Eisai and Cerveau Technologies. R.C.P. has consulted for Roche, Merck, Biogen, Eisai, Genentech and Nestle and receives research funding from the National Institutes of Health (NIH). W.F. has performed contract research for Biogen and Boehringer Ingelheim, has been an invited speaker at Boehringer Ingelheim, Biogen, Danone, Eisai, WebMD Neurology (Medscape) and Springer Healthcare, is consultant to the Oxford Health Policy Forum CIC, Roche and Biogen, participated in the advisory boards of Biogen and Roche and is a member of the steering committee of PAVE and Think Brain Health (all funding/fees were paid to the institution). C.R.J. serves on an independent data monitoring board for Roche, has served as a speaker for Eisai and consulted for Biogen but receives no personal compensation from any commercial entity. C.R.J. receives research support from the NIH and the Alexander Family Alzheimer’s Disease Research Professorship of the Mayo Clinic. The other authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: Rik Ossenkoppele, Alexa Pichet Binette.
These authors jointly supervised this work: Clifford R. Jack Jr, Oskar Hansson.
Contributor Information
Rik Ossenkoppele, Email: r.ossenkoppele@amsterdamumc.nl.
Oskar Hansson, Email: oskar.hansson@med.lu.se.
Extended data
is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41591-022-02049-x.
Supplementary information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41591-022-02049-x.
References
- 1.Jack CR, Jr, et al. A/T/N: an unbiased descriptive classification scheme for Alzheimer disease biomarkers. Neurology. 2016;87:539–547. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002923. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Jack CR, Jr, et al. NIA-AA Research Framework: toward a biological definition of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14:535–562. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.02.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Dubois B, et al. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations of the International Working Group. Lancet Neurol. 2021;20:484–496. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(21)00066-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Brookmeyer R, Abdalla N. Estimation of lifetime risks of Alzheimer’s disease dementia using biomarkers for preclinical disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14:981–988. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Dubois B, et al. Cognitive and neuroimaging features and brain β-amyloidosis in individuals at risk of Alzheimer’s disease (INSIGHT-preAD): a longitudinal observational study. Lancet Neurol. 2018;17:335–346. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(18)30029-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Donohue MC, et al. Association between elevated brain amyloid and subsequent cognitive decline among cognitively normal persons. JAMA. 2017;317:2305–2316. doi: 10.1001/jama.2017.6669. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Jack CR, Jr, et al. Long-term associations between amyloid positron emission tomography, sex, apolipoprotein E and incident dementia and mortality among individuals without dementia: hazard ratios and absolute risk. Brain Commun. 2022;4:fcac017. doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcac017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Nelson PT, et al. Correlation of Alzheimer disease neuropathologic changes with cognitive status: a review of the literature. J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol. 2012;71:362–381. doi: 10.1097/NEN.0b013e31825018f7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Ossenkoppele R, et al. Tau PET patterns mirror clinical and neuroanatomical variability in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2016;139:1551–1567. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ossenkoppele R, et al. Associations between tau, Aβ, and cortical thickness with cognition in Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2019;92:e601–e612. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000006875. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Jack CR, Jr, et al. Associations of amyloid, tau, and neurodegeneration biomarker profiles with rates of memory decline among individuals without dementia. JAMA. 2019;321:2316–2325. doi: 10.1001/jama.2019.7437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Ossenkoppele R, van der Kant R, Hansson O. Tau biomarkers in Alzheimer’s disease: towards implementation in clinical practice and trials. Lancet Neurol. 2022;21:726–734. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(22)00168-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Leuzy A, et al. 2020 update on the clinical validity of cerebrospinal fluid amyloid, tau, and phospho-tau as biomarkers for Alzheimer’s disease in the context of a structured 5-phase development framework. Eur. J. Nucl. Med. Mol. Imaging. 2021;48:2121–2139. doi: 10.1007/s00259-021-05258-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Fleisher AS, et al. Positron emission tomography imaging with [18F]flortaucipir and postmortem assessment of Alzheimer disease neuropathologic changes. JAMA Neurol. 2020;77:829–839. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2020.0528. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Moloney CM, Lowe VJ, Murray ME. Visualization of neurofibrillary tangle maturity in Alzheimer’s disease: a clinicopathologic perspective for biomarker research. Alzheimers Dement. 2021;17:1554–1574. doi: 10.1002/alz.12321. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Leuzy A, et al. Diagnostic performance of RO948 F 18 tau positron emission tomography in the differentiation of Alzheimer disease from other neurodegenerative disorders. JAMA Neurol. 2020;77:955–965. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2020.0989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Ossenkoppele R, et al. Discriminative accuracy of [18F]flortaucipir positron emission tomography for Alzheimer disease vs other neurodegenerative disorders. JAMA. 2018;320:1151–1162. doi: 10.1001/jama.2018.12917. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Young CB, et al. Divergent cortical tau positron emission tomography patterns among patients with preclinical Alzheimer disease. JAMA Neurol. 2022;79:592–603. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2022.0676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Johnson KA, et al. Tau positron emission tomographic imaging in aging and early Alzheimer disease. Ann. Neurol. 2016;79:110–119. doi: 10.1002/ana.24546. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Schöll M, et al. PET imaging of tau deposition in the aging human brain. Neuron. 2016;89:971–982. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.01.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Gordon BA, et al. Tau PET in autosomal dominant Alzheimer’s disease: relationship with cognition, dementia and other biomarkers. Brain. 2019;142:1063–1076. doi: 10.1093/brain/awz019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Pontecorvo MJ, et al. Relationships between flortaucipir PET tau binding and amyloid burden, clinical diagnosis, age and cognition. Brain. 2017;140:748–763. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Ossenkoppele R, et al. Accuracy of tau positron emission tomography as a prognostic marker in preclinical and prodromal Alzheimer disease: a head-to-head comparison against amyloid positron emission tomography and magnetic resonance imaging. JAMA Neurol. 2021;78:961–971. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2021.1858. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Sperling RA, et al. The impact of amyloid-beta and tau on prospective cognitive decline in older individuals. Ann. Neurol. 2019;85:181–193. doi: 10.1002/ana.25395. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Hanseeuw BJ, et al. Association of amyloid and tau with cognition in preclinical Alzheimer disease: a longitudinal study. JAMA Neurol. 2019;76:915–924. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2019.1424. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Chen SD, et al. Staging tau pathology with tau PET in Alzheimer’s disease: a longitudinal study. Transl. Psychiatry. 2021;11:483. doi: 10.1038/s41398-021-01602-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Bucci M, Chiotis K, Nordberg A. Alzheimer’s disease profiled by fluid and imaging markers: tau PET best predicts cognitive decline. Mol. Psychiatry. 2021;26:5888–5898. doi: 10.1038/s41380-021-01263-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Teng E, et al. Baseline [18F]GTP1 tau PET imaging is associated with subsequent cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Res. Ther. 2021;13:196. doi: 10.1186/s13195-021-00937-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Biel D, et al. Tau-PET and in vivo Braak-staging as prognostic markers of future cognitive decline in cognitively normal to demented individuals. Alzheimers Res. Ther. 2021;13:137. doi: 10.1186/s13195-021-00880-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Pontecorvo MJ, et al. A multicentre longitudinal study of flortaucipir (18F) in normal ageing, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease dementia. Brain. 2019;142:1723–1735. doi: 10.1093/brain/awz090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Cho H, et al. Progressive tau accumulation in Alzheimer disease: 2-year follow-up study. J. Nucl. Med. 2019;60:1611–1621. doi: 10.2967/jnumed.118.221697. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Strikwerda-Brown, C. et al. Association of elevated amyloid and tau positron emission tomography signal with near-term development of Alzheimer disease symptoms in older adults without cognitive impairment. JAMA Neurology79, 975–985 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 33.Wolters EE, et al. Clinical validity of increased cortical uptake of [18F]flortaucipir on PET as a biomarker for Alzheimer’s disease in the context of a structured 5-phase biomarker development framework. Eur. J. Nucl. Med. Mol. Imaging. 2021;48:2097–2109. doi: 10.1007/s00259-020-05118-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Papp KV, Rentz DM, Orlovsky I, Sperling RA, Mormino EC. Optimizing the preclinical Alzheimer’s cognitive composite with semantic processing: the PACC5. Alzheimers Dement. (N Y) 2017;3:668–677. doi: 10.1016/j.trci.2017.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Pascoal TA, et al. 18F-MK-6240 PET for early and late detection of neurofibrillary tangles. Brain. 2020;143:2818–2830. doi: 10.1093/brain/awaa180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Insel PS, et al. Determining clinically meaningful decline in preclinical Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2019;93:e322–e333. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000007831. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Dumitrescu L, et al. Genetic variants and functional pathways associated with resilience to Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2020;143:2561–2575. doi: 10.1093/brain/awaa209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Vemuri P, et al. Effect of lifestyle activities on Alzheimer disease biomarkers and cognition. Ann. Neurol. 2012;72:730–738. doi: 10.1002/ana.23665. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Stern Y, et al. Whitepaper: defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimers Dement. 2020;16:1305–1311. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.07.219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Karanth S, et al. Prevalence and clinical phenotype of quadruple misfolded proteins in older adults. JAMA Neurol. 2020;77:1299–1307. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2020.1741. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.DeKosky ST, Scheff SW. Synapse loss in frontal cortex biopsies in Alzheimer’s disease: correlation with cognitive severity. Ann. Neurol. 