Abstract
Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) confers enhanced vulnerability to develop comorbid alcohol use disorder (AUD). Exposure to the scent of a predator, such as the fox odor TMT, has been used to model a traumatic stressor with relevance to PTSD symptomatology. Alcohol produces distinct interoceptive (subjective) effects that may influence vulnerability to problem drinking and AUD. As such, understanding the lasting impact of stressor on sensitivity to the interoceptive effects of alcohol is clinically relevant. The present study used a 2-lever, operant drug discrimination procedure to train male, Long-Evans rats to discriminate the interoceptive effects of alcohol (2 g/kg, i.g.) from water. Upon stable performance, rats underwent a 15-min exposure to TMT. Two weeks later, an alcohol dose-response curve was conducted to evaluate the lasting effects of the TMT stressor on the interoceptive effects of alcohol. The TMT group showed a leftward shift in ED50 of the dose response curve compared to controls, reflecting potentiated interoceptive sensitivity to alcohol. TMT exposure did not affect response rate. GABAergic signaling in both the anterior insular cortex (aIC) and the nucleus accumbens (Acb) is involved in the interoceptive effects of alcohol and stressor-induced adaptations. As such, follow-up experiments in alcohol-naïve rats examined neuronal activation (as measured by c-Fos immunoreactivity) following TMT and showed that TMT exposure increased c-Fos expression in the aIC and the nucleus accumbens core (AcbC). 2 weeks after TMT exposure, Gad-1 gene expression was elevated in the aIC and Gat-1 was increased in the Acb compared to controls. Lastly, the alcohol discrimination and alcohol-naïve groups displayed dramatic differences in stress reactive behaviors during the TMT exposure, suggesting that alcohol exposure may alter the behavioral response to predator odor. Together, these data suggest that predator odor stressor results in potentiated sensitivity to alcohol possibly through GABAergic adaptations in the aIC and Acb, which may be relevant to understanding PTSD-AUD comorbidity.
Keywords: Gad-1, Gat-1, PTSD, AUD, c-Fos, operant drug discrimination
Introduction
Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a neuropsychiatric disorder that develops in some individuals after experiencing or witnessing a traumatic stress event [1]. PTSD is defined by four symptom clusters, including re-experiencing (flashbacks, unwanted memories), avoidance (external reminders), hyperarousal/hypervigilance, and negative mood/thoughts [2]. Additionally, PTSD enhances one’s risk of developing a co-morbid alcohol use disorder (AUD) [3]. Unfortunately, PTSD and AUD are lasting and debilitating disorders for which only limited treatment options exist [1]. Therefore, the goal of the present work was to begin to study how stressor events impact sensitivity to the effects of alcohol, as this may be a factor in PTSD-AUD comorbidity.
One such measure of alcohol sensitivity is the interoceptive effects of alcohol, which refer to the internal bodily state induced by alcohol consumption. Because interoception (e.g., process of sensing, integrating bodily signals) plays an important role in guiding behavior, the interoceptive effects produced by alcohol can influence alcohol intake in both alcohol-dependent and non-dependent individuals by signaling satiety or influencing more drinking [4–11]. Operant drug discrimination is a well-characterized tool for investigating the interoceptive stimulus effect of drugs in preclinical models [12–16]. Using these procedures, the interoceptive drug cue is trained as a discriminative stimulus that guides a specific behavior (e.g., lever press). For example, in the 2-lever operant paradigm used here, responses on one lever (e.g., left lever) are reinforced with sucrose on drug sessions (e.g., following alcohol administration), and responses on the other lever (e.g., right lever) are reinforced with sucrose on vehicle sessions (e.g., following water administration). After many training sessions, the interoceptive state (alcohol stimulus or no alcohol stimulus) informs the correct lever selection. The percent of alcohol-appropriate lever selection serves as a quantitative index of interoceptive sensitivity to alcohol. As such, this methodology is useful for the investigation of neuropharmacological and circuitry mechanisms that underlie interoceptive states [12, 17].
Exposure to predator odors (e.g., bobcat urine, fox feces, soiled litter) in rodents have been employed to model aspects of traumatic stress exposure with relevance to some aspects of PTSD [18–35]. The predator odor 2,5-dihydro-2,4,5-trimethylthiazoline (TMT) is a synthetic component of fox feces that has been shown to produce freezing, avoidance and defensive digging behaviors as well as molecular and physiological adaptations in the brain [29, 30, 34–37]. Additionally, several studies have reported increased alcohol drinking following predator odor exposure [35, 38–41], which may be influenced by a change to the interoceptive effects of alcohol. The reasoning behind this is that an organism may self-administer more alcohol in an effort to achieve a desired interoceptive state. Indeed, prior work has shown that 7-day exposure to the stress-related hormone corticosterone blunts sensitivity to the interoceptive effects of alcohol [42–44]. However, to date the impact of predator odor exposure on alcohol interoceptive effects has yet to be investigated.
The primary goal of the present work was to determine whether TMT exposure affects interoceptive sensitivity to alcohol in male rats trained to discriminate the interoceptive effects of alcohol (2 g/kg) vs. water. The 2 g/kg training dose was chosen because it produces a salient interoceptive stimulus and this dose reflects moderate to high levels of consumption observed in people with AUD. Clinical studies have found a strong link between GABAergic gene expression changes and PTSD in post-mortem brain tissue [45], and alcohol interoceptive effects are in part mediated by GABA receptor activation by alcohol [7, 14, 15, 17, 46, 47]. Furthermore, the anterior insular cortex (aIC) and the nucleus accumbens (Acb) have both been identified as key brain regions in mediating the interoceptive stimulus effects of alcohol [48–51]. Increased activity in the insular cortex was associated with state reexperiencing severity in people with PTSD [52]. In another imaging study, individuals with PTSD showed blunted activation in the nucleus accumbens in response to positive outcome information during a decision-making task [53]. Therefore, in naïve male rats, c-Fos immunoreactivity and GABAergic gene expression analyses were conducted in the anterior insular cortex (aIC) and the nucleus accumbens (Acb). c-Fos was measured immediately following TMT exposure (100 min). Gene expression was evaluated 2 weeks after the exposure, which is the same time point interoceptive sensitivity to alcohol was evaluated. The genes tested were Gad-1, Gad-2 and Gat-1, which encode for GAD-1, GAD-2 and GAT-1, respectively. GAD-1 and GAD-2 (glutamate decarboxylase) are enzymes that convert glutamate to GABA and are often used as a marker of GABAergic cells [54]. GAT-1 is the transporter protein that recycles extracellular GABA back into the presynaptic terminal. As such, GAT-1 expression has been used as an indicator for changes in GABA neurotransmission [55]. Finally, another feature of this study was that by using alcohol-naïve rats for the gene expression analyses, we were able to observationally investigate whether alcohol experience/history influences stress reactive behaviors during the TMT exposure.
Methods
Animals
Male, Long-Evans rats (N=43; Envigo, Indianapolis, IN) were used for all experiments. Rats arrived at 7 weeks and were housed individually. The vivarium was maintained on a 12-hr light/dark cycle (lights on at 7am; lights off at 7pm) and all experiments were conducted during the light cycle. All rats were handled for at least 1 minute daily for 1 week prior to beginning experiments. For Experiment 1 (the drug discrimination experiment), food intake was restricted to maintain body weight (325–340 g). For Experiment 2 (c-Fos experiment) and Experiment 3 (gene expression experiment), rats had ad libitum access to food and were alcohol naive. All rats had ad libitum access to water. Rats were under continuous care and monitoring by veterinary staff from the Division of Comparative Medicine at UNC-Chapel Hill. All procedures were conducted in accordance with the NIH Guide to Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and institutional guidelines.
Experiments (detailed methodology included after the experiment descriptions)
Experiment 1 – The effect of TMT exposure on alcohol interoceptive sensitivity.
Experiment 1 started with daily training of rats to discriminate the interoceptive stimulus effects of alcohol (2 g/kg, i.g.) from water. Upon completion of training criteria (see Discrimination Training; approximately 10 months), a single water and a single alcohol (2 g/kg) test session were conducted on consecutive days (see Discrimination Testing) to confirm stimulus control by alcohol. The following day, rats underwent the 15-min TMT exposure (N=7 CTRL v. N=6 TMT). Rats were then returned to the home cage for 2 weeks. Next, 4 different doses of alcohol (0.0, 0.5, 1.0, 2.0 g/kg, i.g.) were administered prior to a test session on 4 consecutive days to obtain a dose-response curve. Doses were counterbalanced by day and all rats received all alcohol doses.
Experiment 2 – The effect of TMT exposure on c-Fos immunoreactivity.
Experiment 2 is the c-Fos experiment in which alcohol-naïve rats were exposed to TMT for 15 min (N=7 CTRL v. N=8 TMT). After the 15-min exposure, rats were returned to their home cage. Experiment 2 did not include bedding in the test chamber and was not video recorded as this experiment was conducted prior to our lab using bedding during TMT exposures [38]. 100 min after the end of the exposure, rats were euthanized, and brains collected for quantification of c-Fos immunoreactivity (IHC) in the anterior insular cortex (aIC) and the nucleus accumbens core (AcbC).
Experiment 3 – The effects of TMT exposure on GABAergic gene expression.
Experiment 3 is the gene expression experiment in alcohol-naïve rats. Identical to Exp. 1, rats were exposed to TMT for 15-min and returned to their home cage for 2 weeks (N=4 CTRL v. N=11 TMT). A larger sample size was used for the TMT group because prior data showed that TMT exposure results in distinct sub-groups within the TMT-exposed group that differ in brain gene expression [29]. After the 2 weeks, rats were euthanized, and the aIC and Acb (targeting the core) brain regions were punched and collected for qRT-PCR analyses of Gad-1, Gad-2, and Gat-1 expression. See Table 2 for gene expression details.
Table 2 – Gene expression experiment (Experiment 3).
Primer | Forward (5’−3’) | Reverse (5’−3’) |
---|---|---|
Gad-1 | TTGTGTGCTCAGGCTCACCATTGA | ACAAATGCCTGGAGCTGGCTGAAT |
Gad-2 | ATCTTTTCTCCTGGTGGTGCC | GCTCTGACGTGAATGCGATG |
Gat-1 | GTACCGTGGAGGGCTTCATC | CAGGTAGGACACGATGCACA |
β-actin | CTACAATGAGCTGCGTGTGGC | CAGGTCCAGACGCAGGATGGC |
Methodological details
Operant Drug Discrimination
Apparatus –
The chambers (Med Associates, Georgia, VT; measuring 31 × 32 × 24 cm) in which all drug discrimination training and testing occurred have been previously detailed [48]. Briefly, on the right-side wall of the chambers, two retractable levers were on either side of a liquid dipper receptacle. A stimulus light was located above each dipper. Completion of a fixed ratio 10 (FR10) lever responses (i.e., 10 lever responses) activated the dipper to present 0.1 mL of sucrose (10% w/v) for 4 seconds. Chambers were inside of a sound-attenuating cubicle with an exhaust fan. Med Associates program was used to control sessions and record data.
Discrimination Training -
Training sessions occurred daily (Monday-Friday) and are similar to our previously published work [46, 48, 56, 57]. Rats were administered water or alcohol (2 g/kg, i.g.), and then placed in the operant chambers for a 20 min time out period. After this period, both levers (right and left) were introduced to the chamber and the stimulus light was illuminated to indicate the start of the 15-min training session. During an alcohol session, completion of a FR10 on the alcohol-appropriate lever resulted in delivery of sucrose reinforcer (10% w/v, 0.1 mL). Alternatively, during water sessions, completion of FR10 on the water-appropriate lever resulted in delivery of the sucrose reinforcer. During both training sessions, responding on the inappropriate lever was recorded but did not produce a programmed consequence. The alcohol and water appropriate levers were counterbalanced across rats. Training days varied on a double alternation schedule (alcohol, alcohol, water, water,…). Testing began when criteria were met such that the percentage of appropriate lever responses before the first reinforcer and the average of the entire session was >80% for at least 8 out of 10 consecutive days.
Discrimination Testing -
Test sessions were similar to training sessions except they were 2-min in duration (after the 20 min delay) and FR10 on either lever resulted in sucrose delivery so as not to bias lever selection and to enable response rate quantification (measure of nonspecific motor effects). Identical to training sessions, a 20-min delay period elapsed before the start of the 2-min test session.
Predator Odor Exposure
TMT Exposure -
2,5-dihydro-2,4,5-trimethylthiazoline (TMT) exposure test chambers and experimental set-up were identical to our previous publications [34, 35, 58]. Briefly, rats were transported from the vivarium in the home cage to a separate, well-ventilated room that contained the test chambers in which rats were exposed to TMT (45.72 × 17.78 × 21.59 cm; UNC Instrument Shop, Chapel Hill, NC). Only one rat was placed in each chamber. The length of the back wall of the test chambers was opaque white with two opaque black side walls and a clear, plexiglass front wall to enable video recordings and a clear sliding lid. The bottom of the chamber was covered with a layer of white bedding (approximately 1200 ml) [34, 35, 58] except for the c-Fos experiment (Experiment 2) in which no bedding was present. A small, metal basket was hung on the right-side wall (17.8 cm above the floor) to hold a piece of filter paper. 10 μL of TMT (2,5-dihydro-2,4,5-trimethylthiazoline) - or water for controls - was pipetted onto the filter paper in the metal basket immediately prior to putting the rat in the chamber. The control group was always run before the TMT group to prevent odor contamination. The odor exposure session lasted 15 mins and was video recorded (except for Experiment 2) for evaluation of behavior using ANY-maze Video Tracking System (Version 6.12, Stoelting Co. Wood Dale, IL).
Brain tissue collection and sectioning
For the c-Fos experiment (Experiment 2), 100 min after the end of the TMT exposure, rats were injected with pentobarbital (100 mg/kg, i.p) prior to perfusion with 0.1 M PBS (4 °C, pH = 7.4), and then followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA; 4 °C, pH = 7.4) fixation. Brains were removed and stored in 4% PFA for 24 h at 4 °C. Next, brains were rinsed with 0.1 M PBS before being moved to a 30% sucrose in 0.1 M PBS solution. 80-μm coronal sections were collected using a freezing microtome. Table 1 shows regions of quantification and representative images. Sections were stored in cryoprotectant at − 20 °C until starting immunohistochemistry (IHC).
Table 1 – c-Fos Images (Experiment 2).
For the PCR experiment (Experiment 3), rats were euthanized two weeks after the TMT exposure. Rats were anesthetized with 5% isoflurane immediately before brain extraction. Brains were rapidly extracted, and flash frozen with isopentane (Sigma-Aldrich, MI). Brain tissue was stored at −80 °C until brain region sectioning. Brains were sectioned on a cryostat (−20 °C) until reaching a predetermined location (i.e. bregma) for each region of interest (ROI) according to [59]. Next, a micropunch tool was used to extract tissue specific to the brain ROI. These are illustrated in Table 2. Brain tissue punches were then stored at −80°C until RNA extraction.
c-Fos Immunohistochemistry and quantification
Coronal sections (free floating) were rinsed in PBS (0.1 M) before a 5-min wash in 1% hydrogen peroxide. Next, sections were blocked in 3% normal goat serum (NGS; Vector Labs, Burlingame, CA) in 0.3% Triton X-100 for 2 h. Sections were then incubated in rabbit anti-c-Fos antibody (1:4000 in 3% NGS + 0.1% Triton; Synaptic Systems, Gottingen, Germany; Lot# 226003/3–45) for 16 h at 4 °C. This was followed by an incubation with biotinylated goat anti-rabbit secondary antibody (1:200 in 3% NGS + 0.1% Triton X-100; Vector Labs, Burlingame, CA; Lot# 140893). Next, sections were incubated in Vectastain Elite ABC HRP (Vector labs, Burlingame, CA). Lastly, brain sections were treated with diaminobenzidine (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO) and then mounted on glass slides.
Brain section images were obtained using the Olympus CX41 light microscope (Olympus America, Center Valley, PA). The analyses were conducted with Image-Pro Premier image analysis software (Media Cybernetics, Rockville, MD). Bilateral immunoreactivity data (c-Fos-positive cells/mm2; [38, 48]) were taken from a minimum of 2 sections/brain region/animal (e.g., 4 data points per animal), conducted by an experimenter blind to animal groups. These data were then averaged to obtain one value for each subject as in [38, 43, 48]. The regions examined were the aIC (bregma +0.84 to +2.28) and the AcbC (bregma +1.62 to +2.52). Table 1 shows regions of quantification and representative images at 20x magnification.
Gene Expression Analyses
RNA Extraction-
The RNeasy Mini Kit (Qiagen, Venlo, Netherlands) was used to extract RNA from brain tissue according to the manufacturer’s instructions. A Spectrophotometer (Nanodrop 2000, ThermoScientific) was used to determine RNA concentration and purity for each sample.
Reverse Transcription-
The SuperScript™ III First-Strand Synthesis System (ThermoFisher Scientific) was used to reverse transcribed RNA into cDNA according to the manufacturer’s instructions. Next, all samples were diluted 1:5 with water and stored at −20°C before RT-PCR experiments.
RT-PCR-
All experiments used the QuantStudio3 PCR machine (ThermoFisher). All samples were run in triplicate with a 96-well plate, using 10 μL total volume per well with the following components: PowerUp Syber green dye (ThermoFisher, containing ROX dye for passive reference), forward and reverse primers (Eton Biosciences Inc., NC) and cDNA template. The PCR was run with an initial activation for 10 minutes at 95 °C, followed by 40 cycles of the following: denaturation (95°C for 15 seconds), annealing/extension (60°C for 60 seconds). Melt curves were run for all genes to verify synthesis of only one amplicon. β-actin served as the housekeeper gene. Table 2 displays all primer sequences.
Data Analysis
Operant Alcohol Discrimination -
Response accuracy was evaluated as the percentage of alcohol-appropriate lever response upon delivery of the first sucrose reinforcer (i.e. the first FR10; prior to feedback from sucrose delivery). Full expression of the discriminative effects of alcohol was defined as ≥80% alcohol-appropriate responses. No substitution was defined as <40% alcohol-appropriate responding. Partial substitution was defined as between 40% and 80% alcohol-appropriate responding [48, 60, 61]. The response rate was calculated as total lever responses per min and served as an index of motor activity. The effective dose (ED50) was calculated for each rat for the dose-response curve of alcohol-appropriate lever responses (%) as the dose of alcohol which produced 50% alcohol-appropriate lever responding. The 2 doses of alcohol which encompassed 50% alcohol-appropriate responding were used to create a simple linear regression. Next, the x-value (alcohol dose) when y was set to 50% was calculated as the ED50 [62]. For all dose-response curves, a two-way ANOVA was used with dose as a within-subject factor and TMT exposure as a between subject factor. For ED50, a one-tailed, unpaired student’s t-test was used to compare the control to the TMT group. A one-tailed test was used because the direction (increase) of the effect from TMT exposure was already known from the two-way ANOVA. P ≤ 0.05 was set for significance.
c-Fos quantification –
The average number of c-Fos positive cells were compared between the TMT group and the control group using a two-tailed, unpaired student’s t-test. P ≤ 0.05 was set for significance.
Gene Expression -
The ΔΔCt method was used to calculate fold change relative to controls [63]. Fold changes were normalized such that the average control fold change was equal to 1. A two-tailed, unpaired student’s t-test with Welch’s correction was used to compare the TMT to the control group. A Welch’s correction was used due to the difference in sample size between the control and TMT groups (N=4 CTRL v. 11 TMT). In the Acb, 2 samples were lost due to low RNA concentration, resulting in N=4 v. N=9 for the Acb. P ≤ 0.05 was set for significance.
TMT Exposure Behavior -
Analysis of TMT exposure behaviors are identical to our previous work [34, 35, 58]. The length of the TMT exposure chamber (rectangular) was divided in the middle into two compartments for analysis (TMT side and non-TMT side) using ANY-maze software. A metal basket containing TMT (10μL on a piece of filter paper) was located on the far end of one side of the chamber - termed “the TMT side.” The dependent measures evaluated included time spent digging (sec), time spent immobile (sec), time spent grooming (sec), distance traveled (meters), time spent on the TMT side of the chamber (sec), and midline crossings (the number of times the animal crossed between the TMT and non-TMT side). Immobility was operationally defined as the absence of movement except respiration for at least 2 seconds or more and determined automatically using ANYmaze software (see [34]). Time spent digging in the bedding and time spent grooming (see [34, 58]) were quantified manually by an experimenter blind to experimental conditions. For all behaviors, a two-tailed, unpaired student’s t-test was used to compare the control and TMT group. A Welch’s correction was used for the gene expression experiment (Experiment 3) due to the difference in sample size between groups. P ≤ 0.05 was set for significance.
Effect Sizes -
Effect sizes for all statistically significant 2-WAY ANOVA results were calculated as eta squared (η2). For reference, a large effect size is defined as η2 = 0.1379 or greater, a medium effect size is η2 = 0.0588 or greater and a small effect size is η2 = 0.009 or less [64].
Effect sizes for all statistically significant t-test results were calculated as Cohen’s D. For reference, values are considered small (d = 0.2), medium (d = 0.5), and large (d = 0.8) {Cohen, 2013 #498}.
Results
Timeline of Experiments -
Figure 1 shows the experimental timeline for all three experiments: The operant drug discrimination (Experiment 1, Fig. 1A), c-Fos (Experiment 2, Fig. 1B), and gene expression (Experiment 3, Fig. 1C) experiments. For Experiment 1, rats were first trained on the operant discrimination task. Next, a dose response curve was conducted, then rats underwent a 15-min TMT exposure. 2 weeks later, another dose response curve was conducted. For Experiment 2, rats underwent a 15-min TMT exposure, and 100 mins later were euthanized. For Experiment 3, rats underwent a 15-min TMT exposure and 2 weeks later were euthanized.
Operant lever response is under the control of the alcohol training dose (2 g/kg)
Prior to TMT exposure (Figure 2), rats underwent 2 test sessions, one water test and one alcohol (training dose, 2 g/kg) test to confirm high accuracy performance. For the percent alcohol-appropriate lever response (Fig. 2A), there was a main effect of alcohol dose (F (1, 11) = 1475, p<0.0001, η2=0.97) and no effect of group. Rats showed full substitution for the alcohol training dose (2 g/kg) and no substitution at 0 g/kg. For response rate (Fig. 2B), there was a main effect of alcohol dose (F (1, 11) = 14.95, p=0.002, η2=0.18) and no effect of group. Together, these data confirm appropriate discriminative stimulus control in both groups, and no difference in accuracy performance between groups prior to TMT exposure.
TMT exposure potentiates interoceptive sensitivity to alcohol 2 weeks after stressor exposure.
Two weeks after TMT exposure, for the percent alcohol-appropriate lever responses (Fig. 3A), there was a main effect of TMT exposure (F (1, 11) = 5.52, p=0.038, η2=0.04) and alcohol dose (F (3, 33) = 47.18, p<0.0001, η2=0.68), and a trend towards an interaction effect (F (3, 33) = 2.41, p=0.08, η2=0.03). The TMT group showed higher alcohol-appropriate responses than the Control group. At 1 g/kg alcohol, the TMT group displayed partial substitution for the training dose (2 g/kg), but the Control group showed no substitution for the training dose. At the training dose, both TMT and Control groups showed full substitution. Analysis of the ED50 on the dose-response curve for alcohol appropriate responses (%) showed a decrease in the TMT group compared to controls (Fig. 3B, t(11)=1.78, p=0.05, d = 0.98), reflecting a leftward shift of the dose-response curve. For response rate (Fig. 3C), there was a main effect of alcohol dose (F (3, 33) = 5.04, p=0.005, η2=0.15), but no effect of TMT exposure or interaction effect. Together, these data indicate that 2 weeks following TMT exposure, rats displayed potentiated interoceptive sensitivity to alcohol, and this potentiation was not influenced by a nonspecific motor effect.
Molecular adaptations of TMT exposure in the anterior insular cortex and the nucleus accumbens
The results for the c-Fos (Experiment 2) and gene expression (Experiment 3) experiments are shown in Fig. 4. The TMT group showed increased c-Fos immunoreactivity in the anterior insular cortex (aIC, Fig. 4A, t(13)=2.80, p=0.01, d = 1.48) and the nucleus accumbens core (AcbC, Fig. 4B, t(13)=2.79, p=0.02, d = 1.50) compared to controls 100 min after TMT exposure. These data show that TMT exposure results in neuronal activation in the aIC and AcbC.
Two weeks after TMT exposure (Experiment 3), in the anterior insular cortex (aIC) Gad-1 gene expression was increased in the TMT group (Fig. 4C, t(10.76)=2.83, p=0.01, d = 1.41). Gad-2 expression did not differ between TMT and control group but was trending towards an increase in expression (p=0.07; CTRL: 1.0 ± 0.16; TMT: 1.48 ± 0.18). Gat-1 was not tested in the aIC due to an unfortunate loss of samples. In the Acb (Fig. 4D), Gat-1 was elevated as a result of TMT exposure (t(8.69)=3.73, p=0.005, d = 1.78). Gad-1 (CTRL: 1.0 ± 0.068; TMT: 1.5 ± 0.28) and Gad-2 (CTRL: 1.0 ± 0.12; TMT: 1.11 ± 0.10) expression were not affected by TMT exposure in the Acb.
TMT exposure produces stress-reactive behaviors in alcohol-naïve and alcohol-experienced rat cohorts during the TMT exposure.
Rats engaged in numerous stress-reactive behaviors during the TMT exposure. These behaviors during the TMT exposure for Experiments 1 and 3 are illustrated in Figure 5. In rats trained on the operant alcohol discrimination (Experiment 1 – alcohol-experienced), TMT exposure increased time spent digging (Fig. 5A, t(11)=5.11, p=0.0003, d = 2.71), but did not affect time spent immobile (Fig. 5B). TMT exposure decreased time spent grooming (Fig. 5C, t(11)=3.60, p=0.004, d = 2.09) and increased distance traveled (Fig. 5D, t(11)=5.83, p=0.0001, d = 3.17). TMT exposure did not affect the time spent on the TMT side (Fig. 5E) but increased the number of midline crossings (Fig. 5F, t(11)=2.98, p=0.012, d = 1.59) compared to controls.
In alcohol-naïve rats (Experiment 3, gene expression), TMT exposure increased time spent digging (Fig. 5G, t(10.18)=4.22, p=0.002, d = 1.80) and time spent immobile (Fig. 5H, t(6.76)=5.97, p=0.0006, d = 3.30). TMT exposure did not affect time spent grooming (Fig. 5I), but decreased distanced traveled (Fig. 5J, t(7.37)=3.69, p=0.007, d = 2.00), time spent on the TMT side (Fig. 5K, t(3.92)=5.73, p=0.005, d = 3.66) and the number of midline crossings (Fig. 5L, t(5.65)=2.38, p=0.05, d = 1.37) compared to controls. While behavior from Experiment 1 and 3 were not statistically compared, the alcohol discrimination group appears to show a different behavioral response to TMT relative to the alcohol-naïve group.
Discussion
These studies show that TMT exposure produces potentiated sensitivity to the interoceptive effects of alcohol 2 weeks after the exposure. Molecular experiments demonstrate neuronal activation as measured by c-Fos immunoreactivity immediately following TMT, and increased GABAergic gene expression 2 weeks after TMT in the anterior insular cortex (aIC) and nucleus accumbens (Acb). Together, these data indicate that TMT exposure results in lasting potentiation of alcohol interoceptive sensitivity, possibly through GABAergic adaptations in the aIC and Acb. Additionally, the degree of engagement in stress reactive behaviors during the TMT exposure differed in alcohol-naïve and alcohol-experienced rats, suggesting a potential influence of alcohol experience.
We initially hypothesized that TMT exposure would blunt interoceptive sensitivity to alcohol [38]. This hypothesis was supported by previous experiments showing blunted interoceptive sensitivity to alcohol following 7-day exposure to corticosterone in the rats’ drinking water [42–44]. However, contrary to this hypothesis, we found potentiated sensitivity to the interoceptive effects of alcohol 2 weeks after the TMT exposure. As such, these differences may be a consequence of the type of stressor-related models used (i.e., single exposure to an ethologically relevant stressor - predator odor as in the present study vs. a physiological stressor – corticosterone for 7 days). Note that these studies also differ in the alcohol training dose (1 g/kg vs. 2 g/kg) and pretreatment time of alcohol administration (10 min vs. 20 min) used, both of which may also play a role in the different effects observed. The present data are the first to suggest the possibility that different stress models may have opposite effects on alcohol interoceptive sensitivity.
In alcohol-naïve rats, the anterior insular cortex (aIC) and the nucleus accumbens core (AcbC) showed neuronal activation during TMT exposure as evid enced by increased c-Fos immunoreactivity. 2 weeks after the stressor, gene expression data showed increased expression of Gad-1 in the aIC and Gat-1 in the Acb. Gad-1 is a marker of GABAergic cells, which suggests that TMT exposure increased GABAergic cell number in the aIC. Gat-1 is the gene that encodes the GABA neurotransmitter transporter protein (GAT-1), which suggests altered GABA neurotransmitter levels in the synapse in the Acb. Prior data have shown increased NMDA receptor gene expression in the aIC 2 weeks after TMT [58], suggesting increased excitatory signaling in the aIC. As such, increased Gad-1 expression in the aIC observed here may be the result of compensatory increase of local, GABAergic inhibition onto excitatory cells to restore changes in excitatory/inhibitory ratio [65]. GABA receptor potentiation by alcohol in the aIC and Acb both play a key role in the interoceptive effects of alcohol [14, 15, 48]. As such, the GABAergic adaptations in the aIC and Acb 2 weeks after TMT exposure may in part underlie the potentiated sensitivity to alcohol.
We did not conduct statistical comparisons between the alcohol-experienced, discrimination-trained (Experiment 1) and alcohol-naïve (Experiment 3) rats because they are from separate cohorts, and there are several differences other than alcohol experience between groups (i.e., daily gavage, behavioral training, and food restriction in the alcohol-discrimination group). Nevertheless, visual comparison of groups shows some striking differences that may suggest that alcohol experience affects behavioral responses to a predator odor stressor. Both cohorts showed increased digging behavior in the TMT group, consistent with previous work [34, 58]. However, the alcohol-experienced cohort engaged in digging behavior to a greater extent than the alcohol-naïve cohort. Additionally, the alcohol-experienced cohort did not show increased immobility (which in part captures freezing, see [34]) during the TMT exposure, whereas the alcohol-naïve cohort did. Immobility during TMT exposure is consistent with prior work [34, 35]. Time spent grooming during the TMT exposure was decreased in alcohol-experienced rats but unchanged in alcohol-naïve rats. In relation to distanced traveled during the TMT exposure, the alcohol-experienced rats showed an increase, whereas the alcohol-naïve rats showed a decrease, likely because the naïve group was engaging in immobility behavior. The alcohol-experienced group did not show a difference in the time spent on the TMT side, whereas the alcohol-naïve rats spent significantly less time on the TMT side, likely because rats primarily engage in immobility behavior away from the TMT source (i.e., non-TMT side), which would lead to less time on the TMT side [34]. Finally, alcohol-experienced rats showed increased midline crossings during TMT exposure, whereas the alcohol-naïve group showed decreased midline crossings. Overall, the alcohol-experienced cohort (the discrimination cohort) showed a more hyperactive response during TMT (greater digging, decreased grooming, increased distance traveled and midline crossings, and no avoidance of the TMT side or immobility) than the alcohol-naïve rats. Alcohol-naive rats showed behavior more consistent with a fear response in rodents (i.e. freezing and avoidance) than the alcohol-experienced group. As such, alcohol experience may shift the behavioral response to a predator odor from that of freezing and avoidance to hyperactivity and increased active engagement (e.g., digging, increased distance traveled and midline crossings, decreased grooming). Interestingly, the degree of engagement in stress reactive behaviors during TMT exposure observed here in alcohol-naïve rats are consistent with findings published by Ornelas et al. (2021) [35]. In that study, male rats had a limited history of alcohol self-administration (9 days of low to moderate alcohol consumption ~0.5–1 g/kg daily). This limited experience is more similar to the naïve rats than the drug discrimination trained rats used in the present work that were administered 2 g/kg approximately 3 times/week across months of discrimination training, but also suggests that there could be a threshold of alcohol exposure that promotes the shift in TMT-induced behavioral responses.
It is interesting to consider how potentiated sensitivity to alcohol may influence alcohol drinking. A study by Schreiber et al ([66]) found that predator odor exposure (bobcat urine) blunted the conditioned place aversion (CPA) induced by 1 g/kg alcohol in male Wistar rats, suggesting that predator odor exposure may be attenuating the aversive properties of alcohol and/or potentiating the rewarding properties of alcohol. However, the Ornelas et al. (2021) paper showed that male Long Evans rats exposed to TMT exposure in the same manner as used in the present study did not show increased alcohol self-administration. As such, the increased interoceptive sensitivity to alcohol after TMT observed here may be “protective” against escalated alcohol self-administration. However, a major difference between the Ornelas et al study and the present work is the history of alcohol exposure (as discussed in the previous paragraph) and therefore, the effects of TMT exposure on alcohol drinking may be expected to differ in rats that have an extensive history of alcohol exposure. This possibility is supported by the large difference in stress reactivity behavior in rats with an extensive alcohol history vs. naïve rats shown in the present work. Indeed, in another study using bobcat urine as the predator odor exposure, rats with a 5-week history of 2-bottle choice intermittent access to a 20% alcohol solution demonstrated a different stress response to the predator odor context than an alcohol-naïve cohort and showed escalated alcohol self-administration. In that work, classification criteria based on the time spent in proximity to the predator odor source during a context re-exposure test was used to sub-group rats into “avoiders” and “non-avoiders.” Specifically, alcohol experience was associated with a greater proportion of male rats who met criteria for “avoiders” [67], and these “avoider” male rats showed increased alcohol self-administration [39]. Together, these data show that a history of alcohol consumption prior to predator odor exposure may alter the behavioral consequences and subsequent effects on alcohol consumption of the stressor. Therefore, it will be interesting for future work to examine alcohol self-administration in male and female rats following an alcohol treatment protocol like that used in the current study, as potentiated sensitivity to the interoceptive effects of alcohol may be expected to prime escalations in alcohol intake.
Some limitations of these experiments should be addressed. First, these experiments were conducted in male rats only, and there may be important and clinically relevant sex differences in the consequences of predator odor exposure on sensitivity to the interoceptive effects of alcohol. Indeed, our lab has found differences in response to TMT exposure on alcohol-self-administration in males and females such that only a sub-group of female rats showed escalated alcohol self-administration following TMT exposure [35]. Therefore, the way TMT exposure affects interoceptive sensitivity to alcohol may differ by sex. Furthermore, women are at greater risk for developing PTSD-AUD comorbidity than men. A national study found that 26.2% of women and 10.3% of men in a population of individuals with AUD also have PTSD [68], further underlying the importance of follow up work in female rats. Secondly, gene expression and c-Fos experiments were conducted in alcohol-naïve rats, but the potentiated alcohol sensitivity effect was observed in an alcohol-experienced group, potentially affecting the way in which TMT exposure impacts molecular changes. It will be important for future work to examine these molecular changes in discrimination-trained rats. Finally, a functional connection between GABAergic adaptations and potentiated alcohol sensitivity was not made. Therefore, it will be interesting for future work to directly investigate this by testing substitution of a GABAergic positive modulator. Despite these limitations, these data are the first to show that predator odor exposure potentiates sensitivity to the interoceptive effects of alcohol.
These data demonstrate that TMT exposure has long-lasting (2 weeks) effects on alcohol interoceptive sensitivity, specifically potentiation of alcohol effects in male rats. Furthermore, we found increased expression of GABA-related genes in the aIC and Acb. While these molecular changes may underlie the potentiated alcohol sensitivity [7, 13–17, 46, 47, 69], these experiments were conducted in alcohol-naïve rats. Thus, it will be important for future work to examine the functional relevance of these changes to the interoceptive effects of alcohol by manipulating the GABAergic system following TMT exposure in discrimination-trained rats. Because the potentiation of the interoceptive effects of alcohol may increase alcohol consumption by priming continued drinking, these findings may have important implications for understanding PTSD-AUD comorbidity.
Funding:
This work was supported in part by the National Institutes of Health [AA026537 and AA011605 (JB)] and by the Bowles Center for Alcohol Studies. RET was supported in part by AA029946 and MB was supported by NS007431.
Footnotes
Conflicts of interest: none.
Data Availability Statement:
Data available on request from the authors.
References
- 1.Yehuda R, et al. , Post-traumatic stress disorder. Nat Rev Dis Primers, 2015. 1: p. 15057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Association, A.P., Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). 2013: American Psychiatric Pub. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Goldstein RB, et al. , The epidemiology of DSM-5 posttraumatic stress disorder in the United States: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions-III. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol, 2016. 51(8): p. 1137–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Verdejo-Garcia A, Clark L, and Dunn BD, The role of interoception in addiction: a critical review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 2012. 36(8): p. 1857–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hodge CW, et al. , Understanding how the brain perceives alcohol: neurobiological basis of ethanol discrimination. Alcohol Clin Exp Res, 2006. 30(2): p. 203–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Bolin BL, et al. , Human drug discrimination: A primer and methodological review. Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology, 2016. 24(4): p. 214–228. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Allen DC, Ford MM, and Grant KA, Cross-Species Translational Findings in the Discriminative Stimulus Effects of Ethanol. Curr Top Behav Neurosci, 2018. 39: p. 95–111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.LeCocq MR, et al. , Considering Drug-Associated Contexts in Substance Use Disorders and Treatment Development. Neurotherapeutics, 2020. 17(1): p. 43–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Childs E, O’Connor S, and de Wit H, Bidirectional interactions between acute psychosocial stress and acute intravenous alcohol in healthy men. Alcohol Clin Exp Res, 2011. 35(10): p. 1794–803. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kelly TH, et al. , Clinical neuropharmacology of drugs of abuse: a comparison of drug-discrimination and subject-report measures. Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev, 2003. 2(4): p. 227–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Holdstock L, King AC, and de Wit H, Subjective and objective responses to ethanol in moderate/heavy and light social drinkers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res, 2000. 24(6): p. 789–94. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Grant KA, Strategies for understanding the pharmacological effects of ethanol with drug discrimination procedures. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 1999. 64(2): p. 261–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Grant KA and Colombo G, Discriminative stimulus effects of ethanol: effect of training dose on the substitution of N-methyl-D-aspartate antagonists. J Pharmacol Exp Ther, 1993. 264(3): p. 1241–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Hodge CW and Cox AA, The discriminative stimulus effects of ethanol are mediated by NMDA and GABA(A) receptors in specific limbic brain regions. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 1998. 139(1–2): p. 95–107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Stolerman IP, et al. , Role of training dose in drug discrimination: a review. Behavioural pharmacology, 2011. 22(5–6): p. 415–429. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kostowski W and Bieńkowski P, Discriminative stimulus effects of ethanol: neuropharmacological characterization. Alcohol, 1999. 17(1): p. 63–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Lovelock DF, Tyler RE, and Besheer J, Interoception and alcohol: Mechanisms, networks, and implications. Neuropharmacology, 2021. 200: p. 108807. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Albrechet-Souza L and Gilpin NW, The predator odor avoidance model of post-traumatic stress disorder in rats. Behav Pharmacol, 2019. 30(2 and 3-Spec Issue): p. 105–114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Blanchard DC, et al. , Failure to produce conditioning with low-dose trimethylthiazoline or cat feces as unconditioned stimuli. Behav Neurosci, 2003. 117(2): p. 360–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Brodnik ZD, et al. , Susceptibility to traumatic stress sensitizes the dopaminergic response to cocaine and increases motivation for cocaine. Neuropharmacology, 2017. 125: p. 295–307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Cohen H, et al. , The contribution of an animal model toward uncovering biological risk factors for PTSD. Ann N Y Acad Sci, 2006. 1071: p. 335–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Deslauriers J, et al. , Current Status of Animal Models of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: Behavioral and Biological Phenotypes, and Future Challenges in Improving Translation. Biol Psychiatry, 2018. 83(10): p. 895–907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Dielenberg RA and McGregor IS, Defensive behavior in rats towards predatory odors: a review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 2001. 25(7–8): p. 597–609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Endres T, Apfelbach R, and Fendt M, Behavioral Changes Induced in Rats by Exposure to Trimethylthiazoline, a Component of Fox Odor. Behavioral Neuroscience, 2005. 119(4): p. 1004–1010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Endres T and Fendt M, Aversion- vs fear-inducing properties of 2,4,5-trimethyl-3-thiazoline, a component of fox odor, in comparison with those of butyric acid. J Exp Biol, 2009. 212(Pt 15): p. 2324–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.McGregor IS, et al. , Not all ‘predator odours’ are equal: cat odour but not 2,4,5 trimethylthiazoline (TMT; fox odour) elicits specific defensive behaviours in rats. Behav Brain Res, 2002. 129(1–2): p. 1–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Rosen JB, Asok A, and Chakraborty T, The smell of fear: innate threat of 2,5-dihydro-2,4,5-trimethylthiazoline, a single molecule component of a predator odor. Frontiers in neuroscience, 2015. 9: p. 292–292. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Rosen JB, West EA, and Donley MP, Not all rat strains are equal: differential unconditioned fear responses to the synthetic fox odor 2,4,5-trimethylthiazoline in three outbred rat strains. Behav Neurosci, 2006. 120(2): p. 290–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Schwendt M, et al. , A novel rat model of comorbid PTSD and addiction reveals intersections between stress susceptibility and enhanced cocaine seeking with a role for mGlu5 receptors. Translational Psychiatry, 2018. 8(1): p. 209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Shallcross J, et al. , The Divergent Effects of CDPPB and Cannabidiol on Fear Extinction and Anxiety in a Predator Scent Stress Model of PTSD in Rats. Frontiers in behavioral neuroscience, 2019. 13: p. 91–91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Thomas RM, Urban JH, and Peterson DA, Acute exposure to predator odor elicits a robust increase in corticosterone and a decrease in activity without altering proliferation in the adult rat hippocampus. Exp Neurol, 2006. 201(2): p. 308–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Wallace KJ and Rosen JB, Predator odor as an unconditioned fear stimulus in rats: elicitation of freezing by trimethylthiazoline, a component of fox feces. Behav Neurosci, 2000. 114(5): p. 912–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Whitaker AM, Gilpin NW, and Edwards S, Animal models of post-traumatic stress disorder and recent neurobiological insights. Behavioural pharmacology, 2014. 25(5–6): p. 398–409. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Tyler RE, et al. , Exposure to the predator odor TMT induces early and late differential gene expression related to stress and excitatory synaptic function throughout the brain in male rats. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 2020. n/a(n/a): p. e12684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Ornelas LC, et al. , Increased alcohol self-administration following exposure to the predator odor TMT in active coping female rats. Behav Brain Res, 2020: p. 113068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Hwa LS, et al. , Predator odor increases avoidance and glutamatergic synaptic transmission in the prelimbic cortex via corticotropin-releasing factor receptor 1 signaling. Neuropsychopharmacology, 2019. 44(4): p. 766–775. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar] [Retracted]
- 37.Ornelas LC, Van Voorhies K, and Besheer J, The role of the nucleus reuniens in regulating contextual conditioning with the predator odor TMT in female rats. Psychopharmacology, 2021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Makhijani VH, et al. , The synthetically produced predator odor 2,5-dihydro-2,4,5-trimethylthiazoline increases alcohol self-administration and alters basolateral amygdala response to alcohol in rats. Psychopharmacology, 2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Edwards S, et al. , Traumatic stress reactivity promotes excessive alcohol drinking and alters the balance of prefrontal cortex-amygdala activity. Transl Psychiatry, 2013. 3(8): p. e296. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Weera MM, et al. , The role of central amygdala corticotropin-releasing factor in predator odor stress-induced avoidance behavior and escalated alcohol drinking in rats. Neuropharmacology, 2020. 166: p. 107979. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Manjoch H, et al. , Predator-scent stress, ethanol consumption and the opioid system in an animal model of PTSD. Behav Brain Res, 2016. 306: p. 91–105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Besheer J, et al. , The effects of repeated corticosterone exposure on the interoceptive effects of alcohol in rats. Psychopharmacology, 2012. 220(4): p. 809–822. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Besheer J, et al. , Stress Hormone Exposure Reduces mGluR5 Expression in the Nucleus Accumbens: Functional Implications for Interoceptive Sensitivity to Alcohol. Neuropsychopharmacology, 2014. 39(10): p. 2376–2386. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Jaramillo AA, et al. , Activation of mGluR2/3 following stress hormone exposure restores sensitivity to alcohol in rats. Alcohol, 2015. 49(6): p. 525–532. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Girgenti MJ, et al. , Transcriptomic organization of the human brain in post-traumatic stress disorder. Nature Neuroscience, 2021. 24(1): p. 24–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Besheer J, et al. , Gabapentin potentiates sensitivity to the interoceptive effects of alcohol and increases alcohol self-administration in rats. Neuropharmacology, 2016. 101: p. 216–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Kostowski W and Bienkowski P, Discriminative stimulus effects of ethanol: neuropharmacological characterization. Alcohol, 1999. 17(1): p. 63–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Jaramillo AA, et al. , Modulation of sensitivity to alcohol by cortical and thalamic brain regions. The European journal of neuroscience, 2016. 44(8): p. 2569–2580. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Jaramillo AA, et al. , Silencing the insular-striatal circuit decreases alcohol self-administration and increases sensitivity to alcohol. Behavioural brain research, 2018. 348: p. 74–81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Besheer J, Cox AA, and Hodge CW, Coregulation of ethanol discrimination by the nucleus accumbens and amygdala. Alcohol Clin Exp Res, 2003. 27(3): p. 450–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Allen DC, et al. , Characterization of DREADD receptor expression and function in rhesus macaques trained to discriminate ethanol. Neuropsychopharmacology, 2021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Hopper JW, et al. , Neural correlates of reexperiencing, avoidance, and dissociation in PTSD: symptom dimensions and emotion dysregulation in responses to script-driven trauma imagery. J Trauma Stress, 2007. 20(5): p. 713–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Sailer U, et al. , Altered reward processing in the nucleus accumbens and mesial prefrontal cortex of patients with posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuropsychologia, 2008. 46(11): p. 2836–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Feldblum S, Erlander MG, and Tobin AJ, Different distributions of GAD65 and GAD67 mRNAs suggest that the two glutamate decarboxylases play distinctive functional roles. J Neurosci Res, 1993. 34(6): p. 689–706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Bernstein EM and Quick MW, Regulation of gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) transporters by extracellular GABA. J Biol Chem, 1999. 274(2): p. 889–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Besheer J, et al. , Interoceptive effects of alcohol require mGlu5 receptor activity in the nucleus accumbens. J Neurosci, 2009. 29(30): p. 9582–91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Jaramillo AA, et al. , Functional role for cortical-striatal circuitry in modulating alcohol self-administration. Neuropharmacology, 2018. 130: p. 42–53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Tyler RE, et al. , The effects of predator odor (TMT) exposure and mGlu3 NAM pretreatment on behavioral and NMDA receptor adaptations in the brain. Neuropharmacology, 2022. 207: p. 108943. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Paxinos G and Watson C, The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates: Hard Cover Edition. 2006: Elsevier Science. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Solinas M, et al. , Using drug-discrimination techniques to study the abuse-related effects of psychoactive drugs in rats. Nat Protoc, 2006. 1(3): p. 1194–206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Jaramillo AA, et al. , Functional role for suppression of the insular–striatal circuit in modulating interoceptive effects of alcohol. Addiction Biology, 2018. 23(5): p. 1020–1031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Smith DA, et al. , Cocaine-like discriminative stimulus effects of alpha-pyrrolidinovalerophenone, methcathinone and their 3,4-methylenedioxy or 4-methyl analogs in rhesus monkeys. Addict Biol, 2017. 22(5): p. 1169–1178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Livak KJ and Schmittgen TD, Analysis of relative gene expression data using real-time quantitative PCR and the 2(-Delta Delta C(T)) Method. Methods, 2001. 25(4): p. 402–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Richardson JTE, Eta squared and partial eta squared as measures of effect size in educational research. Educational Research Review, 2011. 6(2): p. 135–147. [Google Scholar]
- 65.Barral J and D Reyes A, Synaptic scaling rule preserves excitatory–inhibitory balance and salient neuronal network dynamics. Nature Neuroscience, 2016. 19(12): p. 1690–1696. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Schreiber AL, et al. , Predator odor stress blunts alcohol conditioned aversion. Neuropharmacology, 2019. 144: p. 82–90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Albrechet-Souza L, Schratz CL, and Gilpin NW, Sex differences in traumatic stress reactivity in rats with and without a history of alcohol drinking. Biol Sex Differ, 2020. 11(1): p. 27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Kessler RC, et al. , Lifetime co-occurrence of DSM-III-R alcohol abuse and dependence with other psychiatric disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 1997. 54(4): p. 313–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Bienkowski P, Stefanski R, and Kostowski W, Competitive NMDA receptor antagonist, CGP 40116, substitutes for the discriminative stimulus effects of ethanol. European Journal of Pharmacology, 1996. 314(3): p. 277–280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Data available on request from the authors.