Abstract
Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is a severe mental disorder, comprised of heterogeneous psychological and neurobiological pathologies. Here, we propose a predictive processing (PP) account of BPD to integrate these seemingly unrelated pathologies. In particular, we argue that the experience of childhood maltreatment, which is highly prevalent in BPD, leaves a developmental legacy with two facets: first, a coarse-grained, alexithymic model of self and others – leading to a rigidity and inflexibility concerning beliefs about self and others. Second, this developmental legacy leads to a loss of confidence or precision afforded beliefs about the consequences of social behavior. This results in an over reliance on sensory evidence and social feedback, with concomitant lability, impulsivity and hypersensitivity. In terms of PP, people with BPD show a distorted belief updating in response to new information with two opposing manifestations: rapid changes in beliefs and a lack of belief updating despite disconfirmatory evidence. This account of distorted information processing has the potential to explain both the instability (of affect, self-image, and interpersonal relationships) and the rigidity (of beliefs about self and others) which is typical of BPD. At the neurobiological level, we propose that enhanced levels of dopamine are associated with the increased integration of negative social feedback, and we also discuss the hypothesis of an impaired inhibitory control of the prefrontal cortex in the processing of negative social information. Our account may provide a new understanding not only of the clinical aspects of BPD, but also a unifying theory of the corresponding neurobiological pathologies. We conclude by outlining some directions for future research on the behavioral, neurobiological, and computational underpinnings of this model, and point to some clinical implications of it.
Key words: Active inference, Bayesian brain, belief updating, borderline personality disorder, childhood maltreatment, expectation, predictive processing
Introduction
Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is a severe and heterogeneous mental disorder, characterized by a pervasive pattern of instability of interpersonal relationships, self-image, affects, and a marked impulsivity (Chapman, 2019). Despite recent advances in treating BPD (Storebø et al., 2020), an analysis of the effectiveness in routine clinical care for BPD pointed to the need of improving the reduction of BPD-specific symptoms (Herzog et al., 2020). To achieve this goal, a precise understanding of the psychobiological processes underlying BPD is required. Although a genetic vulnerability in BPD is unquestionable, genetics alone may have little influence on the development of BPD in a favorable environment as a large part of the explained variance is attributable to individually unique environmental factors (Skoglund et al., 2021). Hence, the genetic vulnerability may translate into disordered perceptions and cognitions in the presence of adverse environmental conditions (e.g. childhood maltreatment, CM), that, in turn, likely increase the risk for developing a BPD (Fontaine & Viding, 2008). To improve the understanding of these psychobiological processes in BPD, the present article seeks to apply current theories from neuroscience to provide a novel mechanistic model of BPD. In particular, we use a predictive processing (PP) framework to explain how the experience of CM, which is common in many people with BPD (Kleindienst, Vonderlin, Bohus, & Lis, 2020; Porter et al., 2020), alters perception and cognition, resulting in distinct psychobiological dysfunctions.
Predictive processing – an impactful theory of how the brain works
Considering the brain as a ‘phantastic’, hypothesis-testing organ (Friston, Stephan, Montague, & Dolan, 2014), PP has become a prominent theory of fundamental working principles of the human brain (Clark, 2013, 2016a; Friston, 2005; Hohwy, 2014). Specifically, it has been theorized that the brain relies on principles of Bayesian inference to minimize uncertainty by continuously testing hypotheses regarding the causes of sensory input. In doing so, a prior belief is combined with new information to compute a posterior belief. If new information critically deviates from the prior prediction, a prediction error (PE) is generated. The (healthy) brain seeks to refine its hypotheses and maximize evidence for its internal model of the world by minimizing PEs.
At the neurobiological level, PP has been conceived of in terms of cerebral hierarchies (Friston, 2008). Specifically, in neural representations of higher levels of cortical hierarchies, predictions are generated (encoded by synaptic activity), which then descend to lower levels. In superficial pyramidal cells, such descending top-down predictions are compared with neural representations at lower levels of the cortical hierarchy to compute a PE (Bastos et al., 2012). The PE (i.e. a mismatch signal) is sent back up the hierarchy where it updates prior beliefs or expectations (that generate top-down predictions), associated with the activity of deep pyramidal cells (Kanai, Komura, Shipp, & Friston, 2015). For a more detailed discussion of the neural architecture of PP, see Barrett and Simmons (2015), Parr and Friston (2018), and Shipp (2016).
PP models also account for the outcomes of action, with the outcomes of actions being predicted based on causal knowledge (Heil, Kwisthout, van Pelt, van Rooij, & Bekkering, 2018). The human brain chooses the actions that are expected to produce the most preferred outcome or provides the most salient information. Briefly, this process suggests that such action-perception cycles operate to minimize uncertainty and optimize an individual's internal model of the world – a process referred to as active inference. Since perception in active inference is treated as a constructive process of hypothesis testing, where the brain aims to select the hypothesis that best explains sensory data, the brain must decide how much weight is given to new information relative to prior beliefs. This is referred to as precision-weighting, with precision being defined as ‘the certainty with which a model is believed to be true and the certainty of a particular afferent given an expectation’ (Paulus, Feinstein, & Khalsa, 2019). That is, precision can refer to both the reliability of new information and the confidence afforded to priors. Their balance critically determines the extent to which a prior is updated given new information. Put simply, if the prior is afforded low precision (referred to as ‘weak priors’), new information has more influence on the formation of the posterior, while the opposite is true for ‘strong priors’. In other words, if we are unsure about how much we can trust our beliefs, we prefer to rely on new information, provided that new information appears sufficiently valid. On the other hand, if priors are afforded overly much precision, they dominate perception such that information is consistent with prior predictions is prioritized and discrepant information is largely neglected (Powers, Mathys, & Corlett, 2017).
Neurobiologically, the precision of sensory information is thought to be signaled by neuromodulators such as dopamine and encoded based on synaptic gain control mechanisms (Fiorillo, Newsome, & Schultz, 2008; Galea, Bestmann, Beigi, Jahanshahi, & Rothwell, 2012; Iglesias et al., 2013). Aberrations in precision-weighing have recently been related to a number of psychopathological dysfunctions and mental disorders, such as depression (Barrett, Quigley, & Hamilton, 2016; Clark, Watson, & Friston, 2018; Kube, Schwarting, Rozenkrantz, Glombiewski, & Rief, 2020), stressors and psychological trauma (Krupnik, 2020; Linson, Parr, & Friston, 2020), PTSD (Kube, Berg, Kleim, & Herzog, 2020a; Linson & Friston, 2019; Wilkinson, Dodgson, & Meares, 2017), hallucinations in psychosis (Corlett et al., 2019; Sterzer et al., 2018) and in PTSD (Lyndon & Corlett, 2020), autism (Lawson, Rees, & Friston, 2014; Pellicano & Burr, 2012), and somatization (Henningsen et al., 2018; Kube, Rozenkrantz, Rief, & Barsky, 2020b; van den Bergh, Witthöft, Petersen, & Brown, 2017).
The belief updating process is illustrated in Fig. 1.
In the present article, we apply this account to BPD, proposing that BPD is related to an imbalance of hierarchical priors – specifically, imprecise prior beliefs, relative to sensory evidence – based upon a learning history of CM. Before we will lay out the central tenets of this account, we first briefly review the literature on normal infant learning from a PP perspective to understand how adverse environmental conditions (that is, CM) perturbs the perceptual system, which can ultimately result in psychopathological dysfunctions as manifested in BPD.
Normal infant learning
Research has shown that PP may provide a unifying perspective on infant learning, including statistical learning principles, motor and proprioceptive learning, and developing a basic understanding of the self and their physical and social environment (Köster, Kayhan, Langeloh, & Hoehl, 2020). Using Violation-of-Expectation paradigms (Sokolov, 1963, 1990), research has shown that infants use novel and unexpected experiences (i.e. PEs) to refine their predictive models of the world, as indicated e.g. in studies using event-related potential technique (Köster, Langeloh, & Hoehl, 2019; Langeloh, Buttelmann, Pauen, & Hoehl, 2020). Indeed, recent research has emphasized that infants build and update their predictive models of a changing environment early in their lives (Kayhan, Meyer, O'Reilly, Hunnius, & Bekkering, 2019), suggesting that reducing uncertainty in a changing world is an important developmental goal.
Childhood maltreatment in BPD
Learning from PEs in infancy can be hindered or disrupted by a harmful environment. For instance, a longitudinal study revealed that the experience of attachment disorganization and parental hostility in early childhood was associated with dysfunctions in several domains (i.e. attention, emotion, behavior, relationship, and self-representation) in middle childhood/early adolescence and, ultimately, symptoms of BPD in adulthood (Carlson, Egeland, & Sroufe, 2009). Harmful learning experiences in childhood, such as neglect and abuse, have been referred to as CM. CM constitutes as a transdiagnostic risk factor for mental disorders (Gilbert et al., 2009; Struck et al., 2020) and plays an important role in the development of personality disorders in general (Afifi et al., 2011; Battle et al., 2004) and BPD in particular (Brakemeier et al., 2018; Pietrek, Elbert, Weierstall, Müller, & Rockstroh, 2013; Quenneville et al., 2020; Wota et al., 2014). The majority of patients with BPD experienced some sort of maltreatment in their childhood (Battle et al., 2004; Carlson et al., 2009): A recent study showed that patients with BPD were over 13 times more likely to report childhood adversity than non-clinical controls and other clinical populations (Kleindienst et al., 2020; Porter et al., 2020). Among the most frequent subtypes of CM in BPD are emotional abuse and neglect compared to controls (Scheffers, van Vugt, Lanctôt, & Lemieux, 2019). While studies showed that BPD was associated with higher scores on sexual abuse beside of emotional abuse and neglect (Bradley, Jenei, & Westen, 2005; Hernandez, Arntz, Gaviria, Labad, & Gutiérrez-Zotes, 2012; Igarashi et al., 2010; Lobbestael, Arntz, & Bernstein, 2010; Ogata et al., 1990; Weaver & Clum, 1993; Zhang et al., 2013), particular in women (de Aquino Ferreira, Queiroz Pereira, Neri Benevides, & Aguiar Melo, 2018), other studies indicated no independent relationship between sexual abuse and BPD (Cohen et al., 2014; Widom, Czaja, & Paris, 2009) or reported small effect sizes for sexual abuse (Hengartner, Ajdacic-Gross, Rodgers, Müller, & Rössler, 2013). In line, only emotional instability or vulnerability, impulsivity, and emotional abuse were found to be unique predictors of BPD (Bornovalova, Gratz, Delany-Brumsey, Paulson, & Lejuez, 2006). Moreover, social exclusion (ostracism) might be a psychosocial factor contributing to the development and persistence of BPD, in the sense of a vicious cycle where BPD increases the chance of being ostracized, and ostracism consolidates or even aggravates psychopathology (Reinhard et al., 2020). For example, bullying and violence in schools and emotional abuse appear to be more salient markers of general personality pathology than other forms of childhood adversity (Hengartner et al., 2013). Insights from a sibling design showed that both probands and sisters reported similar prevalence of intrafamilial abuse, although later BPD patients reported more severe intrafamilial physical and emotional abuse, and higher prevalence of physical abuse by peers (Laporte, Paris, Guttman, Russell, & Correa, 2012). Furthermore, another study that prospectively followed a sample who had experienced childhood abuse showed an increase in risk for BPD primarily in children who experienced physical abuse and neglect (Widom et al., 2009). In particular, terrorizing predicted anxiety and somatic concerns, ignoring predicted depression scores and BPD features, and degradation predicted BPD features only (Allen, 2008).
Psychological consequences of childhood maltreatment
Co-regulation and social communication in infancy are thought to underpin emotional dysregulation and social cognition deficits across development and these mechanisms are further potentiated by maladaptive social experiences in a series of positive feedback loops (Winsper, 2018; Winsper et al., 2016). Interestingly, studies suggest an association between CM, especially emotional abuse and neglect, and emotion regulation difficulties in a way that emotion regulation difficulties influence the association between emotional abuse and acute symptomatology in BPD (e.g. Carvalho Fernando et al., 2014), supporting the Emotional Dysregulation theory (Linehan, 1993). While CM impacts impulsivity and anger (Quenneville et al., 2020), one study showed that difficulties in emotion regulation statistically mediated the effect of CM on impulsivity in BPD (Krause-Utz et al., 2019), further supporting this theory.
Consistent with these findings, a study showed a moderate relationship between low emotional awareness (EA) (especially difficulties in identifying and describing emotions) and BPD (Derks, Westerhof, & Bohlmeijer, 2017). Of note, EA has recently been conceptualized within an active inference model: the authors showed that it can successfully acquire a repertoire of emotion concepts in its ‘childhood’, as well as acquire new emotion concepts in synthetic ‘adulthood’, and that these learning processes depend on early experiences, environmental stability, and habitual patterns of selective attention (Smith, Parr, & Friston, 2019b). Distinct neurocomputational processes underlying EA have further been developed, e.g. mechanisms that (either alone or in combination) can produce phenomena – such as somatic misattribution, coarse-grained emotion conceptualization, and constrained reflective capacity – characteristic of low EA (Smith, Lane, Parr, & Friston, 2019a). Relatedly, while one study found no specific association between parenting style with BPD (Hernandez et al., 2012), a meta-synthesis study found that maladaptive parenting is a well-established psychosocial risk factor for the development of BPD (Steele, Townsend, & Grenyer, 2019). A prominent study showed that family environment, parental psychopathology, and history of abuse all independently predicted BPD (Bradley et al., 2005).
Integrating the environmental factors from a lifespan perspective, literature suggests that vulnerability from mother to offspring may be partly transmitted via maladaptive parenting and maternal emotional dysfunction, i.e. mothers with BPD are more likely to engage in maladaptive interactions with their offspring characterized by insensitive, overprotective, and hostile parenting compared to mothers without BPD resulting in adverse offspring outcomes such as BPD symptoms, internalizing (e.g. depression) and externalizing problems, insecure attachment patterns, and emotional dysregulation (Eyden, Winsper, Wolke, Broome, & MacCallum, 2016). Importantly, CM and its link to BPD features exist already in children (Ibrahim, Cosgrave, & Woolgar, 2018). One important factor might be negative emotional reactivity that seems to be a marker of vulnerability that increases the risk for the development of BPD (Stepp, Scott, Jones, Whalen, & Hipwell, 2015). In the supplement, we also provide an extensive review of the psychological consequences of CM on social cognition and their implications for treatment (see online Supplemental Material 1). Here, we want to focus on how the effects of CM in individuals with BPD can be conceived of in PP terms.
Childhood maltreatment and predictive processing
In brief, we are proposing a developmental active inference and learning account of BPD that can be summarized as follows: Early traumatic experiences (i.e. CM) – particularly those involving disorganized attachment or emotional abuse/neglect – lead to an impoverished model of self and others, under which the consequences of social behavior are unpredictable. This has two consequences:
First, a loss of confidence or precision when selecting the course of action in social exchanges. This irreducible uncertainty – about ‘what would happen if I did that?’ – underwrites the emotional lability and impulsivity, characteristic of BDP. The implicit loss of precision, afforded prior beliefs about social narratives, renders belief updating overly sensitive to sensory evidence and social feedback.
To illustrate this key aspect of our argument, consider the example of a child being sad and crying because it feels alone, with the mother invalidating the child's experience by saying there is no reason to be sad and leaving the room. In this case, the child experiences a lack of understanding and feel that their emotions and thoughts are called into question by their parents or significant others (Musser, Zalewski, Stepp, & Lewis, 2018), resulting in an impoverished model of the self in form of low emotional awareness (especially difficulties in identifying and describing emotions) (Smith et al., 2019b; Smith et al., 2022) that has been linked to BPD (Derks et al., 2017). Relatedly, early risk factors for BPD in adulthood include the maternal withdrawal in infancy and separation of 1 month or more from the mother in the first 5 years of life (Steele & Siever, 2010). Furthermore, an interesting candidate for a specific parent-child-relationship risk factor for BPD is parental inconsistency (Boucher et al., 2017), leading to the child's perception that other people's behavior is unpredictable. If such invalidating situations are frequently experienced, it is understandable that the child does not learn to place confidence in their beliefs and thus remains uncertain about the causes of their sensations. Put another way, such repeated experiences leave the child with the interpretation that their thoughts and emotions are ‘wrong’, leading in turn to increased uncertainty about their beliefs and perceptions that results in dysfunctional behavior (e.g. ineffective emotion regulation strategies). Indeed, a recent study indicated that BPD, compared to other mental disorders, is associated with a less frequent use of effective emotion regulation strategies (i.e. cognitive reappraisal, problem solving, and acceptance) and a more frequent use of dysfunctional emotion regulation strategies (i.e. suppression, rumination, and avoidance) (Daros & Williams, 2019).*,1
Second, the failure to learn a suitably expressive generative model, during neurodevelopment, leaves patients with BDP with impoverished, coarse-grained models of self and other. The ensuing interpersonal ‘alexithymia’ manifests as rigidity and inflexibility in social or affective interactions. In other words, everything is ‘black or white’, with no ‘shades of gray’ that would support a nuanced inference about the intention of others (and self). In the absence of an expressive social world model, the only explanations available – for unpredicted sensory or social outcomes – are ‘I am worthless’ (i.e. self-critical explanations) or ‘you are punitive’ (i.e. paranoid explanations).
This developmental account rests upon the intimate relationship between learning and inference. Here, a failure to learn a sufficiently expressive model of interpersonal narratives precludes prosocial inference and planning that precludes subsequent learning or updating of self-other models. This is not unlike some accounts of severe autism and the failure to attain central coherence (Happé & Frith, 2006; van de Cruys et al., 2014). In autism, this developmental failure is sometimes attributed to failure of sensory attenuation (i.e. a failure to attenuate sensory precision in relation to prior precision). This suggests interesting parallels between self stimulation in autism and non-fatal self-harm in BPD.
In terms of the neurochemistry of uncertainty or precision encoding in the brain, our analysis speaks to the same kind of hyperdopaminergic state that may characterize certain forms of schizophrenia or delusional disorders. In our case, this can be read directly from the role of dopamine in active inference, as scoring the resolution of uncertainty about policies afforded by sensory evidence: in BPD, every piece of sensory (or social) information resolves uncertainty. This is because beliefs about the narrative currently being pursued are always uncertain or imprecise. The analogy with schizophrenia here may be useful in the sense that some people interpret delusions as an attempt to make sense of unattenuated sensory evidence (Sterzer et al., 2018).
The predictive processing account of BPD
While most models of BPD take a lifespan approach and consider the complex interplay of biological vulnerabilities (e.g. genetics), psychological factors and social influences (Stepp, Lazarus, & Byrd, 2016), BPD is currently viewed as a disorder of instability, i.e. instability of interpersonal relationships, self-image, affects, and, relatedly, a marked impulsivity (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Here, we recast these core clinical aspects of BPD through the lens of a PP model (see Fig. 2). In doing so, we build upon a few previous accounts offering computational perspectives on BPD (Fineberg, 2019; Fineberg, Stahl, & Corlett, 2017; Fineberg, Steinfeld, Brewer, & Corlett, 2014b). For example, in an elaborated approach, Fineberg et al. (2017) put emphasis on the association between early disruption of mothers' physical care and social dysfunction as a key feature in BPD, explained by social learning depending on reinforcement learning though embodied simulations.
Affective instability
BPD is characterized by an affective instability, in terms of rapid changes of mood, ranging from chronic feelings of emptiness2 to intense anger, anxiety, or dysphoria – and difficulties in handling these mood fluctuations. From a PP point of view, emotional states have been theorized to be a consequence of acting in the world with the aim of minimizing expected free energy, that is, the uncertainty about the future consequences of actions (Kiverstein, Miller, & Rietveld, 2020). In other words, emotional states reflect changes in the uncertainty about the somatic consequences of action (Joffily & Coricelli, 2013; Seth & Friston, 2016; Wager et al., 2015), with uncertainty relating to the precision with which motor and physiological states are predicted and inferred (Clark et al., 2018). By this view, painful emotions have been thought to accompany events that elicit beliefs of unpredictability, whereas pleasant emotions refer to events that resolve uncertainty and convey a sense of control and confidence (Barrett & Satpute, 2013; Clark et al., 2018; Gu, Hof, Friston, & Fan, 2013). Thus, the valence of emotional states relates to the resolution of uncertainty and the precision with which the consequences of action are predicted (Brown & Friston, 2012; Clark et al., 2018). Interestingly, BPD features predicted specific patterns of bias with regard to forecasting future (negative) emotional states (Hughes & Rizvi, 2019).
Linking this account with the corollaries of CM as discussed above, we suggest that BPD is related to the failure to resolve uncertainty and the dominance of predictions of unpredictability, resulting in intense painful emotions. Relatedly, we propose that people with BPD afford their beliefs about the consequences of action little precision; as a result, interoceptive experience is largely influenced by sensory evidence. Hence, the exact emotional experience may vary greatly with respect to the nature and the source of sensory information. In other words, because people with BPD have learned to be uncertain about their consequences of action, their emotional experience fluctuates significantly depending on the (perceived) situational circumstances and other people's behavior. For example, as people with BPD have often grown up in an invalidating environment, they may have learnt that experiencing a certain emotion (e.g. sadness) and expressing a corresponding emotional need (e.g. being consoled by their parents) has no influence on their parents' actual response (e.g. neglect perceived as a negative consequence). Thus, they cannot resolve uncertainty and experience fluctuating intense emotions, by likewise generating a distorted internal model of emotions (e.g. no access to the concept of being consoled) and a reduced self-efficacy. In addition to the uncertainty inherent to beliefs of people with BPD, the experience of CM and the deficit in adaptive emotion regulation strategies may lead to the pervasive belief that emotions are uncontrollable. Due to the high precision afforded to it, this belief is difficult to be revised through new experiences.
Self-image instability
Besides affective instability, BPD is characterized by identity instability which leads to a markedly and persistently unstable, yet often negative, self-concept as well as a lack of a self-coherence. This cognitive instability is related to uncertainty in at least two of the following life domains: self-image, sexual orientation, long-term goals or career choice, type of friends desired, and values. From a PP point of view, this self-image instability manifests through low precision that is afforded to prior beliefs. As a result, beliefs are hastily updated in line with novel information. In other words, because the beliefs of people with BPD – about both the self and others – are fraught with uncertainty, they are often updated based on fairly thin evidence. This account is well in line with evidence from experimental studies, particularly research that has examined how people with BPD update their beliefs in response to social feedback. For instance, Korn, la Rosée, Heekeren, & Roepke (2016a) found that people with BPD, in contrast to healthy people (Korn, Prehn, Park, Walter, & Heekeren, 2012), adjusted their beliefs about themselves significantly in line with undesirable social feedback, that is, a single negative interpersonal experience (Korn et al., 2012, 2016a). In line with that, Liebke et al. (2018) showed in a virtual group-interaction paradigm (where participants interacted with a group of computer-controlled avatars, although they believed them to be real human co-players) that people with BPD updated their beliefs about being socially accepted in response to negative but not positive social feedback (Liebke et al., 2018). Interestingly, the authors demonstrated that people with BPD even behaved less cooperatively in a subsequent trust game if they had previously received positive social feedback. Similarly, another study found that whereas healthy people focus on the positive aspects of social feedback, people with BPD focus more on negative feedback, thereby maintaining negative self-views (Van Schie, Chiu, Rombouts, Heiser, & Elzinga, 2020).
This volatility of belief updating – with more adjustments in response to negative feedback – can again be linked to the effects of early learning experiences and CM. In particular, children who experienced CM often receive negative social feedback, e.g. parents' scolding, which children seek to integrate in their attempt to satisfy their parents. When unexpectedly receiving positive feedback, children might be confused about how to interpret that, thus increasing the uncertainty in the children's perceptions and predictions. In computational terms, the world (i.e. the social feedback by significant others) seems unpredictable as the child is permanently exposed to PEs. Given the prevailing negative social feedback, though, people with CM learn to afford higher precision to negative than to positive feedback, while at the same time affording high precision to negative prior beliefs about the self. Both contributes to the above-described asymmetry in belief updating in BPD, with more rapid changes in line with novel negative information and the persistence of strong negative beliefs about the self3. This affects also social decision making and learning in a way that BPD patients expect higher volatility than control which underpins social and non-social belief updating in BPD (Fineberg et al., 2018b).
Instability of interpersonal relationships
As a consequence of self-image instability and impaired social decision making, BPD is also associated with a pattern of unstable and intense interpersonal relationships, while alternating between extremes in the perception of other people, i.e. idealization and devaluation. For example, patients' perceptions of their therapist can change rapidly, from beliefs such as ‘My therapist is the only person who has ever understood me’ to ‘My therapist wants to get rid of me’. From a PP perspective, idealization and devaluation is related to hasty changes of beliefs about other people, resulting from weak priors and high precision afforded to new information. This instability likely leads to difficulties in interpersonal relationships, with rapid alternations of desperate efforts to avoid abandonment and a premature relationship termination. In line with this notion, research has shown that BPD patients exhibit less trust during interpersonal interactions (Unoka, Seres, Áspán, Bódi, & Kéri, 2009), reflecting the high degree of uncertainty people with BPD experience in social interactions. In a transdiagnostic clinical sample, it has been shown that a reduced confidence in how to act, rather than increased emotional conflicts, explains maladaptive approach-avoidance behaviors, i.e. a greater decision uncertainty during approach-avoidance conflicts (Smith et al., 2020). Consistent with that, a recent study found a 2-fold larger preferred interpersonal distance in BPD patients than in healthy people (Fineberg et al., 2018a).
In addition to instability in their relationships, people with BPD also form strong negative beliefs about relations to other people in their attempt to reduce uncertainty, as touched upon above. This rigidity in beliefs about other people may underlie another symptom cluster of BPD: transient, stress-related paranoid ideation (i.e. the belief that harm is intended by others). In line with this account, a recent study found that paranoia may make it harder to update beliefs and it is linked with an increased risk of violence towards oneself (Reed et al., 2020). More precisely, this study showed that uncertainty may be sufficient to elicit learning differences in paranoid individuals, even without social threat; and paranoia is associated with a stronger prior on volatility, accompanied by elevated sensitivity to perceived changes in the task environment (Reed et al., 2020). In other words, people with paranoia expect the world to change frequently, change their minds repeatedly, and have a harder time learning in response to changing circumstances. This finding is in line with the general high uncertainty of BPD patients and may explain paranoid ideation in BPD, further highlighting the interdependence between inference and learning in BPD. That is, BPD patients with higher scores on paranoid ideations might have more difficulty updating their beliefs in interpersonal situations (i.e. learning difficulties in response to changing circumstances), leading to more interpersonal problems. In fact, difficulties with trusting others and volatile impressions of others' moral character are often problems that result in the premature termination of a relationship. Of note, the moral inference differed: In patients with BPD, beliefs about harmful agents were more certain and less amenable to updating relative to healthy controls (Siegel, Curwell-Parry, Pearce, Saunders, & Crockett, 2020). For instance, when interacting with therapists within a mental health care setting, one specific focus is to build a strong therapeutic alliance. Conceivably, there are two possible outcomes: a weak therapeutic alliance will probably lead to premature treatment discontinuation – a common problem in the treatment of BPD (Barnicot, Katsakou, Marougka, & Priebe, 2011). On the other hand, in the case of a strong therapeutic alliance, patients strive to avoid changes in the therapeutic setting (e.g. changing therapists due to leaves) as they often distrust other therapists – despite previous positive experiences and the fact that other therapists will probably also be kind to them. As such, this example highlights once more the lack of a suitably expressive social world model and the use of an impoverished, coarse-grained model of self and other that leads to rigidity and inflexibility in social interactions preventing a more nuanced inference about the intention of others. In line with that notion, interpersonal functioning was found to predict non-delusional paranoia in BPD (Oliva, Dalmotto, Pirfo, Furlan, & Picci, 2014) – some type of non-delusional paranoia was reported by 87% in a BPD sample (Zanarini, Frankenburg, Wedig, & Fitzmaurice, 2013).
Impulsivity
BPD is also characterized by a variety of impulsive behaviors, such as promiscuity and substance abuse. The most prominent example of such behaviors, which aim at reducing emotional tension, is non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI). According to PP, such impulsive behaviors may seek to resolve uncertainty: if intense aversive emotions reflect the prediction of unpredictability (Clark et al., 2018), any behavior aimed at reducing such intense aversive emotional states may convey a sense of control. In other words, people with BPD have learnt that certain behaviors, such as NSSI, lower their emotional tension, thereby increasing the certainty of the consequences of action.
In our account, impulsive behavior (such as NSSI) is considered more as a consequence of affective instability. In fact, NSSI may be reinforced by its affect stabilization function (Vansteelandt et al., 2017). In line, a recent study showed that affective instability was significantly greater in adolescents engaging in NSSI, and the number of BPD criteria met was positively correlated with affective instability in the NSSI group (Santangelo et al., 2017). Particularly, higher levels of momentary negative affect predicted greater subsequent urges to self-injure, but only when self-concept clarity was low, supporting interactive effects (Scala et al., 2018). Remarkably, impulsivity as a personality trait per se is genetically influenced and heritable (Balestri, Calati, Serretti, & de Ronchi, 2014; Bevilacqua & Goldman, 2013; Bezdjian, Baker, & Tuvblad, 2011; Fineberg et al., 2014a; Khadka et al., 2014).
The basic assumptions of the PP account of BPD are displayed in Fig. 2.
Neural specification of this account
Contemporary etiological theories of BPD assume that biological predispositions (i.e. genetic factors) are potentiated by environmental risk factors (i.e. CM). Studies estimating the heritability of the basic dimensions of personality disorders report approximately between 35% and 56% (Jang, Livesley, Vernon, & Jackson, 1996; Livesley, Jang, Jackson, & Vernon, 1993). Twin and family studies estimated a moderate to high heritability in BPD indicating a general genetic risk factor, but highlight also individually unique environmental influences (Distel et al., 2008, 2010; Skoglund et al., 2021). Indeed, results from a longitudinal discordant twin design show that there might be a genetic influence underlying the association of traumatic events with BPD, rather than BPD being directly caused by a trauma (Bornovalova et al., 2013). Likely contributing biological factors include genes linked to dopamine, serotonin, the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, and neuropeptides (Steele & Siever, 2010). Research points to abnormalities in the dopamine system in people with BPD (Friedel, 2004; Oquendo & Mann, 2000). The efficacy of dopamine D2 receptor (DRD2) blocking antipsychotic drugs in BPD treatment also suggests the involvement of the dopamine system in the neurobiology of BPD (Nemoda et al., 2010). Moreover, dopamine transporter (DAT1) gene variants increase the risk of BPD (Amad, Ramoz, Thomas, Jardri, & Gorwood, 2014; Joyce, Stephenson, Kennedy, Mulder, & McHugh, 2013). This crucial role of dopamine is well in line with the PP account of BPD, because in PP dopamine is thought to encode the precision of beliefs that underwrite choices and behavior (Schwartenbeck, FitzGerald, Mathys, Dolan, & Friston, 2015), and dopamine has been shown to modulate belief updating (Sharot et al., 2012). Drawing on this previous work, we suggest that the increased integration of negative social feedback in BPD corresponds to enhanced levels of dopamine in the respective synaptic gains, accounting for the increased use of that information to update the prior prediction. Computationally, this is associated with high precision with which new information is encoded.
Another psychobiological mechanism that might be involved in the psychopathology of BPD refers to the connectivity of different brain areas. Specifically, the increased integration of negative social feedback in BPD might be related to a reduced inhibitory control of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) in PE processing. In particular, previous research has shown that PE processing in the reward system (i.e. ventral striatum) can be suppressed by the PFC, resulting in a lack of belief updating, as demonstrated for pain perception (Schenk, Sprenger, Onat, Colloca, & Büchel, 2017) and reward processing in depression (Greenberg et al., 2015). More specifically, research in the neuronal coding of PE (Schultz & Dickinson, 2000) has indicated that neurons in the dorsolateral prefrontal, orbitofrontal, and anterior cingulate cortex are activated in relation to errors in the prediction of reward (Niki & Watanabe, 1979; Watanabe, 1989), and these PEs are processed in combination with neurons in the striatum that code rewards relative to their unpredictability (Apicella, Legallet, & Trouche, 1997) and neurons in the amygdala signaling reward-predicting stimuli (Nishijo, Ono, & Nishino, 1988). Drawing on this work, we suggest that in BPD, the PFC might fail to execute inhibitory control over the processing of PEs from social feedback, thereby contributing to its increased integration.
Furthermore, research has focused on the neural base of distorted affective processing in BPD as supported by five meta-analytic reviews (de-Almeida et al., 2012; Mitchell, Dickens, & Picchioni, 2014; Rodrigues et al., 2011; Ruocco, Amirthavasagam, & Zakzanis, 2012; Ruocco, Amirthavasagam, Choi-Kain, & McMain, 2013). In this line of (mostly fMRI) research, investigators have examined how people with BPD and healthy participants process negative emotional stimuli relative to neutral stimuli. Taken together, such neuroimaging studies suggest that dysfunctional fronto-limbic brain regions underlie the emotional dysregulation in BPD (Krause-Utz, Winter, Niedtfeld, & Schmahl, 2014). Specifically, individuals with BPD showed structural and functional abnormalities in a fronto-limbic network including regions involved in emotion processing (e.g. amygdala, insula) and frontal brain regions implicated in regulatory control processes (e.g. anterior cingulate cortex, medial frontal cortex, orbitofrontal cortex, and dorsolateral PFC) (Krause-Utz et al., 2014).
In line with the assumption of BPD as an emotion dysregulation disorder, a more recent multimodal meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies synthesized that BPD is related to abnormal activation of dorsolateral prefrontal and limbic brain regions (Hazlett, 2016; Schulze, Schmahl, & Niedtfeld, 2016). In particular, BPD patients showed an increased activation of the left amygdala and posterior cingulate cortex, along with debilitated responses of the bilateral dorsolateral PFC while processing negative emotional stimuli. Interestingly, the functional corticolimbic connectivity, in particular between the right amygdala and right dorsolateral PFC, mediates the relationship between childhood adversities and symptom severity in BPD (Vai et al., 2018). Another fMRI study (Scherpiet et al., 2014) found abnormalities not only in the perception but also in the anticipation of negative emotional stimuli. Collectively, this line of research is well in line with the PP account of BPD in that it provides neural evidence for the hypothesis BPD is related to the anticipation of negative events and experiences, increasing the likelihood of actually experiencing intense negative emotions, which is reflected by abnormal activity in fronto-limbic networks.
Although depending on the type, frequency and timing of exposure, CM has an influence on the child brain development with functional and structural changes observed even decades later in adulthood (Jedd et al., 2015): Associations between CM and brain structures have been widely documented with the most frequent alterations related to CM in the function and structure of lateral and ventromedial fronto-limbic brain areas and neural networks (i.e. deficits in structural interregional connectivity) that mediate behavioral, cognitive and affect control (Hart & Rubia, 2012; Lim, Radua, & Rubia, 2014). In particular, CM is related to a reduced volume of the (adult) hippocampus (Paquola, Bennett, & Lagopoulos, 2016; Teicher, Anderson, & Polcari, 2012), anterior cingulate and ventromedial and dorsallateral prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortices, as well as to the development of the corpus callosum (Teicher & Samson, 2016; Teicher, Samson, Anderson, & Ohashi, 2016). Moreover, a review of fMRI studies found that CM is associated with altered functioning in a range of neurocognitive systems (i.e. threat processing, reward processing, emotion regulation and executive control) (McCrory, Gerin, & Viding, 2017). In fact, an association was found with an enhanced amygdala response to threatening stimuli (Dannlowski et al., 2012), reduced ventral striatal response to the anticipation or receipt of reward, decreased connectivity between prefrontal regions and the amygdala, and increased volume and network centrality of the precuneus (Teicher et al., 2016). In line with research on CM and brain alterations, a PET study showed a correlation between a dysfunction of the dorsolateral and medial PFC (including anterior cingulate) with the recall of CM specifically in BPD (Schmahl, Vermetten, Elzinga, & Bremner, 2004). Indeed, CM is associated with structural impairment in the (right) ventrolateral PFC and aggressiveness in patients with BPD (Morandotti et al., 2013). Further, some researchers found a hypoconnectivity between structures associated with emotion regulation and structures associated with social cognitive responses in BPD: Higher levels of CM were associated with reduced levels of brain connectivity, with different types of CM having differential effects on connectivity in BPD patients (Duque-Alarcón, Alcalá-Lozano, González-Olvera, Garza-Villarreal, & Pellicer, 2019). In support of our account of CM, there is some evidence for complex gene-environment interactions involving CM that determine the risk or protection against BPD pathology (Goodman, New, & Siever, 2004). Indeed, CM impacts biological processes epigenetically (Prados et al., 2015). Providing evidence of epigenome × environment interactions, epigenetic modifications of the glucocorticoid receptor gene (i.e. hGR methylation) were strongly associated with an increased vulnerability to psychopathology in CM (Radtke et al., 2015). More and more evidence points to a severe relationship dysfunction being the core epigenetic expression of BPD (Steele & Siever, 2010).
On a hormone level, early CM was associated with reduced plasma oxytocin (Kluczniok et al., 2019), highlighting the role of oxytocin in BPD (Herpertz & Bertsch, 2015). In the pathogenesis of BPD, a beneficial effect of oxytocin on threat processing and stress responsiveness was found (Bertsch & Herpertz, 2018), despite considerable heterogeneity in the literature (Amad, Thomas, & Perez-Rodriguez, 2015; Bertsch & Herpertz, 2018).
The neural specification of our PP account of BPD is displayed in Fig. 3.
Novelty of this account
The present article is the first to apply current thinking in neuroscience and computational psychiatry to provide a novel mechanistic model of BPD. Specifically, by highlighting how the experience of CM is thought to alter perception, our account provides a coherent explanation as to why people with BPD show rapid fluctuations of intense emotions, cognitions, and inconsistent behaviors. Moreover, proposing that CM leads to a dysbalance between the precision of prior beliefs and data, our account can also explain why people with BPD are highly sensitive to negative social feedback, whereas information processing in other domains is intact. Thus, in contrast to previous cognitive theories of BPD (Arntz, 1994; Arntz, Dietzel, & Dreessen, 1999; Arntz, Dreessen, Schouten, & Weertman, 2004; Baer, Peters, Eisenlohr-Moul, Geiger, & Sauer, 2012; Beck, Davis, & Freeman, 2015; Butler, Brown, Beck, & Grisham, 2002), we suggest that BPD may not primarily be related to the presence of dysfunctional beliefs per se (i.e. their contents), but to aberrant precision afforded to them. This is consistent with recent theories from computational psychiatry (Kube & Rozenkrantz, 2021; Paulus et al., 2019). Moreover, our account goes beyond previous theories of BPD as a disorder of emotion regulation (Linehan, 1993) by assuming that the instability of affect, cognition, and behavior has one underlying pathology, that is, overly weak prior predictions. At the same time, our model can also account for the rigidity (or: lack of flexibility) of BPD patients by conceiving of it as an expression of the patients' impoverished generative model resulting from CM.
Implications for the treatment and prevention of BPD
On the basis of our review, two major clinical implications can be drawn. First, the crucial role of CM on the perception and cognition of a child and its emotional development. People generate a model of its environment (including the external world and the body) which predicts future sensory inputs and is updated by PEs, depending on how precise these error signals are. Expectation and attention increase the integration of top-down and bottom-up signals in perception (Gordon, Tsuchiya, Koenig-Robert, & Hohwy, 2019). Interventions focusing on both enhancing sensitivity to the validity of sensory input/evidence and therefore PE; and attention on expectations v. sensory input to disentangle imbalance might be promising treatment approaches. In general, individuals tend to have an optimism bias, processing desirable information more frequently than undesirable information. Findings suggest that BPD patients appear initially more pessimistic about their personal future than healthy people but they might be able to overcome their pessimism when provided with relevant information (Korn et al., 2016b). Indeed, optimism training has potential to change individuals with mild dysphoria perceptions about the future (Yoshimura & Hashimoto, 2020). By sampling precise empirical evidence, BPD patients might enhance their predictability of the world and reduce uncertainty in their beliefs. Before, traditional cognitive techniques such as the, Downward Arrow‘-technique – sometimes referred to as Vertical Descent (Leahy, 2017) – might be useful to identify relevant expectations (‘predictions’). Individual treatment should then focus on techniques to increase the attention of BPD patients to PEs by an elevated propensity to counterbalance and weight perceptual beliefs (priors) over sensory evidence and interventions to empirically examine the credibility of one's beliefs in order to strengthen the precision of one's priors and enhance belief updating. Attention alters PP indicating that there are top-down effects of attention on perception (Clark, 2016b). Some researchers have proposed that salience is something that is afforded to actions that realize epistemic affordance, while attention per se is afforded to precise sensory evidence – or beliefs about the causes of sensations (Parr & Friston, 2017). Indeed, attention and PE suggest that information sought by top-down-attention is prioritized (Ramamoorthy, Parker, Plaisted-Grant, Muhl-Richardson, & Davis, 2020). Attention optimizes the expected precision of predictions by modulating the synaptic gain of PE units, that is, attention increases the selectivity for mismatch information in the neural response to a surprising stimulus indicating that attention optimizes precision expectations during hierarchical inference by increasing the gain of PEs (Smout, Tang, Garrido, & Mattingley, 2019). Furthermore, it is widely accepted that predictions across different stimulus attributes (e.g. time and content) facilitate sensory processing. Content (‘what’) and temporal (‘when’) predictions engage complementary neural mechanisms in different brain regions, suggesting domain-specific prediction signaling along the cortical hierarchy (Auksztulewicz et al., 2018). Moreover, as BPD patients perceive the world as ambiguous, building new expectations should be based on repetitions and hints which have been shown to facilitate perceptual experience of ambiguous images (Hertz, Blakemore, & Frith, 2020). Yet, people have two main pathways to determine the veracity of the sensory input: the perceived credibility of the source and direct-evaluation via first-hand evidence, i.e. testing the advice against observation. Beliefs are interpreted in light of the perceived credibility of the source in form of credibility-led biased interpretations of evidence (whether belief or suspicion confirming) that lead to further polarization of the perceived credibility highlighting the crucial role of credibility in belief updating (Pilditch, Madsen, & Custers, 2020), while cues including valence and relevance influence these credibility judgments suggesting a utility and credibility trade off during decision making (Gugerty & Link, 2020). Therefore, creating a more nuanced credibility picture in BPD patients might also be promising treatment target. By providing the brain with an intense inflow of salient and unambiguous bottom-up sensory input to shift the brains mapping of the observed body state, interoceptive interventions might provide a base for more directly targeting and manipulating those (attentional) processes regarding the interoceptive system and correcting somatic errors (Paulus et al., 2019) that also might foster ‘mineness’ (also called ‘subjective presence’ or ‘personalization’) as the feeling that experiences belong to a continuing self (Gerrans, 2020). As such, exposure-based interventions (including exteroceptive as well as interoceptive exposure techniques) are useful to exacerbate somatic errors and therefore to adaptively adjust their prior expectations with new sensory input (evidence). As a slightly different approach to process aversive interoceptive sensations, mindfulness techniques as used as a central part in DBT might help minimize somatic errors by shifting attention away from the predicted body state and toward the observed body state (Farb et al., 2015), i.e. predictions of the body state and somatic error might naturally vanish as the mind attempts to function with low-precision priors triggering fewer regulatory responses and that allows the entire predictive model to be driven by incoming sensory input from the present moment in time. Traditionally not a standard tool in psychotherapy, new interventions such as the floatation-REST (Reduced Environmental Stimulation Therapy)4 might be able to enrich current state-of-the-art treatments such as DBT (Feinstein et al., 2018). Furthermore, whole body hyperthermia (WBH) (Janssen et al., 2016), modulation of muscle tension via Swedish massage (Rapaport et al., 2016), yoga (Jeter, Slutsky, Singh, & Khalsa, 2015), and exercise (Smits, Berry, Tart, & Powers, 2008); repeated brief exposures to high doses of CO2 (Wolpe, 1987); and cyclic activation of the sympathetic nervous system and suppression of the immune response through cold immersion and CO2-modulation using alternating cycles of hyperventilation followed by breath holding (Kox et al., 2014) might also yield a future potential in this regard (Paulus et al., 2019).
Second, this account highlights the role of prevention of CM in risk populations with low educational skills, and in parents suffering also from mental disorders, in particular emotion regulation deficits (such as in the case of mothers with BPD). Both, parenting programs as well as mother-child-interventions might serve as important preventive strategies to reduce the occurrence of CM. For example, in some clinics there exist group psychotherapy for mothers suffering from BPD, and evidence-based parenting programs such as the Triple p – Positive Parenting Program (Bodenmann, Cina, Ledermann, & Sanders, 2008; Sanders, 1999, 2008, 2012). In a critical and sensitive phase in child development, children must be encouraged to build appropriate confidence (i.e. precision) in their expectations and beliefs (i.e. priors) by a validating environment that see their emotions and fulfill their needs appropriately leading to fewer PEs (i.e. minimizing uncertainty and surprise in the environment to make the world and behavior of others more predictable to them).
Limitations and future directions
Although the majority of patients with BPD report maltreatment in their childhood (Battle et al., 2004; Carlson et al., 2009) and patients with BPD were over 13 times more likely to report childhood adversity than non-clinical controls and other clinical populations (Kleindienst et al., 2020; Porter et al., 2020), a major limitation of our model is that it only explains aberrant belief updating in patients with BPD that have experienced some sort of CM. For BPD patients without CM in the past, genetics might be a more relevant risk factor in the pathogenesis of BPD as studies found a moderate to high heritability in BPD (Distel et al., 2008, 2010; Skoglund et al., 2021). However, despite the potentially different etiology, we believe that a similar psychobiological pathology in the sense of PP (i.e. the role of priors, precision and likelihood) underlies also in those patients with BPD. Furthermore, although particularly relevant in BPD (Brakemeier et al., 2018; Pietrek et al., 2013; Quenneville et al., 2020; Wota et al., 2014), CM is considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Gilbert et al., 2009; Struck et al., 2020) and future studies should investigate the specific causal contribution of CM to the pathogenesis of BPD. Though our account can build upon the wealth of literature on childhood adversities, no consensus has been reached about how to measure poor parenting or the invalidating environment to quantify the extent to which these types of specific factors contribute to BPD (Musser et al., 2018). Also, different forms of CM may have different consequences. Similarly, there is a lack of assessment tools to measure a key element of our account: the precision of prior beliefs and new information. Therefore, much of what we proposed here as conceptually-based evidence still needs to be empirically tested in future work using rigorous experimental designs and computational modeling to generate data-based evidence.
Despite strong arguments in favor of PP (Seth, Millidge, Buckley, & Tschantz, 2020; van de Cruys, Friston, & Clark, 2020), it should be noted there have also been some critical voices regarding PP as a ‘theory-of-everything’ (Hutto, 2020; Litwin & Miłkowski, 2020; Sun & Firestone, 2020). Indeed, there seem overlapping features and similarities to traditional theories (F.A. França, 2020) and some researchers urge for refinements of the PP theory in order to increase its explanatory power (Gilead, Trope, & Liberman, 2020; Vilas, Melloni, & Melloni, 2020). This critique is in part also relevant to the present article as it applies one set of theoretical assumptions to explain a fairly heterogeneous disorder. Yet, we believe that PP has the potential to inspire future research and allows to derive some novel hypotheses about the psychopathology of BPD that may contribute to a more nuanced understanding of this complex disorder, particularly in view of the developmental psychopathology perspective PP offers.
For an overview of specific hypotheses that can be derived from our account, see Table 1.
Table 1.
Hypothesis | Empirical examination | ||
---|---|---|---|
Behavioral level | Influence of CM on ones' beliefs throughout early life |
The influence of CM on adult BPD is mediated by altered perception and cognition in adolescence characterized through high uncertainty of beliefs (i.e. weak priors). | Longitudinal study on the influence of CM on perception and cognition in adulthood |
Instability in affect, cognitions and behavior and social-decision making | Specific hypotheses:
|
Experimental studies investigating (negative) social feedback using mixed methods for symptom provocation in the lab, e.g. personalized narratives in the form of script-driven imagery (Kraus et al., 2010) or naturalistic approach (Miano, Fertuck, Roepke, & Dziobek, 2017) and computer-based social rejection paradigms [e.g. virtual-reality-enhanced cyberball paradigm (McCall, 2016) or social feedback and imagined scenarios in NoOneLikesYou! paradigm (D'Astolfo, Kirchner, & Rief, 2020)] or social interactive paradigms to quantify social behavior [e.g. multi-round economic exchange games such as the established trust game (Rilling & Sanfey, 2011; Tzieropoulos, 2013)] | |
Neurobiological and computational level | Precision | BPD is related to reduced precision of prior predictions and increased precision afforded to new information, entailing enhanced updating in line with new information. | Using computational modeling to assess the influence of prior precision and data precision on the posterior |
Dopamine | Elevated levels of dopamine are related to the increased integration of sensory information. | Combining neurochemical analyses with computational modeling | |
Dopamine encodes the precision of PEs rather than PEs per se. | |||
Providing drugs that decrease the dopamine system (DRD2-blocking antipsychotics) weakens the precision afforded to and the increased integration of (negative) social feedback into beliefs. | Administering dopamine-decreasing drugs in social rejection paradigms | ||
Connectivity between brain regions | The lack of inhibitory control of PE processing from social feedback executed by the PFC leads to an enhanced integration of social information into beliefs. | Connectivity analysis in fMRI |
Note: BPD, Borderline personality disorder; CM, Childhood maltreatment; PE, prediction error; PFC, prefrontral cortex.
Conclusions
In this article, we proposed a Bayesian account of BPD that relies on the observation that many people with BPD experienced some sort of maltreatment in childhood. We argued that such adverse experiences lead to pervasive alterations in perception. In essence, we suggested that CM impairs the continuous refinement of predictive models of the world and precipitates the formation of weak and strong prior predictions, relative to sensory information. This entails a distorted belief updating in response to novel information, resulting in a marked instability of affect, cognitions, and behavior – that is, the core features of BPD.
Acknowledgements
Figure 3 was created using a template from Servier Medical Art by Servier (http://smart.servier.com/), licensed under a Creative Common Attribution 3.0 Unported License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/).
The notes appear after the main text.
Notes
In addition to these emotion regulation deficits, CM may also contribute to aberrant social cognition in BPD as discussed in further detail in the supplement (see online Supplemental Material 1).
In the online Supplemental Material 2, we provide an explanation for the other end of the emotional spectrum in BPD, i.e. chronic emptiness.
Of note, in domains that are not core to BPD, such as the integration of future life information, there seem to be less abnormalities in belief updating in BPD, although people with BPD have overall more pessimistic expectations than healthy people (Korn et al., 2016b).
A sensory-reduced intervention that attenuates exteroceptive sensory input to brain by reducing the impact of predictive models through the act of floating supine in a pool of water saturated with Epsom salts in order to heighten interoceptive awareness and physiological relaxation and better achieve mindful states (i.e. attention on the observed body state and on the present-moment sensation) (Feinstein et al., 2018).
Supplementary material
For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291722002458.
Author contributions
P. H. conceived the main original ideas. All authors contributed to the development of the theoretical concept and design of the model. P. H. drafted the paper, and T. K. and E. F. provided critical revisions. All authors approved the final version of the paper for submission.
Financial support
E. F. obtained funding from the Else Kröner-Fresenius-Stiftung (2018_A152). This research did not receive any other specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
Conflict of interest
E. F. has provided trainings, presentations and published books/chapters on the treatment of BPD. P. H. and T. K. have no conflicts of interest to declare.
References
- Afifi, T. O., Mather, A., Boman, J., Fleisher, W., Enns, M. W., MacMillan, H., & Sareen, J. (2011). Childhood adversity and personality disorders: Results from a nationally representative population-based study. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 45(6), 814–822. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allen, B. (2008). An analysis of the impact of diverse forms of childhood psychological maltreatment on emotional adjustment in early adulthood. Child Maltreatment, 13(3), 307–312. 10.1177/1077559508318394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amad, A., Ramoz, N., Thomas, P., Jardri, R., & Gorwood, P. (2014). Genetics of borderline personality disorder: Systematic review and proposal of an integrative model. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 40, 6–19. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amad, A., Thomas, P., & Perez-Rodriguez, M. (2015). Borderline personality disorder and oxytocin: Review of clinical trials and future directions. Current Pharmaceutical Design, 21(23), 3311–3316. 10.2174/1381612821666150619093019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. [Google Scholar]
- Apicella, P., Legallet, E., & Trouche, E. (1997). Responses of tonically discharging neurons in the monkey striatum to primary rewards delivered during different behavioral states. Experimental Brain Research, 116(3), 456–466. 10.1007/PL00005773. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arntz, A. (1994). Treatment of borderline personality disorder: A challenge for cognitive-behavioural therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32(4), 419–430. 10.1016/0005-7967(94)90005-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arntz, A., Dietzel, R., & Dreessen, L. (1999). Assumptions in borderline personality disorder: Specificity, stability and relationship with etiological factors. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37(6), 545–557. 10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00152-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arntz, A., Dreessen, L., Schouten, E., & Weertman, A. (2004). Beliefs in personality disorders: A test with the Personality Disorder Belief Questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42(10), 1215–1225. 10.1016/j.brat.2003.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Auksztulewicz, R., Schwiedrzik, C. M., Thesen, T., Doyle, W., Devinsky, O., Nobre, A. C., … Melloni, L. (2018). Not all predictions are equal: ‘what’ and ‘when’ predictions modulate activity in auditory cortex through different mechanisms. Journal of Neuroscience, 38(40), 8680–8693. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0369-18.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baer, R. A., Peters, J. R., Eisenlohr-Moul, T. A., Geiger, P. J., & Sauer, S. E. (2012). Emotion-related cognitive processes in borderline personality disorder: A review of the empirical literature. Clinical Psychology Review, 32(5), 359–369. 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balestri, M., Calati, R., Serretti, A., & de Ronchi, D. (2014). Genetic modulation of personality traits: A systematic review of the literature. International Clinical Psychopharmacology, 29(1), 1–15. 10.1097/YIC.0b013e328364590b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnicot, K., Katsakou, C., Marougka, S., & Priebe, S. (2011). Treatment completion in psychotherapy for borderline personality disorder – a systematic review and meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 123(5), 327–338. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2010.01652.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrett, L. F., Quigley, K. S., & Hamilton, P. (2016). An active inference theory of allostasis and interoception in depression. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1708). 10.1098/rstb.2016.0011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrett, L. F., & Satpute, A. B. (2013). Large-scale brain networks in affective and social neuroscience: Towards an integrative functional architecture of the brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23(3), 361–372. 10.1016/j.conb.2012.12.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrett, L. F., & Simmons, W. K. (2015). Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(7), 419–429. 10.1038/nrn3950. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bastos, A. M., Usrey, W. M., Adams, R. A., Mangun, G. R., Fries, P., & Friston, K. J. (2012). Canonical microcircuits for predictive coding. Neuron, 76(4), 695–711. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Battle, C. L., Shea, M. T., Johnson, D. M., Yen, S., Zlotnick, C., Zanarini, M. C., … Morey, L. C. (2004). Childhood maltreatment associated with adult personality disorders: Findings from the collaborative longitudinal personality disorders study. Journal of Personality Disorders, 18(2), 193–211. 10.1521/pedi.18.2.193.32777. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck, A. T., Davis, D. D., & Freeman, A. (2015). Cognitive therapy of personality disorders. New York, NY, USA: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Bertsch, K., & Herpertz, S. C. (2018). Oxytocin and borderline personality disorder. In Hurlemann R. & Grinevich V. (Eds.), Behavioral pharmacology of neuropeptides: Oxytocin. Current topics in behavioral neurosciences (Vol. 35, pp. 499–514). Cham: Springer Verlag. 10.1007/7854_2017_26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bevilacqua, L., & Goldman, D. (2013). Genetics of impulsive behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 368(1615). 10.1098/rstb.2012.0380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bezdjian, S., Baker, L. A., & Tuvblad, C. (2011). Genetic and environmental influences on impulsivity: A meta-analysis of twin, family and adoption studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(7), 1209–1223. 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.07.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bodenmann, G., Cina, A., Ledermann, T., & Sanders, M. R. (2008). The efficacy of the Triple p-Positive Parenting Program in improving parenting and child behavior: A comparison with two other treatment conditions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46(4), 411–427. 10.1016/j.brat.2008.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bornovalova, M. A., Gratz, K. L., Delany-Brumsey, A., Paulson, A., & Lejuez, C. W. (2006). Temperamental and environmental risk factors for borderline personality disorder among inner-city substance users in residential treatment. Journal of Personality Disorders, 20(3), 218–231. 10.1521/pedi.2006.20.3.218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bornovalova, M. A., Huibregtse, B. M., Hicks, B. M., Keyes, M., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. (2013). Tests of a direct effect of childhood abuse on adult borderline personality disorder traits: A longitudinal discordant twin design. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122(1), 180–194. 10.1037/a0028328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boucher, M-È, Pugliese, J., Allard-Chapais, C., Lecours, S., Ahoundova, L., Chouinard, R., & Gaham, S. (2017). Parent-child relationship associated with the development of borderline personality disorder: A systematic review. Personality and Mental Health, 11(4), 229–255. 10.1002/pmh.1385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bradley, R., Jenei, J., & Westen, D. (2005). Etiology of borderline personality disorder. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 193(1), 24–31. 10.1097/01.nmd.0000149215.88020.7c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brakemeier, E. L., Dobias, J., Hertel, J., Bohus, M., Limberger, M. F., Schramm, E., … Normann, C. (2018). Childhood maltreatment in women with borderline personality disorder, chronic depression, and episodic depression, and in healthy controls. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 87(1), 49–51. 10.1159/000484481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown, H. R., & Friston, K. J. (2012). Dynamic causal modelling of precision and synaptic gain in visual perception – an EEG study. NeuroImage, 63(1), 223–231. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butler, A. C., Brown, G. K., Beck, A. T., & Grisham, J. R. (2002). Assessment of dysfunctional beliefs in borderline personality disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40(10), 1231–1240. 10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00031-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlson, E. A., Egeland, B., & Sroufe, L. A. (2009). A prospective investigation of the development of borderline personality symptoms. Development and Psychopathology, 21(4), 1311–1334. 10.1017/S0954579409990174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carvalho Fernando, S., Beblo, T., Schlosser, N., Terfehr, K., Otte, C., Löwe, B., … Wingenfeld, K. (2014). The impact of self-reported childhood trauma on emotion regulation in borderline personality disorder and major depression. Journal of Trauma and Dissociation, 15(4), 384–401. 10.1080/15299732.2013.863262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chapman, A. L. (2019). Borderline personality disorder and emotion dysregulation. Development and Psychopathology, 31(3), 1143–1156. 10.1017/S0954579419000658. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(3), 181–204. 10.1017/S0140525X12000477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark, A. (2016a). Attention alters predictive processing. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, e234. 10.1017/S0140525X15002472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark, A. (2016b). Surfing uncertainty: Prediction, action, and the embodied mind. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Clark, J. E., Watson, S., & Friston, K. J. (2018). What is mood? A computational perspective. Psychological Medicine, 48(14), 2277–2284. 10.1017/S0033291718000430. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, L. J., Tanis, T., Bhattacharjee, R., Nesci, C., Halmi, W., & Galynker, I. (2014). Are there differential relationships between different types of childhood maltreatment and different types of adult personality pathology? Psychiatry Research, 215(1), 192–201. 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.10.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corlett, P. R., Horga, G., Fletcher, P. C., Alderson-Day, B., Schmack, K., & Powers, A. R. (2019). Hallucinations and strong priors. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(2), 114–127. 10.1016/j.tics.2018.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dannlowski, U., Stuhrmann, A., Beutelmann, V., Zwanzger, P., Lenzen, T., Grotegerd, D., … Kugel, H. (2012). Limbic scars: Long-term consequences of childhood maltreatment revealed by functional and structural magnetic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry, 71(4), 286–293. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.10.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daros, A. R., & Williams, G. E. (2019). A meta-analysis and systematic review of emotion-regulation strategies in borderline personality disorder. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 27(4), 217–232. 10.1097/HRP.0000000000000212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D’astolfo, L., Kirchner, L., & Rief, W. (2020). No1LikesU! - A pilot study on an ecologically valid and highly standardised experimental paradigm to investigate social rejection expectations and their modification. Clinical Psychology in Europe, 2(2), 1–21. 10.32872/cpe.v2i2.2997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de-Almeida, C. P., Wenzel, A., de-Carvalho, C. S., Powell, V. B., Araújo-Neto, C., Quarantini, L. C., & de-Oliveira, I. R. (2012). Amygdalar volume in borderline personality disorder with and without comorbid post-traumatic stress disorder: A meta-analysis. CNS Spectrums, 17(2), 70–75. 10.1017/S1092852912000466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Aquino Ferreira, L. F., Queiroz Pereira, F. H., Neri Benevides, A. M. L., & Aguiar Melo, M. C. (2018). Borderline personality disorder and sexual abuse: A systematic review. Psychiatry Research, 262, 70–77. 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.01.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Derks, Y. P. M. J., Westerhof, G. J., & Bohlmeijer, E. T. (2017). A meta-analysis on the association between emotional awareness and borderline personality pathology. Journal of Personality Disorders, 31(3), 362–384. 10.1521/pedi_2016_30_257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Distel, M. A., Trull, T. J., Derom, C. A., Thiery, E. W., Grimmer, M. A., Martin, N. G., … Boomsma, D. I. (2008). Heritability of borderline personality disorder features is similar across three countries. Psychological Medicine, 38(9), 1219–1229. 10.1017/S0033291707002024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Distel, M. A., Willemsen, G., Ligthart, L., Derom, C. A., Martin, N. G., Neale, M. C., … Boomsma, D. I. (2010). Genetic covariance structure of the four main features of borderline personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 24(4), 427–444. 10.1521/pedi.2010.24.4.427. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duque-Alarcón, X., Alcalá-Lozano, R., González-Olvera, J. J., Garza-Villarreal, E. A., & Pellicer, F. (2019). Effects of childhood maltreatment on social cognition and brain functional connectivity in borderline personality disorder patients. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10(MAR), 156. 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eyden, J., Winsper, C., Wolke, D., Broome, M. R., & MacCallum, F. (2016). A systematic review of the parenting and outcomes experienced by offspring of mothers with borderline personality pathology: Potential mechanisms and clinical implications. Clinical Psychology Review, 47, 85–105. 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- F.A. França, T. (2020). Predictive processing: Is the future just a memory? European Journal of Neuroscience, 52(10), 4230–4232. 10.1111/ejn.14874. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farb, N., Daubenmier, J., Price, C. J., Gard, T., Kerr, C., Dunn, B. D., … Mehling, W. E. (2015). Interoception, contemplative practice, and health. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(JUN). 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feinstein, J. S., Khalsa, S. S., Yeh, H., al Zoubi, O., Arevian, A. C., Wohlrab, C., … Paulus, M. P. (2018). The elicitation of relaxation and interoceptive awareness using floatation therapy in individuals with high anxiety sensitivity. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 3(6), 555–562. 10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fineberg, N. A., Chamberlain, S. R., Goudriaan, A. E., Stein, D. J., Vanderschuren, L. J. M. J., Gillan, C. M., … Potenza, M. N. (2014a). New developments in human neurocognition: Clinical, genetic, and brain imaging correlates of impulsivity and compulsivity. CNS Spectrums, 19(1), 69–89. 10.1017/S1092852913000801. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fineberg, S. K. (2019). Keep the change: Embracing variability as a path to richer theoretical models of borderline personality disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 86(12), 879–880. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fineberg, S. K., Leavitt, J., Landry, C. D., Neustadter, E. S., Lesser, R. E., Stahl, D. S., … Corlett, P. R. (2018a). Individuals with borderline personality disorder show larger preferred social distance in live dyadic interactions. Psychiatry Research, 260, 384–390. 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.11.054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fineberg, S. K., Leavitt, J., Stahl, D. S., Kronemer, S., Landry, C. D., Alexander-Bloch, A., … Corlett, P. R. (2018b). Differential valuation and learning from social and nonsocial cues in borderline personality disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 84(11), 838–845. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.05.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fineberg, S. K., Stahl, D. S., & Corlett, P. R. (2017). Computational psychiatry in borderline personality disorder. Current Behavioral Neuroscience Reports, 4(1), 31–40. 10.1007/s40473-017-0104-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fineberg, S. K., Steinfeld, M., Brewer, J. A., & Corlett, P. R. (2014b). A computational account of borderline personality disorder: Impaired predictive learning about self and others through bodily simulation. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 5(AUG), 111. 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fiorillo, C. D., Newsome, W. T., & Schultz, W. (2008). The temporal precision of reward prediction in dopamine neurons. Nature Neuroscience, 11(8), 966–973. 10.1038/nn.2159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fontaine, N., & Viding, E. (2008). Genetics of personality disorders. Psychiatry, 7(3), 137–141. 10.1016/j.mppsy.2008.01.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Friedel, R. O. (2004). Dopamine dysfunction in borderline personality disorder: A hypothesis. Neuropsychopharmacology, 29(6), 1029–1039. 10.1038/sj.npp.1300424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston, K. (2005). A theory of cortical responses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 360(1456), 815–836. 10.1098/rstb.2005.1622. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston, K. (2008). Hierarchical models in the brain. PLoS Computational Biology, 4(11). 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston, K. J., Stephan, K. E., Montague, R., & Dolan, R. J. (2014). Computational psychiatry: The brain as a phantastic organ. The Lancet Psychiatry, 1(2), 148–158. 10.1016/S2215-0366(14)70275-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galea, J. M., Bestmann, S., Beigi, M., Jahanshahi, M., & Rothwell, J. C. (2012). Action reprogramming in Parkinson's disease: Response to prediction error is modulated by levels of dopamine. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(2), 542–550. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3621-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerrans, P. (2020). Pain asymbolia as depersonalization for pain experience. An interoceptive active inference account. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.523710. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert, R., Widom, C. S., Browne, K., Fergusson, D., Webb, E., & Janson, S. (2009). Burden and consequences of child maltreatment in high-income countries. The Lancet, 373(9657), 68–81. 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)61706-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilead, M., Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2020). Above and beyond the concrete: The diverse representational substrates of the predictive brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 43. 10.1017/S0140525X19002000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman, M., New, A., & Siever, L. (2004). Trauma, genes, and the neurobiology of personality disorders. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1032, 104–116. 10.1196/annals.1314.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon, N., Tsuchiya, N., Koenig-Robert, R., & Hohwy, J. (2019). Expectation and attention increase the integration of top-down and bottom-up signals in perception through different pathways. PLoS Biology, 17(4). 10.1371/journal.pbio.3000233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenberg, T., Chase, H. W., Almeida, J. R., Stiffler, R., Zevallos, C. R., Aslam, H. A., … Phillips, M. L. (2015). Moderation of the relationship between reward expectancy and prediction error-related ventral striatal reactivity by anhedonia in unmedicated major depressive disorder: Findings from the EMBARC study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 172(9), 881–891. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.14050594. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gu, X., Hof, P. R., Friston, K. J., & Fan, J. (2013). Anterior insular cortex and emotional awareness. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 521(15), 3371–3388. 10.1002/cne.23368. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gugerty, L., & Link, D. M. (2020). How heuristic credibility cues affect credibility judgments and decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 26(4), 620–645. 10.1037/xap0000279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 5–25. 10.1007/s10803-005-0039-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hart, H., & Rubia, K. (2012). Neuroimaging of child abuse: A critical review. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(52), 1–24. 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hazlett, E. A. (2016). Neural substrates of emotion-processing abnormalities in borderline personality disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 79(2), 74–75. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.10.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heil, L., Kwisthout, J., van Pelt, S., van Rooij, I., & Bekkering, H. (2018). One wouldn't expect an expert bowler to hit only two pins: Hierarchical predictive processing of agent-caused events. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(12), 2643–2654. 10.1177/1747021817752102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hengartner, M. P., Ajdacic-Gross, V., Rodgers, S., Müller, M., & Rössler, W. (2013). Childhood adversity in association with personality disorder dimensions: New findings in an old debate. European Psychiatry, 28(8), 476–482. 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2013.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henningsen, P., Gündel, H., Kop, W. J., Löwe, B., Martin, A., Rief, W., … van den Bergh, O. (2018). Persistent physical symptoms as perceptual dysregulation: A neuropsychobehavioral model and its clinical implications. Psychosomatic Medicine, 80(5), 422–431. 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hernandez, A., Arntz, A., Gaviria, A. M., Labad, A., & Gutiérrez-Zotes, J. A. (2012). Relationships between childhood maltreatment, parenting style, and borderline personality disorder criteria. Journal of Personality Disorders, 26(5), 727–736. 10.1521/pedi.2012.26.5.727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herpertz, S. C., & Bertsch, K. (2015). A new perspective on the pathophysiology of borderline personality disorder: A model of the role of oxytocin. American Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 840–851. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.15020216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hertz, U., Blakemore, C., & Frith, C. D. (2020). I haven't a clue! Expectations based on repetitions and hints facilitate perceptual experience of ambiguous images. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 46(8), 831–846. 10.1037/xhp0000749. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herzog, P., Feldmann, M., Voderholzer, U., Gärtner, T., Armbrust, M., Rauh, E., … Brakemeier, E. L. (2020). Drawing the borderline: Predicting treatment outcomes in patients with borderline personality disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 133. 10.1016/j.brat.2020.103692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hohwy, J. (2014). The predictive mind. In Hohwy J (Ed.), The predictive mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199682737.001.0001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hughes, C. D., & Rizvi, S. L. (2019). Biases in affective forecasting and recall as a function of borderline personality disorder features. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 38(3), 200–223. 10.1521/jscp.2019.38.3.200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hutto, D. D. (2020). The cost of over-intellectualizing the free-energy principle. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 43. 10.1017/S0140525X19002851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ibrahim, J., Cosgrave, N., & Woolgar, M. (2018). Childhood maltreatment and its link to borderline personality disorder features in children: A systematic review approach. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 23(1), 57–76. 10.1177/1359104517712778. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Igarashi, H., Hasui, C., Uji, M., Shono, M., Nagata, T., & Kitamura, T. (2010). Effects of child abuse history on borderline personality traits, negative life events, and depression: A study among a university student population in Japan. Psychiatry Research, 180(2–3), 120–125. 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.04.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iglesias, S., Mathys, C., Brodersen, K. H., Kasper, L., Piccirelli, M., denOuden, H. E. M., & Stephan, K. E. (2013). Hierarchical prediction errors in midbrain and basal forebrain during sensory learning. Neuron, 80(2), 519–530. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jang, K. L., Livesley, W. J., Vernon, P. A., & Jackson, D. N. (1996). Heritability of personality disorder traits: A twin study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 94(6), 438–444. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1996.tb09887.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Janssen, C. W., Lowry, C. A., Mehl, M. R., Allen, J. J. B., Kelly, K. L., Gartner, D. E., … Raison, C. L. (2016). Whole-body hyperthermia for the treatment of major depressive disorder: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry, 73(8), 789–795. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.1031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jedd, K., Hunt, R. H., Cicchetti, D., Hunt, E., Cowell, R. A., Rogosch, F. A., … Thomas, K. M. (2015). Long-term consequences of childhood maltreatment: Altered amygdala functional connectivity. Development and Psychopathology, 27(4 0 2), 1577–1589. 10.1017/S0954579415000954. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jeter, P. E., Slutsky, J., Singh, N., & Khalsa, S. B. S. (2015). Yoga as a therapeutic intervention: A bibliometric analysis of published research studies from 1967 to 2013. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 21(10), 586–592. 10.1089/acm.2015.0057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joffily, M., & Coricelli, G. (2013). Emotional valence and the free-energy principle. PLoS Computational Biology, 9(6), e1003094. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joyce, P. R., Stephenson, J., Kennedy, M., Mulder, R. T., & McHugh, P. C. (2013). The presence of both serotonin 1A receptor (HTR1A) and dopamine transporter (DAT1) gene variants increase the risk of borderline personality disorder. Frontiers in Genetics, 4(JAN). 10.3389/fgene.2013.00313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanai, R., Komura, Y., Shipp, S., & Friston, K. (2015). Cerebral hierarchies: Predictive processing, precision and the pulvinar. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 370(1668), 20140169. 10.1098/rstb.2014.0169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kayhan, E., Meyer, M., O'Reilly, J. X., Hunnius, S., & Bekkering, H. (2019). Nine-month-old infants update their predictive models of a changing environment. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 38. 10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100680. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khadka, S., Narayanan, B., Meda, S. A., Gelernter, J., Han, S., Sawyer, B., … Pearlson, G. D. (2014). Genetic association of impulsivity in young adults: A multivariate study. Translational Psychiatry, 4(9), e451–e451. 10.1038/tp.2014.95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiverstein, J., Miller, M., & Rietveld, E. (2020). How mood tunes prediction: A neurophenomenological account of mood and its disturbance in major depression. Neuroscience of Consciousness, 2020(1). 10.1093/nc/niaa003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kleindienst, N., Vonderlin, R., Bohus, M., Lis, S. (2020). Childhood adversity and borderline personality disorder. Analyses complementing the meta-analysis by Porter et al. (2020). Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 143(2), 183–184. 10.1111/acps.13256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kluczniok, D., Dittrich, K., Hindi Attar, C., Bödeker, K., Roth, M., Jaite, C., … Bermpohl, F. (2019). Oxytocin and maltreatment potential: Influence of maternal depression, borderline personality disorder and experience of early childhood maltreatment. Nervenarzt, 90(3), 267–276. 10.1007/s00115-019-0688-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Korn, C. W., la Rosée, L., Heekeren, H. R., & Roepke, S. (2016a). Social feedback processing in borderline personality disorder. Psychological Medicine, 46(3), 575–587. 10.1017/S003329171500207X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Korn, C. W., la Rosée, L., Heekeren, H. R., & Roepke, S. (2016b). Processing of information about future life events in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 246, 719–724. 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.07.067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Korn, C. W., Prehn, K., Park, S. Q., Walter, H., & Heekeren, H. R. (2012). Positively biased processing of self-relevant social feedback. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(47), 16832–16844. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3016-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Köster, M., Kayhan, E., Langeloh, M., & Hoehl, S. (2020). Making sense of the world: Infant learning from a predictive processing perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(3), 562–571. 10.1177/1745691619895071. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Köster, M., Langeloh, M., & Hoehl, S. (2019). Visually entrained theta oscillations increase for unexpected events in the infant brain. Psychological Science, 30(11), 1656–1663. 10.1177/0956797619876260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kox, M., Van Eijk, L. T., Zwaag, J., Van Den Wildenberg, J., Sweep, F. C. G. J., Van Der Hoeven, J. G., & Pickkers, P. (2014). Voluntary activation of the sympathetic nervous system and attenuation of the innate immune response in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(20), 7379–7384. 10.1073/pnas.1322174111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kraus, A., Valerius, G., Seifritz, E., Ruf, M., Bremner, J. D., Bohus, M., & Schmahl, C. (2010). Script-driven imagery of self-injurious behavior in patients with borderline personality disorder: A pilot FMRI study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 121(1), 41–51. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01417.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krause-Utz, A., Erol, E., Brousianou, A. V., Cackowski, S., Paret, C., Ende, G., & Elzinga, B. (2019). Self-reported impulsivity in women with borderline personality disorder: The role of childhood maltreatment severity and emotion regulation difficulties. Borderline Personality Disorder and Emotion Dysregulation, 6(1). 10.1186/s40479-019-0101-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krause-Utz, A., Winter, D., Niedtfeld, I., & Schmahl, C. (2014). The latest neuroimaging findings in borderline personality disorder topical collection on personality disorders. Current Psychiatry Reports, 16(3). 10.1007/s11920-014-0438-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krupnik, V. (2020). Trauma or drama: A predictive processing perspective on the continuum of stress. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kube, T., Berg, M., Kleim, B., & Herzog, P. (2020a). Rethinking post-traumatic stress disorder – a predictive processing perspective. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 113(April), 448–460. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.04.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kube, T., & Rozenkrantz, L. (2021). When beliefs face reality: An integrative review of belief updating in mental health and illness. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(2), 247–274. 10.1177/1745691620931496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kube, T., Rozenkrantz, L., Rief, W., & Barsky, A. (2020b). Understanding persistent physical symptoms: Conceptual integration of psychological expectation models and predictive processing accounts. Clinical Psychology Review, 76. 10.1016/j.cpr.2020.101829. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kube, T., Schwarting, R., Rozenkrantz, L., Glombiewski, J. A., & Rief, W. (2020). Distorted cognitive processes in major depression – A predictive processing perspective. Biological Psychiatry, 87(5), 388–398. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.07.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Langeloh, M., Buttelmann, D., Pauen, S., & Hoehl, S. (2020). 12- to 14-month-olds expect unconstrained agents to act efficiently: Event-related potential (ERP) evidence from the head-touch paradigm. Developmental Psychology, 56(7), 1252–1267. 10.1037/dev0000934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laporte, L., Paris, J., Guttman, H., Russell, J., & Correa, J. A. (2012). Using a sibling design to compare childhood adversities in female patients with BPD and their sisters. Child Maltreatment, 17(4), 318–329. 10.1177/1077559512461173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lawson, R. P., Rees, G., & Friston, K. J. (2014). An aberrant precision account of autism. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8(MAY), 302. 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leahy, R. L. (2017). Cognitive therapy techniques: A practitioner's guide. New York, NY, USA: The Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Liebke, L., Koppe, G., Bungert, M., Thome, J., Hauschild, S., Defiebre, N., … Lis, S. (2018). Difficulties with being socially accepted: An experimental study in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 127(7), 670–682. 10.1037/abn0000373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lim, L., Radua, J., & Rubia, K. (2014). Gray matter abnormalities in childhood maltreatment: A voxelwise metaanalysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 171(8), 854–863. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13101427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive behavioral therapy of borderline personality disorder. New York: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Linson, A., & Friston, K. (2019). Reframing PTSD for computational psychiatry with the active inference framework. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 24(5), 347–368. 10.1080/13546805.2019.1665994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linson, A., Parr, T., & Friston, K. J. (2020). Active inference, stressors, and psychological trauma: A neuroethological model of (mal)adaptive explore-exploit dynamics in ecological context. Behavioural Brain Research, 380. 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.112421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Litwin, P., & Miłkowski, M. (2020). Unification by fiat: Arrested development of predictive processing. Cognitive Science, 44(7). 10.1111/cogs.12867. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Livesley, W. J., Jang, K. L., Jackson, D. N., & Vernon, P. A. (1993). Genetic and environmental contributions to dimensions of personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150(12), 1826–1831. 10.1176/ajp.150.12.1826. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lobbestael, J., Arntz, A., & Bernstein, D. P. (2010). Disentangling the relationship between different types of childhood maltreatment and personality disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 24(3), 285–295. 10.1521/pedi.2010.24.3.285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lyndon, S., & Corlett, P. R. (2020). Hallucinations in posttraumatic stress disorder: Insights from predictive coding. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 129(6), 534–543. 10.1037/abn0000531. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mccall, C. (2016). Mapping social interactions: The science of proxemics. In M. Wöhr & S. Krach (Eds.), Current topics in behavioral neurosciences (Vol. 30, pp. 295–308). Cham: Springer Verlag. 10.1007/7854_2015_431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCrory, E. J., Gerin, M. I., & Viding, E. (2017). Annual Research Review: Childhood maltreatment, latent vulnerability and the shift to preventative psychiatry – the contribution of functional brain imaging. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(4), 338–357. 10.1111/jcpp.12713. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miano, A., Fertuck, E. A., Roepke, S., & Dziobek, I. (2017). Romantic relationship dysfunction in borderline personality disorder-A naturalistic approach to trustworthiness perception. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 8(3), 281–286. 10.1037/per0000196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell, A. E., Dickens, G. L., & Picchioni, M. M. (2014). Facial emotion processing in borderline personality disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychology Review, 24, 166–184. 10.1007/s11065-014-9254-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morandotti, N., Dima, D., Jogia, J., Frangou, S., Sala, M., de Vidovich, G. Z., … Brambilla, P. (2013). Childhood abuse is associated with structural impairment in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and aggressiveness in patients with borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Research – Neuroimaging, 213(1), 18–23. 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Musser, N., Zalewski, M., Stepp, S., & Lewis, J. (2018). A systematic review of negative parenting practices predicting borderline personality disorder: Are we measuring biosocial theory's ‘invalidating environment’? Clinical Psychology Review, 65, 1–16. 10.1016/j.cpr.2018.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nemoda, Z., Lyons-Ruth, K., Szekely, A., Bertha, E., Faludi, G., & Sasvari-Szekely, M. (2010). Association between dopaminergic polymorphisms and borderline personality traits among at-risk young adults and psychiatric inpatients. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 6. 10.1186/1744-9081-6-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niki, H., & Watanabe, M. (1979). Prefrontal and cingulate unit activity during timing behavior in the monkey. Brain Research, 171(2), 213–224. 10.1016/0006-8993(79)90328-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nishijo, H., Ono, T., & Nishino, H. (1988). Single neuron responses in amygdala of alert monkey during complex sensory stimulation with affective significance. Journal of Neuroscience, 8(10), 3570–3583. 10.1523/jneurosci.08-10-03570.1988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ogata, S. N., Silk, K. R., Goodrich, S., Lohr, N. E., Westen, D., & Hill, E. M. (1990). Childhood sexual and physical abuse in adult patients with borderline personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147(8), 1008–1013. 10.1176/ajp.147.8.1008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oliva, F., Dalmotto, M., Pirfo, E., Furlan, P. M., & Picci, R. L. (2014). A comparison of thought and perception disorders in borderline personality disorder and schizophrenia: Psychotic experiences as a reaction to impaired social functioning. BMC Psychiatry, 14(1). 10.1186/s12888-014-0239-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oquendo, M. A., & Mann, J. J. (2000). The biology of impulsivity and suicidality. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 23(1), 11–25. 10.1016/S0193-953X(05)70140-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paquola, C., Bennett, M. R., & Lagopoulos, J. (2016). Understanding heterogeneity in grey matter research of adults with childhood maltreatment – a meta-analysis and review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 69, 299–312. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parr, T., & Friston, K. J. (2017). Working memory, attention, and salience in active inference. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1–21. 10.1038/s41598-017-15249-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parr, T., & Friston, K. J. (2018). The anatomy of inference: Generative models and brain structure. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience, 12, 90. 10.3389/fncom.2018.00090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paulus, M. P., Feinstein, J. S., & Khalsa, S. S. (2019). An active inference approach to interoceptive psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 15(1), 97–122. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050718-095617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pellicano, E., & Burr, D. (2012). When the world becomes ‘too real’: A Bayesian explanation of autistic perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(10), 504–510. 10.1016/j.tics.2012.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pietrek, C., Elbert, T., Weierstall, R., Müller, O., & Rockstroh, B. (2013). Childhood adversities in relation to psychiatric disorders. Psychiatry Research, 206(1), 103–110. 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pilditch, T. D., Madsen, J. K., & Custers, R. (2020). False prophets and Cassandra's curse: The role of credibility in belief updating. Acta Psychologica, 202. 10.1016/j.actpsy.2019.102956. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Porter, C., Palmier-Claus, J., Branitsky, A., Mansell, W., Warwick, H., & Varese, F. (2020). Childhood adversity and borderline personality disorder: A meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 141(1), 6–20. 10.1111/acps.13118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Powers, A. R., Mathys, C., & Corlett, P. R. (2017). Pavlovian conditioning–induced hallucinations result from overweighting of perceptual priors. Science (New York, N.Y.), 357(6351), 596–600. 10.1126/science.aan3458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prados, J., Stenz, L., Courtet, P., Prada, P., Nicastro, R., Adouan, W., … Perroud, N. (2015). Borderline personality disorder and childhood maltreatment: A genome-wide methylation analysis. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 14(2), 177–188. 10.1111/gbb.12197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quenneville, A. F., Kalogeropoulou, E., Küng, A. L., Hasler, R., Nicastro, R., Prada, P., & Perroud, N. (2020). Childhood maltreatment, anxiety disorders and outcome in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 284. 10.1016/j.psychres.2019.112688. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radtke, K. M., Schauer, M., Gunter, H. M., Ruf-Leuschner, M., Sill, J., Meyer, A., & Elbert, T. (2015). Epigenetic modifications of the glucocorticoid receptor gene are associated with the vulnerability to psychopathology in childhood maltreatment. Translational Psychiatry, 5(5). 10.1038/tp.2015.63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramamoorthy, N., Parker, M., Plaisted-Grant, K., Muhl-Richardson, A., & Davis, G. (2020). Attention neglects a stare-in-the-crowd: Unanticipated consequences of prediction-error coding. Cognition. 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rapaport, M. H., Schettler, P., Larson, E. R., Edwards, S. A., Dunlop, B. W., Rakofsky, J. J., & Kinkead, B. (2016). Acute Swedish massage monotherapy successfully remediates symptoms of generalized anxiety disorder: A proof-of-concept, randomized controlled study. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 77(7), e883–e891. 10.4088/JCP.15m10151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reed, E. J., Uddenberg, S., Suthaharan, P., Mathys, C. D., Taylor, J. R., Groman, S. M., & Corlett, P. R. (2020). Paranoia as a deficit in non-social belief updating. ELife, 9, 1–55. 10.7554/eLife.56345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reinhard, M. A., Dewald-Kaufmann, J., Wüstenberg, T., Musil, R., Barton, B. B., Jobst, A., & Padberg, F. (2020). The vicious circle of social exclusion and psychopathology: A systematic review of experimental ostracism research in psychiatric disorders. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 270(5), 521–532. 10.1007/s00406-019-01074-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rilling, J. K., & Sanfey, A. G. (2011). The neuroscience of social decision-making. Annual Review of Psychology, 62(1), 23–48. 10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodrigues, E., Wenzel, A., Ribeiro, M. P., Quarantini, L. C., Miranda-Scippa, A., de Sena, E. P., & De Oliveira, I. R. (2011). Hippocampal volume in borderline personality disorder with and without comorbid posttraumatic stress disorder: A meta-analysis. European Psychiatry, 26(7), 452–456. 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2010.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruocco, A. C., Amirthavasagam, S., Choi-Kain, L. W., & McMain, S. F. (2013). Neural correlates of negative emotionality in borderline personality disorder: An activation-likelihood-estimation meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 73(2), 153–160. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.07.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruocco, A. C., Amirthavasagam, S., & Zakzanis, K. K. (2012). Amygdala and hippocampal volume reductions as candidate endophenotypes for borderline personality disorder: A meta-analysis of magnetic resonance imaging studies. Psychiatry Research – Neuroimaging, 201(3), 245–252. 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanders, M. R. (1999). Triple p-positive parenting program: Towards an empirically validated multilevel parenting and family support strategy for the prevention of behavior and emotional problems in children. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 2(2), 71–90. 10.1023/A:1021843613840. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanders, M. R. (2008). Triple p-Positive Parenting Program as a public health approach to strengthening parenting. Journal of Family Psychology, 22(4), 506–517. 10.1037/0893-3200.22.3.506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanders, M. R. (2012). Development, evaluation, and multinational dissemination of the Triple p-Positive Parenting Program. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8, 345–379. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Santangelo, P. S., Koenig, J., Funke, V., Parzer, P., Resch, F., Ebner-Priemer, U. W., & Kaess, M. (2017). Ecological momentary assessment of affective and interpersonal instability in adolescent non-suicidal self-injury. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 45(7), 1429–1438. 10.1007/s10802-016-0249-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scala, J. W., Levy, K. N., Johnson, B. N., Kivity, Y., Ellison, W. D., Pincus, A. L., … Newman, M. G. (2018). The role of negative affect and self-concept clarity in predicting self-injurious urges in borderline personality disorder using ecological momentary assessment. Journal of Personality Disorders, 32(Suppl), 36–57. 10.1521/pedi.2018.32.supp.36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scheffers, F., van Vugt, E., Lanctôt, N., & Lemieux, A. (2019). Experiences of (young) women after out of home placement: An examination of personality disorder symptoms through the lens of child maltreatment. Child Abuse and Neglect, 92, 116–125. 10.1016/j.chiabu.2019.03.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schenk, L. A., Sprenger, C., Onat, S., Colloca, L., & Büchel, C. (2017). Suppression of striatal prediction errors by the prefrontal cortex in placebo hypoalgesia. Journal of Neuroscience, 37(40), 9715–9723. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1101-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scherpiet, S., Brühl, A. B., Opialla, S., Roth, L., Jäncke, L., & Herwig, U. (2014). Altered emotion processing circuits during the anticipation of emotional stimuli in women with borderline personality disorder. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 264(1), 45–60. 10.1007/s00406-013-0444-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmahl, C. G., Vermetten, E., Elzinga, B. M., & Bremner, J. D. (2004). A positron emission tomography study of memories of childhood abuse in borderline personality disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 55(7), 759–765. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz, W., & Dickinson, A. (2000). Neuronal coding of prediction errors. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23(1), 473–500. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schulze, L., Schmahl, C., & Niedtfeld, I. (2016). Neural correlates of disturbed emotion processing in borderline personality disorder: A multimodal meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 79(2), 97–106. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.03.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwartenbeck, P., FitzGerald, T. H. B., Mathys, C., Dolan, R., & Friston, K. (2015). The dopaminergic midbrain encodes the expected certainty about desired outcomes. Cerebral Cortex, 25(10), 3434–3445. 10.1093/cercor/bhu159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seth, A. K., & Friston, K. J. (2016). Active interoceptive inference and the emotional brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1708). 10.1098/rstb.2016.0007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seth, A. K., Millidge, B., Buckley, C. L., & Tschantz, A. (2020). Curious inferences: Reply to Sun and firestone on the dark room problem. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(9), 681–683. 10.1016/j.tics.2020.05.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sharot, T., Guitart-Masip, M., Korn, C. W., Chowdhury, R., & Dolan, R. J. (2012). How dopamine enhances an optimism bias in humans. Current Biology, 22(16), 1477–1481. 10.1016/j.cub.2012.05.053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shipp, S. (2016). Neural elements for predictive coding. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(NOV), 1792. 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01792. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siegel, J. Z., Curwell-Parry, O., Pearce, S., Saunders, K. E. A., & Crockett, M. J. (2020). A computational phenotype of disrupted moral inference in borderline personality disorder. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 5(12), 1134–1141. 10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.07.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skoglund, C., Tiger, A., Rück, C., Petrovic, P., Asherson, P., Hellner, C., … Kuja-Halkola, R. (2021). Familial risk and heritability of diagnosed borderline personality disorder: A register study of the Swedish population. Molecular Psychiatry, 26(3), 999–1008. 10.1038/s41380-019-0442-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith, R., Kirlic, N., Stewart, J., Touthang, J., Kuplicki, R., Khalsa, S., … Aupperle, R. (2020). Greater decision uncertainty characterizes a transdiagnostic patient sample during approach-avoidance conflict: A computational modeling approach. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience: JPN, 46(1), 200032. 10.31234/osf.io/t2dhn. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith, R., Lane, R. D., Parr, T., & Friston, K. J. (2019a). Neurocomputational mechanisms underlying emotional awareness: Insights afforded by deep active inference and their potential clinical relevance. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 107, 473–491. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith, R., Parr, T., & Friston, K. J. (2019b). Simulating emotions: An active inference model of emotional state inference and emotion concept learning. Frontiers in Psychology, 10. 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02844. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith, R., Steklis, H. D., Steklis, N., Weihs, K. L., Allen, J. J., & Lane, R. D.. (2022). Lower emotional awareness is associated with greater early adversity and faster life history strategy. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences. Advance online publication. 10.31234/OSF.IO/7NZQK. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Smits, J. A. J., Berry, A. C., Tart, C. D., & Powers, M. B. (2008). The efficacy of cognitive-behavioral interventions for reducing anxiety sensitivity: A meta-analytic review. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46(9), 1047–1054. 10.1016/j.brat.2008.06.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smout, C. A., Tang, M. F., Garrido, M. I., & Mattingley, J. B. (2019). Attention promotes the neural encoding of prediction errors. PLoS Biology, 17(2). 10.1371/journal.pbio.2006812. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sokolov, E. N. (1963). Perception and the conditioned reflex. Oxford: Pergamon Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sokolov, E. N. (1990). The orienting response, and future directions of its development. The Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science, 25(3), 142–150. 10.1007/BF02974268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steele, H., & Siever, L. (2010). An attachment perspective on borderline personality disorder: Advances in gene-environment considerations. Current Psychiatry Reports, 12(1), 61–67. 10.1007/s11920-009-0091-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steele, K. R., Townsend, M. L., & Grenyer, B. F. S. (2019). Parenting and personality disorder: An overview and meta-synthesis of systematic reviews. PLoS ONE, 14(10). 10.1371/journal.pone.0223038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stepp, S. D., Lazarus, S. A., & Byrd, A. L. (2016). A systematic review of risk factors prospectively associated with borderline personality disorder: Taking stock and moving forward. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 7(4), 316–323. 10.1037/per0000186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stepp, S. D., Scott, L. N., Jones, N. P., Whalen, D. J., & Hipwell, A. E. (2015). Negative emotional reactivity as a marker of vulnerability in the development of borderline personality disorder symptoms. Development and Psychopathology, 28(1), 213–224. 10.1017/S0954579415000395. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sterzer, P., Adams, R. A., Fletcher, P., Frith, C., Lawrie, S. M., Muckli, L., … Corlett, P. R. (2018). The predictive coding account of psychosis. Biological Psychiatry, 84(9), 634–643. 10.1016/J.BIOPSYCH.2018.05.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Storebø, O. J., Stoffers-Winterling, J. M., Völlm, B. A., Kongerslev, M. T., Mattivi, J. T., Jørgensen, M. S., … Simonsen, E. (2020). Psychological therapies for people with borderline personality disorder. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (5). 10.1002/14651858.CD012955.pub2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Struck, N., Krug, A., Yuksel, D., Stein, F., Schmitt, S., Meller, T., … Brakemeier, E. L. (2020). Childhood maltreatment and adult mental disorders – the prevalence of different types of maltreatment and associations with age of onset and severity of symptoms. Psychiatry Research, 293, 113398. 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sun, Z., & Firestone, C. (2020). The dark room problem. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24, 346–348. 10.1016/j.tics.2020.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teicher, M. H., Anderson, C. M., & Polcari, A. (2012). Childhood maltreatment is associated with reduced volume in the hippocampal subfields CA3, dentate gyrus, and subiculum. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(9), E563–E572. 10.1073/pnas.1115396109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teicher, M. H., & Samson, J. A. (2016). Annual Research Review: Enduring neurobiological effects of childhood abuse and neglect. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(3), 241–266. 10.1111/jcpp.12507. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teicher, M. H., Samson, J. A., Anderson, C. M., & Ohashi, K. (2016). The effects of childhood maltreatment on brain structure, function and connectivity. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(10), 652–666. 10.1038/nrn.2016.111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tzieropoulos, H. (2013). The trust game in neuroscience: A short review. Social Neuroscience, 8(5), 407–416. 10.1080/17470919.2013.832375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Unoka, Z., Seres, I., Áspán, N., Bódi, N., & Kéri, S. (2009). Trust game reveals restricted interpersonal transactions in patients with borderline personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 23(4), 399–409. 10.1521/pedi.2009.23.4.399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vai, B., Sforzini, L., Visintini, R., Riberto, M., Bulgarelli, C., Ghiglino, D., … Benedetti, F. (2018). Corticolimbic connectivity mediates the relationship between adverse childhood experiences and symptom severity in borderline personality disorder. Neuropsychobiology, 76(2), 105–115. 10.1159/000487961. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van de Cruys, S., Evers, K., van der Hallen, R., van Eylen, L., Boets, B., de-Wit, L., & Wagemans, J. (2014). Precise minds in uncertain worlds: Predictive coding in autism. Psychological Review, 121(4), 649–675. 10.1037/a0037665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van de Cruys, S., Friston, K. J., & Clark, A. (2020). Controlled optimism: Reply to Sun and firestone on the dark room problem. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(9), 680–681. 10.1016/j.tics.2020.05.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van den Bergh, O., Witthöft, M., Petersen, S., & Brown, R. J. (2017). Symptoms and the body: Taking the inferential leap. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 74(Pt A), 185–203. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Schie, C. C., Chiu, C. D., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., Heiser, W. J., & Elzinga, B. M. (2020). Stuck in a negative me: FMRI study on the role of disturbed self-views in social feedback processing in borderline personality disorder. Psychological Medicine, 50(4), 625–635. 10.1017/S0033291719000448. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vansteelandt, K., Houben, M., Claes, L., Berens, A., Sleuwaegen, E., Sienaert, P., & Kuppens, P. (2017). The affect stabilization function of nonsuicidal self injury in borderline personality disorder: An ecological momentary assessment study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 92, 41–50. 10.1016/j.brat.2017.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vilas, M. G., Melloni, L., & Melloni, L. (2020). A challenge for predictive coding: Representational or experiential diversity? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 43. 10.1017/S0140525X19003157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wager, T. D., Kang, J., Johnson, T. D., Nichols, T. E., Satpute, A. B., & Barrett, L. F. (2015). A Bayesian model of category-specific emotional brain responses. PLOS Computational Biology, 11(4), e1004066. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watanabe, M. (1989). The appropriateness of behavioral responses coded in post-trial activity of primate prefrontal units. Neuroscience Letters, 101(1), 113–117. 10.1016/0304-3940(89)90450-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weaver, T. L., & Clum, G. A. (1993). Early family environments and traumatic experiences associated with borderline personality disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61(6), 1068–1075. 10.1037/0022-006x.61.6.1068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Widom, C. S., Czaja, S. J., & Paris, J. (2009). A prospective investigation of borderline personality disorder in abused and neglected children followed up into adulthood. Journal of Personality Disorders, 23(5), 433–446. 10.1521/pedi.2009.23.5.433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilkinson, S., Dodgson, G., & Meares, K. (2017). Predictive processing and the varieties of psychological trauma. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(OCT), 1840. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01840. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winsper, C. (2018). The aetiology of borderline personality disorder (BPD). Current Opinion in Psychology, 21, 105–110. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winsper, C., Marwaha, S., Lereya, S. T., Thompson, A., Eyden, J., & Singh, S. P. (2016). A systematic review of the neurobiological underpinnings of borderline personality disorder (BPD) in childhood and adolescence. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 27(8), 827–847. 10.1515/revneuro-2016-0026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolpe, J. (1987). Carbon dioxide inhalation treatments of neurotic anxiety: An overview. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175(3), 129–133. 10.1097/00005053-198703000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wota, A. P., Byrne, C., Murray, I., Ofuafor, T., Nisar, Z., Neuner, F., & Hallahan, B. P. (2014). An examination of childhood trauma in individuals attending an adult mental health service. Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine, 31(4), 259–270. 10.1017/ipm.2014.49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yoshimura, S., & Hashimoto, Y. (2020). The effect of induced optimism on the optimistic update bias. BMC Psychology, 8(1). 10.1186/s40359-020-0389-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., Wedig, M. M., & Fitzmaurice, G. M. (2013). Cognitive experiences reported by patients with borderline personality disorder and axis ii comparison subjects: A 16-year prospective follow-up study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 170(6), 671–679. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13010055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang, T. H., Chow, A., Wang, L. L., Yu, J. H., Dai, Y. F., Lu, X., … Xiao, Z. P. (2013). Childhood maltreatment profile in a clinical population in China: A further analysis with existing data of an epidemiologic survey. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 54(7), 856–864. 10.1016/j.comppsych.2013.03.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291722002458.