Abstract
A significant proportion of the human neurotypical population exhibits some degree of sensory eye dominance (SED), referring to the brain’s preferential processing of one eye’s input versus another. The neural substrates underlying this functional imbalance are not well known. Here, we investigated the relationship between visual white matter tract properties and SED in the human neurotypical population. Observers’ performance on two commonly used dichoptic tasks were used to index SED, along with performance on a third task to address a functional implication of binocular imbalance: stereovision. We show that diffusivity metrics of the optic radiations (ORs) well predict behavioral SED metrics. We found no relationship between SED and stereosensitivity. Our data suggest that SED is not simply reflected by gray matter structural and functional alterations, as often suggested, but relates, at least in part to the microstructural properties of thalamocortical white matter.
Keywords: binocular vision, eye dominance, imaging, stereovision, white matter
Significance Statement
Sensory eye dominance (SED) is a prominent feature of certain clinical populations (i.e., amblyopia). Binocularly neurotypical individuals also have varying extents of SED, with a significant minority exhibiting strong dominance for one eye. We show here that SED in neurotypical individuals is well-predicted by white matter microstructural properties of the optic radiations (ORs). Identifying the neural loci of binocular visual mechanisms allows for targeted paradigms to be developed for shifting eye dominance in the visually impaired.
Introduction
The visual system relies on integrating information from both eyes to carry out binocular functions such as stereopsis. Information received by the brain via each eye may not be processed with the same weighting, however. Sensory eye dominance (SED) is typically presented as a product of neuroatypical visual development, leading to physiological alterations that impair vision in the weak eye and a tendency of the brain to prefer information obtained by the strong eye (McKee et al., 2003; Levi, 2020). While SED is a prominent feature of certain clinical groups (e.g., amblyopes), SED also exists in a varying extents among binocularly neurotypical individuals, with a significant minority (∼39%) exhibiting strong dominance (Li et al., 2010; Yang et al., 2010).
The mechanistic origins of SED remain difficult to unravel as it has been indexed using behavioral measures involving a variety of visual features and task types. SED is commonly indexed by binocular phase combination (Ding and Sperling, 2006; Zhou et al., 2013), binocular rivalry (Levelt, 1965; Blake, 1989), and more recently, dichoptic signal-in-noise (SNR) tasks (Li et al., 2010; Kam and Chang, 2021). Binocular phase combination tasks present slightly different gratings to each eye and typically allow the observer to adjust the phase, or contrast of the inputs, thereby revealing the relative contribution of each eye’s input to the fused percept (i.e., by indexing a contrast ratio and/or relative phase shift between the two eyes). Binocular rivalry reflects the competition between the two eyes’ signals for visual awareness, quantified by the duration of periods of dominance for each incompatible half-image. Dichoptic signal-in-noise tasks require observers to suppress noise information presented to one eye and extract useful information (signal) presented to the other eye. The dichoptic motion SNR task appears to be a sensitive metric for measuring changes in SED after perceptual training (Kam and Chang, 2021). Although these tasks purportedly all reflect SED, it is unclear as to whether they reflect the same mechanisms.
Models of binocular interactions based on data obtained using binocular phase combination protocols have proposed two stages of binocular contrast gain control: an early stage of interocular suppression (Ding and Sperling, 2006; Baker and Meese, 2007), followed by a second stage of contrast gain control postsummation. These models imply that SED is mediated by processes across a distributed intercortical network. This notion is somewhat consistent with neurophysiological data obtained while using rivalrous stimuli that have shown fMRI responses in the primary visual cortex (V1) and the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) to closely couple with perceptual switches (Polonsky et al., 2000; Haynes et al., 2005; Moutoussis et al., 2005; Wunderlich et al., 2005). Further upstream, it has been shown that short-term monocular deprivation in healthy adults appears to modulate activity in V1, as well as in extrastriate visual areas V3 and V4, within which neurons are predominantly binocular (Binda et al., 2018).
While much of the focus on understanding the neural underpinnings of binocular function, and in particular, SED, have focused on gray matter function (Haynes et al., 2005; Wunderlich et al., 2005; Harauzov et al., 2010; H Xu et al., 2016; Binda et al., 2018; K Dougherty et al., 2019), it is entirely possible that SED arises from changes in the efficiency of signal transmission along the visual pathway (i.e., weakened white matter microstructural properties). A potential role of white matter microstructural properties to serving binocular functions has received very little attention, despite the bits and pieces of data available in the literature that suggest they deserve attention. Namely, the optic radiations (OR) between the LGN to V1, have been shown to exhibit greater diffusivity in patients with strabismic amblyopia as compared with healthy controls (Allen et al., 2014; Duan et al., 2015). In addition, left-right asymmetries in diffusion indices in the OR have been found in participants with monocular enucleation as well as in binocular control participants (when comparing contralateral versus ipsilateral tracts to the nondominant eye (Wong et al., 2018). Although each optic radiation does not strictly carry information from one eye only, physiological evidence suggests that V1 responses are largely monocular, and ocular “preference” is preserved beyond layer 4 (Hubel and Wiesel, 1968, 1972; Blasdel and Fitzpatrick, 1984; Douglas et al., 1989; Casagrande and Boyd, 1996). Preservation of eye dominance well beyond known sites of binocular combination could originate from differences in ganglion cell densities. Postmortem analyses on human retinae have revealed an asymmetry in ganglion cells densities, where nasal fibers (i.e., those that eventually decussate) are three times more dense than temporal fibers (Curcio and Allen, 1990). That is, beyond the chiasm then, and within each hemisphere, there is a physiological imbalance as to the white matter representations in terms of the eye of origin. Together, these data suggest the potential for differential monocularly-influenced (albeit not segregated) responses beyond the point of both fiber and gray-matter binocular combination (i.e., beyond the thalamus). Interestingly, while not reflecting eye dominance per se, we note that the posterior portion of the corpus callosum (occipital corpus callosum; OCC) which connects left and right visual cortices, and the vertical occipital fasciculus (VOF), the only currently known long-range tract connecting dorsal and ventral visual areas, have been recently shown to be related to stereoacuity (Oishi et al., 2018).
Here, we asked whether individual variabilities in SED in the neurotypical population can be explained by the microstructural properties of visual white matter tracts along the visual cascade by using diffusion-weighted imaging (DWI). We indexed SED by using two commonly employed tasks: the binocular phase combination and dichoptic motion SNR tasks. We included a third task, depth SNR task to measure depth sensitivity, as it is a functional product of binocular integration (Dieter et al., 2017; Han et al., 2018). Because of low signal integrity for images obtained close to the orbital socket (and nasal cavity), we measured tracts of interest that fell between the thalamic LGN to cortex but did not include the chiasm or optic nerves. Our tracts of interest included: left and right optic radiations, occipital corpus callosum, and left and right vertical occipital fasciculus. Tract tissue properties was indexed using fractional anisotropy (FA) and mean diffusivity (MD). FA quantifies coherence of water diffusion, where higher FA values indicate greater white matter microstructural properties in terms of axon diameter, myelination, etc. (Alexander et al., 2011). MD measures the average rate of water diffusion where lower MD values reflect greater tissue density and/or increased myelination. FA and MD values in visual white matter tracts have been shown to correlate with visual performance and they differ substantially between clinical, e.g., strabismic amblyopia and healthy populations (Thomas et al., 2009; de Blank et al., 2013; Duan et al., 2015; Oishi et al., 2018).
Materials and Methods
Participants
A total of 41 observers [mean age: 22.2 (18–28); 21 females] participated in this study. Our final recruitment size was determined via a power analysis following large effect sizes obtained by a previous study investigating the functional (stereoscopic) importance of visual white matter tracts (Oishi et al., 2018). All except for one (an author) were naive to the purpose of this study and all gave written consent in compliance with the study protocol approved by the ethics committee of the Human Research Ethics Committee, The University of Hong Kong. All observers were screened for MRI contraindications, and for neurotypical visual acuity (Sloan LogMar chart, 20/20–2), neurotypical binocular fusion (Worth four dot test) and neurotypical stereoacuity (Titmus test, ≤40 arcs).
Apparatus and general procedures
Stimuli were generated in MATLAB (The MathWorks) with extensions from the Psychophysics Toolbox (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997). Dichoptic presentation was achieved by using a 120-Hz monitor (27-inch, spatial resolution 1920 × 1080, VG278, ASUS), paired with shutter goggles (Nvidia 3D Vision 2). The experiment was performed in a darkened room. Observers’ heads were stabilized with a chinrest, 50 cm from display. The experiment spanned two sessions occurring on separate days. In the first sessions, observers were visually screened and completed all behavioral tasks. The order of the tasks was randomized. The second session constituted the MRI/DWI acquisition which was completed within three months from the date of behavioral testing.
Stimuli and tasks
Binocular phase combination
Two horizontal sine-wave gratings (3.6° × 5.3°) with opposite vertical phase shifts (±22.5°) at 100% contrast were dichoptically presented to the two eyes (Fig. 1A). Spatial frequency was fixed at 0.3 cycles per degree. In a given trial, observers first viewed a fixation cross surrounded by a binocularly presented frame to aid fusion. Observers then performed calibration during which each eye was presented with a fixation cross and two diagonally positioned dots (in opposite directions for each eye). By pressing the arrow keys, observers shifted the calibration stimuli to form a single fixation cross and four symmetrically positioned dots. When satisfied with alignment, observers were then instructed to press a key to view the main stimulus. The two horizontal sine-wave gratings were dichoptically presented along with a horizontal reference line, 0.04° in thickness. Observers used up and down arrow keys to place the reference line at the perceived center of the fused cyclopean percept (perceived phase) with a fixed step size of 0.04°, corresponding to 4° phase angle of the sinewave grating. The initial position of the reference line was randomly placed within ±2° from the midline on each trial. Observers had a maximum of 120 s to adjust the reference line.
Two configurations were used to avoid positional bias. The –22.5° phase shift grating was presented randomly to the right (Fig. 1A, configuration 1) or left (Fig. 1A, configuration 2) eye on each trial. Each configuration was measured 40 times, totaling 80 trials in each block. Each observer completed one block in ∼5–6 min. The final perceived phase ( ) is given by Equation 1. The center coordinates of the screen were defined as (0,0). Placing the reference line exactly at the center yields = 0°, thus = 0°, indicating perfectly balanced eyes. Placing the line above or below center gives negative or positive values, respectively. A negative denotes right-eye-dominance. We only included responses with reaction time larger than 300 ms to filter out possible mis-clicks that prematurely ended the trial. A total of 12 trials were removed from seven observers;
(1) |
Dichoptic global motion discrimination
In the two-alternative forced choice (2AFC) motion signal-in-noise (SNR) task, one eye was presented with signal dots, i.e., uniform motion direction, and the other eye was presented with noise dots, i.e., random motion direction. Signal and noise dots (0.2°, velocity 2°/s, lifetime 1 s) were composed of equal proportions of black and white dots. The stimulus was displayed on a uniform gray background with a binocularly presented frame of black and white squares (1.5°) to promote binocular fusion. Task difficulty was manipulated by changing the signal-to-noise ratio using the QUEST staircase procedure sampling thresholds at the 82%-correct level (Watson and Pelli, 1983). At 100% signal, all dots were coherently moving to the left or right. Signal dots were either presented to the right (Fig. 1B, configuration 1) or left eye (Fig. 1B, configuration 2). Each trial started with a 500-ms fixation, followed by a 500-ms presentation of the stimulus. Observers had a maximum of 1000 ms to indicate the net motion direction of the moving dots (left or right) by pressing the arrow keys. One block consisted of two randomly interleaved staircases (one per configuration), where each configuration was presented 60 times in a randomized order. Each participant completed two blocks lasting a total of ∼10 min. Motion coherence dominance ratio was given by Equation 2. We obtained the average thresholds for configurations 1 (Thresholdright) and 2 (Thresholdleft) from both blocks. A negative motion coherence dominance ratio indicated right-eye-dominance:
(2) |
Depth signal-in-noise task
In the depth task, stimuli were random dot stereograms (RDSs) presented on a mid-gray background with a grid of black and white squares (1.5°) designed to promote stable vergence (Fig. 1C). The RDS consisted of equal proportions of black and white dots (0.2°, density 12 dots/deg2) depicting a central target (4.5° in diameter) and a surrounding annulus (9° in diameter). Observers were asked to judge whether the central target was in front (“near,” configuration 1) or behind of (“far,” configuration 2) the surround by pressing one of two arrow keys. Dots on the target plane were assigned the same disparity of ±6 arcmin and noise dots were randomly positioned within ±12 arcmin. Task difficulty was manipulated by varying the signal-to-noise ratio using the QUEST staircase procedure sampling thresholds at the 82%-correct level. At 100% signal, all dots (signal and noise) had the same disparity. On a single trial, observers viewed the 500-ms fixation cross, followed by a 500-ms presentation of the stimulus. Observers were allotted a maximum response window of 1000 ms. Each observer completed two blocks and each block consisted of two interleaved staircases (60 trials per configuration). Stereosensitivity was indexed by averaging thresholds of both configurations from the two blocks.
MRI data acquisition and preprocessing
Neuroimaging data were acquired using a 3T Signa Premier (GE Healthcare) equipped with a 48-channel head coil at The University of Hong Kong. We collected an anatomic T1-weighted MP-RAGE image at 1-mm3 spatial resolution (TR = 700 ms, TE = 2.8 ms). Diffusion-weighted imaging (DWI) data were acquired using dual-spin echo planar imaging at 2-mm3 spatial resolution (TR = 4000 ms, TE = 73 ms). 60 20 mm-thick slices in 64 noncollinear diffusion directions (b = 2000 s/mm2) were acquired along with two nondiffusion weighted (b = 0 s/mm2) volumes. The main diffusion sequence was repeated twice. We additionally obtained two reference (b = 0) images in a reversed phase-encoding direction (P-A).
Susceptibility-induced distortions were corrected using combined b0 images from A-P and P-A scans with FSL TOPUP tools (Andersson et al., 2003; Smith et al., 2004). Observers’ motion was corrected using a 14-parameter nonlinear co-registration based on an expected model of eddy-current distortions given the phase encode direction during data acquisition (Andersson and Sotiropoulos, 2016).Tools used in preprocessing are publicly available as part of MRtrix3 and FSL software packages (Smith et al., 2004; Woolrich et al., 2009; Jenkinson et al., 2012; Tournier et al., 2019).
Fiber tractography
Using the automated segmentation procedure in Freesurfer (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/; Fischl, 2012), we performed tissues segmentation on the T1 image to estimate white/gray matter boundary and locate region of interest (ROI) to serve as fiber endpoints. Specifically, we sourced the lateral geniculate nuclei (LGN; Iglesias et al., 2018), primary visual cortex (V1), fusiform, inferior temporal cortex and lateral occipital cortex using automated cortical parcellation in Freesurfer.
Optic radiation (OR)
OR streamlines were identified by performing probabilistic tractography between LGN and V1 based on ConTrack (Stanford University, Stanford, CA; http://github.com/vistalab/vistasoft; Sherbondy et al., 2008a). We sampled 100,000 candidate pathways connecting the LGN and V1 (step size, 1 mm) and retained the 5000 pathways with the highest likelihood based on diffusion measurements at each sampling node in the ConTrack scoring process (Sherbondy et al., 2008b). We further refined the tracts in Quench (Cassette, 2016) by removing OR tracts belonging to the bottom 2.06% for linearity, and restricting their maximum length to 115 mm to filter out feedback streamlines that resemble the template for splenium of the corpus callosum via visual inspection (Renauld et al., 2016). Detailed description of the ConTrack procedure have been described elsewhere (Sherbondy et al., 2008a, b).
Vertical occipital fasciculus (VOF)
We first generated the whole-brain connectome using the iFOD2 probabilistic algorithm (Tournier et al., 2010). This algorithm is reliable for delineating fibers in highly curved and crossing regions. We sampled 2 million streamlines with a maximum length of 250 mm and FOD amplitude cutoff at 0.06. Automated fiber quantification was then used to identify the VOF tracts using the whole-brain connectome and previously identified ROIs (fusiform, inferior temporal cortex, and lateral occipital cortex). Fibers that traveled at least 1.3 cm farther vertically than other directions, were located posterior to the arcuate fasciculus and had ventral endpoints near the ROIs were selected as candidate streamlines. The algorithms are publicly distributed in the VOF toolbox as part of the AFQ software package (http://github.com/yeatmanlab/AFQ; Yeatman et al., 2012). More details of VOF tractography can also be found from previous work (Yeatman et al., 2014; Duan et al., 2015).
Occipital corpus callosum (OCC)
OCC tracts were identified with open-source MATLAB codes from AFQ toolbox (Yeatman et al., 2012). This function tracks fibers that passed through the mid-sagittal plane of the corpus callosum and two ROIs, left and right V1 (RF Dougherty et al., 2007).
Quantifying tissue properties
We obtained fractional anisotropy (FA) and mean diffusivity (MD) values using mrDiffusion tools in the Vistasoft package (Stanford University, Stanford, CA; http://github.com/vistalab/vistasoft). Each tract was sampled at 100 equidistant nodes with first and last 10 nodes removed to exclude voxels that are too close to the gray-white matter interface. We summarized the tract profiles using FA and MD values sampled at the central 80 nodes, averaged across two runs. We successfully obtained FA and MD values on left and right OR and OCC from all 41 observers. Because of possible motion and/or susceptibility artifacts that could not be corrected, despite successful VOF reconstruction, we obtained null FA values of left and right VOF from six and four observers, respectively. For MD, we extracted null tract left VOF means from three observers and that of right VOF from one observer. This is a common, recurring issue with the AFQ toolbox, especially when the tensor could be ill defined near cortical endpoints or in regions with artifacts.
Statistical analysis: DWI and behavior
All statistical analyses were performed in MATLAB using functions from the Statistics and Machine Learning Toolbox. Since we were interested in whether tract properties could explain the degree but not directionality of SED, and owing to the non-normality of the data, behavioral data were first analyzed using Spearman rank correlations to investigate the possible relationships among SED metrics (absolute perceived phase and absolute motion coherence dominance ratio), and SED and stereosensitivity (% depth signal). To investigate tracts that may best predict the various behavioral SED metrics, we entered data into (Bonferroni-corrected) multiple linear regressions. Although the models are not strictly independent, we opted for full seven-way Bonferroni correction to be fully conservative. Before entering the diffusion metrics into the analyses, we derived metrics that were deemed most sensible in accordance with the tract endpoints’ known monocular or binocular status. For instance, while SED references a comparison between left and right eye inputs, it would not make a great deal of sense to compare right versus left tract tissue properties beyond V1, subsequent to known emergence of binocularity.
We derived diffusivity metrics as follows:
OR
Given that neurons of each of the right and left LGNs are exclusively monocular and have representations from both eyes (be it within separate layers), we elected to compute absolute weighted tract differences between left and right OR ( ) – ORweighted. Beyond the chiasm, white matter will not just carry information from the contralateral eye but rather have information originating from both eyes. As noted earlier, while nasal fibers from both eyes decussate, there is a significantly higher density of (i.e., 300% more) ganglion cells carrying information from the nasal retina than the temporal retina (Curcio and Allen, 1990). Beyond the chiasm then, and within each hemisphere, there is a physiological imbalance as to the white matter representations in terms of the eye of origin. For this reason, for the OR, we retained a metric that reflects L versus R asymmetry. We weighted the differences as it is well possible for individual tract/radiation differences to scale with overall tissue properties. Finally, we elected to take the “absolute” asymmetries, as the signage of the perceptual manifestation of SED is not intuitive, especially as it relates to physiological asymmetries (Dieter and Blake, 2015; Stanley et al., 2019). We arrive at a metric of the OR then, that allows us to gauge correlates of the degree of eye dominance.
VOF
As binocular combination occurs at V1, any difference in the weighting (or here, transmission) of the two eyes’ inputs should not be reflected in comparisons of the lateralized tracts, but rather reflected in a weakened average. Hence, VOF tract tissue properties were represented as the average of left and right VOF – VOFaverage.
OCC
We took the FA and MD values as they were.
To infer the stability of our estimate, we computed confidence intervals for nonintercept coefficients by drawing 1000 bootstrapped samples from residuals.
Results
Behavioral indices of sensory eye dominance and stereosensitivity
We first examined observers’ degree of sensory eye dominance and stereosensitivity as indexed using the binocular phase combination (“phase”), dichoptic global motion discrimination (“motion”) and depth signal-in-noise tasks (“depth”; Fig. 2). Observers’ degree of sensory eye dominance was classified according to their absolute perceived phase and absolute motion coherence dominance ratio. Stereosensitivity was estimated as observers’ depth discrimination threshold (percentage of signal dots required for 82%-accurate discrimination). The lower the threshold, the greater the stereosensitivity. Overall, observers had a mean absolute perceived phase of 3.841 ± 0.898°, mean absolute motion coherence dominance ratio of 0.257 ± 0.033 and mean depth discrimination threshold of 41.349 ± 3.242 (% signal).
Strong correlation between SED indices
We next examined relationships among the behavioral metrices using Spearman rank correlations (corrected to hold family-wise error at 0.05). We found a significant positive relationship between the two SED (phase and motion) tasks (ρ = 0.35, p = 0.025; Fig. 3A). Neither SED indices significantly correlated with stereosensitivity [Phase: ρ = 0.081, p = 0.615 (Fig. 3B); Motion: ρ = 0.071, p = 0.657 (Fig. 3C)].
Tract properties of ORs explain variances in SED
To investigate how tract diffusivity measures may contribute to variations in behavioral performance, we entered FA and MD values of the tracts (Fig. 4) in multiple linear regression models to assess their predictive value to behavior.
For each behavioral metric, and for each of the FA and MD properties, we tested seven regression models comprising single or combinations of ORweighted, VOFaverage, as predictors. The multiple regressions were Bonferroni-corrected to hold family-wise error at 0.05 via a seven-way Bonferroni correction. The analyses indicated that MD values of ORweighted could significantly predict perceived phase (phase; R2 = 0.185, F(2,38) = 8.824, p = 0.005; Table 1). The significance of this model is confirmed by the bootstrapped coefficient confidence interval as it does not include 0 (Fig. 5). MD values of ORweighted + OCC also predicted, to a weaker extent, perceived phase although these models did not survive the conservative seven-way statistical correction (R2 = 0.212, F(3,37) = 5.117, p = 0.01; Table 1). Neither FA nor MD could predict SED as indexed by the motion coherence dominance ratio (SED motion) nor stereosensitivity (depth).
Table 1.
Task | Variables in the model | Tract property | R 2 | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
SED phase | ORweighted | FA | 0.034 | 0.251 |
MD | 0.185 | 0.005* | ||
VOFaverage | FA | 0.004 | 0.722 | |
MD | 0.003 | 0.735 | ||
OCC | FA | 0.051 | 0.158 | |
MD | 0.010 | 0.533 | ||
ORweighted, VOFaverage | FA | 0.031 | 0.642 | |
MD | 0.196 | 0.025 | ||
ORweighted, OCC | FA | 0.068 | 0.265 | |
MD | 0.212 | 0.011 | ||
VOFaverage, OCC | FA | 0.050 | 0.490 | |
MD | 0.020 | 0.711 | ||
All tracts | FA | 0.063 | 0.615 | |
MD | 0.225 | 0.036 | ||
SED motion | ORweighted | FA | 0.000 | 0.955 |
MD | 0.067 | 0.102 | ||
VOFaverage | FA | 0.008 | 0.632 | |
MD | 0.013 | 0.504 | ||
OCC | FA | 0.010 | 0.530 | |
MD | 0.002 | 0.803 | ||
ORweighted, VOFaverage | FA | 0.009 | 0.879 | |
MD | 0.075 | 0.266 | ||
ORweighted, OCC | FA | 0.010 | 0.818 | |
MD | 0.067 | 0.267 | ||
VOFaverage, OCC | FA | 0.03 | 0.655 | |
MD | 0.013 | 0.797 | ||
All tracts | FA | 0.030 | 0.842 | |
MD | 0.075 | 0.456 | ||
Depth | ORweighted | FA | 0.001 | 0.870 |
MD | 0.066 | 0.106 | ||
VOFaverage | FA | 0.086 | 0.110 | |
MD | 0.026 | 0.340 | ||
OCC | FA | 0.006 | 0.643 | |
MD | 0.023 | 0.347 | ||
ORweighted, VOFaverage | FA | 0.093 | 0.255 | |
MD | 0.070 | 0.289 | ||
ORweighted, OCC | FA | 0.008 | 0.863 | |
MD | 0.102 | 0.129 | ||
VOFaverage, OCC | FA | 0.111 | 0.194 | |
MD | 0.074 | 0.272 | ||
All tracts | FA | 0.127 | 0.293 | |
MD | 0.128 | 0.205 |
*p ≤ 0.007, **p ≤ 0.001, ***p ≤ 0.00001.
Discussion
Exploiting variations in sensory eye dominance in the neurotypical population, we examined whether major visual white matter tract properties could explain individual variabilities in SED. We found that indices from the two SED tasks (binocular phase combination and dichoptic global motion discrimination tasks) strongly correlated with each other. However, neither SED index correlated with stereosensitivity as measured by the depth signal-in-noise task. Examining the microstructural properties of major visual white matter tracts, left and right OR, left and right VOF, and OCC, we found that only regression models involving MD values of the left and right ORs (and to a weaker extent, OCC) could predict SED as indexed by perceived phase. Neither MD nor FA could predict performance on the dichoptic motion coherence task, nor on the stereo-depth task. These findings suggest that SED is reflected, at least in part, by distributed tract properties along early (thalamocortical) and late cortical visual pathways. The lack of association between each of the motion coherence dominance, and stereosensitivity metric with tract properties suggest a possible neural dissociation between the phase and motion tasks – such that while they may both be effective metrics of SED, the latter may be governed by factors in addition to/other than microstructural properties of visual white matter tracts.
SED tasks are behaviorally correlated
Our psychophysical data are in good agreement with previous studies showing that SED measured with phase and motion stimuli are largely consistent in terms of magnitude (Han et al., 2018; Ooi and He, 2020; Fig. 3A). Notably, the two SED tasks involve the presentation of distinct features: the phase task presents a pair of static, fusible horizontal gratings and the motion task presents dichoptically, moving signal and noise dots. The binocular phase combination task then, is likely suited to orientation selective channels in area V1 (Huang et al., 2010; Zhou et al., 2013) while the dichoptic motion task likely suits motion-sensitive mechanisms in the human middle temporal complex (hMT+; Beckers and Hömberg, 1992). Beyond the featural differences, the strong correlation between the SED tasks indicates that the two processes engage in common circuitry at some stage. One possible location of interaction is the LGN, which receives cortical projections from V1, where V2, V4 and MT provide strong feedback to V1 (Fitzpatrick et al., 1994; C Wang et al., 2007; Briggs et al., 2016). Cortical feedback may modulate gain-control signals and the strength of the inhibitory network at the LGN.
No relationship between SED and stereosensitivity
We did not find correlations between SED and stereosensitivity (Fig. 3B,C). This is somewhat counter to reports from several earlier studies (Dieter et al., 2017; Han et al., 2018), but are in agreement with Y Wang et al. (2018), who found no correlation between SED as measured by the binocular phase combination task and stereopsis, as well as Wu et al. (2018), who indexed SED by means of the continuous flashing technique, and similarly did not find any relationship of SED with stereosensitivity. The lack of correlation observed is perhaps not surprising, as we indexed SED and depth sensitivity from the neurotypical population, whose within-subject variability is relatively minimal compared with the clinical population, e.g., amblyopia or other age groups, e.g., elderly. Moreover, certainly, stereopsis entails the participation of binocular neurons outside of V1, but also along key disparity-sensitive nodes such as V3 and V7 (Tsao et al., 2003; Brouwer et al., 2005). Indeed, Wu et al. (2018) suggested that SED affects the rate of signal integration, while stereoacuity depends on the efficiency of binocular neurons on extracting disparity information from the integrated signals, which should be independent from SED. This would also be consistent with findings of Feng et al. (2017), who found that treated anisometropes still exhibited strong SED as indexed by the binocular phase combination task, but had near to neurotypical stereoacuity indexed by clinical tests. Thus, it seems that SED alone is insufficient to explain stereoscopic outcomes.
The neural correlates of SED
We found that absolute weighted differences of MD values of the optic radiations are well associated with the magnitude of SED indexed by the phase task (Table 1; Fig. 5). These findings suggest that SED is at least in part explained by white matter microstructural properties early in the visual cascade and well fit the gain-control theory (Ding and Sperling, 2006) and particularly, the speculative interocular contrast gain control mechanisms placed before binocular summation. The variability in perceived phase may be associated with different levels of myelination in the left and right ORs that are then reflected by MD. However, prominent to Figure 3A is the presence of several cases of strong eye dominance (paired with larger phase indices). If these individuals are removed, the correlation (p = 0.005) no longer survives a strict seven-way statistical correction. For this reason, while exciting, we caution that the relationship between optic radiation tissue properties and SED will require future empirical verification.
We note here that white matter microstructural properties of the ORs have been previously shown to relate to gray-matter visual function. Specifically, Toosy et al. (2004) reported a positive relationship between white matter microstructural properties (in terms of FA values) of ORs and BOLD activity in the visual cortex. More relevant to the present work, underdevelopment of the ORs has been observed in amblyopes (Xie et al., 2007; Qi et al., 2016) who display extreme forms of eye imbalance. Since ORs carry information bidirectionally, variability in MD values may lead to changes in both the LGN and V1. Indeed, Qi et al. (2016) reported a positive relationship between OR tissue properties and cortical thickness of V1, implying that the impairment of ORs may be associated with structural deficits in the visual cortex. It follows that any structural gray matter deficits in V1, or potentially even earlier in the LGN, may reduce the efficiency in the processing of visual information, with efficiency here loosely encapsulating both readouts and binocular interactions, eventually manifesting in binocular imbalance.
It is worth noting that our data do not preclude the possibility that there are roles for further tracts that occur “postsummation” (i.e., post-V1; Baker and Meese, 2007; Ding et al., 2013; Ding and Levi, 2017). We note the potential involvement in our data of the OCC as it relates to SED, although the model did not survive the full statistical corrections (Table 1). While the exact relationship of the OCC to SED needs to be further validated, it is interesting to speculate on its role given its anatomic placement of connecting the visual cortex in left and right hemispheres. Interhemispheric processing has been studied in the rat visual cortex where the rodent is monocularly deprived during the critical period of visual development (Pietrasanta et al., 2012). By silencing the callosal pathway, Pietrasanta et al. (2012) managed to reduce the deprivation-induced shift in ocular dominance and rats were able to regain binocularity once the deprived eye started receiving visual input. It appears that OCC is key to developmental maturation of the two hemispheres and neurotypical binocularity. Perhaps more efficient neuronal transmission between the two hemispheres may facilitate binocular integration.
Microstructural properties of visual white matter tracts could not predict performance in the dichoptic motion task
While tract properties seem to well-predict SED as indexed by the phase task, the same was not true of the dichoptic motion task SED metric. While at first glance this might be surprising, our motion task, unlike the V1-oriented phase-combination-task, taps into features known to be extrastriate-reliant, e.g., human middle temporal complex hMT+ (Beckers and Hömberg, 1992) and posterior parietal cortex (Patten and Welchman, 2015). The optic radiations connect LGN to V1 and VOFs have their endpoints in fusiform, inferior temporal cortex and lateral occipital cortex. Motion signal extraction, as required by the dichoptic motion task, seems to be largely determined by hMT+ neurons’ ocular preference. hMT+ neurons are mostly (45–60%) binocularly unbiased or balanced, with the rest showing clear ocular dominance (Maunsell and Van Essen, 1983; Kiorpes et al., 1996; El-Shamayleh et al., 2010). The ocular-specific responses motion neurons render a motion-oriented task quite suitable for picking up eye imbalances, but these imbalances should be reflected in terms of hMT+ cortical responses. That is, SED imbalances as reflected by the dichoptic motion task may be better captured by high-resolution functional gray matter responses, rather than by solely microstructural properties of white matter.
Outstanding issues
We close with two outstanding issues that may be worth empirical attention moving forward. The first relates to the fact that many aspects of our data are null (e.g., lack of correlation between SED and certain visual white matter tract properties, lack of correlations between SED and stereosensitivity). Still, we believe that there is still value in reporting this information to the community to help weigh in on prevailing theories on the neural basis of SED. While it is intriguing that the microstructural properties of the optic radiations are a strong predictor of behavioral performance in the phase task, these positive results should be reported with caveats, the possibility that certain correlations could not be detected because of the sensitivity limitations of the techniques, i.e., imaging resolution, reliability of SED tasks.
Moreover, in our pool of neurotypical participants, there is a lack of consistency in eye dominance classifications. In only a little over half of our participants (53.7%) was there eye dominance agreement across both SED indexing tasks. This observation, however, is not surprising in light of previous work demonstrating comparably weak consistency across standard SED tasks when indexing eye dominance in the neurotypical population (García-Pérez and Peli, 2019; Kam and Chang, 2021). If we look only at the subset of our participants with strong eye dominance (2 SD beyond mean), we find that they show high consistency across both SED tasks. We speculate that the inconsistency among eye dominance classifications may be because of the relatively balanced eyes in neurotypical individuals, thus classifications for weakly dominant eyes come down to variance. Yet, it remains an open question as to whether alternate SED indices (i.e., binocular rivalry) may be more sensitive to revealing functional relationships with white matter, as well as with broader binocular function (i.e., stereosensitivity).
In conclusion, the present data suggest that SED is governed at least in part by properties of the optic radiations. Although both dichoptic phase and motion tasks are well-established behavioral metrics of SED, the two tasks may yet capture distinct neural mechanisms, with the latter better characterized by gray matter function. Our data further assert that SED alone is insufficient to predict stereoscopic function.
Synthesis
Reviewing Editor: Anne Keitel, University of Dundee
Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: Hiromasa Takemura.
Please see below for Reviewer #2s comments on the revised version of your manuscript. Unfortunately, Reviewer #1 was not available to assess the revised manuscript. However, the available reviewer and I both thought the revisions were well done and caveats/open questions were highlighted sufficiently.
Reviewer #2 - Advances the Field (Required)
This manuscript reports a novel examination on correlation between sensory eye dominance and tissue properties of the optic radiation. I think that this is a new research question which will motivate future studies. Only issue is that the effect is not large. However, considering that this is a journal which publishes carefully conducted negative results, I think that it is fine to publish this manuscript since authors acknowledge that effect size is limited.
Reviewer #2 - Statistics
Authors made a response to regression models and it seems OK to me, but I am not really confident on this point.
Reviewer #2 - Comments to the Authors (Required)
I thank authors to carefully address all my comments. I have no additional comments.
References
- Alexander AL, Hurley SA, Samsonov AA, Adluru N, Hosseinbor AP, Mossahebi P, Tromp DPM, Zakszewski E, Field AS (2011) Characterization of cerebral white matter properties using quantitative magnetic resonance imaging stains. Brain Connect 1:423–446. 10.1089/brain.2011.0071 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allen AP, Kennedy PJ, Cryan JF, Dinan TG, Clarke G (2014) Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: focus on the Trier social stress test. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 38:94–124. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersson JLR, Sotiropoulos SN (2016) An integrated approach to correction for off-resonance effects and subject movement in diffusion MR imaging. Neuroimage 125:1063–1078. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersson JLR, Skare S, Ashburner J (2003) How to correct susceptibility distortions in spin-echo echo-planar images: application to diffusion tensor imaging. Neuroimage 20:870–888. 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00336-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baker DH, Meese TS (2007) Binocular contrast interactions: dichoptic masking is not a single process. Vision Res 47:3096–3107. 10.1016/j.visres.2007.08.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beckers G, Hömberg V (1992) Cerebral visual motion blindness: transitory akinetopsia induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation of human area V5. Proc Biol Sci 249:173–178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Binda P, Kurzawski JW, Lunghi C, Biagi L, Tosetti M, Morrone MC (2018) Response to short-term deprivation of the human adult visual cortex measured with 7T BOLD. Elife 7:e40014. 10.7554/eLife.40014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blake R (1989) A neural theory of binocular rivalry. Psychol Rev 96:145–167. 10.1037/0033-295x.96.1.145 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blasdel GG, Fitzpatrick D (1984) Physiological organization of layer 4 in macaque striate cortex. J Neurosci 4:880–895. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.04-03-00880.1984 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brainard DH (1997) The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis 10:433–436. 10.1163/156856897X00357 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Briggs F, Kiley CW, Callaway EM, Usrey WM (2016) Morphological substrates for parallel streams of corticogeniculate feedback originating in both V1 and V2 of the macaque monkey. Neuron 90:388–399. 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.02.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brouwer GJ, van Ee R, Schwarzbach J (2005) Activation in visual cortex correlates with the awareness of stereoscopic depth. J Neurosci 25:10403–10413. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2408-05.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casagrande VA, Boyd JD (1996) The neural architecture of binocular vision. Eye 10:153–160. 10.1038/eye.1996.40 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cassette P (2016) QUENCH: a software package for the determination of quenching curves in liquid scintillation counting. Appl Radiat Isot 109:301–307. 10.1016/j.apradiso.2015.11.048 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curcio CA, Allen KA (1990) Topography of ganglion cells in human retina. J Comp Neurol 300:5–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Blank PMK, Berman JI, Liu GT, Roberts TPL, Fisher MJ (2013) Fractional anisotropy of the optic radiations is associated with visual acuity loss in optic pathway gliomas of neurofibromatosis type 1. Neuro Oncol 15:1088–1095. 10.1093/neuonc/not068 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dieter K, Blake R (2015) Sensory eye dominance varies within the visual field. J Vis 15:268. 10.1167/15.12.268 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dieter KC, Sy JL, Blake R (2017) Individual differences in sensory eye dominance reflected in the dynamics of binocular rivalry. Vision Res 141:40–50. 10.1016/j.visres.2016.09.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ding J, Sperling G (2006) A gain-control theory of binocular combination. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103:1141–1146. 10.1073/pnas.0509629103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ding J, Levi DM (2017) Binocular combination of luminance profiles. J Vis 17:4. 10.1167/17.13.4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ding J, Klein SA, Levi DM (2013) Binocular combination in abnormal binocular vision. J Vis 13:14. 10.1167/13.2.14 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dougherty K, Schmid MC, Maier A (2019) Binocular response modulation in the lateral geniculate nucleus. J Comp Neurol 527:522–534. 10.1002/cne.24417 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dougherty RF, Ben-Shachar M, Deutsch GK, Hernandez A, Fox GR, Wandell BA (2007) Temporal-callosal pathway diffusivity predicts phonological skills in children. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104:8556–8561. 10.1073/pnas.0608961104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Douglas RJ, Martin KAC, Whitteridge D (1989) A canonical microcircuit for neocortex. Neural Comput 1:480–488. 10.1162/neco.1989.1.4.480 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Duan Y, Norcia AM, Yeatman JD, Mezer A (2015) The structural properties of major white matter tracts in strabismic amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 56:5152–5160. 10.1167/iovs.15-17097 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- El-Shamayleh Y, Kiorpes L, Kohn A, Movshon JA (2010) Visual motion processing by neurons in area MT of macaque monkeys with experimental amblyopia. J Neurosci 30:12198–12209. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3055-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feng L, Lin H, Chen Y, Wang J, Wang Y, Liao R, Zhou J, Hess RF (2017) The effect of Lasik surgery on myopic anisometropes’ sensory eye dominance. Sci Rep 7:3629. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl B (2012) FreeSurfer. Neuroimage 62:774–781. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fitzpatrick D, Usrey WM, Schofield BR, Einstein G (1994) The sublaminar organization of corticogeniculate neurons in layer 6 of macaque striate cortex. Vis Neurosci 11:307–315. 10.1017/s0952523800001656 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- García-Pérez MA, Peli E (2019) Psychophysical tests do not identify ocular dominance consistently. Iperception 10:2041669519841397. 10.1177/2041669519841397 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Han C, He ZJ, Ooi TL (2018) On sensory eye dominance revealed by binocular integrative and binocular competitive stimuli. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 59:5140–5148. 10.1167/iovs.18-24342 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harauzov A, Spolidoro M, DiCristo G, De Pasquale R, Cancedda L, Pizzorusso T, Viegi A, Berardi N, Maffei L (2010) Reducing intracortical inhibition in the adult visual cortex promotes ocular dominance plasticity. J Neurosci 30:361–371. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2233-09.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haynes JD, Deichmann R, Rees G (2005) Eye-specific effects of binocular rivalry in the human lateral geniculate nucleus. Nature 438:496–499. 10.1038/nature04169 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang CB, Zhou J, Zhou Y, Lu ZL (2010) Contrast and phase combination in binocular vision. PLoS One 5:e15075. 10.1371/journal.pone.0015075 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hubel DH, Wiesel TN (1968) Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex. J Physiol 195:215–243. 10.1113/jphysiol.1968.sp008455 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hubel DH, Wiesel TN (1972) Laminar and columnar distribution of geniculo-cortical fibers in the macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol 146:421–450. 10.1002/cne.901460402 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iglesias JE, Insausti R, Lerma-Usabiaga G, Bocchetta M, Van Leemput K, Greve DN, van der Kouwe A; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative, Fischl B, Caballero-Gaudes C, Paz-Alonso PM (2018) A probabilistic atlas of the human thalamic nuclei combining ex vivo MRI and histology. Neuroimage 183:314–326. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.08.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkinson M, Beckmann CF, Behrens TEJ, Woolrich MW, Smith SM (2012) FSL. Neuroimage 62:782–790. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kam KY, Chang DHF (2021) Dichoptic perceptual training and sensory eye dominance plasticity in normal vision. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 62:12. 10.1167/iovs.62.7.12 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiorpes L, Walton PJ, O’Keefe LP, Movshon JA, Lisberger SG (1996) Effects of early-onset artificial strabismus on pursuit eye movements and on neuronal responses in area MT of macaque monkeys. J Neurosci 16:6537–6553. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-20-06537.1996 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levelt WJM (1965) On binocular rivalry. Assen: Van Gorcum & Comp. N.V. — Dr. H. J. Prakke & H. M. G. Prakke. [Google Scholar]
- Levi DM (2020) Rethinking amblyopia 2020. Vision Res 176:118–129. 10.1016/j.visres.2020.07.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li J, Lam CSY, Yu M, Hess RF, Chan LYL, Maehara G, Woo GC, Thompson B (2010) Quantifying sensory eye dominance in the normal visual system: a new technique and insights into variation across traditional tests. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 51:6875–6881. 10.1167/iovs.10-5549 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maunsell JH, Van Essen DC (1983) Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. I. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed, and orientation. J Neurophysiol 49:1127–1147. 10.1152/jn.1983.49.5.1127 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKee SP, Levi DM, Movshon JA (2003) The pattern of visual deficits in amblyopia. J Vis 3:5. 10.1167/3.5.5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moutoussis K, Keliris G, Kourtzi Z, Logothetis N (2005) A binocular rivalry study of motion perception in the human brain. Vision Res 45:2231–2243. 10.1016/j.visres.2005.02.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oishi H, Takemura H, Aoki SC, Fujita I, Amano K (2018) Microstructural properties of the vertical occipital fasciculus explain the variability in human stereoacuity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 115:12289–12294. 10.1073/pnas.1804741115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ooi TL, He ZJ (2020) Sensory eye dominance: relationship between eye and brain. Eye Brain 12:25–31. 10.2147/EB.S176931 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patten ML, Welchman AE (2015) FMRI activity in posterior parietal cortex relates to the perceptual use of binocular disparity for both signal-in-noise and feature difference tasks. PLoS One 10:e0140696. 10.1371/journal.pone.0140696 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pelli DG (1997) The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat Vis 10:437–442. 10.1163/156856897X00366 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pietrasanta M, Restani L, Caleo M (2012) The corpus callosum and the visual cortex: plasticity is a game for two. Neural Plast 2012:e838672. 10.1155/2012/838672 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polonsky A, Blake R, Braun J, Heeger DJ (2000) Neuronal activity in human primary visual cortex correlates with perception during binocular rivalry. Nat Neurosci 3:1153–1159. 10.1038/80676 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qi S, Mu Y-F, Cui L-B, Li R, Shi M, Liu Y, Xu JQ, Zhang J, Yang J, Yin H (2016) Association of optic radiation integrity with cortical thickness in children with anisometropic amblyopia. Neurosci Bull 32:51–60. 10.1007/s12264-015-0005-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Renauld E, Descoteaux M, Bernier M, Garyfallidis E, Whittingstall K (2016) Semi-automatic segmentation of optic radiations and LGN, and their relationship to EEG alpha waves. PLoS One 11:e0156436. 10.1371/journal.pone.0156436 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sherbondy AJ, Dougherty RF, Ben-Shachar M, Napel S, Wandell BA (2008a) ConTrack: finding the most likely pathways between brain regions using diffusion tractography. J Vis 8:15.1–16. 10.1167/8.9.15 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sherbondy AJ, Dougherty RF, Napel S, Wandell BA (2008b) Identifying the human optic radiation using diffusion imaging and fiber tractography. J Vis 8:12.1–11. 10.1167/8.10.12 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TEJ, Johansen-Berg H, Bannister PR, De Luca M, Drobnjak I, Flitney DE, Niazy RK, Saunders J, Vickers J, Zhang Y, De Stefano N, Brady JM, Matthews PM (2004) Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23 [Suppl 1]:S208–S219. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stanley J, Forte JD, Carter O (2019) Rivalry onset in and around the fovea: the role of visual field location and eye dominance on perceptual dominance bias. Vision (Basel) 3:51. 10.3390/vision3040051 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas C, Avidan G, Humphreys K, Jung K, Gao F, Behrmann M (2009) Reduced structural connectivity in ventral visual cortex in congenital prosopagnosia. Nat Neurosci 12:29–31. 10.1038/nn.2224 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toosy AT, Ciccarelli O, Parker GJM, Wheeler-Kingshott CAM, Miller DH, Thompson AJ (2004) Characterizing function–structure relationships in the human visual system with functional MRI and diffusion tensor imaging. Neuroimage 21:1452–1463. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tournier JD, Calamante F, Connelly A (2010) Improved probabilistic streamlines tractography by 2nd order integration over fibre orientation distributions. Proc Intl Soc Mag Reson Med 18. [Google Scholar]
- Tournier JD, Smith R, Raffelt D, Tabbara R, Dhollander T, Pietsch M, Christiaens D, Jeurissen B, Yeh CH, Connelly A (2019) MRtrix3: a fast, flexible and open software framework for medical image processing and visualisation. Neuroimage 202:116137. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116137 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsao DY, Vanduffel W, Sasaki Y, Fize D, Knutsen TA, Mandeville JB, Wald LL, Dale AM, Rosen BR, Van Essen DC, Livingstone MS, Orban GA, Tootell RBH (2003) Stereopsis activates V3A and caudal intraparietal areas in macaques and humans. Neuron 39:555–568. 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00459-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang C, Waleszczyk WJ, Burke W, Dreher B (2007) Feedback signals from cat’s area 21a enhance orientation selectivity of area 17 neurons. Exp Brain Res 182:479–490. 10.1007/s00221-007-1014-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Y, Cui L, He Z, Lin W, Qu J, Lu F, Zhou J, Hess RF (2018) On the relationship between sensory eye dominance and stereopsis in the normal-sighted adult population: normative data. Front Hum Neurosci 12:357. 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00357 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watson AB, Pelli DG (1983) Quest: a Bayesian adaptive psychometric method. Percept Psychophys 33:113–120. 10.3758/bf03202828 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wong NA, Rafique SA, Kelly KR, Moro SS, Gallie BL, Steeves JKE (2018) Altered white matter structure in the visual system following early monocular enucleation: altered white matter after early enucleation. Hum Brain Mapp 39:133–144. 10.1002/hbm.23831 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woolrich MW, Jbabdi S, Patenaude B, Chappell M, Makni S, Behrens T, Beckmann C, Jenkinson M, Smith SM (2009) Bayesian analysis of neuroimaging data in FSL. Neuroimage 45:S173–S186. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu H, Bi H, Zhang X, Chen Z, Lan W, Li X, Zhang B, Yang Z (2018) Balanced eyes see stereopsis more quickly, but not more finely. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 59:499–504. 10.1167/iovs.17-22849 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wunderlich K, Schneider KA, Kastner S (2005) Neural correlates of binocular rivalry in the human lateral geniculate nucleus. Nat Neurosci 8:1595–1602. 10.1038/nn1554 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xie S, Gong GL, Xiao JXI, Ye JT, Liu HH, Gan XL, Jiang ZT, Jiang XX (2007) Underdevelopment of optic radiation in children with amblyopia: a tractography study. Am J Ophthalmol 143:642–646. 10.1016/j.ajo.2006.12.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu H, Han C, Chen M, Li P, Zhu S, Fang Y, Hu J, Ma H, Lu HD (2016) Rivalry-like neural activity in primary visual cortex in anesthetized monkeys. J Neurosci 36:3231–3242. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3660-15.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang E, Blake R, McDonald JE (2010) A new interocular suppression technique for measuring sensory eye dominance. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 51:588–593. 10.1167/iovs.08-3076 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeatman JD, Dougherty RF, Myall NJ, Wandell BA, Feldman HM (2012) Tract profiles of white matter properties: automating fiber-tract quantification. PLoS One 7:e49790. 10.1371/journal.pone.0049790 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeatman JD, Weiner KS, Pestilli F, Rokem A, Mezer A, Wandell BA (2014) The vertical occipital fasciculus: a century of controversy resolved by in vivo measurements. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 111:E5214–E5223. 10.1073/pnas.1418503111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou J, Clavagnier S, Hess RF (2013) Short-term monocular deprivation strengthens the patched eye’s contribution to binocular combination. J Vis 13:12. 10.1167/13.5.12 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]