Skip to main content
Springer Nature - PMC COVID-19 Collection logoLink to Springer Nature - PMC COVID-19 Collection
. 2022 Nov 19;130(1):65–76. doi: 10.1007/s00702-022-02568-4

Environmental opportunities facilitating cognitive development in preschoolers: development of a multicriteria index

Juliana Nogueira Pontes Nobre 1,, Rosane Luzia de Souza Morais 2, Bernat Viñola Prat 3, Amanda Cristina Fernandes 1, Ângela Alves Viegas 1, Pedro Henrique Scheidt Figueiredo 2, Marco Fabrício Peixoto 1, Fernanda De Oliveira Ferreira 4, Patrícia Martins de Freitas 5, Vanessa Amaral Mendonça 2, Ana Cristina Rodrigues Lacerda 2
PMCID: PMC9676873  PMID: 36401748

Abstract

Access to environmental opportunities can favor children’s learning and cognitive development. The objectives is to construct an index that synthesizes environmental learning opportunities for preschoolers considering the home environment and verify whether the index can predict preschoolers’ cognitive development. A quantitative, cross-sectional, exploratory study was conducted with 51 preschoolers using a multi-attribute utility theory (MAUT). The criteria used for drawing up the index were supported by the literature and subdivided in Group A “Resources from the house” extracted from HOME Inventory including: (1) to have three or more puzzles; (2) have at least ten children’s books; (3) be encouraged to learn the alphabet; (4) take the family out at least every 2 weeks. Group B “Screens” (5) caution with using television; (6) total screen time in day/minutes. Group C “Parental Schooling” (7) maternal and paternal education. Pearson correlation analyses and univariate linear regression were performed to verify the relationship between the established index with cognitive test results. The index correlated with the total score of the mini-mental state exam (MMC) and verbal fluency test (VF) in the category of total word production and word production without errors. Multicriteria index explained 18% of the VF (total word production), 19% of the VF (total production of words without errors) and 17% of the MMC. The present multicriteria index has potential application as it synthesizes the preschooler’s environmental learning opportunities and predicts domains of child cognitive development.

Keywords: Cognitive development, Environmental opportunities, Learning

Introduction

Bioecological theories of human development (Bronfenbrenner 2005; Sameroff 2010) emphasize the importance of positive environments conducive to individual well-being over time (Black et al. 2017), given that the micro-system of the household (Bronfenbrenner 2005) has a direct effect on the child’s cognitive development (Black et al. 2017; Morais et al. 2021; Daelmans et al. 2017; Richter et al. 2017). A growing body of evidence has focused on the impact of environmental factors that affect cognitive development in early childhood, a critical phase in which environmental stimuli have a significant impact on brain architecture and cognition (Black et al. 2017; Morais et al. 2021; Johnson et al. 2016; McCoy et al. 2018; Britto et al. 2017). Academic difficulties during preschool can reflect long-term personal and social problems in adulthood (Camara-Costa et al. 2015; Salamon 2020). Studies have shown that learning difficulties in the preschool phase, such as math and reading skills (Rabiner et al. 2016), have consequences on cognitive performance from preschool to higher education (Camara-Costa et al. 2015; Salamon 2020) and have a negative impact on an individual’s ability to achieve high levels of education (Smart et al. 2017). Learning (defined as the acquisition of new knowledge and skills) is a complex human process primarily developed in early childhood when behaviors, skills, and knowledge are intensively acquired (Jirout et al. 2019).

Strategies applied to reduce academic difficulty early in the educational trajectory tend to reduce educational inequalities (Salamon 2020), and studies have shown that environmental opportunities that favor cognitive improvement are strongly related to economic status (Black et al. 2017; Camara-Costa et al. 2015; Romeo et al. 2018).

Parental education is a predictor of economic status, with the greatest education levels correlating to the highest wages and position levels (Christensen et al. 2014; Andrade et al. 2005; Krieger et al. 1997; Nahar et al. 2020). Maternal education is considered an important predictor of child development (Morais et al. 2021; Vernon-Feagans et al. 2020). Mothers with higher education levels feel more co-responsible for their child’s education than fathers and provide more activities that encourage child development (Christensen et al. 2014; Andrade et al. 2005). The home environment (Bronfenbrenner 2005) directly affects the child’s cognitive development (Black et al. 2017; Morais et al. 2021; Daelmans et al. 2017; Richter et al. 2017). Studies have shown a positive association between higher parental education levels and a home environment with more opportunities for a child’s learning (McCoy et al. 2018; Romeo et al. 2018; Christensen et al. 2014; Vernon-Feagans et al. 2020; Dickson et al. 2016). Thus, the home environment is crucial for a child’s cognitive development (Britto et al. 2017; Camara-Costa et al. 2015; Salamon 2020; Jirout et al. 2019).

Participation in stimulating experiences for development (e.g., walking and travel), availability of toys and materials that present a challenge to thinking (e.g., books, puzzles), encouragement for learning (Christensen et al. 2014; Bradley and Corwyn 2019), and access to family outings offer distinct possibilities for a child’s learning favoring its cognitive development (Britto et al. 2017; Christensen et al. 2014; Rosen et al. 2018). The use of screens at home is part of the daily lives of families in the contemporary context (Strasburger 2015; Guedes et al. 2019; Nobre et al. 2021); however, evidence indicates that using some criteria is essential to favor child development (Nobre et al. 2020). Excessive television exposure is associated with delays, for example, in language development (Valdivia Álvarez et al. 2014; Duch et al. 2013) and poorer performance on behavioral measures of executive function (EF) (Li et al. 2020).

On the other hand, if used with caution (Nobre et al. 2020; Price et al. 2015), interactive media may contribute to child development (Price et al. 2015; Council on Communications Media. Media and young minds 2016; Radesky et al. 2015; Russo-Johnson et al. 2017; Anderson and Subrahmanyam 2017; Skaug et al. 2018), especially in the domains of language and fine motor (Souto et al. 2020) during early childhood (Nobre et al. 2020). The Brazilian Society of Pediatrics (Eisenstein et al. 2019) recommends up to 1 h/day of exposure time to all screens for children aged 2–5 years, corroborating with other international guidelines (Council on Communications Media. Media and young minds 2016; World Health Organization 2019). However, recent studies demonstrate difficulties in complying with this recommendation (Nobre et al. 2020; Tamana et al. 2019), and the majority of preschoolers are exposed to screens for longer periods of time than is advised (Tamana et al. 2019), particularly after the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic (Eyimaya and Irmak 2021; Kracht et al. 2021). Given the difficulty of families in following the current recommendations on maximal daily exposure time to screens for children (Nobre et al. 2020; Council on Communications Media. Media and young minds 2016; Radesky and Christakis 2016), the risks and benefits of screens exposure to children’s cognitive development have been a hot topic of debate (Gerwin et al. 2018). Heller’s study (Heller 2021), for example, highlights the disparity between the current screen time recommendations and children’s actual habits, pointing out the need to increase the use of interactive media to favor children’s cognitive development (Heller 2021).

Taking into account that the cognitive function is a multidimensional construction that reflects general cognitive functioning, executive functioning, learning, and memory (Assari 2020), difficulting the evaluation of learning environments (Munoz-Chereau et al. 2021); the present study aimed to construct an index that synthesizes environmental learning opportunities for preschoolers considering the home environment and verify whether the index can predict preschoolers’ cognitive development.

Materials and methods

Study design

This is a quantitative, exploratory, cross-sectional study with a Multi-Attribute Utility Theory (MAUT) analysis. The study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Universidade Federal dos Vales do Jequitinhonha e Mucuri (UFVJM) (Protocol: 2.773.418). Parents provided written informed consent for children’s participation. The data collection period took place from July to December 2019.

Participants

Preschool children (aged 3–5 years) from public schools in a Brazilian municipality were eligible. Children born preterm or with low birth weight, complications in pregnancy and childbirth, children with signs of malnutrition or diseases that interfere with growth and development were excluded from the study.

The sample size was estimated using the OpenEpi software, version 3.01, following a study with a similar design (Nobre et al. 2020). Initially, 1241 children were from public schools enrolled in the city (Viegas et al. 2021), with a prevalence of 4.58% of language alterations in Brazilian preschoolers from public schools (Melchiors Angst et al. 2015), with a target precision of 10%, a confidence interval of 90% and an effect size of 1(Cordeiro 2001) would require 51 preschoolers.

Instruments

A questionnaire was created with data on the child’s birth and health to characterize the participants. In addition, the education of parents and the economic level of the child’s family were recorded.

The Brazilian economic classification criterion from the Brazilian Association of Research Companies (ABEP) was applied to verify the economic level of the families. The questionnaire stratifies the general economic classification from A1 (high economic class) to E (class economic very low) (ABEP 2019), and considers the assets owned by the family, the head’s education and housing conditions, such as running water and street paving.

The environment in which the child lived was assessed through the Early Childhood Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (EC_HOME) (Caldwell and Bradley 2003). The EC_HOME is standardized for children aged 3–5 years and analyzed through observations and semi-structured interviews during home visits. The instrument contains 55 items divided into 8 scales: I—Learning Materials, II—Language Stimulation, III—Physical Environment, IV—Responsiveness, V—Academic Stimulation, VI—Modeling, VII—Variety, and VIII—Acceptance. The sum of the raw scores of the subscales generates the classification in an environment of low, medium and high stimulation. For the elaboration of the index, dichotomous variables (presence or absence) were used, including in subscales I (presence of 3 or more puzzles, 10 or more children’s books), II (encouragement for learning) and III (walking with the family every 2 weeks). The HOME Inventory has been used in both international (Jones et al. 2017) and transcultural studies (Bradley 2015), presenting psychometric characteristics investigated in Brazilian preschoolers (Cronbach’s Alpha = 0.84 for the 55 items) (Dias et al. 2017).

Screen time was assessed using an adapted questionnaire to measure preschoolers’ physical activity (PA)—“Outdoor playtime checklist”—(Burdette et al. 2004b). that also includes the description for television exposure in minutes (Burdette et al. 2004a). The instrument was adapted for exposure to other media (smartphone and tablets). This questionnaire was validated for Brazilian preschoolers (Gonçalves et al. 2021). The time the child is exposed to television and other screens (cellular, smartphone, or similar) in the morning, afternoon, and the evening was measured. The application of the questionnaire lasted an average of seven minutes. Each question was used to identify the day of the week and the period of the day (from waking up to noon; from noon to 6 AM; from 6 AM to bedtime) in which the child was exposed to screens (television and tablet/smartphone). The time of exposure to the screens was recorded by the parents considering five possible options (0, 1–15, 16–30, 31–60) or more than 90 min).

Assessment of global cognitive function was performed through the mini-mental state exam (MMC), adapted for children according to Jaine Passi (Jain and Passi 2005) (Brazilian version in Moura and collaborators) (Moura et al. 2017). The MMC consists of 13 items covering five domains of cognitive function (orientation, attention and working memory, episodic memory, language, and constructive praxis) with a maximum score of 37. The Brazilian validation and normalization of MMC presented satisfactory psychometric properties, with 82% specificity and 87% sensitivity. MMC can be applied in the age group from 3 to 14 years old. The MMC application lasts from 5 to 7 min and has been used in several countries, including Brazil (Viegas et al. 2021; Jain and Passi 2005; Moura et al. 2017; Shoji et al. 2002; Rubial-Álvarez et al. 2007; Scarpa et al. 2017; Peviani et al. 2020). Cognitive function was assessed according to the total score. Overall, the MMC is an ideal instrument to track general cognitive function (Viegas et al. 2021).

Verbal Fluency (VF) tests have been used to measure EF, vocabulary and mental processing speed (Heleno 2006; Mitrushina et al. 2005), working memory (Henry and Crawford 2004), inhibitory control (Hirshorn and Thompson-Schill 2006) and cognitive flexibility (Amunts et al. 2021). The score was calculated by the number of words produced and the number of wrong words in 60 s per category (toy, animal, body parts, food and color). For the present study, all categories were also added and the total word production and total word production without errors were created.

Procedures

Recruitment took place at the doors of the schools, with the invitation made to the children’s guardians at the time they left the school. After acceptance and signing of the Informed Consent Form, the subsequent steps were scheduled. The first stage was carried out in the child’s home by completing survey questionnaires to assess socioeconomic data (ABEP 2019), quality of the home environment (EC-HOME) (Caldwell and Bradley 2003), and data on learning opportunities, screen time, parental education and child medical history. The second stage was carried out at the Centro Integrado de Pesquisa em Saúde (CIPq-Saúde) at the Universidade Federal dos Vales do Jequitinhonha e Mucuri (UFVJM), where the cognitive tests were applied (VF, MMC).

Data analysis

MAUT, known as Multicriteria Decision Support, was used. MAUT is a tool used in the context of the connection and existence of multiple factors in the evaluation process, such as child development, making it possible to identify, characterize and combine different variables (Keeney and Raiffa 1976), also presented in other studies with similar themes (Nobre et al. 2020). The phases of MAUT are as follows:

Phase 1: selection of criteria

First, the selected criteria must faithfully represent what will be evaluated and were selected based on the literature (Adunlin et al. 2015). Thus, for learning opportunities, the selected criteria, based on the literature, were:

Group A “Home Resources”, containing the following items related to the child: (1) to have three or more puzzles; (2) have at least ten children’s books; (3) be encouraged to learn the alphabet; (4) take the family out at least every 2 weeks. Group B “Screens”, containing item (5) is television used judiciously?; (6) total screen time in day/minutes. Group C “Parental Schooling”, containing: (7) maternal and paternal education.

Phase 2: establishment of a utility scale for scoring each criterion

After selecting the criteria, the subsequent phase aims to place the scores of the selected criteria on the same ordinal scale. In MAUT, it may happen that some selected criteria have different measurement units quantified through attributes (Adunlin et al. 2015). In this study, the selected criteria have answers quantified by attributes described in the fourth column of Table 1. In this phase, the answers were converted into numerical variables using an ordinal scale. For each answer, a positive value was attributed when the practice was considered favorable and null if the criterion does not characterize facilitating opportunities for learning.

Table 1.

Selected groups/criteria with respective values for the composition of the multicriteria Index

Groups Criteria Answers Score Maximum score
A 1.0
Have three or more puzzles? Yes 0.25
No 0
Does the child have at least ten children’s books? Yes 0.25
No 0
Is the child encouraged to learn the alphabet? Yes 0.25
No 0
Does the child go out with the family at least every 2 weeks? Yes 0.25
No 0
B Is television used judiciously? Yes 0.25 1.0
No 0
Total time of all screens in minutes per day:

Up to 90 min/day

(proportional)

0.75

More than 90 min/day

proportional)

0
C Maternal and paternal education Incomplete elementary school 0.25 1.0
Elementary school 0.50
High school 0.75
University education 1

In Group A, “Resources of the house”, the first criterion scores 0.25 for the child who has three or more puzzles (Christensen et al. 2014; Caldwell and Bradley 2003; Pereira et al. 2021); the second criterion scores 0.25 for the child who has at least ten children’s books (Christensen et al. 2014; Caldwell and Bradley 2003; Bradley 2015); the third criterion scores 0.25 for the child who is encouraged to learn the alphabet (Christensen et al. 2014; Pereira et al. 2021) and the fourth criterion scores 0.25 for the child who walks with the family at least every 2 weeks (Britto et al. 2017; Caldwell and Bradley 2003; Bradley 2015). The total sum of the criteria in this group makes a total of 1 point.

In Group B, “Screens”, the fifth criterion scores 0.25 for the child whose use of television is done judiciously, and in the sixth criterion (Nobre et al. 2020; Caldwell and Bradley 2003; Bradley 2015), scores 0.75 for the child that screen time approaches 90 min (Council on Communications Media. Media and young minds 2016; Eisenstein et al. 2019; Radesky and Christakis 2016; Heller 2021; Tremblay et al. 2017; Academy and of Pediatrics. Children et al.. 2013), proportionally. By moving away from this time, the score is lower according to the equation presented (Table 1).

In group C, ‘Maternal and paternal education”, containing the seventh criterion, scores with 0.25 per level of education considering the education of the father and mother (Andrade et al. 2005; Vernon-Feagans et al. 2020; Dickson et al. 2016). Table 1 shows the distribution of weights according to the criteria presented.

Of note, the child with the highest multicriteria index, i.e., facilitating opportunities for learning, will be the one who has three or more puzzles (Christensen et al. 2014; Caldwell and Bradley 2003; Bradley 2015; Pereira et al. 2021; Defilipo et al. 2012); 10 children’s books or more (Christensen et al. 2014; Caldwell and Bradley 2003; Bradley 2015); is encouraged to learn the alphabet, goes out with the family at least every 2 weeks (Britto et al. 2017; Christensen et al. 2014; Caldwell and Bradley 2003; Bradley 2015). In addition, this child uses television judiciously (Nobre et al. 2020; Caldwell and Bradley 2003), and the time of use of all media (tablets, smartphones, and television making up the screen time) approaches 90 min (Council on Communications Media. Media and young minds 2016; Radesky and Christakis 2016; Heller 2021; Tremblay et al. 2017; Academy and of Pediatrics. Children et al.. 2013). Finally, his father or mother are highly educated (higher education or more) (Andrade et al. 2005; Vernon-Feagans et al. 2020; Dickson et al. 2016).

Phase 3: determination of weight for each multicriteria

The numerical measure that measures the importance of each criterion is the weight. It is possible to assign different weights if the decision-maker understands that there is a different relevance between the criteria (supported in the literature or in the opinion of experts on the subject) (Adunlin et al. 2015). For the research, equal weights were used for the different criteria, assuming that each selected factor has the same degree of relevance for children’s cognitive learning.

Phase 4: calculation of the multicriteria index

In the present study, the weights considered for each criterion were the same as described in phase 3, and, for multicriteria index calculation, an average of the evaluations of all criteria was made for each participating child. The multicriteria index represented the weighted sum of the evaluations of the different evaluated criteria. Equation 1 shows how this calculation was performed (n = number of evaluated criteria):

Multicriteria index child i = Evaluation criterion 1 child i weightcriterion 1+.+Evaluation criterion n child i weight criterion n 1

Phase 5: validation of results

At this moment, it is verified whether the performed multicriteria analysis meets the objective (Henry and Crawford 2004; Adunlin et al. 2015). In this study, it was intended to verify if a higher multicriteria index was related to the better performance of the VF and global cognitive function (MMC) tests. Therefore, a correlation analysis between the multicriteria index and the variables of the mentioned tests (VF and MMC) was performed.

First, the Excel Program (version-2010) was used to formulate the multicriteria model; then, for the validation step, the data were transferred to the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (version-22.0). The normality test was obtained using the Shapiro–Wilk test. Subsequently, Pearson’s correlation with the dependent variable (Multicriteria Index) and cognitive tests were performed. With the independent variables that presented a value of p < 0.05 in the correlation analysis, simple linear regression was performed with the dependent variable “Multicriteria Index” to verify how much the created index could explain the results in the MMC and VF tests. The variable age was adjusted in the model.

Results

Fifty-one preschoolers from public schools in a small town in southeastern Brazil participated in this study. More than half children were boys (52.9%), with a mean age of 5 years; children’s mothers had a mean age of 31 years (± 6), and fathers had a mean age of 45 (± 25). Most children’s families belonged to stratum C of the economic classification, which means lower middle class. Of note, children’s parental education was characterized by higher levels of school education; 82.3% of the mothers and 52% of the fathers had 12 years of school education. In addition, most children belonged to the middle quartiles of the EC_HOME scores, which characterizes medium stimulation environments. More than half of children had high screen time exposure (64.7%), and the average exposure time to screen was 133.23 min/day (± 69.75). Table 2 presents the participant’s characteristics and the correlation of the variables with the de multicriteria index.

Table 2.

Participant’s characteristics (n = 51)

Variable Mean (standard deviation) N(%)
Age 4.60 (± 0.60)
Gender Male 27 (52.9)
Female 24 (47.1)
Maternal education Elementary school 9 (16.6)
High school 30 (58.8)
University education 12 (23.5)
Paternal education Elementary school 14 (27.5)
High school 19 (37.3)
University education 7 (13.7)
Economic status Class B 13 (25.4)
Class C 33 (61.1)
Class D and E 5 (9.8)
Quality of the home environment (EC_HOME) 38.84 (± 5.77)a Middle half b 42 (82.4)
Upper fourth b 9 (17.6)
Exposure time to screen 133.23 (± 69.75) Up to 90 min/day 18 (35.3)
More than 90 min/day 33(64.7)

aTotal score of EC HOME; bQuartile classification of EC HOME

Table 3 presents the correlation between the multicriteria index and the cognitive tests. The multicriteria index was correlated with the MMC test total score (p = 0.002). The multicriteria index also showed a positive correlation with the VF test in the subcategories toy (r = 0.333; p = 0.017), animals (r = 0.347; p = 0.013) and body parts (r = 0.325; p = 0.020), word production (p = 0.001) and word production without errors (p = 0.001) (Table 3).

Table 3.

Correlation between Learning Opportunities Index and cognitive tests

Test Mean (standard deviation) p value
MMC (score) 23.78 (± 3.81) 0.002 a
Verbal fluency
 Number of words—toy category 5.04 (± 2.55) 0.017 a
 Number of words—animal category 8.25 (± 3.18) 0.013 a
 Number of words—body parts category 9.96 (± 4.02) 0.020 a
 Number of words—food category 6.98 (± 2.50) 0.170 a
 Number of word—color category 8.69 (± 2.15) 0.088 a
 VF total word production 42.66 (± 10.68) 0.001 a
 VF total word production (without errors) 41.50 (± 10.38) 0.001 a

aPearson’s correlation. Verbal fluency (VF). Mini-mental state exam (MMC)

Figure 1 shows the correlation between the multicriteria index and the cognitive test variables (VF and MMC score). High scores in the index of cognitive stimulation opportunities correlated positively with high scores in the VF tests (for both word production with and without errors). High scores in the multicriteria index also meant high scores in the cognitive test (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Correlation between the multicriteria index and the verbal fluency test [VF, word production with (A) and without errors (B)] and C mini-mental state exam (MMC, total score)

Table 4 presents the simple linear regression between the variables of the cognitive tests and the multicriteria index (p < 0.05).

Table 4.

Simple linear regression between the dependent variable (Multicriteria Index) and Cognitive variables

Variable B β IC 95%
(min–max)
R2 p
VF total word production 0.006 0.433 0.002–0.009 0.188 0.001
VF total word production (no errors) 0.006 0.437 0.002–0.009 0.191 0.001
MMC score 0.015 0.422 0.006–0.025 0.178 0.006

β Standardized coefficient, VF Verbal fluency, MMC mini-mental state exam

A high multicriteria index was linked to improved performance in both VF tests (production of total words and production of words without errors; p = 0.001). Of note, children who presented greater facilitating opportunities for learning (better results in the multicriteria index) also achieved higher scores in the cognitive test (p = 0.006). In addition, a high multicriteria index explained 18% of VF (total word production), 19% of VF (total word production without errors) and 17% of improved performance in global cognitive function (Table 3).

This study presented a power of 0.95, with an effect size of 0.14 and an alpha error of 0.05 for total word production. A power of 0.94, with an effect size of 0.21 and alpha error of 0.05 for the MMC. The effect size was calculated using Cohen’s d which considers Cohen’s d = 0.20, 0.50, and 0.80 to interpret observed effect sizes as small, medium, or large, respectively (Cohen 2013).

Discussion

In summary, the multicriteria index showed the potential to synthesize environmental opportunities that facilitate learning in preschoolers since it correlated positively with the MMC tests and VF in three categories. Similar to MMC test, this multicriteria index was accurate for screening global cognitive function (Viegas et al. 2021; Peviani et al. 2020). Therefore, children with high scores in the multicriteria index also had high scores in the MMC test. In addition, we also verified if the index could predict cognitive development. Child development is influenced by multifactorial aspects, including the child’s reciprocal relationships with the environment (Black et al. 2017; Daelmans et al. 2017). Our index sought to address the sphere of the home environment (Bronfenbrenner 2005), which exerts the greatest influence on a child’s cognitive development (Morais et al. 2021). In the present study, we considered for the analyses the house resources (Morais et al. 2021; Christensen et al. 2014; Caldwell and Bradley 2003), screen time exposure (Nobre et al. 2020; Heller 2021) and parental education (Andrade et al. 2005; Vernon-Feagans et al. 2020; Dickson et al. 2016; Hamadani et al. 2014).

Our results corroborate recent findings showing a direct influence of the home environment on children’s cognitive development (Morais et al. 2021). Overall, improvements in children’s learning opportunities through home stimulation can be essential for promoting early learning (McCoy et al. 2018; Caldwell and Bradley 2003; Bradley 2015), favoring child cognitive development (Britto et al. 2017; Christensen et al. 2014). In a longitudinal study with Bangladeshi children, home environment, child growth, and parental education mediated 86% of the effects of poverty on child cognition in the first 5 years of age (Hamadani et al. 2014); thus, simple environmental interventions at home can positively impact children’s cognitive development (Jeong et al. 2018; Yang et al. 2021).

The healthy use of interactive media as a learning resource has been discussed in the literature (Nobre et al. 2020). Previous studies pointed out that the use of interactive media can positively contribute to child development (Price et al. 2015; Radesky et al. 2015; Russo-Johnson et al. 2017; Anderson and Subrahmanyam 2017; Skaug et al. 2018; Souto et al. 2020) if used sparingly (Nobre et al. 2020). Accordingly, a recent study showed positive results of using interactive media for domains of child development, especially language and fine motor coordination (Souto et al. 2020) in early childhood children (Nobre et al. 2020); thus, if used with caution, tablets and smartphones may improve preschoolers’ knowledge (numbers, alphabets and colors learning). Moreover, animated e-books (e.g., with voice and interactive pictures) can awaken children’s interest in reading and creating. Then, parents with high scholarly education may offer their children the use of media as a learning resource (Nobre et al. 2020).

Recent studies have shown that high parental education (Vernon-Feagans et al. 2020) was positively associated with children’s brain’s temporal cortical area development, which is related to reading ability (Assari 2020). According to previous studies (McCoy et al. 2018), access to material resources and home stimulation may have contributed to the positive association between parental education and reading ability. Then, the variables mentioned above were crucial in constructing the multicriteria index of this study (see Table 1). Children with the highest scores on the index also achieved the highest scores on the VF test in three categories and, consequently, on the total production of words with and without errors. Among these, the subcategory animals (r = 0.347, p = 0.013), toys (r = 0.333; p = 0.017) and body parts (r = 0.325; p = 0.020).

We highlight that the VF test (or task) assesses language (lexical knowledge) and executive functions (Becker et al. 2019). Li and colleagues (Lin et al. 2017), investigated brain activation in adults using VF tests; they emphasized the predictive potential of VF tests which might be employed for executive function screening. These characteristics are related to the components of volition/choice, flexibility, and inhibition of executive functions (Anderson 2002). In addition, we believe that these characteristics are expanded by access to the repertoire of resources that encourage verbal communication and challenges when thinking (Pereira et al. 2021; Jeong et al. 2018; Bornstein and Putnick 2012), facilitating, for example, literacy (Bornstein and Putnick 2012), an essential component for cognitive processes (Jeong et al. 2019), academic learning (Rodriguez and Tamis-LeMonda 2011) and language amplifying (Bornstein and Putnick 2012).

Our data also reinforce the importance of environmental opportunities facilitating joint and simultaneous learning for academic performance (Rodriguez and Tamis-LeMonda 2011); once the multicriteria index explained 18% of the total word production, 19% of the total word production with no errors, and 17% of the highest global cognitive function score. The impact of the children’s home environment was enhanced by the facilitating learning opportunities provided in their early years of age (Munoz-Chereau et al. 2021). Considering the evidence that academic difficulties can be accurately tracked in the preschool years and last throughout life (Camara-Costa et al. 2015), some of these contextual factors probably represent environmental characteristics that can be changed in early life through adequate support to families (McCoy et al. 2018; Camara-Costa et al. 2015) and through efforts to build holistic learning opportunities in developing countries (Camara-Costa et al. 2015).

Our study has some limitations. First, despite being utilized in research with Brazilian preschoolers aged 3–6 years, the MMC test has not been validated for use with children under the age of.(Viegas et al. 2021). Second, screen time exposure was calculated by adding up the use of interactive media and television; therefore, no questionnaire was used to assess cautious use (Nobre et al. 2020). However, the criteria for time exposure to television were measured using a validated instrument subscale (Caldwell and Bradley 2003) as well as current guidelines (Council on Communications Media. Media and young minds 2016; Radesky et al. 2015) and flexibilities according to emerging studies (Heller 2021). To our knowledge, this is the first study to consider multiple factors in the home environment (including interactive media as a resource) to create an index that synthesizes environmental learning opportunities in preschoolers from a Brazilian urban area. In addition, we used MAUT, a robust methodology that considers multiple factors, used in similar studies in the area of health (Nobre et al. 2022), cognitive development, and language (Nobre et al. 2020).

Conclusion

The present multicriteria index has potential application as it synthesizes the preschooler’s environmental learning opportunities and predicts domains of child cognitive development. A positive and significant relationship was found between the high multicriteria index means better performance in both VF tests and better performance in global cognitive function.

Our data point out the importance of family-based interventions to improve preschoolers’ academic performance. Children who have access to books and puzzles, are stimulated to learn the alphabet, take family outings, are encouraged to watch screens sparingly respecting usage criteria and exposure time and have parents with high schooling, probably have a greater global cognitive function and VF.

Summary

Child development is a product of the child’s reciprocal relationships and environment so that access to environmental opportunities can favor learning and cognitive development. The objectives is to establish an index that synthesis the environmental learning opportunities considering relevant factors of the domestic environment and verify how the index can facilitate domains of child cognitive development. Quantitative, cross-sectional, exploratory study with 51 preschoolers with a multi-attribute utility theory (MAUT). The criteria used for drawing up the index were supported by the literature and subdivided in Group A “Resources from the house” including: (1) to have three or more puzzles; (2) have at least ten children’s books; (3) be encouraged to learn the alphabet; (4) take the family out at least every 2 weeks. Group B “Screens” (5) caution with using television; (6) total screen time in day/minutes. Group C “Parental Schooling” (7) maternal and paternal education. Pearson correlation analyzes to verify the relationship between the established index and cognitive tests and univariate linear regression. The index correlated with the total score of the mini-mental state exam (MMC), verbal fluency test (VF) in the category of total word production and word production without errors. High multicriteria index explained 18% of the VF (total word production), 19% of the VF (total production of words without errors) and 17% of the MMC. The multicriteria index developed has the potential to be used. The positive and significant associations between the environmental opportunities facilitating cognitive development and best test FV and MMC.

Acknowledgements

We thank the Universidade Federal dos Vales do Jequitinhonha e Mucuri for institutional support. The CNPq, FAPEMIG, and CAPES. The authors are grateful to municipal education secretary and directors of public schools of Diamantina (MG).

Funding

The FAPEMIG and CAPES—Finance Code 001 for financial support and scholarships.

Data availability

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Declarations

Conflict of interest

We declare no competing interests.

Ethical approval

All the protocols were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. This study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Universidade Federal dos Vales do Jequitinhonha e Mucuri (Protocol: 2.773.418), authorized by the Municipal Education Secretariat of Diamantina (MG), Brazil.

Consent to participate

We declare that all the parents of the children or legal guardians signed the informed consent form in writing, authorizing participation in the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Footnotes

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  1. ABEP (Associação Brasileira de Empresas de pesquisa). Brazilian Associations of Research Companies. Economic classification criterion Brazil. 2019. Available in: <http://www.abep.org/criterio-brasil>.
  2. Adunlin G, Diaby V, Xiao H. Application of multicriteria decision analysis in health care: a systematic review and bibliometric analysis. Health Expect. 2015;18(6):1894–1905. doi: 10.1111/hex.12287. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Amunts J, Camilleri JA, Eickhoff SB, Patil KR, Heim S, von Polier GG, Weis S. Comprehensive verbal fluency features predict executive function performance. Sci Rep. 2021;11(1):1–14. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-85981-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Anderson P. Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychol. 2002;8(2):71–82. doi: 10.1076/chin.8.2.71.8724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Anderson DR, Subrahmanyam K, Cognitive Impacts of Digital Media Workgroup Digital screen media and cognitive development. Pediatrics. 2017;140:S57–S61. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Andrade SA, Santos DN, Bastos AC, Pedromônico MRM, Almeida-Filho ND, Barreto ML. Family environment and child's cognitive development: an epidemiological approach. Rev Saúde Pública. 2005;39(4):606–611. doi: 10.1590/S0034-89102005000400014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Assari S. Parental education and youth inhibitory control in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study: Blacks' diminished returns. Brain Sci. 2020;10(5):312. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10050312. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Becker N, Piccolo LDR, Salles JFD. Verbal fluency development across childhood: normative data from Brazilian–Portuguese speakers and underlying cognitive processes. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2019;34(7):1217–1231. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acz022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Black MM, Walker SP, Fernald LC, Andersen CT, DiGirolamo AM, Lu C, Committee LECDSS. Early childhood development coming of age: science through the life course. Lancet. 2017;389(10064):77–90. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31389-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bornstein MH, Putnick DL. Cognitive and socioemotional caregiving in developing countries. Child Dev. 2012;83(1):46–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01673.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bradley RH. Constructing and adapting causal and formative measures of family settings: the HOME Inventory as illustration. J Fam Theory Rev. 2015;7(4):381–414. doi: 10.1111/jftr.12108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Agreeable mothers: How they manage adverse circumstances and difficult children. J Res Pers. 2019;79:109–118. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2019.03.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  13. Britto PR, Lye SJ, Proulx K, Yousafzai AK, et al. Early childhood development interventions review group, for the lancet early childhood development series steering committee. Nurturing care: Promoting early childhood development. Lancet. 2017;7(389):91–102. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31390-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bronfenbrenner U. Making human beings human: Bioecological perspectives on human development. Thousand Oaks: Sage; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  15. Burdette HL, Whitaker RC, Daniels SR. Parental report of outdoor playtime as a measure of physical activity in preschool-aged children. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2004;158(4):353–357. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.158.4.353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Burdette HL, Whitaker RC, Daniels SR. Parental report of outdoor playtime as a measure of physical activity in preschool children. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2004;158(4):353–357. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.158.4.353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Caldwell BM, Bradley RH. Home observation for measurement of the environment: administration manual. Tempe: Family & Human Dynamics Research Institute, Arizona State University; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  18. Camara-Costa H, Pulgar S, Cusin F, Dellatolas G. Preschool familial environment and academic difficulties: a 10-year follow-up from kindergarten to middle school. Arch Pediatr Organe off De La Soc Francaise De Pediatr. 2015;23(2):136–142. doi: 10.1016/j.arcped.2015.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Christensen DL, Schieve LA, Devine O, Drews-Botsch C. Socioeconomic status, child enrichment factors, and cognitive performance among preschool-age children: results from the follow-up of growth and development experiences study. Res Dev Disabil. 2014;35(7):1789–1801. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.02.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Cambridge: Academic press; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  21. Cordeiro R. Effect of design in cluster sampling to estimate the distribution of occupations among workers. Rev Saúde Pública. 2001;35(1):10–15. doi: 10.1590/S0034-89102001000100002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Council on Communications Media Media and young minds. Am Acad Pediatr. 2016 doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2015.04.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  23. Daelmans B, Darmstadt GL, Lombardi J, Black MM, Britto PR, Lye S, Richter LM. Early childhood development: the foundation of sustainable development. Lancet. 2017;389(10064):9–11. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31659-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Defilipo ÉC, Frônio JDS, Teixeira MTB, Leite ICG, Bastos RR, Vieira MDT, Ribeiro LC. Oportunidades do ambiente domiciliar para o desenvolvimento motor. Rev Saúde Pública. 2012;46:633–641. doi: 10.1590/S0034-89102012005000040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Dias NM, Mecca TP, Pontes JM, Bueno JODS, Martins GLL, Seabra AG. The family environment assessment: study of the use of the EC-HOME in a Brazilian sample. Trends Psychol. 2017;25:1897–1912. doi: 10.9788/TP2017.4-19. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  26. Dickson M, Gregg P, Robinson H. Early, late or never? When does parental education impact child outcomes? Econ J (london) 2016;126:F184–F231. doi: 10.1111/ecoj.12356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Duch H, Fisher EM, Ensari I, Font M, Harrington A, Taromino C, Rodriguez C. Association of screen time use and language development in Hispanic toddlers: a cross-sectional and longitudinal study. Clin Pediatr. 2013;52(9):857–865. doi: 10.1177/0009922813492881. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Eisenstein, E., Pfeiffer, L., Gama, M. C., Estefenon, S., Cavalcanti, S. S., Silva, E. J. C. (2019). MENOS TELAS# MAIS SAÚDE. Manual de orientação: grupo de trabalho saúde na era digital (2019–2021)[Internet]. Rio de Janeiro (RJ): Sociedade Brasileira de Pediatria (SBP), 1–11. https://www.sbp.com.br/fileadmin/user_upload/_22246c-ManOrient_-__MenosTelas__MaisSaude.pdf
  29. Eyimaya AO, Irmak AY. Relationship between parenting practices and children's screen time during the COVID-19 Pandemic in Turkey. J Pediatr Nurs. 2021;56:24–29. doi: 10.1016/j.pedn.2020.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Gerwin RL, Kaliebe K, Daigle M. The interplay between digital media use and development. Child Adolesc Psychiatric Clin N Am. 2018;27(2):345–355. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2017.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Gonçalves WSF, Byrne R, de Lira PIC, et al. Psychometric properties of instruments to measure parenting practices and children's movement behaviors in low-income families from Brazil. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2021;21(1):129. doi: 10.1186/s12874-021-01320-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Guedes SDC, Morais RLDS, Santos LR, Leite HR, Nobre JNP, Santos JN. Children’s use of interactive media in early childhood-an epidemiological study. Rev Paul Pediatr. 2019 doi: 10.1590/1984-0462/2020/38/2018165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hamadani JD, Tofail F, Huda SN, Alam DS, Ridout DA, Attanasio O, Grantham-McGregor SM. Cognitive deficit and poverty in the first 5 years of childhood in Bangladesh. Pediatrics. 2014;134(4):e1001–e1008. doi: 10.1542/peds.2014-0694. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Heleno CT. Fluência verbal semântica em pré- -escolares: Estratégias de associação (Unpublished Dissertation) Minas Gerais: Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  35. Heller NA. Infant media use: a harm reduction approach. Infant Behav Dev. 2021;64:101610. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Henry JD, Crawford JR. Verbal fluency deficits in Parkinson's disease: a meta-analysis. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10(4):608–622. doi: 10.1017/S1355617704104141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hirshorn EA, Thompson-Schill SL. Role of the left inferior frontal gyrus in covert word retrieval: neural correlates of switching during verbal fluency. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(12):2547–2557. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.03.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Jain M, Passi GR. Assessment of a modified Mini-Mental Scale for cognitive functions in children. Indian Pediatr. 2005;42(9):907. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Jeong J, Siyal S, Fink G, McCoy DC, Yousafzai AK. His mind will work better with both of us: a qualitative study on fathers' roles and coparenting of young children in rural Pakistan. BMC Public Health. 2018;18(1):1–16. doi: 10.1186/s12889-018-6143-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Jeong J, Obradović J, Rasheed M, McCoy DC, Fink G, Yousafzai AK. Maternal and paternal stimulation: mediators of parenting intervention effects on preschoolers’ development. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2019;60:105–118. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2018.12.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Jirout J, LoCasale-Crouch J, Turnbull K, Gu Y, Cubides M, Garzione S, Kranz S. How lifestyle factors affect cognitive and executive function and the ability to learn in children. Nutrients. 2019;11(8):1953. doi: 10.3390/nu11081953. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Johnson SB, Riis JL, Noble KG. State of the art review: poverty and the developing brain. Pediatrics. 2016 doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-3075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Jones PC, Pendergast LL, Schaefer BA, Rasheed M, Svensen E, Scharf R, MAL-ED Network Investigators Measuring home environments across cultures: Invariance of the HOME scale across eight international sites from the MAL-ED study. J Sch Psychol. 2017;64:109–127. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2017.06.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Keeney RL, Raiffa H. Decisions with multiple objectives: preferences and value trade-off. New York: Wiley; 1976. [Google Scholar]
  45. Kracht CL, Katzmarzyk PT, Staiano AE. Household chaos, family routines, and young child movement behaviors in the US during the COVID-19 outbreak: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health. 2021;21(1):860. doi: 10.1186/s12889-021-10909-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Krieger N, Williams DR, Moss NE. Measuring social class in US public health research: concepts, methodologies, and guidelines. Annu Rev Public Health. 1997;18(1):341–378. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.18.1.341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Li H, Hsueh Y, Yu H, Kitzmann KM. Viewing fantastical events in animated television shows: immediate effects on Chinese preschoolers’ executive function. Front Psychol. 2020 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.583174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Lin LY, Cherng RJ, Chen YJ. Effect of touch screen tablet use on fine motor development of young children. Phys Occup Ther Pediatr. 2017;37(5):457–467. doi: 10.1080/01942638.2016.1255290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. McCoy DC, Salhi C, Yoshikawa H, Black M, Britto P, Fink G. Home-and center-based learning opportunities for preschoolers in low-and middle-income countries. Child Youth Ser Rev. 2018;88:44–56. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2018.02.021. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  50. Melchiors Angst OV, Pase Liberalesso K, Marafiga Wiethan F, Mota HB. Prevalence of speech-language disorders in kindergarten children of public schools and the social indicators. Revista CEFAC. 2015;17:727–733. doi: 10.1590/1982-0216201516114. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  51. Mitrushina M, Boone KB, Razani J, D’Elia LF. Handbook of normative data for neuropsychological assessment. Oxford University Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  52. Morais RLS, de Castro Magalhães L, Nobre JNP, Pinto PFA, da Rocha Neves K, Carvalho AM. Quality of the home, daycare and neighborhood environment and the cognitive development of economically disadvantaged children in early childhood: a mediation analysis. Infant Behav Dev. 2021;64:101619. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Moura R, Andrade PMO, Fontes PLB, Ferreira FO, Salvador LDS, Carvalho MRS, Haase VG. Mini-mental state exam for children (MMC) in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. Dement Neuropsychol. 2017;11(3):287–296. doi: 10.1590/1980-57642016dn11-030011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Munoz-Chereau B, Ang L, Dockrell J, Outhwaite L, Heffernan C. Measuring early child development across low and middle-income countries: a systematic review. J Early Child Res. 2021 doi: 10.1177/1476718X211020031. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  55. Nahar B, Hossain M, Mahfuz M, Islam MM, Hossain MI, Murray-Kolb LE, Seidman JC, Ahmed T. Early childhood development and stunting: findings from the MAL-ED birth cohort study in Bangladesh. Matern Child Nutr. 2020;16(1):e12864. doi: 10.1111/mcn.12864. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Nobre JN, Vinolas Prat B, Santos JN, Santos LR, Pereira L, Guedes SDC, Morais RLDS. Quality of interactive media use in early childhood and child development: a multicriteria analysis. JPED. 2020;96:310–317. doi: 10.1016/j.jped.2018.11.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Nobre JNP, Santos JN, Santos LR, Guedes SDC, Pereira L, Costa JM, Morais RLDS. Determining factors in children's screen time in early childhood. Cien Saude Colet. 2021;26:1127–1136. doi: 10.1590/1413-81232021263.00602019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Nobre JNP, Morais RLDS, Prat BV, et al. Physical environmental opportunities for active play and physical activity level in preschoolers: a multicriteria analysis. BMC Public Health. 2022 doi: 10.1186/s12889-022-12750-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Pereira L, Guedes SDC, Morais RLDS, Nobre JNP, Santos JN. Environmental resources, types of toys, and family practices that enhance child cognitive development. CoDAS. 2021 doi: 10.1590/2317-1782/20202019128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Peviani V, Scarpa P, Vedovelli S, Bottini G. Mini-Mental State Pediatric Examination (MMSPE) standardization and normative data on Italian children aged 36 to 72 months. Appl Neuropsychol Child. 2020;9(1):92–96. doi: 10.1080/21622965.2018.1522590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Price S, Jewitt C, Crescenzi L. The role of iPads in preschool children’s mark making development. Comput Educ. 2015;87:131–141. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2015.04.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  62. Rabiner DL, Godwin J, Dodge KA. Predicting academic achievement and attainment: the contribution of early academic skills, attention difficulties, and social competence. School Psych Rev. 2016;45(2):250–267. doi: 10.17105/SPR45-2.250-267. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  63. Radesky JS, Christakis DA. Increased screen time: implications for early childhood development and behavior. Pediatr Clin. 2016;63(5):827–839. doi: 10.1016/j.pcl.2016.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Radesky JS, Schumacher J, Zuckerman B. Mobile and interactive media use by young children: the good, the bad, and the unknown. Pediatrics. 2015;135(1):1–3. doi: 10.1542/peds.2014-2251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Richter LM, Daelmans B, Lombardi J, Heymann J, Boo FL, Behrman JR, Committee LECDSS. Investing in the foundation of sustainable development: pathways to scale up for early childhood development. Lancet. 2017;389(10064):103–118. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31698-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Rodriguez ET, Tamis-LeMonda CS. Trajectories of the home learning environment across the first 5 years: associations with children’s vocabulary and literacy skills at prekindergarten. Child Dev. 2011;82(4):1058–1075. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01614.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Romeo RR, Segaran J, Leonard JA, Robinson ST, West MR, Mackey AP, Gabrieli JD. Language exposure relates to structural neural connectivity in childhood. J Neurosci Res. 2018;38(36):7870–7877. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0484-18.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Rosen ML, Sheridan MA, Sambrook KA, Meltzoff AN, McLaughlin KA. Socioeconomic disparities in academic achievement: a multi-modal investigation of neural mechanisms in children and adolescents. Neuroimage. 2018;173:298–310. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.02.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Rubial-Álvarez S, Machado MC, Sintas E, de Sola S, Böhm P, Peña-Casanova J. A preliminary study of the mini-mental state examination in a Spanish child population. J Child Neurol. 2007;22(11):1269–1273. doi: 10.1177/0883073807307098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Russo-Johnson C, Troseth G, Duncan C, Mesghina A. All tapped out: touchscreen interactivity and young children’s word learning. Front Psychol. 2017;8:578. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00578. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Salamon R. A 10-Year prospective study of socio-professional and psychological outcomes in students from high-risk schools experiencing academic difficulty. Front Psychol. 2020;11:1742. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01742. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Sameroff A. A unified theory of development: A dialectic integration of nature and nurture. Child Dev. 2010;81(1):6–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01378.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Scarpa P, Toraldo A, Peviani V, Bottini G. Let's cut it short: Italian standardization of the MMSPE (MiniMental State Pediatric Examination), a brief cognitive screening tool for school-age children. Neurol Sci. 2017;38(1):157–162. doi: 10.1007/s10072-016-2743-2). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Shoji M, Fukushima K, Wakayama M, Shizuka-Ikeda M, Ikeda Y, Kawakami A, Abe K. Intellectual faculties in patients with Alzheimer's disease regress to the level of a 4–5-year-old child. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2002;2(3):143–147. doi: 10.1046/j.1444-1586.2002.00040.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  75. Skaug S, Englund KT, Saksvik-Lehouillier I, Lydersen S, Wichstrøm L. Parent–child interactions during traditional and interactive media settings: a pilot randomized control study. Scand J Psychol. 2018;59(2):135–145. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Smart D, Youssef GJ, Sanson A, Prior M, Toumbourou JW, Olsson CA. Consequences of childhood reading difficulties and behaviour problems for educational achievement and employment in early adulthood. J Educ Psychol. 2017;87(2):288–308. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Souto PHS, Santos JN, Leite HR, Hadders-Algra M, Guedes SC, Nobre JNP, Morais RLDS. Tablet use in young children is associated with advanced fine motor skills. J Mot Behav. 2020;52(2):196–203. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2019.1602505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Strasburger V. Should babies be watching and using screens? The answer is surprisingly complicated. Acta Paediatr. 2015;104(10):967–968. doi: 10.1111/apa.13105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Strasburger VC, Hogan MJ, Mulligan DA, Ameenuddin N, Christakis DA, Cross C, Fagbuyi DB, Hill DL, Levine AE, McCarthy C, Moreno MA, Swanson WSL, American Academy of Pediatrics Children, Adolescents, and the Media. Pediatrics. 2013;132:958–961. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-2656. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Tamana SK, Ezeugwu V, Chikuma J, Lefebvre DL, Azad MB, Moraes TJ, Subbarao P, Becker AB, Turvey SE, Sears MR, Dick BD, Carson V, Rasmussen C, Pei J, Mandhane PJ, CHILD study Investigators Screen-time is associated with inattention problems in preschoolers: results from the CHILD birth cohort study. PLoS ONE. 2019;14(4):e0213995. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0213995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Tremblay MS, Chaput JP, Adamo KB, Aubert S, Barnes JD, Choquette L, Carson V. Canadian 24-hour movement guidelines for the early years (0–4 years): an integration of physical activity, sedentary behaviour, and sleep. BMC Public Health. 2017;17(5):1–32. doi: 10.1186/s12889-017-4859-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Valdivia Álvarez I, Gárate Sánchez E, Regal Cabrera N, Castillo Izquierdo G, Sáez ZM. Exposición a televisión y retardo primario del lenguaje en menores de 5 años. Rev Cubana Pediatr. 2014;86(1):18–25. [Google Scholar]
  83. Vernon-Feagans L, Bratsch-Hines M, Reynolds E, Willoughby M. How early maternal language input varies by race and education and predicts later child language. Child Dev. 2020;91(4):1098–1115. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Viegas ÂA, Mendonça VA, Nobre JNP, Souza Morais RLD, Fernandes AC, Oliveira Ferreira FD, Rodrigues Lacerda AC. Associations of physical activity and cognitive function with gross motor skills in preschoolers: cross-sectional study. J Mot Behav. 2021 doi: 10.1080/00222895.2021.1897508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. World Health Organization . Guidelines on physical activity, sedentary behaviour and sleep for children under 5 years of age. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2019. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Yang Q, Yang J, Zheng L, Song W, Yi L. Impact of home parenting environment on cognitive and psychomotor development in children under 5 years old: a meta-analysis. Front Pediatr. 2021 doi: 10.3389/fped.2021.658094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.


Articles from Journal of Neural Transmission are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES