Skip to main content
Autism & Developmental Language Impairments logoLink to Autism & Developmental Language Impairments
. 2022 Feb 28;7:23969415211073118. doi: 10.1177/23969415211073118

Play-based interventions for mental health: A systematic review and meta-analysis focused on children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder and developmental language disorder

Gill Francis 1, Emre Deniz 1, Carole Torgerson 1, Umar Toseeb 1,
PMCID: PMC9685160  PMID: 36438159

Abstract

Background and aims

Play-based interventions are used ubiquitously with children with social, communication, and language needs but the impact of these interventions on the mental health of this group of children is unknown. Despite their pre-existing challenges, the mental health of children with developmental language disorder (DLD) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) should be given equal consideration to the other more salient features of their condition. To this aim, a systematic literature review with meta-analysis was undertaken to assess the impact of play-based interventions on mental health outcomes from studies of children with DLD and ASD, as well as to identify the characteristics of research in this field.

Methods

The study used full systematic review design reported to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA) guidelines (PRISMA prisma-statement.org) with pre-specified inclusion criteria and explicit, transparent and replicable methods at each stage of the review. The study selection process involved a rigorous systematic search of seven academic databases, double screening of abstracts, and full-text screening to identify studies using randomised controlled trial (RCT) and quasi-experimental (QE) designs to assess mental health outcomes from interventions supporting children with DLD and ASD. For reliability, data extraction of included studies, as well as risk of bias assessments were conducted by two study authors. Qualitative data were synthesised narratively and quantified data were used in the metaanalytic calculation.

Main contribution

A total of 2,882 papers were identified from the literature search which were double screened at the abstract (n  =  1,785) and full-text (n  =  366) levels resulting in 10 papers meeting the criteria for inclusion in the review. There were 8 RCTs and 2 QEs using 7 named play-based interventions with ASD participants only. Meta-analysis of 5 studies addressing positive mental health outcomes (e.g. positive affect and emotional functioning) found a significant overall intervention effect (Cohen's d = 1.60 (95% CI [0.37, 2.82], p = 0.01); meta-analysis of 6 studies addressing negative mental health outcomes (e.g., negative affect, internalising and externalising problems) found a non-significant overall intervention effect (Cohen's d = 0.04 -0.17 (95% CI [-0.04, 0.51], p = 0.88).

Conclusions

A key observation is the diversity of study characteristics relating to study sample size, duration of interventions, study settings, background of interventionists, and variability of specific mental health outcomes. Play-based interventions appear to have a beneficial effect on positive, but not negative, mental health in children with ASD. There are no high quality studies investigating the efficacy of such interventions in children with DLD.

Implications

This review provides good evidence of the need for further research into how commonly used play-based interventions designed to support the social, communication, and language needs of young people may impact the mental health of children with ASD or DLD.

Keywords: Play-based interventions, autism spectrum disorder, developmental language disorder, mental health, systematic literature review

Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and developmental language disorder (DLD) are common neurodevelopmental disorders, with prevalence rates of ∼1% and ∼7%, respectively (Baird et al., 2006, Norbury et al., 2016). The disorders are characterised by social, communication, and or language impairments. Young people with ASD and DLD also tend to have poorer mental health compared to their neurotypical peers (Yew & O’Kearney, 2013). Given their language and communication impairments, these young people may find it difficult to access talking therapies. There is some evidence from observational studies that play may be associated with a decrease in subsequent mental health difficulties in children with ASD and DLD (e.g. Toseeb et al., 2020a). However, observational studies cannot demonstrate whether such associations are causal. There is a growing body of experimental research reporting on mental health outcomes for young people with social, communication, and or language impairments from play-based interventions but the findings are inconsistent. Therefore, this systematic review and meta-analysis sought to review the evidence from experimental studies to investigate the effectiveness of play-based interventions for mental health in children and adolescents with ASD and DLD.

ASD and DLD

ASD and DLD are common neurodevelopmental disorders. Both can be conceptualised as spectrum disorders such that affected individuals have a unique set of strengths and weaknesses. ASD is characterised by difficulties in social communication and interaction and restricted and repetitive behaviours, interests, and activities (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). These features of ASD manifest differently across affected individuals to varying frequencies and severities. Young people with DLD are impaired in their ability to learn and use oral language (Bishop et al., 2017). This is in the absence of certain biomedical conditions, such as ASD and hearing loss. The term DLD has been adopted in recent years and encompasses other terms such as specific language impairment (SLI). Young people with SLI have impaired oral language ability but their non-verbal cognitive ability is within the normal range (Tomblin et al., 1997). The term DLD does not require a discordant verbal and non-verbal cognitive ability; therefore, all young people with SLI can be described as having DLD. For consistency and ease of comprehension, the term DLD is used here to include both SLI and DLD. Additionally, young people with ASD and DLD tend to have poorer mental health compared to neurotypical young people.

Mental health

Mental health is a multi-dimensional complex construct, which consists of both positive and negative features. Positive mental health refers to positive affect and has been described in the literature as happiness, wellbeing, and life-satisfaction (Westerhof & Keyes, 2010). Negative mental health refers to the presence of negative affect and behaviours and is the focus of most mental health research, commonly referred to as mental health disorders. These include symptoms of depression, anxiety, and conduct problems (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). The World Health Organisation defines health as “a state of complete physical, mental, and social wellbeing and not merely the absence of disease or infirmity” (World Health Organisation, 1948), suggesting that both positive and negative mental health are important features of overall mental health. The absence of mental health difficulties is not synonymous with positive mental health. For example, young people may be neither happy nor experiencing low mood, whilst others with a high level of mental health difficulties might have high levels of life-satisfaction (Patalay & Fitzsimons, 2018). This is in line with psychological theory, whereby positive and negative mental health are related but distinct constructs (Dual-continua model of mental health, Westerhof & Keyes, 2010). Given also that the correlation between positive and negative mental health is moderate (Patalay & Fitzsimons, 2016), the evidence suggests that positive and negative mental health should be considered separately.

Diagnosable mental health conditions are not necessarily the same as mental health difficulties. Experiencing mental health difficulties, such as symptoms of low mood, anxiety, irritability etc., are a normal part of life. Low mood is to be expected after a negative life event and anxiety is an adaptive response to alert us to imminent danger. These responses turn into a diagnosable disorder when they become persistent and cause functional impairment1 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Psychiatric diagnoses rely on individuals to be able to explain their symptoms and the related functional impairment. Young people with ASD and DLD are likely to struggle with this due to their language and communication difficulties. In addition to this, individuals who fall slightly below diagnostic thresholds may be precluded from a diagnosis. Such binary categorisations are unhelpful in understanding the frequency and intensity of mental health difficulties. For these reasons, symptom-based measures of mental health may be preferred as they provide a comprehensive account of the nature and type of difficulties rather than simply the presence or absence of disorder. This is important because some interventions may lead to an improvement at a symptom level, which improves the quality of life in the affected individuals.

Child and adolescent mental health can be viewed through the lens of social and emotional development. Features of mental health may manifest differently through development. During the first few years of life, an infant's ability to form relationships, recognise and respond to emotions, explore their environment, and meet major milestones may be considered a positive indicator of their mental health. Difficulties in these areas might sometimes be precursors to mental health difficulties in later life. As infants grow older and become children and then adolescents, it becomes easier to identify their feelings and behaviours as mental health difficulties. This is partly due to more established measures of mental health at these ages but also because behaviour at these ages more closely corresponds to documented symptoms of mental health difficulties. Therefore, in this systematic review, a broad definition of mental health has been adopted to account for the diversity in the manifestation of mental health difficulties across development.

ASD, DLD, and mental health

Young people with ASD and DLD tend to have poorer mental health compared to their unaffected peers. Early research reported that approximately 70% of young people with a language disorder have a diagnosable mental health condition (Cantwell & Baker, 1987). This also translates at the symptom level. Young people with DLD have more symptoms of internalising and externalising problems compared to their unaffected peers (Yew & O’Kearney, 2013). There is a similar state of affairs for young people with ASD. Approximately 40% of children and adolescents with ASD have a diagnosable anxiety disorder (van Steensel et al., 2011). They also have higher rates of a diagnosable depression disorder (Hudson et al., 2019). In addition to the quantitative differences in mental health difficulties in ASD and DLD populations, there is also emerging evidence to suggest that they experience such difficulties in a qualitatively different way. For example, young people with ASD experience sensory symptoms differently (Ben-Sasson et al., 2009) and have different coping strategies (Mazefsky et al., 2013).

The co-occurrence of an ASD or DLD and mental health difficulties may be due to three possible explanations: 1) The presence of ASD or DLD features lead to mental health difficulties, 2) mental health difficulties lead to ASD or DLD, and or 3) ASD or DLD are caused by a third factor (e.g., genetics or a common environmental risk factor). First, the presence of ASD or DLD affects children's cognitive abilities which, according to the social information processing theory (Crick & Dodge, 1994), affect their social interactions. Young people with ASD or DLD may be less able to navigate social situations leading to withdrawal and the onset of psychopathology. For example, young people with DLD have higher levels of social anxiety (Durkin et al., 2017), which is associated with poorer mental health compared to their neurotypical peers In addition to this, it is well documented that young people with ASD or DLD are more likely to be bullied by siblings and peers (Toseeb et al., 2018, 2020b, van den Bedem et al., 2018), and may have poorer quality friendships (e.g., Andres-Roqueta et al., 2016), all of which are associated with mental health difficulties. The second possibility is that earlier social and emotional difficulties exacerbate or trigger the onset of features of ASD or DLD. Usage-based approaches to social, language, and communication development highlight the importance of social context (e.g., Tomasello, 2003). Social interactions between primary caregiver and child, and also between the child and their peers, provide a context in which to develop social, language, and communication skills (e.g., Hoff, 2006). Young people with pre-existing social and emotional difficulties may find it difficult to access these social interactions and so features of ASD or DLD are exacerbated. The final possibility is that the occurrence of ASD or DLD and mental health difficulties is due to a third unmeasured factor. Recent evidence suggests that common environmental factors, primarily consisting of socioeconomic variables, explain much of the variation in social, communication, and mental health outcomes (Bignardi et al., 2021). Additionally, common genetic variants may influence both DLD and mental health (Newbury et al., 2019, Toseeb et al., 2021). Playing with others may be beneficial for the mental health of children and adolescents with ASD or DLD as it enables them to learn and practise key emotional and behavioural regulation skills.

Play

Play is the leading form of activity in most children's lives which provides them with limitless opportunities to satisfy their unrealisable tendencies and sudden desires (Vygotsky, 1933). Play is a difficult term to define, as any activity that is freely chosen, child-driven, and pleasurable for the child can be conceptualised as play (Sturgess, 2009). The overall characteristics of play are described as spontaneous, free, non-literal, intrinsically motivated, pleasurable, and purified from externally imposed rules (Rubin, 1982; Saracho & Spodek, 1998; Hughes, 2003).

Numerous theories have tried to explain the necessity of play in children's lives. Home Ludens, a modern historical theory of play, suggests that play is the core element of children's lives as it provides them freedom, pleasure, and joy (Huizinga, 1949). In addition, the constructivist theory indicates that play is an integral part of children's development, as it is closely related to cognitive and language skills (Piaget, 1952). From a psychosocial perspective, it is claimed that play is children's own way to self-define social reality and provides them with a suitable environment to build their self-control skills (Erikson, 1993). In psychoanalysis, it is proposed that play creates a fantasy world for children to cope with the complexity of reality and provides the opportunity to explore and express their deep emotional states (Freud, 1955). As suggested by these theories, far from being thought to be a trivial activity in children's lives, play has a fundamental role in supporting children's various development, including, but not limited to, their cognitive, emotional, language and behavioural regulation skills.

The term play-based intervention stands for either the socio-cognitive techniques that are specifically built on the elements of play or the implementations that are delivered during the playtime or within the play settings (Ingersoll & Walton, 2013). The importance of play-based interventions in supporting children's development was recognised in the early 20th century. Little Hans is known as the first child who was treated with play for his phobic symptoms (Freud, 1909). Melanie Klein (1929), a student of Freud, also used play as a therapeutic tool to apply Freud's psychoanalytic techniques initially used with adults to subsequently apply to children. Wells (1912) and Lowenfeld (1939) were the other earliest child psychoanalysts that used sand play to support children with emotional problems. However, Virginia Axline (1947), a student and later colleague of Carl Rogers, firstly used the term play therapy and introduced client-centred play therapy as a structured play intervention to treat children's emotional difficulties.

Children of today's world are growing up in a dynamic environment in which they may not be getting enough time and opportunities to play freely. Today, children are playing less than those in previous generations; Natural England (NE) reports that less than 10% of the new generation play outside, naturally, whereas this rate was more than 50% in their parents’ generation (England Marketing, 2009). In the United States, it is claimed that children's playtime is significantly reduced by their parents stealing their spare playtime to invest more in schoolwork (Chudacoff, 2007). The lack of play in children's lives, thus, may be the reason underlying the failure of the new generation of children in reaching some developmental milestones at certain ages. For instance, early opportunities for make-believe play are suggested to be crucial for developing imaginative thought (Goldstein, 1994). It is also suggested that a child's playfulness is connected to their secure attachment, emotion regulation, empathy, and emotional resilience (Whitebread, 2017). Play deprivation in early childhood, however, is suggested to be associated with emotional/self-dysregulation and aggression with higher deprivation results in higher prevalence of dysfunctioning in these areas (Brown, 2014). In addition, children with identifiable deficits in play skills are at further risk in accessing developmental affordances from play (Toseeb et al., 2020b).

Numerous interventions have been built on play to support children's social, emotional, and cognitive development. However, the characteristics of such interventions vary in the sense of the definition and representation of play, play approach (e.g., directive or non-directive), or type of play (make-believe, fantasy, parallel, etc) that the delivered intervention is built on (Thomas & Smith, 2004). It is crucial to well-define play to differentiate the interventions that are solely built on play from any other interventions. Play is defined as activities that are intrinsically motivated, spontaneous, free from externally imposed rules, guided by organism-guided questions, non-literal and requires active engagement (Rubin et al., 1983, p.698). The authors also drew a clear line between play and game as: “Play is further distinguished from game in that the latter phenomenon is goal oriented whereas the former phenomenon is not. One plays for the satisfaction of playing. One engages in games to compete, to win, and to achieve some specified goal” (p.728). Taking Rubin et al.'s broad definition of play into account, this review focuses on the interventions that were solely play-based, delivered during the playtime or within the play settings such as, robot/animal-based play (e.g., canine-assisted play), peer-mediated play (e.g., the SENSE Theatre), parent-child interaction play (e.g. DIR/Floortime), school-based play (e.g., Early Start Denver Model), non-directive play (e.g., child-centred play), cognitive-therapeutic play (e.g., Jungian play), etc.

Although there is considerable evidence supporting the effectiveness of play-based interventions on children's holistic development, the majority of interventions have focused on children's social, communication, and language skills. For example, a study on the relationship between play and children's social development has reported that social-interactive play increases social competence in young children (Newton & Jenvey, 2011). Another study reports a significant increase in social interaction and language skills and a decrease in play deficits and disruptive behaviours of children with special educational needs and disabilities following a play intervention (O’Connor & Stagnitti, 2011). Furthermore, it has been suggested that play increases receptive and expressive vocabulary in at-risk preschoolers (Han et al., 2010), and a strong link has been found between symbolic play skills and functional language abilities of children with ASD (Mundy et al., 1987). Despite this, very few empirical studies have targeted the mental health outcomes of children. A recent systematic review of child-led play interventions also argued that such interventions mostly target children's social development instead of focusing on emotional outcomes (Gibson, et al., 2017).

The effectiveness of play-based interventions on children's emotional outcomes has only grabbed a few researchers’ interests. For example, one study investigated the effectiveness of unstructured play in reducing the anxiety level of young children in paediatric inpatient care and reported that a nurse-delivered play intervention was highly effective in reducing the anxiety levels of hospitalised children (Al-Yateem & Rossiter, 2017). Additionally, another study examined whether an unstructured play intervention reduced the stress level of hospitalised children in inpatient care and reported that children aged 7 to 11 years old showed a significant decrease in cortisol levels (a marker for stress) compared to the control group (Potasz et al., 2013). In addition, a playful approach of Six Bricks and DUPLO® has also been found effective on positive emotions and emotional competence of early adolescents (Harn & Bo, 2019).

