Skip to main content
PLOS Biology logoLink to PLOS Biology
. 2022 Dec 5;20(12):e3001730. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3001730

A brain-wide form of presynaptic active zone plasticity orchestrates resilience to brain aging in Drosophila

Sheng Huang 1,2,#, Chengji Piao 1,2,#, Christine B Beuschel 1,2, Zhiying Zhao 1, Stephan J Sigrist 1,2,‡,*
Editor: Paul J Shaw3
PMCID: PMC9721493  PMID: 36469518

Abstract

The brain as a central regulator of stress integration determines what is threatening, stores memories, and regulates physiological adaptations across the aging trajectory. While sleep homeostasis seems to be linked to brain resilience, how age-associated changes intersect to adapt brain resilience to life history remains enigmatic. We here provide evidence that a brain-wide form of presynaptic active zone plasticity (“PreScale”), characterized by increases of active zone scaffold proteins and synaptic vesicle release factors, integrates resilience by coupling sleep, longevity, and memory during early aging of Drosophila. PreScale increased over the brain until mid-age, to then decreased again, and promoted the age-typical adaption of sleep patterns as well as extended longevity, while at the same time it reduced the ability of forming new memories. Genetic induction of PreScale also mimicked early aging-associated adaption of sleep patterns and the neuronal activity/excitability of sleep control neurons. Spermidine supplementation, previously shown to suppress early aging-associated PreScale, also attenuated the age-typical sleep pattern changes. Pharmacological induction of sleep for 2 days in mid-age flies also reset PreScale, restored memory formation, and rejuvenated sleep patterns. Our data suggest that early along the aging trajectory, PreScale acts as an acute, brain-wide form of presynaptic plasticity to steer trade-offs between longevity, sleep, and memory formation in a still plastic phase of early brain aging.


This study shows that a brain-wide form of presynaptic plasticity ("PreScale") steers trade-offs between longevity, sleep and memory formation in a still plastic phase of early brain aging, illustrating how life strategy manifests at both circuit and synapse levels.

Introduction

The aging of the human societies over the globe, with lifespan extension currently being unmatched by increases in healthspan, urges to better understand the molecular–cellular mechanistic underpinnings underlying the various age-associated alterations [1,2]. The current increase of human lifespan expectancy demands for (i) a better understanding of aging mechanisms [18] and (ii) safe paradigms to allow for healthspan extension [7,915]. While the resilience against the effects of brain aging is an individual trait [16,17], its circuit, neuronal, and molecular basis remains insufficiently understood. Similarly, the resilience relevance of age-associated molecular and behavioral changes observed in humans and in animal models remains largely unclear [14,6,18], and discriminating protective from detrimental changes often remains speculative, impeding the development of safe healthspan extension paradigms.

Aging provokes changes in both sleep pattern and memory, which are suspected to functionally interact [5,6,19,20]. The fruit fly Drosophila has been widely used in discovering molecular, cellular, and circuit mechanisms of memory formation [2123], as well as in understanding the mechanisms and functions of sleep [2426]. Moreover, Drosophila has also been developed into a suitable animal model for studying aging and age-associated sleep pattern alterations [2731] and memory decline [10,32]. With a relative short lifespan, but not too short, Drosophila allows to explore and causally connect age-associated molecular and behavioral changes that might pave the way for diagnosis and therapy.

Previously, a brain-wide form of presynaptic plasticity (PreScale) was described in Drosophila, which is triggered in the course of aging [8] and also acutely upon experimentally and genetically induced sleep loss [33]. PreScale is driven by up-regulations of the presynaptic ELKS family scaffold factor Bruchpilot (BRP), which in turn controls the levels of critical synaptic vesicle release factor (m)Unc13-family protein Unc13A at presynaptic active zones [8,34]. Genetically triggering PreScale by increasing brp gene copy number reduced memory formation [8] and evoked rebound-like additional sleep [33]. Speaking of healthy aging paradigms, supplementing with the body-endogenous polyamine spermidine (Spd) has prominent cardioprotective and neuroprotective effects across the aging of animal models [9]. Notably, Spd facilitates memory formation in mid-aged 30-day-old animals [10] and extends lifespan [15], and, at the same time attenuates the age-associated emergence of PreScale [8]. Similarly, bidirectional regulation of mitochondrial Aconitase 1 was found to modulate PreScale-type plasticity in conjunction with its effects on lifespan and age-associated memory decline [35]. Thus, BRP-driven PreScale seems to operate at a critical intersection of different aspects of brain aging. Still, how exactly PreScale-type plasticity intersects with brain aging, importantly whether in a per se protective or detrimental manner, remained to be investigated.

We here provide evidence that PreScale operates at the intersection of principal organismal fitness and the formation of new memories during early aging. PreScale peaked at mid-age, changed sleep patterns during the early aging phase, and promoted lifespan but at the same time restricted the extent of forming new memories. Healthy aging paradigms (Spd supplementation, pharmacological induction of excessive sleep) apparently reset the need of triggering PreScale-type plasticity in the Drosophila brain. Thus, PreScale seemingly executes behavioral adaptations and trade-offs during a still plastic phase of early brain aging, illustrating how life strategy manifests on a circuit and synaptic plasticity level.

Results

Brain-wide presynaptic active zone plasticity (PreScale) peaks in the mid-aged Drosophila brain

In the nervous system, the operation of brain circuits relies on the proper fusion of neurotransmitter-filled synaptic vesicles at the so-called presynaptic active zone [34,36]. Our previous study described a new form of global presynaptic active zone plasticity upon early Drosophila aging [8], which here we refer to as PreScale. It is characterized by brain-wide increases in the levels of the ELKS-family core active zone scaffold protein BRP and BRP-associated release factors, particularly (m)Unc13-family protein Unc13A, the latter being crucial for synaptic vesicle release at Drosophila synapses [8,34,3739]. BRP seems to operate as a master scaffold of the Drosophila active zone, which recruits a spectrum of synaptic proteins to tune presynaptic active zone composition and consequently drives synaptic plasticity [33,40,41]. Intriguingly, a global increase of BRP levels associated with a corresponding increase in Unc13A was also observed upon sleep loss [33,41,42]. Here, the extent of sleep loss typically correlates with increases of BRP levels [42], while genetically enforced sleep reduces BRP levels below baseline levels [33].

We here first asked for the role of PreScale across the whole aging trajectory of Drosophila. So far, we had found that BRP/Unc13A levels were up-regulated at 30 days (30d) compared to 5 days (5d) of age [8]. Thus, we first quantified the levels of BRP and associated synaptic proteins across the Drosophila life history. We wondered what limits these presynaptic plastic changes might have and whether, for example, BRP would increase gradually across the whole fly lifespan.

Depending on the rearing condition, Drosophila can live up to about 3 months, which provides an opportunity to explicitly dissect age-associated molecular and behavioral changes (Fig 1A), with the chance to identify critical time windows of turning and switching events. We collected brain samples of wild type (wt) at the following ages: 1-day-old (1d), 10d, 20d, 30d, 40d, 50d, 60d, and 70d, for quantitative western blot analysis monitoring a spectrum of pre- and postsynaptic proteins: BRP [37,39], (m)Unc13 family protein Unc13A [34], synaptic vesicle protein Synapsin (Syn) [43], SNARE complex core component Syntaxin (Syx) [44], the homolog of mammalian scaffold protein PSD95 Discs large (Dlg1) [45], as well as the crucial autophagy protein Atg8a, which is regulated by the aging process [46]. Indeed, BRP, Unc13A, Syn, and Dlg1 first increased almost linearly to arrive at a plateau at 30d to 40d (“early aging”) (Fig 1B–1F), while the ratios between activated, lipidated Atg8a (Atg8a-II) and inactive, unlipidated Atg8a (Atg8a-I) showed a clear trend of gradual reduction during early aging (Fig 1B and 1H), consistent with previous reports [10,35,46]. Thereafter, however, the levels of these proteins dropped gradually with further advanced aging (Fig 1A–1F). Syx levels also showed a principally similar behavior (Fig 1B and 1G). Independent experiments using another BRP antibody (Nc82) showed similar results (S1 Fig). Consistent with these quantitative western blot results (Fig 1A–1H), immunostaining identified a stronger increase relative to juvenile levels in BRP levels at early aging stages than at later advanced aging stages (Fig 1I and 1J). Nonlinear fitting via quadratic regression suggests maximum BRP, Unc13A, Dlg1, and Syn levels at an age between 30d and 40d in western blots (Fig 1K). Notably, the dropping of synaptic protein levels at advanced ages, most prominently BRP and Unc13A levels, appeared to be associated with declining survival rates (Fig 1A–1D and 1K). These results thus suggest a switch of PreScale at middle age and uncover biphasic synaptic changes across the Drosophila lifespan.

Fig 1. Synaptic plasticity across the fly lifespan.

Fig 1

(A) A typical survival curve of wt female flies. n = 235. (B-H) Representative western blots (B) and relative levels of a spectrum of synaptic proteins, including BRP (C), Unc13A (D), Dlg1 (E), Syn (F), and Syx (G), and the relative ratios of active lipidated and unlipidated autophagy-related protein Atg8a (Atg8a-II:Atg8-I; H) in wt female flies across the lifespan. n = 6–8. One-way ANOVA with Bonferroni multiple comparisons test is shown. (I and J) Confocal images (I) and whole-mount brain staining analysis (J) of BRP in wt female flies at different ages. n = 15 for 8d, n = 9 for 30d, n = 14–15 for 50d, and n = 9 for 60d. Student t test is shown. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001; ns, not significant. Scale bar: 50 μm. Error bars: mean ± SEM. (K) Nonlinear line fitting quadratic regression of the protein levels of BRP (C), Unc13A (D), Dlg1 (E), and Syn (F) levels in western blot across the fly lifespan. Underlying data can be found in S1 Data. Raw images of this figure are provided in S1 Raw Images. BRP, Bruchpilot; Dlg1, Discs large; Syn, Synapsin; Syx, Syntaxin; wt, wild type.

PreScale dampens both spontaneous activity and membrane excitability of sleep-controlling dFB neurons

What is the circuit and brain activity relevance of early aging-associated presynaptic active zone plasticity we call PreScale? As a kind of master scaffold of the active zone, BRP drives the upscaling of associated synaptic proteins and controls the synaptic vesicle release properties, particularly release probability and release site number [33,34,37]. Genetically triggering PreScale by increasing brp gene copy number reduces memory formation and provokes rebound-like sleep patterns [33]. Furthermore, increasing from wt 2xBRP to 4xBRP at young age was shown to mimic early aging-associated PreScale-type plasticity and memory decline [8]. Thus, we chose to study the potential roles of age-associated PreScale in regulating brain activity by inducing PreScale genetically in young animals.

To address this question, we first analyzed the activity status across the whole brain by utilizing the pan-neuronal driver elav-Gal4 [47] to express the Ca2+-dependent activity reporter CaLexA [48], which is meant to be a proxy for the activity history of neurons. The expression pattern of the per se pan-neuronal elav-Gal4 is known to be particularly strong in antennal lobe (AL) and mushroom body (MB) [47], and indeed elav-Gal4-driven CaLexA showed strong signals in AL and MB (Fig 2A). Interestingly, in 4xBRP animals, CaLexA signal within the ALs was significantly reduced while staying largely unaffected in MBs (Fig 2A and 2B). As the AL operates as the first synaptic relay station of the olfactory system and a central gate of olfactory sensory information processing [49], a generic drop in its activity levels might potentially support sleep.

Fig 2. PreScale plasticity provokes activity reprogramming in the dFB neurons.

Fig 2

(A and B) Confocal images (A) and whole-mount brain staining analysis (B) of CaLexA signal intensity with CaLexA expressed pan-neuronally by elav-Gal4 in 2xBRP compared to 4xBRP female flies. n = 18–20 for all groups. Scale bar: 50 μm. (C and D) Confocal images (C) and whole-mount brain staining analysis (D) of CaLexA signal intensity with CaLexA expressed in dFB by R23E10-Gal4 in 2xBRP compared to 3xBRP and 4xBRP female flies. n = 20 for all groups. Scale bar: 20 μm. (E-H) Spontaneous firing of dFB neurons in 5d 2xBRP and 4xBRP female flies, including representative membrane potential traces (E), mean firing rate (F), interspike interval distributions (G; Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, p < 0.0001), and bursting number for a 30-s window (H). n = 9–10 per group. (I-L) Voltage responses to 1-s current steps (from −10 to 60 pA, 5 pA) in dFB of 5d 2xBRP and 4xBRP female flies, including representative traces (I), input resistance (J), firing rates (K; two-way repeated-measures ANOVA analysis, p = 0.0245 for Current × Genotype interaction), and distribution of interspike interval at current steps till 40 pA (L; Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, p < 0.0001). n = 6–7 per group. (M-P) Voltage responses to 1-s current steps (from −10 to 60pA, 5pA) in dFB of 5d and 20d 2xBRP female flies, including representative traces (M), input resistance (N), firing rates (O; two-way repeated-measures ANOVA analysis, p < 0.0001 for Current × Age interaction), and distribution of interspike interval (P; Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, p < 0.0001). n = 5 for all groups. Student t test is shown for comparison between two groups. One-way ANOVA with Bonferroni multiple comparisons test is shown for comparisons of three groups. #p = 0.05; *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001; ns, not significant. Error bars: mean ± SEM. Underlying data can be found in S2 Data. AL, antennal lobe; dFB, dorsal fan-shaped body; MB, mushroom body.

PreScale might promote the transmission of specific activity patterns in relevant neurons in certain frequency domains, for example, the oscillatory activity pattern changes in the ellipsoid body R5 (previously called R2) network of sleep-deprived animals [50,51]. To analyze in a circuit-specific manner, we next examined the role of PreScale on the CaLexA signal of ellipsoid body R5 neurons using R58H05-Gal4 [33,51]. We have shown recently that R5 CaLexA signal was up-regulated when brp gene copy number was increased from 2xBRP to 3xBRP, representing a sleep-deprived state and higher sleep need [33]. Consistently, we here further show that 4xBRP animals also exhibited an increased CaLexA signal in R5 neurons (S2A–S2C Fig). Reducing brp gene copy number from 2xBRP to 1xBRP did not obviously change the CaLexA signal of R5 neurons (S2D and S2E Fig), likely in line with the only very mild reduction of sleep in 1xBRP flies [33].

The ellipsoid body R5 neurons were recently shown to receive excitatory inputs from helicon cells, within which a receptor of the inhibitory allatostatin-A peptide is expressed [52]. Furthermore, the dorsal fan-shaped body (dFB) neurons release allatostatin-A peptide to inhibit helicon cells, thus forming a negatively interconnected circuit with R5 neurons to regulate sleep (S2F Fig) [52]. Next, we expressed CaLexA in the well-studied sleep-promoting dFB using R23E10-Gal4 [20,53]. Consistent with the inhibitory connection between dFB and R5 (S2B and S2C Fig) [33], we observed a strong decrease in CaLexA signal of dFB neurons in both 3xBRP and 4xBRP animals (Fig 2C and 2D). Unexpectedly, 1xBRP animals also showed significantly reduced CaLexA signal in dFB neurons (S2G and S2H Fig). dFB neurons have been shown to be electrically silent during wakefulness [53]. As CaLexA signal represents the activity history of given neurons [48], and 3xBRP and 4xBRP scenarios were shown to mimic a sleep-deprived state [33], we hypothesized that mechanically sleep-deprived animals might show reduced CaLexA signal in dFB neurons as well. To test this idea, we examined the dFB CaLexA signal of flies that have been sleep deprived for 12 h during nighttime. Indeed, a significant reduction in CaLexA signal in dFB neurons was observed in sleep-deprived animals (S2I and S2J Fig), similar to the 3xBRP/4xBRP scenario, suggesting reduced neuronal activity in dFB neurons during sleep deprivation. While the circuit activity states representing distinct forms of sleep behavior still need to be addressed in more detail, obviously, PreScale has the propensity of reprogramming the cumulative activity status of these sleep-regulating circuits.

As suggested by the CaLexA results in dFB at age 5d genetically triggered by PreScale (Fig 2C and 2D) and the well-characterized role of the dFB neurons in promoting sleep [20,53], we speculated that the plastic remodeling of neuronal activity and firing patterns in dFB neurons might be changed upon early aging. Moreover, if PreScale was really responsible to age-adapt dFB physiology, it might be possible to impose similar dFB changes in young animals through genetically installing PreScale. As 3xBRP and 4xBRP are essentially similar in dFB CaLexA (Fig 2C and 2D) and 4xBRP exhibited stronger sleep phenotype [33], we decided to perform in vivo electrophysiological recording of the dFB neurons in 4xBRP compared to 2xBRP at the age 5d (S2K and S2L Fig), measured their spontaneous spiking patterns, and injected current steps to elicit membrane voltage responses. Notably, though mean spontaneous spiking activity frequency remained unaffected, the regularity of spontaneous spiking was enhanced and bursting spikes were rarely seen in 4xBRP dFB neurons (Fig 2E–2H). Consistent with a recent study showing that the regularity of spontaneous firing of the sleep-regulating DN1p neurons determines sleep quality [54], the pattern changes of spontaneous firing of dFB neurons might contribute to sleep regulation in 4xBRP animals.

To determine the membrane excitability properties of dFB neurons comparing 4xBRP with 2xBRP, we injected current steps to depolarize the membrane and elicit action potentials. The dFB neurons in 4xBRP flies tended to be electrically silent, with lower input resistance, fewer action potentials elicited, and increased interspike intervals (Fig 2I–2L), in agreement with the CaLexA results (Fig 2C and 2D). To directly compare the effects of PreScale to potential effects of aging here, we intended to measure the electric properties of dFB at an age of 30d. Unfortunately, however, rates of successful patching were extremely low in these aged animals. However, at age 20d, we could successfully measure a few dFB neurons. The dFB neurons were indeed more electric silent, which showed reduced input resistance and increased interspike intervals, and current step elicited action potential firing almost failed to raise in 20d flies (Fig 2M–2P). While qualitatively similar, effect sizes were clearly more pronounced in the comparison of 20d to 5d than comparing 2xBRP to 4xBRP at age 5d. These data suggest that the genetically installed PreScale can operate as surrogate for important aspects of early aging and interventions of PreScale-type plasticity might be potential paradigms for healthy aging.

Taken together, our imaging and electrophysiology results point towards a reprograming of neuronal activity across the fly brain under PreScale-type plasticity, which likely reflects a shared synaptic origin of sleep homeostasis and early brain aging. The functional reprograming of the sleep-promoting dFB/R5 network under PreScale suggests that PreScale might reprogram early aging-associated behavioral changes. We went on testing this hypothesis.

PreScale promotes survival over memory formation during early aging

In our previous study, as also mentioned earlier, we could linearly increase BRP levels by titrating the number of brp genomic copies, which promotes sleep in a dosage-dependent manner (S3A–S3F Fig) [33]. Direct quantitative comparisons of BRP levels between early aging and genetic brp titration at age 5d by line-fitting indicate that per se both scenarios provoked BRP up-regulations to similar extents (Fig 3A). If establishing certain BRP and the associated changes in Unc13A levels had specific functions, especially throughout early aging, genetically titrating BRP levels might allow for uncovering mechanisms of age-associated behavioral changes.

Fig 3. PreScale promotes survival over memory formation during early aging.

Fig 3

(A) Direct curve-fitting comparisons between PreScale during early aging from 1d to 30d (Fig 1) and genetic brp titration from 1xBRP to 4xBRP at age 5d [33]. (B and C) Lifespan analysis of flies with BRP titration from 1xBRP to 4xBRP. For male flies (B), n = 394 for 1xBRP (p < 0.001), n = 396 for 3xBRP (p < 0.001) and n = 395 for 4xBRP (p < 0.001), compared to 2xBRP wt (n = 393). For female flies (C), n = 400 for 1xBRP (p = 0.017), n = 399 for 3xBRP (ns) and n = 397 for 4xBRP (p < 0.001), compared to 2xBRP wt (n = 399). (D and E) An independent experiment of the lifespan of 2xBRP and 3xBRP flies. For male flies (D), n = 94 for 3xBRP compared to 2xBRP wt (n = 36, p < 0.001). For female flies (E), n = 98 for 3xBRP compared to 2xBRP wt (n = 59, p < 0.001). (F and G) Lifespan analysis of flies with 3xBRP in wake mutant background. For male flies (F), n = 234 for 2xBRP;wake compared to 2xBRP wt control (n = 233, p < 0.001) or compared to 3xBRP,wake (n = 155, p < 0.001). For female flies (G), n = 235 for 2xBRP;wake compared to 2xBRP wt control (n = 230, p < 0.001) or compared to 3xBRP,wake (n = 231, ns). (H and I) Lifespan analysis of flies with 3xBRP in inc mutant background. For male flies (H), n = 236 for inc;2xBRP compared to 2xBRP wt (n = 232, p = 0.009) or compared to inc;3xBRP (n = 212, p < 0.001). For female flies (I), n = 235 for inc;2xBRP compared to 2xBRP wt (n = 235, p < 0.001, wt is also shown in Fig 1A) or compared to inc;3xBRP (n = 182, ns). (J and K) Lifespan analysis of flies with 3xBRP in atg7 mutant background. For male flies (J), n = 108 for atg7,2xBRP compared to 2xBRP wt (n = 195, p < 0.001) or compared to atg7;3xBRP (n = 87, ns). For female flies (K), n = 109 for atg7,2xBRP compared to 2xBRP wt (n = 208, p < 0.001) or compared to atg7;3xBRP (n = 87, p < 0.001). (L and M) Lifespan analysis of flies with 3xBRP in Alzheimer’s disease model flies (elav>Aβ). For male flies (L), n = 203 for 2xBRP; compared to elav>mCD8-GFP control (n = 238, p < 0.001) or compared to 3xBRP; (n = 151, p = 0.006). For female flies (M), n = 249 for 2xBRP; compared to elav>mCD8-GFP control (n = 228, p < 0.001) or compared to 3xBRP; (n = 241, p = 0.002). Gehan–Breslow–Wilcoxon test with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons is shown for all longevity experiments. (N and O) STM for 5d (N) and 30d (O) 2xBRP and 3xBRP flies. n = 19 for 5d 2xBRP, n = 12 for 5d 3xBRP. n = 7–8 for 30d for both groups at 30d. (P and Q) MTM tested 3 h after training for 5d (P) and 30d (Q) 2xBRP and 3xBRP flies. n = 21 for 5d 2xBRP, n = 16 for 5d 3xBRP. n = 8–9 for 30d for both groups at 30d. Student t test is shown. *p < 0.05; ns, not significant. Error bars: mean ± SEM. Underlying data can be found in S3 Data. BRP, Bruchpilot; MTM, middle-term memory; STM, short-term memory; wt, wild type.

To this end, we first monitored the longevity of flies under BRP-level titration and observed an extension of lifespan when increasing brp gene copy number stepwise from 1xBRP to 3xBRP. However, 4xBRP longevity suffered severely, being clearly shorter than in 2xBRP wt animals (Figs 3B–3E and S3L–S3V). Thus, BRP levels seemingly show an optimum curve here, with moderate increases in 3xBRP representing a level protecting the survival of animals from stress conditions, for example, in response to sleep deprivation [33,41,42,55] and during early aging (Fig 1).

If moderately elevated PreScale was indeed protective for survival, it might be particularly protective for flies challenged by chronic sleep loss and/or flies suffered from reduced lifespan. Thus, we established a 3xBRP situation in two sleep mutants, wide awake (wake) and insomniac (inc) mutants, characterized by reduced sleep [5658]. Both mutants were recently shown to trigger PreScale likely in response to their sleep loss [33]. Indeed, 3xBRP rescued the lifespan of wake and inc male flies back to wt level (Fig 3F and 3H). At the same time, the survival of atg7 autophagy mutants as well as of Alzheimer’s disease model flies was not protected in both sexes (Fig 3J–3M). Interestingly, for female flies, 3xBRP only showed a tendency in lifespan extension in wake (Fig 3G), while the lifespan of inc was per se longer than wt and was not further extended by a 3xBRP background (Fig 3I). Because of the distinct sleep patterns between 5d female and male wt flies (S3G–S3K Fig), the sex difference in lifespan in wake and inc flies might be connected with the different impact from these mutations on sleep between male and female animals. Taken together, these data support a sex-specific, context-dependent role of PreScale in regulating lifespan.

As observed in model organisms and humans, memory formation and consolidation typically dwindle in their efficiency with advancing age [7,59,60]. Healthy aging, as an emerging concept [9,13], demands for not only longer life expectance but also specifically for a maintenance of life quality, importantly proper locomotor ability, as well as efficient sleep and memory formation abilities at older ages. Thus, we asked whether 3xBRP might be able to delay the normal age-associated memory decline, a phenomenon broadly reported for Drosophila [8,32,59]. Somewhat surprisingly, however, 3xBRP animals, while showing normal short-term memory (STM) at age 5d, displayed a significant reduction in STM at age 30d (Fig 3N and 3O). Middle-term aversive olfactory memory scores tested 3 h after training (3h MTM) were already significantly reduced in 3xBRP animals at age 5d (Fig 3P and 3Q).

Thus, moderately elevated PreScale in 3xBRP animals, while promoting lifespan in a context-dependent manner (Fig 3B–3M), at the same time decreased memory formation (Fig 3N–3Q). Given the dynamic nature of PreScale across the fly lifespan (Figs 1 and S1) and the lifespan extension of 3xBRP flies (Fig 3B–3E), an optimal level of PreScale during early aging might execute trade-offs to favor survival over complex and costly behaviors such as forming new memories [61,62].

PreScale mediates early aging-associated alterations in sleep behavior

An age-associated decline of daily locomotor activities is typical for animal models and humans [63,64], which is often accompanied by sleep pattern changes [27,31,65]. Furthermore, PreScale seems to reprogram the activity of the interconnected R5/dFB network (Figs 2C–2P and S2A–S2J). If the age-associated BRP increase was causally connected to locomotor and sleep pattern changes, genetic attenuation of PreScale might result in a delayed onset of locomotor decline and sleep pattern changes during early aging.

We recently showed that removing an endogenous brp gene copy out of two by introducing a null mutation in the brp locus (referred to as 1xBRP) significantly reduces BRP levels and subsequently provokes a down-regulation of other crucial synaptic proteins, importantly Unc13A as well as Syntaxin [33]. Interestingly, we also observed that while 1xBRP animals displayed only slightly reduced total sleep under baseline condition, they showed facilitated wakefulness under mechanical disturbance at age 5d [33]. Here, we examined the daily locomotor activity and sleep patterns across early aging stages from age 5d over 20d and 30d to an age of 40d in 1xBRP compared to 2xBRP.

While a robust gradual reduction of total daily locomotor activity was evident for both 2xBRP control and 1xBRP flies during early aging, 1xBRP exhibited higher daily locomotor activity compared to 2xBRP animals across all ages tested (Fig 4A and 4B). This difference was most prominent at age 20d during daytime, especially at the light/dark transition at ZT12 (zeitgeber time) across the tested ages (Fig 4A and 4B). Notably, 1xBRP protracted early aging-associated changes relative to 2xBRP flies in the pattern and amount of daily locomotor activity, evident in 20d 1xBRP resembling 5d 2xBRP, 30d 1xBRP being similar to 20d 2xBRP flies, and so on (Fig 4A and 4B). In short, it seems that during early aging, 1xBRP animals mimic younger 2xBRP control flies concerning total locomotor activity. A similar but even more pronounced effect was observed for the early aging-associated alterations of sleep pattern, especially daytime sleepiness, which was clearly suppressed in 1xBRP animals at all the ages tested (Fig 4C–4G). The suppression of daytime sleepiness was stronger at younger ages (i.e., 20d) evident in a substantial reduction of sleep episode number and duration (Fig 4C–4F). Of note, protraction of the decrease of sleep latency (at ZT0) was also observed for 1xBRP animals (Fig 4G). Nighttime sleep was also significantly suppressed by 1xBRP at different ages compared with age-matched 2xBRP control flies (Fig 4H). Interestingly, nighttime sleep was less consolidated and the difference in sleep latency at ZT12 diminished with age in 1xBRP animals compared to 2xBRP flies (Fig 4I–4K).

Fig 4. PreScale mediates early aging-associated sleep pattern changes.

Fig 4

(A and B) Locomotor walking activity distribution across the day (A) and averaged daily total walking activity (B) of 1xBRP compared to 2xBRP female flies at ages 5d, 20d, 30d, and 40d. (C-K) Sleep structure of 1xBRP and 2xBRP female flies at ages 5d, 20d, 30d, and 40d averaged from measurements over 2–3 days, including sleep profile plotted in 30-min bins (C), daytime and nighttime sleep amount (D and H), number and duration of sleep episodes (E, F, I, and J), and sleep latencies (G and K). (L) Activity index (average activity count per active minute) of 1xBRP and 2xBRP female flies at ages 5d, 20d, 30d, and 40d. Correlation analysis did not identify any significant age-associated difference in activity index of both 1xBRP and 2xBRP animals. n = 190–191 for 5d, n = 94–95 for 20d, n = 77–78 for 30d, and n = 32 for 40d. Two-way ANOVA with Sidak multiple comparisons is shown. (M) Sleep profile for 5d 2xBRP wt control and 5d 1xBRP female flies for 3 consecutive days. (N) Normalized cumulative sleep loss during 12-h nighttime sleep deprivation and 24-h sleep rebound. n = 116–121 for both groups. Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA with Fisher LSD test detected a significant genotype × time interaction (F(47, 11233) = 3.933; p < 0.0001) during sleep rebound. Asterisks above a line indicate time points at which the sleep percentage recovered differs significantly between female 2xBRP wt and 1xBRP flies, respectively. (O) Sleep recovered at three different time points after sleep deprivation for female 1xBRP compared to 2xBRP wt female flies at age 5d. n = 114–119 per group. Two-way ANOVA with Sidak multiple comparisons is shown. (P and Q) Locomotor walking activity (P) and sleep profile (Q) of 1xBRP female flies at age 20d rescued by introducing a transgenic brp copy (gBRP, brp P[acman]). n = 63–64 for all groups. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001; ns, not significant. Error bars: mean ± SEM. Underlying data can be found in S4 Data. LSD, least significant difference; SD, sleep deprivation; wt, wild type.

We speculated that if the 1xBRP constellation would attenuate the early aging-associated changes of sleep pattern, it might also show a stronger sleep rebound upon sleep deprivation, as aging is typically associated with a decline in sleep rebound in response to sleep deprivation [29]. To test this idea, we sleep-deprived both 1xBRP and 2xBRP flies at age 5d during nighttime for measuring sleep rebound afterwards (Fig 4M). In agreement with our speculation, 1xBRP flies indeed exhibited enhanced sleep rebound (Fig 4N and 4O). We here also performed an additional control experiment by putting an artificial genomic brp gene copy back to 1xBRP flies, which should restore wt-type sleep patterns. Indeed, the early aging-associated sleep pattern changes normally seen in 2xBRP wt flies were restored for these flies (Figs 4P, 4Q, and S4A–S4D), further supporting the specific role of BRP-driven PreScale-type plasticity in promoting early aging-associated sleep pattern changes and survival. At the first glance, an apparent paradox emerges in that 1xBRP animals concerning their sleep pattern regulation appear to age slower but are ultimately shorter lived, when compared to 2xBRP controls (Figs 3B, 3C and 4). Together with the extended lifespan of 3xBRP animals (Fig 3B–3E), this might be explained by the early aging-associated alterations of locomotor activity and sleep behavior being age adaptive and survival protective (Fig 4A–4L).

Contrary to 1xBRP (Fig 4), 3xBRP likely operates as a mimicry of the essential aspects of brain aging, which favors survival over cognitive functions (Figs 1, 2, 3, S1, and S3). Indeed, 3xBRP animals displayed a mild reduction in total locomotor activity (S4E and S4F Fig) and increased and consolidated sleep (S4G–S4O Fig) relative to 2xBRP wt controls at age 5d. The 2xBRP versus 3xBRP difference diminished gradually with age (S4E–S4O Fig). It appears that the genetically installed moderate PreScale increase in 3xBRP animals likely augments an early aging-associated brain reprogramming, which is important for later survival during advanced aging, as also suggested by the consequences of sleep loss early in life [66,67].

Spermidine supplementation attenuates early aging-associated sleep pattern changes

Aging is typically associated with behavioral alterations affecting life quality and consequent burden of aging human societies [9,13]. Spermidine (Spd) supplementation has recently emerged as a promising paradigm to promote healthy aging, supported by studies ranging from flies and rodent models to human clinical trials [9]. In flies, Spd supplementation was shown to increase lifespan and attenuate early aging-associated memory decline [10,15,68]. Spd supplementation effects are based on metabolic alterations, age-protecting autophagic clearance, and mitochondrial respiration [10,15,68,69]. Importantly, we previously showed that age-associated PreScale is efficiently suppressed by Spd supplementation during early aging stages (for example, at age 30d), while it does not affect PreScale in young animals [8]. These studies, together with our current findings (Figs 1 and S1), suggest that Spd supplementation might be able to delay the onset of age-associated behavioral alterations, importantly locomotion and sleep pattern changes (S5A Fig).

Indeed, the daily locomotor activity of Spd-supplied 30d flies was significantly higher when compared with age-matched control flies, while no difference was observed in 5d young animals, consistent with a solely aging-protective role of Spd supplementation (Fig 5A and 5B). Moreover, the early aging-associated sleep pattern changes were suppressed to a certain degree, especially concerning the daytime sleep phenotypes (Fig 5C–5G). Here, daytime sleep increases were substantially suppressed through a significant reduction in the number of sleep episodes with concomitant increases of sleep latency (Fig 5C–5E and 5G). Nighttime sleep changes were also suppressed mainly through an increase of sleep latency towards the values normally found in 5d young flies, while the number of nighttime sleep episodes and mean sleep episode duration stayed comparable to unsupplied siblings (Fig 5C–5G). All these changes were specific for 30d flies, as the sleep of 5d young flies was not affected by Spd supplementation (Fig 5A–5G), again consistent with the notion that Spd supplementation specifically affects the aging process [8,10,68,69]. Importantly, the typical age-associated reduction of sleep rebound upon sleep deprivation (Fig 5H–5J) was suppressed as well, allowing for a more efficient sleep compensation when reacting to sleep loss in 30d flies (Fig 5K–5M), but not in 5d flies (S5B–S5E Fig).

Fig 5. Spd supplementation attenuates early aging-associated sleep pattern changes.

Fig 5

(A) Locomotor walking activity pattern of 5 mM Spd-treated wt female flies across the day at ages 5d and 30d. (B) Averaged daily locomotor walking activity of 5 mM Spd-treated wt female flies at ages 5d and 30d. Student t test is shown. (C-G) Sleep structure of 5 mM Spd-treated wt female flies at ages 5d and 30d averaged from measurements over 2–3 days, including sleep profile plotted in 30-min bins (C), daytime and nighttime sleep amount (D), number and duration of sleep episodes (E and F), and sleep latencies (G). n = 93–96 for all groups. One-way ANOVA with Bonferroni multiple comparisons test is shown. (H) Sleep profile for wt female flies at age 30d compared to 5d for 3 consecutive days. (I) Normalized cumulative sleep loss during 12-h nighttime sleep deprivation and 24-h sleep rebound. Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA with Fisher LSD test detected a significant age × time interaction (F(47, 5280) = 4.227; p < 0.0001) during sleep rebound. Asterisks above a line indicate time points at which the sleep percentage recovered differs significantly between 5d and 30d wt female flies, respectively. (J) Sleep recovered at three different time points after sleep deprivation for 30d wt compared to 5d female flies. n = 55–57 for both groups. Two-way ANOVA with Sidak multiple comparisons is shown. (K) Sleep profile for 30d wt female flies treated with 5 mM Spd compared to untreated for 3 consecutive days. (L) Normalized cumulative sleep loss during 12-h nighttime sleep deprivation and 24-h sleep rebound. Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA with Fisher LSD test detected a significant treatment × time interaction (F(47, 4465) = 7.168; p < 0.0001) during sleep rebound. Asterisks above a line indicate time points at which the sleep percentage recovered differs significantly between 30d 5 mM Spd-treated and untreated female flies, respectively. (M) Sleep recovered at three different time points after sleep deprivation for 30d 5 mM Spd-treated compared to untreated female flies. n = 47–50 for both groups. Two-way ANOVA with Sidak multiple comparisons is shown. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001; ns, not significant. Error bars: mean ± SEM. Underlying data can be found in S5 Data. LSD, least significant difference; Spd, spermidine; wt, wild type.

As mentioned earlier, we previously showed that Spd-supplied flies display suppressed PreScale during early aging [8]. Taken together, the effects of 1xBRP and Spd supplementation on early aging-associated sleep pattern changes and PreScale-type plasticity (Figs 4, 5, S4 and S5) [8] suggest that PreScale is at least partially responsible for early aging-associated sleep pattern changes. We suspect that, under Spd supplementation, the normal early aging-associated increase of PreScale and consequently behavioral changes might no longer be required, allowing for preserved memory and a delay of age-associated synaptic changes coupled with an alleviation of daytime sleepiness. Thus, it is tempting to speculate that metabolic reprogramming by Spd supplementation via rejuvenating autophagy and mitochondrial respiration [10,14,68,69] might suppress early aging-associated sleep patterns triggered by PreScale.

Acute deep sleep reverses early aging-associated memory decline and PreScale-type plasticity

As mentioned earlier, in addition to sleep pattern changes (Figs 4, 5, S4, and S5), Drosophila aging is also associated with a cognitive decline evident in the reduced formation of aversive olfactory memories (Fig 6A and 6B) [10,32,59]. Thus, we intended to further address the intersection of longevity, the ability of forming new memories, and early aging-associated sleep pattern changes, in conjunction with the role of PreScale plasticity. If PreScale-encoded additional sleep during early aging served specific functions, and PreScale indeed represented an acutely operating active mechanism to promote such changes, then triggering excessive deep sleep in mid-aged flies might allow to reset these age-associated behavioral and molecular regulations. To test this, we turned to the acute feeding of sleep-promoting Gaboxadol (4,5,6,7-Tetrahydroisoxazolo[5,4-c]pyridin-3-ol, also called THIP), which has been shown to rescue memory formation in standard memory mutants and Alzheimer’s disease model flies [70,71].

Fig 6. Acute deep sleep reverses age-associated memory decline.

Fig 6

(A and B) Protocol for aging-related memory experiments (A) and typical age-associated memory decline in ages 20d and 30d wt flies (B), including STM, 1h MTM, and 3h MTM. (C and D) Protocol (C) for 0.025 mg ml−1 THIP-treated wt flies at age 2d for 48 h and tested thereafter for STM and 1h MTM immediately, compared to untreated siblings (D). n = 13 per group for STM, n = 8 per group for 1h MTM. (E and F) Protocol (E) for 0.025 mg ml−1 THIP-treated wt flies at age 30d for 48 h and tested thereafter for STM and 1h MTM immediately, compared to untreated siblings (F). n = 12 per group for STM, n = 6 per group for 1h MTM. (G and H) With the protocol in Fig 6E, two higher THIP concentrations 0.05 mg ml−1 (G) and 0.1 mg ml−1 (H) were used to treat wt flies at age 30d for 48 h and tested thereafter for STM immediately, compared to untreated siblings. n = 14 per group for 0.05 mg ml−1, n = 8 per group for 0.1 mg ml−1. (I and J) Protocol (I) for 0.05 mg ml−1 THIP-treated wt flies at age 30d for 72 h and tested thereafter for STM immediately, compared to untreated siblings (J). n = 6 per group. (K and L) Protocol (K) for 0.05 mg ml−1 THIP-treated wt flies at age 30d for 48 h and tested 24 h after treatment for STM, 1h MTM, 1h ARM, and ASM, compared to untreated siblings (L). n = 10–12 for all groups. (M and N) Protocol (M) for 0.1 mg ml−1 THIP-treated wt flies at age 30d for 48 h and tested 24 h after treatment for STM, 1h MTM, 1h ARM, and 1h ASM, compared to untreated siblings (N). n = 16 per group for STM, n = 10 per group for 1h MTM, ARM, and ASM. (O and P) With the protocol in Fig 6K except that 2d wt flies were used, odor avoidance was tested for the two odors (3-Octanol (O) and MCH (P), i.e., 4-methylcyclohexanol) used for olfactory memory experiments. n = 8 per group. Dashed lines indicate the average scores of respective memory components of 5d control wt flies without THIP treatment. (Q and R) Protocol (Q) for 0.1 mg ml−1 THIP-treated wt flies at age 30d and 50d for 48 h and tested 24 h after treatment for 1h MTM compared to untreated siblings (R). n = 7 per group. One-way ANOVA with Bonferroni multiple comparisons test is shown. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001; ns, not significant. Error bars: mean ± SEM. Underlying data can be found in S6 Data. ARM, anesthesia-resistant memory; ASM, anesthesia-sensitive memory; MTM, middle-term memory; STM, short-term memory; THIP, 4,5,6,7-Tetrahydroisoxazolo[5,4-c]pyridin-3-ol; wt, wild type.

Before testing memory with different protocols of THIP treatment, we first confirmed the sleep-promoting effects of THIP by feeding 5d young wt flies with both a low concentration of 0.025 mg ml−1 (0.025THIP) and a higher concentration of 0.1 mg ml−1 (0.1THIP) (S6A–S6F Fig) [70]. As feeding with 0.025THIP had already substantial sleep-promoting effects (S6A–S6F Fig), we first decided to feed young flies with 0.025THIP at age 2d for 2 days (48 h) and measure olfactory memory immediately thereafter (Fig 6C). After training flies using paired associative aversive olfactory conditioning, the animals were tested either right away for STM or 1 h later for 1h MTM. We did not find any differences in both STM and 1h MTM between 0.025THIP fed and unfed flies at age 2d (Fig 6C and 6D). It seems plausible that juvenile flies are still capable of forming memory at a maximum capacity and additional acute sleep might just not sufficient to boost olfactory memory formation. Thus, we continued by testing 30d flies, which displayed clearly reduced memories when compared to 5d young animals (Fig 6A and 6B).

However, feeding 30d wt flies with 0.025THIP for 48 h followed by immediate testing for olfactory memory did not show any improvement for both STM and 1h MTM (Fig 6E and 6F). We wondered if the concentration 0.025THIP was insufficient to provoke any beneficial effects and thus increased the concentrations to 0.05THIP and further to 0.1THIP. However, both concentrations did not provoke any significant improvement in STM (Figs 6E, 6G, 6H, and S6G–S6K). It should be said, however, that when feeding 0.05THIP with this protocol, STM scores tended to increase (Fig 6G), though not significantly. Another obvious protocol was to increase the duration of THIP feeding to 3 days (72 h), which, however, also did not improve STM (Fig 6I and 6J).

We next decided to feed 30d flies with 0.05THIP for 48 h but tested memories only 24 h after the end of THIP feeding (Fig 6K). Indeed, though STM was comparable to unfed control flies, 1h MTM was clearly improved over levels of unfed controls of the same age (Fig 6K and 6L). MTM is composed of two memory components that can be dissected through amnestic cooling, anesthesia-resistant memory (ARM) and anesthesia-sensitive memory (ASM) [22,23]. ARM is measured after amnestic cooling and ASM derives from subtracting ARM from MTM scores [22,23]. Notably, both ARM and ASM were fully restored by 0.05THIP treatment (Fig 6K and 6L). We further tested with a higher concentration of 0.1 mg ml−1 THIP (0.1THIP) (Fig 6M) and found that STM was also improved, and 1h MTM, ARM, and ASM were all fully restored, just like under 0.05THIP treatment (Fig 6M and 6N). Importantly, THIP-feeding did not affect odor sensation, which is critical for olfactory memory formation (Fig 6O and 6P).

Next, we asked whether the same paradigm would suffice to reverse memory decline in flies during advanced aging (for example, 50-day-old), compared to flies during early aging (for example, 30-day-old). Importantly, 50d flies, which normally show a drastic decrease in 1h MTM, did not show any improvement upon 0.1THIP treatment when compared to 30d flies (Fig 6Q and 6R). Collectively, our results suggest that early aging is plastic and reversible and that the molecular and behavioral changes associated with this period are of adaptive nature in the realms of better survival. In contrast, brains of advanced age (50d) appear to lack plasticity and be rather nonreversible.

As shown above, a quasi-linear BRP increase (Fig 3A) is associated with the occurrence of early aging-associated dFB activity reprogramming, sleep pattern changes, memory decline, as well as longevity (Figs 16). Furthermore, Spd supplementation suppresses both sleep pattern changes (Figs 5 and S5) and PreScale during early aging [8] and promotes memory and longevity [8,14,15]. We wondered if acute THIP-feeding, which rescued memory decline (Fig 6K–6N), might also suppress the early aging-associated PreScale indicated by elevated BRP levels and sleep patter changes, similar to that of Spd supplementation (Fig 7A). Indeed, immunohistological examination revealed a clear reduction of BRP levels in 0.1 mg ml−1 THIP-fed 30d flies when compared to unfed siblings (Fig 7B–7D). Notably, this was specific to age 30d flies, as BRP levels were not different in 0.1THIP-fed 5d brains from nonsupplemented sibling controls (Fig 7B–7D). Sleep pattern changes were also suppressed by 0.1THIP treatment tested 1 day after the treatment specifically in 30d animals (Fig 7E–7L), but not in 5d young animals (S7 Fig).

Fig 7. Excessive deep sleep reverses age-associated active zone plasticity/PreScale and boosts lifespan.

Fig 7

(A and B) Rationale (A) and protocol (B) for immunochemistry examinations of fly brains of 0.1 mg ml−1 THIP-treated wt female flies at ages 5d and 30d. Spd supplementation suppresses early aging-associated PreScale, memory decline, and sleep pattern changes, which might functionally intersect for survival. However, though THIP treatment suppresses age-associated memory decline, its effects on PreScale and early aging-associated sleep pattern changes were unclear. (C and D) Confocal images (C) and whole-mount brain staining analysis (D) of BRP in wt female flies at ages 5d and 30d with or without 0.1 mg ml−1 THIP treatment. n = 24–25 for all groups. Scale bar: 50 μm. (E) Protocol for sleep test of wt female flies, which have been treated with 0.1 mg ml−1 THIP for 2 days at age 30d. (F and G) Locomotor walking activity pattern (F) and statistic (G) of 30d wt flies after 2 days of 0.1 mg ml−1 THIP treatment. (H-L) Sleep structure of 30d wt female flies after 2 days of 0.1 mg ml−1 THIP treatment averaged from measurements over 2 days, including sleep profile plotted in 30-min bins (H), daytime and nighttime sleep amount (I), number and duration of sleep episodes (J and K), and sleep latencies (L). n = 90–95 for all groups. (M-P) Protocol (M and O) and 1h MTM (N and P) for 2xBRP and 3xBRP flies, which have been treated with 0.1 mg ml−1 THIP for 2 days at age 2d or 30d. n = 13–16 for all groups. Student t test is shown for comparison between two groups. One-way ANOVA with Bonferroni multiple comparisons test is shown for comparisons of three groups. ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001; ns, not significant. Error bars: mean ± SEM. (Q) Protocol for longevity of 0.05 mg ml−1 THIP-treated wt flies. (R and S) Lifespan analysis of wt male and female flies treated with 0.05 mg ml−1 THIP. For male flies (R), n = 176 for treated compared to untreated (n = 150, ns). For female flies (S), n = 199 for treated compared to untreated (n = 192, p < 0.001). Gehan–Breslow–Wilcoxon test is shown. ***p < 0.001; ns, not significant. Error bars: mean ± SEM. Underlying data can be found in S7 Data. BRP, Bruchpilot; MTM, middle-term memory; Spd, spermidine; THIP, 4,5,6,7-Tetrahydroisoxazolo[5,4-c]pyridin-3-ol; wt, wild type.

If the suppression of PreScale (Fig 7A–7D) was causal for the restoration of memory in THIP-treated mid-aged 30d animals (Fig 6K–6R), increasing brp gene copy number from 2xBRP to 3xBRP might attenuate the improvement in memory formation. Thus, we again utilized the same THIP paradigm (Fig 7M and 7O) for comparing 2xBRP with 3xBRP animals at either age 2d or 30d. While 0.1THIP treatment did not show any obvious effect in 2d animals (Fig 7N), it consistently improved 1h MTM of 30d 2xBRP. In contrast, no such benefit was observed for 30d 3xBRP animals (Fig 7P). Thus, these data suggest a causal relationship between reducing PreScale and being able to effectively form new memories for mid-aged flies.

Motivated by the suppression of early aging-associated PreScale via THIP and Spd supplementation (Fig 7A–D) [8], and the longevity extension effect provoked by Spd supplementation [14,15], we tested the lifespan of wt flies fed with 0.05 mg ml−1 THIP chronically starting at age 2d (Fig 7Q). While wt male flies showed a slight tendency towards lifespan extension (Fig 7R), female flies lived significantly longer upon 0.05THIP-feeding compared to unfed siblings (Fig 7S). The sex-specific difference here might be explained by the lower baseline sleep levels of females and the consequent greater dynamic range of the THIP-mediated sleep promotion (S3G–S3K Fig). In summary, the induction of acute, extensive deep sleep in mid-aged flies seemingly allows for a reset of processes underlying and driving brain aging, in effect also promoting lifespan, though in a sex-specific manner.

Taken together, we here used two distinct rejuvenation paradigms, Gaboxadol/THIP treatment and Spd supplementation, which provoked concordant effects: restoration of memory formation, increased longevity, and, importantly, making PreScale dispensable in the brains of mid-aged flies (Figs 5, 6 and 7). While we propose that PreScale is formed “on demand” in response to neuronal physiological state changes that build in response to normal aging and are susceptible to THIP and Spd, PreScale and subsequent trade-offs might be no longer “on demand” under rejuvenation paradigms. Given the roles of PreScale in controlling early aging-associated sleep pattern changes and longevity (Figs 1, 3 and 4), this form of plasticity emerges at the intersection of sleep, longevity, and age-associated memory decline. Notably, PreScale is also reversibly triggered by sleep deprivation [33,41,42], suggesting a generic and acute requirement of attenuating PreScale in resetting and denoising of circuits.

Discussion

Resilience designates the capacity of a system, enterprise, or person to maintain the core purpose and integrity in the face of drastically changing circumstances [16,72]. Resilience in human physiology is the capacity of adaptation in counteracting stress and adversity, while maintaining normal psychological and physical functions [73]. A key result of stress probably lies in the plastic reorganization of neural architecture and synaptic connections, which in turn may be a sign of successful adaptation [17,72]. Thereby, sleep homeostasis is tightly linked to resilience mediated by the brain [72,73]. Animal and patient data indicate that the impaired sleep, statistically associated with neurodegeneration, is not only a result of an underlying circuit malfunction but might also directly contribute to neurodegeneration [74].

The aging process particularly challenges brain resilience [16,17,72]. Typically, age-associated molecular and behavioral changes occur gradually with aging [2,10,32,64]. In particular, cognitive functions and sleep patterns change progressively with increasing age [18,31,59,65]. For sleep, human studies reported diverse sleep alterations in old adults, including difficulties to fall asleep, the inability to maintain sleep (insomnia), and daytime sleepiness (hypersomnia) [75]. Similarly, in flies, age-associated alterations of sleep patterns have been extensively reported but might be sensitive to the exact experimental conditions chosen [2731,65,76]. Still, the mechanisms mediating the age-associated sleep pattern changes are poorly understood. Specifically, we still miss critical information of how aging affects neuronal and synaptic organization and plasticity, how such changes intersect with age-associated alterations of sleep pattern and cognitive function, whether neuronal and synaptic changes might be causally involved in age-associated behavioral changes and resilience, and whether critical periods for establishing resilience might exist across the aging trajectory. As a matter of fact, brain resilience might protect these entities in a concordant fashion, suggesting shared mechanisms to be at play, while differentiating truly causative from only correlative or even adaptive and protective changes is a fundamental challenge.

Our previous studies described a shared molecular signature at central Drosophila brain synapses upon aging [8] or sleep loss [33], which we here collectively refer to as PreScale. In this study, we investigated the dynamic range and temporal profile of PreScale across the fly lifespan and explored its consequences for cognitive and organismal fitness and longevity. We provide evidence suggesting that this form of broadly distributed presynaptic plasticity provides a resilience module, which forms on demand and allows the brain to cope with the consequences of early aging and sleep deprivation. At the same time, PreScale might acutely prioritize tasks and establish trade-offs between survival and longevity on the one and memory formation on the other hand (Fig 8). These conclusions are based on the following new findings.

Fig 8. A model for presynaptic active zone plasticity (PreScale) in executing trade-offs between memory and longevity.

Fig 8

A model for the core AZ scaffold protein BRP-driven PreScale in executing trade-offs between sleep homeostasis, memory formation, and longevity. Early aging provokes PreScale-type plasticity and adaptive sleep pattern changes and subsequently steers trade-offs between memory formation and longevity, which can be mimicked by titrating the gene copies of BRP. Rejuvenation paradigms like spermidine supplementation and Gaboxadol/THIP treatment during early aging eliminate the need of PreScale for regulating adaptive sleep patterns to steer trade-offs between memory and longevity. As a consequence, these paradigms suppress PreScale to allow for new memory formation and lifespan extension. Thus, PreScale likely executes behavioral adaptations and trade-offs during a still plastic phase of early brain aging, illustrating how life strategy manifests on a circuit and synaptic plasticity level. AZ, active zone; BRP, Bruchpilot; Spd, spermidine.

Firstly, PreScale normally builds up with age to peak at mid-age, but at later advanced age, it declines again (Fig 1). Aging and sleep loss as two important stressors seemingly converge to trigger an at least very similar scenario of presynaptic active zone plasticity here. Notably, PreScale was reverted when mid-aged animals were pharmacologically triggered with extensive sleep for 2 days (Fig 7B–7D) and also does not form in mid-aged animals when supplied with Spd [8].

Secondly, concerning resilience, when genetically mimicked to a moderate degree (3xBRP), PreScale promoted organismal longevity (Fig 3A–3E). It is certainly possible that chronic genetically induced PreScale (via BRP copy number titration) might, in some regards, differ from the presynaptic plasticity observed in either acutely sleep-deprived animals or flies of mid-age. Still, direct comparisons indeed support that our experimental BRP titration can serve as a surrogate of early aging concerning behavioral alterations (Figs 27). Importantly, 3xBRP efficiently promoted longevity in flies chronically challenged by sleep loss (Fig 3F and 3H) but proved inefficient or even counterproductive in neurodegenerative conditions of other kind (Fig 3J–3M). In contrast, genetically triggering very high, likely unphysiological BRP levels (4xBRP) robustly shortened lifespan (Figs 3A–3C and S3L–S3V), arguing in direction of a “sweet spot” concerning its resilience-promoting function. Consistent with the dynamic range of PreScale over the aging trajectory (Figs 1 and S1), the critical period in relation to longevity promotion and memory restoration might indeed be the early aging period until mid-age, while flies of advanced age are seemingly no longer plastic and nonreversible in essential organismal functions including memory formation, at least under standard rearing conditions (Fig 6Q and 6R). Consistently, a recent study showed that rapamycin treatment in early adulthood suffices to extend lifespan but proves inefficient when applied at advanced age [77]. As we found BRP to promote sleep and regulate locomotion patterns in this early aging period (Figs 4 and S4), PreScale-driven processes might well be causally related to organismal resilience and extended longevity (Fig 8).

Thirdly, moderate BRP increases (3xBRP) tended to slightly reduce learning and memory capacity and block the beneficial effects of THIP treatment in the early aging period (Figs 3N–3Q and 7M–7P). Notably, high (4xBRP) levels even more severely reduce olfactory learning and memory [8]. The increased energy investment to support formation of long-term memories operates in a trade-off relation to save energy for better survival under food shortage or at advanced age [61,62]. Similarly, early aging-associated sleep pattern changes, for example, daytime sleepiness, could favor lower levels of energy consumption and consequently promote survival. Thus, PreScale might steer the trade-offs between the acute needs to entertain costly behaviors and/or to support sheer organismal fitness and longevity.

It might be asked why our artificial increase of brp gene dose from wt 2xBRP to 3xBRP happens to be advantageous concerning longevity under our rearing conditions. Interestingly, learning ability can be substantially improved by artificial selection in animals ranging from Drosophila to rats [78], while Drosophila has been shown to make use of learned cues when tested in seminatural environments [79]. Along those lines, it will be very interesting to measure PreScale across the lifespan under different culture conditions, for example, aging at lower temperatures (i.e., 18°C compared to 25°C) was shown to extend lifespan and memory formation period [32].

PreScale might be a molecular/physiological manifestation of life strategy decisions balancing principal life history–related trade-offs. However, how would PreScale possible execute trade-offs between memory and longevity, given that PreScale itself is sleep-promoting? As healthy sleep supports subsequent learning, it might be possible that genetic installation of PreScale promotes a kind of sleep, which does not support all functional aspects of memory-supporting sleep, leading to specific lifespan extension without benefiting cognitive functions. Considering the recently reported different sleep stages in flies [80,81] and the sleep-supporting slow wave activities in the R5 network [50], PreScale-encoded sleep need might define certain sleep stages that specifically promote lifespan but not memory formation. Indeed, we recently showed that the STM impairment of 4xBRP animals was fully rescued when BRP was eliminated specifically in the R5 neurons, while sleep was only partially suppressed [33]. Thus, it is possible that such a manipulation allowed for a rebalance of the trade-offs, which might reopen the window for executing the memory function of PreScale-encoded sleep, providing a plausible alternative explanation for early aging-associated memory impairment.

From our collective data, both Spd supplementation as well as triggering excessive sleep by THIP treatment allowed to attenuate PreScale in mid-aged flies (Figs 7A–7D and 8) [8]. We speculate that some vital physiological cellular regulations might be no longer sufficiently plastic but instead steer into a detrimental direction at advanced age. Notably, Spd supplementation protects both mitochondrial respiration and autophagic flux from age-associated functional decay [68,69]. Efficient mitochondrial electron transport and autophagy in turn are coupled to sleep regulation [82,83]. Moreover, mitochondrial functionality bidirectionally regulates early aging-associated PreScale-type plasticity revealed by BRP levels [35], and autophagy defects trigger PreScale in a non-cell-autonomous manner [84]. Thus, we speculate that Spd might delay the onset of early aging-associated sleep pattern changes and memory decline through its improvements of mitochondrial functions and autophagy, while making PreScale dispensable. Acute Gaboxadol/THIP treatment might equally reset the aging brain by reversing the early aging-associated decline in mitochondrial and autophagic functions [83]. It should be warranting to further dissect the molecular mechanisms that mediate the coupling between mitochondrial and autophagic status and the trade-offs of PreScale with early aging-associated sleep pattern changes and memory decline.

Concerning the mechanistic action of PreScale on a synaptic and neurophysiological level, we observed a reprogramming of neuronal activity patterns of R5 neurons (S2A–S2E Fig) [33] and dFB neurons (Fig 2C and 2D), and the firing pattern and membrane excitability of the dFB neurons were changed upon genetic installation of PreScale and early aging (Fig 2E–2P). Interestingly, both reducing and decreasing brp gene copies suppressed the activity of dFB neurons, indicated by the Ca2+-dependent activity reporter CaLexA [48], which is meant to be a proxy for the activity history of neurons (Figs 2C, 2D and S2F–S2J). Taken together, we reasoned that the reduced dFB CaLexA signal in 1xBRP animals likely represents reduced and less consolidated sleep, while in 3xBRP and 4xBRP animals, it mimics a sleep-deprived state [33]. Alternatively, because dFB and R5 neurons receive different information input in the brain network, for example, dFB neurons receive inputs from wake-promoting dopaminergic neurons [53]. In addition, a circadian output circuit innervates R5 neurons to regulate sleep [85,86]. Thus, the different activity states of dFB and R5 neurons might represent a complex physiological state that integrates different input information to coordinate behaviors.

Notably, a recent study suggests that dFB neurons promote a specific form of sleep with brain activity that is different from spontaneous sleep [81]. As PreScale can per se change short-term plasticity and the transmission efficacy in a frequency-dependent manner [33], we speculate that this form of synapse remodeling might promote the transmission of specific activity patterns. Favoring transmission in certain frequency domains might also explain its sleep-promoting action, as previous work identified slow wave oscillatory activities to be associated with sleep regulation via ellipsoid body R5 neurons [50]. In addition to R5 neurons, the interconnected dFB neurons have been shown to exhibit oscillatory activity and regulate both sleep and memory [20,70,71,80,81]. It is tempting to speculate that dFB neurons might be at the intersection of PreScale-mediated trade-offs between longevity and cognitive functions, and the reduced neuronal activity and membrane excitability might explain the impaired memory in 3xBRP and 4xBRP animals. Potentially in the dFB/R5 network, skewing the representation of sensory information might also explain the reduced proclivity of flies under PreScale for forming new memories (Fig 8). How exactly the distinct effects of PreScale integrate from synapse over circuit and neuronal activity to behavioral level remains a warranting question for future analysis.

Materials and methods

Fly genetics and maintenance

w1118 (iso31, BDSC#5905) was used as wt, 2xBRP, and background control. A brp null mutation (c04298, BDSC#85966) was utilized to reduce brp gene copy [33,40]. A genomic brp P[acman] construct [87], which was mapped to be integrated into the 5′ UTR of CG11357 (S3L and S3M Fig) [88], was used to increase brp gene copy number from 2xBRP to 3xBRP and 4xBRP [33] or to rescue the sleep phenotypes of 1xBRP flies (gBRP). A line with a P-element inserted at the 5′ UTR of CG11357 (EY12484, BDSC#20838) was acquired to mimic the effects of a P-element at CG11357 5′ UTR, which was largely comparable to wt (S3N–S3V Fig). Short sleep mutants wake (EY02219, BDSC#15858) and inc (f00285, BDSC#18307) and autophagy atg7 mutant (d06996, BDSC#19257) were described previously [56,57,89]. Alzheimer’s disease model flies were generated by pan-neuronal elav-Gal4-driven expression of Aβ-arctic (BDSC#458 and #33774). UAS-mCD8.GFP (BDSC#32188) was expressed by elav-Gal4 as control for Alzheimer’s disease model flies or by R23E10-Gal4 for patch-clamp electrophysiology. CaLexA (BDSC#66542) was expressed pan-neuronally by elav-Gal4 or in either dFB neurons marked by R23E10-Gal4 [53] or R5 neurons driven by R58H05-Gal4 [51]. All fly strains were backcrossed to w1118 wt background for at least six generations to remove potential genetic modifiers.

Flies were raised under standard laboratory conditions on semidefined medium (Bloomington recipe) under 12/12h light/dark cycles with 65% humidity at 25°C. Unless specifically stated, female flies at certain ages were used except aversive olfactory memory experiments in which mixed populations of both sexes were used.

Aging and longevity

During the development at larval stages, the density of larvae was purposely controlled across groups and genotypes. Aging was performed with mixed populations of male and female flies. To avoid stressing the flies during aging (for example, flies step on each other), the amount of flies within each individual vials was grossly controlled across groups, genotypes, and experiments. The aging flies were routinely flipped onto fresh food every second day or over the weekends until a desired age was reached for experiments.

Longevity experiments were carried out similarly, except that female and male flies were separated and sorted into a population of approximately 20 to 25 flies per vial/replicate at age 2d after fully mating. To reduce the variability in lifespan, a few different cohorts of flies were used for each experiment. Similar to aging flies, the longevity flies were regularly transferred onto fresh food and the amount of dead flies in each vial was recorded at each time of transfer until the death of the last fly of a replicate.

Western blotting

Western blot analysis was carried out as previously reported [33]. Basically, female flies at specific ages were dissected in ice-cold Ringer’s solution (pH = 7.3, mOsm = 290 to 310, with 5 mM HEPES-NaOH, 130 mM NaCl, 5 mM KCl, 2 mM MgCl2, 2 mM CaCl2, and 36 mM sucrose) between ZT6 and ZT10 (ZT, zeitgeber time). The brain samples were stored at −20°C for short term (approximately 2 weeks) or at −80°C for longer. Brain samples of the same experiment were fully homogenized by three rounds of intense vortexing in lysis buffer (0.5% Triton X-100, 2% SDS, 1× protease inhibitor, 1× sample buffer in PBS) followed by full-speed centrifugation for 6 min at 18°C. The samples were then intensively vortexed to resuspend the brain tissue and centrifuged again for 6 min at 18°C. To avoid any saturation in the amount of proteins for detecting the differences, less than one brain’s supernatant was loaded into each lane for SDS-PAGE and immunoblotting. Thereafter, the blots were manually developed with ECL solutions and Kodak/GE films. The following primary antibodies were used: mouse anti-BRP Nc82 (1:1,000 to 1:2,000), rabbit anti-BRP D2 (rb5228, 1:100,000), guinea pig anti-Unc13A (14GP17, 1:2,000), mouse anti-Dlg1 (4F3, 1:3,000), mouse anti-Syn (3C11, 1:2,000), mouse anti-Tubulin (Sigma T9026, 1:100,000), mouse anti-Syx (8C3, 1:2,000), and rabbit anti-Atg8a (Ab109364, 1:1,000). The developed films without saturation were scanned by an EPSON V330 scanner in 16-bit grayscale tiff format [33].

Immunostaining

Immunostaining was performed exactly as previously described [33]. Adult female flies at certain ages, after sleep deprivation, or after THIP treatment were dissected in ice-cold Ringer’s solution and immediately fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde (pH = approximately 7.3) for 30 min at room temperature. After fixing, brains were washed in 0.7% PBST (PBS with 0.7% Triton X-100, v/v) for 3 or 4 times for a total of 1 h and blocked in 0.7% PBST with 10% normal goat serum (v/v) for at least 2 h at room temperature. Primary antibodies were diluted in 0.7% PBST with 5% NGS for primary antibody incubation at 4°C. Afterwards, brains were washed again in 0.7% PBST for at least 4 times and then incubated with secondary antibodies diluted in 0.7% PBST with 5% NGS in darkness. Finally, after secondary antibody incubation, brains were washed for at least 4 times and mounted in Vectashield and stored at 4°C in darkness for confocal microscopy. Primary and secondary antibodies were incubated over a night, except for BRP Nc82 primary antibody, which requires at least two nights’ incubation for proper staining quality. The following primary antibodies were used: mouse anti-BRP Nc82 (1:50) and chicken anti-GFP (ab13970, 1:1,500). Goat anti-rabbit Cy5 and Alexa Fluor 647, goat anti-mouse Alexa 488, and goat anti-chicken Alexa 488 were diluted at 1:300 for secondary antibody incubation.

Confocal microscope image acquisition, processing, and analysis

Whole-mount brain samples were scanned with a Leica TCS SP8 confocal microscope under oil objectives. All the parameters of the microscope were set to avoid saturation and kept constant throughout each experiment for the purpose of comparing fluorescence intensity. All images (confocal and western blot images) were processed and analyzed in ImageJ (Fiji) software (https://fiji.sc/). For stacks of brain images, an average intensity projection was chosen and the intensity of the single projected image was analyzed by manually drawing a region of interest to determine the gray value within this ROI. The values for each replicate were normalized to wt or control, and different replicates were pooled after normalization.

Sleep and sleep deprivation

Sleep and sleep deprivation experiments were performed as previously published [33]. Briefly, locomotor walking activity and sleep were recorded by Drosophila Activity Monitors (DAM2) from Trikinetics (Waltham, MA) in 12/12-h light/dark cycles at 25°C with 65% humidity. Single flies at specific age were individually housed in Trikinetics glass tubes (5 mm inner diameter and 65 mm length), which had 5% sucrose and 2% agar in one side of the tube. The locomotor walking activity was uploaded every 1 min, and the data from the first day was excluded due to the entrainment to new environments. A period of immobility without locomotor walking activity lasting for at least 5 min was defined as sleep [24]. Sleep and activity were analyzed using the Sleep and Circadian Analysis MATLAB Program [90].

Sleep deprivation was performed exactly as previously described [33]. The DAM2 monitors were fixed onto a Vortexer Mounting Plate (Trikinetics) on an Analog Multi-Tube Vortexer controlled by a Trikinetics LC4 light controller and an acquisition software for sleep deprivation configuration. Pulses of vortex lasting for 1.2 s were applied randomly with interpulse intervals between 0 s and 40 s to fully sleep deprive flies during night from ZT12 to ZT24. As female flies show strong and consistent early aging-associated alterations in sleep patterns, females were used for all sleep experiments, except for S3A–S3K Fig, within which male or female flies were used.

Aversive olfactory memory

Associative aversive olfactory memory experiments were performed as previously described [8,33]. Two aversive odors, 3-octanol (Oct) (1:100 dilution) and 4-methylcyclohexanol (MCH) (1:100 dilution), were used as olfactory cues (odors were diluted in mineral oil) and 120 V AC current electrical shocks served as a behavioral reinforcer. Briefly, during training, about 100 flies were sequentially exposed to the first odor (conditioned stimulus, CS+, MCH or Oct) paired with electrical shocks (unconditioned stimulus, US) for 60 s followed by 60 s rest and then the second odor (CS−, Oct or MCH) without electrical shocks for 60 s. During testing, these flies were exposed simultaneously to both odors (CS+ and CS−) and had 60 s to choose between the two odors. A reciprocal experiment in which the other of the two odors was paired with electrical shocks was performed simultaneously. A memory index was calculated as the number of flies that chose the CS− odor minus the number of flies that chose the CS+ odor divided by the total number of flies. The final memory index was averaged from the two memory indices from the reciprocal experiments.

STM was tested immediately after training, while 1h MTM and 3h MTM were tested 1 h or 3 h after training. For 1h ARM, one of the two 1h MTM components, flies received cold shock on ice for 90 s, 30 min after training. 1h ARM was measured 30 min after cold shock, and ASM was calculated by subtracting ARM from MTM.

Odor avoidance experiments were carried out similarly. Basically, naive flies were exposed to only one of the two odors and were allowed to choose between this odor and the air. The performance index was calculated as the number of flies that chose the air minus the number of flies that chose the odor divided by the total number of flies.

Spermidine supplementation and Gaboxadol/THIP treatment

For acute THIP (Sigma-Aldrich, Cat# T101) treatment for memory tests, adult flies at certain ages were transferred onto normal fly food with specific concentrations of THIP at approximately ZT1 for either 48 h or 72 h. Flies were either tested immediately after treatment or flipped back to normal fly food without THIP and tested 24 h after THIP treatment. For acute THIP treatment for sleep experiments, specific concentrations of THIP were contained in 5% sucrose and 2% agar medium, and sleep was measured as described above. For acute THIP treatment for immunostaining, flies were treated with 0.1 mg ml−1 THIP for 48 h and dissected 24 h after treatment.

Spd (Sigma-Aldrich, Cat# S2626) treatment has been described previously [8,10]. Flies were raised and aged to specific days on 5 mM Spd containing normal fly food for sleep experiments with 5% sucrose and 2% agar medium.

In vivo patch-clamp

Five- and 20-day-old female flies (R23E10>mCD8-GFP) were used for in vivo patch-clamp electrophysiology recording of dFB neurons. Adult Drosophila preparation for in vivo electrophysiology recording was performed based on a former study [91]. Flies were anesthetized by keeping them on ice for 1 to 2 min, and single flies were then fixed to the chamber with paraffin wax and dissected in external solution (103 mM NaCl, 3 mM KCl, 1.5 mM CaCl2, 4 mM MgCl2, 1 mM NaH2PO4, 26 mM NaHCO3, 5 mM TES (N-tris[hydroxymethyl]methyl-2-aminoethane sulfonic acid), 8 mM Trehalose, 10 mM Glucose, and 7 mM Sucrose, Osm = 280 ± 3) preequilibrated with carbogen (95% O2/5% CO2). The head cuticle on the posterior surface was peeled off to reveal the dFB neurons somata, and the glial sheath around the targeted area was focally removed with sharpened forceps. GFP-labeled dFB neurons were visualized under a 40 × objective. Signals were captured with a CCD digital camera (HAMAMATSU, ORCA-ER) mounted on the microscope.

Recording from dFB neurons was performed as described previously [53]. During recording, the preparation was superfused continuously with the external solution. Somata of dFB neurons were targeted with pipettes (8 to 10 MΩ) filled with internal solution (140 mM K-aspartate, 1 mM KCl, 10 mM HEPES, 1 mM EGTA, 4 mM MgATP, and 0.5 mM Na3GTP, pH = 7.3 and Osm = 265). Recordings were acquired with a MultiClamp 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices) and sampled with a Digidata 1440A interface (Molecular Devices). Signals were filtered at 6 kHz and digitized at 10 to 20 kHz. Data were analyzed using Clampfit 10.7 (Molecular Devices).

To measure the input resistance, small steps of 1-s hyperpolarizing current pulses were injected into the patched cell. To estimate the excitability of dFB neurons, cells were pre-hold at −60 mV followed by injection of a series of current steps (from −10 to 100 pA, 5 pA increment per step). Liquid junction potential of 13 mV was corrected offline. Spikes were searched with the event detection function in Clampfit. The following criteria were used for bursts identification: interspike interval <80 ms, number of spikes >4. For comparisons of interspike interval distribution, Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests were performed. Cells with an access resistance higher than 50 MΩ were excluded from the analysis.

Statistics

GraphPad Prism 7 was used to create the figures and perform most statistics. The Student t test was used for comparison between two groups, and one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni multiple comparisons test was used for multiple comparisons between multiple groups (≥3). Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA was used for sleep rebound experiments. Longevity data was analyzed by Gehan–Breslow–Wilcoxon test in order to put more weight on earlier time points to compare differences between survival curves. Asterisks or ns (not significant) above a group indicate the comparison of this group to 2xBRP wt or controls; asterisks or ns above a line denote the comparison between the two specific groups for most statistical analyses, unless stated otherwise.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Synaptic plasticity with aging.

(A-E) Representative western blots (A) and statistics of a spectrum of synaptic proteins, including BRP Nc82 (B), Dlg1 (C), Syn (D), and Syx (E) in wt female flies with aging. n = 3. One-way ANOVA with Bonferroni multiple comparisons test is shown. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001; ns, not significant. Error bars: mean ± SEM. Underlying data can be found in S1 Data Sheet. Raw images of this figure are provided in S1 Raw Images. Dlg1, Discs large; Syn, Synapsin; Syx, Syntaxin; wt, wild type.

(TIF)

S2 Fig. PreScale plasticity provokes activity reprogramming in R5 and dFB neurons.

(A) CaLexA signal is likely the representation of activity history of neurons. (B and C) Confocal images (B) and whole-mount brain staining analysis (C) of CaLexA signal intensity with CaLexA expressed in R5 neurons by R58H05-Gal4 in 4xBRP compared to 2xBRP flies. n = 20 for all groups. (D and E) Confocal images (D) and whole-mount brain staining analysis (E) of CaLexA signal intensity with CaLexA expressed in R5 neurons by R58H05-Gal4 in 1xBRP compared to 2xBRP flies. n = 12 for all groups. (F) Scheme of an interconnected sleep circuit in the central complex composed of the dFB (marked by R23E10-Gal4), Helicon cells, and the ellipsoid body R5 neurons (R5 marked by R58H05-Gal4). R5 neurons also receive circadian inputs. (G and H) Confocal images (G) and whole-mount brain staining analysis (H) of CaLexA signal intensity with CaLexA expressed in R5 neurons by R23E10-Gal4 in 1xBRP compared to 2xBRP flies. n = 10–12. (I and J) Confocal images (I) and whole-mount brain staining analysis (J) of CaLexA signal intensity with CaLexA expressed in R5 neurons by R23E10-Gal4 in 1xBRP compared to 2xBRP flies, and in sleep-deprived flies. Sleep deprivation was performed between ZT12 to ZT24 during nighttime. n = 12 for all groups. Student t test is shown. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001; ns, not significant. Scale bar: 20 μm. Error bars: mean ± SEM. (K) Confocal image of the expression pattern of R23E10-Gal4 indicated by GFP staining. Scale bar: 100 μm. (L) Widefield image of the cell bodies of R23E10-Gal4 indicated by live GFP signal, the cell body localization is indicated in the dashed circles of (J). Scale bar: 10 μm. Underlying data can be found in S1 Data Sheet. dFB, dorsal fan-shaped body; ZT, zeitgeber time.

(TIF)

S3 Fig. BRP promotes sleep in a dosage-dependent manner in male flies, similar to females.

(A-E) Sleep structure of 1xBRP-4xBRP male flies averaged from measurements over 2–4 days, including sleep profile plotted in 30-min bins (A), daily sleep amount (B), number and duration of sleep episodes (C and D), and sleep latencies (E). n = 63–80. One-way ANOVA with Bonferroni multiple comparisons test is shown. (F) Linear regression analysis of daily sleep amount in flies with different brp copies in both male (R2 = 0.81) and female (R2 = 0.95) flies. n = 63–80 per group for male, n = 123–128 per group for female. (G-K) Sleep structure of 5d female and male wt flies from measurements over 2–4 days, including sleep profile plotted in 30-min bins (G), daily sleep amount (H), number and duration of sleep episodes (I and J), and sleep latencies (K). n = 62–64 per group. (L) Genomic mapping and sequence of the integration site of the brp P[acman] transgenic construct. Red letters indicate brp P[acman] sequence, and black letters indicate genomic CG11357 gene sequence. (M) Simplified gene span of CG11357 and the integration site of the brp P[acman] and another P-element mediated allele EY12484 that are both localized at the 5′ UTR region of CG11357. (N-R) Sleep structure of EY12484 female flies averaged from measurements over 2–4 days, including sleep profile plotted in 30-min bins (N), daily sleep amount (O), number and duration of sleep episodes (P and Q), and sleep latencies (R). n = 55 for wt control and n = 32 for EY12484. Student t test is shown. (S and T) An independent experiment of the lifespan analysis of 2xBRP and 4xBRP flies. For male flies (S), n = 134 for 4xBRP compared to 2xBRP (n = 132, p < 0.001). For female flies (T), n = 134 for 4xBRP compared to 2xBRP wt control flies (n = 141, p < 0.001). (U and V) Lifespan analysis of 2xBRP wt and EY12484 flies. For male flies (U), n = 128 for EY12484 compared to 2xBRP (n = 127, ns). For female flies (V), n = 129 for EY12484 compared to 2xBRP wt control flies (n = 127, ns). Gehan–Breslow–Wilcoxon test is shown for all longevity experiments. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001; ns, not significant. Error bars: mean ± SEM. Underlying data can be found in S1 Data Sheet. BRP, Bruchpilot; CDS, coding DNA sequence; wt, wild type.

(TIF)

S4 Fig. Early aging-associated sleep pattern changes of 3xBRP animals.

(A-D) Sleep structure of 1xBRP female flies at age 20d rescued by a transgenic brp copy (gBRP) and averaged from measurements over 2–3 days, including daily sleep amount (A), number and duration of sleep episodes (B and C), and sleep latencies (D). n = 63–64 for all groups. One-way ANOVA with Bonferroni multiple comparisons test is shown. (E and F) Locomotor walking activity distribution across the day (E) and averaged daily total walking activity (F) of 3xBRP compared to 2xBRP female flies at ages 5d, 20d, 30d, and 40d. (G-O) Sleep structure of 3xBRP female flies at ages 5d, 20d, 30d, and 40d averaged from measurements over 2–3 days, including sleep profile plotted in 30-min bins (G), daytime and nighttime sleep amount (H and L), number and duration of sleep episodes (I, J, M, and N), and sleep latencies (K and O). n = 246–247 for 5d, n = 61–63 for 20d, n = 59–62 for 30d, and n = 32 for 40d. Two-way ANOVA with Sidak multiple comparisons is shown. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001; ns, not significant. Error bars: mean ± SEM. Underlying data can be found in S1 Data Sheet.

(TIF)

S5 Fig. Spd supplementation does not affect sleep rebound of 5d young animals.

(A and B) Rationale (A) and protocol (B) for the consequence of Spd supplementation in age-associated alterations of sleep pattern. Early aging trigger PreScale, memory decline, and sleep pattern changes, which might functionally intersect for survival. Spd supplementation was shown to suppress PreScale and memory decline, but its effect on early aging-associated sleep pattern changes was unclear. (C) Sleep profile for 5d wt female flies treated with 5 mM Spd compared to untreated for 3 consecutive days. (D) Normalized cumulative sleep loss during 12-h nighttime sleep deprivation and 24-h sleep rebound. Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA with Fisher LSD test did not detect any significant treatment × time interaction (F(47, 2784) = 0.0038; p > 0.9999) during sleep rebound. (E) Sleep recovered at three different time points after sleep deprivation for 5d 5 mM Spd-treated compared to untreated female flies. n = 29–31 for both groups. Two-way ANOVA with Sidak multiple comparisons is shown. ns, not significant. Error bars: mean ± SEM. Underlying data can be found in S1 Data Sheet. LSD, least significant difference; Spd, spermidine; wt, wild type.

(TIF)

S6 Fig. Acute deep sleep induced by Gaboxadol/THIP feeding.

(A) Protocol for sleep test of wt female flies treated with different concentrations of THIP at either age 3d or 30d. (B-F) Sleep structure of 5d wt female flies fed with 0.05 mg ml−1 and 0.1 mg ml−1 THIP from measurements over 2–3 days, including sleep profile plotted in 30-min bins (B), daily sleep amount (C), number and duration of sleep episodes (D and E), and sleep latencies (F). n = 64 for untreated control wt flies, n = 32 for both 0.05 mg ml−1 and 0.1 mg ml−1 THIP-treated groups. One-way ANOVA with Bonferroni multiple comparisons test is shown. (G-K) Sleep structure of 30d wt female flies fed with 0.05 mg ml−1 THIP from measurements over 2–3 days, including sleep profile plotted in 30-min bins (G), daily sleep amount (H), number and duration of sleep episodes (I and J), and sleep latencies (K). n = 31 for both groups. Student t test is shown. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001; ns, not significant. Error bars: mean ± SEM. Underlying data can be found in S1 Data Sheet.

(TIF)

S7 Fig. Acute Gaboxadol/THIP treatment for 2 days does not alter sleep behavior 1 day after the treatment in young animals.

(A) Protocol for sleep test of wt female flies that have been treated with 0.1 mg ml−1 THIP for 2 days at age 2d. (B and C) Locomotor walking activity pattern (B) and statistic (C) of 30d wt female flies after 2 days of 0.1 mg ml−1 THIP treatment. (D-H) Sleep structure of 2d wt female flies after 2 days of 0.1 mg ml−1 THIP treatment averaged from measurements over 2 days, including sleep profile plotted in 30-min bins (D), daytime and nighttime sleep amount (E), number and duration of sleep episodes (F and G), and sleep latencies (H). n = 32 for all groups. Student t test is shown. ns, not significant. Error bars: mean ± SEM. Underlying data can be found in S1 Data Sheet.

(TIF)

S1 Data. Data underlying Fig 1.

(XLSX)

S2 Data. Data underlying Fig 2.

(XLSX)

S3 Data. Data underlying Fig 3.

(XLSX)

S4 Data. Data underlying Fig 4.

(XLSX)

S5 Data. Data underlying Fig 5.

(XLSX)

S6 Data. Data underlying Fig 6.

(XLSX)

S7 Data. Data underlying Fig 7.

(XLSX)

S1 Data Sheet. Data underlying S1S7 Figs.

(XLSX)

S1 Raw Images. Original uncropped immunoblots for Figs 1 and S1.

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

We thank S. Yildirim-Brochno, D. Wachmann, and Y. Ni for technical support; the Bloomington Stock Center (BDSC) for fly lines; and the Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank (DSHB) for antibodies.

Abbreviations

AL

antennal lobe

ARM

anesthesia-resistant memory

ASM

anesthesia-sensitive memory

BRP

Bruchpilot

dFB

dorsal fan-shaped body

Dlg1

Discs large

MB

mushroom body

MTM

middle-term memory

Spd

spermidine

STM

short-term memory

Syn

Synapsin

Syx

Syntaxin

THIP

4,5,6,7-Tetrahydroisoxazolo[5,4-c]pyridin-3-ol

wt

wild type

ZT

zeitgeber time

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by grants from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG; German Research Foundation) to S.J.S. (SFB1315 TP A08, NeuroNex2, CoE NeuroCure and FOR2705 TP05). S.H. was supported by the Chinese Scholarship Council (201504910753) as well as by the Leibniz Association (SAW-2019-ISAS-4-SyMetAge). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Partridge L, Deelen J, Slagboom PE. Facing up to the global challenges of ageing. Nature. 2018;561(7721):45–56. Epub 2018/09/07. doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0457-8 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Lopez-Otin C, Blasco MA, Partridge L, Serrano M, Kroemer G. The hallmarks of aging. Cell. 2013;153(6):1194–1217. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.05.039 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3836174 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Morrison JH, Baxter MG. The ageing cortical synapse: hallmarks and implications for cognitive decline. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13(4):240–50. Epub 2012/03/08. doi: 10.1038/nrn3200 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3592200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Bishop NA, Lu T, Yankner BA. Neural mechanisms of ageing and cognitive decline. Nature. 2010;464(7288):529–35. Epub 2010/03/26. doi: 10.1038/nature08983 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2927852. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Mander BA, Rao V, Lu B, Saletin JM, Lindquist JR, Ancoli-Israel S, et al. Prefrontal atrophy, disrupted NREM slow waves and impaired hippocampal-dependent memory in aging. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16(3):357–64. Epub 2013/01/29. doi: 10.1038/nn.3324 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4286370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Mander BA, Winer JR, Walker MP. Sleep and Human Aging. Neuron. 2017;94(1):19–36. Epub 2017/04/07. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.02.004 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5810920. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Wang M, Gamo NJ, Yang Y, Jin LE, Wang XJ, Laubach M, et al. Neuronal basis of age-related working memory decline. Nature. 2011;476(7359):210–3. Epub 2011/07/29. doi: 10.1038/nature10243 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3193794. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Gupta VK, Pech U, Bhukel A, Fulterer A, Ender A, Mauermann SF, et al. Spermidine Suppresses Age-Associated Memory Impairment by Preventing Adverse Increase of Presynaptic Active Zone Size and Release. PLoS Biol. 2016;14(9):e1002563. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002563 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Madeo F, Eisenberg T, Pietrocola F, Kroemer G. Spermidine in health and disease. Science. 2018;359(6374). Epub 2018/01/27. doi: 10.1126/science.aan2788 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Gupta VK, Scheunemann L, Eisenberg T, Mertel S, Bhukel A, Koemans TS, et al. Restoring polyamines protects from age-induced memory impairment in an autophagy-dependent manner. Nat Neurosci 2013;16(10):1453–1460. doi: 10.1038/nn.3512 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Ulgherait M, Midoun AM, Park SJ, Gatto JA, Tener SJ, Siewert J, et al. Circadian autophagy drives iTRF-mediated longevity. Nature. 2021;598(7880):353–8. Epub 2021/10/01. doi: 10.1038/s41586-021-03934-0 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Gill S, Le HD, Melkani GC, Panda S. Time-restricted feeding attenuates age-related cardiac decline in Drosophila. Science. 2015;347(6227):1265–9. Epub 2015/03/15. doi: 10.1126/science.1256682 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4578815. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kaeberlein M, Rabinovitch PS, Martin GM. Healthy aging: The ultimate preventative medicine. Science. 2015;350(6265):1191–3. Epub 2016/01/20. doi: 10.1126/science.aad3267 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4793924. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Eisenberg T, Knauer H, Schauer A, Buttner S, Ruckenstuhl C, Carmona-Gutierrez D, et al. Induction of autophagy by spermidine promotes longevity. Nat Cell Biol. 2009;11(11):1305–14. Epub 2009/10/06. doi: 10.1038/ncb1975 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Tain LS, Jain C, Nespital T, Froehlich J, Hinze Y, Gronke S, et al. Longevity in response to lowered insulin signaling requires glycine N-methyltransferase-dependent spermidine production. Aging Cell. 2020;19(1):e13043. Epub 2019/11/14. doi: 10.1111/acel.13043 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6974722. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.McEwen BS. In pursuit of resilience: stress, epigenetics, and brain plasticity. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2016;1373(1):56–64. Epub 2016/02/27. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13020 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Aron L, Zullo J, Yankner BA. The adaptive aging brain. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2022;72:91–100. Epub 2021/10/25. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2021.09.009 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC8901453. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Donlea JM, Ramanan N, Silverman N, Shaw PJ. Genetic rescue of functional senescence in synaptic and behavioral plasticity. Sleep. 2014;37(9):1427–37. Epub 2014/08/22. doi: 10.5665/sleep.3988 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4153059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Dissel S. Drosophila as a Model to Study the Relationship Between Sleep, Plasticity, and Memory. Front Physiol. 2020;11:533. Epub 2020/06/18. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2020.00533 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC7270326. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Donlea JM, Thimgan MS, Suzuki Y, Gottschalk L, Shaw PJ. Inducing sleep by remote control facilitates memory consolidation in Drosophila. Science. 2011;332(6037):1571–1576. doi: 10.1126/science.1202249 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4064462 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Keene AC, Waddell S. Drosophila olfactory memory: single genes to complex neural circuits. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8(5):341–54. Epub 2007/04/25. doi: 10.1038/nrn2098 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Margulies C, Tully T, Dubnau J. Deconstructing memory in Drosophila. Curr Biol. 2005;15(17):R700–13. Epub 2005/09/06. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.08.024 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3044934. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Quinn WG, Dudai Y. Memory phases in Drosophila. Nature. 1976;262(5569):576–7. Epub 1976/08/12. doi: 10.1038/262576a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Shaw PJ, Cirelli C, Greenspan RJ, Tononi G. Correlates of sleep and waking in Drosophila melanogaster. Science. 2000;287(5459):1834–1837. doi: 10.1126/science.287.5459.1834 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Hendricks JC, Finn SM, Panckeri KA, Chavkin J, Williams JA, Sehgal A, et al. Rest in Drosophila is a sleep-like state. Neuron. 2000;25(1):129–38. Epub 2000/03/09. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80877-6 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Ly S, Pack AI, Naidoo N. The neurobiological basis of sleep: Insights from Drosophila. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;87:67–86. Epub 2018/02/03. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.01.015 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5845852. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Wiggin TD, Goodwin PR, Donelson NC, Liu C, Trinh K, Sanyal S, et al. Covert sleep-related biological processes are revealed by probabilistic analysis in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2020;117(18):10024–34. Epub 2020/04/19. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1917573117 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC7211995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Curran JA, Buhl E, Tsaneva-Atanasova K, Hodge JJL. Age-dependent changes in clock neuron structural plasticity and excitability are associated with a decrease in circadian output behavior and sleep. Neurobiol Aging. 2019;77:158–68. Epub 2019/03/03. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.01.025 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6491500. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Vienne J, Spann R, Guo F, Rosbash M. Age-Related Reduction of Recovery Sleep and Arousal Threshold in Drosophila. Sleep. 2016;39(8):1613–1624. doi: 10.5665/sleep.6032 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4945321 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Ly S, Naidoo N. Loss of DmGluRA exacerbates age-related sleep disruption and reduces lifespan. Neurobiol Aging. 2019;80:83–90. Epub 2019/05/20. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.04.004 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6679765. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Koh K, Evans JM, Hendricks JC, Sehgal A. A Drosophila model for age-associated changes in sleep:wake cycles. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103(37):13843–7. Epub 2006/08/30. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0605903103 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC1564207. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Yamazaki D, Horiuchi J, Nakagami Y, Nagano S, Tamura T, Saitoe M. The Drosophila DCO mutation suppresses age-related memory impairment without affecting lifespan. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10(4):478–484. doi: 10.1038/nn1863 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Huang S, Piao C, Beuschel CB, Gotz T, Sigrist SJ. Presynaptic Active Zone Plasticity Encodes Sleep Need in Drosophila. Curr Biol. 2020. Epub 2020/03/07. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.01.019 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Böhme MA, Beis C, Reddy-Alla S, Reynolds E, Mampell MM, Grasskamp AT, et al. Active zone scaffolds differentially accumulate Unc13 isoforms to tune Ca(2+) channel-vesicle coupling. Nat Neurosci. 2016;19(10):1311–20. Epub 2016/08/16. doi: 10.1038/nn.4364 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Cho YH, Kim GH, Park JJ. Mitochondrial aconitase 1 regulates age-related memory impairment via autophagy/mitophagy-mediated neural plasticity in middle-aged flies. Aging Cell. 2021;20(12):e13520. Epub 2021/11/21. doi: 10.1111/acel.13520 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC8672789. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Sudhof TC. The presynaptic active zone. Neuron. 2012;75(1):11–25. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.012 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3743085 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Kittel RJ, Wichmann C, Rasse TM, Fouquet W, Schmidt M, Schmid A, et al. Bruchpilot promotes active zone assembly, Ca2+ channel clustering, and vesicle release. Science. 2006;312(5776):1051–1054. doi: 10.1126/science.1126308 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Piao C, Sigrist SJ. (M)Unc13s in Active Zone Diversity: A Drosophila Perspective. Frontiers in Synaptic. Neuroscience. 2022:13. doi: 10.3389/fnsyn.2021.798204 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Wagh DA, Rasse TM, Asan E, Hofbauer A, Schwenkert I, Durrbeck H, et al. Bruchpilot, a protein with homology to ELKS/CAST, is required for structural integrity and function of synaptic active zones in Drosophila. Neuron. 2006;49(6):833–844. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.02.008 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Fouquet W, Owald D, Wichmann C, Mertel S, Depner H, Dyba M, et al. Maturation of active zone assembly by Drosophila Bruchpilot. J Cell Biol. 2009;186(1):129–145. doi: 10.1083/jcb.200812150 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2712991 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Huang S, Sigrist SJ. Presynaptic and postsynaptic long-term plasticity in sleep homeostasis. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2020;69:1–10. Epub 2020/12/18. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2020.11.010 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Gilestro GF, Tononi G, Cirelli C. Widespread changes in synaptic markers as a function of sleep and wakefulness in Drosophila. Science. 2009;324(5923):109–112. doi: 10.1126/science.1166673 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2715914 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Klagges BR, Heimbeck G, Godenschwege TA, Hofbauer A, Pflugfelder GO, Reifegerste R, et al. Invertebrate synapsins: a single gene codes for several isoforms in Drosophila. J Neurosci. 1996;16(10):3154–65. Epub 1996/05/15. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-10-03154.1996 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Schulze KL, Broadie K, Perin MS, Bellen HJ. Genetic and electrophysiological studies of Drosophila syntaxin-1A demonstrate its role in nonneuronal secretion and neurotransmission. Cell. 1995;80(2):311–20. Epub 1995/01/27. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(95)90414-x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Woods DF, Bryant PJ. The discs-large tumor suppressor gene of Drosophila encodes a guanylate kinase homolog localized at septate junctions. Cell. 1991;66(3):451–64. Epub 1991/08/09. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(81)90009-x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Simonsen A, Cumming RC, Brech A, Isakson P, Schubert DR, Finley KD. Promoting basal levels of autophagy in the nervous system enhances longevity and oxidant resistance in adult Drosophila. Autophagy. 2008;4(2):176–84. Epub 2007/12/07. doi: 10.4161/auto.5269 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Lin DM, Goodman CS. Ectopic and increased expression of Fasciclin II alters motoneuron growth cone guidance. Neuron. 1994;13(3):507–23. Epub 1994/09/01. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(94)90022-1 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Masuyama K, Zhang Y, Rao Y, Wang JW. Mapping neural circuits with activity-dependent nuclear import of a transcription factor. J Neurogenet. 2012;26(1):89–102. Epub 2012/01/13. doi: 10.3109/01677063.2011.642910 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3357894. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Davis RL. Traces of Drosophila memory. Neuron. 2011;70(1):8–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.012 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3374581 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Raccuglia D, Huang S, Ender A, Heim MM, Laber D, Suarez-Grimalt R, et al. Network-Specific Synchronization of Electrical Slow-Wave Oscillations Regulates Sleep Drive in Drosophila. Curr Biol. 2019. Epub 2019/10/22. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.08.070 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Liu S, Liu Q, Tabuchi M, Wu MN. Sleep Drive Is Encoded by Neural Plastic Changes in a Dedicated Circuit. Cell. 2016;165(6):1347–1360. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2016.04.013 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4892967 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Donlea JM, Pimentel D, Talbot CB, Kempf A, Omoto JJ, Hartenstein V, et al. Recurrent Circuitry for Balancing Sleep Need and Sleep. Neuron. 2018;97(2):378–89 e4. Epub 2018/01/09. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.12.016 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5779612. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Pimentel D, Donlea JM, Talbot CB, Song SM, Thurston AJ, Miesenbock G. Operation of a homeostatic sleep switch. Nature. 2016;536(7616):333–337. doi: 10.1038/nature19055 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4998959 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Tabuchi M, Monaco JD, Duan G, Bell B, Liu S, Liu Q, et al. Clock-Generated Temporal Codes Determine Synaptic Plasticity to Control Sleep. Cell. 2018;175(5):1213–27 e18. Epub 2018/10/16. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2018.09.016 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6239908. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Weiss JT, Donlea JM. Sleep deprivation results in diverse patterns of synaptic scaling across the Drosophila mushroom bodies. Curr Biol. 2021;31(15):3248–61 e3. Epub 2021/06/10. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2021.05.018 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC8355077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Liu S, Lamaze A, Liu Q, Tabuchi M, Yang Y, Fowler M, et al. WIDE AWAKE mediates the circadian timing of sleep onset. Neuron. 2014;82(1):151–166. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.01.040 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3982794 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Stavropoulos N, Young MW. insomniac and Cullin-3 regulate sleep and wakefulness in Drosophila. Neuron. 2011;72(6):964–76. Epub 2011/12/27. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.12.003 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3244879. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Pfeiffenberger C, Allada R. Cul3 and the BTB adaptor insomniac are key regulators of sleep homeostasis and a dopamine arousal pathway in Drosophila. PLoS Genet. 2012;8(10):e1003003. Epub 2012/10/12. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1003003 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3464197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Saitoe M, Horiuchi J, Tamura T, Ito N. Drosophila as a novel animal model for studying the genetics of age-related memory impairment. Rev Neurosci. 2005;16(2):137–49. Epub 2005/06/17. doi: 10.1515/revneuro.2005.16.2.137 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Buckner RL. Memory and executive function in aging and AD: multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate. Neuron. 2004;44(1):195–208. Epub 2004/09/29. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.006 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Placais PY, Preat T. To favor survival under food shortage, the brain disables costly memory. Science. 2013;339(6118):440–2. Epub 2013/01/26. doi: 10.1126/science.1226018 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Placais PY, de Tredern E, Scheunemann L, Trannoy S, Goguel V, Han KA, et al. Upregulated energy metabolism in the Drosophila mushroom body is the trigger for long-term memory. Nat Commun. 2017;8:15510. Epub 2017/06/06. doi: 10.1038/ncomms15510 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5465319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Marck A, Berthelot G, Foulonneau V, Marc A, Antero-Jacquemin J, Noirez P, et al. Age-Related Changes in Locomotor Performance Reveal a Similar Pattern for Caenorhabditis elegans, Mus domesticus, Canis familiaris, Equus caballus, and Homo sapiens. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2017;72(4):455–63. Epub 2016/08/16. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glw136 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5861937. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Carey JR, Papadopoulos N, Kouloussis N, Katsoyannos B, Muller HG, Wang JL, et al. Age-specific and lifetime behavior patterns in Drosophila melanogaster and the Mediterranean fruit fly, Ceratitis capitata. Exp Gerontol. 2006;41(1):93–7. Epub 2005/11/22. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2005.09.014 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2387245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Metaxakis A, Tain LS, Gronke S, Hendrich O, Hinze Y, Birras U, et al. Lowered insulin signalling ameliorates age-related sleep fragmentation in Drosophila. PLoS Biol. 2014;12(4):e1001824. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001824 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3972082 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Kayser MS, Yue Z, Sehgal A. A critical period of sleep for development of courtship circuitry and behavior in Drosophila. Science. 2014;344(6181):269–274. doi: 10.1126/science.1250553 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4479292 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Seugnet L, Suzuki Y, Donlea JM, Gottschalk L, Shaw PJ. Sleep deprivation during early-adult development results in long-lasting learning deficits in adult Drosophila. Sleep. 2011;34(2):137–46. Epub 2011/02/03. doi: 10.1093/sleep/34.2.137 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3022932. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Schroeder S, Hofer SJ, Zimmermann A, Pechlaner R, Dammbrueck C, Pendl T, et al. Dietary spermidine improves cognitive function. Cell Rep. 2021;35(2):108985. Epub 2021/04/15. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2021.108985 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Liang Y, Piao C, Beuschel CB, Toppe D, Kollipara L, Bogdanow B, et al. eIF5A hypusination, boosted by dietary spermidine, protects from premature brain aging and mitochondrial dysfunction. Cell Rep. 2021;35(2):108941. Epub 2021/04/15. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2021.108941 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Dissel S, Angadi V, Kirszenblat L, Suzuki Y, Donlea J, Klose M, et al. Sleep restores behavioral plasticity to Drosophila mutants. Curr Biol. 2015;25(10):1270–1281. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.03.027 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4465363 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Dissel S, Klose M, Donlea J, Cao L, English D, Winsky-Sommerer R, et al. Enhanced sleep reverses memory deficits and underlying pathology in Drosophila models of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Sleep Circadian Rhythms. 2017;2:15–26. Epub 2017/11/03. doi: 10.1016/j.nbscr.2016.09.001 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5662006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.McEwen BS, Bowles NP, Gray JD, Hill MN, Hunter RG, Karatsoreos IN, et al. Mechanisms of stress in the brain. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18(10):1353–63. Epub 2015/09/26. doi: 10.1038/nn.4086 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4933289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Parrino L, Vaudano AE. The resilient brain and the guardians of sleep: New perspectives on old assumptions. Sleep Med Rev. 2018;39:98–107. Epub 2017/10/22. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2017.08.003 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Owen JE, Veasey SC. Impact of sleep disturbances on neurodegeneration: Insight from studies in animal models. Neurobiol Dis. 2020;139:104820. Epub 2020/02/23. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2020.104820 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC7593848. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Pandi-Perumal SR, Seils LK, Kayumov L, Ralph MR, Lowe A, Moller H, et al. Senescence, sleep, and circadian rhythms. Ageing Res Rev. 2002;1(3):559–604. Epub 2002/06/18. doi: 10.1016/s1568-1637(02)00014-4 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Brown MK, Chan MT, Zimmerman JE, Pack AI, Jackson NE, Naidoo N. Aging induced endoplasmic reticulum stress alters sleep and sleep homeostasis. Neurobiol Aging. 2014;35(6):1431–41. Epub 2014/01/22. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.12.005 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4019391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Juricic P, Lu Y-X, Leech T, Drews LF, Paulitz J, Lu J, et al. Long-lasting geroprotection from brief rapamycin treatment in early adulthood by persistently increased intestinal autophagy. Nat Aging. 2022;2(9):824–836. doi: 10.1038/s43587-022-00278-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Burger JM, Kolss M, Pont J, Kawecki TJ. Learning ability and longevity: a symmetrical evolutionary trade-off in Drosophila. Evolution. 2008;62(6):1294–304. Epub 2008/03/28. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00376.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Zrelec V, Zini M, Guarino S, Mermoud J, Oppliger J, Valtat A, et al. Drosophila rely on learning while foraging under semi-natural conditions. Ecol Evol. 2013;3(12):4139–48. Epub 2013/12/11. doi: 10.1002/ece3.783 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3853559. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Yap MHW, Grabowska MJ, Rohrscheib C, Jeans R, Troup M, Paulk AC, et al. Oscillatory brain activity in spontaneous and induced sleep stages in flies. Nat Commun. 2017;8(1):1815. Epub 2017/11/29. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-02024-y ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5704022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Tainton-Heap LAL, Kirszenblat LC, Notaras ET, Grabowska MJ, Jeans R, Feng K, et al. A Paradoxical Kind of Sleep in Drosophila melanogaster. Curr Biol. 2021;31(3):578–90 e6. Epub 2020/11/26. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.10.081 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC8993047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Kempf A, Song SM, Talbot CB, Miesenbock G. A potassium channel beta-subunit couples mitochondrial electron transport to sleep. Nature. 2019;568(7751):230–4. Epub 2019/03/22. doi: 10.1038/s41586-019-1034-5 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Bedont JL, Toda H, Shi M, Park CH, Quake C, Stein C, et al. Short and long sleeping mutants reveal links between sleep and macroautophagy. Elife. 2021;10. Epub 2021/06/05. doi: 10.7554/eLife.64140 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC8177895. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Bhukel A, Beuschel CB, Maglione M, Lehmann M, Juhasz G, Madeo F, et al. Autophagy within the mushroom body protects from synapse aging in a non-cell autonomous manner. Nat Commun. 2019;10(1):1318. Epub 2019/03/23. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-09262-2 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6428838. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Lamaze A, Kratschmer P, Chen KF, Lowe S, Jepson JEC. A Wake-Promoting Circadian Output Circuit in Drosophila. Curr Biol. 2018;28(19):3098–105 e3. Epub 2018/10/03. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.07.024 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Guo F, Holla M, Diaz MM, Rosbash M. A Circadian Output Circuit Controls Sleep-Wake Arousal in Drosophila. Neuron. 2018;100(3):624–35 e4. Epub 2018/10/03. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.09.002 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Matkovic T, Siebert M, Knoche E, Depner H, Mertel S, Owald D, et al. The Bruchpilot cytomatrix determines the size of the readily releasable pool of synaptic vesicles. J Cell Biol. 2013;202(4):667–683. doi: 10.1083/jcb.201301072 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3747298 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Potter CJ, Luo L. Splinkerette PCR for mapping transposable elements in Drosophila. PLoS ONE. 2010;5(4):e10168. Epub 2010/04/21. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010168 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2854151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Bartlett BJ, Isakson P, Lewerenz J, Sanchez H, Kotzebue RW, Cumming RC, et al. p62, Ref(2)P and ubiquitinated proteins are conserved markers of neuronal aging, aggregate formation and progressive autophagic defects. Autophagy. 2011;7(6):572–83. Epub 2011/02/18. doi: 10.4161/auto.7.6.14943 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3127048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Donelson NC, Kim EZ, Slawson JB, Vecsey CG, Huber R, Griffith LC. High-resolution positional tracking for long-term analysis of Drosophila sleep and locomotion using the "tracker" program. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(5):e37250. Epub 2012/05/23. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0037250 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3352887. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Murthy M, Turner G. Whole-cell in vivo patch-clamp recordings in the Drosophila brain. Cold Spring Harb Protoc. 2013;2013(2):140–8. Epub 2013/02/05. doi: 10.1101/pdb.prot071704 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Lucas Smith

29 Jun 2022

Dear Dr Sigrist,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript entitled "A brain-wide form of presynaptic active zone plasticity orchestrates resilience to brain aging in Drosophila" for consideration as a Research Article by PLOS Biology.

Your manuscript has now been evaluated by the PLOS Biology editorial staff as well as by an academic editor with relevant expertise, and I am writing to let you know that we would like to send your submission out for external peer review. While we are, in principle, interested in the study, I should note that we have yet to make a firm call about whether the findings reported here go far enough beyond other work on this topic, and so we will be looking for enthusiasm amongst the reviewers towards the overall advance of the study and its fit for PLOS Biology.

Before we can send your manuscript to reviewers, we need you to complete your submission by providing the metadata that is required for full assessment. To this end, please login to Editorial Manager where you will find the paper in the 'Submissions Needing Revisions' folder on your homepage. Please click 'Revise Submission' from the Action Links and complete all additional questions in the submission questionnaire.

Once your full submission is complete, your paper will undergo a series of checks in preparation for peer review. After your manuscript has passed the checks it will be sent out for review. To provide the metadata for your submission, please Login to Editorial Manager (https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology) within two working days, i.e. by Jul 01 2022 11:59PM.

If your manuscript has been previously peer-reviewed at another journal, PLOS Biology is willing to work with those reviews in order to avoid re-starting the process. Submission of the previous reviews is entirely optional and our ability to use them effectively will depend on the willingness of the previous journal to confirm the content of the reports and share the reviewer identities. Please note that we reserve the right to invite additional reviewers if we consider that additional/independent reviewers are needed, although we aim to avoid this as far as possible. In our experience, working with previous reviews does save time.

If you would like us to consider previous reviewer reports, please edit your cover letter to let us know and include the name of the journal where the work was previously considered and the manuscript ID it was given. In addition, please upload a response to the reviews as a 'Prior Peer Review' file type, which should include the reports in full and a point-by-point reply detailing how you have or plan to address the reviewers' concerns.

During the process of completing your manuscript submission, you will be invited to opt-in to posting your pre-review manuscript as a bioRxiv preprint. Visit http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/preprints for full details. If you consent to posting your current manuscript as a preprint, please upload a single Preprint PDF.

Feel free to email us at plosbiology@plos.org if you have any queries relating to your submission.

Kind regards,

Luke

Lucas Smith, Ph.D.

Associate Editor

PLOS Biology

lsmith@plos.org

Decision Letter 1

Lucas Smith

28 Jul 2022

Dear Dr Sigrist,

Thank you for your patience while your manuscript "A brain-wide form of presynaptic active zone plasticity orchestrates resilience to brain aging in Drosophila" was peer-reviewed at PLOS Biology. It has now been evaluated by the PLOS Biology editors, an Academic Editor with relevant expertise, and by several independent reviewers.

The reviews of your manuscript are appended below. As you will see, while the reviewers have commented that the study is potentially interesting, they have also raised a number of important and overlapping concerns which would need to be thoroughly addressed before we can consider your manuscript further for publication. The reviewers have highlighted that currently, the manuscript is disorganized and the writing is unclear, to the extent that it is difficult to judge the advance of your findings. Additionally, the reviewers have suggested additional data and analyses be provided to strengthen the study.

In light of these concerns, we do not feel able to accept your manuscript in its current form, but we would welcome resubmission of a much revised manuscript that addresses the reviewer comments. We would expect the revised manuscript to provide additional data, and to carefully re-write the manuscript to make it more accessible to a broad audience, clarify the model, and to better explain the advance of the study.

Given the extent of revision needed, we cannot make a decision about publication until we have seen the revised manuscript and your response to the reviewers' comments. Your revised manuscript is likely to be sent for further evaluation by all or a subset of the reviewers. I should note that it is possible that, after rewriting the manuscript to improve its clarity, the reviewers may identify new concerns that were not apparent in the current version and we will need to take those into account.

We expect to receive your revised manuscript within 3 months. Please email us (plosbiology@plos.org) if you have any questions or concerns, or would like to request an extension.

At this stage, your manuscript remains formally under active consideration at our journal; please notify us by email if you do not intend to submit a revision so that we may withdraw it.

**IMPORTANT - SUBMITTING YOUR REVISION**

Your revisions should address the specific points made by each reviewer. Please submit the following files along with your revised manuscript:

1. A 'Response to Reviewers' file - this should detail your responses to the editorial requests, present a point-by-point response to all of the reviewers' comments, and indicate the changes made to the manuscript.

*NOTE: In your point-by-point response to the reviewers, please provide the full context of each review. Do not selectively quote paragraphs or sentences to reply to. The entire set of reviewer comments should be present in full and each specific point should be responded to individually, point by point.

You should also cite any additional relevant literature that has been published since the original submission and mention any additional citations in your response.

2. In addition to a clean copy of the manuscript, please also upload a 'track-changes' version of your manuscript that specifies the edits made. This should be uploaded as a "Revised Article with Changes Highlighted" file type.

*Re-submission Checklist*

When you are ready to resubmit your revised manuscript, please refer to this re-submission checklist: https://plos.io/Biology_Checklist

To submit a revised version of your manuscript, please go to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/ and log in as an Author. Click the link labelled 'Submissions Needing Revision' where you will find your submission record.

Please make sure to read the following important policies and guidelines while preparing your revision:

*Published Peer Review*

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out. Please see here for more details:

https://blogs.plos.org/plos/2019/05/plos-journals-now-open-for-published-peer-review/

*PLOS Data Policy*

Please note that as a condition of publication PLOS' data policy (http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/data-availability) requires that you make available all data used to draw the conclusions arrived at in your manuscript. If you have not already done so, you must include any data used in your manuscript either in appropriate repositories, within the body of the manuscript, or as supporting information (N.B. this includes any numerical values that were used to generate graphs, histograms etc.). For an example see here: http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pbio.1001908#s5

*Blot and Gel Data Policy*

We require the original, uncropped and minimally adjusted images supporting all blot and gel results reported in an article's figures or Supporting Information files. We will require these files before a manuscript can be accepted so please prepare them now, if you have not already uploaded them. Please carefully read our guidelines for how to prepare and upload this data: https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/figures#loc-blot-and-gel-reporting-requirements

*Protocols deposition*

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols

Thank you again for your submission to our journal. We hope that our editorial process has been constructive thus far, and we welcome your feedback at any time. Please don't hesitate to contact us if you have any questions or comments.

Sincerely,

Lucas

Lucas Smith, Ph.D.

Associate Editor

PLOS Biology

lsmith@plos.org

------------------------------------

REVIEWS:

Reviewer #1: Huang et al. characterize a presynaptic phenomenon called PreScale and propose that age-dependent induction of PreScale may be responsible for regulating lifespan and age-related changes in sleep and memory. Overall, their manuscript is somewhat disorganized, the presentation of their data seems haphazard, and the description of their model is difficult to understand. Unfortunately, the amount of progress in this work compared to previously published work from this group is unclear.

Specific comments:

1) The Introduction is short and vague. The first two paragraphs, which make up the bulk of the Introduction, are general background, and the important third paragraph, which introduces PreScale and spermidine, is very brief, making it difficult to understand what PreScale is and why it is important. Because the Introduction is short, much introductory material is inserted into the Results section making the logic of the manuscript more difficult to follow. Without first reading Gupta et al., 2016 and Huang et al., 2020, many readers may have difficulty understanding the rationale behind most of the experiments in the current manuscript. In fact, the authors reference a supplementary figure in Huang et al., 2020, requiring readers to look up this figure. A better introduction to these papers may improve the organization and apparent significance of this manuscript.

2) The model presented by the authors is vague, and the interactions between sleep, lifespan, memory and PreScale are unclear. For example, in the graphic abstract and in Figure S7, both spermidine and gaboxadol decrease PreScale, improve memory and extend lifespan, but they have opposite effects on sleep. Do the authors think that PreScale and sleep are unrelated? This seems unlikely since this group published Huang et al., 2020. The partial models presented in Figures S5A and 7A are difficult to understand. They seem to suggest that if PreScale is inhibited, sleep is reduced. On the other hand, if sleep is increased, PreScale is inhibited. These seem to be opposing effects that the authors could discuss more clearly.

3) Many of the results in this manuscript are correlative, and causative relationships are unclear. For example, both spermidine and gaboxadol inhibit PreScale. However, they have opposing effects on sleep, suggesting that at least for one of these interventions, effects on sleep are not mediated by PreScale. Since sleep is known to be linked to memory and lifespan, it is unclear whether the effects of eg. gaboxadol on lifespan and memory are caused by changes in PreScale or by changes in sleep. Epistatic experiments addressing causation were not performed.

4) Did the authors examine the effects of age-dependent induction of bruchpilot RNAi on sleep, lifespan, and memory? Does this increase age-dependent lifespan and memory and maintain young sleep amounts?

5) Did the authors examine the effects of 1xBRP on dFB and R5? Are these effects the opposite of 3xBRP?

6) How spontaneous activity and membrane excitability of dFB neurons are affected by spermidine and gaboxadol?

Reviewer #2: Huang et al. examined age-associated changes in synaptic protein levels and neuronal excitability, as well as sleep, memory, and longevity of flies with genetically manipulated BRP levels or those treated with spermidine or THIP. The results are potentially interesting, but I have several major concerns regarding the conceptual framework, data interpretation, and experimental design. A primary concern is that they do not explore sex differences in their data and do not examine reproductive fitness as a critical factor in a fly's life. Moreover, the authors conclude that "a brain-wide form of presynaptic active zone plasticity ("PreScale") promotes resilience by coupling sleep, longevity and memory during aging (abstract)." However, while the manuscript provides correlative data, it is unclear whether they point to a causal role of PreScale in the coordinated age-associated changes. Specific concerns are listed below.

1. In what sense is the tradeoff between longevity and memory during early aging optimal? What is optimized? It seems that simply living longer wouldn't be the core purpose of a fly. One could argue that producing lots of healthy progeny would be the fly's core mission. The authors should examine reproductive fitness (e.g., # of offspring) as a critical aspect of the necessary tradeoffs as flies age.

2. The data show substantial sex differences (e.g., Fig. 3), but there is no serious discussion of the subject. As mentioned in Point #1, the authors should examine how reproductive success is impacted by various genetic and drug manipulations.

3. The authors propose that PreScale promotes resilience during early aging by favoring longevity over memory. The finding that 3xBRP promotes longevity at the expense of memory seems consistent with the view. However, Spd and THIP promote both memory and longevity while suppressing PreScale. If Prescale has adaptive advantages, why do spermidine and THIP, which suppress Prescale, improve both memory and longevity? The authors suggest that Spd and THIP treatment promote efficient mitochondrial electron transport and autophagy, making PreScale unnecessary. How can we tell a priori whether a particular PreScale manipulation will enhance both memory and longevity or favor one over the other? What is meant by "a specific, context-dependent role of PreScale in regulating lifespan (Line 182)." Please elaborate on the specific contexts. If wake and inc mutants already have increased BRP, why does it help to have even more?

4. The increased longevity due to PreScale and THIP treatment could be because they sleep more and therefore spend less energy. The disadvantage may be that they do not have the opportunity to produce many offspring. Again, if the authors consider reproductive fitness, the increased longevity accompanied by increased sleep may not be a good tradeoff.

5. Fig. 2 shows striking physiological effects of increasing BRP levels to 4x. However, sleep and longevity data suggest that whereas 3xBRP provides some beneficial effects similar to PreScale, 4xBRP is detrimental. Given this finding, they should examine the electrophysiological properties for 3xBRP, not 4xBRP.

6. Why does PreScale have opposite effects on the neuronal excitability of R5 vs. dFB?

7. In the model (graphical abstract and Fig. S7), they show "longevity" as something affected by aging. Since longevity stays constant across the lifespan, mortality, which increases with aging, would be more helpful.

8. The effects of THIP on memory seem variable across experiments. For example, 0.1THIP had no effect on STM in one experiment (H) but a significant effect in another (N). The effects of 3xBRP on longevity are also variable across experiments (Fig. 3C vs. E). What accounts for the differences?

9. Why do synaptic protein levels decline after middle age? Do older flies remember better after PreScale returns to the young fly level?

10. Some figure legends say "flies." Please specify whether they are male or female flies.

Decision Letter 2

Lucas Smith

1 Nov 2022

Dear Dr Sigrist,

Thank you for your patience while we considered your revised manuscript "A brain-wide form of presynaptic active zone plasticity orchestrates resilience to brain aging in Drosophila" for publication as a Research Article at PLOS Biology. This revised version of your manuscript has been evaluated by the PLOS Biology editors and by the Academic Editor.

Overall, the Academic Editor appreciates the amount of work that has gone into this revision and is satisfied by the changes made in response to the reviewers, and so we are likely to accept this manuscript for publication. However before we can editorially accept your study, we need you to address the following data and other policy-related requests.

**IMPORTANT: Please attend to the following editorial requests in a revised manuscript:

1) BLOT AND GEL REPORTING REQUIREMENTS:

We require the original, uncropped and minimally adjusted images supporting all blot and gel results reported in an article's figures or Supporting Information files. We will require these files before a manuscript can be accepted so please prepare and upload them now. Please carefully read our guidelines for how to prepare and upload this data: https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/figures#loc-blot-and-gel-reporting-requirements

>>Please provide the raw, unadjusted and uncropped images supporting Fig 1A-Q; Figure S1A;

2) BLURB: Please provide a blurb which (if accepted) will be included in our weekly and monthly Electronic Table of Contents, sent out to readers of PLOS Biology, and may be used to promote your article in social media. The blurb should be about 30-40 words long and is subject to editorial changes. It should, without exaggeration, entice people to read your manuscript. It should not be redundant with the title and should not contain acronyms or abbreviations

3) DATA POLICY:

You may be aware of the PLOS Data Policy, which requires that all data be made available without restriction: http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/data-availability. For more information, please also see this editorial: http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1001797

Note that we do not require all raw data. Rather, we ask that all individual quantitative observations that underlie the data summarized in the figures and results of your paper be made available in one of the following forms:

a. Supplementary files (e.g., excel). Please ensure that all data files are uploaded as 'Supporting Information' and are invariably referred to (in the manuscript, figure legends, and the Description field when uploading your files) using the following format verbatim: S1 Data, S2 Data, etc. Multiple panels of a single or even several figures can be included as multiple sheets in one excel file that is saved using exactly the following convention: S1_Data.xlsx (using an underscore).

b. Deposition in a publicly available repository. Please also provide the accession code or a reviewer link so that we may view your data before publication.

>>Regardless of the method selected, please ensure that you provide the individual numerical values that underlie the summary data displayed in the following figure panels as they are essential for readers to assess your analysis and to reproduce it:

Fig 1A,C-H,J-K; 2B,D,F-H,J-L,N-P; Fig 3; Fig 4; Fig 5; Fig 6B,D,J,F-H,L,N-P,R; Fig7D,F-L,N,P,R-S

Fig S1B-E; S2C,E,H,J; S3A-K,N-V; S4; S5C-E; S6B-K; S7B-H;

NOTE: the numerical data provided should include all replicates AND the way in which the plotted mean and errors were derived (it should not present only the mean/average values).

>>Please also ensure that figure legends in your manuscript include information on where the underlying data can be found, and ensure your supplemental data file/s has a legend.

>>Please ensure that your Data Statement in the submission system accurately describes where your data can be found.

As you address these items, please take this last chance to review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the cover letter that accompanies your revised manuscript.

We expect to receive your revised manuscript within two weeks.

To submit your revision, please go to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/ and log in as an Author. Click the link labelled 'Submissions Needing Revision' to find your submission record. Your revised submission must include the following:

- a cover letter that should detail your responses to any editorial requests, if applicable, and whether changes have been made to the reference list

- a Response to Editors file that provides a detailed response to the the requests, above

- a track-changes file indicating any changes that you have made to the manuscript.

NOTE: If Supporting Information files are included with your article, note that these are not copyedited and will be published as they are submitted. Please ensure that these files are legible and of high quality (at least 300 dpi) in an easily accessible file format. For this reason, please be aware that any references listed in an SI file will not be indexed. For more information, see our Supporting Information guidelines:

https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/supporting-information

*Published Peer Review History*

Please note that you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out. Please see here for more details:

https://blogs.plos.org/plos/2019/05/plos-journals-now-open-for-published-peer-review/

*Press*

Should you, your institution's press office or the journal office choose to press release your paper, please ensure you have opted out of Early Article Posting on the submission form. We ask that you notify us as soon as possible if you or your institution is planning to press release the article.

*Protocols deposition*

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols

Please do not hesitate to contact me should you have any questions.

Sincerely,

Lucas

Lucas Smith, Ph.D.

Associate Editor,

lsmith@plos.org,

PLOS Biology

Decision Letter 3

Lucas Smith

7 Nov 2022

Dear Dr Sigrist,

Thank you for the submission of your revised Research Article "A brain-wide form of presynaptic active zone plasticity orchestrates resilience to brain aging in Drosophila" for publication in PLOS Biology. On behalf of my colleagues and the Academic Editor, Paul J Shaw, I am pleased to say that we can now, in principle, accept your manuscript for publication, provided you address any remaining formatting and reporting issues. These will be detailed in an email you should receive within 2-3 business days from our colleagues in the journal operations team; no action is required from you until then. Please note that we will not be able to formally accept your manuscript and schedule it for publication until you have completed any requested changes.

Please take a minute to log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/, click the "Update My Information" link at the top of the page, and update your user information to ensure an efficient production process.

PRESS

We frequently collaborate with press offices. If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper at this point, to enable them to help maximise its impact. If the press office is planning to promote your findings, we would be grateful if they could coordinate with biologypress@plos.org. If you have previously opted in to the early version process, we ask that you notify us immediately of any press plans so that we may opt out on your behalf.

We also ask that you take this opportunity to read our Embargo Policy regarding the discussion, promotion and media coverage of work that is yet to be published by PLOS. As your manuscript is not yet published, it is bound by the conditions of our Embargo Policy. Please be aware that this policy is in place both to ensure that any press coverage of your article is fully substantiated and to provide a direct link between such coverage and the published work. For full details of our Embargo Policy, please visit http://www.plos.org/about/media-inquiries/embargo-policy/.

Thank you again for choosing PLOS Biology for publication and supporting Open Access publishing. We look forward to publishing your study. 

Sincerely, 

Lucas Smith, Ph.D., Ph.D.

Associate Editor

PLOS Biology

lsmith@plos.org

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Fig. Synaptic plasticity with aging.

    (A-E) Representative western blots (A) and statistics of a spectrum of synaptic proteins, including BRP Nc82 (B), Dlg1 (C), Syn (D), and Syx (E) in wt female flies with aging. n = 3. One-way ANOVA with Bonferroni multiple comparisons test is shown. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001; ns, not significant. Error bars: mean ± SEM. Underlying data can be found in S1 Data Sheet. Raw images of this figure are provided in S1 Raw Images. Dlg1, Discs large; Syn, Synapsin; Syx, Syntaxin; wt, wild type.

    (TIF)

    S2 Fig. PreScale plasticity provokes activity reprogramming in R5 and dFB neurons.

    (A) CaLexA signal is likely the representation of activity history of neurons. (B and C) Confocal images (B) and whole-mount brain staining analysis (C) of CaLexA signal intensity with CaLexA expressed in R5 neurons by R58H05-Gal4 in 4xBRP compared to 2xBRP flies. n = 20 for all groups. (D and E) Confocal images (D) and whole-mount brain staining analysis (E) of CaLexA signal intensity with CaLexA expressed in R5 neurons by R58H05-Gal4 in 1xBRP compared to 2xBRP flies. n = 12 for all groups. (F) Scheme of an interconnected sleep circuit in the central complex composed of the dFB (marked by R23E10-Gal4), Helicon cells, and the ellipsoid body R5 neurons (R5 marked by R58H05-Gal4). R5 neurons also receive circadian inputs. (G and H) Confocal images (G) and whole-mount brain staining analysis (H) of CaLexA signal intensity with CaLexA expressed in R5 neurons by R23E10-Gal4 in 1xBRP compared to 2xBRP flies. n = 10–12. (I and J) Confocal images (I) and whole-mount brain staining analysis (J) of CaLexA signal intensity with CaLexA expressed in R5 neurons by R23E10-Gal4 in 1xBRP compared to 2xBRP flies, and in sleep-deprived flies. Sleep deprivation was performed between ZT12 to ZT24 during nighttime. n = 12 for all groups. Student t test is shown. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001; ns, not significant. Scale bar: 20 μm. Error bars: mean ± SEM. (K) Confocal image of the expression pattern of R23E10-Gal4 indicated by GFP staining. Scale bar: 100 μm. (L) Widefield image of the cell bodies of R23E10-Gal4 indicated by live GFP signal, the cell body localization is indicated in the dashed circles of (J). Scale bar: 10 μm. Underlying data can be found in S1 Data Sheet. dFB, dorsal fan-shaped body; ZT, zeitgeber time.

    (TIF)

    S3 Fig. BRP promotes sleep in a dosage-dependent manner in male flies, similar to females.

    (A-E) Sleep structure of 1xBRP-4xBRP male flies averaged from measurements over 2–4 days, including sleep profile plotted in 30-min bins (A), daily sleep amount (B), number and duration of sleep episodes (C and D), and sleep latencies (E). n = 63–80. One-way ANOVA with Bonferroni multiple comparisons test is shown. (F) Linear regression analysis of daily sleep amount in flies with different brp copies in both male (R2 = 0.81) and female (R2 = 0.95) flies. n = 63–80 per group for male, n = 123–128 per group for female. (G-K) Sleep structure of 5d female and male wt flies from measurements over 2–4 days, including sleep profile plotted in 30-min bins (G), daily sleep amount (H), number and duration of sleep episodes (I and J), and sleep latencies (K). n = 62–64 per group. (L) Genomic mapping and sequence of the integration site of the brp P[acman] transgenic construct. Red letters indicate brp P[acman] sequence, and black letters indicate genomic CG11357 gene sequence. (M) Simplified gene span of CG11357 and the integration site of the brp P[acman] and another P-element mediated allele EY12484 that are both localized at the 5′ UTR region of CG11357. (N-R) Sleep structure of EY12484 female flies averaged from measurements over 2–4 days, including sleep profile plotted in 30-min bins (N), daily sleep amount (O), number and duration of sleep episodes (P and Q), and sleep latencies (R). n = 55 for wt control and n = 32 for EY12484. Student t test is shown. (S and T) An independent experiment of the lifespan analysis of 2xBRP and 4xBRP flies. For male flies (S), n = 134 for 4xBRP compared to 2xBRP (n = 132, p < 0.001). For female flies (T), n = 134 for 4xBRP compared to 2xBRP wt control flies (n = 141, p < 0.001). (U and V) Lifespan analysis of 2xBRP wt and EY12484 flies. For male flies (U), n = 128 for EY12484 compared to 2xBRP (n = 127, ns). For female flies (V), n = 129 for EY12484 compared to 2xBRP wt control flies (n = 127, ns). Gehan–Breslow–Wilcoxon test is shown for all longevity experiments. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001; ns, not significant. Error bars: mean ± SEM. Underlying data can be found in S1 Data Sheet. BRP, Bruchpilot; CDS, coding DNA sequence; wt, wild type.

    (TIF)

    S4 Fig. Early aging-associated sleep pattern changes of 3xBRP animals.

    (A-D) Sleep structure of 1xBRP female flies at age 20d rescued by a transgenic brp copy (gBRP) and averaged from measurements over 2–3 days, including daily sleep amount (A), number and duration of sleep episodes (B and C), and sleep latencies (D). n = 63–64 for all groups. One-way ANOVA with Bonferroni multiple comparisons test is shown. (E and F) Locomotor walking activity distribution across the day (E) and averaged daily total walking activity (F) of 3xBRP compared to 2xBRP female flies at ages 5d, 20d, 30d, and 40d. (G-O) Sleep structure of 3xBRP female flies at ages 5d, 20d, 30d, and 40d averaged from measurements over 2–3 days, including sleep profile plotted in 30-min bins (G), daytime and nighttime sleep amount (H and L), number and duration of sleep episodes (I, J, M, and N), and sleep latencies (K and O). n = 246–247 for 5d, n = 61–63 for 20d, n = 59–62 for 30d, and n = 32 for 40d. Two-way ANOVA with Sidak multiple comparisons is shown. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001; ns, not significant. Error bars: mean ± SEM. Underlying data can be found in S1 Data Sheet.

    (TIF)

    S5 Fig. Spd supplementation does not affect sleep rebound of 5d young animals.

    (A and B) Rationale (A) and protocol (B) for the consequence of Spd supplementation in age-associated alterations of sleep pattern. Early aging trigger PreScale, memory decline, and sleep pattern changes, which might functionally intersect for survival. Spd supplementation was shown to suppress PreScale and memory decline, but its effect on early aging-associated sleep pattern changes was unclear. (C) Sleep profile for 5d wt female flies treated with 5 mM Spd compared to untreated for 3 consecutive days. (D) Normalized cumulative sleep loss during 12-h nighttime sleep deprivation and 24-h sleep rebound. Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA with Fisher LSD test did not detect any significant treatment × time interaction (F(47, 2784) = 0.0038; p > 0.9999) during sleep rebound. (E) Sleep recovered at three different time points after sleep deprivation for 5d 5 mM Spd-treated compared to untreated female flies. n = 29–31 for both groups. Two-way ANOVA with Sidak multiple comparisons is shown. ns, not significant. Error bars: mean ± SEM. Underlying data can be found in S1 Data Sheet. LSD, least significant difference; Spd, spermidine; wt, wild type.

    (TIF)

    S6 Fig. Acute deep sleep induced by Gaboxadol/THIP feeding.

    (A) Protocol for sleep test of wt female flies treated with different concentrations of THIP at either age 3d or 30d. (B-F) Sleep structure of 5d wt female flies fed with 0.05 mg ml−1 and 0.1 mg ml−1 THIP from measurements over 2–3 days, including sleep profile plotted in 30-min bins (B), daily sleep amount (C), number and duration of sleep episodes (D and E), and sleep latencies (F). n = 64 for untreated control wt flies, n = 32 for both 0.05 mg ml−1 and 0.1 mg ml−1 THIP-treated groups. One-way ANOVA with Bonferroni multiple comparisons test is shown. (G-K) Sleep structure of 30d wt female flies fed with 0.05 mg ml−1 THIP from measurements over 2–3 days, including sleep profile plotted in 30-min bins (G), daily sleep amount (H), number and duration of sleep episodes (I and J), and sleep latencies (K). n = 31 for both groups. Student t test is shown. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001; ns, not significant. Error bars: mean ± SEM. Underlying data can be found in S1 Data Sheet.

    (TIF)

    S7 Fig. Acute Gaboxadol/THIP treatment for 2 days does not alter sleep behavior 1 day after the treatment in young animals.

    (A) Protocol for sleep test of wt female flies that have been treated with 0.1 mg ml−1 THIP for 2 days at age 2d. (B and C) Locomotor walking activity pattern (B) and statistic (C) of 30d wt female flies after 2 days of 0.1 mg ml−1 THIP treatment. (D-H) Sleep structure of 2d wt female flies after 2 days of 0.1 mg ml−1 THIP treatment averaged from measurements over 2 days, including sleep profile plotted in 30-min bins (D), daytime and nighttime sleep amount (E), number and duration of sleep episodes (F and G), and sleep latencies (H). n = 32 for all groups. Student t test is shown. ns, not significant. Error bars: mean ± SEM. Underlying data can be found in S1 Data Sheet.

    (TIF)

    S1 Data. Data underlying Fig 1.

    (XLSX)

    S2 Data. Data underlying Fig 2.

    (XLSX)

    S3 Data. Data underlying Fig 3.

    (XLSX)

    S4 Data. Data underlying Fig 4.

    (XLSX)

    S5 Data. Data underlying Fig 5.

    (XLSX)

    S6 Data. Data underlying Fig 6.

    (XLSX)

    S7 Data. Data underlying Fig 7.

    (XLSX)

    S1 Data Sheet. Data underlying S1S7 Figs.

    (XLSX)

    S1 Raw Images. Original uncropped immunoblots for Figs 1 and S1.

    (PDF)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Huang et al Plos biology R1 rebuttal final.pdf

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.


    Articles from PLOS Biology are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES