Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2022 Nov 1;11:e73353. doi: 10.7554/eLife.73353

Instructions and experiential learning have similar impacts on pain and pain-related brain responses but produce dissociations in value-based reversal learning

Lauren Y Atlas 1,2,3,, Troy C Dildine 1,4, Esther E Palacios-Barrios 5, Qingbao Yu 1, Richard C Reynolds 3, Lauren A Banker 1, Shara S Grant 1, Daniel S Pine 3
Editors: Joshua Johansen6, Christian Büchel7
PMCID: PMC9681218  PMID: 36317867

Abstract

Recent data suggest that interactions between systems involved in higher order knowledge and associative learning drive responses during value-based learning. However, it is unknown how these systems impact subjective responses, such as pain. We tested how instructions and reversal learning influence pain and pain-evoked brain activation. Healthy volunteers (n=40) were either instructed about contingencies between cues and aversive outcomes or learned through experience in a paradigm where contingencies reversed three times. We measured predictive cue effects on pain and heat-evoked brain responses using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Predictive cues dynamically modulated pain perception as contingencies changed, regardless of whether participants received contingency instructions. Heat-evoked responses in the insula, anterior cingulate, and other regions updated as contingencies changed, and responses in the prefrontal cortex mediated dynamic cue effects on pain, whereas responses in the brainstem’s rostroventral medulla (RVM) were shaped by initial contingencies throughout the task. Quantitative modeling revealed that expected value was shaped purely by instructions in the Instructed Group, whereas expected value updated dynamically in the Uninstructed Group as a function of error-based learning. These differences were accompanied by dissociations in the neural correlates of value-based learning in the rostral anterior cingulate, thalamus, and posterior insula, among other regions. These results show how predictions dynamically impact subjective pain. Moreover, imaging data delineate three types of networks involved in pain generation and value-based learning: those that respond to initial contingencies, those that update dynamically during feedback-driven learning as contingencies change, and those that are sensitive to instruction. Together, these findings provide multiple points of entry for therapies designs to impact pain.

Research organism: Human

Introduction

Predictions and expectations shape perception across many domains, through processes such as predictive coding. This is particularly apparent in the context of pain as evidenced by data on placebo analgesia and expectancy-based pain modulation (Büchel et al., 2014; Ongaro and Kaptchuk, 2018; Kaptchuk et al., 2020). While most studies of predictive coding examine probabilistic error-driven learning, humans also use verbal instructions to shape predictions, with instructions acting either alone or through effects on learning (for reviews, see Koban et al., 2017; Mertens et al., 2018; Atlas, 2019). Placebo analgesia depends on expectations formed through conditioning or associative learning (e.g. prior treatment experiences) as well as verbal instruction and explicit knowledge (e.g. the doctor’s instruction), yet it is unknown how these factors combine dynamically to shape pain and pain-related brain responses. We introduced a novel pain reversal learning task to measure the dynamic effects of predictive cues on subjective pain and brain responses to noxious heat and isolate whether instructions and learning shape pain through independent mechanisms.

Most studies of placebo analgesia combine suggestion and conditioning to maximize expectations and measure downstream responses. These experiments indicate that placebos reliably reduce acute pain (Forsberg et al., 2017; Zunhammer et al., 2018) and alter stimulus-evoked responses in multiple brain regions, including the insula, dorsal anterior cingulate, and thalamus, as well as pain modulatory regions including the opioid-rich periaqueductal gray (PAG), the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), and the rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC) (Atlas and Wager, 2014b). To what extent do these mechanisms depend on instructed knowledge or associative learning? Behavioral experiments indicate the potential for dissociations (Montgomery and Kirsch, 1996; Benedetti et al., 2003; Colloca et al., 2008a; Colloca et al., 2008b). In one study (Benedetti et al., 2003), participants underwent several days of conditioning with active treatments for pain, motor performance in Parkinson’s disease, or drugs that affect hormonal responses (cortisol or growth hormone). Participants subsequently received verbal instructions that they would receive a drug that leads to the opposite effect of conditioning. All participants actually received placebo. Placebo effects on outcomes that could be consciously monitored (pain and motor responding) reversed with instruction, while hormonal responses continued to mimic conditioning. Other studies indicate that instructions only reverse placebo analgesia after brief conditioning (Schafer et al., 2015). Thus, placebo effects on specific outcomes manifest unique sensitivities to instructed knowledge alone or through effects on learning, suggesting the two processes may act through distinct mechanisms.

These behavioral studies also highlight the use of instructed reversals to distinguish between purely associative processes and those that are sensitive to higher order knowledge. This connects placebo with an established literature on how instructions influence appetitive and aversive learning (Grings, 1973; McNally, 1981; Costa et al., 2015; Mertens and De Houwer, 2016; Atlas, 2019). Neuroimaging studies of reinforcement learning indicate that instructions can shape reward learning, and that this occurs through interactions between the DLPFC and striatum (Doll et al., 2009; Doll et al., 2011; Li et al., 2011a). We previously showed that corticostriatal interactions also support the effect of instructed reversals on aversive learning, but that the amygdala learned from aversive outcomes irrespective of instruction (Atlas et al., 2016; Atlas, 2019). This provides a potential mechanism by which some outcomes may continue to respond to associative learning in spite of instructions, while others may update with instruction, consistent with behavioral dissociations (Benedetti et al., 2003). Importantly, most previous work on how instructions shape learning has measured autonomic responses during classical conditioning or binary choices in instrumental learning tasks. Acute pain tasks provide a unique opportunity to measure how learning and instructions shape conscious, subjective decisions, which are likely to be distinct from autonomic responses or instrumental choice.

We asked how instructions and learning combine to dynamically shape pain and pain-related brain responses. Participants underwent a pain reversal learning task and were assigned to an Instructed Group, who was informed about contingencies and reversals, or an Uninstructed Group, who learned purely through experience (Figure 1). We used multilevel mediation analysis to identify brain regions that are modulated by instructions or learning and modulate subjective pain. We also fit computational models of instructed learning (Atlas et al., 2016; Atlas et al., 2019) to pain ratings to determine how instructions and associative learning dynamically shape pain, and to isolate brain regions that track expected value during pain reversal learning. We were most interested in understanding how instructions and learning affect brain responses within brain networks involved in pain and value-based learning. We hypothesized that instructions and learning would both dynamically shape pain, and that instructed reversals would lead to immediate reversals of pain reports and heat-evoked brain responses in the DLPFC and pain processing network.

Figure 1. Experimental design.

Figure 1.

(A) Experimental design. Participants underwent a pain calibration that identified temperatures corresponding to maximum tolerable pain (high pain; 8), pain threshold (low pain; 2), or medium pain (5). They were then positioned in the fMRI scanner and randomly assigned to group. Participants in the Instructed Group were informed about contingencies, while participants in the Uninstructed Group were told to pay attention to the associations between auditory cues and heat but were not informed about the specific cue-outcome contingencies. (B) Trial structure. On each trial, a 2 s auditory cue preceded heat delivered to the participants left forearm. Participants rated perceived pain following offset using an 8-point continuous visual analogue scale. Trials were 48 s long. (C) Instructed and experience-based reversals. Participants first underwent a brief conditioning phase of 5–6 trials in which Original Low Cues (gray) were followed by heat calibrated to elicit low pain (level 2) and Original High Cues (black) were followed by heat calibrated to elicit high pain (level 8). Conditioning was immediately followed by intermittent test trials, in which we delivered medium heat following each cue to test the effects of predictive cues on perceived pain. Following the initial test phase, participants in the Instructed Group were informed about reversals and we delivered medium stimuli to test the effects of instructions. We then paired high heat with the Original Low cue and low heat with the Original High cue, which should act as an experiential reversal, and again administered medium heat to test whether pain reverses upon experience. (D) Example trial order. There were three reversals across the entire task. We used two trial orders that were counterbalanced across participants.

Results

Heat intensity effects on pain, autonomic responses, and brain responses to noxious heat are similar across groups

Prior to the fMRI experiment, all participants underwent an adaptive pain calibration procedure (Atlas et al., 2010; Mischkowski et al., 2019; Dildine et al., 2020; Amir et al., 2021) to identify each participant’s pain threshold, tolerance, and the reliability of the temperature-pain association (i.e. r2; see Materials and methods). Consistent with our IRB protocol, four participants were dismissed prior to the fMRI portion of the experiment due to low reliability (n=3) or pain tolerance above 50℃ (n=1). For each participant who continued to the fMRI phase, we used linear regression to identify temperatures associated with ratings of low pain (M=42.04 °C, SE = 0.43), medium pain (M=44.71 °C, SE = 0.37), and high pain (M=47.30 °C, SE = 0.30). There were no differences between groups in the reliability of the association between temperature and pain, as measured by r2 (M=0.803, SE = 0.022; p>0.2), or in temperatures applied during the task (all p’s>0.1).

We next examined pain as a function of heat intensity (i.e. temperature level: low, medium, or high) during the fMRI experiment (see Figure 2A). Bayesian model comparison indicated that the best model included fixed effects of Heat Intensity, Cue, Phase, and Group and all possible interactions, along with random intercepts and slopes for all factors. All models revealed significant effects of Heat Intensity, Cue, Phase, Cue x Phase, and Heat Intensity x Cue x Phase interactions across participants (see Table 1). We also observed a significant Group x Cue x Phase interaction and a significant Group x Heat Intensity x Cue x Phase interaction, which were likely to be driven by the critical medium heat trials, as reported below. Bayesian posterior estimates indicated that the effects of Heat Intensity, Cue x Phase interactions, and Heat Intensity x Cue x Phase interactions were practically significant with enough evidence to reject the null (<1% in ROPE), while the main effect of Phase supported the null (i.e. no effect of Phase; 99.8% in ROPE), despite being statistically significant. All other effects were of undecided significance (i.e. not enough evidence to accept or reject the null); complete results are reported in Table 1. We observed similar results when we restricted analyses to pain ratings from the 36 participants with useable fMRI data; see Supplementary file 1.

Figure 2. Effects of heat intensity on pain, autonomic responses, and brain responses to noxious heat.

(A-D) There were no differences between groups in the effect of heat intensity on pain (A), skin conductance responses (SCR; B), or pattern expression in the neurologic pain signature (NPS; C) or stimulus-intensity independent pain signature (SIIPS; D). All outcomes showed robust effects of heat intensity (see Tables 1–2 and Figure 2—source data 1). Data were visualized using the R toolboxes ggplot2 (Wickham, 2016) and Raincloud plots (Allen et al., 2021). (E-H): Whole-brain voxel-wise analyses revealed robust effects of variations in stimulus intensity on heat-evoked activation within brain regions involved in pain, controlling for group (E), which were confirmed with separate analyses within the Instructed Group (F) and the Uninstructed Group (G). FDR-corrected p-values for contrasts E-G exceeded P<.005 and therefore we used maps thresholded at P<.001 for inference. Only the left hippocampus and right S1 showed significant group differences at corrected levels (H). Differences were driven by temperature-induced deactivation in the Uninstructed Group, as depicted in G. For additional regions identified in voxelwise results, see Figure 2—figure supplement 1 and Figure 2—source data 2 and 3.

Figure 2—source data 1. Heat intensity effects on heat-evoked autonomic responses.
elife-73353-fig2-data1.docx (115.6KB, docx)
Figure 2—source data 2. Heat intensity effects: Small-volumes corrected results.
Figure 2—source data 3. Heat intensity effects: Uncorrected results.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Heat intensity effects: Whole-brain uncorrected results.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

Results are reported in Figure 2—source data 1.

Table 1. Heat intensity effects on pain across all participants (n=40)*.

Predictors Estimates Confidence intervals P-Value / probability of direction Bayesian estimates
LMER NLME BRMS§ LMER NLME BRMS§ LMER NLME BRMS§ % in ROPE Rhat ESS
(Intercept) 3.64 3.646 3.641 3.40–3.88 [3.408, 3.885] [ 3.438, 3.849] <0.001 0.000 100.00% 0 1.005 1597.656
Group 0.11 0.101 0.112 –0.13–0.35 [–0.144, 0.347] [–0.092, 0.318] 0.356 0.409 80.83% 82.783 1.001 1632.917
Heat Level 2.09 2.078 2.083 1.90–2.27 [1.897, 2.258] [ 1.929, 2.238] <0.001 0.000 100.00% 0 1 4205.817
Cue 0.32 0.293 0.323 0.17–0.47 [0.148, 0.437] [ 0.203, 0.447] <0.001 0.000 100.00% 12.692 1 9174.052
Phase 0.1 0.107 0.099 0.01–0.19 [0.022, 0.193] [ 0.026, 0.170] 0.026 0.014 98.40% 99.842 1.001 7792.54
Group x Heat Level –0.04 –0.064 –0.042 –0.23–0.14 [–0.245, 0.116] [–0.195, 0.113] 0.636 0.484 67.15% 97.225 1.001 4410.973
Group x Cue 0.1 0.115 0.098 –0.05–0.25 [–0.029, 0.259] [–0.025, 0.220] 0.197 0.119 90.05% 96.508 1 8745.241
Heat Level x Cue 0.16 0.128 0.158 –0.03–0.35 [–0.055, 0.311] [ 0.002, 0.307] 0.097 0.169 95.27% 79.3 1 17260.576
Group x Phase 0 –0.001 –4.95E-04 –0.09–0.09 [–0.086, 0.085] [–0.074, 0.071] 0.999 0.985 50.43% 100 1 8609.937
Heat Level * Phase –0.06 –0.055 –0.059 –0.15–0.04 [–0.139, 0.03] [–0.133, 0.017] 0.23 0.205 89.04% 100 1 15179.351
Cue * Phase 0.58 0.615 0.575 0.39–0.77 [0.424, 0.806] [ 0.410, 0.731] <0.001 0.000 100.00% 0.025 1 9874.616
(Group * Heat Level) * Cue –0.04 –0.033 –0.036 –0.22–0.15 [–0.216, 0.15] [–0.185, 0.112] 0.709 0.724 64.35% 98.242 1 17007.967
(Group * Heat Level) * Phase –0.03 –0.037 –0.029 –0.12–0.07 [–0.121, 0.047] [–0.105, 0.049] 0.546 0.390 73.22% 100 1 14459.934
(Group *Cue) * Phase 0.23 0.249 0.231 0.04–0.42 [0.058, 0.44] [ 0.073, 0.392] 0.017 0.011 98.79% 52.8 1 10768.063
(Heat Level *Cue) *Phase 1.79 1.774 1.782 1.60–1.97 [1.59, 1.958] [ 1.634, 1.939] <0.001 0.000 100.00% 0 1 21294.817
(Group *Heat Level *Cue) *Phase –0.2 –0.167 –0.203 –0.39 to –0.01 [–0.351, 0.018] [-0.359,–0.054] 0.038 0.076 98.48% 63.242 1 23048.272
*

This table presents results of linear mixed models predicting subjective pain as a function of Heat Level (High vs Medium vs Low), Group (Instructed vs Uninstructed), Cue (Original High vs Original Low), and Phase (Original vs Reversed). All predictors were dummy-coded and mean centered to facilitate interpretation of coefficients and interactions. Model specification was based on Bayesian model comparison. We compared three types of linear mixed models: frequentist analysis using the “lmer” function of lme4 (Bates et al., 2015), frequentist analysis using the “lme” function of nlme (Pinheiro et al., 2021) accounting for autoregression, and Bayesian estimation using mildly informative conservative priors (i.e. centered on 0 for all effects). Effects that are both statistically and practically significant are bolded, whereas effects that are statistically significant but not practically significant (i.e. >2.5% in the region of partial equivalence (ROPE)) are italicized.

Estimates based on a linear mixed effects model implemented in the “lmer” function of lme4 (Bates et al., 2015) using the following code: lmer(Pain~Group*Templevels*Cue*Phase+(1+Templevels + Cue*Phase||Subject)). Confidence intervals were obtained using the “tab_model” function from sjPlot (Lüdecke, 2021) and corresponds to the 95% confidence interval.

Estimates based on a linear mixed effects model implemented in the “lme” function of nlme (Pinheiro et al., 2021) including autoregression using the following code: lme(Pain~Group*Templevels*Cue*Phase, random = ~1 + Templevels +Cue*Phase|Subject, correlation = corAR1(), na.action=na.exclude). Confidence intervals were obtained using the ‘intervals’ function from nlme (Pinheiro et al., 2021) and corresponds to the 95% confidence interval.

§

Estimates based on Bayesian model linear mixed models using the “brms” function (Bürkner, 2017) using the following code: brm Pain~Group*Templevels*Cue*Phase+(1+Templevels + Cue*Phase|Subject,prior = set_prior("normal(0,2.5)", class="b"), save_all_pars = TRUE, silent = TRUE, refresh = 0, iter = 4000, warmup = 1000). Posterior estimates, including the probable direction (which is roughly equivalent to 1- frequentist p-value), 89% confidence intervals, and the ROPE were obtained using the “describe_posterior” function from the package BayesTestR (Makowski et al., 2019a) and interpreted as in Makowski et al., 2019b. The Region of Partial Equivalence (ROPE) was defined as [–0.237, 0.237]. We report the median estimate for each parameter.

Next, we analyzed heat-evoked autonomic responses during the experiment. SCR and pupil dilation were both significantly influenced by Heat Intensity and exhibited Heat Intensity x Cue X Phase interactions (see Figure 2—source data 1). Both factors had practically significant effects on SCR (<1% in ROPE), whereas Bayesian analyses of pupillary outcomes indicated that evidence was not sufficient to reject the null hypothesis (100% in ROPE). Because there was no meaningful effect of temperature on pupil dilation and the number of subjects with useable pupil data was substantially less than those with useable skin conductance, we focused on SCR in subsequent analyses of cue effects on physiological arousal. There was no main effect of Group on pupil dilation or SCR, nor any interactions between Group and Heat Intensity for either outcome, suggesting that temperature effects on physiological arousal were similar regardless of whether individuals were instructed about contingencies (see Figure 2B and Figure 2—source data 1). For complete results, see Figure 2—source data 1.

We also evaluated brain responses to noxious stimulation as a function of heat intensity. We note that FDR-corrected thresholds exceeded 0.001 for all voxelwise analyses apart from moderation by group; we therefore interpret main effects of heat intensity at p<0.001. We observed robust intensity-related changes within pain modulatory regions, including bilateral insula, striatum, dorsal anterior cingulate, thalamus, and other regions that did not differ between groups (see Figure 2E–G and Figure 2—source data 2 and 3). Consistent with this, we observed robust expression of both the Neurologic Signature Pattern (NPS; Wager et al., 2013) and Stimulus-Intensity Independent Pain Signature (SIIPS; Woo et al., 2017) as a function of temperature-related changes in both groups (all p’s<0.001, see Table 2) and signature pattern expression did not differ by group (all p’s>0.2; see Figure 2C&D and Table 2). Thus variations in heat intensity were positively associated with increases in pain-related activation in pain-related regions regardless of whether individuals were instructed about contingencies. Whole brain FDR-correction did reveal significant group differences in the left hippocampus and right primary somatosensory cortex driven by stronger intensity effects in the Instructed Group (see Figure 2H and Figure 2—source data 2 and 3). Within value-related ROIs, we observed positive effects of heat intensity on the bilateral striatum that did not differ by Group, whereas the VMPFC showed significant Group differences, driven by negative associations between temperature and VMPFC activation in the Uninstructed Group, but not the Instructed Group (see Table 2). There were no associations between heat intensity and amygdala activation.

Table 2. Effects of heat, cues, and learning on responses in value-related regions of interest and pain-related signature patterns*.

Analysis Effect Left striatum Right striatum Left amygdala Right amygdala VMPFC NPS SIIPS
Effect of heat intensity All participants, controlling for Group b=0.19, p<0.001 b=0.14, p<0.001 - - b=–0.27, p<0.001 b=3.74, p<0.001 b=439.87,
p<0.001
Instructed vs Uninstructed - - - - b=0.15, p=0.048 - -
Instructed Group CI = [0.08 0.22],
t(17) = 3.05, p=0.007
CI = [0.14 0.25], t(17) = 5.37, p<0.001 - - ns CI = [2.39 5.49]; t(35) = 5.49; p<0.001 CI = [197.59 572.36], t(35) = 4.33; p<0.001
Uninstructed Group CI = [0.07 0.25], t(17) = 3.60, p=0.002 CI = [0.12 0.28], t(17) = 5.34, p<0.001 - - CI = [-0.67–0.19],
t(17) = –3.81, p=0.001
CI = [2.28 4.91]; t(17) = 5.77; p<0.001 CI = [317.40 672.13]; t(17) = 5.89; p<0.001
Mediation of current cue contingencies Path a a=0.05, p=0.058 a=0.05, p=0.079 - - ns ns n.s.
Path b b=0.13, p=0.007 b=0.16, p<0.001 - - ns b=0.01, p=0.004 b=0.00, p<0.001
Path a*b - - - - ns n.s. n.s.
Mediation of original cue contingencies Path a - - - - a=–0.09, p=0.015 n.s. n.s.
Path b b=0.13, p=0.006 b=0.16, p=0.001 - - ns b=0.01, p=0.006 b=0.00, p<0.001
Path a*b - - - - ns ns a*b=0.01, p=0.065
Association with expected value based on fits to pain All participants, controlling for Group - - - - - - -
Instructed vs Uninstructed - b=0.24, p=0.03 - - - - -
Instructed Group CI = [0.078 0.51]; t(17) = 2.85; p=0.011 CI = [0.03 0.42]; t(17) = 2.47; p=0.024 - - - - -
Uninstructed Group - - - - - - -
Association with unsigned prediction error All participants, controlling for Group b=1.02, p=0.003 b=0.67, p=0.062 b=1.61, p=0.004 b=1.31, p=0.007 - - -
Instructed vs Uninstructed - - - - - - -
Instructed Group - CI = [0.17 2.59]; t(17) = 2.41; p=0.028 CI = [0.57 3.40]; t(17) = 2.96; p=0.009 CI = [0.88 3.77]; t(17) = 3.39; p=0.004 - - -
Uninstructed Group - CI = [0.06 1.26]; t(17) = 2.33; p=0.033 - - - - -
Instructed vs feedback-driven expected value within Instructed Participants Instruction vs Feedback-driven EV - - - - - - -
Instruction-based EV CI = [0.07 0.53]; t(17) = 2.73; p=0.014 CI = [0.02 0.44]; t(17) = 2.33; p=0.03 - - - - -
Feedback-driven EV - - - - - - -
*

This table reports results of tests within a priori regions of interest (ROIs) involved in expected value and pain-related signature patterns, the Neurologic Pain Signature (NPS; Wager et al., 2013) and the Stimulus Intensity Independent Pain Signature (SIIPS; Woo et al., 2017). For mediation analyses, trial-level responses (i.e. area-under-the-curve estimates) were extracted and averaged across each ROI or computed as the dot-product between trial estimates and pattern expression for NPS and SIIPS, and then multilevel mediation analyses were evaluated. For regressions with heat intensity, expected value, and unsigned prediction error, we used linear models and one-sample t-tests across beta estimates and contrast maps. See Materials and Methods for additional details and Figure 5—figure supplement 1 for ROI images.

Predictive cues modulate expectations and pain whether learned through instruction or experience

Analyses across all trials indicated potential influences of predictive cues and cue-based reversals on pain, as indicated by the Cue x Phase and Heat Intensity x Cue x Phase interactions. To measure cue-based expectancy effects more directly, we measured cue effects on (1) expectancy ratings and (2) pain reports on medium heat trials, which were crossed with predictive cues. We first examined expectations as a function of Cue prior to conditioning, that is immediately after instruction. Consistent with our manipulation, there was a significant Group x Cue interaction on expectancy at baseline (F(1,38) = 8.959, p=0.005), driven by significant differences in the Instructed Group (p=0.0027) but not the Uninstructed Group (p>0.3), as shown in Figure 3A. There were no main effects of Group or Cue prior to conditioning (all p’s>0.1). Following the first acquisition block, we collected a second set of expectancy ratings. We again observed a significant Group x Cue interaction (F(1,38) = 7.102, p=0.011) as well as a main effect of Cue (F(1,38) = 31.195, p<0.001). Post-hoc comparisons indicated that both groups reported higher expectancy with the high pain cue (see Figure 3A), but that differences were larger in the Instructed Group (p<0.001), relative to the Uninstructed Group (p=0.003). Thus, instructions and learning both modulated cue-based expectations about pain.

Figure 3. Effects of instructions and learning on expected pain and pain ratings prior to reversal.

Figure 3.

(A) Expectancy ratings prior to reversal. Participants in the Instructed Group (Top Left) expected higher pain in response to the Original High Cue relative to the Original Low Cue at baseline (left) and differences in expectations grew larger following conditioning and the first test phase (right). Participants in the Uninstructed Group did not report differences prior to the task (left), consistent with the fact that they were not instructed about specific cue-outcome contingencies. Following conditioning and the first test phase, Uninstructed Group participants expected higher pain in response to the Original High Cue, relative to the Original Low Cue. Cue-based differences in expectancy ratings were larger in the Instructed Group. (B) Predictive cue effects on pain prior to reversal. We measured the effects of predictive cues on perceived pain prior to the first reversal (see Table 3). Both groups reported higher pain when medium heat was preceded by the high pain cue (black) relative to the low pain cue (gray) and this effect was present in nearly all participants. (C) Cue effects increase over time. Both groups show larger cue-based differences in perceived pain on medium heat trials as a function of experience prior to the first reversal, but effects of time were larger in the Instructed Group. Data were visualized using the R toolboxes ggplot2 (Wickham, 2016) and Raincloud plots (Allen et al., 2021). Error bars and shaded areas denote standard error of the mean (n = 20 per group).

We next asked whether cue-based expectations in turn modulate subjective pain on medium heat trials. We first measured effects of cues on pain ratings during the acquisition phase, that is prior to the first reversal, and asked whether effects vary based on whether learning is paired with verbal instruction. Bayesian model comparison indicated that the best model included fixed effects of Group, Cue, and Trial, with random intercepts and random slopes for Cue and Trial. Consistent with other studies of expectancy-based pain modulation (Atlas et al., 2010; Wiech et al., 2014; Reicherts et al., 2016; Fazeli and Büchel, 2018; Michalska et al., 2018; Abend et al., 2021), all models indicated that participants reported higher pain when medium heat was preceded by high pain cues than low pain cues (main effect of Cue: see Figure 3B and Table 3), and this effect was practically significant based on Bayesian modeling (0% in ROPE). There was a significant Group x Cue interaction (see Table 3) which was of undecided significance (8% in ROPE). Importantly, post-hoc analyses within groups indicated that both groups reported practically significant effects of Cue on pain prior to the first reversal (see Figure 3B and Table 3), although effects were larger in the Instructed Group. We also observed a statistically significant Group x Cue x Trial interaction, although this was of undecided significance (35.45% in ROPE; see Table 3). Post-hoc analyses within groups indicated that Cue effects increased over time in the Instructed Group (see Figure 3C and Table 3), as did pain reports overall, whereas there were no interactions with time in Uninstructed Group participants. Together, these results indicate that instructions and learning both shape pain prior to reversal, that effects are somewhat larger in Instructed Group participants, and that the dynamics of expectancy effects on pain may differ as a function of whether individuals learn from experience or instruction. For complete results, please see Table 3.

Table 3. Multilevel model evaluating effects of Group, Cue, and Trial on medium heat pain prior to reversal*.