Relevant systematic literature reviews

There are a handful of published reviews and meta-analysis synthesising work that address the mental health of individuals with social, communication and or language impairments generally or assess the impact of a play-based intervention on such individuals’ mental health. For instance, in a meta-analysis of cross-sectional studies reporting the prevalence of depressive disorders among individuals with ASD, Hudson, Hall, and Harkness (2019) reported that lifetime and prevalence of depressive disorders was 14.4% (95% CI 10.3–19.8) and 12.3% (95% CI 9.7–15.5), respectively, across samples of children, adolescents, and adults with ASD. Their findings indicate that the rates of depressive disorders are high among individuals with ASD. In a separate narrative review, Blewitt et al. (2019) investigated the extent to which preservice education programmes led by early childhood teachers and carers address ASD children's social and emotional development. After screening papers from MEDLINE Complete, PsychINFO and ERIC between January 1999 and July 2019, the authors identified only a limited number of studies (n  =  19) and concluded that teachers are not adequately prepared to deal with ASD children's social, emotional and behavioural challenges. Newton and Jenvey (2011) recommended that programmes should promote naturalistic and embedded social skill instruction within and across everyday interactions, play, activities and the environment, thereby offering contextually relevant opportunities to strengthen children's social-emotional skills can help.

Three additional review studies were found that synthesise the effect of different types of play-based intervention - Child-Centered Play-therapy (Hillman, 2018), DIR/Floortime (Mercer, 2017), and Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT) (Verschuur, et al., 2014). These are recognised play-based interventions derived from play-therapy approaches developed to support ASD children's social, emotional, and cognitive development and the reviews included in their synthesis at least one mental health outcome. Child-centered play therapy is used to improve core concerns related to ASD, such as social communication skills, joint attention and emotional regulation (Hillman, 2018). The effectiveness of Child-centered play therapy with ASD populations was investigated by Hillman (2018) in a comprehensive search of the Educational Resources Information Center (ERIC) and PsycINFO databases between 1900 and September 2017. Only four studies were identified in this review, of which only two addressed mental health outcomes - emotional growth and empathy, using single-case and multiple-baseline designs, with mixed results. According to Hillman (2018) more research is needed to understand the impact of Child-centered play therapy on ASD children. The other review, Mercer (2017), assessed the plausibility of, and evidentiary support for, the treatment DIR/Floortime - an intervention that uses therapeutic goals to work with autistic children and typically involves a functional emotional assessment along with other social communication and language evaluations. Mercer (2017) searched the Academic Search Complete, PsycINFO, and PubMed databases articles up to January 2015 and identified ten studies of which two studies used experimental designs and included an assessment of ASD children's emotions as a study outcome. Mercer concluded that although there is some support for the effectiveness of the DIR/Floortime intervention; the support is weak because of design flaws. The third review used PRT to teach pivotal behaviours to children with ASD in order to achieve generalised improvements in their functioning on four aspects of pivotal functioning: (a) motivation, (b) self-initiations, (c) responding to multiple cues, and (d) self-management (Verschuur et al., 2014). The authors screened studies published in ERIC, Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts, Medline, PubMed, and PsychINFO databases between December and June 2013 and identified thirty five intervention studies of which three studies reported mental health outcomes (increase in positive affect).

Whereas, the first two meta-analyses highlighted mental health challenges as prevalent among individuals with ASD none of the reviews of play-based interventions explicitly focused on the impact of mental health outcomes. This indicates a gap in the literature and a need for a review of play-based interventions and mental health outcomes.

The current study

A better understanding of how interventions also impact the mental health of children is valuable for evaluating the utility of these interventions in fostering holistic child development in neurodiverse populations. This review is important as it illuminates how play-based interventions designed for children with social, communication, and or language impairments related to diagnoses of ASD and DLD impact mental health outcomes. Questions addressing both the contextual design and the effectiveness of interventions can validate good practice in the field or make recommendations for improvement. This systematic review and meta-analysis addresses the following research questions:

  1. What are the characteristics of play-based interventions used to support children and young people with ASD or DLD to improve their mental health outcomes?

  2. Are play-based interventions designed for children with ASD or DLD effective in supporting their mental health?

Methods

A systematic literature review with meta-analysis was conducted following standards from the Cochrane Handbook for systematic reviews and the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines for evaluating and reporting on the effectiveness of interventions (Higgins et al., 2021; Page et al., 2021). This involved searching for, locating, quality appraising and synthesising, both narratively and statistically, all the relevant studies that can address the question about the effectiveness of play-based interventions on the social, communication, language, and mental health outcomes of children with social, communication, and language impairments. The systematic review included studies exploring all outcomes in a broad ‘mapping’ of the field but narrowed to an in-depth synthesis and a meta-analysis of studies with the findings for mental health outcomes only. At the inception of the review, a-priori criteria for including studies in the review were developed in a protocol through collaborative discussions among the study authors before commencing the search process to identify potentially relevant studies.

Eligibility criteria

Pre-specified inclusion criteria were developed to identify studies evaluating high-quality interventions based on topic, study designs, characteristics of participants and intervention and control conditions, in order to include studies that can address the research questions. These pre-specified inclusion criteria provided the basis for selecting studies for inclusion in the review (see Table 1).

Table 1.

Criteria for Selecting Studies for this Review.

Study Criteria Inclusion Exclusion
Design Experimental Designs Experimental designs using single group comparisons
  • RCT

  • non-equivalent group quasi-experimental designs (pre/post test design)

  • regression discontinuity design,

  • time-series designs

Date Studies published between 2000–2021 Studies published < 2000
Language Studies published in English Non-English publications
Participants Participants age ranges include: Adults
  • Early years (0-7yrs)

  • Middle childhood (8-12 yrs)

  • Adolescence (13-19 yrs)

Classification of participants with social-communication needs e.g.:
  • DLD

  • Autism

  • Asperger's Syndrome

  • SLI

  • Social-communication disorder

Interventions Play interventions addressing mental health as well as social, communication, and language outcomes in children with social-communication impairments Non-play-based interventions
Comparisons Comparison of play-based interventions to a control group using no intervention, wait-list control, usual care, or an active control group which is a nonplay-based intervention. Comparisons of two play-based interventions
Outcomes Mental-Health/Wellbeing outcomes Non social-communication, socio-emotional or mental health outcomes
  • Happiness

  • Self-esteem

  • Wellbeing

  • Depression

  • Anxiety

  • Stress

  • Internalising/Externalising/Total difficulties

  • Emotional difficulties

Social - Communication outcomes
  • Speech, Language, and communication skills

  • Language Learning outcomes

  • Friendships (peer engagement)

  • Prosociality (empathy)

  • Social skills (play skills)

Setting Research settings e.g. No exclusions on the basis of description of research settings.
  • Schools

  • Clinical settings

  • Research settings

Play intervention setting e.g.:
  • Therapy centre

  • School

  • Home

  • Other

Play intervention provider e.g.
  • Play therapists

  • Psychologists

  • Teachers

  • Parents

  • Peers

  • Researchers

  • Animal/Robot/Canine assisted

Only studies using experimental designs were included. These include: (1) randomized controlled trials (RCTs): studies that randomly assign participants to intervention and control or comparison conditions; (2) quasi-experimental designs (QEs) using pre/post- tests designs with non-equivalent comparison groups where participants are not randomly assigned to conditions; (3) regression discontinuity designs which assign participants to conditions on the basis of a predetermined cut-off on a continuous variable; and (4) time-series designs involving at least three observations made both before and after a treatment. All included studies were required to have a control or comparison group that either did not receive a treatment or were on a waitlist to receive treatment after the end of the experiment (Shadish, Cook, & Campbell, 2002). As the aim was to evaluate intervention designs that infer causality, studies that employed single case experimental designs and multiple-baseline designs were excluded. Although they are commonly used in research with children with social, communication, and language disorders and can provide limited evidence of causality, for the purposes of this review the inclusion criteria specified only the most robust designs were included.

In addition, as mentioned earlier, this review focuses on the studies that used interventions that were inherently playful but not based on any other behavioural modification techniques. Therefore, studies that focus on behavioural interventions, teaching or training, peer-based group activities, and game-with-rules (e.g., script-fading, activity schedules, group activity schedules, computer/online-games, video-prompting, behavioural intervention, wearable devices/virtual reality, and gaming activities) were kept out of the scope of this review. Hence, to meet this inclusion criterion, interventions should either be solely play-based or delivered during the playtime or within the play settings. Also, studies that compared the effectiveness of two or more play-based interventions on the mental health outcome of children with ASD or DLD were also excluded.

An initial scoping exercise was conducted by the first and second authors to inform the aspects of the study's eligibility criteria. The scoping exercise was especially useful for two reasons: (1) to ascertain the period during which a significant record of publications reporting play-based interventions with children with social-communication impairments exist so as to specify the time-line for the review, and; (2) to identify relevant terms, specifically the name of play-based interventions and terms used to refer to children with ASD and DLD. The list of play-based interventions identified include: Play Therapy, Theraplay, Play-based interventions, PRT, Joint Attention, Symbolic Play, Engagement & Regulation (JASPER, Advancing Social-communication And Play (ASAP), LEGO Therapy, The Developmental, Individual Difference, Relationship-Based (DIR) Floortime Model, The Paediatric Autism, Communication Therapy (PACT), Stay, Play, & Talk, PLAY Project, Project ImPACT, Early Start Denver Model (ESDM), E-PLAYS, CCPT (Child-centred play therapy), Filial therapy, Animal/Parent/Peer/Sibling assisted play, Non-named play-based interventions. Classification terms used to refer to individuals with social-communication needs were also identified.

Only relevant publications dating from the last two decades (2000 to 2021) published in English Language journals were included. The scoping exercise indicated the majority of play-based experimental interventions were undertaken during that timeline. We screened for peer-reviewed journal articles as well as masters and doctoral theses but excluded conference papers and grey literature due to insufficient resources.

To identify eligible studies, detailed specifications of the population, intervention, comparators, outcomes, and settings (PICOS) were outlined as key components of interest (Thomas et al., 2021). Study participants were inclusive of children diagnosed with a social, communication, and language impairment including ASD or DLD. Participants’ ages ranged from infancy to adolescence and we categorised infancy as less than two years, childhood from 2 to 12 years, and adolescents 12 to 21 years (Hardin et al., 2017). The intervention types considered are all play-based interventions whether using validated or less formally developed programmes, as well as those supporting children with social-communication disorders through peer-mediated and parent-mediated designs. Comparisons between intervention groups are based on studies having the following control conditions: no intervention; wait-list control; usual care; or interventions with active controls if the comparison is a nonplay-based control. Studies with outcomes relating to the social communication, language, and mental health needs of children were deemed eligible for inclusion. Studies needed to describe the setting of the intervention in detail as this level of description is important for understanding the context under which play-based interventions may or may not be effective.

Information sources

To identify studies, a comprehensive search of academic research databases was undertaken between January and March 2021. The searchable publication databases used were: ‘Ebscohost/ERIC’, ‘Web of Science’, ‘Pubmed’, ‘SCOPUS’, ‘PsycINFO’, ‘PROQuest’, and ‘ETHOS’.

Search strategy

A consistent search strategy was applied across databases. Boolean search strings were developed using keywords. These included references to play as a generic term stringed with types of language impairments e.g., Play AND “language disorder”, Play AND “specific language impairment”, etc or types of play-based interventions, only e.g. “PRT”, or “Theraplay”. The full list of Boolean search strings is presented in Appendix A.

Selection process

An exhaustive search of the selected academic databases using all combinations of the Boolean search strings was undertaken by one author [ED] and all hits were downloaded into the Mendeley Reference Manager software (Mendeley, 2021). Systematic reviews and meta-analyses based on the same topic area generated from the search were retained to undertake manual hand-searching of their reference lists as a strategy for capturing additional peer-review articles not published in the databases searched. The titles and abstracts of all studies in Mendeley were independently double screened and the papers which both authors [ED & GF] agreed met the eligibility criteria were carried over for independent double screening of full texts and agreement. To identify any other potential articles overlooked from the database searches, backward chaining was done by authors [ED & GF] by consulting the reference lists of papers recommended for full-text screening when in-text citations indicate an article might be highly relevant.

Data extraction process

A data extraction form was designed to abstract relevant bibliographic details and information about the sample and sample characteristics, intervention and control or comparison conditions, and quantified outcomes from individual papers included in the review. To quality assure the robustness of the completion of the data extraction form for each included study, the following process was followed: first, all four authors [ED, GF, CT, UT], extracted data from one paper and the results were compared for consistency and sufficiency of the evidence to answer the research questions; subsequently, data extraction was divided between three authors [ED, GF, CT] such that data from each paper was independently extracted by two different authors and compared for agreement, with any adjustments being made if there was any initial disagreement, and arbitration by the fourth author [UT] where necessary.

Outcomes

The review investigated the effect of play-based interventions on mental health and wellbeing outcomes including variables like happiness, self-esteem, wellbeing, depression, anxiety, stress, emotional difficulties, internalising or externalising difficulties, etc. All post-intervention outcomes were extracted. The decision was taken to include both primary and secondary outcomes, and experimenter-developed outcome measures as well as validated measures, where applicable.

Mid-intervention data and post-intervention follow-up data were excluded. Other pertinent data extracted include information about participant characteristics, e.g., social communication and language diagnosis, age of participants, etc., and intervention characteristics e.g., sample size, the background of the person delivering the intervention, setting of the intervention, etc. Missing data were sourced by contacting the corresponding author of a given study, if necessary.

Study risk of bias

An assessment of the risk of bias2 was conducted for all studies included in the review using sample questions from the revised ‘Cochrane Risk of Bias Tool for Randomized Trials’ (RoB 2) and ‘Cochrane Risk of Bias Tool in Non-randomized Studies of Interventions’ (ROBINS-1) to appraise RCTs and QE studies, respectively (see Appendix B). Both instruments focus on different aspects of trial design, conduct and reporting comprising six domains under which signal questions are posed to elicit information about features of the trial that are relevant to the risk of bias (Sterne et al., 2016, 2019). The six domains addressed the risk of bias arising from:

  • randomisation in RCT or confounds in QEs

  • deviations from the intended interventions in RCTs or effect of assignment to interventions in QEs

  • missing outcome data

  • measurement of the outcome

  • selection of the reported result

In each domain, a judgement of ‘low’, ‘high’, or ‘some concerns’ for risk of bias is asserted and used to make an overall judgment about the ‘risk of material bias’ of the study. A judgement of ‘high’ risk of bias for any individual domain will lead to the result being at ‘high’ risk of bias overall, and a judgement of ‘some concerns’ for any individual domain will lead to the result being at ‘some concerns’, or ‘high’ risk, overall. The aim is to expressly identify issues that are likely to affect the ability to draw reliable conclusions from the study (Sterne et al., 2019). The shortened versions of the risk of bias tools used were trialled by all four authors on a selected paper and a final version was agreed upon through discussion. The review papers were appraised independently by the first and second authors [GF & ED] and 25% of each author's sample was independently appraised by the other two authors [CT & UT].

Effect measures

Effect sizes with 95% confidence intervals were computed based on post-intervention measurements as study outcomes were continuous. Effect sizes were calculated based on Cohen's d using the online Practical Meta-analysis Calculator and replicated in Stata (StataCorp, 2021. Standardised mean difference and standard error values for each outcome were generated from values of means and standard deviations reported in the individual papers. . Where data were not available, the effect size was computed using other appropriate statistical indices, e.g. F-test statistics and sample sizes. Issues of missing data were addressed by contacting the corresponding author of individual papers to request additional information. According to White, Redford, & MacDonald (2020), suggested thresholds of statistically significant effects of Cohen's d in social science research include: (1) 0 < d < 0.1 indicates a trivial effect; (2) 0.1 < d < 0.2 indicates a small effect; (3) 0.2 < d < 0.5 indicates a moderate effect; (4) 0.5 < d < 0.8 indicates a medium size effect; (5) 0.8 < d < 1.3 indicates a large effect; (6) 1.3 < d < 2.0 indicates a very large effect; (7) d > 2.0 indicates an extremely large effect.

It was necessary to correct for differences in the direction of measurement scales because the standardised mean difference method does not account for such differences (Higgins, et al., 2021). This was necessary for studies grouped as measuring negative mental health. For example, some scales increased with trait severity (e.g., a higher score indicated severe negative mental health) whilst others decreased (e.g., a lower score indicated less severe negative mental health). To ensure that all scales pointed in the same direction, the mean values for scales with positive directionality were multiplied by −1, before standardisation (Higgins, et al., 2021).

To account for statistical dependencies, cases where the data are dependent i.e., multiple outcomes from one study, the average effects were computed to yield a single effect estimate (Borenstein, 2009). Some studies were comparisons of two intervention approaches; hence, these were included only if it was a comparison between a play-based and non-play-based intervention. Therein, the non-play-based intervention was treated as the control group and the play-based approach as the intervention group.

Synthesis methods

Study outcomes were classified based on whether the measures assessed positive or negative mental health. In addition, both narrative and statistical analyses were undertaken. The narrative analysis involved comparing intervention characteristics across studies [RQ1] and statistical analysis involved meta-analytic calculations of the mental health outcomes to make inferences about the effectiveness of play-based interventions [RQ2]. To undertake statistical syntheses of the review data, meta-analyses of study outcomes were conducted in Stata using the inverse variance estimation method and a random effects model. The random-effects model assumes the existence of within-and between-study variability or heterogeneity in estimating individual and combined study effects and attempts to minimise both sources of variance (Borenstein, 2009). The meta-analytic results are depicted with forest plots which show effect sizes and corresponding confidence intervals for studies at the individual and combined level, as well as heterogeneity statistics. Within study variability is assessed by the χ2 (Q) test and a significant p-value (or a large χ2 statistic relative to its degree of freedom) is indicative of evidence of heterogeneity of intervention effects (Hedges, 1982; Higgins et al., 2021). Between-study variance is inferred from tau (T²). The proportion of variance in study effect size is indicated by I² (Higgins & Thompson, 2002). Values of 25%, 50%, and 75% are proposed as benchmarks of low, moderate, and high variance with large I² inviting speculation for the reasons of the variance (Higgins, et al., 2003; Borenstein, 2009). A high I² suggests that variability between effect size estimates are due to between-study differences rather than the sampling variation (Borenstein et al., 2017). Also, the prediction interval or range of predicted effects (standard deviation of the overall effect size estimate) is reported. However, this should be interpreted cautiously, as the results can be very spuriously wide or spuriously narrow when the number of included studies are small (Higgins et al., 2021). Further inspection of the data included assessment of publication bias to explore concerns around the under-reporting of non-significant results. This involves visual inspections of funnel plots to check for study asymmetry and statistical tests of small study effects using the non-parametric trim and fill method to assess the impact of missing studies from potential publication bias. A sensitivity analysis to compare studies by research design or risk of bias results was undertaken. Meta-regression analyses were undertaken to investigate whether study heterogeneity could be explained by study moderators or pre-specified study characteristics depending on whether there are sufficient numbers of homogenous studies.

Results

Study selection results

The automated searching of electronic databases yielded 2882 papers including existing related systematic review papers used for citation searches and additional sources obtained from backward chaining, that is, articles identified from checking the reference list of review papers and from following up on relevant citations flagged during the full-text screening (n  =  59). After deduplication, 1785 papers were retained. Titles and abstracts were double screened by the first and second authors [GF & ED]. The two authors [GF & ED], agreed on the inclusion of 366 papers, the exclusion of 1362 papers, and identified 42 papers that could not be evaluated due to being inaccessible3 at the time of screening. There was disagreement on 15 papers that were discussed and resolved through discussion with the research team. Cohen's kappa coefficient for inter-rater reliability was κ  =  .88 (95% CI, .85 - .91) which indicates that the agreement between the first two authors on the selection of articles for full-text screening was very good.

The process of double screening was repeated on the 366 papers eligible for full-text screening. Through a process of consensus agreement, a total of 314 papers were excluded for the following reasons: a) no control or comparison group (n  =  179); b) published in languages other than English (n  =  7); c) sample participants did not have a diagnosed social communication or language impairment (8); (d) intervention was not play-based (n  =  13); e) comparison between two play-based interventions (n  =  7); f) only social, communication, and language outcomes (n  =  100). In total, 10 papers met the eligibility criteria, i.e., explicitly reported mental health outcomes for study participants which is the focus of this review (see PRISMA Flow Diagram in Figure 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

PRISMA Flow Diagram for Record of Systematic Searches of Databases

Of particular note was the fact that the 10 studies were published during the period 2008 to 2021, although the database screening was conducted from the period January 1st, 2000 to January 31st, 2021. The absence of records of play-based interventions using RCTs and QEs addressing mental health outcomes of children with ASD or DLD before 2008 is striking.

Study characteristics

The characteristics of the ten studies are described based on study design, sample sizes, and characteristics of play-based interventions used. A full description of studies at the individual level is presented in Table 2.

Table 2.

Included Studies and Study Characteristics

Study ID Study reference, publication, and country Sample characteristics Research design Play intervention Outcome variable Summary of results
S01 Corbett et al. (2017)
Publication: Journal of Autism
Country: USA
Sample size: N = 30
Sample: Children with ASD
Age: 8-14 years old
Girls: N = 6 (20%) Boys: N = 24 (80%)
INV: N = 17 CON: N = 13
Design: RCT
Type of control: Randomly allocated, waitlist control
Control group treatment: SENSE Theatre®
Outcome Measure: The STAI-C (Spielberger et al., 1983)
Measure standardised: Yes
Name of intervention: SENSE Theatre®
Intervention valid: Yes
Type of play: Role play
Place: Outside of school
Provider: Not reported
Duration: 40 hours
Reported outcome:Trait anxiety
Category: Negative mental health
Test of between-subject effects revealed a significant group effect on post-STAI-C Trait, with pre-STAI-C Trait included as a covariate (F(1, 27) = 9.16, p = 0.005). Changes in play did not show a significant mediational effect on changes in trait-anxiety (B = −0.32; CI = −3.35 to 2.11). Conversely, the direct effect of the intervention on changes in trait-anxiety remained significant (B = −6.97, CI = −12.62 to −1.31).
Reported outcome: State anxiety
Category: Negative mental health
No group effect was observed for STAI-C State (F(1, 27) = 0.03, p = 0.86)).
S02 Doernberg et al. (2021)
Publication: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
Country: USA
Sample size: N = 25
Sample: Children with HF ASD
Age: 6-9 years old
Girls: N = 3 (12%) Boys: N = 22 (88%)
INV: N = 18 CON: N =  7
Design: RCT
Type of control: Randomly allocated, waitlist control
Control group treatment: Treatment as usual
Outcome Measure: The APS (Fehr & Russ (2014))
Measure standardised: Yes
Name of intervention: Pretend Play
Intervention valid: Yes
Type of play: Pretend play
Place: School
Provider: The researchers
Duration: 100 minutes
Reported outcome: Total positive affect
Category: Positive mental health
Results did not indicate any significant effects for children’s abilities to generate a list of positive feelings, nor define complex emotions appropriately.
Reported outcome: Total negative affect
Category: Negative mental health
Results did not indicate any significant effects for children’s abilities to generate a list of negative feelings, nor define complex emotions appropriately.
S03 Ioannou et al. (2020).
Publication: Frontiers in Psychology
Country: USA
Sample size: N = 77
Sample: Children with HF ASD
Age: 8-16 years old
Girls: N = 18 (24%) Boys: N = 59 (76%)
INV: N = 44 CON: N = 33
Design: RCT
Type of control: Randomly allocated, waitlist control
Control group treatment: SENSE Theatre®
Outcome Measure: The STAI-C (Spielberger et al., 1983)
Measure standardised: Yes
Name of intervention: SENSE Theatre®
Intervention valid: Yes
Type of play: Role play
Place: Outside of school
Provider: Not reported
Duration: 40 hours
Reported outcome: State anxiety
Category: Negative mental health
There was no difference in State anxiety between EXP and WCL groups [F(2,71) = 0.07, p = 0.935].
Reported outcome: Trait anxiety
Category: Negative mental health
Children in the EXP group reported significantly less Trait anxiety than children in the WLC group following intervention [F(2,71) = 6.87, p = 0.01].
S04 Pajareya & Nopmaneejumruslers (2011)
Publication: Journal of Autism
Country: Thailand
Sample size: N = 32
Sample: Children with ASC
Age: 2-6 years old
Girls: N = 5 (15%) Boys: N = 28 (85%)
INV: N = 16 CON: N = 16
Design: RCT
Type of control: Randomly allocated control group
Control group treatment: Treatment as usual
Outcome Measure: (1) The FEAS (Greenspan, DeGangi & Wieder, (2001).
(2) The FEDQ (Pajareya, Sutchritpongsa & Sanprasath, 2014).
Measures standardised: Yes
Name of intervention: DIR/Floortime Intervention valid: Yes
Type of play: Parent and child interaction play
Place: Home
Provider: The first author
Duration: 240 hours
Reported outcome: Functional Emotional Assessment Score (FEAS)
Category: Positive mental health
The change of the FEAS score for the control group reflects the overall developmental progression of only 1.9 (SD = 6.1), compared to the increment of 7.0 (SD = 6.3) for the intervention group. The Student t test shows that the difference is statistically significant (p = .031).
Reported outcome: Functional Emotional Developmental Score (FEDQ)
Category: Positive mental health
Developmental rating of the children was estimated by the parent using the Thai version of the Functional Emotional Questionnaires at baseline and follow-up. The change in data for the intervention group shows that there was a more statistically significant gain in it than in the data of the control group.
S05 Rezaei et al. (2018)
Publication: Journal of Children
Country: Iran
Sample size: N = 34
Sample: Children with ASD
Age: Mean = 12.36
Girls: N = 12 (35%) Boys: N = 22 (65%)
INV: N = 17 CON: N = 17
Design: RCT
Type of control: Randomly allocated, waitlist control
Control group treatment: PRT + Risperidone
Measures: The ABC (Akhondzadeh et al., 2010)
Measures standardised: Yes
Name of intervention: Pivotal response treatment (PRT) (Koegel, 2011) + Risperidone
Intervention valid: Yes
Type of play: Parent and child interaction play
Place: School
Provider: Speech/language therapist
Duration: 27 hours
Reported outcome: Irritability
Category: Negative mental health
There was no significant difference between the INV and Control groups in Irritability subscale.
Reported outcome: Hyperactivity
Category: Negative mental health
There was no significant difference between the INV and Control groups in Hyperactivity subscale.
S06 Schottelkorb et al.. (2020)
Publication: Journal of Counseling & Development
Country: USA
Sample size: N = 23
Sample: Children with ASD
Age: 4-10 years old
Girls: N = 4 (17%) Boys: N = 19 (83%)
INV: N = 12 CON: N = 11
Design: RCT
Type of control: Randomly allocated, waitlist control
Control group treatment: Treatment as usual
Measure: CBCL (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001).
Measures standardised: Yes
Name of intervention: Child-centred play (Axline, 1947)
Intervention valid: Yes
Type of play: Non-directive play
Place: Not reported
Provider: Graduate-level counselling students and two licensed counsellors
Duration: 12 hours
Reported outcome: Externalising problems
Category: Negative mental health
Following the same trend as previous analyses, participants in the play therapy treatment group were reported to have decreased externalising symptoms from pre- to post-testing (M = 68.67, SD = 9.35; M = 63.08, SD = 7.90), whereas control group scores increased (M = 65.36, SD = 9.54; M = 67.27, SD = 8.72).
S07 Siller et al. (2014)
Publication: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
Country: USA
Sample size: N = 70
Sample: Children with ASD
Age: 2-6 years old
Girls: N = 6 (9%) Boys: N = 64 (91%)
INV: N = 36 CON: N = 34
Design: RCT
Type of control: Randomly allocated control group
Control group treatment: Parent Advocacy Coaching (PAC)
Measures: (1) PCSB (Ainsworth, 1978)
(2)AB (Ainsworth, 1978)
(3) MPCA (Hoppes & Harris, 1990)
Measures standardised: Yes
Name of intervention: Focused Playtime Intervention (Siller et al.,2013)
Intervention valid: Yes
Type of play: Parent and child interaction play
Place: Research lab + participants’ home
Provider: Trained graduate and postdoctoral students in developmental psychology and counselling.
Duration: 18 hours
Reported outcome: Maternal Perceptions of Child Attachment (MPCA)
Category: Positive mental health)
A significant main effect of treatment group allocation on gains in parent reported attachment behaviours (MPCA scores), t(48) = 3.0, p < .01.
Reported outcome: Proximity/ Contact Seeking Behavior Scale (PCSB)
Category: Positive mental health)
Proximity and Contact Seeking Behaviors was only marginally significant, t(54) = 1.8, p\.08.
Reported outcome: Avoidant Behavior Scale (AB)
Category: Positive mental health)
For children’s Avoidant Behaviors, results revealed a significant main effect of treatment group allocation on improvements in Avoidant Behaviors from Time 1 to Time 2, t(54) = 2.2, p\.05.
S08 Solomon et al. (2014)
Publication: Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics
Country: USA
Sample size: N = 128
Sample: Children with ASD
Age: 2-5 years old
Girls: N = 20 (16%) Boys: N = 108 (84%)
INV: N = 64 CON: N = 64
Design: RCT
Type of control: Randomly allocated control group
Control group received treatment: Treatment as usual
Measure: The FEAS (Greenspan et al., (2001).
Measures standardised: Yes
Name of intervention: PLAY Project home consultation programme (Solomon et al., 2007)
Intervention valid: Yes
Type of play: Parent and child interaction play
Place: Home
Provider: 6 PLAY consultants (1 occupational therapist, 2 speech and language therapists, and 3 special educators)
Duration: 36 hours
Reported outcome: Functional Emotional Assessment Score (FEAS)
Category: Positive mental health
The FEAS video ratings showed a significant moderate time 3 group effect with the PLAY group showing improvement in observed socioemotional behaviour, whereas the CS group remained stable.
S09 Duifhuis et al. (2017)
Publication:Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
Country: Netherlands
Sample size: N = 24
Sample: Children with ASD
Age: 3-8 years old
Girls: N = 4 (16%) Boys: N = 20 (84%)
INV: N = 11 CON: N = 13
Design: QE
Type of control: Non-randomly allocated control group
Control group received treatment: Yes, treatment as usual
Measure: Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001).
Measures standardised: Yes
Name of intervention: Pivotal response treatment (PRT) (Koegel, 2011)
Intervention valid: Yes
Type of play: Parent and child interaction play
Place: School
Provider: Therapist
Duration: 15 hours
Reported outcome: Internalizing score
Category: Negative mental health
The analysis did not show any treatment effects on internalising score of the INV group on CBCL.
Reported outcome: Externalizing score
Category: Negative mental health
The analysis did not show any treatment effects on externalising score of the INV group on CBCL.
S10 Pilarz (2009)
Publication: PhD Dissertation
Country: USA
Sample size: N = 26
Sample: Children with ASD
Age: 3-12 years old
Girls: N = 5 (19%) Boys: N = 21 (81%)
INV: N = 13 CON: N = 13
Design: QE
Type of control: Non-randomly allocated control group
Control group received treatment: No treatment
Measure: The Functional Emotional Assessment Scale (FEAS) (Greenspan, DeGangi & Wieder, (2001).
Measures standardised: Yes
Name of intervention: DIR/Floortime Intervention valid: Yes
Type of play: Parent and child interaction play
Place: Not reported
Provider: Certified school psychologist
Duration: 16 hours
Reported outcome: Functional Emotional Assessment Score (FEAS)
Category: Positive mental health
The slopes of the pretest scores for the total scale score did not significantly vary across conditions; p-values ranged from .092 to .549.

Study designs

The 10 studies comprised RCTs (n  =  8) and QEs (n  =  2). Overall, RCT studies randomly allocated participants to intervention (n  =  224) and control (n  =  195) groups and QE studies assigned participants to the intervention (n  =  24) and control (n  =  26) groups non-randomly. In the majority of studies, participants received either no treatment and then the same treatment as the intervention group on a waiting list (n  =  5), treatment as usual (TAU, n  =  3), a different intervention (Parent advocacy coaching (PAC), n  =  1) or no treatment (n  =  1).

Sample sizes of sStudies

The sample size of the included studies ranged between 23 and 128 (mean sample  =  47), with a total of 469 participants. Intervention groups’ sample sizes varied from 11 to 64 with a total of 248 participants, while control groups’ samples were between 7 and 64 with a total of 221 children. Of the total sample, 386 were boys (82%) and 83 were girls (18%). Of the included studies, seven were focused on childhood with an age range of 2 to 12, while the other three studies focused on late childhood and adolescence with their samples’ ages ranging from 8 to 16.

Participant identification

All studies were conducted with children with ASD with two being focused on children with “high functioning” ASD. There was no study reporting any other type of social, communication, and or language impairment or used participants with multiple diagnoses. To confirm children's ASD diagnosis, eight out of the 10 studies used additional autism screening tools. The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule - Generic (ADOS-G, Lord et al., 2000) was the most commonly used measure as six of the included studies (Siller et al., 2014; Corbett et al., 2017; Solomon et al., 2014; Duifhuis et al., 2017; Ioannou et al., 2020; Doernberg et al., 2021) used this scale to test the autistic traits of their sample. ADOS-G is a gold standard, semi-structured, standardised observational tool administered by a clinician or researcher that focuses on children's social, communication, and cognitive skills. It consists of two domains, namely social and communication domains with four distinct sections which serve to diagnose children at different language levels. The ADOS-G is suitable to use with children from 2 years old to adulthood and the diagnostic classification relies on two different cutoff values, autism (cutoff  =  12) and ASD (cutoff  =  8) (Lord et al., 1999). In addition, Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R, Lord et al., 1994) scale was also used by two studies to test the ASD characteristics of their samples (Siller et al., 2014; Doernberg et al., 2021). ADI-R is a semi-structured interview for parents/caregivers of children at risk for possible autism diagnosis. The scale is suitable to use with children from 18 months old to adulthood and consists of 93 items which focuses on three developmental domains: Language/ communication, reciprocal social interactions, and restricted, repetitive and stereotyped behaviours. The ADI-R cutoff values for autism diagnosis are as follows: Social interaction (cutoff  =  10), communication and language (cutoff  =  7–8) and restricted/repetitive behaviours (cutoff  =  3). Moreover, the Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS, Schopler et al., 1986), Social Responsiveness Scale–2nd Edition (SRS-2; Constantino & Gruber, 2012), and Early Social Communication Scale (ESCS, Seibert et al., 1982) were the other measures that were used in some studies, respectively; Pajareya and Nopmaneejumruslers (2011), Schottelkorb et al. (2020), and Siller et al. (2014).

Characteristics of play-based interventions

A range of different types of play-based interventions were used in the included studies, such as parent-child interaction play (n  =  6), role/pretend play (n  =  3), and non-directive play (n  =  1). Although nine studies applied named play-based interventions (SENSE, DIR/Floortime, PRT, PLAY Project, CCPT, FPI), only one study used a non-named play intervention (pretend play, see S02). Interventions’ frequencies ranged from once a month to 4 times a week, and the total duration of the interventions ranged from 100 min to 240 h. Three interventions were conducted at participants’ schools, two interventions were conducted at home, two interventions were undertaken outside of school (exact locations were not reported) and one intervention was conducted at a research lab; nevertheless, two papers did not indicate the intervention place. Intervention providers were licensed counsellors (n  =  3), therapists (n  =  2), researcher/s (n  =  2), and not reported (n  =  3). Detailed information about the characteristics of the play-based interventions is provided in Table 3.

Table 3.

Characteristics of the Play-Based Interventions

Name of the intervention Type of Play and Leadership Roles Target age, developmental areas and skills Core Features of the intervention Study ID
The Developmental, Individual-Difference, Relationship-Based (DIR / Floortime®) Type of Play: Parent-child interaction play
Provider: Therapist Role: Trains the parent
Leader: Child Role: Leads the play
Mediator: Parent Role: Joins the child's play and supports them according to the therapist's feedback.
Specifically developed for children with ASD: Yes
Age: Early childhood
Developmental Area: Functional and Emotional Development
Targeted skills:
1-Auditory processing
2-Visual–spatial processing
3-Tactile processing
4-Motor planning
5-Sensory modulation
The DIR / Floortime is a developmental, individual-difference, and relationship-based approach that was introduced by (Greenspan & Wieder (1997). It is a child-centered, parent-mediated play intervention that aims to support children's social functioning, and emotional and language development.
The DIR / Floortime intervention targets children in their early childhood years who show delay in their developmental progression and provides them a range of support according to their developmental needs (Pajareya and Nopmanejumruslers, 2011).
In the DIR / Floortime intervention, first, the parent(s) attend initial training sessions about the principles of DIR/Floortime. During sessions, the therapist trains the parents and other individuals, who play a role in the child's everyday life, about how to set a playground in their home to play with their children, how to extend parent-child interaction their play, and increase circles of social communication and interaction between them (Pilarz, 2009)
S04, S10
The Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT) Type of Play: Parent-child interaction play
Provider: Therapist
Role: Trains the parent
Leader: Child
Role: Leads the play
Mediator: Parent
Role: Joins the child's play and supports them according to the therapist's feedback.
Specifically developed for children with ASD: Yes
Age: Early childhood
Developmental Area: Social and Emotional Development
Targeted skills:
1- Motivation
2- Self-initiation,
3- Self-Management
4- Responsiveness to multiple cues
5- Empathy
The PRT is a play-based intervention that was developed by Koegel et al., (1987) to increase motivation while teaching new skills to children with ASD. It targets five pivotal skills of children with ASD: motivation, self-initiation, self-management, and responsiveness to multiple cues, and empathy (Koegel et al., 1987).
The PRT has been shown effective in improving social, communication skills and reducing behaviour problems in children with ASD (Rezai et al., 20018).
In the PRT sessions, first, the therapist teaches parents how to use the PRT techniques while the child gets used to the therapist. Second, the therapist and parents discuss the developmental needs of the child and set the therapy goals. Third, the parents start to play with their children using the PRT techniques under the therapist's supervision.Fourth, the therapist explains the other possible ways that the parents can apply the PRT techniques into the daily life activities at home. Lastly, the therapist gives the parents videotaped and written feedback about how to improve their PRT skills and apply them in a different range of activities (Duifhuis, 2017).
S05, S09
The Play and Language for Autistic Youngsters (PLAY) Project Home Consultation Type of Play: Parent-child interaction play
Provider: Therapist
Role: Trains the parent
Leader: Child
Role: Leads the play
Mediator: Parent
Role: Joins the child's play and supports them according to the therapist's feedback
Specifically developed for children with ASD: Yes
Age: Early childhood
Developmental Area: Social and Functional Development
Targeted skills:
1- Social reciprocity
2- Social competency
3- Child's autism symptomatology (autistic traits)
The PLAY Project was developed by Solomon et al. (2007) based on the DIR / Floortime approach (Greenspan & Wieder, 1997) which aims to help the parents to connect with their own child with ASD.
The PLAY is a child-centred, parent-mediated intervention that targets to improve social development and reduce autism symptomatology of children with ASD.
In the PLAY, the therapist/consultant trains the parent about how to interact with their child with ASD “through coaching, modelling, and video feedback. During coaching, consultants help parents identify their child's subtle and hard to detect cues, respond contingently to the child's intentions, and effectively engage the child in reciprocal exchanges” (Solomon et al., 2014, p. 478).
S07
The Focused Playtime Intervention (FPI) Type of Play: Parent-child interaction play
Provider: Therapist
Role: Observes, guides, models, and provides feedback to the parent
Leader: Child
Role: Plays independently
Mediator: Parent
Role: Joins the child's play and guides and supports the child's social responsiveness and communication attempts
Specifically developed for children with ASD: Yes
Age: Early childhood
Developmental Area: Social - Communication Development, Attachment
Targeted skills:
1- Attachment skills
2- Social responsiveness
3- Child's spoken communication skills
The FPI is a home-based naturalistic intervention that was developed by Siller et al. (2013). It aims to provide a naturalistic playground to the child to understand the child's developmental stage to set parents’ goals and plan a play environment that would support communication and attachment behaviours of the child (Siller et al., 2014).
The FPI is a family-centred, parent-mediated intervention that targets responsive parental communication to improve participatory and attachment skills of the child in a naturalistic play setting in their homes.
In the FPI, parent advocacy coaching sessions hold in their homes, in which the consultant helps the parents to understand their child's developmental needs, guides them to initiate functional and meaningful social interactions during the play with their child, and trains them about how to respond their child's social-communication attempts in a naturalistic way during their playtime (Siller et al., 2013).
S08
The Social Emotional NeuroScience Endocrinology Theatre® (SENSE) Type of Play: Role / Pretend play
Provider: Therapist
Role: Trains the peers to support the child with ASD during their role play
Leader: Child
Role: Demonstrates role play
Mediator: Peers
Role: Join the child's play to encourage and motivate their friend with ASD, and provide a warm social environment to them.
Specifically developed for children with ASD: Yes
Age: Early childhood
Developmental Area: Social, Cognitive and Emotional Development
Targeted skills:
1- Imagination
2- Theory of mind
3- Social competence
The SENSE is a theatre-based play intervention that was developed by Corbett et al. (2011) and targets social and emotional functioning in children with ASD.
The SENSE is designed as a peer-mediated intervention that aims to improve social competence and reduce the stress level of children with ASD with an intensive peer support. SENSE includes theatrical games, role-playing, and exercises in which children work on their roles for the play (Corbett et al., 2017).
In the SENSE, peers receive training on ten core principles (Corbett et al., 2014) that are mainly about how to socially support the child with ASD, provide them with a warm social environment, and engage in positive social interactions with them. Following the training, peers and the child with ASD engage in role-playing activities under the guidance of the interventionist. While performing role-play, children with ASD experience positive social interactions, develop their level of imagination and TOM skills, learn how to engage with their peers, and respond to social invitations (Ioannou et al., 2020).
S01, S03
The Child-Centered Play Therapy (CCPT) Type of Play: Non-directive play
Provider: Therapist
Role: Joins the child during their play, follows the child's leadership, and reflects the child's emotions.
Leader: Child
Role: Leads the play
Mediator: None
Specifically developed for children with ASD: No
Age: Early childhood
Developmental Area: Social, Emotional and Behavioural Development
Targeted skills:
1- Attachment skills
2- Emotional regulation
3- Emotional recognition
4- Self-control
5- Self-regulation
6- Social competence
The CCPT is a naturalistic and therapeutic intervention that was firstly introduced by Axline (1947) based on Carl Rogers's client-based psycho-therapy principles. It is a theoretically grounded intervention that targets children's emotional and behavioural problems.
The CCPT is a child-centered, non-directive approach that aims to help children to explore and express their deep feelings, emotions, and thoughts during a free play session.
During the CCPT, the therapist develops a warm and friendly relationship with the child, accept the child unconditionally, provide them a suitable atmosphere to freely express their feelings, reflect their feelings, believe in the child's problem-solving potential, does not direct or limit the child unless it is necessary (Axline, 1969, p.73).
In the CCPT, the therapist creates a play environment that would suit CCPT principles. At the beginning of the session, the therapist informs the child about the time of the play. During the play, the therapist follows the child's lead and takes notes about the play themes, play behaviours, verbalisations, expressed emotions and informs the parents accordingly after each session (Schottelkorb et al., 2020).
S06
Pretend Play Type of Play: Pretend / Symbolic play
Provider: Therapist
Role: Joins the child during their pretend-play activity, follows the child's leadership, and reflects the child's emotions.
Leader: Child
Role: Leads the play
Mediator: None
Specifically developed for children with ASD: No
Age: Early childhood
Developmental Area: Social, Emotional and Cognitive Development
Targeted skills:
1- Imagination
2- Perspective taking
3- Affect expression
5- Social communication
6- Prosociality
Pretend play is a common type of play that is closely related to a range of cognitive abilities of a child such as symbolic functioning, memory, and imagination. Children start pretend-playing (make-believe) at around 2, and become masters in pretend play at around 4 years of age (Piaget, 1952).
The Pretend play intervention is a child centred play intervention that targets the child's imaginative skills, expressed emotions, verbalisation, and role-play skills.
In the pretend play intervention, in which the therapist creates a free play environment with unstructured toys for the child. During the intervention, the therapist joins the child's play, follows the child's lead, uses prompts, and reflects the child's emotions to help children to developing new skills such as making up new things, creating stories, and to improve their level of imagination and organisation (Doernberg et al., 2021).
S02

Mental health outcomes

Of the included studies, five studies used parent-report, three used self-report, and four used observational scales to test the effectiveness of the play-based interventions on the mental health outcome of children with ASD. In terms of measuring the negative mental health outcomes, the researchers used the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children (STAI-C, Spielberger et al., 1983) to measure the anxiety level of children with ASD. In addition, the Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL, Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001) was used to measure internalising and externalising problems, the Aberrant Behavior Checklist (ABC, Aman et al., 1985) was used to test irritability and hyperactivity, and the Kusche Affective Inventory-Revised (KAI-R, Kusche et al., 1988) was used to measure total negative affect. In terms of testing the positive mental health outcomes; however, the Functional Emotional Assessment Scale (FEAS, Greenspan et al., 2001) was used to assess emotional functioning; the Functional Emotional Developmental Questionnaire (FEDQ, Greenspan & Greenspan, 2002) was used to assess emotional development; the Proximity and Contact Seeking Behaviors (PCSB, Ainsworth, 1978) and Maternal Perceptions of Child Attachment Scales (MPCA, Hoppes and Harris, 1990) were used to evaluate children's attachment-related behaviours; and the KAI-R scale was used to measure total positive affect in children with ASD. The measures noted are well-used standardised measures of the relevant concepts. More information about the mental health classifications of the reported outcomes and the outcome measures can be found in Appendix D.

Geographical location of studies

Studies were undertaken in the United States of America (n  =  7), Thailand (n  =  1), Netherlands (n  =  1), and Iran (n  =  1).

Risk of bias

Included RCT and QE studies were appraised separately and independently by four raters (all four study authors) using a risk of bias tool appropriate to each study design. The results of the risk of bias ratings are reported for RCTs (n  =  8) and QEs (n  =  2) separately and presented in Table 4 and Table 5, respectively. Of the eight RCT studies, one study (S02) did not meet the random allocation criterion whereas the other seven randomly allocated the participants into the intervention and control groups. Although the allocation process was concealed in the majority of the RCTs (S01, SO2, SO3, S06, S07, S08), two studies did not conceal the allocation sequence (S04 & S05). Only one of the studies (S08) clearly reported that participants were blind to the intervention; three studies reported that carers and interventionists were blind to the intervention (S01, S03, & S05). In one study (S02), the number of missing observations was high which means the reported outcome data were not available for all or nearly all participants. Additionally, five studies reported having a pre-specified analysis plan (S01, S04, S05, S06, & S08), and three studies had a priori protocols (S03, S05, & S06). No studies reported selected outcome variables and all RCTs used appropriate measures to report the outcome variables. In summary, of the eight RCTs, one study was judged to have a high risk of bias (S02), five demonstrated some concerns (S01, S03, S04, S07, S08) and two was judged to have a low risk of bias (S05 & S06).

Table 4.

Risk of bias assessment of the RCT research

Domain 1 Domain 2 Domain 3 Domain 4 Domain 5 Overall Risk of Bias
Study ID 1.1 Random allocation? 1.2 Allocation sequence concealed? 1.3 ROB 2.1. Participants blinded to the intervention? 2.2. Carers/interventionists blinded to the intervention? 2.3 ROB 3.Outcome data available for all participants? 3.2 Bias due to Missing outcome? 3.3 ROB 4.1 Inappropriate method to measure the outcome? 4.3 ROB 5.1 Pre-specified analysis plan? 5.2 Selected reported outcome? 5.3 A priori protocol? 5.4 ROB Summary ROB
S01 Yes Yes Low NI PY SC Yes No Low No Low Yes No No Low SC
S02 No PY SC NI NI SC No NI SC No Low No PN N High High
S03 Yes PY Low PN PY SC Yes PN Low No Low No No PY SC SC
S04 Yes PN Low NI NI Low Yes No Low No Low PY N N SC SC
S05 PY PN Low PN PY Low Yes PN Low No Low PY PN PY Low Low
S06 Yes PY Low No PN Low Yes PN Low No Low PY PN Yes Low Low
S07 Yes PY Low No No Low PY Y SC No Low PN No No SC SC
S08 Yes Yes Low PY PN SC PY PN Low PY Low PY PN PN SC SC

Table 5.

Risk of bias assessment of the QE research

Domain 1 Domain 2 Domain 3 Domain 4 Domain 5 Overall Risk of Bias
Study ID  1.1 Confounding effect of intervention? 1.2 Participants selected based on their characteristics? 1.3 ROB 2.1. Intervention group clearly defined? 2.2. 2.4 Intervention groups defined before the intervention? 2.3 ROB 3.1 Outcome data available for all participants? 3.2 Bias due to Missing outcome? 3.3 ROB Inappropriate method to measure the outcome? 4.3 ROB 5.1 Pre-specified analysis plan? 5.2 Selected reported outcome? 5.3 A priori protocol? 5.4 ROB Summary ROB
S09 PN PN Low Yes PY Low Yes PN Low No Low PY PN Yes Low Low
S10 PY No High Yes Yes Low PY PN Low No Low PY No No SC High

Of the two QE studies (S09 & S10) none allocated the participants into the intervention group based on their observed characteristics after the intervention started. However, the authors identified a potential confounding effect of the intervention in one study (S10), which posed a high risk of bias. Both QE studies clearly defined their intervention groups based on the information recorded before the intervention. Additionally, outcome data were available for all participants in both studies and none reported selected outcomes or potential bias due to missing outcomes. Both studies used appropriate methods to measure their outcomes and had a pre-specified analysis plan; however, only one of the QE studies had a priori protocol (S09). Consequently, of the QE studies, one was judged to have a low risk of bias (S09) and the other demonstrated a high risk of bias (S10).

Results of synthesis

Of the ten studies which met the criteria for inclusion in the review, all had sufficient numerical data to be included in the meta-analysis. Eighteen mental health outcomes were identified and categorised as either positive mental health (n  =  10) or negative mental health (n  =  8). Average scores were computed in cases where an individual study had multiple outcomes in the same category (either positive or negative mental health), resulting in fewer outcomes; positive mental (n  =  5) and negative mental health (n  =  6) outcomes.

Intervention effects

The effect of play-based interventions on positive and negative mental health outcomes are discussed and the results of the meta-analysis are displayed in the forest plots. The forest plots (see Figure 2 and Figure 4) show the study labels and corresponding effect size estimate represented by a blue square with an orange circle or marker at the centre of the square that indicates the study-specific effect size with respective horizontal lines that show the magnitude of the confidence intervals. Generally, longer lines indicate wider confidence intervals which suggest smaller samples. The absence of a horizontal line from some boxes indicates the study has restricted confidence intervals which may suggest more precise effect sizes (Higgins, et al., 2021). The black vertical line running through zero, known as the line of no effect, indicates differences between significant and non-significant interventions. The red vertical line shows the overall measure of the effect. All intervention effects in this sample were statistically significant.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Forest Plot Showing Effect of Positive Mental Health Outcomes is 1.60 (95% CI [0.37, 2.82], p = .01).

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Forest Plot Showing Effect of Negative Mental Health Outcomes is 0.04 (95% CI [-0.44, 0.51], p = .88).

Play-based interventions intervening on positive mental health outcomes for children with ASD had an overall significant positive effect of 1.60 (95% CI [.37, 2.82], p  =  .01). The results are shown in the forest plot in Figure 2. There is evidence of significant statistical heterogeneity, Q(4)  =  34.30, p < .001 and high proportional variance, I²  =  91.72% which confirms observations of variability in study characteristics. The predicted interval of intervention effects ranges between −5 and 5 but this should be treated cautiously given the small number of studies. Interestingly, there is a distinct pattern of a decrease in the size of study effects from 2008 to the present (Figure 3).

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Cumulative Forest Plot Showing Effect of Positive Mental Health Outcomes by Year.

In contrast, interventions addressing negative mental health outcomes show overall intervention effects were not significant of .04 (95% CI [-.44, 0.51], p  =  .88). In addition, there is significant study heterogeneity Q(5)  =  12.03, p  =  .003 and high proportional variance, I²  =  60.68%, which confirms observations of variability in study characteristics with the predicted interval range between −2 and 2. The results are shown in the forest plot in Figure 4. The pattern of change in intervention effects in progressive years of research is sporadic for studies researching negative mental health outcomes (see Figure 5).

Figure 5.

Figure 5.

Cumulative Forest Plot Showing Effect of Negative Mental Health Outcomes by Year

Small-Study effects

From scrutinising the funnel plots, there was no indication of publication bias despite the small sample of studies included in each category. Funnel plots for positive mental and negative mental health outcomes are portrayed in Figure 6 and Figure 7, respectively. Trim-and-fill analyses were conducted to further investigate potential publication bias due to small study effects and between study heterogeneity (Duval & Tweedie, 2000). However, trim-and-fill analysis results did not recommend imputations for either category again suggesting that study effects may not be impacted by publication bias (Shi & Lin, 2019).

Figure 6.

Figure 6.

Funnel Plot of Positive Mental Health Outcomes.

Figure 7.

Figure 7.

Funnel Plot of Negative Mental Health Outcomes.

Sensitivity analysis

Sensitivity analyses were used to check if the findings for positive and negative mental health were robust to factors relating to the research design or risk of bias. With the five positive mental health studies, classification by research design comprised 4 RCTs and 1 QE and 2 studies with high risk of bias and 3 studies where there were some concerns about risk of bias. When the meta-analysis is repeated excluding the QE study, the resulting pooled effect size is slightly smaller but remains positive: 1.14 (95% CI [.22, 2.07], p  =  .02) with significant study heterogeneity Q(3)  =  23.42, p < .001, and high proportional variance I²  =  86.20%. Sensitivity analysis factoring risk of bias results using sub-group analysis (see Figure 8) indicated there was no significant difference in intervention effects depending on the risk of bias rating of studies addressing positive mental health (p  =  .81).

Figure 8.

Figure 8.

Sensitivity Analysis: Positive Mental Health by Risk of Bias Appraisal.

A similar check was done with the six studies assessing negative mental health for research design - RCT (n  =  5) and QE (n  =  1) and risk of bias - high risk of bias (n  =  1), some concerns (n  =  2), and low risk of bias (n  =  3). In this case, when the meta-analysis is repeated excluding the QE study, the intervention effect is positive but remains non-significant : .24 (95% CI [-.06, .53 ], p  =  .12) with no indication of heterogeneity among the studies Q(4)  =  4.80, p  =  .31, and proportional variance I²  =  0%. The results of the sensitivity analysis by the risk of bias results using sub-group analysis (see Figure 9) revealed no significant difference in intervention effects depending on the risk of bias rating of studies addressing negative mental health (p  =  .07).

Figure 9.

Figure 9.

Sensitivity Analysis: Negative Mental Health by Risk of Bias Appraisal.

Discussion

A headline finding from this systematic review is the paucity of experimental work using play-based interventions to address the mental health needs of children with ASD and DLD. Only ten studies met the inclusion criteria after screening the extant literature from the past twenty years and these were reportedly undertaken only in the past twelve years. All of the studies were conducted with children with ASD; thus emphasising the existence of a gap within the play-based and mental health experimental literature for children with DLD. A recent review to identify the efficacy of support for treatment of DLD did not identify any named play-based intervention, although several studies reported delivering activities in a playful manner or via the use of computer games, storytelling, etc. (Rinaldi et al., 2021). In comparison, the use of play-based interventions with children with ASD is ubiquitous except there are fewer experimental designs that explicitly assess mental health outcomes.

Positive mental health outcomes

The meta-analysis results support the efficacy of play-based interventions to address the positive mental health needs of children with ASD (Cohen's d  =  1.60). There were five studies grouped as positive mental health in the meta-analysis which used four different play-based interventions and assessed three different mental health outcomes. Specifically, outcomes assessed related to emotional development from a DIR/Floortime intervention (Pilarz, 2009 & Pajareya & Nopmaneejumruslers, 2011); attachment from a Focused Playtime Intervention and the PLAY Project Home Consultation, (Siller et al., 2014 & Solomon et al., 2014, respectively); and positive affect from a Pretend Play Intervention (Doernberg et al., 2021). The five individual studies all reported significant positive intervention effects and the play-based interventions, although different, were grouped as a common dependent variable.

DIR/Floortime interventions are explicitly designed to focus on emotional development and emotion building. DIR/Floortime measures changes in the ASD child's capacity for shared attention, the ability to form and have warm, intimate relationships and the ability to initiate using intentioned actions and social engagements that lead to spontaneous communication (Hess, 2012). The PLAY Project, also based on DIR/Floortime, was developed as a programme to train child development professionals to coach parents, typically at home, to help their child with ASD make developmental gains through play-based interactions (Solomon et al., 2014). Given its broader focus, the PLAY project includes other related outcomes measures like attachment (Solomon et al., 2014). Alternatively, validated interventions like Focused Playtime Intervention and the Pretend Play Intervention, generally facilitates capacity within families to meet the needs of their children by supporting social communication, play-skills, etc. and similarly assesses mental health outcomes like attachment (Siller et al., 2014; Doernberg et al., 2021).

Negative mental health outcomes

The overall non-significant effect of negative mental health outcomes (Cohen's d  =  .04) from play-based interventions raises an important query about what factors may contribute to results different from positive mental health. Six studies were identified which used four different play-based interventions and six different measures of mental health. The outcomes related to anxiety from Sense Theatre interventions (Corbett et al., 2017; Ioannou et al., 2020); internalising and externalising behaviour using PRT and Child Centred Play Therapy (Duifhuis et al., 2017; Schottelkorb et al., 2020, respectively); irritability & hyperactivity also using PRT (Rezaei et al., 2018); and negative affect from the Pretend Play intervention by Doernberg, et al. (2021), which is the only study which included both a positive and negative measure of mental health.

The Sense Theatre is an expressive intervention that uses peer mediation with typically developing peers of ASD children to teach adaptive social behaviour. Sense theatre tries to counter negative social experiences that cause increased anxiety and stress that may lead to avoidance of social play (Ioannou et al., 2020). Both Corbett et al. (2017) and Ioannou et al. (2020) studies reported that participants receiving SENSE intervention showed significant intervention group effects on trait anxiety but not state anxiety. Child-centred Play Therapy, is another individualised mental health intervention, designed to use the relationship between therapist and child to reduce relational or physically harmful ways of interacting, as well as increasing their sense of self-responsibility toward behaviour (Ray et al., 2012). Externalising behaviour significantly decreased for participants in the Child-centered Play Therapy group at the end of the intervention but there was not a significant effect by group (Schottelkorb et al., 2020). PRT is another method used to reduce behavioural problems of children with autism and improve communication and social interaction (Rezai, et al., 2018). After using the PRT, Duifhuis et al., (2017) measured internalising and externalising behaviour as a secondary outcome and found the difference between the treatment and control group was not significant. Similarly, Rezai et al. (2018) reported there was no significant difference in behaviours relating to irritability and hyperactivity for the treatment and control group post intervention. The Pretend play intervention by Doernberg et al. (2021) reported that intervention and control groups were not dissimilar in their capacity to identify negative affect emotions.

Intervention characteristics

As pointed out by Gibson et al. (2021), in their scoping review of play-based interventions with two- to eight-year-old children with ASD, research in this field is heterogeneous. The diversity of play-based approaches included means it is not possible to conclude that any one play-based intervention is more effective in this review. There is little detail on the extent of play engagement beyond identifying activities as including either role-play, pretend-play, toy-play, etc. The person delivering the intervention has a key role and is typically a play partner to the ASD child or facilitates training to mediate play between the ASD child and their parents or peers. The sample disproportionately represented boys (82%) and interventions were more frequently implemented with participants younger than twelve (n  =  7). The study settings were between home, school, and outside school, and durations of interventions were between one-hundred to two-hundred and forty minutes.

Evaluation of evidence

We know that social, communication, and or language difficulties can have an adverse impact on the mental health of individuals at all ages of development (Conti-Ramsden et al., 2019; Hudson et al., 2019). The contrasting results for positive and negative mental health is particularly interesting and raises an important query about whether there are some types of [mental] outcomes that play-based interventions may not be suited to address. The limited data do not allow firm conclusions to be drawn. For instance, some play-based interventions are well-established, standardised programmes, but reported non-significant intervention effects on mental health outcomes, e.g. irritability or internalising/externalising behaviour, (Duifhuis et al., 2017; Rezaei et al., 2018). In response, we may need a theory that elucidates the characteristics of play-based interventions for supporting mental health outcomes in samples of young persons with social, communication and or language impairments and intervention studies that treat mental health as primary outcomes to minimise the influence of confounds.

We acknowledge that the studies in the review focused on children with ASD but play-based interventions have a well-established history of being used with diverse samples e.g., learning disabilities and is a popular strategy for directly addressing psychosocial problems in children (Genç et al., 2021). Interventions with DLD are necessary because children with DLD may have different mental health profiles from children with ASD. We expect that our dichotomous categorisation of positive and negative mental health may change to reflect more nuance and specific aspects of mental health once research in this field becomes less disparate.

Strengths and limitations of the review

To minimise the introduction of potential sources of bias into the systematic review, a rigorous selection process was followed which involved pre-specified eligibility criteria, a comprehensive search of electronic databases, double screening of titles and abstracts, consensus discussions between study authors to resolve disagreements, and independent double data extraction by two pairs of study authors. In the end, there was agreement among all authors on the papers included in the review. As indicated at the beginning of the methods section, additional outcomes specific to social communication, and language were part of the literature screening but are not addressed in this paper. The risk of bias from the level of individual studies can also influence the overall meta-analysis results (Higgins et al., 2021); hence studies were appraised by two pairs of authors. About seven studies did not meet the low risk of bias threshold. From the risk of bias activity undertaken for each study, the most common issues flagged had to do with allocation concealment (6 studies) for RCTs and pre-specifying data analysis plans (5 studies), applicable to both RCTs and QEs.

Implications and recommendations

A key implication of this work is that it raises awareness among researchers, clinicians, and practitioners to consider mental health outcomes when using play-based interventions. There is an imperative to better understand the benefits and possible drawbacks of using play-based interventions to support the mental health of individuals with social, communication, and or language impairments so stakeholders can make informed choices about treatment. We hope this review highlights the need to prioritise research investigating the efficacy of play-based interventions to support the mental health of children with SLI. It is not only important to embark on more experimental work but it is also imperative that new research strive to meet gold standards in experimental designs. The specific examples of positive mental health in the context of this review are limited to positive affect, attachment, and emotional functioning. Therefore, evidence for other plausible positive mental health variables like happiness, well-being, life satisfaction, etc. remain lacking. There is a distinct need for newer research particularly RCTs and QEs to discriminate between the diverse range of play-based interventions and their potential effect across the range of mental health outcomes - positive and negative, that exists. Furthermore, the technique of using play as a medium of engagement in interventions with children with ASD or DLD, means play cannot be used as a dependent variable in RCTs. Thus, it becomes challenging to fully assess the efficacy of play in such treatments. Gibson et al. (2021) recommends conceptualising the role of play in play-based interventions using the broad categories of “context”, “component”, or a “key mechanism” to help practitioners reflect on their views and practices regarding play. There is also still limited understanding of the intensity required to optimise intervention effects in this field.

Conclusion

There were few published high quality RCTs or QEs investigating play-based interventions for mental health in children with ASD or DLD in the last twenty years. The few studies identified investigated play-based interventions in children with ASD and showed a beneficial effect on positive mental health but not negative mental health. This review makes a valuable contribution in presenting the state of evidence [or lack thereof] addressing the mental health needs of children with ASD or DLD.

Acknowledgements

We thank Professor Catherine Hewitt for statistical advice on the meta-analysis.

Appendix A. Boolean Search Strings

Play (Title) AND “language disorder” (Title & Abstract & Keyword)
Play (Title) AND “language impairment” (Title & Abstract)
Play (Title) AND “language delay” (Title & Abstract)
Play (Title) AND “language disorder” (Title & Abstract)
Play (Title) AND “speech and language” (Title & Abstract)
Play (Title) AND “speech and communication” (Title & Abstract)
Play (Title) AND “specific language impairment” (Title & Abstract)
Play (Title) AND “communication need” (Title & Abstract)
Play (Title) AND “Slow talk” (Title & Abstract)
Play (Title) AND “developmental language disorder” (Title & Abstract)
Play (Title) AND Autism (Title & Abstract)
Play (Title) AND Austistic (Title & Abstract)
Play (Title) AND pervasive develop (Title & Abstract & Field)
Play (Title) AND PDP (Title & Abstract)
Play (Title) AND Asperger (Title & Abstract & Field)
Play (Title) AND “SLI” (Title & Abstract & Full Text)
JASPER ((Joint Attention, Symbolic Play, Engagement & Regulation) (Title) & Autism
ASAP (Advancing Social-communication And Play) (Title)
DIR: Floortime (Title)
CCPT (Child-Centered Play Therapy) (Title) AND Autism (Title & Abstract & Full Text)
Filial therapy (Title)
Lego (Title) AND Autism (Title & Abstract & Full Text)
Lego (Title) AND Language (Title & Abstract & Full Text)
Lego (Title) AND speech (Title & Abstract & Full Text)
Lego (Title) AND communication (Title & Abstract)
Canine-assisted play (Title)
Theraplay (Title)
Jungian Play (Title)
Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT) (Title)

Appendix B. Risk-of-Bias Tool (extracted from Cochrane RoB 2 and Cochrane RoB1)

Domain 1: Risk of bias arising from the randomisation process
Questions Comments Response
1.1 Was the allocation sequence random? Y / PY / PN / N / NI
1.2 Was the allocation sequence concealed until participants were enrolled and assigned to interventions? Y / PY / PN / N / NI
QE relevant questions - Risk of bias due to confounding
1.3 Is there potential for confounding of the effect of intervention in this study? e.g allocations using baseline equivalents, appropriate statistical analysis of difference on pre-tests, etc. Y / PY / PN / N / NI
1.4. Was selection of participants into the study (or into the analysis) based on participant characteristics observed after the start of intervention? Y / PY / PN / N / NI
Risk-of-bias judgement Low / High / Some concerns
Optional: What is the overall predicted direction of bias for this outcome? NA / Favours experimental / Favours comparator / Towards null /Away from null / Unpredictable
Domain 2: Risk of bias due to deviations from the intended interventions (effect of assignment to intervention)
2.1. Were participants aware of their assigned intervention during the trial? Y / PY / PN / N / NI
2.2. Were carers and people delivering the interventions aware of participants’ assigned intervention during the trial? Y / PY / PN / N / NI
QE relevant questions
2.3Was the intervention clearly defined Y / PY / PN / N / NI
2.4 Was the information used to define intervention groups recorded at the start of the intervention? Y / PY / PN / N / NI
Risk-of-bias judgement Low / High / Some concerns
Optional: What is the overall predicted direction of bias for this outcome? NA / Favours experimental / Favours comparator / Towards null /Away from null / Unpredictable
Domain 3: Missing outcome data
3.1 Were data for this outcome available for all, or nearly all, participants randomised? Y / PY / PN / N / NI
3.2 Is there evidence that the result was not biased by missing outcome data? Y / PY / PN / N / NI
Risk-of-bias judgement Low / High / Some concerns
Optional: What is the overall predicted direction of bias for this outcome? NA / Favours experimental / Favours comparator / Towards null /Away from null / Unpredictable
Domain 4: Risk of bias in measurement of the outcome
4.1 Was the method of measuring the outcome inappropriate? Y / PY / PN / N / NI
Risk-of-bias judgement Low / High / Some concerns
Optional: What is the overall predicted direction of bias for this outcome? NA / Favours experimental / Favours comparator / Towards null /Away from null / Unpredictable
Domain 5: Risk of bias in selection of the reported result
5.1 Were the data that produced this result analysed in accordance with a pre-specified analysis plan that was finalised before unblinded outcome data were available for analysis? Y / PY / PN / N / NI
5.2 Is the numerical result being assessed likely to have been selected, on the basis of the results, from… Y / PY / PN / N / NI
5.3 Did they have a priori protocol? Yes No Not clear
Risk-of-bias judgement Low / High / Some concerns
Optional: What is the overall predicted direction of bias for this outcome? NA / Favours experimental / Favours comparator / Towards null /Away from null / Unpredictable

(Y) Yes, (PY) Probably Yes, (PN) Probably No, (N) No, (NI) No information.

Appendix C.

Database Screening Results By Search Terms

  Keywords searching results / Number of Studies Identified (N)
Data Base Ebscohost Web of Science Pubmed SCOPUS PsycINFO PROQuest Ethos Google Scholar  
Search Terms Date of search 18/01/2021
22/01/2021
23/01/2021
26/01/2021
27/01/2021
31/01/2021
01/02/2021
03/02/2021
04/02/2021
06/02/2021
07/02/2021
10/02/2021
11/02/2021
12/02/2021
13/02/2021
14/02/2021
Total Identified
Play (Title) AND “Language” (Title & Abstract) 2160 / 321 1553 / 153 7080 / 155 617 / 54 298 / 69 620 / 58 13 / 3 Not Screened 813
Play (Title) AND “Speech” (Title & Abstract) 610 / 36 75 / 12 91 / 21 130 / 9 40 / 7 47 / 4 3 / 1 Not Screened 90
Play (Title) AND “Communication” (Title) 174 / 24 254 / 20 146 / 21 519 / 28 142 / 33 216 / 33 20 / 4 Not Screened 163
Play (Title) AND “specific Language Impairment” (Title & Abstract) 2/2 4 / 4 3 / 2 2 / 2 4 / 3 3 / 2 4 / - Not Screened 15
Play (Title) AND “Developmental Language Disorder” (Title & Abstract) 5 / 5 2 / 2 2 / 2 3 / 2 27 / 6 - - Not Screened 17
Play (Title) AND Autis* (Title & Abstract) 477 / 396 370 / 321 298 / 220 504 / 307 459 / 399 146 / 39 130 / 19 Not Screened 1701
Play (Title) AND Pervasive Develop* (Title & Abstract & Field) 124 / 104 15 / 3 - 53 / 2 1 / - - 6 / - Not Screened 109
Play (Title) AND Asperger (Title & Abstract & Field) 19 / 8 21 / 11 6 / 3 8 / 5 3 / 2 10 / 5 12 / 1 Not Screened 35
Play (Title) AND “SLI*” (Title & Abstract & Full Text) 23 / 6 5 / 2 3 / 2 - 4 / - - 7 / 1 Not Screened 11
Joint Attention, Symbolic Play, Engagement & Regulation (Title) AND Autis* (Title) 3 / 3 5 / 5 6 / 6 1 / 1 1 / 1 1 / 1 - 11 / 11 28
Advancing Social-communication (Title) AND Play (Title) 3 / 3 4 / 4 2 / 2 1 / 1 5 / 2 1 / 1 - 5 / 5 18
DIR: Floortime (Title) 15 / 11 26 / 9 12 / 8 12 / 4 50 / 15 10 / 7 1 / 1 105 / 13 68
Child-Centered Play Therapy (Title) AND Autis* (Title & Abstract & Full Text) 9 / 5 - - 1 / 1 187 / 5 4 / 4 - 23 / 10 25
Filial therapy (Title) 46 / - 17 / - 4 / - 74 / 1 212 / 2 83 / - - 3 / 2 5
Lego (Title) AND Autis* (Title & Abstract & Full Text) 47 / 20 11 / 9 9 / 7 14 / 11 30 / 12 10 / 10 8 / 6 43 / 31 106
Lego (Title) AND Language (Title & Abstract & Full Text) 145 / 4 59 / 1 4 / 1 19 / 1 40 / 3 3 / 1 - 40 / 2 13
Lego (Title) AND Speech (Title & Abstract & Full Text) 36 / - - - 8 / 2 24 / 1 - 3 / - 12 / - 3
Lego (Title) AND Communication (Title & Abstract) 35 / 2 61 / 3 7 / 3 86 / 19 1 / - 4 / 1 15 / 4 43 / 4 36
Canine-assisted play (Title) 1 / 1 - - 1 / 1 2 / 2 1 / - - 5 / 2 6
Theraplay (Title) 27 / 3 26 / 2 5 / - 28 / 5 66 / 8 19 / 1 1 / - 11 / 8 27
Jungian Play (Title) 42 / 1 - 45 / - 6 / 1 3 / - - - 22 / 1 3
Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT) (Title) 96 / 12 61 / 33 34 / 27 60 / 30 60 / 38 56 / 39 - 168 / 40 219
Results
Number of Studies Screened 4099 2569 7755 2147 1659 1234 223 514 19995
Number of Studies Identified 625 516 384 383 561 187 39 128 2823
Records identified Through other sources 59
Total Number of Studies Carried to Abstract Screening 2882

Note. Limiter: Studies published between January 1st, 2000- January 15th, 2021.

Appendix D.

Mental Health Classification of Studies

MH Classification Author Study Measures Relating to Review Aims: Description Extracted from Individual Papers Measurement Scales Selected Measurement Scales excluded
Positive Mental Health Pajareya and Nopmaneejumruslers (2019) Primary Outcome The Functional Emotional Assessment Scale (FEAS) (child behaviours) - to measure changes in children's functional development. A 15-min videotaped child–parent interaction was collected for each child. Each parent was asked to play with their child as they normally would at home using a standard set of toys (including symbolic, tactile and movement play materials). Secondary Outcome Functional Emotional Developmental Questionnaire (FEDQ) - The questionnaire was related to Greenspan's Six Functional Development Levels (FDL): 1) shared attention and regulation; 2) engagement and relating; 3) purposeful emotional interaction; 4) social problem solving; 5) creating ideas; and 6) thinking logically. The difference between the increments determined the clinical progression. FEAS scoring reported did not include sub-scales so the summary score is used in the meta-analysis. The score is interpreted as having positive directionality FEDQ scoring reported did not include sub-scales so the summary score is used in the meta-analysis. The score is interpreted as having positive directionality No sub-scales were excluded No sub-scales were excluded
 Positive Mental Health Solomon et al. (2014) Primary Outcome The Functional Emotional Assessment Scale – is a video assessment of a child's interactional/ social functioning. The FEAS has 6 sections and 34 items based on Greenspan's 6 functional developmental levels (FDLs), 33 which progress from simple attention (FDL 1) and engagement (FDL 2) to 2-way purposeful reciprocal exchanges (FDL 3), to problem solving gestures (FDL 4), and then to the consistent use of words (FDL 5) leading to rich pretend play, emotional thinking, and complex interaction (FDL 6). Items are rated as 0 (not at all or very brief), 1 (present some of the time, observed several times), or 2 (consistently present, observed many times). Ratings were summed to compute scores. Higher raw scale scores indicate greater social–emotional development. Items are rated as 0 (not at all or very brief), 1 (present some of the time, observed several times), or 2 (consistently present, observed many times). Ratings were summed to compute scores. Higher raw scale scores indicate greater social–emotional development. Parent child free play with toys in the home was video recorded for 15 min at pre- and post-assessment and coded by raters blind to group allocation and assessment time. Child Behaviour Rating Scale (CBRS) - The CBRS is composed of 7 items, which assesses 2 interactive style dimensions for children: Attention (4 items, a 5 .88 at baseline and .89 at follow-up) and Initiation (3 items, a 5 .70 at baseline and .83 at follow-up). FEAS scoring reported did not include sub-scales so the summary score is used in the meta-analysis. The score is interpreted as having positive directionality. CBRS measure selected and both subscales are used: Attention Initiation  No sub-scales were excluded No sub-scales were excluded
 Positive Mental Health Pilarz (2009) Primary Outcome The Functional Emotional Assessment Scale (FEAS) - The FEAS assesses children whose developmental age is between 7 months and 4 years, who are at risk or have problems in the development of attachment, regulation, social engagement, play interactions and emotional functioning. The FEAS focuses on the dynamic interaction between caregiver-child. The FEAS provides a formal system of coding child and caregiver's behaviours using the six developmental levels of the DIR/Floortime Model. FEAS measure selected but only the ‘total sub-scale’ is used in the meta-analysis. The score is interpreted as having positive directionality. FEAS subscales excluded: Subscale 1 - Attachment Subscale 2 - Regulation Subscale 3 - Social engagement Subscale 4 - Play interactions Subscale 5 - Emotional functioning
Positive Mental Health Outcomes Doernberg, et al. (2021) Primary Outcome The Kusche Affective Inventory Revised (KAI-R) - a child interview-based measure used to directly evaluate children's emotional understanding abilities. Two sections: 1) their ability to generate lists of feelings and describe emotional experiences of simple and complex emotions, while defining more complex emotions, and 2. their ability to understand the emotional states of self and other). (For the emotional experience section, children were asked to generate a list of as many positive and negative feelings that they could without a time limit, and up to three prompts of “Any more?” by the researcher. A total frequency score was then calculated for positive emotions, negative emotions, and total number of emotions. Responses were coded only if they qualified as valid positive/negative emotions per the manual (Greenberg et al., 1995). Each child was next asked to provide examples of times when he/she had felt 10 specific emotions, ranging from simple to complex: happy, sad, angry, scared, love, proud, guilty, jealous, anxious, and lonely (e.g., “Tell me about a time when you felt happy?”). If a child had difficulty providing an example for a particular emotion, he/she was asked to give an example of a time when someone else felt that emotion (Greenberg et al., 1995). Responses were coded on a 4-point developmental scale according to the manual (Greenberg et al., 1995). A sum score was calculated for the child's ability to appropriately describe an emotional experience (sum of all coded responses across the 10 different emotions based on the 4-point scale). Additionally, a sum score was calculated for the child's quality in defining a more complex emotion (sum of all coded responses across the five latter complex emotions— proud, guilty, jealous, anxious, and lonely—based on a 0–2 scale from Greenberg et al., 1995). For the understanding of emotional states of self and other section of the KAI-R, children were asked to provide cues used to recognise emotional states in themselves and others. The children were asked about five emotional states: happy, sad, scared, angry, and love (e.g., for self: “How do you know when you are feeling happy?”, for other: “How do you know when other people are feeling happy?). The responses were coded in accordance with the cognitive developmental framework of Carroll and Steward (1984), on a 4-point developmental scale according to the manual (Greenberg et al., 1995). The scores for each of the five emotions for self and other were summed to provide two total scores of understanding of emotional states of self, and understanding of emotional states of other. KAIR measure selected but only the following sub-scales are used: Total positive affect (coded as positive mental health) This scale consists of counts of positive feelings and is interpreted as having positive directionality. KAIR excluded Scales: Total affect Total appropriate Total quality Total understanding of self Total understanding of other
Negative Mental Health Doernberg, et al. (2021) Primary Outcome The Kusche Affective Inventory Revised (KAI-R) - KAIR measure selected but only the following was sub-scales used: Total negative affect (coded as negative mental health). The scale consists of counts of negative feelings and is interpreted as having positive directionality.  KAIR excluded Scales: Total affect Total appropriate Total quality Total understanding of self Total understanding of other
Negative Mental Health Corbett, et al. (2017) Primary Outcome STAI-C. The STAI-C is a questionnaire - The scale is based on the original STAI, modified for children, and has been used to measure anxiety in numerous previous intervention studies. The inventory measures state (current) and trait (persistent) anxiety. Alpha reliability of the STAI-C is high, ranging from 0.78 to 0.91 (Muris 2002). Test–retest reliability for STAI-C Trait is higher (0.65–0.71) than the test–retest reliability for STAI-C State (0.31–0.41), but this is valid, as the STAI-C State is intended to measure a transient state (Julian, 2011). Furthermore, STAI-C effectively distinguishes between those with and without anxiety disorders (Seligman et al., 2004) and correlates with other measures of anxiety (Spielberger & Edwards, , 1973). STAIC measure selected and both subscales are used: Trait anxiety State anxiety This scale expresses negative emotions and is interpreted as having negative directionality or lower scores imply less anxiety  No subscales were excluded
 Negative Mental Health  Ioannou, et al., (2020) Primary Outcome State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children - The STAIC is a self-report questionnaire aimed to measure both State (current) and Trait (enduring) anxiety (Spielberger et al., 1983). It has been utilised among both TD youth (e.g., Muris & Merckelbach, 1998) and youth with ASD (Lanni et al., 2012; Park et al., 2013; Simon & Corbett, 2013). Alpha reliability ranges from 0.78 to 0.91; test–retest reliability for the STAIC-Trait is 0.65–0.71 (Julian, 2011). Participants in the EXP and WLC groups were administered the STAIC after the PIP. STAIC measure selected and both subscales are used: Trait anxiety State anxiety This scale expresses negative emotions and is interpreted as having negative directionality or lower scores imply less anxiety  No subscales were excluded
 Negative Mental Health Rezaei et al. (2018) Primary Outcome Aberrant Behavior Checklist (ABC): The checklist includes 58 questions designed to assess the presence and severity of maladaptive behaviors in people with developmental disabilities. This tool evaluates five categories of behavioural disorders, three of which are the main defects in autism: lethargy, stereotypical behaviour, and inappropriate speech, with the other two being related to hyperactivity/restlessness and irritability. ABC measure selected but only the following subscales were used: hyperactivity/restlessness Irritability The scale expresses negative emotions and is interpreted as having negative directionality or lower score imply less hyperactivity/restlessness and irritability, ABC subscales excluded: Lethargy Stereotypical behaviour Inappropriate speech
Umar to decide the categorisation of the attachment outcome variable (According to the attachment scales that the authors used) Siller et al., (2014) Primary Outcome The Proximity and Contact Seeking Behaviors (PCSB) ScaleThe PCSB evaluates the intensity of a child's effort to regain contact with, or proximity to, their mother. Higher PCSB scores indicate that the child took initiative in achieving contact, where lower scores indicate the child made no effort to make contact with their mother (p.1725)”. The Avoidant Behaviors (AB) ScaleThe AB evaluates the intensity and duration of the child's avoidance toward their mother. Lowest AB scores indicate that the child did not greet his/her mother upon reunion despite the mother's attempts at interaction, where higher scores indicate that the child did not display avoidant behaviors toward his/her mother. Please note that both measures were scored so that higher numbers represent stronger signs of attachment (p.1725)”. Observed Attachment Behaviours (mean of PCSB and AB) The authors created a global measure of observed attachment behaviours, which was computed as the average of children's PCSB and AB scores. Higher means represent stronger signs of attachment. Maternal Perceptions of Child Attachment questionnaire (MPCA;Hoppes and Harris 1990).This parent-report measure consists of 23 items rated on a 5-point rating scale, ranging from frequently (1) to never (5). High scores indicate maternal perceptions of strong child attachment (p.1725)”. PCSB measure did not include sub-scales so the total score is used in the meta-analysis. PCSB total score is interpreted as higher numbers represent stronger signs of attachment. AB measure did not include sub-scales so the total score is used in the meta-analysis. AB total score is interpreted as higher numbers represent stronger signs of attachment. Observed Attachment Behaviour measure did not include any subscales as it is created as the sum of PCSB and AB measures. Therefore, the total score in this scale is interpreted as higher means represent stronger signs of attachment. MPCA measure did not include sub-scales so the total score is used in the meta-analysis. MPCA total score is interpreted as higher numbers represent stronger signs of attachment. No subscales were excluded
 Negative Mental Health Schottelkorb et al. (2020) Primary Outcome Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) - The CBCLs for children ages 1 to 5 and 6 to 18 are instruments of the Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment. Both measures are used for examining emotional and behavioral problems as well as adaptive functioning of children as rated by parents/guardians. In our study, parents rated the items on the CBCL on a 3-point Likert-type scale ranging from 0 (not true) to 2 (very true or often true). The instruments are designed to evaluate child behaviors across three domains—externalizing, internalizing, and total behavior problems—using various subscales. Due to the varied ages of participants in this study, the two different CBCL versions were used. Because the two versions offer differing subscales, we focused on comparing pre-post data from those subscales that were consistent across the two types: Attention Problems, Aggressive Behavior, and Externalizing Problems. Scoring procedures for the CBCL involve calculating T scores and percentiles for each subscale. T scores between 60 and 63 are considered borderline, suggesting an area of concern, and T scores higher than 63 are considered clinical. Both versions of the CBCL (1–5 and 6–18) are reliable (test-retest coefficients between .68 and .92) and valid for identifying individuals with internalizing and externalising behaviours (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). CBCL measure selected but only the following subscale was used: externalising problems The scale expresses negative emotions and is interpreted as having negative directionality or lower score imply less externalising problems CBCL subscales excluded: Attention Problems Aggressive Behaviour
Negative Mental Health Duifhuis et al. (2017) Secondary Outcome Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) - behavioral problems were measured using parents’ ratings on the Dutch translation of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL). This is a widely used standardised questionnaire for those aged either 1 − 5 or 6–18 years (Achenbach & Rescorla 2000). The CBCL is a parent rating scale which measures children's general problem behavior and internalizing and externalizing behavior, while more specific problem behaviors are assessed with supplementary scales. In the analyses of this study, three major scales in the CBCL were focused on: The total scale, externalizing behavior, and internalizing behavior. Cut-off scores for clinically elevated symptoms are based on T-scores ≥ 68 (Achenbach ∧ Rescorla 2000). Internal consistencies (Cronbach's Alpha) for the Dutch version were found to be >0.90 (Verhulst et al., 1996). CBCL measure selected and the following subscales were used: Externalising behaviour Internalising behaviour High scores are indicators of negative mental health so the scale is interpreted as having positive directionality CBCL subscales excluded: Total scale
1.

see DSM-V for further details about diagnosable mental health conditions.

2.

Original source of Cochrane risk of bias tools sourced at https://www.riskofbias.info/.

3.

Inaccessible papers were either because it was no longer available online or being behind a paywall, etc.

Footnotes

The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding: Gill A Francis is a Postdoctoral Fellow at the Department of Education, University of York. ORCID https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0795-2544

Emre Deniz is a PhD Candidate at the Department of Education, University of York funded by the Republic of Turkey Ministry of National Education. ORCID https://orcid.org/0000-0001-7083-5562

Carole Torgerson is a Professor of Education Evaluation at the Department of Education, University of York. ORCID https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4771-6263

Umar Toseeb is a Senior Lecturer at the Department of Education, University of York. ORCID https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7536-2722

Author note

The Authors declare that there is no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Achenbach T. M., Rescorla L. A. (2000). Manual for the ASEBA preschool forms & profiles. University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families. [Google Scholar]
  2. Achenbach T. M., Rescorla L. A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles. University of Vermont Center for Children, Youth, and Families. [Google Scholar]
  3. Al-Yateem N., Rossiter R. C. (2017). Unstructured play for anxiety in pediatric inpatient care. Journal for Specialists in Pediatric Nursing, 22(1), e12166. 10.1111/jspn.12166 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Ainsworth M. D. S. (1978). The Bowlby-Ainsworth attachment theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(3), 436–438. 10.1017/S0140525X00075828 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Akhondzadeh S., Fallah J., Mohammadi M.-R., Imani R., Mohammadi M., Salehi B., Ghanizadeh A., Raznahan M., Mohebbi-Rasa S., Rezazadeh S.-A., Forghani S. (2010). Double-blind placebo-controlled trial of pentoxifylline added to risperidone: Effects on aberrant behavior in children with autism. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 34(1), 32–36. 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.09.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Aman M. G., Singh N. N., Stewart A. W., Field C. J. (1985). The aberrant behavior checklist: A behavior rating scale for the assessment of treatment effects. American Journal of Mental deficiency 89(5), 485–491. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th edition). American Psychiatric Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  8. Andres-Roqueta C., Adrian J. E., Clemente R. A., Villanueva L. (2016). Social cognition makes an independent contribution to peer relations in children with specific language impairment. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 49-50, 277–290. 10.1016/j.ridd.2015.12.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Axline V. M. (1947). Nondirective therapy for poor readers. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 11(2), 61–69 10.1037/h0063079 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Axline V.M. (1969). Play therapy. New York: Ballantine. [Google Scholar]
  11. Baird G., Simonoff E., Pickles A., Chandler S., Loucas T., Meldrum D., Charman T. (2006). Prevalence of disorders of the autism spectrum in a population cohort of children in South Thames: the Special Needs and Autism Project (SNAP). Lancet, 368(9531), 210–215. 10.1016/s0140-6736(06)69041-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Ben-Sasson A., Carter A. S., Briggs-Gowan M. J. (2009). Sensory over-responsivity in elementary school: Prevalence and social-emotional correlates. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37(5), 705–716. 10.1007/s10802-008-9295-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bignardi G., Dalmaijer E. S., Astle D. (2021). Testing the specificity of environmental risk factors for developmental outcomes. Child Development, 1–17. 10.1111/cdev.13719 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bishop D. V. M., Snowling M. J., Thompson P. A., Greenhalgh T., Adams C., Archibald L., Baird G., Bauer A., Bellair J., Boyle C., Brownlie E., Carter G., Clark B., Clegg J., Cohen N., Conti-Ramsden G., Dockrell J., Dunn J., Ebbels S., et al. (2017). Phase 2 of CATALISE: A multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development: Terminology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(10), 1068–1080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Blewitt C., O’Connor A., May T., Morris H., Mousa A., Bergmeier H., Jackson K., Barrett H., Skouteris H. (2019). Strengthening the social and emotional skills of pre-schoolers with mental health and developmental challenges in inclusive early childhood education and care settings: A narrative review of educator-led interventions. Early Child Development and Care, 0(0), 1–22. 10.1080/03004430.2019.1704283 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. Borenstein M. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. Wiley-Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
  17. Borenstein M., Higgins J. P. T., Hedges L. V., Rothstein H. R. (2017). Basics of meta-analysis: I2 is not an absolute measure of heterogeneity. Research Synthesis Methods, 8(1), 5–18. 10.1002/jrsm.1230 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Brown S. L. (2014). Consequences of play deprivation. Scholarpedia, 9(5), 30449 10.4249/scholarpedia.30449 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cantwell D. P., Baker L. (1987). Prevalence and type of psychiatric disorder and developmental disorders in three speech and language groups. Journal of Communication Disorders, 20(2), 151–160. 10.1016/0021-9924(87)90006-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Carroll J. J., Steward M. S. (1984). The role of cognitive development in children’s understandings of their own feelings. Child Development, 55(4), 1486–1492. 10.2307/1130018 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  21. Chudacoff H. P. (2007). Children at play: An American history. NYU Press. [Google Scholar]
  22. Conti-Ramsden G., Mok P., Durkin K., Pickles A., Toseeb U., Botting N. (2019). Do emotional difficulties and peer problems occur together from childhood to adolescence? The case of children with a history of developmental language disorder (DLD). European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(7), 993–1004. 10.1007/s00787-018-1261-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Constantino J. N., Gruber C. P. (2012). Social responsiveness scale, second edition (SRS-2). Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services. [Google Scholar]
  24. Corbett B. A., Gunther J. R., Comins D., Price J., Ryan N., Simon D., Schupp C. W., Rios T. (2011). Brief report: Theatre as therapy for children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(4), 505–511. 10.1007/s10803-010-1064-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Corbett B. A., Swain D. M., Coke C., Simon D., Newsom C., Houchins-Juarez N., Jenson A., Wang L., Song Y. (2014). Improvement in social deficits in autism spectrum disorders using a theatre-based, peer-mediated intervention. Autism Research: Official Journal of the International Society for Autism Research, 7(1), 4–16. 10.1002/aur.1341 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Corbett B. A., Blain S. D., Ioannou S., Balser M. (2017). Changes in anxiety following a randomized control trial of a theatre-based intervention for youth with autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 21(3), 333–343 10.1177/1362361316643623. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Crick N. R., Dodge K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children's Social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115(1), 74–101. 10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.74 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. Doernberg E. A., Russ S. W., Dimitropoulos A. (2021). Believing in make-believe: Efficacy of a pretend play intervention for school-aged children with high-functioning autism Spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 51(2), 576–588 10.1007/s10803-020-04547-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Duifhuis E. A., Den Boer J. C., Doornbos A., Buitelaar J. K., Oosterling I. J., Klip H. (2017). The effect of pivotal response treatment in children with autism spectrum disorders: A non-randomized study with a blinded outcome measure. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(2), 231–242 10.1007/s10803-016-2916-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Durkin K., Toseeb U., Botting N., Pickles A., Conti-Ramsden G. (2017). Social confidence in early adulthood among young people with and without a history of language impairment. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 60(6), 1635–1647. 10.1044/2017_JSLHR-L-16-0256 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Duval S., Tweedie R. (2000). Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics, 56(2), 455–463. 10.1111/j.0006-341x.2000.00455.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. England Marketing. (2009). Report to natural England on childhood and nature: A survey on changing relationships with nature across generations. Retrieved from Natural England Avaliable online at: website: http://goo.gl/ienawl
  33. Erikson E. H. (1993). Childhood and society. WW Norton & Company. [Google Scholar]
  34. Fehr K. K., Russ S. W. (2014). Assessment of pretend play in preschool-aged children: Validation and factor analysis of the affect in play scale–preschool version. Journal of Personality Assessment, 96(3), 350–357. 10.1080/00223891.2013.838171 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Freud S. (1909). Analysis of a phobia in a five-year-old boy. Standard Edition, 10, 5–147. [Google Scholar]
  36. Freud S. (1955). Beyond the pleasure principle. In S., Strachey, S., Freud, J., Strachey, A., & Tyson, A. (Ed). The standard edition of the complete psychological works of sigmund freud, volume XVIII (1920-1922): Beyond the pleasure principle, group psychology and other works (pp. 1–64). The Hogarth Press and the Institute of Psychoanalysis. [Google Scholar]
  37. Genç M., Çakmak Tolan Ö. (2021). Play therapy practices in psychological and developmental disorders that Are common in preschool period. Psikiyatride Guncel Yaklasimlar, 13(2), 207–231. 10.18863/pgy.757366 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  38. Gibson J. L., Cornell M., Gill T. (2017). A systematic review of research into the impact of loose parts play on children’s cognitive, social and emotional development. School Mental Health, 9(4), 295–309. 10.1007/s12310-017-9220-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Gibson J. L., Pritchard E., de Lemos C. (2021). Play-based interventions to support social and communication development in autistic children aged 2–8 years: A scoping review. Autism & Developmental Language Impairments, 6, 23969415211015840. 10.1177/23969415211015840 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Goldstein R. (1994). Toys, play, and child development. Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  41. Greenberg M. T., Kusche C. A., Cook E. T., Quamma J. P. (1995). Promoting emotional competence in school-aged children: The effects of the PATHS curriculum. Development and Psychopathology, 7(1), 117–136. 10.1017/S0954579400006374 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  42. Greenspan S. I., DeGangi G., Wieder S. (2001). The functional emotional assessment scale (FEAS): For infancy & early childhood. Interdisciplinary Council on Development & Learning Disorders. [Google Scholar]
  43. Greenspan J., Greenspan S. I. (2002). Functional emotional developmental questionnaire for childhood: A preliminary report on the questions and their clinical meaning. Journal of Developmental and Learning Disorders, 6, 71–116. [Google Scholar]
  44. Greenspan S. I., Wieder S. (1997). An integrated developmental approach to interventions for young children with severe difficulties in relating and communicating. Zero to Three, 17, 5–18. [Google Scholar]
  45. Han M., Moore N., Vukelich C., Buell M. (2010). Does play make a difference? How play intervention affects the vocabulary learning of at-risk preschoolers. American Journal of Play, 3(1), 82–105. [Google Scholar]
  46. Hardin A. P., Hackell J. M., & Medicine, C. on P. and A. (2017). Age limit of pediatrics. Pediatrics, 140(3), 1–3. 10.1542/peds.2017-2151 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Harn P. L., Bo S. H. (2019). The effectiveness of playful positive psychology interventions with Six bricks and DUPLO® play Box for Taiwan children on emotional adaptation. Journal of Research and Review, 9(5), 5–8. 10.31871/WJRR.9.5.4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hedges L. V. (1982). Estimation of effect size from a series of independent experiments. Psychological Bulletin, 92(2), 490–499. 10.1037/0033-2909.92.2.490 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  49. Hess E. (2012). DIR floortime: A developmental/relational play erapy approach for treating children impacted by autism. In Play-based interventions for children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  50. Higgins JPT, Thomas J, Chandler J, Cumpston M, Li T, Page MJ, Welch VA. (2021). Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions version 6.2 Available from www.training.cochrane.org/handbook.
  51. Higgins J. P. T., Thompson S. G. (2002). Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Statistics in Medicine, 21(11), 1539–1558. 10.1002/sim.1186 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Higgins J. P. T., Thompson S. G., Deeks J. J., Altman D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ (Clinical Research Ed.), 327(7414), 557–560. 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Hillman H. (2018). Child-centered play therapy as an intervention for children with autism: A literature review. International Journal of Play Therapy, 27(4), 198–204. 10.1037/pla0000083 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  54. Hoff E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26(1), 55–88. 10.1016/j.dr.2005.11.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  55. Hoppes K., Harris S. L. (1990). Perceptions of child attachment and maternal gratification in mothers of children with autism and Down syndrome. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 19(4), 365–370 10.1207/s15374424jccp1904_8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  56. Hudson C. C., Hall L., Harkness K. L. (2019). Prevalence of depressive disorders in individuals with Autism Spectrum disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 47(1), 165–175. 10.1007/s10802-018-0402-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Hughes F. P. (2003). Spontaneous play in the 21st century. In Spodek, B., & Saracho, O. (Eds.), Contemporary perspectives on play in early childhood education, (pp. 21-39). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  58. Huizinga J. (1949). Homo ludens: A study of the play-element in our culture. Routledge & Kegan Paul. [Google Scholar]
  59. Ingersoll B., Walton K. (2013). Play intervention. In Volkmar F. R. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Autism Spectrum disorders (pp. 2287–2291). Springer New York. 10.1007/978-1-4419-1698-3_1898 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  60. Ioannou S., Key A. P., Muscatello R. A., Klemencic M., Corbett B. A. (2020). Peer actors and theater techniques play pivotal roles in improving social play and anxiety for children With Autism. Frontiers in Psychology, 11(908), 1–9. https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00908 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Julian L. J. (2011). Measures of anxiety: State-trait anxiety inventory (STAI), Beck anxiety inventory (BAI), and Hospital anxiety and depression scale-anxiety (HADS-A). Arthritis Care & Research, 63(11), S467–472. 10.1002/acr.20561 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Klein M. (1929). Personification in the play of children. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 10, 193–204. https://www.proquest.com/scholarly-journals/personification-play-children/docview/1298177097/se-2?accountid=15181 [Google Scholar]
  63. Koegel L. K. (2011). Pivotal response treatment: Using motivation as a pivotal response. Santa Barbara: University of California. [Google Scholar]
  64. Koegel R. L., O'dell M. C., Koegel L. K. (1987). A natural language teaching paradigm for nonverbal autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17(2), 187–200. 10.1007/BF01495055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Kusche C. A., Beilke R. L., Greenberg M. T. (1988). Kusche affective interview—revised. Unpublished measure, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington. [Google Scholar]
  66. Lanni K. E., Schupp C. W., Simon D., Corbett B. A. (2012). Verbal ability, social stress, and anxiety in children with autistic disorder. Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice, 16(2), 123–138. 10.1177/1362361311425916 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Lord C., Risi S., Lambrecht L., Cook E. H., Leventhal B. L., DiLavore P. C., Pickles A., Rutter M. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule—generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223. 10.1023/A:1005592401947 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Lord C., Rutter M., DiLavore P. C., Risi S. (1999). Autism diagnostic observation schedule-WPS (ADOS-WPS). Western Psychological Services, Los Angeles. [Google Scholar]
  69. Lord C., Rutter M., Le Couteur A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659–685. 10.1007/BF02172145 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Lowenfeld M. (1939). The world pictures of children 1: A method of recording and studying them. British journal of Medical Psychology, 18(1), 65–101. 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1939.tb00710.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  71. Mazefsky C. A., Herrington J., Siegel M., Scarpa A., Maddox B. B., Scahill L., White S. W. (2013). The role of emotion regulation in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of American Academy of Child Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(7), 679–688. 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.05.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Mendeley. (2021). Mendeley Reference Manager for Desktop. https://www.mendeley.com. Accessed 09.11. 2021.
  73. Mercer J. (2017). Examining DIR/floortime™ as a treatment for children With Autism Spectrum disorders: A review of research and theory. Research on Social Work Practice, 27(5), 625–635. 10.1177/1049731515583062 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  74. Mundy P., Sigman M., Ungerer J., Sherman T. (1987). Nonverbal communication and play correlates of language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17(3), 349–364. 10.1007/BF01487065 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Muris P. (2002). Relationships between self-efficacy and symptoms of anxiety disorders and depression in a normal adolescent sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 32(2), 337–348. 10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00027-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  76. Muris P., Merckelbach H. (1998). Perceived parental rearing behaviour and anxiety disorders symptoms in normal children. Personality and Individual Differences, 25(6), 1199–1206. 10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00153-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  77. Newbury D. F., Gibson J. L., Conti-Ramsden G., Pickles A., Durkin K., Toseeb U. (2019). Using polygenic profiles to predict variation in language and psychosocial outcomes in early and middle childhood. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 62(9), 3381–3396. 10.1044/2019_jslhr-l-19-0001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Newton E., Jenvey V. (2011). Play and theory of mind: Associations with social competence in young children. Early Child Development and Care, 181(6), 761–773. 10.1080/03004430.2010.486898 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  79. Norbury C. F., Gooch D., Wray C., Baird G., Charman T., Simonoff E., Vamvakas G., Pickles A. (2016). The impact of nonverbal ability on prevalence and clinical presentation of language disorder: Evidence from a population study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(11), 1247–1257. 10.1111/jcpp.12573 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. O’Connor C., Stagnitti K. (2011). Play, behaviour, language and social skills: The comparison of a play and a non-play intervention within a specialist school setting. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32(3), 1205–1211. 10.1016/j.ridd.2010.12.037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Page M. J., McKenzie J. E., Bossuyt P. M., Boutron I., Hoffmann T. C., Mulrow C. D., Shamseer L., Tetzlaff J. M., Akl E. A., Brennan S., Chou R., Glanville J., Grimshaw J. M., Hróbjartsson A., Lalu M. M., Li T., Loder E. W., Mayo-Wilson E., McDonald S., … McKenzie J. E. (2021). PRISMA 2020 explanation and elaboration: Updated guidance and exemplars for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ, 372, 1–32 10.1136/bmj.n160 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Pajareya K., Nopmaneejumruslers K. (2011). A pilot randomized controlled trial of DIR/floortime™ parent training intervention for pre-school children with autistic spectrum disorders. Autism, 15(5), 563–577. 10.1177/1362361310386502 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Patalay P., Fitzsimons E. (2016). Correlates of mental illness and wellbeing in children: Are they the same? Results From the UK millennium cohort study. Journal of the American Academy of Child Adolescent Psychiatry, 55(9), 771–783. 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.05.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Park S., Park M.-H., Kim H. J., Yoo H. J. (2013). Anxiety and depression symptoms in children with asperger syndrome compared with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and depressive disorder. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 22(4), 559–568. 10.1007/s10826-012-9611-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  85. Patalay P., Fitzsimons E. (2018). Mental ill-health and wellbeing at age 14 – Initial findings from the Millennium Cohort Study Age 14 Survey. https://cls.ucl.ac.uk/wp-content/uploads/2018/05/MenatlHealthWellbeing-initial-findings.pdf.
  86. Piaget J. (1952). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. W W Norton & Co. [Google Scholar]
  87. Pilarz K. M. (2009). Evaluation of The Efficacy of a Seven Week Public School Curriculum Based DIR/Floortime Parent Training Program for Parents of Children on The Autism Spectrum [Doctoral Thesis, Temple University]. https://scholarshare.temple.edu/handle/20.500.12613/3721.
  88. Potasz C., de Varela M. J., de Carvalho L. C., do Prado L. F., doPrado G. F. (2013). Effect of play activities on hospitalized children's stress: A randomized clinical trial. Scandinavian Journal of OccupationalTherapy, 20(1), 71–79. 10.3109/11038128.2012.729087 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Ray D. C., Sullivan J. M., Carlson S. E. (2012). Relational intervention: Child-centered play therapy with children on the autism spectrum. In Play-based interventions for children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders (pp. 159–175). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group. [Google Scholar]
  90. Rezaei M., Moradi A., Tehrani-Doost M., Hassanabadi H., Khosroabadi R. (2018). Effects of combining medication and pivotal response treatment on aberrant behavior in children with autism spectrum disorder. Children, 5(2), 19. 10.3390/children5020019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Rinaldi S., Caselli M. C., Cofelice V., D’Amico S., De Cagno A. G., Della Corte G., Di Martino M. V., Di Costanzo B., Levorato M. C., Penge R., Rossetto T., Sansavini A., Vecchi S., Zoccolotti P. (2021). Efficacy of the treatment of developmental language disorder: A systematic review. Brain Sciences, 11(3), 1–36. 10.3390/brainsci11030407 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Rubin K. H. (1982). Nonsocial play in preschoolers: Necessarily evil? Child Development, 53(3), 651–657 10.2307/1129376. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  93. Rubin K. H., Fein G., Vandenberg B. (1983). Play. In Hetherington E. M. (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol 4. Socialization, personality, and social development. Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  94. Saracho O. N., Spodek B. (1998). A historical overview of theories of play. Multiple perspectives on play in early childhood education, SUNY Press. [Google Scholar]
  95. Schottelkorb A. A., Swan K. L., Ogawa Y. (2020). Intensive child-centered play therapy for children on the Autism Spectrum: A pilot study. Journal of Counseling & Development, 98(1), 63–73. 10.1002/jcad.12300 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  96. Seligman, Parks M., Steen A., T. (2004). A balanced psychology and a full life. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 359, 1379–1381. 10.1098/rstb.2004.1513 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Schopler E., Reichler R. J., Renner B. R. (1986). The childhood autism rating scale (CARS). Western Psychological Services. [Google Scholar]
  98. Seibert J. M., Hogan A. E., Mundy P. C. (1982). Assessing interactional competencies: The early social-communication scales. Infant Mental Health Journal, 3(4), 244–258. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  99. Seligman, Parks M., Steen A., T. (2004). A balanced psychology and a full life. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 359, 1379–1381. 10.1098/rstb.2004.1513 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Shadish W. R., Cook T. D., Campbell T. D. (2002). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference. Houghton Mifflin. [Google Scholar]
  101. Shi L., Lin L. (2019). The trim-and-fill method for publication bias: Practical guidelines and recommendations based on a large database of meta-analyses. Medicine, 98(23), 1–11. 10.1097/MD.0000000000015987 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Siller M., Hutman T., Sigman M. (2013). A parent-mediated intervention to increase responsive parental behaviors and child communication in children with ASD: A randomized clinical trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(3), 540–555. 10.1007/s10803-012-1584-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Siller M., Swanson M., Gerber A., Hutman T., Sigman M. (2014). A parent-mediated intervention that targets responsive parental behaviors increases attachment behaviors in children with ASD: Results from a randomized clinical trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(7), 1720–1732. 10.1007/s10803-014-2049-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Simon D. M., Corbett B. A. (2013). Examining associations between anxiety and cortisol in high functioning male children with autism. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 5(1), 32. 10.1186/1866-1955-5-32 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Solomon R., Necheles J., Ferch C., Bruckman D. (2007). Pilot study of a parent training program for young children with autism: The PLAY Project Home Consultation program. Autism, 11(3), 205–224. 10.1177/1362361307076842 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Solomon R., Van Egeren L. A., Mahoney G., Huber M. S. Q., Zimmerman P. (2014). PLAY Project home consultation intervention program for young children with autism spectrum disorders: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 35(8), 475–485. 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000096 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Spielberger C. D., Edwards C. D. (1973). STAIC preliminary manual for the State-Trait anxiety inventory for children (‘How I Feel Questionnaire’). Consulting Psychologists Press. [Google Scholar]
  108. Spielberger C., Gorsuch R., Lushene R., Vagg P., Jacobs G. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait anxiety inventory (Form Y1 – Y2). In Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press; Vol. IV. [Google Scholar]
  109. StataCorp (2021). Stata Statistical Software: Release 17.
  110. Sterne J. A., Hernán M. A., Reeves B. C., Savović J., Berkman N. D., Viswanathan M., Henry D., Altman D. G., Ansari M. T., Boutron I., Carpenter J. R., Chan A.-W., Churchill R., Deeks J. J., Hróbjartsson A., Kirkham J., Jüni P., Loke Y. K., Pigott T. D.… Higgins J. P. (2016). ROBINS-I: A tool for assessing risk of bias in non-randomised studies of interventions. BMJ, 355, i4919. 10.1136/bmj.i4919 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Sterne J. A. C., Savović J., Page M. J., Elbers R. G., Blencowe N. S., Boutron I., Cates C. J., Cheng H.-Y., Corbett M. S., Eldridge S. M., Emberson J. R., Hernán M. A., Hopewell S., Hróbjartsson A., Junqueira D. R., Jüni P., Kirkham J. J., Lasserson T., Li T., … Higgins J. P. T. (2019). RoB 2: A revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ (Clinical Research Ed .), 366, 1–8. 10.1136/bmj.l4898 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Sturgess J. (2009). Play as child-chosen activity. Play as therapy: Assessment and therapeutic interventions. Jessica Kingsley Publishers. [Google Scholar]
  113. Thomas J., Kneale D., McKenzie J. E., Brennan S. E., Bhaumik S. (2021). Chapter 2: Determining the scope of the review and the questions it will address. In Higgins J. P. T., Thomas J., Chandler J., Cumpston M., Li T., Page M. J., Welch V. A. (Eds.), Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions version 6.2 (updated February 2021). Cochrane. 2021. Available from www.training.cochrane.org/handbook. [Google Scholar]
  114. Thomas N., Smith C. (2004). Developing play skills in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Educational Psychology in Practice, 20(3), 195–206. 10.1080/0266736042000251781 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  115. Tomasello M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  116. Tomblin J. B., Records N. L., Buckwalter P., Zhang X. Y., Smith E., O'Brien M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 40(6), 1245–1260. 10.1044/jslhr.4006.1245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Toseeb U., Gibson J. L., Newbury D. F., Orlik W., Durkin K., Pickles A., Conti-Ramsden G. (2020a). Play and prosociality are associated with fewer externalizing problems in children with developmental language disorder: The role of early language and communication environment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 55(4), 583–602. 10.1111/1460-6984.12541 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Toseeb U., McChesney G., Oldfield J., Wolke D. (2020b). Sibling bullying in middle childhood is associated with psychosocial difficulties in early adolescence: The case of individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 50(5), 1457–1469. 10.1007/s10803-019-04116-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Toseeb U., McChesney G., Wolke D. (2018). The prevalence and psychopathological correlates of sibling bullying in children with and without autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(7), 2308–2318. 10.1007/s10803-018-3484-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Toseeb U., Oginni O., Dale P. S. (2021). Developmental language disorder and psychopathology: Disentangling shared genetic and environmental influences. Journal of Learning Disabilities., 1–15. 10.1177/00222194211019961 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. van den Bedem N. P., Dockrell J. E., van Alphen P. M., Kalicharan S. V., Rieffe C. (2018). Victimization, bullying, and emotional competence: Longitudinal associations in (pre)adolescents with and without developmental language disorder. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 61(8), 2028–2044. 10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-17-0429 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. van Steensel F. J. A., Bogels S. M., Perrin S. (2011). Anxiety disorders in children and adolescents with autistic spectrum disorders: A meta-analysis. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 14(3), 302–317. 10.1007/s10567-011-0097-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Verhulst, F. C., Van der Ende, J., Koot, H.M. (1996). Handleiding voor de CBCL/4-18 [Manual for the CBCL/4-18]. Rotterdam: Afdeling Kinder- en Jeugdpsychiatrie, Sophia Kinderziekenhuis/Academisch Ziekenhuis Rotterdam/Erasmus Universiteit Rotterdam.
  124. Verschuur R., Didden R., Lang R., Sigafoos J., Huskens B. (2014). Pivotal response treatment for children with Autism Spectrum disorders: A systematic review. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 1(1), 34–61. 10.1007/s40489-013-0008-z [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  125. Vygotsky L. (1933). Play and its role in the mental development of the child. Retrieved from https://www.marxists.org/archive/vygotsky/works/1933/play.htm.
  126. Wells H. G. (1912). Floor games. Small, Maynard and Company. [Google Scholar]
  127. Westerhof G. J., Keyes C. L. M. (2010). Mental illness and mental health: The Two Continua model across the lifespan. Journal of Adult Development, 17(2), 110–119. 10.1007/s10804-009-9082-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. White P., Redford P., Macdonald J. (2020). Cohen’s d for two independent samples. Retrieved from https://uwe-repository.worktribe.com: https://uwe-repository.worktribe.com/output/3294892/cohens-d-for-two-independent-samples.
  129. Whitebread D. (2017). Free play and children's mental health. The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health, 1(3), 167–169. 10.1016/S2352-4642(17)30092-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. World Health Organisation (1948). Constitution of the World Health Organisation. https://apps.who.int/gb/bd/PDF/bd47/EN/constitution-en.pdf?ua=1.
  131. Yew S. G. K., O’Kearney R. (2013). Emotional and behavioural outcomes later in childhood and adolescence for children with specific language impairments: Meta-analyses of controlled prospective studies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(5), 516–524. 10.1111/jcpp.12009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Autism & Developmental Language Impairments are provided here courtesy of SAGE Publications

RESOURCES