1990;27:457–464. doi: 10.1002/ana.410270502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Heneka MT, et al. Neuroinflammation in Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 2015;14:388–405. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(15)70016-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Junghans C, Jones M. Consent bias in research: how to avoid it. Heart. 2007;93:1024–1025. doi: 10.1136/hrt.2007.120113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Barthélemy NR, et al. A soluble phosphorylated tau signature links tau, amyloid and the evolution of stages of dominantly inherited Alzheimer’s disease. Nat. Med. 2020;26:398–407. doi: 10.1038/s41591-020-0781-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Janelidze S, et al. Plasma P-tau181 in Alzheimer’s disease: relationship to other biomarkers, differential diagnosis, neuropathology and longitudinal progression to Alzheimer’s dementia. Nat. Med. 2020;26:379–386. doi: 10.1038/s41591-020-0755-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Palmqvist S, et al. Prediction of future Alzheimer’s disease dementia using plasma phospho-tau combined with other accessible measures. Nat. Med. 2021;27:1034–1042. doi: 10.1038/s41591-021-01348-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Milà-Alomà M, et al. Plasma p-tau231 and p-tau217 as state markers of amyloid-β pathology in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Nat. Med. 2022;28:1797–1801. doi: 10.1038/s41591-022-01925-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Roberts RO, et al. The Mayo Clinic Study of Aging: design and sampling, participation, baseline measures and sample characteristics. Neuroepidemiology. 2008;30:58–69. doi: 10.1159/000115751. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Ossenkoppele R, et al. Is verbal episodic memory in elderly with amyloid deposits preserved through altered neuronal function? Cereb. Cortex. 2014;24:2210–2218. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Dagley A, et al. Harvard Aging Brain Study: dataset and accessibility. Neuroimage. 2017;144:255–258. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Fowler C, et al. Fifteen years of the Australian Imaging, Biomarkers and Lifestyle (AIBL) study: progress and observations from 2,359 older adults spanning the spectrum from cognitive normality to Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. Rep. 2021;5:443–468. doi: 10.3233/ADR-210005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Slot RER, et al. Subjective Cognitive Impairment Cohort (SCIENCe): study design and first results. Alzheimers Res. Ther. 2018;10:76. doi: 10.1186/s13195-018-0390-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Jack CR, Jr, et al. Defining imaging biomarker cut points for brain aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2017;13:205–216. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2016.08.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Leuzy A, et al. Biomarker-based prediction of longitudinal tau positron emission tomography in Alzheimer’s disease. JAMA Neurol. 2022;79:149–158. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2021.4654. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Berron D, et al. Early stages of tau pathology and its associations with functional connectivity, atrophy and memory. Brain. 2021;144:2771–2783. doi: 10.1093/brain/awab114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J. Intern. Med. 2004;256:183–194. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Petersen RC, et al. Practice guideline update summary: mild cognitive impairment: report of the Guideline Development, Dissemination, and Implementation Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology. 2018;90:126–135. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000004826. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.McKhann GM, et al. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7:263–269. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Donohue MC, et al. The preclinical Alzheimer cognitive composite: measuring amyloid-related decline. JAMA Neurol. 2014;71:961–970. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.803. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Fischl B, et al. Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron. 2002;33:341–355. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00569-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Due to the multicenter design of the study, access to individual participant data from each cohort would need to be made available through the principal investigators or project websites of the respective cohorts. For the MCSA, raw and analyzed de-identified data can be requested at https://ras-rdrs.mayo.edu/Request/IndexRequest. The request will be reviewed by the MCSA investigators and Mayo Clinic to verify whether it is subject to any intellectual property or confidentiality obligations. A data sharing agreement must be obtained before release. For BioFINDER-1 and BioFINDER-2, anonymized data will be shared by request to O.H. from a qualified academic investigator for the sole purpose of replicating the procedures and results presented in the article and as long as data transfer is in agreement with European Union legislation on the general data protection regulation and decisions by the Swedish Ethical Review Authority and Region Skåne, which should be regulated in a material transfer agreement. For BACS, data are available on request to W.J.J. Requests for data from the open access part of HABS can be submitted to https://habs.mgh.harvard.edu. Requests for access to the AIBL data can submitted via an online form available at https://aibl.csiro.au/adni/index.html. For the ADC, the dataset used for the present study is available from R.O. and/or W.F. upon reasonable request.
The codes used for the data analyses in our study can be requested from the corresponding authors (R.O., O.H.).