Abstract
Inhibitory control is a key executive function that limits unnecessary thoughts and actions, enabling an organism to appropriately execute goal-driven behaviors. The efficiency of this inhibitory capacity declines with normal aging or in neurodegenerative dementias similar to memory or other cognitive functions. Acetylcholine signaling is crucial for executive function and also diminishes with aging. Acetylcholine’s contribution to the aging- or dementia-related decline in inhibitory control, however, remains elusive. We addressed this in Drosophila using a Go/No-Go task that measures inhibition capacity. Here, we report that inhibition capacity declines with aging in wild-type flies, which is mitigated by lessening acetylcholine breakdown and augmented by reducing acetylcholine biosynthesis. We identified the mushroom body (MB) γ neurons as a chief neural site for acetylcholine’s contribution to the aging-associated inhibitory control deficit. In addition, we found that the MB output neurons MBON-γ2α’1 having dendrites at the MB γ2 and α’1 lobes and axons projecting to the superior medial protocerebrum and the crepine is critical for sustained movement suppression per se. This study reveals, for the first time, the central role of acetylcholine in the aging-associated loss of inhibitory control and provides a framework for further mechanistic studies.
Subject terms: Motor control, Neural ageing
Introduction
Acetylcholine is important for executive function in human subjects and rodents. In healthy non-smokers, for example, acute nicotine administration enhances attention when tested in the spatial attentional resource allocation and rapid visual information processing tasks1. Consistently, the nicotinic acetylcholine receptor (nAChR) antagonist mecamylamine administration in rats and the nAChR beta2 knockout in mice impair attention performance in the 5-choice serial reaction time task (5-CSRTT)2,3, substantiating a crucial role of acetylcholine in attention. The role of acetylcholine in inhibitory control, however, seems rather complex. In healthy non-smokers, acute nicotine administration improves behavioral inhibition in the stop signal task4. Nicotine treatment in rats, on the other hand, impairs inhibitory action and choice when tested in the 5- or 3-CSRTT, go/no-go task and systemic delayed reward task2,5,6. The inhibitory effect of nicotine is blocked by mecamylamine pretreatment6 or the infusion of the alpha4beta2 nAChR antagonist dihydro-beta-erythroidine (DHbetaE) in the infralimbic cortex5. Moreover, the DHbetaE infusion5 or systemic mecamylamine administration2 without nicotine administration is sufficient to suppress impulsive action, suggesting pro-impulsive function of endogenous acetylcholine in rats, which is not in line with the findings in human subjects. It seems of great value to study an additional model organism for deeper understanding of the mechanism by which acetylcholine regulates inhibitory response control.
Acetylcholine neurotransmission declines with normal aging and the decline is highly augmented in Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias such as frontotemporal dementia and Lewy body dementia7–11. Executive dysfunctions, in particular impaired inhibitory control, represent prominent symptoms of early-stage Alzheimer’s disease, frontotemporal dementia and other dementias12–16; however, there is no information on the role of acetylcholine in inhibitory control dysfunction associated with normal aging or dementias. To address this knowledge gap, we developed a simple behavioral paradigm to study a fundamental form of inhibitory response control in Drosophila melanogaster. Here, we show that acetylcholine significantly contributes to the aging-dependent deficit in inhibitory control and its major neural site is the mushroom body neurons.
Materials and methods
Fly strains and culture
The wild-type strain used in this study is Canton-S (CS). The following fly strains were obtained from the Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center (BDSC; Bloomington, IN): Acec00215 (BDSC #10026), ChATMI08244 (BDSC #55439), ChATMI04508 (BDSC #37817), OK107-GAL4 (BDSC #854), MB010B-GAL4 (BDSC #68293), 201y-GAL4 (BDSC #4440), MB009B-GAL4 (BDSC #68292), c739-GAL4 (BDSC #7362), MB008B-GAL4 (BDSC #68291), c305a-GAL4 (BDSC #30829), MB005B-GAL4 (BDSC #68306), MB077B-GAL4 (BDSC #68283), MB051B-GAL4 (BDSC #68275), MB018B-GAL4 (BDSC #68296), MB082C-GAL4 (BDSC #68286), MB543B-GAL4 (BDSC #68335), MB027B-GAL4 (BDSC #68301), MB549C-GAL4 (BDSC #68373), MB542B-GAL4 (BDSC #68372), GH146-GAL4 (BDSC #30026) and UAS-ChATRNAi (BDSC #25856); pBDP-GAL4 from David Anderson (California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, CA); NP225-GAL4 from Andreas Thum (Universität Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany); MB247-GAL4 from Scott Waddell (University of Oxford, Oxford, UK); and NP1131-GAL4 from Josh Dubnau (Stony Brook University School of Medicine, Stony Brook, NY).
Flies were raised on a standard cornmeal/sucrose/yeast/agar medium at 25° C with 50% relative humidity under a 12 h light/12 h dark cycle. Flies were collected under carbon dioxide within two days after eclosion and were housed in mixed sex groups. For aging, all fly strains were transferred to fresh food every 2–3 day until the ages of 4 day, 2 week and 4 week. These ages were selected to examine flies at young (4 day), mid (2 week) and early old (4 week) ages. The 4-wk old allows capturing the brain and behavioral changes associated with early-stage dementia and potentially distinguishing them from those occurring during normal aging17. Prior to behavioral tests, flies were sorted into a group of 13 per food vial (representing n = 1) and were then left to rest for 2 day in the light-, temperature-, and humidity- controlled incubator before behavioral or immunohistochemical analyses. Both males and females were examined separately but there was no sex difference thus combined data are presented.
Behavioral analysis
Flies were gently transferred into a rectangular plexiglass chamber (60 mm L × 60 mm W × 15 mm H) connected to filtered air and acclimated to the chamber for 10 min. The 10 L/min airflow was delivered to the chamber for 10 min. The chamber was video recorded to monitor fly movements before and after airflow. Videos were analyzed manually to score the number of flying events or using the Viewer3 tracking software (BiObserve Technologies, Bonn, Germany) that allows tracking and measuring the average speed of individual flies in mm/s per fly. Raw data were transported to Excel (Microsoft, Redmond, WA) and the number of the movements exceeding 60 mm/s (flying event) that we defined as loss of inhibition events (LIE) was scored per fly per min. To quantitively compare different conditions (e.g., age or genotype), we analyzed LIE numbers in 1 min bins for the entire 10 min of the No-Go phase and compared the 1 min bin having the highest LIE counts. All behavioral experiments were performed blind to the experimenter. The control and experimental groups were tested in the same experimental session in a randomized manner. Multiple independent sets of flies obtained from independent crosses were used for behavioral experiments.
Immunohistochemical analysis
Immunostaining was performed as previously described18,19 with several modifications. Fly brains were dissected in ice-cold 1X phosphate buffered saline (1X PBS), fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde in 1X PBS for 3 h at 4 °C. The fixed brains were washed with 1X PBS once for 10 min, washed twice with 1X PBHT (20 mM PO4, 0.5 M NaCl, 0.2% Triton X-100, pH 7.4) for 10 min each, solubilized with 1% Triton X-100 in 1X PBHT for 1 h at room temperature, blocked with 5% normal goat serum (NGS) in 1X PBHT for 16–18 h at 4 °C and incubated with the 1X PBHT containing 1:1000 anti-ChAT antibody (ChAT 4B120; DSHB, Iowa City, IA) and 5% NGS for 48 h at 4 °C. After two 10 min washes at room temperature and a 16–24 h wash at 4 °C with 1X PBHT, the brains were incubated with the Alexa Flour 488 conjugated goat anti-mouse IgG (A-11001; Invitrogen/Life Technologies, Eugene, OR) for 48 h at 4 °C followed by three 20 min washes with 1X PBHT and three 10 min washes with 0.12 M Tris–HCl, pH 7.2. The brains were then mounted in the Vectashield mounting medium (H-1000; Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA) for imaging with the 40X oil immersion objective in the LSM700 confocal microscope (Zeiss, Thornwood, NY). Pixel resolution was set at 2048 × 2048. Optical sections were made at 1 μm thickness and 50 sections were stacked for the representative images in Fig. 2B. ChAT immunoreactivity in the superior medial protocerebrum was quantified using the ImageJ (NIH, Bethesda, MD). The fluorescent signals were converted to grayscale and the fluorescence intensity per pixel in the SMP was calculated by [Integrated densityTotal/AreaTotal] per brain. The fold change of the fluorescent intensities in the ChAT/+ mutant brains from those in the control Canton-S brains was used for data presentation.
Acetylcholine analysis
To measure acetylcholine, we utilized a commercially available Choline/Acetylcholine assay kit (catalog # ab65345, Abcam, Cambridge, United Kingdom). Briefly, 50 frozen fly heads per condition (genotype and age) were homogenized 50 μl of the Choline Assay Buffer (1 fly head per 1 μl) on ice with the KONTES Micro Tissue Grinder (Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA) for 30 to 45 s. The homogenates were transferred to fresh tubes, centrifuged at 14,000 rpm for 10 min at 4 °C, and subjected to the Choline/Acetylcholine assay per the manufacture’s instruction. Multiple independent sets of fly heads (e.g., 4 to 8 sets) obtained from independent crosses were used for acetylcholine quantification.
Statistical analysis
All statistical analyses were performed using the Minitab software (Minitab, State College, PA) or JMP (SAS, Cary, NC). Raw data were analyzed using the Anderson–Darling goodness-of-fit test for distribution and are reported as mean + SEM. Normally distributed data were analyzed by either two-tailed Student’s t test for two groups or by ANOVA followed by post hoc Tukey’s multiple-comparison for three or more groups or Dunnett’s test to compare experimental groups with a control group. Significant difference among the groups under comparison was determined using an α level of 0.05 in all analyses. All raw data files are available on request.
Results
Inhibitory control diminishes with age in Drosophila
Inhibitory control confers the ability to suppress inappropriate actions or thoughts and is a key executive function supporting flexible and goal-directed behaviors. This capacity declines with aging in human subjects, for example when tested in the go/no-go (GNG)21. A GNG task measures action restraint, requiring subjects to withhold a motor response to a “no-go” signal 22. To assess whether inhibitory control declines with aging in Drosophila similar to humans, we developed a Drosophila version of a GNG test, in which a group of 13 flies placed in a chamber were video-tracked to quantify their movements. Without salient stimuli, the wild-type Canton-S (CS) flies freely move around. Upon introduction of strong airflow, flies halt movements presumably to keep their behavior under control (Fig. 1A)23,24. The fly’s ability to suppress ongoing activity (walking) inappropriate in a given context (strong “wind”), and we postulate that it represents a fundamental form of inhibitory response control. We tested the female and male CS flies at three different ages 4 day (young), 2 week (mid), and 4 week (early old). Regardless sex and age, the CS flies exhibited robust movement suppression under strong airflow with infrequent instances of flying behavior, which is an impulsive act resulting from loss of movement inhibition (Fig. 1A). To identify whether the frequency of the loss of inhibition events (LIE) changes with aging, we counted the movement with the speed over 60 mm/s, consisting of flying. Notably, older CS flies exhibited more LIE (Fig. 1B: one way ANOVA; R2 = 0.704, F2,33 = 39.24, p < 0.0001; n = 12) at shorter latency for LIE (Fig. 1C: one way ANOVA; R2 = 0.372, F2,33 = 9.77, p = 0.0005), indicating that aging diminishes inhibitory capacity. We did not observe any difference in LIE between females and males at all ages thus the data obtained from both sexes are combined and presented in Fig. 1B and C.
It is known in human subjects and rodents that acetylcholine neurotransmission modestly declines with aging7,8 but this has not been demonstrated in flies. To test whether diminished acetylcholine neurotransmission is responsible for the aging-associated increase in LIE, we examined the Acec00215/+ flies with the heterozygous mutation in acetylcholinesterase (Ace; EC 3.1.1.7) that breaks down acetylcholine and is a target for the first line medication to treat Alzheimer’s disease. The Acec00215 allele is a severe hypomorph or null since it has a piggyBac insertion in the 5’ non-coding region of the gene and homozygous lethal25. We found that the heterozygous Ace mutation rescued fully at 2 week and partly at 4 week the movement suppression deficit (Fig. 1B: two-way ANOVA, F5,66 = 37.98, p < 0.0001; genotype effect, p < 0.0001; age effect, p < 0.0001; genotype × age, p = 0.0009) as well as the LIE latency at 4 week (Fig. 1C: two-way ANOVA, F5,66 = 5.236, p = 0.0004; genotype effect, p = 0.0112; age effect, p = 0.0007; genotype × age, p = 0.2021). Consistently, the Acec00215/+ flies showed higher acetylcholine levels compared to CS at all ages tested and both genotypes have declining acetylcholine levels with aging (Fig. 1D: two-way ANOVA, F5,18 = 36.93, p < 0.0001; genotype effect, p < 0.0001; age effect, p < 0.0001; genotype × age, p = 0.116). This finding supports that diminished acetylcholine neurotransmission accounts for inhibitory control decline in normal aging, and the GNG test is well suited to study aging-related changes in inhibitory control in flies.
Choline acetyltransferase (ChAT) deficit augments inhibitory control impairments with aging
To directly address the role of acetylcholine in inhibitory control, we used the flies defective in ChAT. ChAT (EC 2.3.1.6) is the biosynthetic enzyme for acetylcholine, which aids in the transfer of an acetyl group in Acetyl-CoA to choline, and Drosophila has a single gene for ChAT as in humans25. We examined two mutant alleles ChATMI04508 and ChATMI08244 containing the gene trap MiMIC vector26 (Fig. 2A). The homozygous ChATMI04508 or ChATMI08244 mutants die during development thus we investigated their heterozygous mutants. The ChAT immunoreactivity was widespread in the wild-type CS brain and its level was significantly reduced in both mutants at the age of 4 to 5 day old (Fig. 2B, representative images; 2C, immunoreactivity quantifications; ANOVA, F2,26 = 15.96, p < 0.0001, n = 8—11). When subjected to the GNG test, both heterozygous mutants showed dysfunctional movement suppression in an aging-dependent manner (Fig. 2D and E: the representative movement traces per chamber containing 13 flies of the ChATMI04508/+ or ChATMI08244/+ genotypes at the ages of 4 day, 2 week or 4 week). Specifically, the heterozygous ChAT mutants at all ages showed robust movement suppression when strong air was introduced (time 0 on the X-axis in Fig. 2D and E) similar to CS; however, they exhibited highly augmented LIEs as they got older (Fig. 2F: F2,33 = 65.28, p < 0.0001, n = 12 for ChATMI04508/+ ; F2,33 = 61.56, p < 0.0001, n = 12 for ChATMI08244/+). At the age of 4 day, there was no difference in LIEs of the ChATMI04508/+ and ChATMI08244/+ mutants compared to that of CS (Fig. 2G 4 day: F2,33 = 0.1377, p = 0.87, n = 12); however, there was significant increases in the LIEs of the ChATMI04508/+ and ChATMI08244/+ flies at the ages of 2 week (F2,33 = 65.35, p < 0.0001, n = 12) and 4 week (F2,33 = 41.3, p < 0.0001, n = 12) compared to those of CS. Also, the LIE onsets of ChATMI04508/+ and ChATMI08244/+ flies were significantly earlier than CS at 2 week (Fig. 2H; F2,33 = 26.996, p < 0.0001, n = 12) and 4 week (F2,33 = 32.365, p < 0.0001, n = 12) but not at 4 day (F2,33 = 0.0346, p = 0.97, n = 12). Together, these data indicate that the flies with reduced ChAT expression have augmented sensitivity to aging in movement suppression deficit, indicating the critical role of acetylcholine in the aging-dependent loss of inhibitory control.
Ace deficiency rescues anomalous inhibitory control of the ChAT mutants
We reasoned that dampened cholinergic transmission causes the dysfunctional inhibitory control of the ChAT mutant. Should it be the case, we should be able to rescue the phenotype by reducing the amount of the acetylcholine degradation enzyme Ace. We tested this notion by examining the ChAT/+ mutant flies carrying the heterozygous Ace mutation27 (i.e. ChATMI04508/Acec00215) along with the flies with the heterozygous mutation only in ChAT or Ace and CS as controls. When subjected to the GNG test, there was no differences in LIEs of all genotypes at the age of 4 day as expected (Fig. 3A: ANOVA; F3,44 = 0.1621, p = 0.92, n = 12). Notably, the double heterozygous ChATMI04508/Acec00215 mutants exhibited the LIEs comparable to those of CS and Acec00215/+ but significantly different from that of ChATMI04508/+ at the ages of 2 week (Fig. 3A: ANOVA; F3,44 = 89.13, p < 0.0001, n = 12) and 4 week (F3,44 = 87.43, p < 0.0001, n = 12). Likewise, the double heterozygous ChATMI04508/Acec00215 mutants exhibited the latency to the first LIE comparable to those of CS and Acec00215/+ but significantly delayed than that of ChATMI04508/+ at the age of 2 week (Fig. 3B: ANOVA; F3,44 = 23.07, p < 0.0001, n = 12). At the age of 4 week, the ChATMI04508/Acec00215 mutants showed the latency comparable to that of Acec00215/+ but significantly delayed than those of ChATMI04508/+ as well as CS (F3,44 = 49.22, p < 0.0001, n = 12). Together, the heterozygous Acec00215 mutation fully suppressed the ChATMI04508/+ mutant’s LIE and latency phenotypes. In line with these results, the acetylcholine levels in the double heterozygous ChATMI04508/Acec00215 were indistinguishable from those of CS at all ages and they were significantly higher than those of ChATMI04508/+ (Fig. 3C: ANOVA with post hoc Tukey multiple comparison of the genotypes in each age. For 4 day: F3,28 = 62.71, p < 0.0001; For 2 week: F3,12 = 63.26, p < 0.0001; For 4 week: F3,12 = 89.62, p < 0.0001; ns, p > 0.05; **, p < 0.005; ***, p < 0.0001; n = 4–8). This indicates that the acetylcholine deficiency is responsible for the aging-associated deficit in inhibitory control.
The cholinergic neurons contributing to the aging-associated deficit in inhibitory control
Acetylcholine neurons consist of approximately 58% of the central brain28 and their axons project to most brain areas (Fig. 2B). The mushroom body (MB) neurons in the central brain are known to modulate locomotor behaviors29,30 and their three substructures, namely α/β, α’/β’ and γ neurons, are cholinergic28. We asked whether the MB ChAT neurons contribute to the aging-sensitive LIE by using the RNA interference (RNAi)-mediated ChAT knockdown (KD). When ChAT was knocked down in all MB neurons (via the OK107- or MB010B-GAL4 driver), two MB substructures γ and α/β (via MB247- or 201y-GAL4) or γ (via NP1131- or MB009B-GAL4) but not in α/β (via c739- or MB008B-GAL4) or α’/β’ (via c305a- or MB005B-GAL4) neurons led to the aging-dependent increase in LIEs (Fig. 4A, two-way ANOVA: F32,231 = 79.83, p < 0.0001; genotype effect, p < 0.0001; age effect, p < 0.0001; genotype × age, p < 0.0001; n = 8). These data indicate that the MB γ cholinergic neurotransmission is important for the aging-related LIEs.
Eight types of the mushroom body output neurons (MBON) are known to be cholinergic and their dendrites are mapped to the γ2α’1, α2sc, α2p3p, α3, α’1, α’2, α’3ap and α’3 m lobes31. ChAT KD in the MBON-γ2α’1 (via MB077B- or MB051B-GAL4) but not in other cholinergic MBONs, namely MBON-α’2 (via MB018B-GAL4), MBON-α3 (via MB082C-GAL4), MBON-α’1 (via MB543B-GAL4), MBON-α’3ap and -α’3 m (via MB027B-GAL4), MBON-α2sc (via MB549C-GAL4), and MBON-α2p3p (via MB542B-GAL4), resulted in significant LIEs in all ages (4 day, 2 week and 4 week) tested (Fig. 4B, two-way ANOVA: F26,189 = 39.01, p < 0.0001; genotype effect, p < 0.0001; age effect, p = 0.448; genotype × age, p = 1; n = 8). Thus, the MBON-γ2α’1 cholinergic transmission is critical for inhibitory control. The MBN receive input from the projection neurons (PN) and the PN are cholinergic20. We found that ChAT KD in the PN (via NP225- or GH146-GAL4) had no effect on movement inhibition at all ages under test (Fig. 4C, two-way ANOVA: F8,63 = 22.33, p < 0.0001; genotype effect, p = 0.94; age effect, p = 0.0001; genotype × age, p = 1; n = 8). This suggests that the PN cholinergic transmission is dispensable for inhibitory control.
Discussion
Inhibition or inhibitory control is a fundamental brain function and is critical for fitness and survival. Poor inhibition is associated with many brain disorders including attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, autism spectrum disorder, addiction and neurodegenerative disorders such as Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia32–36. Mechanistic studies of inhibition have been done on neurodevelopmental disorders and addiction whereas limited studies have been reported on neurodegenerative dementias. In the report, we show that inhibition capacity declines with aging in wild-type flies, which is halted by the mutation in the acetylcholine breakdown enzyme Ace and augmented by the decreased acetylcholine biosynthesis in the γ MB neurons.
Motor behaviors have been studied in Drosophila and they typically entail startle responses to olfactory, visual or mechanosensory stimuli in which increases in locomotor activity or movements represent the expected behavioral outcome37–40. In the startle-induced negative-geotaxis/climbing assay, for example, flies exhibit rapid climbing movements when tapped down. Older flies in this assay exhibit slower or less negative-geotaxis movements than young flies41,42. The GNG test used in our study measures movement suppression (no or decreased movements) in the presence of strong airflow or predator sound (unpublished data), and we show that aging causes poor suppression represented by hyperkinetic or flying behavior. Reduced or lack of motor activity of aged flies in the startle-induced negative-geotaxis assay could be confounded by general slowness of behavior related to weakened muscle or other physiological changes. The GNG test is beneficial to uncover aging-related changes in neural control of movements, in particular those leading to impulsive acts.
The genetic mutation in the rate-limiting enzyme for acetylcholine biosynthesis ChAT in mice causes lethality at birth43 but all mutant mice defective in the individual subunits of nAChR and muscarinic acetylcholine receptor (mAChR) except for α3 are viable to adulthood44. The mice lacking the α7 nAChR subunit or the mice with the conditional knockout of M4 mAChR in the D1 receptor expressing neurons45,46 show significant increases in impulsive acts, supporting our finding on the acetylcholine’s role in inhibitory control. The genetic knockout studies are very insightful however no human diseases identified to date involves total absence of acetylcholine production nor signaling. It is conceivable that acetylcholine signaling via α7 nAChR and M4 mAChR may decline with aging, which in turn contributes to aging-associated inhibitory control dysfunction. The role of acetylcholine signaling in aging is unknown in Drosophila. The only related studies are those demonstrating either reduced or overexpressed vesicular acetylcholine transporter dramatically reduces lifespan of flies, suggesting that acetylcholine is important for longevity47,48. Our approach to employ the heterozygous ChAT mutant offers a useful strategy to identify the molecular and cellular players crucial for inhibitory control that is sensitive to aging, a key risk factor for dementia.
The MB is a key neural site processing olfactory behaviors as well as non-olfactory brain functions49–52. Our study points to the cholinergic transmission in the γ MB being responsible for aging-related loss of inhibitory control. We have previously shown that the γ neurons, upon receiving dopaminergic and octopaminergic inputs, form appetitive and aversive memory, modulate courtship motivation and develop behavioral sensitization to the disinhibition effect of ethanol19,53–55. The γ independently with α’/β’ neurons are also shown to modulate startle-induced negative geotaxis or climbing behavior in which ectopic activation of their activities inhibits the locomotor reactivity to startle but ectopic inhibition of their synaptic outputs has no effect30. Since the ChAT RNAi used in this study would dampen cholinergic output from the γ neurons, the neural controls for startle-induced negative geotaxis and movement suppression in the GNG task are likely independent. The study by Martin et al.29 shows that the MB ablation or inhibition of γ and/or α/β synaptic output causes basal hyperactivity. While it seems possible that higher basal activity could be associated with more frequent loss of movement suppression, we do not see significant difference in basal activities of the flies with and without ChAT KD at 4 weeks (data not shown). Thus, the neural control of the stimulus-induced movement suppression is distinct from that of basal activity. Together, our study is the first identifying progressive loss of acetylcholine and inhibitory control with aging in which γ cholinergic neurotransmission plays a key role.
The MB axons convey information to 21 types of MBONs among which 8 types are cholinergic31. Our study uncovers only 1 type of the cholinergic MBONs—MBON-γ2α’1 (2 neurons per hemisphere) receiving inputs from the MB γ2 and α’1 lobes and sending outputs to the crepine and the superior medial protocerebrum (Fig. 4D)—contributing to movement suppression, which is further supported by our unpublished study demonstrating that ectopic inhibition of MBON-γ2α’1 via Shits leads to loss of inhibitory control. MBON-γ2α’1 activity is shown to be crucial for various aspects of appetitive and aversive memory31,56–60, courtship memory61, sleep promotion31,62,63, fat accumulation64, food seeking65 and attraction/positive valence31. Regarding stimulus-induced motor behaviors, ectopic activation of MBON-γ2α’1 causes inhibition of proboscis extension response to sucrose but ectopic inhibition has no effect66. The MBONs important for startle-induced negative geotaxis include the cholinergic MBON-α2sc and α’330 but they are not important for movement suppression in the present study. Together, our study reveals a novel role for MBON-γ2α’1 in sustained inhibitory control per se. It remains to be determined whether this function requires the input from the γ MB lobes or dopamine/octopamine neurons or both, and how this MBON with only 2 neurons in each hemisphere accommodates so many functions.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the Orville Edward Egbert, M.D. Endowment fund, Office of Research Sponsored Project and the NIH NIMH R21MH109953, Brain & Behavior Research Foundation, NIH NIGMS 1R16GM145548 and NIH NIMHD 3U54MD007592-29S5 grants. We are grateful for Keelung Hong Fellowship for supporting E.B.S. and the Bloomington Stock Center for fly lines, and the Cytometry, Screening, and Imaging Core at the BBRC on confocal microscopy. We appreciate the past and current lab members for their discussion and support.
Author contributions
P.R.S. and K.A.H. conceived the experiments; P.R.S. and E.B.S. performed and analyzed all experiments; K.A.H. wrote the main text of manuscript with input from P.R.S. and E.B.S.; P.R.S. and E.B.S. prepared all figures. All authors reviewed the manuscript.
Data availability
All raw data reported and materials used in the study are available upon request from the corresponding authors.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Paul Rafael Sabandal, Email: pbsabandal@utep.edu.
Kyung-An Han, Email: khan@utep.edu.
References
- 1.Hahn B, et al. Evidence for positive allosteric modulation of cognitive-enhancing effects of nicotine in healthy human subjects. Psychopharmacology. 2020;237:219–230. doi: 10.1007/s00213-019-05363-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Balachandran RC, et al. Cholinergic and dopaminergic interactions alter attention and response inhibition in Long-Evans rats performing the 5-choice serial reaction time task. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2018;175:160–173. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2018.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Guillem K, et al. Nicotinic acetylcholine receptor beta2 subunits in the medial prefrontal cortex control attention. Science. 2011;333:888–891. doi: 10.1126/science.1207079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Logemann HN, Bocker KB, Deschamps PK, Kemner C, Kenemans JL. Differences between nicotine-abstinent smokers and non-smokers in terms of visuospatial attention and inhibition before and after single-blind nicotine administration. Neuroscience. 2014;277:375–382. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.07.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Tsutsui-Kimura I, et al. Nicotine provokes impulsive-like action by stimulating alpha4beta2 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors in the infralimbic, but not in the prelimbic cortex. Psychopharmacology. 2010;209:351–359. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-1804-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Kolokotroni KZ, Rodgers RJ, Harrison AA. Acute nicotine increases both impulsive choice and behavioural disinhibition in rats. Psychopharmacology. 2011;217:455–473. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2296-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Schliebs R, Arendt T. The cholinergic system in aging and neuronal degeneration. Behav. Brain. Res. 2011;221:555–563. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.11.058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Ballinger EC, Ananth M, Talmage DA, Role LW. Basal forebrain cholinergic circuits and signaling in cognition and cognitive decline. Neuron. 2016;91:1199–1218. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.09.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Barrett MJ, Cloud LJ, Shah H, Holloway KL. Therapeutic approaches to cholinergic deficiency in Lewy body diseases. Expert Rev. Neurother. 2020;20:41–53. doi: 10.1080/14737175.2020.1676152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Noufi P, Khoury R, Jeyakumar S, Grossberg GT. Use of cholinesterase inhibitors in non-Alzheimer's dementias. Drugs Aging. 2019;36:719–731. doi: 10.1007/s40266-019-00685-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Murley AG, Rowe JB. Neurotransmitter deficits from frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Brain. 2018;141:1263–1285. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Martyr A, Boycheva E, Kudlicka A. Assessing inhibitory control in early-stage Alzheimer's and Parkinson's disease using the Hayling Sentence Completion Test. J. Neuropsychol. 2017 doi: 10.1111/jnp.12129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Crawford TJ, Higham S. Distinguishing between impairments of working memory and inhibitory control in cases of early dementia. Neuropsychologia. 2016;81:61–67. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Collette F, et al. Comparison of inhibitory functioning in mild Alzheimer's disease and frontotemporal dementia. Cortex. 2007;43:866–874. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70686-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Crawford TJ, et al. Inhibitory control of saccadic eye movements and cognitive impairment in Alzheimer's disease. Biol. Psychiatry. 2005;57:1052–1060. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Johns EK, et al. Executive functions in frontotemporal dementia and Lewy body dementia. Neuropsychology. 2009;23:765–777. doi: 10.1037/a0016792. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Tonoki A, Davis RL. Aging impairs intermediate-term behavioral memory by disrupting the dorsal paired medial neuron memory trace. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2012;109:6319–6324. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1118126109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Yasuyama K, Salvaterra PM. Localization of choline acetyltransferase-expressing neurons in Drosophila nervous system. Microsc. Res. Tech. 1999;45:65–79. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0029(19990415)45:2<65::AID-JEMT2>3.0.CO;2-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Lim J, et al. The mushroom body D1 dopamine receptor controls innate courtship drive. Genes Brain Behav. 2018;17:158–167. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12425. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Takagawa K, Salvaterra P. Analysis of choline acetyltransferase protein in temperature sensitive mutant flies using newly generated monoclonal antibody. Neurosci. Res. 1996;24:237–243. doi: 10.1016/0168-0102(95)00999-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Rey-Mermet A, Gade M. Inhibition in aging: What is preserved? What declines? A meta-analysis. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2018;25:1695–1716. doi: 10.3758/s13423-017-1384-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Chikazoe J, et al. Functional dissociation in right inferior frontal cortex during performance of go/no-go task. Cereb. Cortex. 2009;19:146–152. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Yorozu S, et al. Distinct sensory representations of wind and near-field sound in the Drosophila brain. Nature. 2009;458:201–205. doi: 10.1038/nature07843. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Tsubouchi A, et al. Topological and modality-specific representation of somatosensory information in the fly brain. Science. 2017;358:615–623. doi: 10.1126/science.aan4428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Larkin A, et al. FlyBase: Updates to the Drosophila melanogaster knowledge base. Nucleic Acids Res. 2021;49:D899–D907. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkaa1026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Venken KJ, et al. MiMIC: a highly versatile transposon insertion resource for engineering Drosophila melanogaster genes. Nat. Methods. 2011;8:737–743. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1662. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Kim W, et al. Pharmacogenetic regulation of acetylcholinesterase activity in Drosophila reveals the regulatory mechanisms of AChE inhibitors in synaptic plasticity. Neurochem. Res. 2011;36:879–893. doi: 10.1007/s11064-011-0418-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Croset V, Treiber CD, Waddell S. Cellular diversity in the Drosophila midbrain revealed by single-cell transcriptomics. Elife. 2018 doi: 10.7554/eLife.34550. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Martin JR, Ernst R, Heisenberg M. Mushroom bodies suppress locomotor activity in Drosophila melanogaster. Learn. Mem. 1998;5:179–191. doi: 10.1101/lm.5.1.179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Sun J, et al. Neural control of startle-induced locomotion by the mushroom bodies and associated neurons in Drosophila. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2018;12:6. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2018.00006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Aso Y, et al. Mushroom body output neurons encode valence and guide memory-based action selection in Drosophila. Elife. 2014;3:e04580. doi: 10.7554/eLife.04580. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Logue SF, Gould TJ. The neural and genetic basis of executive function: Attention, cognitive flexibility, and response inhibition. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2014;123:45–54. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2013.08.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Bari A, Robbins TW. Inhibition and impulsivity: Behavioral and neural basis of response control. Prog. Neurobiol. 2013;108:44–79. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Rochat L, et al. Assessing impulsivity changes in Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 2008;22:278–283. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e318166d692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Migliaccio R, et al. Cognitive and behavioural inhibition deficits in neurodegenerative dementias. Cortex. 2020;131:265–283. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Opwonya J, et al. Inhibitory control of saccadic eye movements and cognitive impairment in mild cognitive impairment. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2022;14:871432. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.871432. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Erlenmeyer-Kimling L, Hirsch J. Measurement of the relations between chromosomes and behavior. Science. 1961;134:1068–1069. doi: 10.1126/science.134.3485.1068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Rogers I, et al. Ageing increases vulnerability to abeta42 toxicity in Drosophila. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e40569. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0040569. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Campbell JL, Nash HA. The visually-induced jump response of Drosophila melanogaster is sensitive to volatile anesthetics. J. Neurogenet. 1998;12:241–251. doi: 10.3109/01677069809108561. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Cho W, Heberlein U, Wolf FW. Habituation of an odorant-induced startle response in Drosophila. Genes Brain Behav. 2004;3:127–137. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183x.2004.00061.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Rhodenizer D, Martin I, Bhandari P, Pletcher SD, Grotewiel M. Genetic and environmental factors impact age-related impairment of negative geotaxis in Drosophila by altering age-dependent climbing speed. Exp. Gerontol. 2008;43:739–748. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2008.04.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Riemensperger T, et al. A single dopamine pathway underlies progressive locomotor deficits in a Drosophila model of Parkinson disease. Cell Rep. 2013;5:952–960. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2013.10.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Misgeld T, et al. Roles of neurotransmitter in synapse formation: development of neuromuscular junctions lacking choline acetyltransferase. Neuron. 2002;36:635–648. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01020-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Zhang, L. in Animal Models of Cognitive Impairment Frontiers in Neuroscience (eds E. D. Levin & J. J. Buccafusco) (2006).
- 45.Hoyle E, Genn RF, Fernandes C, Stolerman IP. Impaired performance of alpha7 nicotinic receptor knockout mice in the five-choice serial reaction time task. Psychopharmacology. 2006;189:211–223. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0549-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Klawonn AM, et al. Muscarinic M4 receptors on cholinergic and dopamine D1 receptor-expressing neurons have opposing functionality for positive reinforcement and influence impulsivity. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 2018;11:139. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2018.00139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.White D, et al. Deficits in the vesicular acetylcholine transporter alter lifespan and behavior in adult Drosophila melanogaster. Neurochem. Int. 2020;137:104744. doi: 10.1016/j.neuint.2020.104744. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Showell SS, Martinez Y, Gondolfo S, Boppana S, Lawal HO. Overexpression of the vesicular acetylcholine transporter disrupts cognitive performance and causes age-dependent locomotion decline in Drosophila. Mol. Cell Neurosci. 2020;105:103483. doi: 10.1016/j.mcn.2020.103483. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Modi MN, Shuai Y, Turner GC. The Drosophila mushroom body: From architecture to algorithm in a learning circuit. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2020;43:465–484. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-080317-0621333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Boto T, Stahl A, Tomchik SM. Cellular and circuit mechanisms of olfactory associative learning in Drosophila. J. Neurogenet. 2020;34:36–46. doi: 10.1080/01677063.2020.1715971. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Donlea JM. Neuronal and molecular mechanisms of sleep homeostasis. Curr. Opin. Insect. Sci. 2017;24:51–57. doi: 10.1016/j.cois.2017.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Sabandal JM, Sabandal PR, Kim YC, Han KA. Concerted actions of octopamine and dopamine receptors drive olfactory learning. J. Neurosci. 2020;40:4240–4250. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1756-19.2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Kim YC, Lee HG, Han KA. D1 dopamine receptor dDA1 is required in the mushroom body neurons for aversive and appetitive learning in Drosophila. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:7640–7647. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1167-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Kim YC, Lee HG, Lim J, Han KA. Appetitive learning requires the alpha1-like octopamine receptor OAMB in the Drosophila mushroom body neurons. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:1672–1677. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3042-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Aranda GP, Hinojos SJ, Sabandal PR, Evans PD, Han KA. Behavioral sensitization to the disinhibition effect of ethanol requires the Dopamine/Ecdysone receptor in Drosophila. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2017;11:56. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2017.00056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Felsenberg J, Barnstedt O, Cognigni P, Lin S, Waddell S. Re-evaluation of learned information in Drosophila. Nature. 2017;544:240–244. doi: 10.1038/nature21716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Yamazaki D, et al. Two parallel pathways assign opposing odor valences during drosophila memory formation. Cell Rep. 2018;22:2346–2358. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Berry JA, Phan A, Davis RL. Dopamine neurons mediate learning and forgetting through bidirectional modulation of a memory trace. Cell Rep. 2018;25:651–662 e655. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.09.051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Chouhan NS, Griffith LC, Haynes P, Sehgal A. Availability of food determines the need for sleep in memory consolidation. Nature. 2021;589:582–585. doi: 10.1038/s41586-020-2997-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Ichinose T, et al. Mushroom body output differentiates memory processes and distinct memory-guided behaviors. Curr. Biol. 2021;31:1294–1302 e1294. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.12.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Montague SA, Baker BS. Memory elicited by courtship conditioning requires mushroom body neuronal subsets similar to those utilized in appetitive memory. PLoS ONE. 2016;11:e0164516. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0164516. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Sitaraman D, et al. Propagation of homeostatic sleep signals by segregated synaptic microcircuits of the Drosophila mushroom body. Curr. Biol. 2015;25:2915–2927. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.09.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Lei Z, Henderson K, Keleman K. A neural circuit linking learning and sleep in Drosophila long-term memory. Nat. Commun. 2022;13:609. doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-28256-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Al-Anzi B, Zinn K. Identification and characterization of mushroom body neurons that regulate fat storage in Drosophila. Neural Dev. 2018;13:18. doi: 10.1186/s13064-018-0116-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Tsao CH, Chen CC, Lin CH, Yang HY, Lin S. Drosophila mushroom bodies integrate hunger and satiety signals to control innate food-seeking behavior. Elife. 2018 doi: 10.7554/eLife.35264. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Chia J, Scott K. Activation of specific mushroom body output neurons inhibits proboscis extension and sucrose consumption. PLoS ONE. 2020;15:e0223034. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0223034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
All raw data reported and materials used in the study are available upon request from the corresponding authors.