Predictors Estimates Confidence intervals P-Value / probability of direction Bayesian estimates
LMER NLME§ BRMS LMER NLME§ BRMS LMER NLME§ BRMS % in ROPE Rhat ESS
All participants (n=40) (Intercept) 3.89 3.875 3.883 3.55–4.23 [3.53, 4.221] [ 3.598, 4.181] <0.001 0.000 100.00% 0 1 4860.246
Group 0.18 0.167 0.178 –0.16–0.51 [–0.183, 0.518] [–0.106, 0.480] 0.305 0.339 83.67% 45.258 1 5043.495
Cue 1.27 1.254 1.261 0.89–1.66 [0.857, 1.651] [ 0.939, 1.568] <0.001 0.000 100.00% 0 1 11882.091
Trial 0.11 0.107 0.11 0.02–0.21 [0.011, 0.202] [ 0.031, 0.190] 0.023 0.029 98.39% 88.483 1 13555.299
Group * Cue 0.44 0.425 0.442 0.06–0.83 [0.028, 0.823] [ 0.116, 0.758] 0.024 0.036 98.45% 8.892 1 12094.57
Group * Trial 0.14 0.133 0.135 0.04–0.23 [0.035, 0.231] [ 0.049, 0.213] 0.007 0.008 99.52% 75.492 1 13395.889
Cue * Trial 0.14 0.132 0.142 –0.01–0.30 [–0.032, 0.296] [ 0.005, 0.274] 0.071 0.114 95.07% 62.758 1 14333.687
(Group *Cue) *Trial 0.2 0.186 0.201 0.04–0.37 [0.016, 0.357] [ 0.060, 0.343] 0.016 0.033 98.70% 35.45 1 16585.744
Instructed Group (n=20) (Intercept) 4.07 4.034 4.076 3.63–4.51 [3.594, 4.475] [3.691, 4.455] <0.001 0 100.00% 0 1 4421.861
Cue 1.73 1.694 1.724 1.14–2.32 [1.088, 2.3] [1.204, 2.218] <0.001 0 100.00% 0 1 7914.074
Trial 0.26 0.252 0.26 0.08–0.44 [0.073, 0.43] [0.106, 0.422] 0.005 0.0063 99.28% 17.733 1 6993.832
Cue * Trial 0.35 0.294 0.361 0.04–0.66 [–0.031, 0.618] [0.088, 0.631] 0.027 0.0755 97.65% 13.558 1 7952.078
Uninstructed Group (n=20) (Intercept) 3.74 3.737 3.741 3.22–4.25 [3.215, 4.259] [ 3.261, 4.185] <0.001 0 100.00% 0 1.001 3062.443
Cue 0.89 0.885 0.874 0.40–1.38 [0.378, 1.392] [ 0.450, 1.262] <0.001 0.0008 99.88% 0.55 1.001 9645.377
Trial 0 –0.005 –0.005 –0.11–0.10 [–0.113, 0.103] [–0.095, 0.087] 0.928 0.9333 53.46% 99.433 1 8985.322
Cue * Trial –0.03 –0.035 –0.035 –0.20–0.14 [–0.211, 0.142] [–0.184, 0.112] 0.704 0.6975 65.33% 90.7 1 12054.973
*

This table presents results of a linear mixed model predicting subjective pain on medium heat trials as a function of Group (Instructed vs Uninstructed), Cue (Original High vs Original Low), and Trial prior to the first reversal, as well as post-hoc tests in each Group. See Table 1 for additional information about model specification and presentation.

Estimates based on Bayesian model linear mixed models using the ‘brms’ function (Bürkner, 2017) using the following code: brmPain~Group*Cue*Trial+(1+Cue*Trial|Subject,prior = set_prior("normal(0,2.5)", class="b"), save_all_pars = TRUE, silent = TRUE, refresh = 0, iter = 4000,, warmup = 1000). Posterior estimates and the Region of Partial Equivalence were obtained using the “describe_posterior” function from the package BayesTestR (Makowski et al., 2019a) and interpreted as in Makowski et al., 2019b. The Region of Partial Equivalence (ROPE) was defined as [–0.17, 0.17] across all participants, [–.172,.172] when restricted to the Instructed Group, and [–.168,.168] when restricted to the Uninstructed Group.

Estimates based on a linear mixed effects model implemented in the ‘lmer’ function of lme4 (Bates et al., 2015) using the following code: lmer(PainMedium~Group*Cue*Trial+(1+Cue*Trial||Subject)).

§

Estimates based on a linear mixed effects model implemented in the ‘lme’ function of nlme (Pinheiro et al., 2021) including autoregression using the following code: lme(Pain~Group *Cue*Trial, random = ~1 + Cue*Trial|Subject, correlation = corAR1, na.action=na.exclude).

Cue-based expectations and cue effects on pain and SCR update as contingencies reverse

We next tested whether expectations and cue effects on pain updated as contingencies reversed, and whether they did so differently as a function of instruction. We computed an expectancy rating difference score (Original High Pain expectancy – Original Low Pain expectancy; see Figure 4B) for each pre-block rating and measured effects across the entire task as a function of Group and Phase (i.e. Original vs. Reversed Contingencies; see vertical dashed lines in Figures 1D and 4A). We observed a main effect of Phase (B=–2.03, p<0.001), indicating that differential expectations varied as contingencies reversed, and significant Group x Phase interaction (B=4.12, p<0.001). Post-hoc analyses indicated that only the Instructed Group reported differences in expectation that varied significantly as a function of Phase, whereas the Uninstructed Group showed weaker variations in expectations as contingencies reversed (see Figure 4A and B).

Figure 4. Expectations and pain ratings update as contingencies change.

We analyzed cue-based expectations and the effects of cues on pain ratings in response to medium heat across the entire task, including reversals. Reversals were coded relative to instructions in the Instructed Group and relative to experience in the Uninstructed Group (see Figure 1C). (A) Expectancy ratings across the entire task. Both groups updated expectations as contingencies reversed. (B) Cue-based differences in expectancy. The Instructed Group (Red) shows larger differences in expectancy as a function of phase, although both groups show significant Cue x Phase interactions across the task, indicating that both instructions and experiential learning dynamically shape expectations. (C) Effects of current cue contingencies on subjective pain. We analyzed Cue x Phase interactions on pain to evaluate whether individuals report higher pain with the cue that is currently paired with high heat (Original High Cue on original contingency blocks, Original Low Cue on reversed blocks). Both groups reported higher pain when medium heat was paired with the current high cue relative to the current low cue. (D) Effects of current cue contingencies on heat-evoked SCR. Similar to pain, both groups displayed elevated heat-evoked SCR when medium heat was paired with the current high cue relative to the current low cue. (E) Pain reversals are larger in Instructed Group participants. As with expectancy ratings, both groups showed significant reversals of cue effects on subjective pain as contingencies changed, but reversals were larger in Instructed Group participants. Individual participants’ ratings are presented in Figure 4—figure supplement 1 and retrospective ratings are reported in Figure 4—figure supplement 2. Errors and shaded regions denote standard error of the mean (n = 20 per group).

Figure 4—source data 1. Cue effects on skin conductance responses.

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Cue and phase effects for individual participants.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

This figure depicts effects of Cue (original low = yellow; original high = orange) and Phase (original = ‘orig’, reversed = ‘rev”) on subjective pain on medium heat trials for each participant. Instructed Group participants are displayed with solid lines and Uninstructed Participants are displayed in dashed lines. Shaded areas denote standard error of the mean.
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. Retrospective ratings.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

Following the task, participants provided retrospective ratings of affect (A) and expected pain at the beginning and end of the task (B) as a function of Cue. Error bars denote standard error of the mean (n = 20 per group).

We next examined pain reports in response to medium heat across all trials, including reversals (see Figure 4). Bayesian model comparison using a normal distribution indicated the most likely model included fixed effects of Group, Cue, Phase, and Trial, with random intercepts and slopes. All models revealed significant Cue x Phase interactions on pain, indicating that cue effects on pain varied as contingencies reversed (see Figure 4C and D and Table 4), and this effect was sufficient to reject the null hypothesis of no interaction (<1% in ROPE). All models also revealed main effects of Cue, such that individuals reported higher pain in response to the original high pain cue than the original low pain cue, and main effects of Phase, such that pain was higher on original contingencies relative to reversals, and these effects were significant in frequentist analyses but were of undecided significance based on Bayesian estimates (see Table 4). Finally, frequentist analysis approaches revealed significant Group x Cue x Phase interactions, driven by stronger reversals of Cue effects in the Instructed Group (see Figure 4D). Post hoc analyses conducted separately by Group indicated nearly 100% probability of positive Cue x Phase interactions in each group, although evidence was only sufficient to reject the null hypothesis in the Instructed Group (see Table 4). We observed similar results when we restricted analyses to pain ratings from the 36 participants with useable fMRI data, although the Group x Cue x Phase interaction was marginally significant in frequentist approaches; see Supplementary file 1 for complete details. We also observed consistent findings when we tested the model with a beta distribution, which was found to provide better fits based on posterior prediction (see Supplementary file 2). Thus predictive cues shape pain perception even as contingencies change, whether or not participants are instructed about contingencies. In addition, reversals may be slightly larger in participants who are explicitly instructed about contingencies and reversals, however group differences were not practically meaningful based on Bayesian statistics.

Table 4. Multilevel model evaluating effects of Group, Cue, and Phase on medium heat pain across the entire task*.

Predictors Estimates Confidence intervals P-Value / probability of direction Bayesian estimates
LMER NLME§ BRMS LMER NLME§ BRMS LMER NLME§ BRMS % in ROPE Rhat ESS
All participants (n=40) (Intercept) 3.63 3.624 3.621 3.31–3.95 [3.301, 3.948] [ 3.342, 3.889] <0.001 0.000 100.00% 0 1.001 2237.328
Group 0.11 0.100 0.103 –0.22–0.43 [–0.233, 0.433] [–0.162, 0.379] 0.52 0.546 73.12% 62.4 1.002 2181.022
Cue 0.29 0.286 0.287 0.14–0.44 [0.146, 0.426] [ 0.160, 0.413] <0.001 0.000 100.00% 8.425 1 17294.743
Phase 0.1 0.100 0.104 0.00–0.20 [0.001, 0.198] [ 0.015, 0.183] 0.046 0.047 97.38% 91.908 1 10287.406
Group * Cue 0.06 0.095 0.065 –0.09–0.21 [–0.045, 0.235] [–0.062,, 0.189] 0.401 0.182 79.30% 92.3 1 17827.085
Group * Phase –0.01 –0.002 –0.005 –0.11–0.09 [–0.1, 0.097] [–0.087, 0.079] 0.907 0.974 54.11% 99.925 1 10239.068
Cue * Phase 0.58 0.643 0.582 0.38–0.78 [0.443, 0.843] [ 0.415, 0.740] <0.001 0.000 100.00% 0.042 1 11512.539
(Group * Cue) * Phase 0.24 0.248 0.241 0.04–0.44 [0.048, 0.447] [ 0.077, 0.400] 0.018 0.015 98.97% 25.2 1 10802.199
Instructed Group (n=20) (Intercept) 3.74 3.732 3.731 3.32–4.16 [3.313, 4.15] [ 3.363, 4.107] <0.001 0.000 100.00% 0 1.001 1614.785
Cue 0.36 0.386 0.353 0.14–0.58 [0.182, 0.59] [ 0.156, 0.530] 0.001 0.000 99.83% 5.775 1 13912.092
Phase 0.1 0.100 0.098 –0.02–0.22 [–0.03, 0.23] [–0.003, 0.194] 0.105 0.133 94.46% 88.158 1 12203.321
Cue * Phase 0.84 0.904 0.836 0.52–1.17 [0.58, 1.227] [ 0.559, 1.112] <0.001 0.000 99.99% 0.05 1 9283.385
Uninstructed Group (n=20) (Intercept) 3.53 3.531 3.526 3.04–4.02 [3.04, 4.022] [ 3.098, 3.955] <0.001 0.000 100.00% 0 1.001 2390.769
Cue 0.23 0.194 0.228 0.01–0.44 [–0.004, 0.391] [ 0.058, 0.411] 0.037 0.054 98.09% 33.975 1 19966.344
Phase 0.11 0.099 0.107 –0.05–0.27 [–0.05, 0.248] [–0.025, 0.245] 0.19 0.192 90.18% 81.167 1 9498.139
Cue * Phase 0.35 0.411 0.354 0.11–0.60 [0.169, 0.653] [ 0.158, 0.555] 0.004 0.001 99.58% 8.133 1 12670.552
*

This table presents results of a linear mixed model predicting subjective pain on medium heat trials as a function of Group (Instructed vs Uninstructed), Cue (Original High vs Original Low), and Phase (Original vs Reversed) across all participants, as well as post-hoc tests in each Group. See Table 1 for additional information about model specification and presentation.

Estimates based on Bayesian model linear mixed models using the ‘brms’ function (Bürkner, 2017) using the following code: brmPain~Group *Cue*Phase+(1+Cue*Phase|Subject,prior = set_prior("normal(0,2.5)", class="b"), save_all_pars = TRUE, silent = TRUE, refresh = 0, iter = 4000,, warmup = 1000). Posterior estimates and the Region of Partial Equivalence were obtained using the “describe_posterior” function from the package BayesTestR (Makowski et al., 2019a) and interpreted as in Makowski et al., 2019b. The Region of Partial Equivalence (ROPE) was defined as [–0.176, 0.176] across all participants, [–.170,.170] when restricted to the Instructed Group, and [–.181,.181] when restricted to the Uninstructed Group.

Estimates based on a linear mixed effects model implemented in the ‘lmer’ function of lme4 (Bates et al., 2015) using the following code: lmer(PainMedium~Group*Cue*Phase+(1+Cue*Phase|Subject)).

§

Estimates based on a linear mixed effects model implemented in the ‘lme’ function of nlme (Pinheiro et al., 2021) including autoregression using the following code: lme(Pain~Group *Cue*Phase, random = ~1 + Cue*Phase|Subject, correlation = corAR1(), na.action=na.exclude).

We also tested whether cues and reversals impacted physiological responses to medium heat, as measured by heat-evoked SCR. Heat-evoked SCRs were influenced by predictive cues on medium trials and reversed as contingencies changed, but the magnitude of these differences did not differ by Group (see Figure 4D and Figure 4—source data 1). While effects were statistically significant based on frequentist models, they were not sufficient to reject the null hypothesis of no difference based on Bayesian models (see Figure 4—source data 1). Importantly, only 13 Instructed Group participants had variations in heat-evoked SCR on medium heat trials and were included in analyses; we therefore take these results with caution and did not analyze associations between trial-by-trial SCR and brain responses. We also evaluated cue effects on anticipatory SCR, that is responses to the cue in the interval prior to heat stimulation, in exploratory analyses. In contrast to other outcomes, anticipatory arousal was associated with a main effect of Cue and a significant Group x Phase interaction (see Figure 4—source data 1), but we did not observe any interactions between Cue and Phase, suggesting that anticipatory responses did not vary as contingencies change. However, analyses were limited to 29 participants and Bayesian analyses indicated that the data support the null hypothesis of no effect, and thus we do not make inference based on anticipatory arousal.

Cue effects on heat-evoked responses in pain-related regions reverse as contingencies change, irrespective of instruction, and prefrontal regions mediate cue effects on subjective pain

Behavioral analyses indicated that predictive cues modulated expectations and subjective pain, and that cue effects on both outcomes updated as contingencies reversed. We next asked which brain regions mediated these effects. We were most interested in current cue effects on brain responses to medium heat, that is the Cue x Phase interaction (see Materials and methods and Figure 5A).

Figure 5. Mediation of current cue effects on medium heat pain.

We examined brain mediators of current contingency effects on perceived pain on medium heat trials. Results are FDR-corrected within pain modulatory regions and across the whole brain. (A) Mediation model. We tested for brain regions that mediate the effects of current cue contingencies on subjective pain, corresponding to the reversals we observed (Figure 4). (B) Path A: effects of current contingencies. Path a identifies brain regions that show greater activation with the current high pain cue (e.g. Original High Cue during original contingencies, Original Low Cue during reversed contingencies), relative to the current low pain cue. Within pain modulatory regions (see Figure 5—figure supplement 1), we observed positive Path a effects (HM >LM) in the bilateral anterior insula, dorsal anterior cingulate, right anterior prefrontal cortex, and left dorsal posterior insula, and negative Path a effects (LM >HM) in the left subgenual ACC. We extracted trial-by-trial estimates from the left anterior insula and visualized average responses as a function of Group, Cue, and Phase (bottom left; Figure 2). Both groups showed greater left insula activation when medium heat was preceded by the Current High Cue, and cue effects did not differ by group. Differences were confirmed with extracted average timecourses (see Figure 5—figure supplement 3). Whole brain FDR-correction (bottom right) additionally identified positive Path a effects in the M1, S1, and right inferior parietal lobule (see Figure 5—figure supplement 4). (C) Mediation of current cue effects on pain. We observed significant negative mediation by several pain modulatory regions, including (from left to right) the subgenual ACC, the right VMPFC/OFC, the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, and the left DLPFC. Extracting responses from each of these regions indicated that individuals who showed larger cue effects (i.e. Path a effects) showed more negative associations between brain activation and subjective pain (i.e. Path b). This is consistent with the fact that mediation can be driven by the covariance between paths, and is consistent with a modulatory suppression effect. Representative correlations are depicted for the right OFC; r=–0.40, P<.001, with Instructed Group participants in red and Uninstructed Group in blue. (D) Path b: associations with pain controlling for cue. Path b regions are positively associated with pain, controlling for cue (and temperature, since we tested only medium heat trials). We observed positive Path b effects in the VLPFC, bilateral putamen, bilateral anterior insula, and other regions within the pain modulatory network (top). We also observed significant Path b effects on NPS and SIIPS expression. Spaghetti plots in the lower right illustrate associations between trial-level pattern expression and pain, controlling for cue, for all individuals (blue) and the entire sample (95% CI illustrated in gray). For additional regions identified in whole brain search and uncorrected results, see Figures 35 and Figure 5—source data 1 (whole-brain corrected results) and Figure 5—source data 2 (uncorrected results).

Figure 5—source data 1. Mediation of current cue contingencies: Small-volumes corrected results.
Figure 5—source data 2. Mediation of current cue contingencies: Uncorrected results.

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. A priori regions of interest.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

We searched within two sets of a priori regions of interest (ROIs). (A) Pain modulation ROIs were identified through a meta-analysis of placebo analgesia and expectancy-based pain modulation (Atlas and Wager, 2014b). We created a mask combined across modulatory regions whose responses to noxious stimuli increase with expectations for reduced pain (red) and regions whose responses to noxious stimuli decrease with expectations for reduced pain (blue). (B) We also examined responses within value-related ROIs (right), including the bilateral striatum (cyan), bilateral amygdala (violet), and the VMPFC/OFC (yellow). See Materials and methods for details of ROI identification and ROI-wise analyses.
Figure 5—figure supplement 2. Current contingency mediation: Effects of cue and phase within pain modulatory network.

Figure 5—figure supplement 2.

This figure depicts extracted responses within pain modulatory regions for Path a and mediation of current contingency effects on medium heat pain. We extracted responses and averaged as a function of Group (Instructed vs Uninstructed), Cue (Original low = ‘Orig LM’; Original high = ‘Orig HM’), Phase (Left two lighter bars = Original contingencies; Right two darker bars = reversed contingencies). (A) Responses in pain modulatory clusters identified in analyses of Path a with positive effects, hat is higher activation in response to current high pain cues relative to current low pain cues. This confirms that these regions update responses as contingencies change and do so similarly across both groups. (B) Responses within the sgACC region that showed negative path a effects. (C) Pattern expression within the neurologic signature pattern (NPS; Wager et al., 2013). (D) Pattern expression within the stimulus-intensity independent pain signature pattern (SIIPS; Woo et al., 2017).
Figure 5—figure supplement 3. Raw timecourses in Path A regions.

Figure 5—figure supplement 3.

Images depict average baseline-corrected trial timecourse for regions identified in FDR-Corrected Path A analyses (see Figure 5—figure supplement 3) as a function of Group and temperature. Average responses to low heat trials (green) and high heat trials (red) are depicted in the left column, and responses to medium heat trials are depicted in the center column (original contingencies) and right column (reversed contingencies) as a function of original cue type.
Figure 5—figure supplement 4. Current contingency mediation: Whole-brain FDR correction.

Figure 5—figure supplement 4.

Results are reported in Figure 5—source data 1.
Figure 5—figure supplement 5. Raw timecourses in value-related regions of interest.

Figure 5—figure supplement 5.

Images depict average baseline-corrected trial timecourse for value-related ROIs (see Figure 5—figure supplement 1) as a function of Group and temperature. Average responses to low heat trials (green) and high heat trials (red) are depicted in the left column, and responses to medium heat trials are depicted in the center column (original contingencies) and right column (reversed contingencies) as a function of original cue type.
Figure 5—figure supplement 6. Current contingency mediation: Whole-brain uncorrected.

Figure 5—figure supplement 6.

Results are reported in Figure 5—source data 2.
Figure 5—figure supplement 7. Current contingency mediation including Group as a moderator: Whole-brain uncorrected.

Figure 5—figure supplement 7.

Results are reported in Figure 5—source data 2.

Path a identified regions that showed stronger activation in response to medium heat following current high pain cues relative to current low pain cues. Within pain modulatory regions, we observed significant positive Path a effects (current high cue >current low cue) in the bilateral anterior insula, left dorsal posterior insula, dACC, and right anterior prefrontal cortex, and negative associations in the left subgenual ACC (sgACC; see Figure 5B and Figure 5—source data 1). Extracting trial-level responses confirmed that regions with positive Path a activation showed greater activation when medium heat was preceded by the initial high pain cue relative to the initial low pain cue during the original contingences, whereas they showed greater activation when medium heat was paired with the initial low pain cue when contingencies were reversed, and these reversals were observed for both groups (see Figure 5B and Figure 5—figure supplements 2 and 3). Whole brain FDR correction additionally indicated positive Path a effects in left M1, S1, and right inferior parietal lobule (see Figure 5B, Figure 5—figure supplement 4, and Figure 5—source data 1). We observed marginal Path a effects on the bilateral striatum (see Table 2 and Figure 5—figure supplement 5); no other ROIs were modulated by current cue contingencies and there were no effects of current cues on the NPS or SIIPS (see Table 2).

Path b identified voxels that were associated with subjective pain while controlling for cue (see Materials and Methods). We observed positive Path b effects within pain-related regions including bilateral anterior insula, pregenual ACC, bilateral putamen, bilateral amygdala, left thalamus, right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC), and right middle insula (see Figure 5D and Figure 5—source data 1). Whole brain FDR-correction also revealed positive Path b effects in the bilateral cerebellum, right S1, bilateral superior parietal lobule, and other regions (see Figure 5D, Figure 5—figure supplement 4, and Figure 5—source data 1). No negative Path b effects survived correction within the pain modulatory mask or whole brain search. We observed significant Path b effects on responses to medium heat in the bilateral striatum, as well as both signature patterns (see Figure 5D and Table 2).

Finally, we tested for voxelwise mediation of current cue effects on pain. Within regions previously implicated in studies of pain and placebo, we observed significant negative mediation by the right VMPFC/OFC, left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC), and the sgACC (see Figure 5C and Figure 5—source data 1). Whole brain FDR correction additionally identified negative mediation in the left inferior parietal lobule (IPL; see Figure 5—figure supplement 4). There were no positive mediators of cue effects in pain in voxel-wise analyses. Negative mediation is consistent with suppression; indeed, extracting responses within mediators indicated that individuals with the strongest positive Path a effects (i.e. HM >LM) in these regions showed large negative associations between activation and pain (see Figure 5C). This suppression may be consistent with down-regulation in modulatory regions. We did not observe mediation by any value-related ROI or signature pattern (see Table 2). See Figure 5—figure supplement 6 and Figure 5—source data 2 for whole-brain uncorrected results.

Notably, we did not observe significant moderation by Group in any of the paths at FDR-corrected thresholds or in any of our a priori regions of interest (i.e. correction within pain modulatory regions or whole brain, ROI-wise analyses, or pain signature patterns). This suggests that the dynamic effects of predictive cues and reversals on pain-related brain responses are similar whether individuals learn through instruction or experience, despite stronger influences of cues on subjective pain within the Instructed Group. Uncorrected results, which do point to potential group differences in Path a effects in the rostral ACC, left hippocampus, and left thalamus, are presented in Figure 5—figure supplement 7 and Figure 5—source data 2.

Responses in brainstem, orbitofrontal cortex, and right prefrontal cortex maintain initial contingencies despite reversals, particularly in uninstructed participants

While the main mediation analysis isolated brain regions whose responses to cues on medium heat trials updated upon reversal, some regions may show sustained responses to initial contingencies. We therefore conducted a second mediation analysis to identify regions that responded to original contingencies and did not reverse as contingencies changed. We were most interested in Path a, which identified regions that showed stronger activation in response to cues that were originally paired with high pain relative to cues that were originally paired with low pain (see Figure 6), while controlling for current contingencies. No regions survived FDR correction within pain modulatory regions, and there were no effects of original cues on the NPS or SIIPS (all p’s>0.2). Whole brain correction revealed significant positive Path a effects (original high cue >original low cue) in a brainstem cluster overlapping with the rostroventral medulla (RVM; consistent with the pontine reticular nucleus based on the Brainstem Navigator; Singh et al., 2021), as well as the left medial OFC (area Fo3), right lateral prefrontal cortex, right DLPFC, medial cerebellum, and right occipital cortex (see Figure 6B, Figure 6—figure supplement 1, and Figure 6—source data 1) there were no negative Path a effects. Extracting trial-level responses from Path a regions (see Figure 6B and Figure 6—figure supplements 2 and 3) indicated that effects in most regions were driven primarily by lack of reversal in the Uninstructed Group, although we did not observe significant moderation by Group in any regions at FDR-corrected thresholds (see Figure 6—figure supplement 5 and Figure 6—source data 1), and that only the RVM maintained original contingencies in both groups. ROI-wise analyses within value-related regions indicated the VMPFC was significantly modulated by initial contingencies (see Table 2) driven by greater activation in responses to the original low pain cue. Extracting timecourses suggests that these differences were driven by the Uninstructed Group (see Figure 5—figure supplement 5) and an adjacent region of VMPFC showed significant moderation by Group in uncorrected voxelwise analyses (see Figure 6—figure supplement 4 and Figure 6—source data 2), although we did not observe significant group differences in ROI-wise analyses when we included Group as a potential moderator (p>0.6), and Path a effects remained significant when controlling for Group (a=–0.09, p=0.019).

Figure 6. Original cue effects on medium heat pain.

We conducted a second mediation analysis to isolate effects of original contingencies, controlling for current contingencies. (A) Effects of original contingencies. The goal of our second mediation analysis was to specifically identify regions that continued to respond to the original contingencies across the entire task, regardless of reversals. (B) Path a: Regions that show greater activation to original high pain contingencies despite reversals. Path a identified regions that showed greater activation to the Original High Cue (dark gray) relative to the Original Low Cue (light gray) across the entire task, while controlling for current contingencies. No voxels survived correction within pain modulatory regions. However, whole brain correction revealed that a number of regions including the brainstem’s rostroventral medulla (RVM), right DLPFC, left medial OFC (mOFC), and other regions (see Figure 6—figure supplement 2 and Figure 6—source data 1) continued to show higher activation when medium heat was paired with the original high pain cue regardless of Phase. Extracting trial-by-trial responses from the RVM (top) confirmed that this region showed greater heat-evoked activation with the Original High Cue during both original and reversed contingencies and that effects were present in both the Instructed Group and the Uninstructed Group. In the mOFC, however, responses did reverse within the Instructed Group (bottom), suggesting that failure to reverse was driven by Uninstructed Group participants. Similar effects were observed in the VMPFC region of interest (See Figure 5—figure supplement 5). See Figure 6—figure supplements 2 and 3 for means within other Path a regions. (B) Associations between original contingencies and pain were statistically mediated by a cluster in the right superior frontal gyrus, in which individuals who had larger effects of original cues on brain responses (controlling for current contingencies; i.e. Path a, x-axis) also had stronger negative associations between brain activation and subjective pain (Path b, y-axis). There were no additional mediators of original cue effects on pain based on whole brain correction identified additional effects in the right DLPFC, precuneus, and cerebellum (see Figure 6—figure supplement 1 and Figure 6—source data 1). Whole brain uncorrected results are presented in Figure 6—figure supplements 4 and 5 and Figure 6—source data 2.

Figure 6—source data 1. Mediation of original cue contingencies: Small-volumes corrected results.
Figure 6—source data 2. Mediation of original cue contingencies: Uncorrected results.

Figure 6.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1. Original contingency mediation: Whole-brain FDR correction.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

Results are reported in Figure 6—source data 1.
Figure 6—figure supplement 2. Original contingency mediation: Effects of cue and phase.

Figure 6—figure supplement 2.

This figure depicts extracted responses in clusters identified in analyses of Path a of the mediation based on original contingencies, i.e. regions identified as showing higher activation in response to original high pain cues relative to original low pain cues, regardless of phase. We extracted responses and averaged as a function of Group (Instructed vs Uninstructed), Cue (Original low = ‘Orig LM’; Original high = ‘Orig HM’), Phase (Left two lighter bars = Original contingencies; Right two darker bars = reversed contingencies). This confirms that these regions maintain initial responses despite contingency reversals and that responses are similar across both groups.
Figure 6—figure supplement 3. Raw timecourses in Path A regions.

Figure 6—figure supplement 3.

Images depict average baseline-corrected trial timecourse for regions identified in FDR-Corrected Path A analyses (see Figure 6—figure supplement 1) as a function of Group and temperature. Average responses to low heat trials (green) and high heat trials (red) are depicted in the left column, and responses to medium heat trials are depicted in the center column (original contingencies) and right column (reversed contingencies) as a function of original cue type.
Figure 6—figure supplement 4. Original contingency mediation: Whole-brain uncorrected results.

Figure 6—figure supplement 4.

Results are reported in Figure 6—source data 2.
Figure 6—figure supplement 5. Original contingency mediation, including Group as a moderator: Whole-brain uncorrected results.

Figure 6—figure supplement 5.

Results are reported in Figure 6—source data 2.

Path b effects were similar to those observed when controlling for current cues; see Figure 6—figure supplement 1, Figure 6—source data 1, and Table 2. Whole brain correction revealed significant negative mediation of original cue effects on pain in the right superior temporal gyrus (see Figure 6B, Figure 6—figure supplement 1, and Figure 6—source data 1). Extracting responses from this region indicated that, similar to mediators of current cues on pain, mediation was driven by the covariance between Paths a and b, such that individuals who showed stronger original cue effects on right superior temporal gyrus responses to heat also showed stronger negative associations between activation and subjective pain (see Figure 6B and Figure 6—figure supplement 2). Additional regions identified in uncorrected voxelwise analyses are reported in Figure 6—figure supplement 3 and Figure 6—source data 2. Finally, consistent with mediation of current contingencies, Group did not moderate Path b or mediation effects even at uncorrected thresholds, indicating associations between brain activation and pain were similar regardless of instruction (see Figure 6B and Figure 6—figure supplement 4).

Quantitative models reveal that instructed participants reverse expectations upon instructions and learning is faster in uninstructed participants

We observed no group differences in the effects of cues and reversals on brain responses to noxious stimuli in pain-related regions, suggesting that pain-related responses are mediated similarly whether or not participants are instructed about contingencies. However, we did observe possible group differences in the VMPFC and other regions that maintained original contingencies in the Uninstructed Group, but not the Instructed Group, although group differences were only evident in uncorrected voxelwise analyses. Our mediation models and behavioral analyses that include effects of Phase assume that expectations and responses update completely upon reversal, either through instruction in the Instructed Group or when contingencies reverse in the Uninstructed Group. However, these models may not capture differences if dynamic learning proceeds more gradually (i.e. continuously as a function of pairings between cues and temperatures), and it is possible that groups differ in the dynamics of learning and associations between learning and brain activation, consistent with previous work (Atlas et al., 2016).

To formally examine these dynamics, we applied a quantitative model of instructed reversal learning (Atlas et al., 2016; Atlas and Phelps, 2018) which accounts for how expectations update dynamically as a function of both experience and instruction. The model includes two parameters: ɑ, a standard learning rate that captures the extent to which expected value (EV) updates in response to prediction errors, and ρ, which guides whether and how EV reverses upon instruction (see Materials and methods). Here, we extended this model to predict subjective pain on medium heat trials. This model accounted for variations in pain reports better than other plausible models, including a standard Rescorla-Wagner model without the ρ parameter and a hybrid model of adaptive learning modified to reverse upon instruction (Atlas et al., 2019).

Consistent with our task manipulation, instructed reversal parameters (i.e. ρ) varied as a function of Group (fit to individuals: t(38) = 3.013, p=0.005; see Figure 7A), such that participants in the Instructed Group showed larger reversals at the time of verbal instruction (fit to individuals: Instructed: M=0.62, SD = 0.35; Uninstructed: M=0.31, SD = 0.31). This confirms our task manipulation (instructed reversals should only be seen in the group that was exposed to instructions) and validates the model’s application to subjective pain. Consistent with our previous work on instructed threat learning (Atlas et al., 2016), learning rates (i.e. ɑ) were close to zero in the Instructed Group (M=0.065, SD = 0.22), indicating that there was little additional learning as a function of experience between instructed reversals, as might be expected given that feedback was entirely consistent with instructions. Learning rates were indeed higher in the Uninstructed Group (M=0.28, SD = 0.34), and differed significantly between groups (fit to individuals: t(38) = –2.32, p=0.026). Differences in ρ and ɑ were observed when models were fit to individuals, and when they were fit across the group using a jack-knife model fitting procedure. Jack-knife estimates revealed a significant group difference in the learning rate ɑ (t(1,38) = 33.07, p<0.001, CI = [0.69, 0.78]), driven by higher ɑ values in the Uninstructed Group (M=0.264, SE = 0.028) relative to the Instructed Group (M=0, SE = 0), and a significant group difference in the instructed reversal parameter ρ (t(1,38) = –9.41, p<0.001, CI = [–0.32, –0.21]) driven by higher ρ parameters in the Instructed Group (M=0.875, SE = 0.003), relative to the Uninstructed Group (M=0.139, SE = 0.022). Thus expected value updates primarily upon instruction in the Instructed Group with very little additional learning between reversals (consistent with the Cue x Phase interactions we modeled behaviorally and that formed the foundation of our mediation models), whereas individuals in the Uninstructed Group update expected value over time as a function of experience, that is pairings between cues and heat, as depicted in Figure 7B.

Figure 7. Instructed learning model fit to pain on medium heat trials.

Figure 7.

We fit a computational model of instructed reversal learning (Atlas et al., 2016) to pain reports on medium heat trials to isolate the dynamics of expected value and how expected value updates with instruction. (A) Group differences in learning parameters. Fitting models to individuals revealed group differences in learning rate (α, left), such that participants in the Uninstructed Group (blue) showed stronger updates of expected value in response to prediction errors relative to the Instructed Group (red), whereas the Instructed Group showed stronger reversals at the time when instructions were delivered, based on the instructed reversal parameter (ρ, right). (B) Predicted timecourse of expected value based on jack-knife model fits. We used model parameters from a jack-knife model fitting procedure (see Materials and Methods) to generate predicted timecourse of expected value (EV) for each group. Here we depict model predictions for an example participant in the Instructed Group (left) and the Uninstructed Group (right). As shown in the second row, EV reverses immediately upon instruction in the Instructed Group and reverses more gradually in the Uninstructed Group. We focused on responses fit to medium pain ratings (fourth row) and modeled associations between heat-evoked activation and the timecourse of EV and unsigned PE on medium heat trials (bottom row).

Expected value dynamically modulates responses to noxious stimulation, with differences between groups in the rostral anterior cortex

We next searched for neural correlates of dynamic expected value (EV) signals on medium heat trials. We used the learning time-course generated from fits to pain ratings in each group and searched for regions that correlated with EV. Figure 7B depicts example EV timecourses using the same parameters that were used to evaluate associations between EV and medium heat-evoked brain responses in each group. We used robust regression to evaluate associations within each group, and those that were consistent while controlling for Group, or differed significantly between Groups.

Whole brain correction revealed significant positive associations with instruction-based EV in the right MPFC in the Instructed Group (see Figure 8—figure supplement 1, and Figure 8—source data 1) and negative associations with feedback-driven EV in the left rACC in the Uninstructed Group (see Figure 8—figure supplement 1, and Figure 8—source data 1). Consistent with this, FDR correction within a priori pain modulatory regions revealed positive group differences (Instructed >Uninstructed) in the rACC, as well as the left anterior insula, left dorsal posterior insula, and the bilateral thalamus (see Figure 8A and Figure 8—source data 1). Extracting responses from these regions revealed that there were positive associations with EV in the Instructed Group, and negative associations in the Uninstructed Group. Whole brain correction additionally revealed positive differences in the DMPFC and left middle cingulate (see Figure 8A, Figure 8—figure supplements 1 and 2, and Figure 8—source data 1). ROI-wise tests within a priori value related regions indicated that groups differed in the left striatum, driven by positive associations in the Instructed Group (see Table 2 and Figure 8B). Robust regression did not identify any significant associations across all participants (i.e. main effect, controlling for group) between EV and responses to medium heat based on corrected voxelwise analyses or in regions of interest or signature patterns. However, uncorrected results indicate negative associations in the left lateral OFC, hippocampus, and other regions that have been implicated in prior studies (see Figure 8—figure supplement 3 and Figure 8—source data 2). Fitting models to heat-evoked autonomic responses revealed similar patterns of activation based on whole-brain and ROI-based correction (see Figure 8—figure supplement 4 and Figure 8—source data 3), and while group differences did not survive correction when we fit models to anticipatory SCR (see Figure 8—figure supplement 5 and Figure 8—source data 4), we observed group differences in associations with EV in overlapping portions of the VMPFC and left putamen at uncorrected thresholds in all three models (see Figure 8—figure supplement 2 and Figure 8—source data 2–4). For complete results of models fit to SCR, see Figure 8—figure supplements 45 and Figure 8—source data 3 and 4.

Figure 8. Group differences in associations with expected value and prediction error on medium heat trials.

We used the timecourse of expected value (EV) based on fitting computational models to pain reports from each group (see Figure 7) to isolate the neural correlates of instructed and uninstructed expected value (EV) and prediction error (PE) during pain processing. We examined associations between brain responses to medium heat and the timecourse of EV and unsigned PE (i.e. absolute value of PE) and used robust regression (Wager et al., 2005) to compare groups. (A) Group differences in expected value within pain modulatory regions. The left dorsal posterior insula, left thalamus, and rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC) showed positive associations with EV within the Instructed Group (red) and negative associations within the Uninstructed Group (blue). Whole brain FDR-correction additionally identified group differences in the left middle cingulate cortex (see surface map). (B) Associations with EV in value-related ROIs. Extracting contrast values within a priori value-related regions of interest (ROIs) revealed significant associations in the bilateral striatum within the Instructed Group and significant group differences in the left striatum (see Table 4). (C) Comparing instructed and feedback-driven EV within the Instructed Group. Direct comparisons of the timecourse of EV within Instructed Group participants who were exposed to both types of information revealed significant differences in the middle cingulate cortex, driven by positive associations with instruction-based EV. (D) Main effects of unsigned PE. Unsigned PE was associated with activation across groups in the right insula, striatum, and right amygdala. (E) Group differences in unsigned PE. There were stronger associations with unsigned PE in the right SII and temporal gyrus, driven by negative associations in the Instructed Group and positive associations in the Uninstructed Group. (F) Associations with PE in value-related ROIs. ROI-wise analyses revealed significant associations between unsigned PE and activation in the right striatum and bilateral amygdala. See also Figure 8—figure supplements 19 and Figure 8—source data 1 and 2. Error bars denote standard error of the mean (n = 18 per group).

Figure 8—source data 1. Associations with expected value (EV): Small-volumes corrected results.
Figure 8—source data 2. Associations with expected value (EV): Uncorrected results.
Figure 8—source data 3. Associations with expected value (EV) based on fits to heat-evoked SCR.
Figure 8—source data 4. Associations with expected value (EV) based on fits to cue-evoked SCR.
Figure 8—source data 5. Comparing instructed and feedback-driven expected value (EV) within the Instructed Group.
Figure 8—source data 6. Associations with unsigned prediction error (PE).

Figure 8.

Figure 8—figure supplement 1. Expected value: Whole-brain correction.

Figure 8—figure supplement 1.

Results are reported in Figure 8—source data 1.
Figure 8—figure supplement 2. Expected Value on medium heat trials: Moderation by group (Instructed >Uninstructed).

Figure 8—figure supplement 2.

(I) FDR-correction revealed stronger positive associations between DMPFC activation and EV in the Instructed Group than the Uninstructed Group, whether models were fit to pain ratings (left) or SCR (right) on medium heat trials. For complete results, see Figure 8—source data 1. (II) Cluster color depicts overlaps between the three computational models based on uncorrected results. For complete results, see Figure 8—figure supplements 35 and accompanying Source Data.
Figure 8—figure supplement 3. Expected value based on fit to pain ratings: Whole-brain uncorrected results.

Figure 8—figure supplement 3.

Results are reported in Figure 8—source data 2.
Figure 8—figure supplement 4. Expected value based on fit to heat-evoked SCR: Whole-brain uncorrected results.

Figure 8—figure supplement 4.

Results are reported in Figure 8—source data 3.
Figure 8—figure supplement 5. Expected value based on fit to anticipatory SCR: Whole-brain uncorrected results.

Figure 8—figure supplement 5.

Results are reported in Figure 8—source data 4.
Figure 8—figure supplement 6. Instructed vs Feedback driven Expected Value within Instructed Group: Whole-brain corrected results.

Figure 8—figure supplement 6.

Associations with Uninstructed EV did not survive whole-brain FDR correction. See Figure 7 for uncorrected results. Results are reported in Figure 8—source data 5.
Figure 8—figure supplement 7. Instructed vs feedback driven expected value within instructed group: whole-brain uncorrected results.

Figure 8—figure supplement 7.

Results are reported in Figure 8—source data 5.
Figure 8—figure supplement 8. Absolute value of prediction error on medium trials: whole-brain corrected results.

Figure 8—figure supplement 8.

Results are reported in Figure 8—source data 6.
Figure 8—figure supplement 9. Absolute value of prediction error on medium trials: whole-brain uncorrected results.

Figure 8—figure supplement 9.

Results are reported in Figure 8—source data 6.

We searched for correlates of instructed and feedback-driven EV signals within Instructed Group participants to test whether brain responses were preferentially related to instructed or feedback-driven learning within participants exposed to both types of information. Controlling for uninstructed EV, instructed EV was positively associated with activation near the left nucleus accumbens based on correction within pain-related regions (see Figure 8—source data 5), and ROI-wise analyses revealed significant associations with instructed EV bilaterally in the striatum (see Table 2). Whole brain correction additionally identified positive associations in the left anterior insula, left rACC, and right DMPFC (see Figure 8—figure supplement 6 and Figure 8—source data 5). There were no regions that were preferentially associated with uninstructed EV within the Instructed Group in corrected voxelwise search or ROI-wise analyses. Finally, whole brain correction revealed significant differences between instructed and uninstructed EV in the bilateral middle cingulate (see Figure 8C, Figure 8—figure supplement 6 and Figure 8—source data 5). Uncorrected voxelwise results are reported in Figure 8—figure supplement 7 and Figure 8—source data 5.

Associations with unsigned prediction error differ between groups

In addition to analyses of EV, we evaluated associations between brain responses to medium heat and unsigned prediction errors (PEs). No regions showed significant associations within pain related ROIs; however, whole brain correction revealed a positive association with PE, controlling for group, in a wide swath of contiguous activation encompassing the right anterior insula, right striatum, and right amygdala (see Figure 8D). Whole brain correction also revealed a significant difference between groups in a large contiguous cluster that included the right SII, right superior temporal gyrus, and right temporo-parietal cortex (see Figure 8E) driven by negative associations with PE in the Instructed Group and positive associations in the Uninstructed Group (see Figure 8—figure supplement 8 and Figure 8—source data 6). Within value-related ROIs, unsigned PEs were positively associated with responses to heat in the right striatum and the bilateral amygdala (see Figure 8F and Table 2). Associations with right striatum were observed within each group, whereas associations with amygdala were only observed in the Instructed Group; however, group differences were not significant in any region. Finally, there was no association between PE and NPS or SIIPS expression in either group (see Table 2). Voxelwise uncorrected results are reported in Figure 8—figure supplement 9 and Figure 8—source data 6.

Post-task ratings

We used ANOVAs to evaluate effects of Group and Cue on post-task affect ratings, and to measure effects of Group, Cue, and Phase for retrospective expectancy ratings (i.e. retrospective ratings of expected pain at the beginning and end of the task as a function of Cue). There were no differences in reported affect as a function of Group or Cue (all p’s>0.09; see Figure 4—figure supplement 2). We observed a significant Group x Phase interaction on retrospective expectancy (F(1,36) = 4.386, p=0.043) and a marginal Group x Cue x Phase interaction (F(1,36) = 3.635, p=0.065). Post-hoc pairwise comparisons indicated that the Instructed Group reported differences in expected pain as a function of Cue at the beginning of the task (padjusted = 0.041), but not the end of the task (p>0.9), whereas the Uninstructed Group did not report significant differences at any point (all p’s>0.1).

Discussion

We measured whether cue-based expectancy effects on pain and brain responses to noxious heat update dynamically as contingencies change, and whether these relationships vary as a function of whether individuals learn through instruction or experience. All participants demonstrated robust cue-based expectancy effects on pain, consistent with previous work from our group and others (Colloca et al., 2008a; Atlas et al., 2010; Wiech et al., 2014; Fazeli and Büchel, 2018; Jepma et al., 2018; Michalska et al., 2018; Koban et al., 2019; Abend et al., 2021). Here, we provide new evidence that predictive cues shape pain, autonomic responses, and heat-evoked brain activation even when contingencies change repeatedly, whether reversals are accompanied by instructions or learned through experience. Reinforcement learning models indicated that these effects emerge dynamically, consistent with error-driven learning. We observed dissociations in the associations between expected value and brain responses to heat in several brain regions, including the rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC), which was positively associated with expected value in the Instructed Group and negatively associated in the Uninstructed Group. Finally, cue effects on several pain-related regions including the bilateral anterior insula and dorsal anterior cingulate updated dynamically as contingencies changed regardless of group, whereas the RVM responded to original contingencies throughout the task. Dynamic cue effects on pain were mediated by modulatory frontal regions implicated in placebo analgesia, including the VMPFC/OFC and DLPFC. Here we discuss these findings and their implications for future work and our understanding of pain, predictive processing, and the interaction between learning and instructed knowledge.

Our study presents a novel examination of expectancy-based pain modulation during reversal learning. Cue effects on pain reports, skin conductance, and responses to heat in several pain-related regions, including the bilateral anterior insula, dACC, and sgACC, reversed as contingencies changed, whether participants were instructed about contingencies or learned purely through experience. Thus pain, arousal, and brain responses to noxious stimulation are highly sensitive to changing contingencies. However, we did not observe significant cue-based modulation of well-validated neural signature patterns that predict pain (Wager et al., 2013; Woo et al., 2017), although both patterns predicted pain when controlling for cue. This suggests that cue-based predictions may alter dynamic learning and update decision-making without changing underlying nociception, consistent with previous findings in placebo analgesia (Zunhammer et al., 2018). The brain mediators of dynamic cue effects on pain, DLPFC, VMPFC, DMPFC and sgACC, further link our findings with placebo analgesia. Meta-analysis indicates that these regions all play a modulatory role in placebo analgesia, with activation being inversely related to pain (Atlas and Wager, 2014b). The negative mediation (i.e. suppression) we observed is consistent with this modulation or down-regulation: Individuals who showed greater activation of these regions in response to high pain cues showed stronger negative associations between brain activation and subjective pain, controlling for cue. Identifying factors that predict individual differences in brain mediators is an important direction for future research, as is testing the causal contributions of these modulatory regions to placebo analgesia and expectancy-based pain modulation through interventions such as transcranial magnetic stimulation (Krummenacher et al., 2010) or in individuals with neurosurgical lesions (Motzkin et al., 2021).

While most regions that have been implicated in prior studies of placebo and expectancy-based pain modulation updated dynamically as contingencies changed, we observed sustained responses to initial contingencies in a portion of the brainstem consistent with the rostroventral medulla (RVM). RVM responses to medium heat stimuli were elevated in response to the original high pain cue relative to the original low pain cue and remained elevated throughout the task in both groups. Together with the periaqueductal gray (PAG), the RVM is a key component of the descending pain modulatory system (Fields, 2004; Fields, 2006), which regulates the release of endogenous opioids that can block ascending pain signals, a process thought to be critical for expectancy-based placebo analgesia (Levine et al., 1978; Ter Riet et al., 1998; Amanzio and Benedetti, 1999). Previous neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that placebo analgesia engages the RVM (Eippert et al., 2009; Crawford et al., 2021; Yanes and Akintola, 2022) and that placebo effects on the RVM are blocked by the opioid antagonist naloxone (Eippert et al., 2009). Preclinical work demonstrates that endogenous opioid release engages long lasting pain modulation (Watkins and Mayer, 1982a; Watkins and Mayer, 1982b). We have hypothesized that placebo analgesia may engage endogenous opioids to lead to long lasting pain modulation, whereas pain-predictive cues may modulate pain dynamically through dopaminergic prediction errors (Atlas and Wager, 2012; Atlas, 2021). The fact that RVM responds to initial contingencies throughout the task might be consistent with such long-lasting effects, and may explain why instructions reverse placebo analgesia only after brief conditioning (Schafer et al., 2015). Interestingly, we did not observe cue-based modulation of the PAG in our analyses; however, an initial processing pipeline that used affine normalization rather than nonlinear warping (Atlas et al., 2021) indicated that the PAG also responded to initial contingencies throughout the task, similar to the RVM. Thus both opioidergic regions may be associated with long-lasting effects that do not update as contingencies change. Future studies should combine pain-predictive cues with opioid and dopamine antagonists or employ positron emission tomography to understand the contribution of endogenous opioids and dopamine to different forms of pain modulation.

Cue effects on pain-related brain responses did not differ between groups, i.e. as a function of whether individuals were instructed about contingencies, indicating that responses to contingency reversals and links with subjective pain are similar regardless of whether individuals learn through experience or instruction. Thus once an expectation or prediction is generated, it has similar effects on downstream responses regardless of how contingencies were established. Based on these findings, one might assume that expectancy-based pain modulation and predictive coding operate similarly whether predictions are generated via learning or instruction. However, quantitative learning models revealed differences between groups in how responses updated dynamically from trial to trial, which were in turn associated with differences in brain activation. Individuals who were exposed to instructed reversals updated pain immediately upon instruction without additional learning from intermittent reinforcement, whereas individuals who learned purely from experience had higher learning rates, meaning they updated expectations as a function of pairings between predictive cues and heat outcomes. We observed similar differences when we fit models to heat-evoked skin conductance on medium trials in exploratory analyses. This is consistent with prior work focusing on autonomic arousal during threat learning (Atlas et al., 2016) and confirmation bias in reward-related decision making (Doll et al., 2009). Isolating the different timecourses of expected value and prediction error then allowed us to identify differences between groups in the brain circuitry associated with these computations.

Several regions, including the rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC), showed differential associations with expected value (EV) depending on group. RACC activation was positively associated with EV in the Instructed Group (i.e. greater activation with high pain expectancy), and negatively associated with EV in the Uninstructed Group (i.e. greater activation with low pain expectancy). The rACC also showed preferential associations with instruction-based EV when we compared associations between instruction-based and experience-based EV within the Instructed Group. The rACC has been implicated in numerous studies of placebo analgesia and expectancy-based pain modulation (Petrovic et al., 2002; Bingel et al., 2006; Eippert et al., 2009; Geuter et al., 2013), and is a key component of the opioidergic endogenous pain modulation circuit (Zubieta et al., 2005; Wager et al., 2007; Navratilova et al., 2015), along with the PAG and the RVM. However, in contrast to the RVM, which showed stable responses over time regardless of instruction, the rACC showed different dynamics and different patterns of responses to threat during reversal learning depending on whether individuals were exposed to contingency instructions. This may be consistent with other studies that implicate the rACC as a key region that determines whether or not placebos engage descending modulation through connectivity with the PAG (Bingel et al., 2006; Eippert et al., 2009); in other words, rACC might act as a hub to flexibly engage downregulation depending on contextual factors. Future studies should examine the relationship between these regions and whether connectivity differs in more dynamic environments or as a function of instruction. We observed similar differences in value coding in the left dorsal posterior insula, a region thought to be specific for pain (Segerdahl et al., 2015), and in portions of the bilateral thalamus that connect with the prefrontal cortex and the temporal lobe (Behrens et al., 2003; Johansen-Berg et al., 2005).

In addition to regions implicated in prior studies of placebo and pain modulation, we evaluated the contribution of systems involved in value-based learning, namely the striatum, OFC/VMPFC, and the amygdala. We previously observed dissociations in these regions during aversive learning (Atlas et al., 2016), such that responses in the striatum and OFC/VMPFC tracked EV that updated with instructions, whereas the right amygdala was associated with EV based purely on experiential learning. The present study builds on this work by focusing on heat-evoked responses (i.e. the US in a fear conditioning model), rather than anticipation (i.e. responses to the CS), which allows us to test whether error-based learning shapes responses to noxious stimuli and whether this learning is modulated by instructions. ROI-based analyses indicated that the bilateral striatum was associated with EV that updated based on instruction in the Instructed Group, consistent with our previous findings (Atlas et al., 2016). VMPFC/OFC associations with value were also sensitive to instructions, although we observed different patterns across the broad region and did not observe consistent findings in ROI-based or corrected analyses. For instance, uncorrected voxelwise analyses revealed that the association between EV and VMPFC activation differed between groups (positive associations with EV in the Instructed Group and negative associations with EV in the Uninstructed Group), regardless of whether models were fit to pain, heat-evoked SCR, or anticipatory SCR. In contrast, we observed negative associations with EV in the left lateral OFC in both groups. Both portions of the VMPFC/OFC have been implicated in previous studies of placebo analgesia (Lieberman et al., 2004; Wager et al., 2004; Petrovic et al., 2010; Ellingsen et al., 2013), and we previously showed that pain-predictive cues modulate responses in both regions, with greater activation when medium heat is paired with low pain cues than high pain cue (Atlas et al., 2010). Lateral OFC is thought to evaluate value of independent options, whereas medial OFC and VMPFC have been linked to value-based choice for the purpose of comparison (Rudebeck and Murray, 2014). We recently showed that surgical lesions of the bilateral VMPFC enhance instructed cue effects on pain in a stable environment (Motzkin et al., 2021). Additional studies that use lesion symptom mapping in heterogeneous frontal samples or measure aversive learning in nonhuman primates can provide further insights on the contribution of different portions of the VMPFC/OFC to pain modulation and instructed learning. Together, these findings build on previous work indicating that the striatum and VMPFC update upon instruction in both appetitive and aversive learning (Li et al., 2011a; Atlas et al., 2016) and that OFC value signals are sensitive to higher order knowledge across species (Wilson et al., 2014; Lucantonio et al., 2015; Schuck et al., 2018).

Because we focused on responses to the US rather than the CS here, we also were able to extend previous work by examining the role of unsigned prediction errors (PEs), which are linked to associability, which gates attention and learning dynamically. Associability has been linked to the amygdala in previous studies of pain and aversive learning (Li et al., 2011a; Zhang et al., 2016; Atlas et al., 2019), meta-analyses indicate that unsigned PEs are associated with activation in the dorsal and ventral striatum, as well as other regions (Corlett et al., 2022), and one study of aversive learning showed responses to both associability and unsigned PE in the amygdala (Boll et al., 2013). Consistent with these studies, we observed associations between unsigned PE and activation in the bilateral amygdala and the right striatum in both groups in ROI-based analyses. Whole brain analyses revealed associations between unsigned PEs and activation in the right hemisphere encompassing the insula, striatum, and amygdala, and that the associations between PE and activation in the right SII and temporal cortex differed across groups (negative associations in the Instructed Group and positive in the Uninstructed Group). Thus, although cue effects on subjective pain were mediated similarly across groups, we still observe unique responses to PE that differed between groups.

We note that the amygdala’s association with PE within the Instructed Group indicates that instructions do have some impact on amygdala activation, seemingly in contrast to our previous work on instructed learning during fear conditioning (Atlas et al., 2016; Atlas, 2019). However, because error-driven learning was nearly absent between instructed reversals in the Instructed Group, the PE regressor in this group captures a constant effect that decreases linearly over time, consistent with prior evidence that amygdala responses habituate during aversive learning (Büchel et al., 1998). Thus we did not observe evidence of amygdala sensitivity to instructions per se in the present study. The absence of pure experiential learning signals in the right amygdala may reflect the present study’s focus on BOLD responses to noxious heat (i.e. unconditioned stimuli), in contrast to our previous study (Atlas et al., 2016) which focused on responses to unreinforced cues (i.e. conditioned stimuli). In addition, our experimental design was optimized for analyses of the heat period rather than the cue (i.e. we used a fixed anticipation period to avoid variations in dread that might accompany jittered anticipatory intervals Berns et al., 2006), so we were unable to separately analyze responses to pain-predictive cues to determine whether the amygdala showed similar responses in the present study. We note that raw timecourses suggest that cue-based differences in the amygdala may emerge in response to predictive cues, as well as during the heat period (see Figure 5—figure supplement 5). This contrasts with the bilateral striatum and nearly all regions identified in mediation analyses, in which cue-based differences do not emerge until the heat period. Future work should incorporate longer delays between cues and aversive outcomes or utilize intermittent reinforcement (e.g. catch trials without heat presentation) to separately analyze responses to cues and noxious heat in the amgydala, VMPFC, and other key regions.

These results highlight that pain-related outcomes (e.g. subjective ratings, physiological arousal, learning systems, sensory circuits) may show distinct patterns of learning and sensitivity to higher order factors. Studies of expectancy-based pain modulation and predictive coding in other domains should measure parallel behavioral outcomes to capture the complexity of perceptual decision-making, rather than focusing on a single outcome measure (e.g. choices during instrumental learning).

Future directions and outstanding questions

This work highlights several promising avenues of inquiry that should be addressed in future work, in addition to those highlighted above. Comparisons between appetitive and aversive learning would reveal whether the differences observed here are driven by threat-specific processes or general differences in adaptive learning and flexibility. One limitation of our design is that the two groups received slightly different instructions at the start of the experiment: The Instructed Group was informed about cue-heat contingencies, whereas the Uninstructed Group was instructed to pay attention and try to figure out the relationships between cues and heat outcomes. Although these differences did not impact brain mediators of expectancy or pain, the latter instruction would be more likely to engage inference and model-based learning (Doll et al., 2012; Doll et al., 2015; Dayan and Berridge, 2014). These differences should be resolved in future work by directly comparing instructed and model-based learning. In addition, future studies should include a group that undergoes initial learning in the absence of instructions and then receives instructed reversals to determine whether learned associations can be reversed on the basis of higher order knowledge, consistent with dissociations observed in studies of placebo (Benedetti et al., 2003; Schafer et al., 2015). Alternatively, future studies can include outcomes that diverge from instructions to examine the interplay between instructed and experiential learning (e.g. Doll et al., 2009). In addition, future work should manipulate not only instructions and learning but also the uncertainty of the expectation to understand how instructions, learning, uncertainty, and precision drive perception from a predictive coding framework. Prior work on placebo analgesia indicates that the precision of the expectation is linked to placebo responses in the PAG and RVM (Grahl et al., 2018); it is unknown how precision and uncertainty modulated responses in the present study.

We chose to focus on within-subjects effects and did not examine how these differences vary across individuals as a function of factors such as anxiety, which has previously been shown to impact adaptive learning (Browning et al., 2015). However, we recently examined how instructed reversals impact pain expectations in youth with clinical anxiety and found that youth with and without anxiety showed similar responses to expectancy and instruction, although youth with anxiety showed greater autonomic arousal during pain anticipation (Abend et al., 2021). In exploratory analyses, we measured the association between anticipatory responses to the cues themselves and brain responses to noxious heat, however we did not observe associations between anticipatory arousal and responses within a priori networks. We also found that quantitative models fit to heat-evoked and anticipatory SCR were highly similar to models fit to subjective pain. However, as the number of participants with useable skin conductance or pupil dilation data was extremely limited due to technical malfunctions and variations in arousal, we consider these findings exploratory and do not make strong claims based on these results. Future studies should continue to compare the effects of learning, instructions, and expectations on subjective pain with effects on physiological outcome measures to determine whether autonomic responses to noxious stimuli are shaped by pain decision-making (Mischkowski et al., 2019) or whether physiological responses are shaped through independent pathways, e.g. through a dual process model (Ohman and Soares, 1993; Mineka and Ohman, 2002).

Conclusion

Together, these findings reveal that instructions and learning lead to both interactive and dissociable processes even within individuals. We view these findings in light of theories on the relationship between conditioning and expectancy (Rescorla, 1988; Kirsch, 1997; Kirsch et al., 2004) and long-standing debates about whether placebo effects depend on conditioning or expectancy. We suggest that considering the brain mechanisms that mediate dynamic expectancy-based pain modulation shines new light on these distinctions. The human brain contains parallel pain modulatory circuits that (i) update as contingencies change (e.g. insula), (ii) continue to respond to initial contingencies regardless of whether they were learned through instruction or experience (e.g. RVM), or (iii) respond to experiential learning differentially as a function of whether or not individuals were exposed to instructions (e.g. rACC). These findings indicate that we gain new insights on clinically relevant outcomes from measuring how instructions and learning interact to shape outcomes, rather than assuming that circuits and processes are sensitive to either expectancy or conditioning. Understanding these processes in clinical populations may shed light directly on the mechanisms of therapeutic interventions, for example the interplay between instructed and exposure-based interventions in cognitive behavioral therapy for chronic pain and affective disorders.

Materials and methods

Participants

Forty-nine participants (25 female, Mage = 28.04 years, SDage = 7.04) were recruited and consented to participate in an fMRI study designed to measure ‘how pain and emotions are processed in the human brain and influenced by psychological factors’. Participants provided informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and the protocol was approved by the NIH’s Combined Neuroscience Institutional Review Board (Protocol 15-AT-0132, PI: Atlas). Participants were eligible to participate if they were between 18 and 50, fluent in English, healthy (i.e. had no medical conditions that affect pain or somatosensation, no psychiatric, neurological, autonomic, or cardiovascular disorders, no chronic systemic diseases, and no medication that can affect pain perception), right-handed, and had received a medical exam at NIH within the previous year. All participants underwent urine toxicology testing to ensure they had not used recreational drugs that alter pain. Participants were drawn from a pool of subjects who had completed an initial screening visit that tested whether participants reliably reported increased pain with increased temperatures (r2 >0.4) and exhibited pain tolerance at or below 50 °C (the maximum temperature we applied during the study). Nine participants who provided consent did not complete the experiment due to ineligibility based on calibration (n=4) technical failures (n=1), compliance with procedures (n=2), or anatomical abnormalities identified in a clinical scan (n=2) and were not included in the current analyses. The final sample included 40 participants (22 female; Mage = 27.00 years, SDage = 6.21). As detailed in our clinical protocol (15-AT-0132; https://clinicaltrials.gov/ identifier NCT02446262), sample size was based on power analyses from our previous studies and on a behavioral pilot experiment conducted prior to the fMRI study. We computed the effect size of cue-based differences in reported pain in a sample of 12 participants (6 per group) and determined that we need a minimum of 5–9 participants to achieve 80–95% power to detect cue-based differences in pain, including reversals (data available upon request). We therefore included 20 participants per group in the fMRI experiment. Two participants in the Uninstructed Group and two participants in the Instructed Group were excluded from fMRI analyses due to excessive head motion or technical issues during the scan (see Procedure), leaving a final sample of 36 participants for fMRI analyses (18 Uninstructed Group, 18 Instructed Group). All 40 participants were included in behavioral analyses.

Materials and procedure

Stimuli and apparatus

We delivered thermal stimulation to the left (non-dominant) volar forearm using a 16x16 ATS contact heat thermode controlled with a Pathway pain and sensory evaluation system (Medoc Ltd, Ramat Yisha, Israel). Each heat stimulus lasted 8 s and consisted of three phases: a 1.5 s on-ramp phase in which the temperature of the thermode rose from 32 °C to the target temperature level, a 5 s plateau phase in which target temperature was maintained, and a 1.5 s off-ramp phase in which the temperature returned to 32 °C. Thermode placement was adjusted between each block of trials (i.e. every 12 trials) to avoid sensitization, habituation, and skin damage. Temperatures ranged from 36°C to 50°C, in increments of 0.5 °C, and were selected based on a thermal pain calibration conducted immediately prior to the experiment. Thermode temperature was maintained at 32 °C between trials.

Experiment Builder (SR-Research, Ontario, Canada) was used to deliver visual and auditory stimuli, to trigger noxious stimulation on the Pathways computer, and to synchronize task timing with physiological recording. Physiological data, including electrodermal activity (EDA), respiration, electrocardiography, and peripheral pulse, were recorded from the left hand using Biopac recording equipment and accompanying AcqKnowledge software (Goleta, CA) at a sample rate of 500 Hz. Participants recorded pain ratings using a trackball with their right hand while EDA was recorded from the left hand and heat was applied to the left arm. EDA was collected using Biopac’s EDA100C-MRI module (Biopac Systems, Inc, Goleta, CA). For each participant, two pre-gelled EDA electrodes (EL509; Biopac Systems, Inc, Goleta, CA) were prepared and applied to the hypothenar muscles of the left hand. Pupillometry and gaze position was recorded with Eyelink 1000-Plus (SR-Research, Ontario, Canada).

Participants also completed questionnaires prior to the experiment, including the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI Form X Gaudry et al., 1975), the Positive and Negative Affect Scale (Watson et al., 1988), and Behavioral Inhibition/Activation Scale (Carver and White, 1994). For the present manuscript, we focused on pain reports and brain responses evoked by painful heat.

Procedure

Participants underwent an adaptive staircase pain calibration prior to the experimental task (see Figure 1A). The adaptive staircase calibration procedure has been described in depth in previous work (Atlas et al., 2010; Atlas et al., 2012; Mischkowski et al., 2019; Dildine et al., 2020; Amir et al., 2021). Participants experience temperatures across eight skin sites on the left volar forearm and provide ratings on a 10-point scale, where 1 denotes warmth, 2 denotes pain threshold, 5 denotes moderate pain, 8 denotes maximum tolerable pain, and 10 denotes the most pain imaginable. We use iterative regression to isolate each participant’s threshold, tolerance, and the strength of the correlation between temperature and pain (i.e. r2). All participants had previously completed the task outside of the MRI facility to establish initial eligibility, and completed the task again on the day of the fMRI scan (Amir et al., 2021). Participants were eligible to continue if they reported reliable increases in pain as a function of temperature (r2 >0.4) and reported maximum pain tolerance at 50 degrees or less. Four participants were deemed ineligible based on calibration on the day of the study. The calibration procedure also allowed us to identify four skin sites per individual that responded most similarly across temperatures (i.e. lowest average residuals based on overall temperature-pain regression) and to individually calibrate temperatures associated with ratings of low pain (2 on 10-point scale), medium pain (5 on 10-point scale), and high pain (maximum tolerable pain; 8 on 10-point scale). These temperatures and skin sites were used during the main experiment, as described below.

Following the pain calibration, each eligible participant was positioned in the fMRI scanner. They were then randomized (n=20 per group) to either the Instructed Group (12 female; Mage = 27.05 years, SDage = 6.64) or the Uninstructed Group (10 female; Mage = 26.95 years, SDage = 5.91) and given instructions about the experiment (see Figure 1A). Both groups were told “In this task you will hear two sounds, followed by heat from the thermode. The sound will last a few seconds and will be followed by a short variable delay period, and then heat from the thermode. You do not need to respond when you hear the cue.” Instructed Group participants were then told, “It is there just to let you know what level of heat will be next.” They then heard both cues and were informed which cue would be followed by high heat and which would be followed by low heat stimulation. Uninstructed Group participants were instead told “Your job is to pay attention to the cues and try to figure out the relationship between the sounds that you hear and the heat that you feel.” They then heard both cues but were simply told that one was the first cue and the other was the second cue. We used two auditory cues (a cymbal and an accordion), which were counterbalanced across participants. Following instructions and between each block of the task, participants provided expectancy ratings in response to each cue and an experimenter moved the thermode to the next inner arm location.

During the experiment, each trial began with a 2 s auditory cue, followed by a 6 s anticipation interval, and then heat from the thermode (see Figure 1B). After an 8–10 s temporal jitter, participants provided pain ratings using an 8-point visual analogue scale. They were instructed to use the same anchors as the adaptive staircase calibration (i.e. 1=non-painful warmth, 2=pain threshold, 8=maximum tolerable pain) and we included boxes at 0 and above 8 to denote ‘no sensation’ and ‘too hot’, respectively (see Figure 1B). Participants viewed the scale for 3 s then had 5 s to record their ratings. There was a 4–6 s temporal jitter before the next cue was presented. The two jitters always combined to 14 s within a single trial, for a total trial duration of 48 s. There were 7 blocks of trials with 12 trials per block (i.e. 84 trials total). We used two trial orders (counterbalanced across participants within Group) that each included (1) a brief conditioning phase, (2) a test of cue-based expectancy effects, and (3) three contingency reversals (Figure 1C and D). During the conditioning phase, Original Low cues were followed by stimulation calibrated to elicit ratings of low pain and Original High cues were followed by stimulation calibrated to elicit high pain (see Figure 1C and D). The conditioning phase included 3 Original High cue +high heat pairings and 2–3 Original Low cue +low heat pairings (i.e. 5–6 trials; see Figure 1D). Following conditioning, each cue was paired intermittently with stimulation calibrated to elicit ratings of medium pain. This provides a test of cue-based expectancy effects, consistent with our previous work (Atlas et al., 2010; Johnston et al., 2012; Michalska et al., 2018; Abend et al., 2021). Intermittent reinforcement continued at a 50% reinforcement rate until contingencies reversed.

Halfway through runs 2, 4, and 6, the screen displayed instructions to the Instructed Group indicating that contingencies had reversed (“The relationship between the cues and heat will now reverse”; see Figure 1C). Immediately following instructions, participants experienced at least one medium heat trial paired with each cue, which provides an immediate test of instructed reversals in the Instructed Group (Atlas et al., 2016; Atlas and Phelps, 2018; Abend et al., 2021). Following the medium heat trials, the new contingencies were reinforced (i.e. the high pain temperature was paired with the previous low pain cue and the low pain temperature was paired with the previous high pain cue), leading to an experience-based reversal. Medium trials were then delivered, and learning continued with the same reinforcement rates until the next reversal (see Figure 1D). Uninstructed Group participants experienced the same trials, but a fixation cross was displayed instead of instructions.

We used two pseudorandom trial orders with three contingency reversals, which were counterbalanced across participants. Each trial order ensured that no condition was repeated three times in a row. When we visualized responses as a function of trial order, we noticed that one trial order presented the same cue-heat condition as the first trial on 6 out of the 7 blocks (i.e. medium heat paired with the original high pain cue). Because the first trial of each block is applied to a new skin site, the novel stimulus was rated as much higher than all other trials. We therefore omitted the first trial from all analyses in the main manuscript, because this novelty response contaminated the otherwise strong reversal behavior. Furthermore, due to a programming error, Instructed Group participants in one of the two trial orders received two incompatible trials following the third instructed reversal (i.e. they received one low stimulus with the previous low cue and one high stimulus with the previous high cue). Because this experience contradicted instructions and we are interested in measuring the effects of veridical instructions on reversal learning, we only analyzed the trials prior to these instructions (i.e. two reversals instead of three) in these participants (n=10).

Following the fMRI scan, participants rated affect associated with each cue and provided retrospective expectancy ratings to report how much pain they expected in response to each cue at the beginning and the end of the task.

BOLD FMRI data acquisition and preprocessing

BOLD fMRI data were collected on a 3T Siemens Skyra scanner at the NIH’s MRI Research Facility / Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Facility. After positioning the participant in the scanner bore, we collected a localizer followed by a T1-MPRAGE collected in the sagittal plane (256 slices). We collected 7 runs of multi-echo data with a 2.5 s TR and 3 mm isotropic voxels (191 volumes collected anterior to posterior; flip angle = 90°; acquisition matrix = 70 x 0 x 0x64; 1st echo = 11ms; 2nd echo = 22.72ms; 3rd echo 34.44ms).

Multi-echo data were preprocessed and combined using the ‘afni_proc.py’ program in Analysis of Functional Images (AFNI; Cox, 1996). We used ‘@SSwarper’ to nonlinearly align the anatomical scans to the MNI152_T1 template (MNI152_2009_template_SSW.nii.gz in AFNI). These nonlinear transformations were passed to afni_proc.py for general preprocessing and quality control. We removed the first 4 TRs of functional data to reach magnetization steady state, leaving a total of 187 TRs of fMRI data per run during subsequent processing and analysis steps. We performed slice time correction using AFNI’s 3dTshift program then performed motion correction by aligning echo 2 volumes to a low-motion minimum outlier base image (MIN_OUTLIER, estimated by ‘3dToutcount’) and warped to align with the anatomical volume and with the template. All volumetric transformations, both linear and non-linear, were combined into a single transformation to avoid repeated resampling of the EPI volumes. Motion correction estimates were computed based on echo 2 volumes using 6 parameters (3 translations, 3 rotations) and we applied the same corrections to each echo. We made sure there was valid data for each echo and each TR prior to combining multi-echo data. We combined across the three echoes using AFNI’s @compute_OC_weights function to generate a weighted combination of the three echoes. These ‘optimally combined’ data were used for subsequent analyses. Preliminary analyses indicated that combining across the echoes with optimal combination led to better heat-related activation in pain-related regions than analyses of a single echo or using other approaches to combine multi-echo data (e.g. TE-dependent analysis Kundu et al., 2012; Lombardo et al., 2016). In future analyses, we may formally compare optimal combination with other approaches for echo combination and denoising. Following optimal combination, data were normalized to percent signal change and smoothed using a 4 mm full-width half max smoothing kernel. Data were then analyzed using single trial estimates in MATLAB (The Mathworks, Inc, Natick, MA), as described below. Four participants were excluded from fMRI analyses due to technical issues with the fMRI scanner during data collection (n=2) or excessive head motion (motion >2 mm; n=2), leaving a final sample of 36 participants for fMRI analyses (18 in Instructed Group).

Psychophysiological data processing

Skin conductance data was preprocessed in AcqKnowledge (Biopac Systems, Inc, Goleta, CA). During preprocessing, data were smoothed (1000-sample Gaussian smoothing kernel) and filtered (25 Hz FIR low-pass filter) in AcqKnowledge, then imported into MATLAB and downsampled to 250 Hz. Data were then analyzed using Ledalab’s continuous decomposition analysis (CDA), which accounts for phasic and tonic signals and can capture multiple responses during the 8 s heat period (Benedek and Kaernbach, 2010). We analyzed both (a) cue-evoked anticipatory responses that occurred within 4 s following cue presentation; and (b) responses that occurred between heat onset and 4 s after heat offset. We used the average phasic driver (‘SCR’) in trial-wise analyses. Participants were included in analyses if they had >4 trials with measurable SCR. Thirty-six participants were included in analyses across temperatures (18 in Instructed Group); 30 participants were included in analyses of responses to medium heat (13 in Instructed Group); and 29 participants were included in analyses of anticipatory responses (15 in Instructed Group).

Pupillometry data were processed in MATLAB using both publicly available software and custom code. Data were imported to MATLAB and blinks were interpolated using the ‘GazeVisToolbox’ (available at https://github.com/djangraw/GazeVisToolbox, copy archived at swh:1:rev:afbb6cb89e6fc494e0a578d44461af085fa5d46b; Jangraw et al., 2014). Consistent with our previous work (Mischkowski et al., 2019), we interpolated from 100ms prior to each blink to 100ms following each blink to avoid extreme values surrounding each blink. Data were aligned to event markers and we visualized individual trials to exclude trials that were contaminated by artifacts. Subjects with fewer than seven useable trials (n=20; 9 in Instructed Group) were excluded from analyses of pupillary data. Useable subjects had a mean of 42.9 useable trials (M=54.4% of trials). 1000 Hz data was downsampled to 10 Hz and then we computed baseline-corrected mean pupil dilation and area-under the curve as measures of pupillary response during the heat period.

Statistical analysis of expectations, pain, autonomic responses, and heat-evoked neural signature pattern expression

We used the statistical software R (R Development Core Team, 1996) to analyze effects of our experimental manipulations on expectancy ratings, pain reports, physiological arousal, and heat-evoked brain signature pattern expression (see below, “Brain-based classifier analyses”). We measured effects across the entire task, as well as before the first reversal. We used two-way mixed ANOVAs implemented through the R function ‘anova_test’ from the package “rstatix” (Kassambara, 2021) to analyze the effects of Group and Cue on expectancy ratings prior to the task and after conditioning, as well as post-task ratings. All other analyses were conducted using multilevel linear mixed effects models in R.

In each analysis, we modeled within-subjects effects of Cue (original high pain cue / CS+ versus original low pain cue / CS-), Phase (original versus reversed contingencies), and Cue x Phase interactions (i.e. current high pain cue / CS+ versus current low pain cue / CS-) at the first level, and Group was modeled at the second (i.e. between-subjects) level. Consistent with our previous work on aversive reversal learning (Atlas et al., 2016; Atlas and Phelps, 2018), reversals (i.e. Phase effects) were coded relative to instructed reversals in the Instructed Group, and relative to experienced reversals in the Uninstructed Group (i.e. the first time the previous high pain cue was paired with low heat, or vice versa; see Figure 1C). Analyses across temperatures also included a first-level factor for Heat Intensity, and analyses during acquisition omitted the effect of Phase. We also included an effect of Time (linear effect of trial) in model comparisons to evaluate whether cue effects varied over time, although Bayesian model comparison indicated that including Time did not improve any models.

We evaluated linear mixed models using both Bayesian and frequentist statistics packages to ensure results were robust to analysis approaches. Bayesian linear mixed models were implemented using the brms package (Bürkner, 2017) in R (R Development Core Team, 1996). All coefficients were evaluated with a Gaussian prior centered on 0 with a 2.5 SD (i.e. a mildly informative conservative prior). For each outcome measure, we evaluated all potential models ranging from intercepts only to a maximal model (Barr et al., 2013) with random slopes for every factor and all potential interactions, then used ‘bayesfactor_models’ from the bayestestR package (Makowski et al., 2019a) to perform model comparison and to draw inferences about acceptance and rejection of null effects. In most cases, Bayesian model comparisons supported maximal models (Barr et al., 2013), that is including all fixed factors (except Time), all interactions, and random intercepts and slopes for each subject. We used the ‘describe_posterior’ function (Makowski et al., 2019a) to evaluate probability of direction (similar to a frequentist p-value with values >95% indicating the effect is likely to exist), and the percentage of the posterior distribution within the region of partial equivalence (ROPE), which can delineate significance. Published guidelines suggest that when if <1% of the distribution is within the ROPE, one can reject the null hypothesis, whereas if >99% is in the ROPE, one can accept the null hypothesis (Makowski et al., 2019b).

We then evaluated the corresponding model using ‘lmer’ from the lme4 package (Bates et al., 2015) to provide frequentist statistics and using ‘lme’ from the nlme package (Pinheiro et al., 2021) to incorporate autoregression. If frequentist models corresponding to the best Bayes model did not converge, we removed the covariance between random effects (i.e. specified that slopes and intercepts are uncorrelated using ||), and we used optimizers to achieve convergence. Final models are specified in Tables 1–3. We acknowledge findings from all approaches in our Results, using guidelines for Bayesian modeling from Makowski et al., 2019b; Makowski et al., 2019a. Results across approaches were largely consistent (see Tables 1–3).

Computational modeling of pain reversal learning and modulation by instructions

We applied a computational model of instructed reversal learning (Atlas et al., 2016; Atlas and Phelps, 2018) to predict pain reports on medium heat trials. The model is a standard reinforcement learning model (Rescorla and Wagner, 1972) that describes how expected value (EV) updates in response to prediction errors in the environment, depending on learning rate. We include an additional parameter, ρ, which guides whether cues’ expected values (EV) are exchanged when instructions are delivered. If ρ = 0, each cue’s EV remains stable (i.e. retains the same EV prior to instructions) whereas if ρ = 1, the EVs of the two respective cues are exchanged. Mathematically for two cues (a and b), this is implemented for trial (t) as:

EVt+1(xb)=ρVt(xa)+(1ρ)Vt(xb)

For complete details, see Atlas et al., 2016. The present model fitting procedure differed in that we fit models to pain ratings on medium heat trials, rather than SCR on unreinforced trials, although we fit models to SCR in exploratory analyses, as described below. In the present study, we assume that pairings between predictive stimuli and high and low intensity heat engages stimulus-based learning, similar to CS-US pairings in classical conditioning experiments. Fitting to medium heat trials isolates the timecourse of expected value, since the stimulus temperature is constant, and therefore the only factor likely to guide cue-based variation in pain is presumably the cue’s dynamic expected value based on learning and/or instructions.

Consistent with our previous work (Atlas et al., 2016; Atlas and Phelps, 2018), the initial EV was set as 0.5 for Uninstructed Group participants, while we used asymmetric initial expected values for Instructed Participants who were informed about contingencies (1 for high pain cue; 0 for low pain cue). Models were fit at the level of the individual, with two free parameters (ɑ and ρ), and we used 20 iterations with a random starting value for each parameter for each iteration. We minimized the deviance between expected value and pain on medium heat trials. EV was bounded between 0 and 1; however, we used MATLAB’s glmfit.m function to relate EV with pain on medium heat trials using the normal link function, which includes an intercept and therefore accounts for differences in scale between EV and pain reports. Consistent with other analyses, the first trial of every block was not included during model fitting due to novelty responses.

To evaluate goodness-of-fit, we compared the instructed Rescorla-Wagner model described above with three other variations of plausible learning models: (a) an instructed Rescorla-Wagner model with initial value, ɑ, and ρ as free parameters; (b) a traditional Rescorla-Wagner model without an instructed reversal parameter (initial value and ɑ as free parameters); and (c) an instructed model of associability (i.e. a Hybrid model; Li et al., 2011b) with ρ,η, and κ as free parameters (Atlas et al., 2019). Models were fit to each individual’s pain ratings on medium heat trials, since the stimulus temperature was constant and therefore the only factor likely to guide cue-based variation in pain would presumably be dynamic expected value based on learned and/or instructed value. Models were analyzed at the level of the individual, which provides estimates that can be tested using standard statistics, and through an iterative jack-knife procedure, which is less sensitive to noise based on individual estimates (Wu, 1986; Miller et al., 1998; Atlas and Phelps, 2018). See Atlas and Phelps, 2018 for complete details on jack-knife model fitting procedures. Group analyses and model comparisons were conducted using individual estimates. Goodness-of-fit was evaluated based on Akaike’s Information Criterion (Akaike, 1974), which evaluates model fit with a penalty for additional parameters and is appropriate for nested models, and models were compared based on Bayesian Model Selection, as implemented in SPM’s SPM_bms.m (Stephan et al., 2009).

The instructed Rescorla-Wagner model with pre-determined initial values and free parameters for ɑ and ρ (i.e. the same model used in our previous work; Atlas et al., 2016; Atlas and Phelps, 2018) fit the data better than any of the alternatives, as indicated by Bayesian Model Selection, which gave this model a posterior probability of 87.95% relative to the other alternatives (Rescorla-Wagner with initial free parameters: 3.61%; Rescorla-Wagner without instruction parameter: 5.1%; Hybrid model: 3.35%). We therefore focus on this model in the main manuscript and used this model to generate regressors for use in fMRI analyses. We used t-tests on subject-level fits to assess whether each parameter (ɑ and ρ) varied as a function of group. The mean parameters based on jack-knife estimates were used to generate regressors for fMRI analyses (see ‘Neural correlates of expected value’, below).

We used the same approach to fit computational models to anticipatory SCR and SCR evoked during medium heat simulation.

FMRI analyses

Single-trial analyses

Following preprocessing in AFNI, we used single trial analyses to estimate heat-evoked responses on a trial-by-trial basis and avoid assumptions about the fixed shape of the hemodynamic response. We used flexible basis functions optimized to capture heat-evoked BOLD responses, consistent with previous work on heat-evoked fMRI (Atlas et al., 2010; Atlas et al., 2012; Atlas et al., 2014a; Wager et al., 2013; Woo et al., 2017). We applied principal components analysis-based spike detection (scn_session_spike_id.m, available at https://canlab.github.io/; Wager, 2022) to identify potential spikes and noise in the data which were modeled as nuisance covariates, along with the movement parameters from AFNI’s preprocessing pipeline. We used the function single_trial_analysis.m (https://canlab.github.io/) to generate trial-by-trial estimates of height, width, delay, and area-under-the-curve (AUC) for each heat period. Trials in which subjects failed to respond were omitted from analyses (Mall = 1.69, SDall = 2.38; MInstructed = 1.11, SDInstructed = 1.08, rangeInstructed = 0–3; MUninstructed = 2.28, SDUninstructed = 3.12, rangeUninstructed = 0–11). We focused on AUC estimates in subsequent analyses, consistent with our previous work. We computed variance inflation factors (VIFs) using the single_trial_weights_vifthresh.m function to identify bad trials, that is those who coincided with spikes or motion and were therefore not reliable estimates. We excluded any trials with VIFs >2 from subsequent analyses (M=3.39, SD = 2.38), and smoothed trial estimates with a 4 mm gaussian kernel, consistent with previous work (Atlas et al., 2010; Atlas et al., 2012; Atlas et al., 2014c). Trial estimates were passed into voxel-wise second level analyses across trials and across participants using the general linear model (fit_gls_brain.m; https://canlab.github.io/) and robust regression (robfit.m; https://canlab.github.io/) to examine neural correlates of associative learning (see below, ‘Neural correlates of expected value’). Trial-level estimates were also employed in multilevel mediation analyses (see below, ‘Multilevel mediation analyses’).

Multilevel mediation analyses

We used multilevel mediation to examine whether brain activity mediated the effect of predictive cues on subjective pain on medium heat trials. Mediation was implemented by the MATLAB function mediation.m (https://canlab.github.io/). Cue was included as the input variable (i.e. X; coded as 1 for High cue, –1 for Low cue), pain was included as the output variable (i.e. Y), and we searched for potential mediators. Voxelwise mediation, or mediation effect parametric mapping (Wager et al., 2009; Atlas et al., 2010) yields interpretable maps that are similar to simultaneous partial regressions, although implemented using mixed effects. The mediation effect (a*b) identifies regions whose activity contributes to variance in the effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable (Path c). For individual i, trial j:

.Yij=d0j+cjXij+e0ij (1)

Path a denotes the effect of the input variable on the potential mediator (M; brain activation in a given voxel), thereby representing cue effects on brain responses to medium heat:

.Mij=d1j+ajXij+e1ij (2)

Path b measures the association between the mediator and outcome, controlling for the input variable. Here, this represents brain regions that predict pain, controlling for cue type:

.Yij=d2j+cjXij+biMij+e2ij (3)

In multilevel mediation, the difference between the total effect (Path c: the effect of cues on subjective pain) and the direct effect (Path c`: the effect of cues on subjective pain when controlling for the mediator, see equation 3) is equivalent to the sum of the product of Path a and Path b coefficients and their covariance (Shrout and Bolger, 2002; Kenny et al., 2003). Our mixed models included intercepts (d0-2) and error terms (e0-2) for each individual. Group level estimates are computed by treating person-level intercepts and slopes as random effects. For complete details, see Atlas et al., 2010.

We performed two voxel-wise multilevel mediation analyses: (1) a search for brain mediators of current cue effects on pain, i.e. the Cue x Phase interaction (see Figure 5), and (2) a search for mediators of original cue effects on pain, controlling for current contingencies (see Figure 6). For both models, Path a evaluates the effect of predictive cue on brain response to medium heat, Path b captures the association between brain response and pain, controlling for cue, and the mediation effect (Path a*b) tests whether brain responses contribute to variance between cues and subjective pain on medium heat trials. We evaluated mediation overall irrespective of group, and with Group as a potential moderator of all paths.

We focus in particular on effects of current contingencies (i.e. Cue x Phase interactions) on pain to capture the behavioral reversals we observed (Figure 5). In this main analysis, we coded the input variable (X) as the contrast between current high cues (i.e. original high cues during original contingencies and original low cues during reversed contingencies both coded as 1) and current low cues (i.e. original low cues during original contingencies and original high cues during reversed contingencies both coded as –1). Our second mediation searched for mediators of original cue effects (i.e. original high cue coded as 1, original low cue coded as –1) and controlled for current contingencies by including the X variable from the current cue contingencies mediation (i.e. the Cue x Phase interaction) as a covariate in all paths. These effects are thus most likely to be driven by responses during the reversed runs. In both mediations, Path b evaluates the link between brain activation and pain, while controlling for the cue (see equation 2 above).

We ran two types of mediation analyses on medium heat trials: (a) voxel-wise mediation analyses, which search for brain regions that mediate the effects of predictive cues on pain; (b) statistical tests of whether brain responses within ROIs formally mediated cue effects on subjective pain. We evaluated mediation analyses irrespective of Group, and with Group as moderator. We used bootstrapping to estimate the significance of the mediation effect (Shrout and Bolger, 2002; Kenny et al., 2003) in analyses irrespective of Group, and used ordinary least squares to estimate moderated mediation when Group was included in the model. We omitted the first trial of each run from analyses to be consistent with behavioral results.

Neural correlates of expected value and prediction error

Whereas our mediation analyses tested effects of Phase, i.e. immediate changes in response to instruction or contingency reversal, we used quantitative models to test whether expected value dynamically shapes responses to noxious stimulation. We used parameters from the best-fitting models for each group, based on jack-knife estimation, to generate the timecourse of expected value (EV) for each subject based on their sequence of trials. We chose to use the mean of the group-level estimates to avoid noise that might come from individual-level model fits. We examined the neural correlates of expected value on medium heat trials only, which avoids confounds due to temperature. We focused on how expected value influenced responses to medium intensity heat, rather than responses to cues themselves, as we were most interested in how pain-related responses are influenced by learned expectations, and we did not optimize the anticipatory period to jointly estimate cue-evoked responses and responses to heat. Our main manuscript focuses on the timecourse of EV based on fits to pain reports; we include associations based on fits to heat-evoked and anticipatory SCR in Supplementary Materials.

Noxious stimulation might also be accompanied by prediction errors; for example, if an individual expects high pain and receives medium heat, this should generate an appetitive PE (i.e. better than expected) if the deviation is noticed. However, expected value and prediction error are inversely correlated in the standard RL model we used. We therefore measured associations with unsigned PE (i.e. the absolute value of PE), as signed PE is the inverse of expected value on our medium heat trials. We focused on responses to medium heat trials only as PE would be correlated with temperature if we analyzed responses across all trials (i.e. low temperatures are always expected or better than expected, whereas high temperatures are always expected or worse than expected).

We report three group-level analyses: (1) Analysis across all participants testing for differences by group; (2) Analyses within each group to isolate effects of instructed learning (Instructed Group) or feedback-driven learning (Uninstructed Group); (3) Comparisons of instructed and feedback-driven learning within the Instructed Group. Individual results were computed using the MATLAB function fit_gls_brain (https://canlab.github.io/) and group results were computed using robust regression (Wager et al., 2005) using the function robfit.m (https://canlab.github.io/).

Pain modulatory regions of interest

We tested for cue-based modulation of brain regions that have been previously implicated in studies of expectancy-based pain modulation by applying an a priori mask generated from our previous meta-analysis of fMRI studies of placebo analgesia and expectancy-based modulation (Atlas and Wager, 2014b). We included regions that showed either expectancy-related increases or decreases in activation within the mask. Figure 5—figure supplement 1A depicts the mask, which includes regions that show increased activation with expected pain relief (i.e. activation inversely related to subjective pain) such as the DLPFC, rACC, PAG, and VMPFC, and regions that show reduced activation with expected pain relief, including the insula, thalamus, cingulate, and secondary somatosensory cortex. We report results FDR-corrected within this mask to evaluate responses within pain modulatory regions.

We also tested for effects on two recently developed brain-based classifiers that have been shown to be sensitive and specific to acute pain, the Neurologic Pain Signature (NPS; Wager et al., 2013) and the Stimulus Intensity Independent Pain Signature (SIIPS; Woo et al., 2017). We used the unthresholded NPS pattern for all analyses, and the function apply_mask.m (https://canlab.github.io/) to compute dot products. We computed the dot-product of each signature with beta coefficients and contrast maps for analyses of computational models, and trial-level images to use the brain response as an outcome in our multilevel mediation analyses.

Value-processing regions of interest

In addition to pain modulatory networks, we were also interested in testing effects of predictive cues on brain regions involved in value-based learning. To this end, we examined responses within 5 a priori regions of interest (see Figure 5—figure supplement 1B): the bilateral striatum, bilateral amygdala, and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC). We used the same ROI masks that were applied in our prior work on instructed reversal learning (Atlas et al., 2016). While the amygdala and striatum masks were defined based on Atlases in MNI space (amygdala ROI available at https://canlab.github.io/; striatum ROI based on combining putamen and caudate masks from the Automated Anatomical Labeling atlas for SPM8 [http://www.gin.cnrs.fr/AAL; Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002]), the VMPFC ROI was functionally defined in our previous work (Atlas et al., 2016) by analyzing deactivation in response to shock. We used the same ROI mask here since analyses as a function of heat intensity elicited significant decreases in this region. We averaged trial-level AUC estimates across each ROI to conduct mediation analyses and averaged across beta coefficients and contrast maps to analyze ROI-wise associations with expected value. Results are reported in Table 4.

Whole brain exploratory analyses

In addition to the analyses in a priori networks and regions of interest involved in pain, placebo, and value-based processing, we also conducted exploratory voxel-wise whole brain analyses. We report whole brain results at FDR-corrected q<0.05 in the main manuscript, and present exploratory uncorrected results at p<0.001 in Source Data accompanying each figure for completeness and for use in future meta-analyses. In cases when FDR-corrected p-values exceeded p=0.001, we use uncorrected results for inference. Anatomical labels were identified using the SPM Anatomy Toolbox (Eickhoff et al., 2005), which includes the Thalamic Connectivity Toolbox (Behrens et al., 2003; Johansen-Berg et al., 2005), and the Brainstem Navigator (http://www.nitrc.org/projects/brainstemnavig/; Singh et al., 2021).

Acknowledgements

This research was funded by the Intramural Research Program of the National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health (Project ZIAAT-000030; PI Lauren Atlas) as well as NIMH Intramural Research Program Project ZIAMH-002782 (PI Daniel Pine). We thank Linda Ellison-Dejewski and Adebisi Ayodele for assistance with participant screening and clinical support, and thank Paul Taylor for assistance with fMRI preprocessing pipelines. The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Lauren Y Atlas, Email: lauren.atlas@nih.gov.

Joshua Johansen, RIKEN Center for Brain Science, Japan.

Christian Büchel, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Germany.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health ZIAAT-000030 to Lauren Y Atlas.

  • National Institute of Mental Health ZIAMH-002782 to Daniel S Pine.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Project administration, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – review and editing.

Resources, Data curation, Software, Methodology, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – review and editing.

Resources, Supervision, Writing – review and editing.

Ethics

Clinical trial registration NCT02446262.

Consent was obtained as described in Materials and methods. Data were collected under NIH protocol 15-AT-0132, identifier NCT02446262 at clinicaltrials.gov.

Additional files

Supplementary file 1. Heat intensity effects on pain in fMRI participants (n = 36).
elife-73353-supp1.docx (128.9KB, docx)
Supplementary file 2. Bayesian multilevel model evaluating effects of Group, Cue, Phase, and Trial on medium heat pain using Beta family.
elife-73353-supp2.docx (50.9KB, docx)
Transparent reporting form

Data availability

Pain, SCR, and signature data are available on OSF; neuroimaging data has been uploaded to neurovault.

The following datasets were generated:

Atlas LY. 2022. Instructions and experiential learning have similar impacts on pain and pain-related brain responses but produce dissociations in value-based reversal learning. NeuroVault. XSHXWBEV

Atlas LY. 2022. How instructions and learning shape pain and expectancy-based pain modulation: Pain, SCR, and brain signatures. Open Science Framework. adc8e

References

  1. Abend R, Bajaj MA, Harrewijn A, Matsumoto C, Michalska KJ, Necka E, Palacios-Barrios EE, Leibenluft E, Atlas LY, Pine DS. Threat-anticipatory psychophysiological response is enhanced in youth with anxiety disorders and correlates with prefrontal cortex neuroanatomy. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience. 2021;46:E212–E221. doi: 10.1503/jpn.200110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Akaike H. In: Selected Papers of Hirotugu Akaike. Akaike H, editor. Springer; 1974. A new look at the statistical model identification; pp. 215–222. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. Allen M, Poggiali D, Whitaker K, Marshall TR, van Langen J, Kievit RA. Raincloud plots: a multi-platform tool for robust data visualization. Wellcome Open Research. 2021;4:63. doi: 10.12688/wellcomeopenres.15191.2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Amanzio M, Benedetti F. Neuropharmacological dissection of placebo analgesia: expectation-activated opioid systems versus conditioning-activated specific subsystems. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;19:484–494. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-01-00484.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Amir C, Rose-McCandlish M, Weger R, Dildine TC, Mischkowski D, Necka EA, Lee I, Wager TD, Pine D, Atlas LY. Test-Retest Reliability of an Adaptive Thermal Pain Calibration Procedure in Healthy Volunteers. PsyArXiv. 2021 doi: 10.31234/osf.io/wk7y6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  6. Atlas LY, Bolger N, Lindquist MA, Wager TD. Brain mediators of predictive cue effects on perceived pain. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:12964–12977. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0057-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Atlas LY, Wager TD. How expectations shape pain. Neuroscience Letters. 2012;520:140–148. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.03.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Atlas LY, Whittington RA, Lindquist MA, Wielgosz J, Sonty N, Wager TD. Dissociable influences of opiates and expectations on pain. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:8053–8064. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0383-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Atlas LY, Lindquist MA, Bolger N, Wager TD. Brain mediators of the effects of noxious heat on pain. PAIN. 2014a;155:1632–1648. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2014.05.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Atlas LY, Wager TD. A meta-analysis of brain mechanisms of placebo analgesia: consistent findings and unanswered questions. Experimental Pharmacology. 2014b;225:37–69. doi: 10.1007/978-3-662-44519-8_3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Atlas LY, Wielgosz J, Whittington RA, Wager TD. Specifying the non-specific factors underlying opioid analgesia: expectancy, attention, and affect. Psychopharmacology. 2014c;231:813–823. doi: 10.1007/s00213-013-3296-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Atlas LY, Doll BB, Li J, Daw ND, Phelps EA. Instructed knowledge shapes feedback-driven aversive learning in striatum and orbitofrontal cortex, but not the amygdala. eLife. 2016;5:e15192. doi: 10.7554/eLife.15192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Atlas LY, Phelps EA. Prepared stimuli enhance aversive learning without weakening the impact of verbal instructions. Learning & Memory. 2018;25:100–104. doi: 10.1101/lm.046359.117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Atlas LY. How instructions shape aversive learning: higher order knowledge, reversal learning, and the role of the amygdala. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2019;26:121–129. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.12.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Atlas LY, Doll BB, Li J, Daw ND, Phelps EA. How Instructed Knowledge Shapes Adaptive Learning. PsyArXiv. 2019 doi: 10.31234/osf.io/f4sh9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  16. Atlas LY. A social affective neuroscience lens on placebo analgesia. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2021;25:992–1005. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2021.07.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Atlas LY, Dildine TC, Palacios-Barrios EE, Yu Q, Reynolds RC, Banker LA, Grant SS, Pine DS. Instructions and Experiential Learning Have Similar Impacts on Pain and Pain-Related Brain Responses but Produce Dissociations in Value-Based Reversal Learning. bioRxiv. 2021 doi: 10.1101/2021.08.25.457682. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  18. Barr DJ, Levy R, Scheepers C, Tily HJ. Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language. 2013;68:255–278. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software. 2015;67:i01. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  20. Behrens TEJ, Johansen-Berg H, Woolrich MW, Smith SM, Wheeler-Kingshott CAM, Boulby PA, Barker GJ, Sillery EL, Sheehan K, Ciccarelli O, Thompson AJ, Brady JM, Matthews PM. Non-Invasive mapping of connections between human thalamus and cortex using diffusion imaging. NATURE NEUROSCIENCE. 2003;6:750–757. doi: 10.1038/nn1075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Benedek M, Kaernbach C. A continuous measure of phasic electrodermal activity. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2010;190:80–91. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.04.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Benedetti F, Pollo A, Lopiano L, Lanotte M, Vighetti S, Rainero I. Conscious expectation and unconscious conditioning in analgesic, motor, and hormonal placebo/nocebo responses. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23:4315–4323. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-10-04315.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Berns GS, Chappelow J, Cekic M, Zink CF, Pagnoni G, Martin-Skurski ME. Neurobiological substrates of dread. Science. 2006;312:754–758. doi: 10.1126/science.1123721. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Bingel U, Lorenz J, Schoell E, Weiller C, Büchel C. Mechanisms of placebo analgesia: racc recruitment of a subcortical antinociceptive network. PAIN. 2006;120:8–15. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2005.08.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Boll S, Gamer M, Gluth S, Finsterbusch J, Büchel C. Separate amygdala subregions signal surprise and predictiveness during associative fear learning in humans. The European Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;37:758–767. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12094. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Browning M, Behrens TE, Jocham G, O’Reilly JX, Bishop SJ. Anxious individuals have difficulty learning the causal statistics of aversive environments. Nature Neuroscience. 2015;18:590–596. doi: 10.1038/nn.3961. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Büchel C, Morris J, Dolan RJ, Friston KJ. Brain systems mediating aversive conditioning: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron. 1998;20:947–957. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80476-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Büchel C, Geuter S, Sprenger C, Eippert F. Placebo analgesia: a predictive coding perspective. Neuron. 2014;81:1223–1239. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.02.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Bürkner PC. Brms: an R package for bayesian multilevel models using stan. Journal of Statistical Software. 2017;80:1–28. doi: 10.18637/jss.v080.i01. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. Carver CS, White TL. Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: the BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1994;67:319–333. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. Colloca L, Sigaudo M, Benedetti F. The role of learning in nocebo and placebo effects. PAIN. 2008a;136:211–218. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2008.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Colloca L, Tinazzi M, Recchia S, Le Pera D, Fiaschi A, Benedetti F, Valeriani M. Learning potentiates neurophysiological and behavioral placebo analgesic responses. PAIN. 2008b;139:306–314. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2008.04.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Corlett PR, Mollick JA, Kober H. Meta-analysis of human prediction error for incentives, perception, cognition, and action. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2022;47:1339–1349. doi: 10.1038/s41386-021-01264-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Costa VD, Bradley MM, Lang PJ. From threat to safety: instructed reversal of defensive reactions. Psychophysiology. 2015;52:325–332. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Cox RW. AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance NeuroImages. Computers and Biomedical Research, an International Journal. 1996;29:162–173. doi: 10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Crawford LS, Mills EP, Hanson T, Macey PM, Glarin R, Macefield VG, Keay KA, Henderson LA. Brainstem mechanisms of pain modulation: a within-subjects 7T fMRI study of placebo analgesic and nocebo hyperalgesic responses. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2021;41:9794–9806. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0806-21.2021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Dayan P, Berridge KC. Model-Based and model-free Pavlovian reward learning: revaluation, revision, and revelation. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2014;14:473–492. doi: 10.3758/s13415-014-0277-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Dildine TC, Necka EA, Atlas LY. Confidence in subjective pain is predicted by reaction time during decision making. Scientific Reports. 2020;10:21373. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-77864-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Doll BB, Jacobs WJ, Sanfey AG, Frank MJ. Instructional control of reinforcement learning: a behavioral and neurocomputational investigation. Brain Research. 2009;1299:74–94. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.07.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Doll BB, Hutchison KE, Frank MJ. Dopaminergic genes predict individual differences in susceptibility to confirmation bias. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:6188–6198. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6486-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Doll BB, Simon DA, Daw ND. The ubiquity of model-based reinforcement learning. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2012;22:1075–1081. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Doll BB, Duncan KD, Simon DA, Shohamy D, Daw ND. Model-Based choices involve prospective neural activity. Nature Neuroscience. 2015;18:767–772. doi: 10.1038/nn.3981. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Eickhoff SB, Stephan KE, Mohlberg H, Grefkes C, Fink GR, Amunts K, Zilles K. A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. NeuroImage. 2005;25:1325–1335. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Eippert F, Bingel U, Schoell ED, Yacubian J, Klinger R, Lorenz J, Büchel C. Activation of the opioidergic descending pain control system underlies placebo analgesia. Neuron. 2009;63:533–543. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.07.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Ellingsen DM, Wessberg J, Eikemo M, Liljencrantz J, Endestad T, Olausson H, Leknes S. Placebo improves pleasure and pain through opposite modulation of sensory processing. PNAS. 2013;110:17993–17998. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1305050110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Fazeli S, Büchel C. Pain-Related expectation and prediction error signals in the anterior insula are not related to aversiveness. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2018;38:6461–6474. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0671-18.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Fields H. State-Dependent opioid control of pain. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2004;5:565–575. doi: 10.1038/nrn1431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Fields HL. In: Proceedings of the 11th World Congress on Pain. Flor H, Kalso E, editors. IASP Press; 2006. A motivation-decision model of pain: the role of opioids; pp. 11–20. [Google Scholar]
  49. Forsberg JT, Martinussen M, Flaten MA. The placebo analgesic effect in healthy individuals and patients: a meta-analysis. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2017;79:388–394. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Gaudry E, Vagg P, Spielberger CD. Validation of the state-trait distinction in anxiety research. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 1975;10:331–341. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr1003_6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Geuter S, Eippert F, Hindi Attar C, Büchel C. Cortical and subcortical responses to high and low effective placebo treatments. NeuroImage. 2013;67:227–236. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Grahl A, Onat S, Büchel C. The periaqueductal gray and Bayesian integration in placebo analgesia. eLife. 2018;7:e32930. doi: 10.7554/eLife.32930. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Grings WW. Cognitive factors in electrodermal conditioning. Psychological Bulletin. 1973;79:200–210. doi: 10.1037/h0033883. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Jangraw DC, Wang J, Lance BJ, Chang SF, Sajda P. Neurally and ocularly informed graph-based models for searching 3D environments. Journal of Neural Engineering. 2014;11:046003. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/11/4/046003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Jepma M, Koban L, van Doorn J, Jones M, Wager TD. Behavioural and neural evidence for self-reinforcing expectancy effects on pain. Nature Human Behaviour. 2018;2:838–855. doi: 10.1038/s41562-018-0455-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Johansen-Berg H, Behrens TEJ, Sillery E, Ciccarelli O, Thompson AJ, Smith SM, Matthews PM. Functional-anatomical validation and individual variation of diffusion tractography-based segmentation of the human thalamus. Cerebral Cortex. 2005;15:31–39. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Johnston NE, Atlas LY, Wager TD, El-Deredy W. Opposing effects of expectancy and somatic focus on pain. PLOS ONE. 2012;7:e38854. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038854. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Kaptchuk TJ, Hemond CC, Miller FG. Placebos in chronic pain: evidence, theory, ethics, and use in clinical practice. BMJ. 2020;10:m1668. doi: 10.1136/bmj.m1668. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Kassambara A. Pipe-friendly framework for basic statistical tests. Rstatix. 2021 https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=rstatix
  60. Kenny DA, Korchmaros JD, Bolger N. Lower level mediation in multilevel models. Psychological Methods. 2003;8:115–128. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.8.2.115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Kirsch I. Response expectancy theory and application: a decennial review. Applied and Preventive Psychology. 1997;6:69–79. doi: 10.1016/S0962-1849(05)80012-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  62. Kirsch I, Lynn SJ, Vigorito M, Miller RR. The role of cognition in classical and operant conditioning. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2004;60:369–392. doi: 10.1002/jclp.10251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Koban L, Jepma M, Geuter S, Wager TD. What’s in a word? how instructions, suggestions, and social information change pain and emotion. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2017;81:29–42. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.02.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Koban L, Jepma M, López-Solà M, Wager TD. Different brain networks mediate the effects of social and conditioned expectations on pain. Nature Communications. 2019;10:4096. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-11934-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Krummenacher P, Candia V, Folkers G, Schedlowski M, Schönbächler G. Prefrontal cortex modulates placebo analgesia. PAIN. 2010;148:368–374. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.09.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Kundu P, Inati SJ, Evans JW, Luh W-M, Bandettini PA. Differentiating BOLD and non-BOLD signals in fMRI time series using multi-echo EPI. NeuroImage. 2012;60:1759–1770. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Levine JD, Gordon NC, Fields HL. The mechanism of placebo analgesia. Lancet. 1978;2:654–657. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(78)92762-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Li J, Delgado MR, Phelps EA. How instructed knowledge modulates the neural systems of reward learning. PNAS. 2011a;108:55–60. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1014938108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Li J, Schiller D, Schoenbaum G, Phelps EA, Daw ND. Differential roles of human striatum and amygdala in associative learning. Nature Neuroscience. 2011b;14:1250–1252. doi: 10.1038/nn.2904. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Lieberman MD, Jarcho JM, Berman S, Naliboff BD, Suyenobu BY, Mandelkern M, Mayer EA. The neural correlates of placebo effects: a disruption account. NeuroImage. 2004;22:447–455. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Lombardo MV, Auyeung B, Holt RJ, Waldman J, Ruigrok ANV, Mooney N, Bullmore ET, Baron-Cohen S, Kundu P. Improving effect size estimation and statistical power with multi-echo fmri and its impact on understanding the neural systems supporting mentalizing. NeuroImage. 2016;142:55–66. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.07.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Lucantonio F, Gardner MPH, Mirenzi A, Newman LE, Takahashi YK, Schoenbaum G. Neural estimates of imagined outcomes in basolateral amygdala depend on orbitofrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:16521–16530. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3126-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Lüdecke D. SjPlot: data visualization for statistics in social science. SjPlot. 2021 https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=sjPlot
  74. Makowski D, Ben-Shachar MS, Chen SHA, Lüdecke D. Indices of effect existence and significance in the bayesian framework. Frontiers in Psychology. 2019a;10:2767. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02767. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Makowski D, Ben-Shachar M, Lüdecke D. BayestestR: describing effects and their uncertainty, existence and significance within the bayesian framework. Journal of Open Source Software. 2019b;4:1541. doi: 10.21105/joss.01541. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  76. McNally RJ. Phobias and preparedness: instructional reversal of electrodermalconditioning to fear-relevant stimuli. Psychological Reports. 1981;48:175–180. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1981.48.1.175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Mertens G, De Houwer J. Potentiation of the startle reflex is in line with contingency reversal instructions rather than the conditioning history. Biological Psychology. 2016;113:91–99. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.11.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Mertens G, Boddez Y, Sevenster D, Engelhard IM, De Houwer J. A review on the effects of verbal instructions in human fear conditioning: empirical findings, theoretical considerations, and future directions. Biological Psychology. 2018;137:49–64. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Michalska KJ, Feldman JS, Abend R, Gold AL, Dildine TC, Palacios-Barrios EE, Leibenluft E, Towbin KE, Pine DS, Atlas LY. Anticipatory effects on perceived pain: associations with development and anxiety. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2018;80:853–860. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000608. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Miller J, Patterson T, Ulrich R. Jackknife-based method for measuring LRP onset latency differences. Psychophysiology. 1998;35:99–115. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Mineka S, Ohman A. Phobias and preparedness: the selective, automatic, and encapsulated nature of fear. Biological Psychiatry. 2002;52:927–937. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01669-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Mischkowski D, Palacios-Barrios EE, Banker L, Dildine TC, Atlas LY. Pain or nociception? subjective experience mediates the effects of acute noxious heat on autonomic responses-corrected and republished. Pain. 2019;160:1469–1481. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001573. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Montgomery G, Kirsch I. Mechanisms of placebo pain reduction: an empirical investigation. Psychological Science. 1996;7:174–176. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00352.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  84. Motzkin JC, Hiser J, Carroll I, Wolf R, Baskaya MK, Koenigs M, Atlas LY. Human Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Lesions Enhance Expectation-Related Pain Modulation. bioRxiv. 2021 doi: 10.1101/2021.11.30.470579. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  85. Navratilova E, Xie JY, Meske D, Qu C, Morimura K, Okun A, Arakawa N, Ossipov M, Fields HL, Porreca F. Endogenous opioid activity in the anterior cingulate cortex is required for relief of pain. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:7264–7271. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3862-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Ohman A, Soares JJ. On the automatic nature of phobic fear: conditioned electrodermal responses to masked fear-relevant stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1993;102:121–132. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.102.1.121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Ongaro G, Kaptchuk TJ. Symptom perception, placebo effects, and the bayesian brain. PAIN. 2018;1:98882. doi: 10.1097/00006396-900000000-98882. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Petrovic P, Kalso E, Petersson KM, Ingvar M. Placebo and opioid analgesia -- imaging a shared neuronal network. Science. 2002;295:1737–1740. doi: 10.1126/science.1067176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Petrovic P, Kalso E, Petersson KM, Andersson J, Fransson P, Ingvar M. A prefrontal non-opioid mechanism in placebo analgesia. Pain. 2010;150:59–65. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.03.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Pinheiro J, Bates D, Debroy S, Sarkar D. Nlme: linear and nonlinear mixed effects models. Nlme. 2021 https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme
  91. R Development Core Team . Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 1996. https://www.R-project.org [Google Scholar]
  92. Reicherts P, Gerdes ABM, Pauli P, Wieser MJ. Psychological placebo and nocebo effects on pain rely on expectation and previous experience. The Journal of Pain. 2016;17:203–214. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2015.10.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Rescorla RA, Wagner AR. In: Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory. Black A, Prokasky W, editors. Appleton-Century-Crofts; 1972. A theory of pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcment; pp. 64–99. [Google Scholar]
  94. Rescorla RA. Pavlovian conditioning: it’s not what you think it is. The American Psychologist. 1988;43:151–160. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.43.3.151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Rudebeck PH, Murray EA. The orbitofrontal oracle: cortical mechanisms for the prediction and evaluation of specific behavioral outcomes. Neuron. 2014;84:1143–1156. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.10.049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Schafer SM, Colloca L, Wager TD. Conditioned placebo analgesia persists when subjects know they are receiving a placebo. The Journal of Pain. 2015;16:412–420. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2014.12.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Schuck NW, Wilson R, Niv Y. A State Representation for Reinforcement Learning and Decision-Making in the Orbitofrontal Cortex. Elsevier Inc; 2018. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  98. Segerdahl AR, Mezue M, Okell TW, Farrar JT, Tracey I. The dorsal posterior insula subserves a fundamental role in human pain. Nature Neuroscience. 2015;18:499–500. doi: 10.1038/nn.3969. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Shrout PE, Bolger N. Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: new procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods. 2002;7:422–445. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Singh K, García-Gomar MG, Bianciardi M. Probabilistic atlas of the mesencephalic reticular formation, isthmic reticular formation, microcellular tegmental nucleus, ventral tegmental area nucleus complex, and caudal-rostral linear raphe nucleus complex in living humans from 7 Tesla magnetic resonance imaging. Brain Connectivity. 2021;11:613–623. doi: 10.1089/brain.2020.0975. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Stephan KE, Penny WD, Daunizeau J, Moran RJ, Friston KJ. Bayesian model selection for group studies. NeuroImage. 2009;46:1004–1017. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.03.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Ter Riet G, de Craen AJM, de Boer A, Kessels AGH. Is placebo analgesia mediated by endogenous opioids? A systematic review. PAIN. 1998;76:273–275. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(98)00057-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, Mazoyer B, Joliot M. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. NeuroImage. 2002;15:273–289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Wager TD, Rilling JK, Smith EE, Sokolik A, Casey KL, Davidson RJ, Kosslyn SM, Rose RM, Cohen JD. Placebo-induced changes in fmri in the anticipation and experience of pain. Science. 2004;303:1162–1167. doi: 10.1126/science.1093065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Wager T.D, Keller MC, Lacey SC, Jonides J. Increased sensitivity in neuroimaging analyses using robust regression. NeuroImage. 2005;26:99–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Wager TD, Scott DJ, Zubieta JK. Placebo effects on human mu-opioid activity during pain. PNAS. 2007;104:11056–11061. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0702413104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Wager T.D, Waugh CE, Lindquist M, Noll DC, Fredrickson BL, Taylor SF. Brain mediators of cardiovascular responses to social threat. NeuroImage. 2009;47:821–835. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Wager T.D, Atlas LY, Lindquist MA, Roy M, Woo C, Kross E. An fmri-based neurologic signature of physical pain. The New England Journal of Medicine. 2013;368:1388–1397. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1204471. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Wager T. CanlabCore tools. version unknownGitHub. 2022 https://canlab.github.io
  110. Watkins LR, Mayer DJ. Involvement of spinal opioid systems in footshock-induced analgesia: antagonism by naloxone is possible only before induction of analgesia. Brain Research. 1982a;242:309–326. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(82)90314-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Watkins LR, Mayer DJ. Organization of endogenous opiate and nonopiate pain control systems. Science. 1982b;216:1185–1192. doi: 10.1126/science.6281891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988;54:1063–1070. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Wickham H. Ggplot2: Elegrant Graphics for Data Analysis. Springer; 2016. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  114. Wiech K, Vandekerckhove J, Zaman J, Tuerlinckx F, Vlaeyen JWS, Tracey I. Influence of prior information on pain involves biased perceptual decision- making. Current Biology. 2014;24:R679–R681. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.06.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Wilson RC, Takahashi YK, Schoenbaum G, Niv Y. Orbitofrontal cortex as a cognitive map of task space. Neuron. 2014;81:267–279. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Woo CW, Schmidt L, Krishnan A, Jepma M, Roy M, Lindquist MA, Atlas LY, Wager TD. Quantifying cerebral contributions to pain beyond nociception. Nature Communications. 2017;8:14211. doi: 10.1038/ncomms14211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Wu CFJ. Jackknife, bootstrap and other resampling methods in regression analysis. The Annals of Statistics. 1986;14:0142. doi: 10.1214/aos/1176350142. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  118. Yanes JA, Akintola T. Parabrachial projections to PAG-RVM axis may promote placebo hypoalgesia and nocebo hyperalgesia. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2022;42:3302–3304. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2544-21.2022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Zhang S, Mano H, Ganesh G, Robbins T, Seymour B. Dissociable learning processes underlie human pain conditioning. Current Biology. 2016;26:52–58. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.10.066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Zubieta JK, Bueller JA, Jackson LR, Scott DJ, Xu Y, Koeppe RA, Nichols TE, Stohler CS. Placebo effects mediated by endogenous opioid activity on mu-opioid receptors. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:7754–7762. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0439-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Zunhammer M, Bingel U, Wager TD, Placebo Imaging Consortium Placebo effects on the neurologic pain signature: A meta-analysis of individual participant functional magnetic resonance imaging data. JAMA Neurology. 2018;75:1321–1330. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2018.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Editor's evaluation

Joshua Johansen 1

This fundamental paper advances our understanding of the commonalities and differences in the neural basis of directly experienced and instructed aversive learning in humans. The study uses compelling experimental design and analyses along with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to inform neuro-computational models of how explicitly informed vs experientially acquired information influences learning about cues predicting painful stimuli. This work will be of broad interest to neuroscientists interested in pain and aversive learning and memory.

Decision letter

Editor: Joshua Johansen1
Reviewed by: Christoph W Korn2

Our editorial process produces two outputs: (i) public reviews designed to be posted alongside the preprint for the benefit of readers; (ii) feedback on the manuscript for the authors, including requests for revisions, shown below. We also include an acceptance summary that explains what the editors found interesting or important about the work.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Instructions and experiential learning have similar impacts on pain and pain-related brain responses but produce dissociations in value-based reversal learning" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by 3 peer reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors, and the evaluation has been overseen by Christian Büchel as the Senior Editor. The following individual involved in review of your submission has agreed to reveal their identity: Christoph W Korn (Reviewer #3).

The reviewers have discussed their reviews with one another and feel that new analyses, clarification of methods and possibly new data are required to make a definitive assessment of the manuscript. The Reviewing Editor has drafted this to help you prepare a revised submission.

Essential revisions:

1) It appears that a limited number of subjects were used in the study and it is unclear from the description of the methods whether this is sufficient to draw strong conclusions. The authors need to address this point.

2) It is not clear how to interpret the findings that some brain regions don't follow reversals but maintain initial contingencies. The authors should explore in more detail a) what stimuli are actually driving these changes (e.g. CS, US, etc), b) possibly incorporate autonomic indices into the analyses to help clarify this and c) provide a better explanation of the methods and analyses used so that the reader can better understand them and interpret the findings.

3) The authors trained subjects with high and low pain intensities and then tested with a medium intensity stimulus to control for objective differences. The study would be improved by including an analysis which incorporates the different shock intensities particularly for fitting the Reinforcement Learning models.

Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

1) Although it appears that path b effects refer to brain activity associations with pain ratings, this should be more explicitly defined in the paper (possibly where it is introduced on line 668). For example, how was cue controlled for, how were reversals incorporated?

2) The authors show that there are differences in the brain regions mediating instructed vs. directly experienced learning (Figure 6). However, the regions responsible for this difference differ from those discovered in the senior author's previous work (Atlas et al. eLife 2016). The authors should discuss the reasons for these differences.

3) It is not clear whether their imaging approach can temporally differentiate cue from heat pain. The authors should discuss the details of what could be represented in the bold response (including analysis periods during cue and shock, temporal dynamics of the bold response, etc.). If the bold response to cues cannot be distinguished from heat stimulation this could explain differences between cortical and subcortical structures in how they reverse (or do not reverse) their representations when contingencies change. Furthermore, because the Atlas et al. 2016 paper used a design in which some cues were not followed by aversive stimulation, the differences they see in the representations here could be due to that.

4) The paper could be strengthened if the authors incorporated the autonomic measures which they said they don't report these here because they will use them in another paper. This may allow them to determine whether the subcortical regions which don't reverse are more closely related to autonomic indices of learning (rather than self-report), as they suggest in the introduction, and whether the differences between self-report and autonomic measures explain differences in the brain regions engaged during instructed vs. experiential learning between this paper and the group's past work.

Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

1. The sample size of N = 20 in each of the two groups seems rather low and raises the question whether the study was sufficiently powered. The small sample size is of particular concern because due to a programming error, data of only two instead of three reversals could be used in half of the instructed group (N = 10). It is mentioned in the methods section that the sample size is based on power analyses from previous studies but these studies are not further specified or referenced. The authors also state that the sample size is "deemed sufficient based on a behavioural pilot experiment" and refer to their clinical protocol registered at clinicaltrials.gov. Unfortunately, I was unable to find information about the pilot study, sample size and sample size calculation in the preregistration that would justify the low N. The site only seems to mention a sample size of N = 400 for a set of five studies but no breakdown for single studies. Please add this important information to the main manuscript.

2. In contrast to the very detailed and complex methods and Results sections, the discussion of the findings is very cautious and largely descriptive. I appreciate that the authors want to stay close to the data in their discussion, but the most relevant and surprising findings could be explored in more detail to add depth. As a minimum, the authors could offer potential explanations for their findings which could be tested in future studies. Please find below four examples of surprising findings that would benefit from deeper exploration/discussion:

– The PAG kept responding to initial contingencies following reversals. How does this relate to the role of this structure in fear learning and descending pain modulation?

– As evident from Figure 2C, cue effects show a sharp increase from trial 2 to trial 3 whereas trial 1 and 2 are almost identical. This pattern can be seen in both groups but seems to be more prominent in the instructed group. How do the authors interpret the difference between reversals and the difference between groups?

– The fact that pain ratings increased over time only in the instructed group is surprising. How does this relate to other findings of this group?

– Effects of predictive cues and reversals on pain-related brain responses are not significantly different between group, although pain ratings indicate a stronger effect in the instructed group (page 30). Please discuss this dissociation in more detail.

3. As mentioned in the introduction, the authors reported sustained amygdala engagement in response to original cues in one of their previous papers, indicating that this structure might maintain a representation of original contingencies. In the present study the amygdala does not seems to show this pattern. How do the authors explain this difference in findings? How are reversals in the current paradigm different from those previously studied?

4. Although peripheral-physiological data including pupil dilation, gaze position and EDA were recorded, they are not included in the manuscript because "they may be analyzed and reported separately in future work". This decision seems unfortunate as this data could potentially aid the more detailed interpretation of the findings (see point 2).

5. The instructions that were given to the two groups differ in one key aspect that is not discussed in the manuscript. While the instructed group was only informed about the meaning of the cues ("It is there just to let you know what level of heat will be next"), the uninstructed group was given a specific task ("Your job is to pay attention to the cues and try to figure out the relationship between the sounds that you hear and the heat that you feel."). To what extent could this task difference have influenced the results? Related to this issue, it is also worth noting that explicit information about reversals in the instructed group provided orientation to those who had lost track of contingencies because at least at the time point of the reversal they could be sure about the correct contingency. In contrast, the uninstructed group was not given an external reference throughout the whole experiment.

6. If I understand the methods section correctly, the authors used a 100% reinforcement rate during the conditioning phase. In the subsequent test phase, each cue was followed by medium pain and reinforcement was lowered to 50%. Were participants informed about the change in reinforcement rate or was there a break between conditioning and test phase? Please clarify.

7. The figures are very small and difficult to read. Please particularly check the violin plots in Figure 4 and 5 where parts of the figure legend seem to be missing.

Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

Below, I list a few suggestions for improvement.

1. Focus on stimuli of medium intensity: The authors nicely control for objective pain intensity by focusing on analyzing pain stimuli of medium intensity. However, I would find it very interesting if the reported pattern of results – especially the fit of the RW model variants – holds if stimuli of all intensities are included in the analyses.

a. Specifically, in the uninstructed group, the exact temporal succession of stimuli of high and low intensity should inform stimulus-by-stimulus learning. Maybe I just did not grasp correctly, which trials were included for fitting.

b. As a sanity check, it would be nice to see the ratings and also the fMRI signals for all received intensities. I understand that the medium intensity stimuli control for objective differences – but still it would be nice to see the reflection of these objective differences in behavior and fMRI signals.

c. By including all stimuli, the authors could also look into prediction errors (PEs) and into the expectations during the cue phase. The authors note that the study design was not optimized for such analyses and that PEs and expectations are negatively correlated. But I am wondering if this profoundly precludes such analyses – even though these would be exploratory.

2. Physiological data: The authors note that they might look into those data in the future. A slightly more nuanced explanation of the reasons for refraining from analyzing the physiological data might be helpful (especially given that the authors have experience with such analyses).

3. Brain responses that are unaffected by reversals: In my view, this is an interesting an unexpected finding (see e.g., Figure 5). Can it be excluded that these signals do not "just" relate to the differences between the cues in a rather unspecific way? Can it be that the first reversal induces a second "context" and that there are brain signals (maybe in the same regions) that "remain stable" in this new "switched context?" Relatedly, the authors might want to conjecture whether such "stable" responses would become unstable with longer learning periods in the switched context. I am rather unfamiliar with the role of these regions for pain perception.

4. Learning rates of the instructed group: I find the wording regarding the learning rate a bit confusing. It sounds a bit as if the authors did not expect the learning rates to be low (or even zero). But given the explicit instructions and the nice RW model variant, this is exactly how they should look. The instructions say everything and no learning is needed. Testing instructions that are incorrect (as might be the case for some types of placebo/nocebo instructions) would be really interesting here; i.e., participants get information but still have to learn from experience (in spite of the received information). Maybe the authors have that idea in their mind when reporting their findings about the learning rates of the instructed group.

eLife. 2022 Nov 1;11:e73353. doi: 10.7554/eLife.73353.sa2

Author response


Essential revisions:

1) It appears that a limited number of subjects were used in the study and it is unclear from the description of the methods whether this is sufficient to draw strong conclusions. The authors need to address this point.

We now include further information on our power analyses and choice of sample size. Please see Response 2.1 below.

2) It is not clear how to interpret the findings that some brain regions don't follow reversals but maintain initial contingencies. The authors should explore in more detail a) what stimuli are actually driving these changes (e.g. CS, US, etc), b) possibly incorporate autonomic indices into the analyses to help clarify this and c) provide a better explanation of the methods and analyses used so that the reader can better understand them and interpret the findings.

We will briefly address each of these editorial suggestions, which are covered in greater detail in individual responses below.

a) To determine which stimuli are driving responses, we provide additional data to distinguish responses to the CS (visual cue) from responses to the US (i.e. heat delivery) in Figure 5 —figure supplements 3 and 5 and Figure 6 —figure supplement 3, although we acknowledge some constraints due to task timing and address these in our revised Discussion. Based on the timecourse of extracted data within regions identified as showing differential cue effects (i.e. Path A in either mediation analysis), differential responses emerge at the time of thermal stimulation, rather than in response to the cue. The only regions that seem to be sensitive to cue during the anticipatory interval are the amygdala regions of interest (ROIs). In our revised discussion, we now address the role of the amygdala and encourage future studies to specifically evaluate effects of instructions on amygdala responses to both the CS and US. Thus while it is the integration of CS and US that generates value signals and updates based on prediction errors, we believe the most important differences based on expectancy are present at the time of heat stimulation / US outcome. We address this further below in response to points 1.2 and 1.3.

We have also examined regions that were identified as maintaining original contingencies in more detail by inspecting timecourses and trial-wise responses in these regions (see Figure 6 —figure supplements 2 and 3). Of regions identified in these analyses, only the rostroventral medulla (RVM) shows clear sensitivity to initial contingencies on both original and reversed runs in both groups across participants. We also observe heterogeneity within the VMPFC (i.e. some portions that reverse, and others that do not), which we address in the discussion. For additional discussion on findings in the RVM, please see response 2.2.

b) We have also incorporated autonomic indices (skin conductance [SCR], pupil dilation) as suggested by all three reviewers. We focus primarily on SCR, as we only had useable pupillary data from 20 participants. Quantitative models fit to SCR were largely consistent with models fit to pain ratings, and analyses of anticipatory SCR were largely inconclusive. Specific results are discussed below in response 1.4.

c) We have provided more detail on the methods and analyses throughout the manuscript, as discussed in individual responses below. We hope this clarifies the details of our analyses and aids interpretation.

3) The authors trained subjects with high and low pain intensities and then tested with a medium intensity stimulus to control for objective differences. The study would be improved by including an analysis which incorporates the different shock intensities particularly for fitting the Reinforcement Learning models.

We have clarified in our revision that all heat intensities are included in the reinforcement learning model but that the model’s goodness-of-fit is based on the medium trials since these trials would show the strongest modulation by expected value, as the stimulus intensity is fixed. these are modified strictly by expected value. In addition, we evaluated a model in which goodness-of-fit was evaluated across all temperatures. Findings were largely consistent with models fit to medium heat trials alone. Please see Response 3.1 for complete information.

Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

1) Although it appears that path b effects refer to brain activity associations with pain ratings, this should be more explicitly defined in the paper (possibly where it is introduced on line 668). For example, how was cue controlled for, how were reversals incorporated?

The Reviewer is correct that Path b effects refer to brain activity associations with pain ratings when controlling for cue, which is different from a standard brain-behavior analysis. This is done in the context of mediation, which simultaneously evaluates partial regression for all paths. We did not initially include this information because it has been discussed in previous papers using this approach, but we have provided additional information about Path b on page 56 as suggested by the reviewer.

Reversals are incorporated as a function of how we modeled cue, i.e. in the first mediation model we assume cue values reverse entirely upon reversal whereas in the second mediation model we model Cue as the X variable and include reversals as a covariate. In both cases, Path b evaluates positive associations with subjective pain controlling for cue. We note that we also control for temperature as we evaluate associations only within medium heat trials.

To clarify this point, we provided additional information in the Methods section regarding multilevel mediation and how each path is computed. We made the following changes (additions in red):

– P. 18: “Path b identified voxels that were associated with subjective pain while controlling for cue (see Methods).”

– P. 55: “Mediation was implemented by the Matlab function mediation.m (https://canlab.github.io/). Cue was included as the input variable (i.e. X; coded as 1 for High cue, -1 for Low cue), pain was included as the output variable (i.e. Y), and we searched for potential mediators. Voxelwise mediation, or mediation effect parametric mapping (Wager et al., 2009; Atlas et al., 2010) yields interpretable maps that are similar to simultaneous partial regressions, although implemented using mixed effects. The mediation effect (a*b) identifies regions whose activity contributes to variance in the effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable (Path c; for individual i, trial j: (1) Yij = d0j + cjXij + e0ij). Path a denotes the effect of the input variable on the potential mediator (M; brain activation in a given voxel), thereby representing cue effects on brain responses to medium heat (i.e. (2) Mij = d1j + ajXij + e1ij). Path b measures the association between the mediator and outcome, controlling for the input variable. Here, this represents brain regions that predict pain, controlling for cue type (i.e. (3) Yij = d2j + cj' Xij + bi Mij + e2ij). In multilevel mediation, the difference between the total effect (Path c: the effect of cues on subjective pain) and the direct effect (Path c`: the effect of cues on subjective pain when controlling for the mediator; see equation 3) is equivalent to the sum of the product of Path a and Path b coefficients and their covariance (Shrout and Bolger, 2002; Kenny et al., 2003). Our mixed models included intercepts (d0-2) and error terms (e0-2) for each individual. Group level estimates are computed by treating person-level intercepts and slopes as random effects. For complete details, see Atlas et al., 2010.”

– P. 56: “We focus in particular on effects of current contingencies (i.e. Cue x Phase interactions) on pain to capture the behavioral reversals we observed (Figure 4). In this main analysis, we coded the input variable (X) as the contrast between current high cues (i.e. original high cues during original contingencies and original low cues during reversed contingencies both coded as 1) and current low cues (i.e. original low cues during original contingencies and original high cues during reversed contingencies both coded as -1). Our second mediation searched for mediators of original cue effects (i.e. original high cue coded as 1, original low cue coded as -1) and controlled for current contingencies by including the X variable from the current cue contingencies mediation (i.e. the Cue x Phase interaction) as a covariate in all paths. These effects are thus most likely to be driven by responses during the reversed runs. In both mediations, Path b evaluates the link between brain activation and pain, while controlling for the cue (see equation 2 above).”

2) The authors show that there are differences in the brain regions mediating instructed vs. directly experienced learning (Figure 6). However, the regions responsible for this difference differ from those discovered in the senior author's previous work (Atlas et al. eLife 2016). The authors should discuss the reasons for these differences.

Thank you for encouraging us to compare our current findings with our previous work in the domain of fear conditioning. We note that there are both similarities and differences with our previous findings. As noted in point 1.3 below, the biggest difference is that our previous work studied responses to the CS on unreinforced trials, whereas our current study tests responses to the painful outcome itself. Thus our 2016 study isolates mechanisms of aversive learning, but this study tests the consequences of learning on perception and pain-related responses. We think both are important for the fields of learning, predictive coding, pain, and perception. We have been more explicit about this point on pages 34 and 37.

Despite this methodological and theoretical difference, we observe both similarities and differences between the present study and our prior work. In both studies, instructions altered value-based processing in the striatum and OFC/VMPFC, as well as the DLPFC, rACC, ACC, and insula, key nodes implicated in pain modulation. We have added additional discussion to highlight the similarities, as this provides further evidence that the striatum and VMPFC/OFC reverse based on instruction, consistent with our previous work and several studies of appetitive learning.

While our initial submission was agnostic as to the role of the amygdala in instructed or uninstructed learning, the additional analysis of unsigned prediction error suggested by Reviewer 3 (see point 3.1.c) revealed new findings indicating that there were associations between unsigned PE and amygdala responses to heat in both groups, including the Instructed Group, in which the timecourse of learning updated with instructions. This is distinct from our previous work, in which we only examined EV (since EV was correlated with PE) and extends findings from other studies that indicate unsigned PEs in the amygdala in the absence of instruction (Boll et al., 2012). We now discuss these differences in depth on pp. 36-37.

We have expanded our discussion on the similarities and differences between these studies and implications for our understanding of the amygdala’s response to instructions in the revised discussion:

– Pp. 34-37, added: “In addition to regions implicated in prior studies of placebo and pain modulation, we evaluated the contribution of systems involved in value-based learning, namely the striatum, OFC/VMPFC, and the amygdala. We previously observed dissociations in these regions during aversive learning (Atlas et al., 2016), such that responses in the striatum and OFC/VMPFC tracked EV that updated with instructions, whereas the right amygdala was associated with EV based purely on experiential learning. The present study builds on this work by focusing heat-evoked responses (i.e. the US in a fear conditioning model), rather than anticipation (i.e. responses to the CS), which allows us to test whether error-based learning shapes responses to noxious stimuli and whether this learning is modulated by instructions. ROI-based analyses indicated that the bilateral striatum was associated with EV that updated based on instruction in the Instructed Group, consistent with our previous findings (Atlas et al., 2013). VMPFC/OFC associations with value were also sensitive to instructions, although we observed different patterns across the broad region and did not observe consistent findings in ROI-based or corrected analyses. For instance, uncorrected voxelwise analyses revealed that the association between EV and VMPFC activation differed between groups (positive associations with EV in the Instructed Group and negative associations with EV in the Uninstructed Group), regardless of whether models were fit to pain, heat-evoked SCR, or anticipatory SCR. In contrast, we observed negative associations with EV in the left lateral OFC in both groups. Both portions of the VMPFC/OFC have been implicated in previous studies of placebo analgesia (Lieberman et al., 2004; Wager, 2004; Petrovic et al., 2010; Ellingsen et al., 2013), and we previously showed that pain-predictive cues modulate responses in both regions, with greater activation when medium heat is paired with low pain cues than high pain cue (Atlas et al., 2010). Lateral OFC is thought to evaluate value of independent options, whereas medial OFC and VMPFC have been linked to value-based choice for the purpose of comparison (Rudebeck and Murray, 2014). We recently showed that surgical lesions of the bilateral VMPFC enhance instructed cue effects on pain in a stable environment (Motzkin et al., 2021) additional studies that use lesion symptom mapping in heterogeneous frontal samples or measure aversive learning in nonhuman primates can provide further insights on the contribution of different portions of the VMPFC/OFC to pain modulation and instructed learning. Together, these findings build on previous work indicating that the striatum and VMPFC update upon instruction in both appetitive and aversive learning (Li et al., 2011a; Atlas et al., 2016) and that OFC value signals are sensitive to higher order knowledge across species (Wilson et al., 2014; Lucantonio et al., 2015; Schuck et al., 2018).

Because we focused on responses to the US rather than the CS here, we also were able to extend previous work by examining the role of unsigned prediction errors (PEs), which are linked to associability, which gates attention and learning dynamically. Associability has been linked to the amygdala in previous studies of pain and aversive learning (Li et al., 2011; Zhang et al., 2016; Atlas et al., 2019), meta-analyses indicate that unsigned PEs are associated with activation in the dorsal and ventral striatum, as well as other regions (Corlett et al., 2022), and one study of aversive learning showed responses to both associability and unsigned PE in the amygdala (Boll et al., 2012). Consistent with these studies, we observed associations between unsigned PE and activation in the bilateral amygdala and the right striatum in both groups in ROI-based analyses. Whole brain analyses revealed associations between unsigned PEs and activation in the right hemisphere encompassing the insula, striatum, and amygdala, and that the associations between PE and activation in the right SII and temporal cortex differed across groups (negative associations in the Instructed Group and positive in the Uninstructed Group). Thus, although cue effects on subjective pain were mediated similarly across groups, we still observe unique responses to PE that differed between groups.

We note that the amygdala’s association with PE within the Instructed Group indicates that instructions do have some impact on amygdala activation, seemingly in contrast to our previous work on instructed learning during fear conditioning (Atlas et al., 2016; Atlas 2019). However, because error-driven learning was nearly absent between instructed reversals in the Instructed Group, the PE regressor in this group captures a constant effect that decreases linearly over time, consistent with prior evidence that amygdala responses habituate during aversive learning (Büchel et al., 1998). Thus we did not observe evidence of amygdala sensitivity to instructions per se in the present study. The absence of pure experiential learning signals in the right amygdala may reflect the present study’s focus on BOLD responses to noxious heat (i.e. unconditioned stimuli), in contrast to our previous study (Atlas et al., 2016) which focused on responses to unreinforced cues (i.e. conditioned stimuli).” [See additional edits provided in response to point 1.3, below]

3) It is not clear whether their imaging approach can temporally differentiate cue from heat pain. The authors should discuss the details of what could be represented in the bold response (including analysis periods during cue and shock, temporal dynamics of the bold response, etc.). If the bold response to cues cannot be distinguished from heat stimulation this could explain differences between cortical and subcortical structures in how they reverse (or do not reverse) their representations when contingencies change. Furthermore, because the Atlas et al. 2016 paper used a design in which some cues were not followed by aversive stimulation, the differences they see in the representations here could be due to that.

We agree with the Reviewer and have acknowledged the difference in study design (i.e. focus on US vs focus on CS; lack of intermittent reinforcement) as a likely reason for why we see differences between the previous work and the present study, as we discuss above in point 1.2. We use a slow event related analysis to facilitate single trial analysis and attempt to separate responses to heat from responses to cues but cannot rule out the possibility that anticipatory processes might contribute to differences observed during heat, since we use a fixed anticipatory interval. We chose not to jitter the duration of anticipation because previous studies indicate that longer anticipation periods are associated with increased dread, which can in turn modulate responses to aversive outcomes (Berns et al., 2006).

To explore whether anticipatory responses contributed to our findings, we extracted raw timecourses for regions identified as showing cue-based differences in responses to medium heat (i.e. Path a in either mediation analysis) as well as a priori regions of interest. Most regions that were identified as showing cue-based modulation during medium heat showed differences based on cue only after heat onset, and usually late in the heat period (see Figure 5 —figure supplement 3 and Figure 6 —figure supplement 3). Within value-related ROIs (Figure 5 —figure supplement 5), cue-based differences in the striatum and VMPFC emerge at the time of heat, rather than cue, whereas the amygdala shows some sensitivity to cue prior to heat onset. We therefore believe that our findings are unlikely to be driven by responses to cues per se.

We agree with the Reviewer that the focus on heat rather than cue, as well as the intermittent reinforcement used in our prior paper, can account for some of our findings, as we discuss in revisions provided in response 1.2 above. We made the following additional changes to address this point in the revision:

Supplemental Figures: Added raw timecourses relative to cue onset for regions that showed differential responses to determine specificity to heat vs anticipation.

P. 37, added: “In addition, our experimental design was optimized for analyses of the heat period rather than the cue (i.e. we used a fixed anticipation period to avoid variations in dread that might accompany jittered anticipatory intervals (Berns et al., 2006)), so we were unable to separately analyze responses to pain-predictive cues to determine whether the amygdala showed similar responses in the present study. We note that raw timecourses suggest that cue-based differences in the amygdala may emerge in response to predictive cues, as well as during the heat period (see Figure 5 —figure supplement 5). This contrasts with the bilateral striatum and nearly all regions identified in mediation analyses, in which cue-based differences do not emerge until the heat period. Future work should incorporate longer delays between cues and aversive outcomes or utilize intermittent reinforcement (e.g. catch trials without heat presentation) to separately analyze responses to cues and noxious heat in the amgydala, VMPFC, and other key regions.”

4) The paper could be strengthened if the authors incorporated the autonomic measures which they said they don't report these here because they will use them in another paper. This may allow them to determine whether the subcortical regions which don't reverse are more closely related to autonomic indices of learning (rather than self-report), as they suggest in the introduction, and whether the differences between self-report and autonomic measures explain differences in the brain regions engaged during instructed vs. experiential learning between this paper and the group's past work.

We note that our initial decision to omit analyses of concurrent autonomic data was informed by initial editorial comments that the paper risked being too complicated. Nonetheless, we have analyzed autonomic data that was collected during the fMRI experiment based on the Reviewers’ and Editors’ suggestions.

Pupil dilation: Following quality control assessment, we had useable pupillometry data from 20 participants (9 in Instructed Group) and an average of 54.4% of trials per participant. We attribute the limited useable data to technical issues, as our scanner’s head coil obscured the eye for many participants. Nonetheless, for participants with useable data, we computed baseline-corrected mean pupil dilation (PD) and area under the curve for each trial, and analyzed effects of Temperature, Cue, Phase, and Group on pupillary response during the heat period. Both outcomes were significantly influenced by Temperature across all trials, but there were no influences of any experimental factors, and effects of Temperature were insufficient to reject the null hypothesis based on Bayesian models (see Figure 2 – Source data 1). We report these findings in the revised manuscript on page 10, as delineated below, but did not analyze PD further due to power limitations.

Skin conductance responses: To analyze skin conductance data, we used Ledalab’s continuous decomposition analysis (CDA), which accounts for phasic and tonic signals and can capture multiple responses during the 8-second heat period (Benedek and Kaernbach, 2010). We focused on responses that occurred between heat onset and 4s after offset to capture heat-evoked responses, and responses between cue-onset and 4s later as anticipatory responses. We used the average phasic driver (“SCR”) in all trial-wise analyses. Analyses were limited to participants who had greater than 4 SCR responses for each analysis and thus sample sizes varied across analyses.

Thirty-six participants were included in analyses across temperatures (18 in Instructed Group). We observed significant effects of Temperature and all experimental factors on heat-evoked SCR (see Figure 2 and Figure 2 – Source data 1). We therefore analyzed effects of Cue and Phase on responses to medium heat. Thirty participants were included in analyses (13 in Instructed Group). Heat-evoked SCRs were influenced by predictive cues on medium trials and reversed as contingencies changed, but the magnitude of these differences did not differ by Group (see Figure 4 and Figure 4 – Source data 1), in contrast to subjective pain ratings, which showed larger reversals in the Instructed Group. These effects were statistically significant, but of undecided significance in Bayesian models. We report these results in the revised manuscript (see below for specific changes). Because only 13 Instructed Group participants had variations in heat-evoked SCR on medium heat trials, we take these results with caution and did not analyze associations between trial-by-trial SCR and brain responses.

We also examined anticipatory SCR by analyzing cue-evoked responses, i.e. responses following cue presentation, before heat onset. 29 participants were included in the analysis (15 Instructed Group, 14 Uninstructed Group), which included all trials since responses should be unrelated to subsequent temperature. We analyzed cue-evoked SCR as a function of Cue, Group, and Phase. Like heat-evoked SCR, the magnitude of cue-evoked SCR did not differ by group (see Figure 4 – Source data 1). However, in contrast to heat-evoked SCRs, we did not observe any impact of contingency reversals: Anticipatory responses were shaped by original contingencies consistently across time, as evidenced by a main effect of Cue without a significant interaction with Phase (all p’s > 0.8, see Figure 4 – Source data 1). However, there was not sufficient evidence to reject the null hypothesis based on Bayesian models. Although we therefore avoid making inference based on these findings, we acknowledge that this mirrors results of our secondary mediation analysis, in which cues shaped responses in the RVM but did not vary as contingencies changed.

We conducted an exploratory analysis to test for associations between trial-by-trial anticipatory SCR and subsequent brain response to medium heat. Twenty-one subjects had variation in cue-induced SCR on medium trials and were included in this analysis (11 in Instructed Group); we therefore did not explore potential group differences due to the low sample sizes. We did not observe any association between anticipatory SCR and subsequent heat-evoked activation in value-related ROIs, pain signature patterns, or within regions involved in pain and placebo. Whole brain correction did reveal one cluster of activation in left SII, and we observed additional regions in uncorrected voxelwise results, including positive associations in the right ventral striatum (but no other subcortical regions); uncorrected results are presented within Author response table 1. Due to the limited sample size and failure to observe any associations in a priori regions of interest or corrected analyses, as well as the fact that Bayesian analyses supported the null hypothesis of no effect of experimental factors on anticipatory arousal, we have decided not to include this analysis in the revision.

Author response table 1.

Contrast Region x y z voxels Volume (mm3)
Positive association with anticipatory SCR R Caudate Nucleus (Nucleus Accumbens) 10 10 -8 6 162
Negative association with anticipatory SCR R Superior Temporal Gyrus (Area TE 3) 62 -10 -4 104 2808
L operculum -56 14 -2 32 864
R Middle Occipital Gyrus (Area hOc4la) 46 -74 4 83 2241
L Superior Temporal Gyrus (Area TE 3) -62 -32 16 200 5400
L Insula Lobe (Area Ig2) -38 -20 14 33 891
R Superior Temporal Gyrus 50 -40 14 25 675
L Postcentral Gyrus (Area 3b) -44 -16 46 44 1188
L dACC -8 -4 44 19 513
R Postcentral Gyrus (Area 4p) 34 -28 46 15 405
L Posterior-Medial Frontal -2 -8 68 38 1026

Fitting models to SCR: While the sparse SCR data limited our ability to directly measure associations between SCR and brain activation, our quantitative models may be less impacted by the missing data as our iterative jackknife approach leverages responses across all participants. We therefore fit quantitative models to (a) heat-evoked SCR on medium heat trials and (b) cue-evoked SCR on all trials. We included all participants in the analysis and used a jack-knife approach with the same model reported in the main manuscript.

Results from fits to heat-evoked SCR were largely consistent with our findings when we fit models to pain reports: Expected value updated upon instruction in the Instructed Group (Mrho = 0.79, SD = 0.024) without any additional learning as a function of stimulus outcome pairings (Malpha = 0; SD = 0), and learning rates were higher in Uninstructed Participants (Uninstructed Group: Malpha = 0.40, SD = 0.09; t(36) = -98.43, p <.001), whereas the instructed reversal parameter was higher in the Instructed Group (Uninstructed Group: Mrho = 0.09, SD = 0.04; t(36) = 64.48, p <.001). Next, we measured the association between brain responses to medium intensity heat and the dynamic timecourse of expected value (EV) based on the model. We observed group differences in associations with EV in the DMPFC, overlapping with the region identified when we fit models to pain reports (see Figure 8 —figure supplement 2). Furthermore, although there were no other regions in which group differences or main effects survived multiple comparisons correction, uncorrected results were nearly identical to the regions that showed group differences and main effects when we fit quantitative models to pain ratings. Thus fitting quantitative models to heat-evoked SCR did not reveal any meaningful findings beyond what we discovered based on models fit to pain reports. We now briefly report these results in the manuscript’s supplementary materials.

We also fit the same models to cue-evoked SCR, under the assumption that anticipatory arousal indexes EV on a given trial. There were no group differences in the instructed reversal parameter (Instructed Group: Mrho = 0.23, SD = 0.13; Uninstructed Group: Mrho = 0.28, SD = 0.15; p > 0.2) but a significant difference in learning rates again driven by faster learning in Uninstructed Group participants (Instructed Group: Malpha = 0.33, SD = 0.44; Uninstructed Group: Malpha = 0.79, SD = 0.42; t(36) = -3.28, p = .002). We searched for regions that displayed correlations with EV based on fits to cue-evoked SCR. Although no regions survived multiple comparisons correction whether we searched for consistent associations across groups or group differences in associations with EV, we again observed largely overlapping findings in uncorrected analyses, as shown in Figure 8 —figure supplement 2. All three models revealed group differences in the VMPFC and the left putamen, regardless of whether EV was fit to pain, heat-evoked SCR, or anticipatory SCR. We report these findings in supplementary materials and include them in the discussion, as they mirror our previous findings in which learning-related responses in the striatum and OFC updated with instruction (Atlas et al., 2016) and builds on similar findings in reward learning (Li et al., 2011, Doll et al., 2009).

In summary, our results using heat-evoked SCR as an outcome in quantitative models are largely consistent with dynamic models fit to pain, while analyses of cue-evoked SCR suggest preliminary support that anticipatory arousal is sensitive to initial contingencies throughout the task, but are not sufficient to reject the null hypothesis. Interestingly, our investigation of SCR did not provide additional insights into the amygdala or other subcortical regions. However, the additional analysis of unsigned prediction error does provide further insight on the role of the amygdala (see Response 3.1.c, below).

We have reported the full results of these analyses here for Reviewers for full transparency in response to all three Reviewers’ suggestions and editorial comments and have included results of models fit to SCR in Supplementary Materials. We take these findings as highly exploratory, however, and want to be quite cautious with interpreting these results, as (a) the number of participants that showed measurable SCR in either period is limited, particularly in the Instructed Group; (b) Bayesian analyses indicate that these effects have limited practical significance; (c) Ledalab’s continuous decomposition analysis is only one of many different approaches to measuring SCR, and we are now formally comparing different approaches across datasets to identify the most sensitive measure to changes in noxious stimulation and associations with pain, which will be reported in a separate paper.

We made the following changes:

– Updated Figures 2 and 4 to include effects on heat-evoked SCR

– Updated Figure 8 —figure supplements 1, 2, 4, and 5 and associated Source Data to report associations between brain activation and computational models fit to SCR

– P. 8, additions in red: “Heat intensity effects on pain, autonomic responses, and brain responses to noxious heat are similar across groups.”

– P. 10, added: “Next, we analyzed heat-evoked autonomic responses during the experiment. SCR and pupil dilation were both significantly influenced by Heat Intensity and exhibited Heat Intensity x Cue X Phase interactions (see Figure 2 – Source Data 1). Both factors had practically significant effects on SCR (< 1% in ROPE), whereas Bayesian analyses of pupillary outcomes indicated that evidence was not sufficient to reject the null hypothesis (100% in ROPE). Because there was no meaningful effect of temperature on pupil dilation and the number of subjects with useable pupil data was substantially less than those with useable skin conductance, we focused on SCR in subsequent analyses of cue effects on physiological arousal. There was no main effect of Group on pupil dilation or SCR, nor any interactions between Group and Heat Intensity for either outcome, suggesting that temperature effects on physiological arousal were similar regardless of whether individuals were instructed about contingencies (see Figure 2B and Figure 2 – Source Data 1). For complete results, see Figure 2 – Source Data 1.”

– P. 14, additions in red: “Cue-based expectations and cue effects on pain and SCR update as contingencies reverse.”

– Pp. 15-16, added: “We also tested whether cues and reversals impacted physiological responses to medium heat, as measured by heat-evoked SCR. Heat-evoked SCRs were influenced by predictive cues on medium trials and reversed as contingencies changed, but the magnitude of these differences did not differ by Group (see Figure 4D and Figure 4 – Source data 1). While effects were statistically significant based on frequentist models, they were not sufficient to reject the null hypothesis of no difference based on Bayesian models (see Figure 4 – Source data 1). Importantly, only 13 Instructed Group participants had variations in heat-evoked SCR on medium heat trials and were included in analyses; we therefore take these results with caution and did not analyze associations between trial-by-trial SCR and brain responses. We also evaluated cue effects on anticipatory SCR, i.e. responses to the cue in the interval prior to heat stimulation, in exploratory analyses. In contrast to other outcomes, anticipatory arousal was associated with a main effect of Cue and a significant Group x Phase interaction (see Figure 4 – Source data 1), but we did not observe any interactions between Cue and Phase, suggesting that anticipatory responses did not vary as contingencies change. However, analyses were limited to 29 participants and Bayesian analyses indicated that the data support the null hypothesis of no effect, and thus we do not make inference based on anticipatory arousal.”

– Pp. 26-27, added: “Fitting models to heat-evoked autonomic responses revealed similar patterns of activation based on whole-brain and ROI-based correction (see Figure 8 —figure supplement 4 and Figure 8 – Source Data 3), and while group differences did not survive correction when we fit models to anticipatory SCR (see Figure 8 —figure supplement 5 and Figure 8 – Source Data 4), we observed group differences in associations with EV in overlapping portions of the VMPFC and left putamen at uncorrected thresholds in all three models (see Figure 8 —figure supplement 2 and Figure 8 – Source Data 2-4). For complete results of models fit to SCR, see Figure 8 —figure supplements 4-5 and Figure 8 – Source Data 3-4.”

– P. 35, added: “uncorrected voxelwise analyses revealed that the association between EV and VMPFC activation differed between groups (positive associations with EV in the Instructed Group and negative associations with EV in the Uninstructed Group), regardless of whether models were fit to pain, heat-evoked SCR, or anticipatory SCR.”

– P. 39, added: “In exploratory analyses, we measured the association between anticipatory responses to the cues themselves and brain responses to noxious heat, however we did not observe associations between anticipatory arousal and responses within a priori networks. We also found that quantitative models fit to heat-evoked and anticipatory SCR were highly similar to models fit to subjective pain. However, as the number of participants with useable skin conductance or pupil dilation data was extremely limited due to technical malfunctions and variations in arousal, we consider these findings exploratory and do not make strong claims based on these results. Future studies should continue to compare the effects of learning, instructions, and expectations on subjective pain with effects on physiological outcome measures to determine whether autonomic responses to noxious stimuli are shaped by pain decision-making (Mischkowski et al., 2019) or whether physiological responses are shaped through independent pathways, e.g. through a dual process model (Ohman and Soares, 1993; Mineka and Ohman, 2002).”

– P. 42, added: “EDA was collected using Biopac’s EDA100C-MRI module (Biopac Systems, Inc, Goleta, CA). For each participant, two pre-gelled EDA electrodes (EL509; Biopac Systems, Inc, Goleta, CA) were prepared and applied to the hypothenar muscles of the left hand.”

– Pp. 48-49, added: “Psychophysiological data processing. Skin conductance data was preprocessed in AcqKnowledge (Biopac Systems, Inc, Goleta, CA). During preprocessing, data were smoothed (1000-sample Gaussian smoothing kernel) and filtered (25-Hz FIR low-pass filter) in AcqKnowledge, then imported into MATLAB and downsampled to 250 Hz. Data were then analyzed using Ledalab’s continuous decomposition analysis (CDA), which accounts for phasic and tonic signals and can capture multiple responses during the 8-second heat period (Benedek and Kaernbach, 2010). We analyzed both (a) cue-evoked anticipatory responses that occurred within 4s following cue presentation; and (b) responses that occurred between heat onset and 4s after heat offset. We used the average phasic driver (“SCR”) in trial-wise analyses. Participants were included in analyses if they had >4 trials with measurable SCR. Thirty-six participants were included in analyses across temperatures (18 in Instructed Group); 30 participants were included in analyses of responses to medium heat (13 in Instructed Group); and 29 participants were included in analyses of anticipatory responses (15 in Instructed Group).

Pupillometry data were processed in MATLAB using both publicly available software and custom code. Data were imported to MATLAB and blinks were interpolated using the “GazeVisToolbox” (available at https://github.com/djangraw/GazeVisToolbox; (Jangraw et al., 2014)). Consistent with our previous work (Mischkowski et al., 2019), we interpolated from 100ms prior to each blink to 100ms following each blink to avoid extreme values surrounding each blink. Data were aligned to event markers and we visualized individual trials to exclude trials that were contaminated by artifacts. Subjects with fewer than seven useable trials (n = 20; 9 in Instructed Group) were excluded from analyses of pupillary data. Useable subjects had a mean of 42.9 useable trials (M = 54.4% of trials). 1000-Hz data was downsampled to 10-Hz and then we computed baseline-corrected mean pupil dilation and area-under the curve as measures of pupillary response during the heat period.”

– P. 49, additions in red: “Statistical analysis of expectations, pain, autonomic responses, and heat-evoked neural signature pattern expression. We used the statistical software R (R Core Team, 1996) to analyze effects of our experimental manipulations on expectancy ratings, pain reports, physiological arousal,”

– P. 53, added: “We used the same approach to fit computational models to anticipatory SCR and SCR evoked during medium heat simulation.”

– P. 57, added: “Our main manuscript focuses on the timecourse of EV based on fits to pain reports; we include associations based on fits to heat-evoked and anticipatory SCR in Supplementary Materials.”

Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

1. The sample size of N = 20 in each of the two groups seems rather low and raises the question whether the study was sufficiently powered. The small sample size is of particular concern because due to a programming error, data of only two instead of three reversals could be used in half of the instructed group (N = 10). It is mentioned in the methods section that the sample size is based on power analyses from previous studies but these studies are not further specified or referenced. The authors also state that the sample size is "deemed sufficient based on a behavioural pilot experiment" and refer to their clinical protocol registered at clinicaltrials.gov. Unfortunately, I was unable to find information about the pilot study, sample size and sample size calculation in the preregistration that would justify the low N. The site only seems to mention a sample size of N = 400 for a set of five studies but no breakdown for single studies. Please add this important information to the main manuscript.

We are pleased that the Reviewer took the time to review our protocol and apologize that this information was not readily available. Indeed, we state in our IRB-approved clinical protocol that all studies use a sample size of 20 per group, unless power analyses based on pilot behavioral participants indicate that the behavioral effect requires more than 20 participants to observe the effect. In Author response table 2 present the data from the pilot subjects (n = 6 per group), which suggested we need a sample of 5-9 per group to achieve adequate power to observe the behavioral difference in cue effects on pain. We therefore moved forward with 20 participants per group. We have added this information to the main manuscript:

Author response table 2.

N LM mean (SD) HM mean (SD) HM-LM mean (SD) HM & LM correlation Standardized effect size (cohen’s dz) Repeated measures effect size (drepeated measures, pooled) Required sample size
All participants 12 3.05
(1.17)
3.62 (1.23) 0.57 (0.36) 0.91 1.57 1.60 6 for 80% power;8 for 95% power
Instructed Group 6 3.21 (0.79) 3.87 (0.66) 0.66 (0.45) 0.82 1.47 1.52 6 for 80% power;9 for 95% power
Uninstructed Group 6 2.88 (1.53) 3.37 (1.65) 0.48 (0.26) 0.99 1.83 2.07 5 for 80% power;7 for 95% power

– P. 41: “As detailed in our clinical protocol (15-AT-0132; clinicaltrials.gov identifier NCT02446262), sample size was based on power analyses from our previous studies and on a behavioral pilot experiment conducted prior to the fMRI study. We computed the effect size of cue-based differences in reported pain in a sample of 12 participants (6 per group) and determined that we need a minimum of 5-9 participants to achieve 80%-95% power to detect cue-based differences in pain, including reversals (data available upon request). We therefore included 20 participants per group in the fMRI experiment.”

2. In contrast to the very detailed and complex methods and Results sections, the discussion of the findings is very cautious and largely descriptive. I appreciate that the authors want to stay close to the data in their discussion, but the most relevant and surprising findings could be explored in more detail to add depth. As a minimum, the authors could offer potential explanations for their findings which could be tested in future studies. Please find below four examples of surprising findings that would benefit from deeper exploration/discussion:

– The PAG kept responding to initial contingencies following reversals. How does this relate to the role of this structure in fear learning and descending pain modulation?

– As evident from Figure 2C, cue effects show a sharp increase from trial 2 to trial 3 whereas trial 1 and 2 are almost identical. This pattern can be seen in both groups but seems to be more prominent in the instructed group. How do the authors interpret the difference between reversals and the difference between groups?

– The fact that pain ratings increased over time only in the instructed group is surprising. How does this relate to other findings of this group?

– Effects of predictive cues and reversals on pain-related brain responses are not significantly different between group, although pain ratings indicate a stronger effect in the instructed group (page 30). Please discuss this dissociation in more detail.

Thank you for encouraging us to address these theoretical issues in greater detail in the discussion and for reflecting on all these interesting points. Because the second and third points focus only on the pre-reversal phase, and the fourth point would be highly speculative, we thought it best to focus on the maintenance of initial contingencies in the expanded discussion, which also has a number of additional points we needed to incorporate given the new analyses suggested by the reviewers.

Because we included an additional participant in fMRI analyses and used nonlinear normalization to provide better between-subject correspondence (see comparison in Author response image 1), we no longer observe the lack of reversal in PAG; instead, we observe that a portion of the brainstem near the RVM maintains initial contingencies over time. The RVM works together with the PAG to regulate endogenous opioid release, which is implicating in descending pain modulation. We now discuss this important point in the Discussion.

Author response image 1. Mean structural (T1) image comparison – Images depict the average of all participants’ structural scans following normalization, with the initial pipeline on the left and the pipeline used for the revision, which includes nonlinear normalization, on the right.

Author response image 1.

Nonlinear warping is associated with more consistent normalization, as can be seen in better gray-white matter separation in the group mean image.

We made the following changes:

– Pp. 31-32, added “While most regions that have been implicated in prior studies of placebo and expectancy-based pain modulation updated dynamically as contingencies changed, we observed sustained responses to initial contingencies in a portion of the brainstem consistent with the rostroventral medulla (RVM). RVM responses to medium heat stimuli were elevated in response to the original high pain cue relative to the original low pain cue and remained elevated throughout the task in both groups. Together with the periaqueductal gray (PAG), the RVM is a key component of the descending pain modulatory system (Fields, 2004, 2006), which regulates the release of endogenous opioids that can block ascending pain signals, a process thought to be critical for expectancy-based placebo analgesia (Levine et al., 1978; ter Riet et al., 1998; Amanzio and Benedetti, 1999). Previous neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that placebo analgesia engages the RVM (Eippert et al., 2009; Crawford et al., 2021; Yanes and Akintola, 2022) and that placebo effects on the RVM are blocked by the opioid antagonist naloxone (Eippert et al., 2009). Preclinical work demonstrates that endogenous opioid release engages long lasting pain modulation (Watkins and Mayer, 1982a, 1982b). We have hypothesized that placebo analgesia may engage endogenous opioids to lead to long lasting pain modulation, whereas pain-predictive cues may modulate pain dynamically through dopaminergic prediction errors (Atlas and Wager, 2012; Atlas, 2021). The fact that RVM responds to initial contingencies throughout the task might be consistent with such long-lasting effects, and may explain why instructions reverse placebo analgesia only after brief conditioning (Schafer et al., 2015). Interestingly, we did not observe cue-based modulation of the PAG in our analyses; however, an initial processing pipeline that used affine normalization rather than nonlinear warping (Atlas et al., 2021) indicated that the PAG also responded to initial contingencies throughout the task, similar to the RVM. Thus both opioidergic regions may be associated with long-lasting effects that do not update as contingencies change. Future studies should combine pain-predictive cues with opioid and dopamine antagonists or employ positron emission tomography to understand the contribution of endogenous opioids and dopamine to different forms of pain modulation.”

3. As mentioned in the introduction, the authors reported sustained amygdala engagement in response to original cues in one of their previous papers, indicating that this structure might maintain a representation of original contingencies. In the present study the amygdala does not seems to show this pattern. How do the authors explain this difference in findings? How are reversals in the current paradigm different from those previously studied?

This comment builds on point 1.2 above. As noted above, we believe the strongest difference has to do with our focus on heat-evoked responses, rather than anticipatory responses. In addition, we also now find evidence of unsigned prediction errors in the amygdala, including in the Instructed Group, suggesting that at least in this task, the amygdala is not entirely impervious to instruction. We discuss the open questions about laterality in the amygdala as well as its role in associability and prediction error. Please see changes provided in response to point 1.2 above for full discussion on the differences between the previous paper and the current study with regard to the amygdala.

4. Although peripheral-physiological data including pupil dilation, gaze position and EDA were recorded, they are not included in the manuscript because "they may be analyzed and reported separately in future work". This decision seems unfortunate as this data could potentially aid the more detailed interpretation of the findings (see point 2).

We thank the reviewer for suggesting we include the autonomic data here. We did not include it in our initial submission because we felt the paper was already rather complex with the different analyses, and this was confirmed in eLife’s initial editorial decision. Nonetheless, we have included analyses of skin conductance data and pupil dilation in the revised manuscript. Please see response 1.4 above for details.

5. The instructions that were given to the two groups differ in one key aspect that is not discussed in the manuscript. While the instructed group was only informed about the meaning of the cues ("It is there just to let you know what level of heat will be next"), the uninstructed group was given a specific task ("Your job is to pay attention to the cues and try to figure out the relationship between the sounds that you hear and the heat that you feel."). To what extent could this task difference have influenced the results? Related to this issue, it is also worth noting that explicit information about reversals in the instructed group provided orientation to those who had lost track of contingencies because at least at the time point of the reversal they could be sure about the correct contingency. In contrast, the uninstructed group was not given an external reference throughout the whole experiment.

We now address the differences in instruction and the type of attention each is likely to elicit in our revised discussion. We believe that the task instructions given to the Uninstructed Group might engage model-based learning, as they are searching for a rule that explains the relationship between the sounds they hear and the heat they feel. However, the same task can be learned strictly through model-free learning, so we cannot determine empirically whether this is the case. We also believe that studies should directly compare instructed and model-based learning, as both involve maintenance of higher order contingencies and studies point to similar circuit mechanisms (e.g. corticostriatal interactions). Finally, we believe that the impact of these differences is likely to be relatively minor in the present experiment, as we did not find any evidence of group differences in cue effects on pain, or associations between pain and brain responses to noxious heat. We have included this information in the revised discussion.

In addition, the reviewer makes a good point about the instructions leading to increases in attention at the time of reversal. However, we must clarify that the language simply stated “The relationship between the cues and the heat will now reverse” so if someone had not learned the contingencies correctly (which is admittedly less likely here given the pairings with high and low stimuli which are easily discriminable) the instructions would not tell them the correct contingency. We have clarified this in the manuscript.

We made the following changes:

– P. 45, additions in red: “Halfway through runs 2, 4, and 6, the screen displayed instructions to the Instructed Group indicating that contingencies had reversed (“The relationship between the cues and heat will now reverse”; see Figure 1C).”

– P. 38, added: “One limitation of our design is that the two groups received slightly different instructions at the start of the experiment: The Instructed Group was informed about cue-heat contingencies, whereas the Uninstructed Group was instructed to pay attention and try to figure out the relationships between cues and heat outcomes. Although these differences did not impact brain mediators of expectancy or pain, the latter instruction would be more likely to engage inference and model-based learning (Doll et al., 2012, 2015; Dayan and Berridge, 2014). These differences should be resolved in future work by directly comparing instructed and model-based learning.”

6. If I understand the methods section correctly, the authors used a 100% reinforcement rate during the conditioning phase. In the subsequent test phase, each cue was followed by medium pain and reinforcement was lowered to 50%. Were participants informed about the change in reinforcement rate or was there a break between conditioning and test phase? Please clarify.

The Reviewer is correct, but we should also clarify that there were only five conditioning trials before the medium temperatures were introduced (2-3 per temperature), so that we could measure expectancy effects on pain prior to reversal. Thus subjects were not informed about the change in reinforcement rate and there was no break between conditioning and test. We have clarified this in Figure 1 and in the Methods section:

– Figure 1: “Participants first underwent a brief conditioning phase of 5-6 trials in which Original Low Cues (gray) were followed by heat calibrated to elicit low pain (level 2) and Original High Cues (black) were followed by heat calibrated to elicit high pain (level 8). Conditioning was immediately followed by intermittent test trials, in which we delivered medium heat following each cue to test the effects of predictive cues on perceived pain. Following the initial test phase, participants in the Instructed Group were informed about reversals and we delivered medium stimuli to test the effects of instructions.”

– P. 45: “We used two trial orders (counterbalanced across participants within Group) that each included (1) a brief conditioning phase, (2) a test of cue-based expectancy effects, and (3) three contingency reversals (Figures 1C and 1D). During the conditioning phase, Original Low cues were followed by stimulation calibrated to elicit ratings of low pain and Original High cues were followed by stimulation calibrated to elicit high pain (see Figure 1C and 1D). The conditioning phase included 3 Original High cue + high heat pairings and 2-3 Original Low cue + low heat pairings (i.e. 5-6 trials; see Figure 1D). Following conditioning, each cue was paired intermittently with stimulation calibrated to elicit ratings of medium pain.”

7. The figures are very small and difficult to read. Please particularly check the violin plots in Figure 4 and 5 where parts of the figure legend seem to be missing.

We have edited the figures to ensure that they are easier to read and no information is omitted.

Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

Below, I list a few suggestions for improvement.

1. Focus on stimuli of medium intensity: The authors nicely control for objective pain intensity by focusing on analyzing pain stimuli of medium intensity. However, I would find it very interesting if the reported pattern of results – especially the fit of the RW model variants – holds if stimuli of all intensities are included in the analyses.

This is an interesting question. We believe that fitting to medium intensity trials isolates the pure effect of expected value, as the medium trials are otherwise identical, which the Reviewer acknowledges. But it is true that the learning process depends on pairings with the low and high stimuli. We note that these are indeed included in the model, it’s just that goodness of fit is only evaluated on the medium heat trials. So in other words, they are included in the analysis, they are just not included in the evaluation of goodness of fit (see 3.1.a).

We fit the same RW models and evaluated goodness of fit using all trials rather than just medium trials. We again found that there was a complete reversal upon instruction in the Instructed Group, based on the instructed reversal parameter ρ (M = 0.93, SD = 0.5), consistent with our findings when we fit to medium heat trials. In addition, ρ values were significantly higher in the Instructed Group relative to the Uninstructed Group based on fits to individuals (t(1,38) = 7.40, CI = [0.20, 0.36], p <.0001). We also observed that learning rates were near-zero in the Instructed Group (M = 0.01 when fitting to all trials vs M = 0.06 when fitting to medium trials). However, within the Uninstructed Group the learning rate decreased from M = 0.28 to M = 0.04, and the between-groups difference in learning rate was no longer statistically significant (p = 0.19). We believe this is consistent with our position that pain on medium heat trials is the best index of value-based learning and learned expectations, whereas ratings on high and low intensity trials are influenced by stimulus intensity and not learning. We therefore have decided to retain our focus on models fit to medium heat trials in the revised manuscript but believe that this indicates that our results regarding how instructions guide learning are similar when we fit to all intensities as parameters were highly consistent within the group that was exposed to instructions.

a. Specifically, in the uninstructed group, the exact temporal succession of stimuli of high and low intensity should inform stimulus-by-stimulus learning. Maybe I just did not grasp correctly, which trials were included for fitting.

The reviewer is correct; the low and high intensity trials are what leads to stimulus-based learning. It is only pain reports on the medium trials that are tested as the outcome of such learning. We have clarified this in the revised Methods section:

– Pp. 51-52 additions in red: “In the present study we assume that pairings between predictive stimuli and high and low intensity heat engages stimulus-based learning, similar to CS-US pairings in classical conditioning experiments. Fitting to medium heat trials isolates the timecourse of expected value, since the stimulus temperature is constant, and therefore the only factor likely to guide cue-based variation in pain is presumably the cue’s dynamic expected value based on learning and/or instructions.”

b. As a sanity check, it would be nice to see the ratings and also the fMRI signals for all received intensities. I understand that the medium intensity stimuli control for objective differences – but still it would be nice to see the reflection of these objective differences in behavior and fMRI signals.

In fact, we did include a figure of pain ratings as a function of heat intensity in our original submission (Figure 2 —figure supplement 1), to accompany our statistical analysis of pain ratings as a function of temperature. We now include an additional analysis of brain responses as a function of stimulus intensity in response to the Reviewer’s suggestion. We also added a new figure (Figure 2), which includes the plot that was previously presented as a supplement along with images of brain responses as a function of heat intensity, pattern expression by the NPS and SIIPS, and effects of heat intensity on SCR. Brain responses show reliable increases as a function of heat intensity, and there was no difference between groups in the effect of heat intensity on signature pattern expression or responses within pain-related regions. Interestingly, ROI-wise analyses suggest that the Uninstructed Group showed stronger negative associations between temperature and VMPFC activation than the Instructed Group. However, we did not observe group differences in this region in uncorrected voxelwise analyses so we do not make strong inferences based on this comparison. We made the following changes:

– Added Figure 2, Figure 2 —figure supplement 1, accompanying Source Data, and updated Table 2 to include heat intensity effects on regions of interest.

– P. 9, deleted “figure supplement 1”: “We next examined pain as a function of heat intensity (i.e. temperature level: low, medium, or high) during the fMRI experiment (see Figure 2). ”

– P. 10, added: “We also evaluated brain responses to noxious stimulation as a function of heat intensity. We note that FDR-corrected thresholds exceeded 0.001 for all voxelwise analyses apart from moderation by group; we therefore interpret main effects of heat intensity at p <.001. We observed robust intensity-related changes within pain modulatory regions, including bilateral insula, striatum, dorsal anterior cingulate, thalamus, and other regions that did not differ between groups (see Figure 2E-G and Figure 2 – Source Data 2-3). Consistent with this, we observed robust expression of both the NPS and SIIPS as a function of temperature-related changes in both groups (all p’s < 0.001, see Table 2) and signature pattern expression did not differ by group (all p’s > 0.2; see Figure 2C and D and Table 2). Thus variations in heat intensity were positively associated with increases in pain-related activation in pain-related regions regardless of whether individuals were instructed about contingencies. Whole brain FDR-correction did reveal significant group differences in the left hippocampus and right primary somatosensory cortex driven by stronger intensity effects in the Instructed Group (see Figure 2H and Figure 2 – Source Data 2-3). Within value-related ROIs, we observed positive effects of heat intensity on the bilateral striatum that did not differ by Group, whereas the VMPFC showed significant Group differences, driven by negative associations between temperature and VMPFC activation in the Uninstructed Group, but not the Instructed Group (see Table 2). There were no associations between heat intensity and amygdala activation.”

c. By including all stimuli, the authors could also look into prediction errors (PEs) and into the expectations during the cue phase. The authors note that the study design was not optimized for such analyses and that PEs and expectations are negatively correlated. But I am wondering if this profoundly precludes such analyses – even though these would be exploratory.

As mentioned above, unfortunately our task and TR make it impossible for us to look at brain responses to cues independent of pain (see response 1.3). In addition, because outcomes are delivered on every trial, if we look at signed prediction errors (PEs) in across all trials (i.e. including all stimulus intensities), then PE will be correlated with temperature: High intensity stimuli will only be expected or worse than expected, whereas low intensity stimuli will always be expected or better than expected. If we control for stimulus intensity using a parametric modulator, most of the variance will be driven by medium trials, which are equally likely to be better or worse than expected; this is the inverse of the expected value.

With these caveats, we did analyze prediction error in two ways: (1) We analyzed associations between brain responses to all heat intensities and both signed and unsigned PEs, as suggested by the Reviewer; (2) We analyzed associations between brain responses to medium heat and unsigned PE (i.e. the absolute value of PE) which is only partially correlated with EV. We report the second analysis in the revised manuscript, and provide results of the first analyses here for transparency.

PE on all trials: When we included all trials in the analysis and examined signed PEs, we did indeed observe robust activation in the bilateral insula and thalamus, and no differences between groups, similar to what we observed when we analyzed responses to heat as a function of temperature (see Author response image 2). We therefore do not feel comfortable making inferences based on these results, for the reasons described above (i.e. signed PEs are correlated with stimulus intensity).

Author response image 2.

Author response image 2.

No regions survived multiple comparisons correction when we examined unsigned PE across temperatures, whether we searched whole brain or restricted to value-related ROIs. We believe that the best test of unsigned PE is on trials restricted to medium heat trials, as described in the following section and the revised manuscript.Unsigned PE on medium heat trials: We did observe interesting findings when we examined unsigned prediction errors at the time of heat onset. We focused on value-related regions based on voxelwise and ROI-based analyses. The bilateral amygdala and right striatum were significantly associated with unsigned PE and effects did not differ significantly across groups. Voxelwise analyses revealed positive associations with PE, controlling for group, in a wide swath of contiguous activation encompassing the right anterior insula, right striatum, and right amygdala. Groups also differed in response to unsigned prediction error: We observed group differences in the large contiguous cluster that included the right SII, right superior temporal gyrus, and right temporo-parietal cortex, driven by stronger associations in the Uninstructed Group.

We believe these findings provide important additional information about the effects of instruction and error-driven learning during pain modulation, and have included them in the revision. These results provide further evidence that striatal prediction errors update with instruction, building on previous findings in bot8/29/22 12:20:00 PMh appetitive and aversive learning (Doll et al., 2009, 2011; Atlas et al., 2016). Although a large body of work focuses on reward prediction errors in the striatum, many studies indicate that the striatum is also sensitive to aversive PEs (Delgado et al., 2008; Garrison et al., 2013; Robinson et al., 2013; Corlett et al., 2022), and meta-analysis indicates that unsigned PEs are also reliably activated by unsigned PEs (Corlett et al., 2022). These results also raise new questions about the role of the amygdala in learning and the impact of instructions, which we hope will be addressed in future research.

Please note that while the PE signal varied strongly from trial to trial in the Uninstructed Group, who had higher learning rates, PE was associated with a gradual reduction in the Instructed Group (see revised Figure 7), as expected value only updated upon instruction in this group. Although the overall difference in unsigned PE magnitude from the beginning of the study to the end was small based on model fits, when used as a parametric modulator, this isolates effects that vary linearly across the duration of the study. This is consistent with associability, which is high early in learning and decreases as an environment becomes stable, and is also consistent with findings that amygdala responses habituate during learning (Büchel et al., 1998). We acknowledge these links explicitly in our discussion, as we recognize that the lack of stochasticity in the Instructed Group’s unsigned PE pattern could be surprising to some readers and deserves attention.

We have incorporated the results from the analysis of unsigned PEs on medium trials in our revision. We made the following changes:

– Updated Figure 7 to visualize responses across all trials and restricted to medium trials and to incorporate timecourse of PE

– Added results of PE analyses to Figure 8 and associated Supplementary Figures and Source Data

– Pp. 28-29, added: “Associations with unsigned prediction error differ between groups. In addition to analyses of EV, we evaluated associations between brain responses to medium heat and unsigned prediction errors (PEs). No regions showed significant associations within pain related ROIs; however, whole brain correction revealed a positive association with PE, controlling for group, in a wide swath of contiguous activation encompassing the right anterior insula, right striatum, and right amygdala (see Figure 8D). Whole brain correction also revealed a significant difference between groups in a large contiguous cluster that included the right SII, right superior temporal gyrus, and right temporo-parietal cortex (see Figure 8E) driven by negative associations with PE in the Instructed Group and positive associations in the Uninstructed Group (see Figure 8 —figure supplement 8 and Figure 8 – Source Data 6). Within value-related ROIs, unsigned PEs were positively associated with responses to heat in the right striatum and the bilateral amygdala (see Figure 8F and Table 2). Associations with right striatum were observed within each group, whereas associations with amygdala were only observed in the Instructed Group; however, group differences were not significant in any region. Finally, there was no association between PE and NPS or SIIPS expression in either group (see Table 2). Voxelwise uncorrected results are reported in Figure 8 —figure supplement 9 and Figure 8 – Source Data 6.”

– P. 36, added: “Because we focused on responses to the US rather than the CS here, we also were able to extend previous work by examining the role of unsigned prediction errors (PEs), which are linked to associability, which gates attention and learning dynamically. Associability has been linked to the amygdala in previous studies of pain and aversive learning (Li et al., 2011; Zhang et al., 2016; Atlas et al., 2019), meta-analyses indicate that unsigned PEs are associated with activation in the dorsal and ventral striatum, as well as other regions (Corlett et al., 2022), and one study of aversive learning showed responses to both associability and unsigned PE in the amygdala (Boll et al., 2012). Consistent with these studies, we observed associations between unsigned PE and activation in the bilateral amygdala and the right striatum in both groups in ROI-based analyses. Whole brain analyses revealed associations between unsigned PEs and activation in the right hemisphere encompassing the insula, striatum, and amygdala, and that the associations between PE and activation in the right SII and temporal cortex differed across groups (negative associations in the Instructed Group and positive in the Uninstructed Group). Thus, although cue effects on subjective pain were mediated similarly across groups, we still observe unique responses to PE that differed between groups.”

– Pp. 36-37, added: “We note that the amygdala’s association with PE within the Instructed Group indicates that instructions do have some impact on amygdala activation, seemingly in contrast to our previous work on instructed learning during fear conditioning (Atlas et al., 2016; Atlas 2019). However, because error-driven learning was nearly absent between instructed reversals in the Instructed Group, the PE regressor in this group captures a constant effect that decreases linearly over time, consistent with prior evidence that amygdala responses habituate during aversive learning (Büchel et al., 1998). Thus we did not observe evidence of amygdala sensitivity to instructions per se in the present study.”

– P. 57: “Neural correlates of expected value and prediction error”

– Pp. 57-58, additions in red: “Noxious stimulation might also be accompanied by prediction errors; for example, if an individual expects high pain and receives medium heat, this should generate an appetitive PE (i.e. better than expected) if the deviation is noticed. However, expected value and prediction error are inversely correlated in the standard RL model we used. We therefore measured associations with unsigned PE (i.e. the absolute value of PE), as signed PE is the inverse of expected value on our medium heat trials. We focused on responses to medium heat trials only as PE would be correlated with temperature if we analyzed responses across all trials (i.e. low temperatures are always expected or better than expected, whereas high temperatures are always expected or worse than expected).”

2. Physiological data: The authors note that they might look into those data in the future. A slightly more nuanced explanation of the reasons for refraining from analyzing the physiological data might be helpful (especially given that the authors have experience with such analyses).

Thank you for suggesting we revisit our decision to exclude the autonomic data. As mentioned above we felt the paper was already complicated and that this might provide confusion, and the number of subjects and trials who displayed autonomic responses was limited. However in response to suggestions from all three reviewers, we now incorporate SCR and pupil dilation data, along with appropriate caveats. Please see Response 1.4 above for details.

3. Brain responses that are unaffected by reversals: In my view, this is an interesting an unexpected finding (see e.g., Figure 5). Can it be excluded that these signals do not "just" relate to the differences between the cues in a rather unspecific way? Can it be that the first reversal induces a second "context" and that there are brain signals (maybe in the same regions) that "remain stable" in this new "switched context?" Relatedly, the authors might want to conjecture whether such "stable" responses would become unstable with longer learning periods in the switched context. I am rather unfamiliar with the role of these regions for pain perception.

We agree that this finding is interesting and unexpected: While we expected that some regions would respond to temperature independent of expectancy (e.g. those that track nociception rather than pain), it is more surprising to find that there are regions that are modulated by expectancy, but do not update as contingencies change, despite the fact that pain does reverse. We have expanded our discussion on the role of the RVM in pain perception in general (see Response 2.2 above), and included raw timecourses for all regions that show this pattern (Figure 6 —figure supplement 3). In both groups, all regions that responded to original contingencies show a specific increase in response to high temperatures during the heat period, and a similar (albeit weaker) increase at the same time when medium heat is preceded by the high pain cue, although based on the average timecourses this effect is strongest in the Instructed Group and during initial contingencies. So we believe that this means this is not an unspecific difference between cues, which we think is in agreement with the Reviewer’s point, if we are interpreting the first question correctly.

The Reviewer has also asked us to consider the role of learning and what would happen if we had longer learning periods. The timecourses are also instructive toward this point. Indeed, the influence of original cues is most pronounced for both groups during original contingencies, which could mean that rather than these regions being impervious to reversals, they may have a slower learning rate than other regions. One putative mechanism for this would be the engagement of endogenous opioids, which we have hypothesized lead to longer-lasting effects than dopaminergic prediction errors. Although our initial findings pointed to the PAG as a key region that did not reverse, we no longer observe consistent findings in the PAG; however, we instead see maintenance of initial learning in a region consistent with the rostroventral medulla (RVM). The RVM is a hub for the engagement of descending pain modulation through the µ-opioid receptor system, and preclinical work suggests that µ-opioid receptor-mediated pain inhibition can be systemic and long lasting. Thus if pain-predictive cues modulate pain by engaging descending inhibition, it is possible that the brain systems that mediate these effects are slower to update than dopaminergic circuits or prefrontal systems involved in higher order decision making. We acknowledge that this is highly speculative, but have included this logic, as well as the possibility that different circuits have different learning rates and that this appears in our task as failure to reverse (since our task has relatively rapid reversals), in our revised discussion, provided above in response 2.2. We thank the reviewer for suggesting we consider these points.

4. Learning rates of the instructed group: I find the wording regarding the learning rate a bit confusing. It sounds a bit as if the authors did not expect the learning rates to be low (or even zero). But given the explicit instructions and the nice RW model variant, this is exactly how they should look. The instructions say everything and no learning is needed. Testing instructions that are incorrect (as might be the case for some types of placebo/nocebo instructions) would be really interesting here; i.e., participants get information but still have to learn from experience (in spite of the received information). Maybe the authors have that idea in their mind when reporting their findings about the learning rates of the instructed group.

The Reviewer is correct; because participants receive valid instructions, it is not surprising that we do not observe substantial additional learning from reinforcement. We agree that approaches such as those used by Doll et al., 2009 in which experiential learning is inconsistent with instructions would engage dynamic learning and necessitate a higher learning rate. We modified the following sections accordingly:

– P. 24, added: “, as might be expected given that feedback was entirely consistent with instructions”

– P. 38, added: “Alternatively, future studies can include outcomes that diverge from instructions to examine the interplay between instructed and experiential learning (e.g., Doll et al., 2009).”

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Atlas LY. 2022. Instructions and experiential learning have similar impacts on pain and pain-related brain responses but produce dissociations in value-based reversal learning. NeuroVault. XSHXWBEV [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
    2. Atlas LY. 2022. How instructions and learning shape pain and expectancy-based pain modulation: Pain, SCR, and brain signatures. Open Science Framework. adc8e

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 2—source data 1. Heat intensity effects on heat-evoked autonomic responses.
    elife-73353-fig2-data1.docx (115.6KB, docx)
    Figure 2—source data 2. Heat intensity effects: Small-volumes corrected results.
    Figure 2—source data 3. Heat intensity effects: Uncorrected results.
    Figure 4—source data 1. Cue effects on skin conductance responses.
    Figure 5—source data 1. Mediation of current cue contingencies: Small-volumes corrected results.
    Figure 5—source data 2. Mediation of current cue contingencies: Uncorrected results.
    Figure 6—source data 1. Mediation of original cue contingencies: Small-volumes corrected results.
    Figure 6—source data 2. Mediation of original cue contingencies: Uncorrected results.
    Figure 8—source data 1. Associations with expected value (EV): Small-volumes corrected results.
    Figure 8—source data 2. Associations with expected value (EV): Uncorrected results.
    Figure 8—source data 3. Associations with expected value (EV) based on fits to heat-evoked SCR.
    Figure 8—source data 4. Associations with expected value (EV) based on fits to cue-evoked SCR.
    Figure 8—source data 5. Comparing instructed and feedback-driven expected value (EV) within the Instructed Group.
    Figure 8—source data 6. Associations with unsigned prediction error (PE).
    Supplementary file 1. Heat intensity effects on pain in fMRI participants (n = 36).
    elife-73353-supp1.docx (128.9KB, docx)
    Supplementary file 2. Bayesian multilevel model evaluating effects of Group, Cue, Phase, and Trial on medium heat pain using Beta family.
    elife-73353-supp2.docx (50.9KB, docx)
    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    Pain, SCR, and signature data are available on OSF; neuroimaging data has been uploaded to neurovault.

    The following datasets were generated:

    Atlas LY. 2022. Instructions and experiential learning have similar impacts on pain and pain-related brain responses but produce dissociations in value-based reversal learning. NeuroVault. XSHXWBEV

    Atlas LY. 2022. How instructions and learning shape pain and expectancy-based pain modulation: Pain, SCR, and brain signatures. Open Science Framework. adc8e


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES