Abstract
There is an established consensus among researchers that contact with nature improves mental health, wellbeing, and quality of life in urbanised environments. Studies tend to examine the health impacts of nature without identifying specific physical and spatial landscape features that could guide health-promoting design of urban green spaces. A growing body of evidence suggests that landscape features described in the Contemplative Landscape Model (CLM) can be used to measure therapeutic value of urban landscapes. CLM assesses urban landscapes across seven sub-scales: Layers of the Landscape, Landform, Vegetation, Color and Light, Compatibility, Archetypal Elements and Character of Peace and Silence. We exposed 74 healthy adults to six urban landscapes in laboratory (video representations) and naturalistic outdoor settings. We explored the associations between the visual quality of urban landscapes annotated with CLM, with self-reported positive emotions and brain activity consistent with mindfulness (Theta waves), relaxation (Alpha waves) and attention restoration (Beta waves), and differences between laboratory and naturalistic setting. CLM scores predicted self-reported Valence and Arousal, and low frequency power bands: Alpha and Theta in the naturalistic setting. Landscape features showing the strongest associations were Character of Peace and Silence, Layers of the Landscape and Archetypal Elements. Alpha, Theta brain reactivity and Arousal scores, were significantly different between laboratory and naturalistic settings (p < 0.05), while Valence scores between those settings were statistically identical (p = 0.22). Self-reported Valence and Arousal, but not brain activity, were significantly associated with the majority of landscape features in the laboratory setting. The results of the study provide guidelines on the urban landscape features most beneficial for human health, to inform urban green space design.
Subject terms: Urban ecology, Psychology, Quality of life, Sensory processing, Stress and resilience, Environmental impact, Environmental social sciences
Introduction
It has been well-established that the benefits of exposure to nature scenes go beyond aesthetic appreciation, extending to cognitive, affective as well as mental and physical health benefits (for detailed review see1). Mental health benefits of greenspaces have been supported by multiple studies synthesized in several systematic reviews (e.g.,2–4), but the causal pathways have not been fully established. One of the leading theories is Attention Restoration Theory, which proposes that greenspaces help replenish depleted attention capacity through experiencing natural settings5,6, thus promoting wellbeing. The complementary Stress Reduction theory7 suggests that natural environments promote recovery from stress, while the Biophilia hypothesis offers a somewhat philosophical explanation, which assumes an intrinsic affection towards unthreatening nature, shaped by the evolution8. With the rapid urbanization leading to overstimulating living environments, natural environments give an opportunity for attention restoration through “being away” from the city noise9. In the face of global decline in mental health10, city inhabitants are at higher risk of developing mental disorders such as depression and anxiety than their rural counterparts11,12. Therefore, nature in city, also known as urban green spaces (UGS), nature-based solutions, green infrastructure or simply urban parks and gardens, can be a promising medium to offset the negative mental health outcomes associated with living in high-density cities.
The challenge that landscape architects and urban planners are facing is the lack of evidence-based guidelines to include in their UGS designs and maintenance plans, to promote positive affect, mental health and well-being. Importantly, there is a clear lack of typology of UGS based on their associations with mental health promotion13. Previous research on UGS for health and wellbeing predominantly compared the effects of urban versus nature conditions, where “nature” had rather broad definition—a landscape containing natural elements for example trees, water, grass (e.g.14), or simply focused on the quantity of greenspace as measured with aerial photography techniques, (e.g.15). Both approaches offer little or no implications for landscape architectural design as perceived by people. Other studies used different UGS typologies as comparators with the urban space, for example forest (e.g.16), park (e.g.17), garden (e.g.18). However, these approaches seem to leave too much room for ambiguity as there is a myriad of landscape scenes and physical attributes within each of the UGS type, different styles of design and quality of maintenance, likely providing different levels of salutogenic potential. The inconsistencies in findings across the studies suggest that a more granular analysis of the landscape features is required to help inform health promoting urban greenspace designs. The lack of specific knowledge on the landscape quality in the area of evidence-based landscape design has been highlighted by researchers19,20.
Another issue lies with the methodological aspects of existing studies, more specifically their ecological validity and replicability. A vast majority of studies have been conducted with photographic representations of nature, in the laboratory environment (e.g.21), or only in the outdoor setting (e.g.22). Trials in the controlled laboratory settings have the advantage of fewer confounding variables, particularly since participant responsiveness to the real in-vivo landscape setting and the corresponding photograph of such setting are highly correlated. Nevertheless, that important sensory-cognitive factors are excluded in laboratory-based experiments thus may not have the same effect on the participants23. Further research is needed to compare the associations between landscape exposure and participant outcomes in both laboratory and naturalistic settings. This leads to a need for systematic methods of evaluation of the visual landscape quality and/or features, taking into account the complexity and the dynamic character of living landscapes. The existing frameworks include the Scenic Beauty Estimation method24 and Visual Resources Management tool25 developed for evaluation and management of the vast areas of natural parks, however, their applicability in dense, urban landscapes is limited. Elements of the urban visual quality assessment can be found in more modern frameworks including Urban Landscape Quality Index26 or RECITAL27, which have been developed and tested, however have not been calibrated specifically for UGS and not yet extensively validated.
Contemplative Landscape Model (CLM), is a validated, expert-based UGS assessment instrument with scope of mental-health and well-being promotion. CLM borrows from the previous visual quality assessment methods28,29, traditions of landscape design theory30 as well as insights from Jungian psychoanalysis31. According to the CLM, each UGS view can be scored according to seven key-categories (see Fig. 1): Layers of the Landscape—assessing the depth of the view and possibility of noticing fore, middle and background in the scene; Landform—focusing on the natural asymmetry of the topography and characteristics of the skyline—whether the landscape stimulates our eyes to look upwards; Vegetation—scenes rich in species with plants that seem self-sown and not overly tended and subject to changes along the daily/seasonal/life cycle; Color & Light—scoring the possibility of seeing light and shade movement casted on the ground, view-point away from direct sun exposure as well as presence of less saturated colors; Compatibility—assessment of harmony and balance of the composition of the scene, and absence of distracting or incompatible elements; Archetypal Elements—explicit presence of elements of the landscape loaded with symbolic and universal meaning (e.g. waterfall, single tree, stone), and Character of Peace and Silence—assessment of the potential for resting, comfort and a sense of solitude, offering contrast to the busy urban space. CLM allows the assessment of individual features of urban landscapes to inform design.
Further research is needed to improve our understanding on the effects of features of UGS on human health and well-being, as well as individual differences in responses to various UGS. Furthermore, it is unclear whether there are differences in participant responses to landscapes presented in the laboratory conditions vs in-situ naturalistic exposure. Finally, a deeper understanding of the cognitive processes that mediate the effects of UGS on health and well-being, using established objective measures is necessary to elucidate causal mechanisms.
The aim of this study was to investigate whether CLM predicts patterns of brain activity associated with attention restoration (beta), wakeful relaxation (alpha) and mindfulness (theta), and self-reported affect (Valence and Arousal). And, what specific CLM features are associated with these positive outcomes. Moreover, to explore the similarities and differences in participant responsiveness to landscapes in laboratory and naturalistic settings.
Methods
Participants
We recruited 79 healthy adults, 48 were female, with age ranging between 21 and 74 years old. The participants were recruited using snowball sampling methodology. The inclusion criteria were age between 21 and 75 years old, right handedness (due to differences in brain activity between left and right-handed people33) and availability to attend the required study visits. The exclusion criteria were serious visual impairment or clinically diagnosed psychiatric, neurological, or cognitive disorders. All participants were reimbursed for their time. Procedures were reviewed by the National University of Singapore Ethics Committee and obtained ethics approval, NUS-IRB_S-20-12, and experiments were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. All participants provided informed consent for taking part in the study.
Psychometric tools
Contemplative Landscape Model (CLM)
Landscape views were scored according to seven CLM categories on 1–6-point scale, and the total score for a view is calculated by averaging all categories scores by 4 independent experts in landscape architecture. CLM is an expert-based psychometric tool with good reliability (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.854) and validity measured by correlation with the validating set of data (r = 0.772)32. In one existing laboratory-based within subjects neuroscience experiment, highly contemplative landscapes (above 4.45 points) induced a statistically different pattern of brain activity as compared to less contemplative views, in the brain of healthy adults passively exposed to them34.
Self-Assessment Manikin (SAM)
This tool was used to record the self-reported affective response of participants, after exposure to each landscape scene in the laboratory as well as the naturalistic setting. SAM is a non-verbal, pictorial assessment of momentary emotions. In our study we used two SAM scales to measure Valence (i.e., pleasantness) and Arousal (i.e., intensity of emotion)35. The instrument consists of five pictograms with facial expressions for Valence and five for Arousal and ranged from − 2 to 2 points. A higher Valence score indicates more positive emotion towards the stimuli and a higher Arousal score indicates higher intensity of that emotion towards the stimuli.
Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II)
This instrument was used at the start of the study, to assess the depression levels of participants. This 21-item multiple choice self-reported questionnaire measures the severity of depression, and is often used by psychiatrists due to its high sensitivity (81%) and specificity (91%)36. Questions refer to feelings experienced during the two weeks prior to and including the assessment day. Subjects were to circle one of four to seven statements under each of 21 items, which have assigned scores of 0, 1, 2 or 3 points. To compute total BDI-II score points from all items are summed up. Total score of 0–13 denotes minimal depression; 14–19, mild depression; 20–28, moderate depression and 29–63, severe depression.
Neuroscience tools
Electroencephalography (EEG) signal was recorded using a 16-channel V-amp amplifier (Brain Products GmbH, Munich, Germany) equipped with dry active electrodes mounted on an elastic cap according to the modified 10/20 system. The fact that dry electrodes were selected was especially meaningful for the outdoor scans—using wet electrodes and applying gel outdoors under the hot and humid climate of Singapore could reduce participants’ comfort and significantly increase the time of experiment. Electrode impedance was kept below 100 kΩ throughout the experiment, which is considered an acceptable value for dry electrodes37. Signal was recorded at 500 Hz and stored for further processing. The following indices were utilised to evaluate the results of EEG scans:
Frontal alpha activity—relaxation
Alpha rhythm (8–13 Hz) in awake state is typically strongest when individuals are not actively engaged in cognitive tasks. Alpha waves are often considered to reflect cortical “idling”, with a reduction in its power when the individual attends actively to stimuli and/or undertake a cognitive task. Thus, magnitude of the band is inversely proportional to cortical activation38. Increased alpha power in the frontal cortex is associated with lower level of psychological and emotional arousal, akin to wakeful relaxation39. Previous environmental neuroscience studies found the increased frontal alpha power in participants exposed to nature scenes as compared to urban scenes40, and in environments with less stressful conditions41. This pattern of brain activity is especially important for city dwellers as it can be contributing to reduction of stress and burnout and increasing life satisfaction (e.g.42). In our experiment we averaged alpha power from three pairs of electrodes located on the frontal lobes (AFp1-AFp2; AFF5h-AFF6h and F7-F8) to obtain the Alpha/Wakeful Relaxation index.
Frontal theta activity—mindfulness
Theta rhythm (3–7 Hz) is often related to cognitive processing in prefrontal cortex39. At the same time, increased relative theta power was associated with physiological relaxation43,44 and high presence in the moment45. Therefore, researchers relate the increased frontal theta power with meditative state of a non-directive and non-concentrative style46,47. The concentrative meditation (e.g., Transcendental) aims to eliminate the thoughts through intense focus on a single stimulus (e.g., mantra), while non-directive meditation (e.g. Mindfulness) allows thoughts to flow freely, without focusing on anything that arises internally. Mindfulness, by its psychology definition, is associated with being attentive to the present moment (sometimes referred to as “hyper-presence”) and lack of interpretation or judgement of the experienced phenomena48 without cognitive processing of external stimuli. Inducing this state in brain is increasingly recognized as a self-care and wellness intervention to reduce stress49, with range of mindfulness-based therapies and interventions targeting various mental health disorders50. Previous research in environmental neuroscience associated frontal theta brainwave pattern with the walk in nature51 and the area satisfaction52. The mindfulness index for this study was calculated by averaging Theta power from three pairs of electrodes located on the frontal lobes (AFp1-AFp2; AFF5h-AFF6h and F7-F8).
Temporal beta asymmetry—attention restoration
Beta rhythms (14–30 Hz) mark attentional processing and are increased during task engagement. Temporal Beta Asymmetry is characterized by more beta power in the right temporal lobe than in the left. The temporal region of the right hemisphere are, among other functions, responsible for visual attention53, interpreting visual information and memory of pictures, visual scenes and familiar faces54. Previous studies associated this pattern of brain activity with bottom-up, stimuli driven attention directed at the salient stimuli55. This bottom-up type of attention is triggered by the external stimuli, and opposite to goal—oriented attention, typical to processing of the task, which, when performed for too long, leads to mental fatigue. Bottom-up attention is the central concept of the Attention Restoration Theory, according to which contact with the natural environments is considered a triggering factor leading to restoration of depleted attention capacity and recovery from mental fatigue. In previous studies attention restoration was conceptually linked to ‘fascination'—a key-component of restorative environments, according to ART56,57. The Attention Restoration Index in this study was computed with Beta power values from the left (electrodes FT7 and F7) and from the right temporal lobes (electrodes FT8 and F8) were extracted. Then asymmetry values were calculated using the common formula (right-left)/(right + left) (e.g.58).
Site selection
Six urban green spaces scenes were selected at two locations in Singapore. Each scene included different natural and built elements and composition types and had previously been annotated by four landscape architecture experts, blinded to hypothesis, using the CLM. Landscape scenes with their scores are presented in the Fig. 2.
Three of the six scenes (S1.1, S1.2 and S1.3) were located within a Therapeutic Garden—a part of a larger urban park named HortPark. This site was selected deliberately due to previous research showing health benefits of contact with nature conducted there, including therapeutic horticulture sessions to promote wellbeing59 and reduction of depressive mood among healthy individuals60 and depressed patients61. Two scenes selected at the Therapeutic Garden (S1.1 and S1.2) scored above the 4.45 point in CLM—the threshold indicating highly contemplative scene, according to previous study34. One scene within that garden (S1.3) was selected to represent below-threshold score (3.78 points). S1.1, S1.2 and S1.3 contained unique combinations of physical attributes and had relatively different CLM scores for each of the seven features (see Fig. 2). For example, S1.2 was the only site with a full score in the component Layers of the Landscape due to the far distance view over the hills, and view towards the sky wasn’t canopied there. On the contrary, S1.3 was the only one overlooking main constructed walking path with a relatively lower scores in Landform and Compatibility.
Other scenes (S2.1, S2.2 and S2.3) were located within the roof garden in a public residential estate called Casa Clementi. This site represents a contemporary style of design of public neighbourhoods (developed by the Housing Development Board or HDB)—places where approximately 80% of Singaporeans live62. HDB estates are open to public and the green spaces within these estates are an important part of the city green infrastructure63. Scenes selected in this location, like the ones in Therapeutic Garden, vary in terms of CLM scoring. On average they have fewer contemplative values, due to the built-up elements and tall buildings dominating almost every view, which limits the reorientation of the viewer from city to nature- like landscape. The highest CLM score here was 3.35 points, at site S2.3 which was overlooking playground and manicured greenery partly covering the façade of the building. The lowest CLM score (2.4 points) was at the view from the void deck of one of the blocks (see Fig. 2).
Procedures
The experiment consisted of three sessions, one in the laboratory (first one) within the university premises and two remaining ones in the naturalistic settings in the urban green spaces. Data was collected between March 2019 and September 2020, during morning or late afternoon hours of the working week. Experimental sessions were scheduled individually, and the gap between sessions was kept under 30 days.
At the beginning of the first session (in the laboratory), participants signed the informed consent, and had a portable EEG cap adjusted on their head. They were then instructed to sit comfortably on the chair and passively watch the presentation of six fixed-frame videos repeating three times each, in a fully randomized order by the presentation software—Psychopy 3 (2002–2018 Jonathan Peirce, UK64). Videos were displayed on a 108 × 178 cm roll-up screen positioned about 200 cm in front of their eyes; projected using the HD29 Darbee Optoma Home Theatre Full HD projector with 1080p (1920 × 1080) screen resolution. There was a 60 s long resting state preceding the video presentations, where participants were looking at a grey, blank screen. Videos were 20 s long (passive task) with a 15 s pause in between, at which time a fixation cross was displayed on the screen (2° of visual angle). The natural sound, as recorded, was also played with the video using standard PC audio speakers placed near the projector, behind the participant’s chair. During the experiment, a daylight-imitating lamp (with a light hue of 5500 K) was turned on (Fig. 3A). After stimuli presentation, the data acquisition cap was removed, and participants were invited to complete the SAM for the individual scenes, BDI-II and Socio-demographic questionnaires. The whole procedure took approximately 50 min. Figure 3C shows the experimental laboratory setup.
During each naturalistic session, one site with three scenes was measured (order randomized65). Upon arriving at site, participants were seated on a portable chair facing the selected scene and EEG apparatus was adjusted on their head. Then participants were instructed to put on the white goggles to block the view and relax, while equipment was calibrated, and raw signal recording was initiated. After 1 min recording of the resting state, the participants were asked to remove goggles and passively observe the landscape scene in front for 1 min. Once this was completed, the 1 min resting state with goggle on and 1 min scene watching was repeated for the same scene. This process was repeated for all the three scenes, using the same chair, and the order of the scenes was randomized. After removal of the EEG apparatus, participants were asked to evaluate the scenes with the SAM Valence and Arousal questionnaire (Fig. 3B). The duration of an outdoors session took between 30 and 45 min. Participants were allowed to consume water, but not food, between the scenes. Environmental variables (temperature, humidity, brightness and noise) were recorded with 4-in-1 environment meter (CEM, DT-8820) at each scene at each session to control for confounding variables. The experiment setup in the naturalistic setting is presented in Fig. 3D.
EEG data processing
Data was processed offline in Brain Analyzer 2 software (Brain Products GmbH, Munich, Germany). The raw signal was filtered with a 50 Hz notch filter, a low-pass at 40 Hz and a high-pass at 0.5 Hz (all were zero phase shift Butterworth filters, order 2). Channels were referenced to an average reference of 16 electrodes and visually inspected for noisy or missing channels. Topographic interpolation of noisy or lost channels was performed where necessary. Ocular artefacts (eye blinks and eye movement) were captured by Independent Component Analysis (ICA) and removed from the data. The signal was epoch time-locked to the video onset (0–20 s). All data underwent Fast Fourier Transform and were output as power. Power values were then averaged over each condition and theta (4–7 Hz), alpha (8–13 Hz) and beta (14–30 Hz) bands power extracted. To compute the wakeful relaxation indices, alpha power values from the viewing condition, from three pairs of frontal electrodes (AFp1-AFp2; AFF5h-AFF6h and F7-F8) was averaged. To compute the mindfulness index, theta power values from the viewing condition, from three pairs of frontal electrodes (AFp1-AFp2; AFF5h-AFF6h and F7-F8) was averaged. To compute the attention restoration index beta power values from the left (electrodes FT7 and F7) and from the right temporal lobe (electrodes FT8 and F8) were extracted. Then asymmetry values were calculated using the common formula (right-left)/(right + left) (e.g.58). Separate dataset was created for the laboratory and for the naturalistic setting for each of three power bands.
Statistical analyses
The CLM scores as well as brain pattern activity scores and self-reported affect scores did not show normal distribution and required non-parametric analyses to account for skewed data. Brain activity data were normalised during data processing to derive alpha, beta and theta scores, and this normalisation did not improve skewness. Spearman's rank-order correlation was run to investigate the relationships between CLM scores and brain pattern activity, as well as self-reported affect scores. A Wilcoxon signed-rank test was conducted to determine differences between outdoor and laboratory datasets between each of the five psychophysiological measurements. We conducted a thorough analysis of environmental conditions, noise, temperature, humidity, brightness, and total mood, and individual situational confounders (alcohol intake in the last 24 h, sleep quality and duration) in relation to the study outcomes. These factors were not linked to the study outcomes and were not included as a part of analysis.
Results
Descriptive findings
Due to five participants failing to follow up with all the sessions, only 74 participants (44 females), were included in the study. The average age was 38 ± 17.42 years old. Most of the participants were Chinese. And a large majority had university level of education. The average level of depressive scores, as measured with BDI-II was 8.43 ± 7.31, which indicates overall minimal level of depression. Details about the sample are illustrated in Table 1.
Table 1.
Frequency | Percent | Min | Max | Mean | Std. Dev. | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 21 | 79 | 38.00 | 17.42 | ||
Youth (18–25) | 25 | 33.8 | ||||
Adults (26–44) | 26 | 35.1 | ||||
Older adults (45–59) | 9 | 12.2 | ||||
Seniors (60+) | 14 | 18.9 | ||||
Severity of depression | ||||||
Total BDI-II | 0 | 36 | 8.43 | 7.31 | ||
Minimal 0–13 pt | 61 | 82.4 | ||||
Mild 14–19 pt | 6 | 8.1 | ||||
Moderate 20–28 pt | 6 | 8.1 | ||||
Severe 29–63 pt | 1 | 1.4 | ||||
Gender | ||||||
Female | 44 | 59.5 | ||||
Male | 30 | 40.5 | ||||
Ethnicity | ||||||
Chinese | 49 | 41.9 | ||||
Indian | 14 | 12.0 | ||||
Malay | 2 | 1.8 | ||||
Others | 9 | 7.7 | ||||
Education | ||||||
Below tertiary | 17 | 23 | ||||
Tertiary and above | 57 | 77 | ||||
Vision | ||||||
Corrected | 47 | 40.2 | ||||
Normal | 27 | 23.1 | ||||
Hearing | ||||||
Corrected | 3 | 2.6 | ||||
Normal | 71 | 60.7 |
Does CLM predict brain pattern activity and self-reported affect? Which specific CLM features contribute to these positive outcomes?
CLM scores and Alpha band
There was a significant positive association between Alpha power band recorded outdoors and total CLM score, as well as in the sub-scores Layers of the Landscape, Archetypal Elements, Character of Peace & Silence, and Vegetation, in order of decreasing rho value. Alpha, however, was not significantly related to Color and Light, Landform and Compatibility. There were no significant correlations between Alpha power band and CLM score, or any of the sub-scores in the laboratory settings (Table 2).
Table 2.
CLM | LAY | LAN | VEG | COL | CPB | ARE | CPS | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Brain activity (EEG) | Alpha (out) | rs | 0.121* | 0.145** | 0.065 | 0.121* | 0.086 | 0.062 | 0.135** | 0.130** |
p | 0.013 | 0.003 | 0.179 | 0.013 | 0.078 | 0.200 | 0.005 | 0.007 | ||
Alpha (lab) | rs | 0.000 | 0.003 | − 0.004 | 0.000 | − 0.001 | − 0.010 | 0.001 | 0.008 | |
p | 0.997 | 0.954 | 0.938 | 0.997 | 0.980 | 0.835 | 0.978 | 0.875 | ||
Beta (out) | rs | − 0.005 | − 0.009 | 0.003 | − 0.005 | 0.002 | − 0.002 | − 0.007 | − 0.006 | |
p | 0.923 | 0.851 | 0.952 | 0.923 | 0.969 | 0.973 | 0.885 | 0.903 | ||
Beta (lab) | rs | − 0.019 | − 0.026 | − 0.005 | − 0.019 | − 0.009 | − 0.005 | − 0.023 | − 0.025 | |
p | 0.700 | 0.586 | 0.923 | 0.700 | 0.856 | 0.916 | 0.638 | 0.606 | ||
Theta (out) | rs | 0.143** | 0.168** | 0.084 | 0.143** | 0.104* | 0.086 | 0.158** | 0.149** | |
p | 0.003 | 0.001 | 0.083 | 0.003 | 0.032 | 0.075 | 0.001 | 0.002 | ||
Theta (lab) | rs | − 0.009 | − 0.003 | − 0.015 | − 0.009 | − 0.012 | − 0.019 | − 0.006 | − 0.001 | |
p | 0.852 | 0.949 | 0.753 | 0.852 | 0.802 | 0.697 | 0.899 | 0.980 | ||
Self-reported Affect (SAM) | Valence (out) | rs | 0.199** | 0.237** | 0.116* | 0.199** | 0.151** | 0.087 | 0.221** | 0.239** |
p | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.017 | 0.000 | 0.002 | 0.074 | 0.000 | 0.000 | ||
Valence (lab) | rs | 0.357** | 0.344** | 0.288** | 0.357** | 0.329** | 0.221** | 0.355** | 0.358** | |
p | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.000 | ||
Arousal (out) | rs | 0.152** | 0.156** | 0.125* | 0.152** | 0.143** | 0.087 | 0.156** | 0.174** | |
p | 0.002 | 0.001 | 0.010 | 0.002 | 0.003 | 0.073 | 0.001 | 0.000 | ||
Arousal (lab) | rs | 0.148** | 0.140** | 0.122* | 0.148** | 0.146** | 0.075 | 0.146** | 0.150** | |
p | 0.002 | 0.004 | 0.012 | 0.002 | 0.003 | 0.122 | 0.003 | 0.002 |
Significant values are in [bold].
LAY, layers of the landscape; LAN, landform; VEG, vegetation; COL, color and light; CPB, compatibility; ARE, Archetypal elements; CPS, character of peace and silence; rs, Spearman’s rho; CLM, total contemplative landscape model score.
**Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).
*Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).
CLM scores and Beta band
There were no significant associations between Beta and total CLM score or any of the CLM sub-scores, for both outdoors and laboratory measurements (Table 2).
CLM scores and Theta band
Theta scores recorded outdoors were significantly positively associated with total CLM score, as well as the sub-scores Layers of the Landscape, Archetypal Elements, Character of Peace & Silence, Vegetation and Colour & Light, in order of decreasing rho value. Theta, however, was not significantly associated with Landform and Compatibility. There were not significant correlations between Theta power band and CLM score, or any of the sub-scores in the laboratory setting (Table 2).
CLM scores and Valence
There were significant positive associations of the SAM Valence recorded outdoors as well as in the laboratory with total CLM, as well as all of the landscape sub-scores, Character of Peace & Silence, Layers of the Landscape, Archetypal Elements, Vegetation, Colour & Light, Landform, and Compatibility. The only exception was for Combability measured outdoors (Table 2).
CLM scores and Arousal
There were significant positive associations of SAM Arousal recorded outdoors with total CLM, as well as the sub-scores Character of Peace & Silence, Layers of the Landscape, Archetypal Elements, Vegetation, Colour & Light and Landform, in order of decreasing rho value. SAM Arousal, however, was not significantly associated with Compatibility scores. At the laboratory setting, there were significant positive associations with total CLM, as well as the sub-scores Character of Peace & Silence, Layers of the Landscape, Vegetation, Archetypal Elements, Colour & Light and Landform, in order of decreasing rho value. SAM Arousal, however, was not significantly associated with Compatibility scores (Table 2).
What are the similarities and differences in outcomes measured in laboratory and outdoors?
A Wilcoxon Signed-Ranks Test indicated that the median Alpha, Beta and Theta values, as well as SAM Arousal scores were significantly lower in the laboratory compared to the naturalistic setting (Table 3). There was no statistically significant difference between the median SAM Valence scores from the laboratory and the outdoors (p = 0.22, Table 3).
Table 3.
Mean | Median | Test statistics | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Z | p | |||
Alpha out | 6.32 | 4.64 | 53,504.00 | 0.032* |
Alpha lab | 9.01 | 4.59 | ||
Beta out | 0.19 | 0.20 | 35,952.00 | < 0.005* |
Beta lab | 0.08 | 0.11 | ||
Theta out | 15.16 | 11.29 | 37,517.00 | < 0.005* |
Theta lab | 14.52 | 8.05 | ||
Valence out | 0.92 | 1.00 | 13,793.00 | 0.215 |
Valence lab | 0.86 | 1.00 | ||
Arousal out | 0.08 | 0.00 | 12,139.00 | < 0.005* |
Arousal lab | − 0.26 | 0.00 |
*Denotes statistically significant difference.
Discussion
The primary goal of this study was to explore the CLM and its specific features association with positive affect and brain activity consistent with relaxation, attention restoration and mindfulness. The secondary goal was to compare these associations in the laboratory and naturalistic outdoor setting.
The findings of this study show that naturalistic exposure to landscapes with higher CLM scores is associated with greater frontal alpha and theta activity, indicative of higher mindfulness46,47 and wakeful relaxation39. This association was only observed when participants were exposed to naturalistic settings and was not observed while video exposures in the laboratory. These findings are important for the specificity of urban living characterized by overload of stimulation leading to cognitive strain1,9—exposed to the highly contemplative landscape, brain activation can simply slow down through increased frontal Alpha and Theta activity, leading to relaxation of that cognitive strain. Higher CLM scores were associated with higher scores for self-reported valence and arousal in both laboratory and outdoor settings indicative of stronger positive emotions, which also are critical for the well-being of the urbanites. Our findings demonstrate that UGS features most strongly associated with brain activity are Character of Peace and Silence, Layers of the Landscape, Archetypal Elements and Vegetation. Our findings also suggest that there are differences between these associations depending on the exposure setting i.e., the associations are stronger in the outdoor setting as compared to the laboratory setting (Table 2).
CLM score was not associated with Beta temporal asymmetry linked with the Attention Restoration. This may be due to Beta being a high frequency powerband (14–30 Hz), and unlike lower frequency bands (Alpha 8–13 Hz and Theta 4–7 Hz), it is associated with cognitive performance, solving of the tasks and information processing. It seems that this pattern may not be achieved through passive observation of landscapes, as opposed to activities performed in nature. Previous studies with self-reported measures of attention restoration, called Restorative Outcome Scale (ROS66), found effect on attention restoration after 30 min–2 h of walking in the park or forest, and not after passive exposure67,68. Here we used passive exposure of short duration which might have been insufficient to induce the expected patterns, we also used objective (not self-reported) measures). Interestingly, it was previously found that level of tranquillity (calmness, serenity, peacefulness) of the scene is a major component of the attention restoration theory69 but also of relaxation70–it then may be possible that what environmental psychologists refer to as Attention Restoration has more to do with the slow brain frequencies rather than Beta waves, associated with attention and cognitive processing. More research is required to unravel the neuroelectric signatures of Attention Restoration as defined by the Attention Restoration Theory.
Features of urban landscapes and participant outcomes
The landscape features mainly contributing to increased frontal Alpha activity pattern associated with the Wakeful Relaxation, appeared to be Layers of the Landscape, Archetypal Elements, Character of Peace and Silence and Vegetation respectively (Table 2). The observed significant associations were positive, meaning that higher scores within these landscape categories the greater Alpha power in the frontal cortex. Landscape scenes scoring high in Layers of the Landscape category are characterized by a long-distance view and visibility of fore, middle and background within the view. Looking afar has been associated with the psychological comfort and sense of personal freedom; physical distance of the observer may create a sense of psychological distance–seeing things from afar, without overly focusing on details. In other words, seeing a “bigger picture” or sense of “being away”71. This can lead to stress reduction through distracting from rumination and fostering contemplation72, and aligns with the environmental psychology Prospect-Refuge theory which posits that humans derive feelings of safety and pleasure from exposure to environments offering both far-away views and a sense of enclosure73. Presence of the Archetypal Elements in the landscape also was highly correlated with the Wakeful Relaxation. Such elements in our experimental, settings were single tree, forest and path. It seems that the explicit presence of these objects that, according to Jungian psychoanalysis evoke a subconscious emotional response in all humans, corresponded with relaxation patterns31,74. In the traditional Visual Resource Management landscape quality assessment one of the assessment categories was “scarcity”—present of distinct and rare elements, which makes the scene more valuable28. Perhaps the presence of Archetypal Elements in the scene induced the relaxation pattern in the brain because of the way that they stand out from the landscape, however the specific mechanisms of that being the case are unknown, and further research would be required. Moreover, it is unknown whether other Archetypal Elements such as waterfall or stone, not present in our experimental settings would also correlate with the frontal Alpha power. High CLM scores for Character of Peace and Silence were also highly related to Wakeful Relaxation, which is not surprising, as it directly corresponds to spaces for resting, comfort and solitude. Higher scores for Vegetation (more species diversity, plants looking spontaneous, natural and changing with time) was positively associated with the relaxation patterns in the brain. This observation can be explained by the Biophilia hypothesis, according to which humans feel at home surrounded by unthreatening nature, as well as range of experimental studies reporting less stress and more positive affective response while looking at plants vs looking at other objects75. Color and Light, Compatibility of the design and Landform scores of the scenes were not found to be associated with the frontal Alpha power oscillations. One of the potential reasons could be that the other features of landscapes elicited stronger responses compared to these features, thus reducing the impact that these characteristics had on brain activity. Further research is required where these features are tested in isolation, in the absence of other, perhaps more influential features.
Landscape features mainly contributing to the frontal Theta activity associated with the Mindfulness state, were again: Layers of the Landscape, Archetypal Elements, Character of Peace & Silence and Vegetation. The explanation for this would be consistent with the previous discussion. Aside from these, a feature that contributed less, but still significantly was Colour & Light. Mindfulness, by its definition is associated with being attentive to the present moment, and lack of interpretation or judgement of the experienced phenomena, it involves attention and processing, it is then more than relaxation48. Color & Light category of CLM is the one involving the motion and feeling that scenery one perceives is alive (passage of the sun across the sky, moving shades of leaves on the ground), thus, being attentive to these subtle changes in the space can stimulate the Mindfulness, but not quite the Wakeful Relaxation, as results of Alpha analysis suggests. Landform and Compatibility were two features, which did not significantly predict the Mindfulness pattern. It means that the brain may not “need” to detect the undulating topography or diverse skyline or explicit spatial harmony in order to trigger that specific pattern of brain activity.
The contemplative landscapes feature most strongly correlating with the self-reported Valence and Arousal scores was the Character of Peace and Silence, suggesting that the higher level of that feature as recognized by experts corresponded with the participants’ self-reported positive affect. Moreover, participants reported high SAM Valence of landscape scenes with explicit presence of Archetypal Element(s) and scenes with naturally looking, diverse Vegetation. Interestingly, the SAM Valence and Arousal recorded outdoors did not significantly correlate with the Compatibility scores. Compatibility of a landscape scene, among its other characteristics, depends on the scale, form and composition of the objects within in the view30. In the case of the photographs and videos the viewing frame is limited by distinct edges while in the actual park there is no such sharp edge of the view, therefore the Compatibility of the view may have been more difficult to grasp by participants—hence the affect associated with the view may be more difficult to assess within this specific subscale.
Naturalistic and laboratory setting
Our findings suggest the possible weakness of study designs which employ laboratory-only results highlighted in previous research. According to our results, the brainwave oscillations as well as the self-reported Arousal scores were different during the laboratory and outdoor experience. Only the self-reported Valence scores were comparable between the two settings, which corroborates previous research in landscape preference and perceived restorativeness76,77. This suggests that the self-reports about the emotional response to landscape settings, collected indoors, based on the photographic or videographic representations can well represent the Valence of emotions (characterized as pleasantness/"good"-ness or averseness/"bad"-ness of a perceived emotion), that would be experienced in the naturalistic setting. In the case of affective Arousal (which corresponds to the intensity of the emotion), it tends to be higher during the exposition to a naturalistic setting than to the photo representation. The above findings are likely related to the immersivity of each experience, looking at the picture or watching a movie is not providing the same level of immersiveness as when being present in the real site. However, people may be able to recognize the landscape from the picture and imagine how they would feel like being there aka, what emotions (positive or negative) they would carry towards the scene, hence they are capable of scoring the scene representation in terms of Valence as they would be when in a real place. This finding can be particularly useful in the conceptual, participatory or competition phases of new UGS developments—if the visualisations of the UGS the proposal have high contemplative values they will likely induce more positive emotional response in raters. But their self-reported Arousal as well as the brainwave scores seem to not be so comparable between the laboratory and naturalistc setting. Future studies could explore more immersive methods of laboratory-based experiments (e.g., 3-dimensional virtual reality) and compare them with in-situ exposure.
The findings of this study are correlational in their nature, therefore should be interpreted with caution. Due to a relatively small number of assessed scenes and lack of representation of other landscape elements described in CLM, assessment of individual features of landscapes in absence of other features was not possible. Due to the recruitment methodology and a relatively small sample size, the sample is not representative of the local population, hence generalisability of findings cannot be ascertained. Also, only the landscapes typical for Singaporean UGS were considered. The generalizability of the findings to other climatic and geographical as well as the urban morphology contexts remains unknown, leaving the room for more research in other countries. Finally, time of stimulation was different in the laboratory (3 × 20 s) and outdoor (2 × 60 s) conditions. There is no gold standard protocol for urban landscape exposure in the naturalistic and outdoor settings. These parameters were selected based on the environmental conditions in Singapore and based on the previous experiences with similar settings and expertise of the study team. To avoid habituation bias and participant fatigue, participants were exposed to 3 shorter stimulations, in the indoor condition. To reduce walking in hot and humid environmental conditions, with the EEG equipment, participants were exposed to 2 longer lasting stimulations outdoors. Due to these methodological differences, comparison of indoor and outdoor stimulations should be interpreted with caution.
Conclusions
The study findings can help guide planning and design of the UGS with consideration to mental health and well-being of visitors, e.g., therapeutic gardens. CLM can be a useful tool to highlight the existing healthy UGS exposures and design new ones, filling the gap in knowledge about the quality aspects of the UGS design for health. Our study expands on the notion that the quality of elements within the perceived scene is linked to the self-reported affect as well as subconscious brain activity. Our findings suggest that UGS that offer visibility of the Layers of the Landscape, contain predominant Character of Peace and Silence, with presence of the Archetypal Elements and diverse, naturalistic Vegetation could be the most valuable from the standpoint of visitors’ wellbeing. Examples of strategies to include in design are:
opening the views to far-away scenery so that the visitor can see both nearby and faraway objects;
emphasizing visibility of the aerial perspective, where faraway objects are perceived bluer and more blurred due to volume of the air between them and the eye of the observer;
creating visual and noise buffers dividing the garden from the city environment, planning for the comfortable seating for solitary resting;
highlighting the existing archetypal element with design so that it dominates the view (e.g. by clearing the surrounding of the solitary tree to make its silhouette more distinct).
incorporating more naturalistic planting plans (seemingly planted by nature) including spontaneous and diverse vegetation that displays seasonal and diurnal changes.
Historically, UGS have been envisioned as public spaces for urban populations to seek fresh air and respite, yet the current literature is limited in informing the specific guidelines for design for mental health promotion. Urban planning and design fields should recognise the tremendous mental health benefits that may be harvested from contact with adequately designed and maintained green spaces and include strategies for targeted psychophysiological responses in UGS planning in the national mental health promotion strategies.
Author contributions
A.O.-G.: conceptualization and investigation, methodology, data curation, A.O.-G., A.F.: validation. A.O.-G., A.F.: formal analysis. R.H., A.S.: resources. A.O.-G. and A.S.: writing—original draft preparation. A.O.-G., A.F., R.H.: writing—review and editing. A.O.-G. and A.S.: visualization. R.H.: supervision. R.H. and A.S.: project administration and funding acquisition. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding
This work was supported by Ministry of National Development—Singapore (Grant no. R-722-000-010-490), Institute for Health Innovation and Technology, National University of Singapore (Grant no. A-0001415-09-00), NUS, Department of Psychological Medicine (Grant no. R-177-000-100-001/R-177-000-003-001/ R177000702733).
Data availability
The datasets generated during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Olszewska-Guizzo A, Fogel A, Benjumea D, Tahsin N. Sustainable Policies and Practices in Energy, Environment and Health Research. Springer; 2022. pp. 223–243. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Gascon M, et al. Mental health benefits of long-term exposure to residential green and blue spaces: A systematic review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2015;12:4354–4379. doi: 10.3390/ijerph120404354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Houlden V, Weich S, Porto-de-Albuquerque J, Jarvis S, Rees K. The relationship between greenspace and the mental wellbeing of adults: A systematic review. PLoS ONE. 2018;13:3000. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0203000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Hung S-H, Chang C-Y. Health benefits of evidence-based biophilic-designed environments: A review. J. People Plants Env. 2021;24:1–16. doi: 10.11628/ksppe.2021.24.1.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Berman MG, Jonides J, Kaplan S. The cognitive benefits of interacting with nature. Psychol. Sci. 2008;19:1207–1212. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02225.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Kaplan S. Meditation, restoration, and the management of mental fatigue. Environ. Behav. 2001;33:480–506. doi: 10.1177/00139160121973106. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Ulrich RS, et al. Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 1991;11:201–230. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(05)80184-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Kellert SR, Wilson EO. The Biophilia Hypothesis. Island Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Stack K, Shultis J. Implications of attention restoration theory for leisure planners and managers. Leisure/Loisir. 2013;37:1–16. doi: 10.1080/14927713.2013.776747. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Steel Z, et al. The global prevalence of common mental disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis 1980–2013. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2014;43:476–493. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyu038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Mueller DP. The current status of urban-rural differences in psychiatric disorder. An emerging trend for depression. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 1981;169:18–27. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198101000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Peen J, Schoevers RA, Beekman AT, Dekker J. The current status of urban-rural differences in psychiatric disorders. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2010;121:84–93. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01438.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Taylor L, Hochuli DF. Defining greenspace: Multiple uses across multiple disciplines. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2017;158:25–38. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2016.09.024. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.en K Staats H. Restorative Environments The Oxford Handbook of Environmental and Conservation Psychology. 445. Oxford University Press; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Wood L, Hooper P, Foster S, Bull F. Public green spaces and positive mental health–investigating the relationship between access, quantity and types of parks and mental wellbeing. Health Place. 2017;48:63–71. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2017.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Tsunetsugu Y, et al. Physiological and psychological effects of viewing urban forest landscapes assessed by multiple measurements. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2013;113:90–93. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2013.01.014. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Gidlow CJ, et al. Where to put your best foot forward: Psycho-physiological responses to walking in natural and urban environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 2016;45:22–29. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.11.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Lee J. Experimental study on the health benefits of garden landscape. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2017;14:829. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14070829. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Fuller RA, Irvine KN, Devine-Wright P, Warren PH, Gaston KJ. Psychological benefits of greenspace increase with biodiversity. Biol. Lett. 2007;3:390–394. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2007.0149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Thompson CW, Aspinall P, Bell S. Innovative Approaches to Researching Landscape and Health: Open Space: People Space 2. Routledge; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Tsutsumi M, Nogaki H, Shimizu Y, Stone TE, Kobayashi T. Individual reactions to viewing preferred video representations of the natural environment: A comparison of mental and physical reactions. Jpn. J. Nurs. Sci. 2017;14:3–12. doi: 10.1111/jjns.12131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Grazuleviciene R, et al. Tracking restoration of park and urban street settings in coronary artery disease patients. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2016;13:550. doi: 10.3390/ijerph13060550. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Bostancı, S. H. In New Approaches to Spatial Planning and Design (ed Murat Özyavuz) Ch. 32, 435–451 (Peter Lang, 2019).
- 24.Daniel, T. C. Measuring Landscape Esthetics: The Scenic Beauty Estimation Method, vol. 167 (Department of Agriculture, Forest Service, Rocky Mountain Forest and Range…, 1976).
- 25.Bacon, W. R. In (eds Elsner G. H. et al) Technical Coordinators. Proceedings of our national landscape: A conference on applied techniques for analysis and management of the visual resource [Incline Village, Nev., April 23–25, 1979]. Gen. Tech. Rep. PSW-GTR-35. Berkeley, CA. Pacific Southwest Forest and Range Exp. Stn., Forest Service, US Department of Agriculture 660–665 (1979).
- 26.Gavrilidis AA, Ciocănea CM, Niţă MR, Onose DA, Năstase II. Urban landscape quality index—planning tool for evaluating urban landscapes and improving the quality of life. Procedia Environ. Sci. 2016;32:155–167. doi: 10.1016/j.proenv.2016.03.020. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Knobel P, et al. Development of the urban green space quality assessment tool (RECITAL) Urban For. Urban Green. 2021;57:126895. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2020.126895. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Bacon, W. R. & Dell, J. National Forest Landscape Management (Forest Service, US Department of Agriculture, 1973).
- 29.Kaplan R, Kaplan S, Ryan R. With People in Mind: Design and Management of Everyday Nature. Island Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Smardon R, Palmer J, Felleman JP. Foundations for Visual Project Analysis. Wiley; 1986. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Jung CG. Man and His Symbols Garden City. Doubleday and Co; 1964. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Olszewska A, Marques PF, Ryan RL, Barbosa F. What makes a landscape contemplative? Env. Plan. B Urban Anal. City Sci. 2016;45:7–25. doi: 10.1177/0265813516660716. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Tarkka IM, Hallett M. Cortical topography of premotor and motor potentials preceding self-paced, voluntary movement of dominant and non-dominant hands. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1990;75:36–43. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(90)90150-I. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Olszewska-Guizzo A, Paiva TO, Barbosa F. Effects of 3D contemplative landscape videos on brain activity in a passive exposure EEG experiment. Front. Psychiatry. 2018;9:317. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00317. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Bradley MM, Lang PJ. Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry. 1994;25:49–59. doi: 10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Beck depression inventory-II. San Antonio. 1996;78:490–498. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Ferree TC, Luu P, Russell GS, Tucker DM. Scalp electrode impedance, infection risk, and EEG data quality. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2001;112:536–544. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(00)00533-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Stroganova TA, Orekhova EV. EEG and infant states. Infant EEG Event-Relat. Potentials. 2007;251:280. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Cacioppo JT, Tassinary LG, Berntson G. Handbook of Psychophysiology. Cambridge University Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Ulrich RS. Natural versus urban scenes: Some psychophysiological effects. Environ. Behav. 1981;13:523–556. doi: 10.1177/0013916581135001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Choi Y, Kim M, Chun C. Measurement of occupants' stress based on electroencephalograms (EEG) in twelve combined environments. Build. Environ. 2015;88:65–72. doi: 10.1016/j.buildenv.2014.10.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Gorji MAH, Davanloo AA, Heidarigorji AM. The efficacy of relaxation training on stress, anxiety, and pain perception in hemodialysis patients. Indian J. Nephrol. 2014;24:356. doi: 10.4103/0971-4065.132998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Cahn BR, Polich J. Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies. Psychol. Bull. 2006;132:180. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Gruzelier J. A theory of alpha/theta neurofeedback, creative performance enhancement, long distance functional connectivity and psychological integration. Cogn. Process. 2009;10:101–109. doi: 10.1007/s10339-008-0248-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Vecchiato G, et al. Neurophysiological correlates of embodiment and motivational factors during the perception of virtual architectural environments. Cogn. Process. 2015;16:425–429. doi: 10.1007/s10339-015-0725-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Lagopoulos J, et al. Increased theta and alpha EEG activity during nondirective meditation. J. Altern. Complement. Med. 2009;15:1187–1192. doi: 10.1089/acm.2009.0113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Wascher E, et al. Frontal theta activity reflects distinct aspects of mental fatigue. Biol. Psychol. 2014;96:57–65. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Kabat-Zinn J. Mindfulness. Mindfulness. 2015;6:1481–1483. doi: 10.1007/s12671-015-0456-x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 49.McGarrigle T, Walsh CA. Mindfulness, self-care, and wellness in social work: Effects of contemplative training. J. Relig. Spiritual. Soc. Work Soc. Thought. 2011;30:212–233. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Grossman P, Niemann L, Schmidt S, Walach H. Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health benefits: A meta-analysis. J. Psychosom. Res. 2004;57:35–43. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00573-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Bailey AW, Allen G, Herndon J, Demastus C. Cognitive benefits of walking in natural versus built environments. World Leisure J. 2018;60:293–305. doi: 10.1080/16078055.2018.1445025. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Qin J, Zhou X, Sun C, Leng H, Lian Z. Influence of green spaces on environmental satisfaction and physiological status of urban residents. Urban For. Urban Green. 2013;12:490–497. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2013.05.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Kolb B, Whishaw IQ. Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology. Freeman; 1990. [Google Scholar]
- 54.Milner B. Visual recognition and recall after right temporal-lobe excision in man. Neuropsychologia. 1968;6:191–209. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(68)90019-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002;3:201–215. doi: 10.1038/nrn755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Chang C-Y, Chen P-K. Human response to window views and indoor plants in the workplace. HortScience. 2005;40:1354–1359. doi: 10.21273/HORTSCI.40.5.1354. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Herzog TR, Black AM, Fountaine KA, Knotts DJ. Reflection and attentional recovery as distinctive benefits of restorative environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 1997;17:165–170. doi: 10.1006/jevp.1997.0051. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Baehr E, Rosenfeld JP, Baehr R. Clinical use of an alpha asymmetry neurofeedback protocol in the treatment of mood disorders: Follow-up study one to five years post therapy. J. Neurother. 2001;4:11–18. doi: 10.1300/J184v04n04_03. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Sia A, et al. Nature-based activities improve the well-being of older adults. Sci. Rep. 2020;10:1–8. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-74828-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Olszewska-Guizzo A, Sia A, Fogel A, Ho R. Can exposure to certain urban green spaces trigger frontal alpha asymmetry in the brain?—Preliminary findings from a passive task EEG study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2020;17:394. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17020394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Olszewska-Guizzo A, et al. Therapeutic garden with contemplative features induces desirable changes in mood and B rain activity in depressed adults. Front. Psychiatry. 2021 doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.757056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Tan, S. B., Vignesh, L. N. & Donald, L. Public Housing in Singapore: Examining Fundamental Shifts (Lee Kuan Yew School of Public Policy, National University of Singapore, 2014).
- 63.Tan PY. Nature, Place & People: Forging Connections Through Neighbourhood Landscape Design. World Scientific Publishing Co.; 2018. [Google Scholar]
- 64.Peirce J, et al. PsychoPy2: Experiments in behavior made easy. Behav. Res. Methods. 2019;51:195–203. doi: 10.3758/s13428-018-01193-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Edwards AL. Balanced Latin-square designs in psychological research. Am. J. Psychol. 1951;64:598–603. doi: 10.2307/1418200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Korpela KM, Ylén M, Tyrväinen L, Silvennoinen H. Determinants of restorative experiences in everyday favorite places. Health Place. 2008;14:636–652. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2007.10.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Ojala A, Korpela K, Tyrväinen L, Tiittanen P, Lanki T. Restorative effects of urban green environments and the role of urban-nature orientedness and noise sensitivity: A field experiment. Health Place. 2019;55:59–70. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2018.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Tyrväinen L, et al. The influence of urban green environments on stress relief measures: A field experiment. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014;38:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.12.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Herzog TR, Barnes GJ. Tranquility and preference revisited. J. Environ. Psychol. 1999;19:171–181. doi: 10.1006/jevp.1998.0109. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Neale C, et al. The impact of walking in different urban environments on brain activity in older people. Cities Health. 2020;4:94–106. doi: 10.1080/23748834.2019.1619893. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Kaplan R, Kaplan S. The Experience of Nature: A Psychological Perspective. CUP Archive; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- 72.Treib, M. In Contemporary Landscapes of Contemplation (ed Rebecca Krinke) 27–49 (Routledge, 2005).
- 73.Appleton J. The Experience of Landscape. Wiley Chichester; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- 74.Grahn P, Ottosson J, Uvnäs-Moberg K. The oxytocinergic system as a mediator of anti-stress and instorative effects induced by nature: The calm and connection theory. Front. Psychol. 2021;2021:12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.617814. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Hartig T, Mang M, Evans GW. Restorative effects of natural environment experiences. Environ. Behav. 1991;23:3–26. doi: 10.1177/0013916591231001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Stamps Iii AE. Use of photographs to simulate environments: A meta-analysis. Percept. Mot. Skills. 1990;71:907–913. doi: 10.2466/pms.1990.71.3.907. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Menardo E, Brondino M, Hall R, Pasini M. Restorativeness in natural and urban environments: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Rep. 2021;124:417–437. doi: 10.1177/0033294119884063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.