Abstract
Common variation in the gene encoding the neuron-specific RNA splicing factor RNA Binding Fox-1 Homolog 1 (RBFOX1) has been identified as a risk factor for several psychiatric conditions, and rare genetic variants have been found causal for autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Here, we explored the genetic landscape of RBFOX1 more deeply, integrating evidence from existing and new human studies as well as studies in Rbfox1 knockout mice. Mining existing data from large-scale studies of human common genetic variants, we confirmed gene-based and genome-wide association of RBFOX1 with risk tolerance, major depressive disorder and schizophrenia. Data on six mental disorders revealed copy number losses and gains to be more frequent in ASD cases than in controls. Consistently, RBFOX1 expression appeared decreased in post-mortem frontal and temporal cortices of individuals with ASD and prefrontal cortex of individuals with schizophrenia. Brain-functional MRI studies demonstrated that carriers of a common RBFOX1 variant, rs6500744, displayed increased neural reactivity to emotional stimuli, reduced prefrontal processing during cognitive control, and enhanced fear expression after fear conditioning, going along with increased avoidance behaviour. Investigating Rbfox1 neuron-specific knockout mice allowed us to further specify the role of this gene in behaviour. The model was characterised by pronounced hyperactivity, stereotyped behaviour, impairments in fear acquisition and extinction, reduced social interest, and lack of aggression; it provides excellent construct and face validity as an animal model of ASD. In conclusion, convergent translational evidence shows that common variants in RBFOX1 are associated with a broad spectrum of psychiatric traits and disorders, while rare genetic variation seems to expose to early-onset neurodevelopmental psychiatric disorders with and without developmental delay like ASD, in particular. Studying the pleiotropic nature of RBFOX1 can profoundly enhance our understanding of mental disorder vulnerability.
Subject terms: Autism spectrum disorders, Genetics, Schizophrenia
Introduction
Mental disorders are characterised by substantial heritability but have a complex genetic architecture contributed by common genetic variants of individually small effects and rare variants of intermediate or large effects [1]. A high degree of comorbidity and substantial genetic correlation among psychiatric diseases point to pleiotropic effects of etiologic factors. Genetic pleiotropy is also seen in loss-of-function variants causing rare, severe genetic syndromes, while regulatory variations are associated with milder, more frequent forms of a disorder. In the most recent Psychiatric Genomics Consortium (PGC) cross-disorder genome-wide association studies (GWAS) meta-analysis [2, 3], RNA Binding Fox-1 Homolog 1 (RBFOX1) was the second most pleiotropic locus and found to be associated with seven out of eight disorders analysed. Since then, GWAS on mood and anxiety disorders and neuroticism have extended the spectrum of disorders and behavioural traits involving common variants of RBFOX1 [4–7]. Beyond these studies of common genetic variation, rare genetic variants in RBFOX1 such as copy number variants (CNVs) and loss-of-function mutations have been related to early-onset neurodevelopmental disorders, especially autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) [8–13]; furthermore, case (series) reports have also implicated rare variants in RBFOX1 in developmental delay, intellectual disability, epilepsy, and aggression [14]. RBFOX1 encodes a splicing factor that regulates alternative splicing and the expression of large networks of genes involved in brain development [15, 16]. These data collectively implicate RBFOX1 as one of the most relevant risk genes for psychopathology; however, neither the specific behavioural domains nor the involved neural circuits have yet been identified.
To delineate the genetic contribution of RBFOX1 to mental disorders, we comprehensively data-mined and synthesised large-scale datasets on common and rare genetic variations in psychiatric disorders and traits. We next studied the influence of genetic variation in RBFOX1 on human neural circuits to uncover its functional consequences. To do so, we focused on a common genetic variant, which emerged as the most promising SNP (rs6500744; risk allele: C) in a previous study [14]. Given the association of common variants in RBFOX1 with mood and anxiety disorders and neuroticism (see above), we investigated the effect of rs6500744 on circuits underlying emotion processing, fear conditioning, and executive functioning using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), and a test assessing standardised fear reactions and avoidance behaviour towards an aversive stimulus, the Behavioural Avoidance Task (BAT [17]). The functional consequences of the SNPs and CNVs in RBFOX1 are yet unknown. However, given the decreased expression of RBFOX1 observed in post-mortem studies of ASD and SCZ patients, and the observed over-abundance of RBFOX1 CNV-deletions in (early-onset) mental disorders, we reasoned that loss of RBFOX1 function might underlie at least part of the observed associations. We, therefore, generated a neuron-specific Rbfox1 heterozygous (HET) and homozygous (KO) knockout mouse line to determine the behavioural consequences of decreased Rbfox1 expression in the brain.
Materials and methods
Common and rare genetic risk variants in RBFOX1 in psychiatric phenotypes
The contribution of common variants (MAF > 0.01) in the RBFOX1 gene to psychiatric disorders or related behavioural traits was assessed through SNP-based and gene-based association studies using GWAS summary statistics from previous studies (Supplementary Table 1). In total, eleven psychiatric conditions or traits were investigated: attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), aggression (AGG), anorexia (ANO), anxiety (ANX), autism spectrum disorder (ASD), bipolar disorder (BIP), major depressive disorder (MDD), obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), risk tolerance (RT), schizophrenia (SCZ) and Tourette’s syndrome (TS), and the cross-disorder meta-analysis of eight of them (CD-MA). Further information about the SNP- and gene-based analyses can be found in Supplementary Methods.
CNVs in RBFOX1 were collected from publicly available data from the above disorders or traits (in patients and in controls when reported), either in published papers (until April 2020) or databases (DECIPHER, https://decipher.sanger.ac.uk; ClinVar, https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/clinvar; ISCA, http://dbsearch.clinicalgenome.org/search/). To inspect the overlap between CNVs identified in patients and putative cis-regulatory elements we used epigenetic data from ENCODE (https://www.encodeproject.org/) from seven neural tissues and brain-related Hi-C data from 3DIV to identify interactions with the first distal promoter. We performed burden analysis for RBFOX1 CNVs in 18 out of 34 studies where information in controls was available using PLINK v.1.07 considering CN loss and CN gains separately as well as both together.
Expression of RBFOX1 in brain samples of ASD and SCZ patients
Alterations in the expression of RBFOX1 in the brain were assessed using transcriptomic data from post-mortem brain samples of ASD and SCZ patients, compared to controls, using publicly available human datasets, either in GEO (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/geo) or published articles (Supplementary Table 7). RBFOX1 expression was explored in different brain areas, including the hippocampus, cerebellum, or cortex, depending on the dataset.
RBFOX1 rs6500744 in functional MRI
Genotyping
DNA was extracted from whole blood according to standard procedures for all participants. Then, genome-wide SNP genotyping was performed using a standard GWAS chip (PsychChip, Illumina Human610-Quad BeadChip [Illumina, Inc., San Diego]). Based on this genome-wide chip, genotype information for the rs6500744 RBFOX1 SNP was retrieved for each individual using plink (http://zzz.bwh.harvard.edu/plink/). The observed genotype distribution of rs6500744 did not deviate from the Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (Flanker/Go-NoGo: p = 0.826 [C/C carrier: n = 71, C/T carrier: n = 158, T/T carrier: n = 95]; Face matching: p = 0.821 [C/C carrier: n = 70, C/T carrier: n = 154, T/T carrier: n = 89]; computed based on the CRAN R-package, https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/Hardy-Weinberg /index.html;). Analogous to previous imaging genetic studies on common genetic risk variants for psychiatric disorders [18–21], we compared brain activation for risk-allele carrier (C/C and C/T) to no-risk allele carrier (T/T carrier) of rs6500744 (as proposed by [14]).
Flanker/Go-NoGo and face matching tasks
Sample
We included the data of 324 (Flanker/Go-NoGo task) and 313 (Face matching task) healthy adults of European ancestry who have been recruited as healthy controls within the framework of a multi-site imaging genetics study assessing the intermediate phenotypes of psychiatric disorders such as depression, schizophrenia, and bipolar disorder (for previous work, see refs. [19, 22–26]). Data collection was carried out at the Central Institute of Mental Health in Mannheim, at the Medical Faculty of the University of Bonn, and the Charité University Medicine in Berlin. All participants provided a whole-blood sample for DNA extraction and underwent a well-established implicit emotion processing paradigm (face matching task [27]) and a Flanker/Go-NoGo task [28] during fMRI. All participants provided written informed consent for study protocols approved by the Ethics committees of the Medical Faculty of Mannheim at the Ruprecht-Karls-University in Heidelberg, the Medical Faculty of the University of Bonn, and the Charité University Medicine in Berlin. General exclusion criteria for the healthy controls were a lifetime history of significant general medical, psychiatric, or neurological disorders, a family history of psychiatric disorders, current or past psychotropic pharmacological treatment, drug or alcohol use as well as head trauma (compare also, refs. [19, 26]). Detailed information about the task procedures for the Face-matching and Flanker/Go-NoGo task can be found in the Supplementary Methods.
fMRI
Functional MRI data were acquired on three comparable 3T TrioTim MRI scanners (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) in Mannheim, Bonn, and Berlin using a gradient-recalled echo-planar imaging sequence (GRE-EPI) with the following MR parameters: 28 axial slices per volume, 4 mm slice thickness, 1 mm gap, TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, field of view (FOV) = 192 mm, flip angle = 80°, acquired in descending order. We acquired 135 volumes for the face matching task and 306 volumes for the Flanker/Go-Nogo task. Additionally, high-resolution T1 structural data were acquired using a 3D magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo (MP-RAGE) sequence with the following sequence parameters: 176 sagittal slices, 1 mm slice thickness, TR = 1570 ms, TE = 2.75 ms, TI = 800 ms, FOV = 256 mm, flip angle = 15°. Preprocessing and estimation of functional task-dependent brain activation at the subject level were carried out using the MATLAB-based statistical parametric mapping software (version SPM8, Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London, UK, http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/). Functional images were preprocessed for each participant. fMRI data were slice time corrected, realigned to the first image of the time series, spatially normalised to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template, resampled to 3 mm isotropic voxels, and smoothed with a 9 mm full-width at half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian filter. Second-level analyses testing for genotype effects across participants were carried out using SPM12.
Individual brain activation maps were subjected to separate 3 (rs6500744 genotype: C/C, C/T, T/T carrier) x 2 (sex: men, women) full-factorial models including age and imaging site as regressors of no interest using SPM12 to test for the effects of rs6500744 on brain responses during response inhibition (“nogo > neutral” contrast), conflict monitoring (“incongruent > congruent” contrast), overall executive functioning (combined contrast: [nogo & incongruent] > [neutral & congruent]), and implicit emotion processing (“faces > forms”) for second level fMRI analyses. Sex was included as a between-subject factor into the full-factorial model to identify potential genotype x sex interactions in imaging space due to significant main effects of sex and sex by genotype interactions on behavioral performance (see supplementary Table 8) for both tasks and previously reported sex by genotype effects for comparable intermediate phenotypes for genetic variation of the MAOA gene [28]. We additionally included behavioural performance corresponding to the respective fMRI contrast (“faces > forms”; [incongruent & nogo] > [congruent & neutral]) as a regressor of no interest into second-level analyses to control for genotype effects on behavioral performance (i.e., face matching: accuracy faces—accuracy forms, Flanker Go-NoGo: accuracy [incongruent & nogo]—accuracy [congruent & neutral]). Given that altered ACC functioning during executive functioning measured with the Flanker/Go-NoGo task, during implicit emotion processing measured with the face matching task, and during fear associative learning measured with fear conditioning has previously been associated with different psychiatric risk genotypes (e.g., MAOA, 5-HTTLPR, BDNF Val66MET) [18, 19, 28], we tested genotype effects in an a priori defined standard anatomical mask of the ACC derived from the Automated Anatomical Labeling (AAL) atlas [29]. The significance level was set to P < 0.05 family-wise error (FWE) corrected for multiple comparisons across all voxels within the ACC AAL mask. For fear conditioning and extinction tasks, we additionally conducted ROI analyses (P < 0.001 at voxel level) in the amygdala (also derived from the AAL atlas) due to its high relevance. Outside this pre-hypothesised ROI, findings were considered significant if they passed a significance threshold of P < 0.05 FWE corrected for multiple comparisons across the whole brain.
Fear conditioning and extinction
Sample
We included the data of 47 patients who have participated in the fMRI and genetic subprojects of the MAC multicenter psychotherapy study, which recruited in total 369 patients of European descent meeting DSM-IV (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition) criteria for PD/AG, as assessed by the Composite International Diagnostic Interview. The CONSORTs and overview of the number of participants for every substudy (genetic data, psychophysiological assessment, and fMRI) are published in ref. [30]. The study with all of its subprojects was approved by the respective local ethical committees and informed consent was obtained. Among the 47 patients with valid fMRI data sets and sequenced RBFOX1 SNP rs6500744, the distribution of C/C, C/T, and T/T genotypes is 15, 21, and 11, respectively. Despite the restricted sample size of our clinical sample (N = 47), subgroup comparisons with groups of 10 to 20 subjects are expected to be robust with such a specific functional MRI task (e.g., ref. [31]). Especially group average reproducibility has been shown to be high, starting from a sample size of N = 10 (see Supplementary Fig. 17 in Marek et al. [32]). Detailed information on the task procedure of the fear conditioning task can be found in the Supplementary Methods.
Behavioural avoidance task
The BAT (behavioural avoidance task) assessment was part of two study waves of the German national research network PANIC-NET. Genotypes for rs6500744 were available for a total of 333 participating patients (n(C/C) = 119; n(C/T) = 156; n(T/T) = 58) with a primary DSM-IV-TR diagnosis of panic disorder with agoraphobia with at least moderate disorder severity (238 females; mean age: m = 35.50 years, SD = 10.72; no significant differences between genotype groups). Criteria of inclusion and exclusion and patient recruitment procedure are described in detail elsewhere [33]. Diagnosis of PD/AG was established by a standardised computer-administered face-to-face Computer Assisted Personal Interview-World Health Organization-Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CAPI-WHO-CIDI) by trained and certified interviewers. All patients were free from psychotropic medication. Patients gave written informed consent after receiving a detailed description of the study. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Medical Faculty of the Technical University of Dresden, which was valid for all participating centres. The highly standardised BAT procedure is described in detail elsewhere [17] and in the Supplementary Methods.
Animals
Male Rbfox1fl/fl (Rbfox1tm1.1Dblk/J; JAX strain 014089) and Synapsin1-Cre (B6.Cg-Tg(Syn1-cre)671Jxm/J; JAX strain 003966) mice were maintained on a C57Bl/6J background and housed in groups of 2–5 in standard individually ventilated cages on a 12 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 7:00) under controlled ambient conditions (21 ± 1 °C, 55 ± 5% humidity). Food and water were available ad libitum unless specified otherwise. To generate mice with neuron-specific deletion of Rbfox1 (Rbfox1-KO), Rbfox1fl/fl mice were crossed to mice carrying Cre-recombinase under the direction of the rat Synapsin I promoter (Synapsin1-Cre). The resulting heterozygous Rbfox1fl/+/Synapsin1-Cre+/− (HET) female mice were crossed to Rbfox1fl/fl (CTRL) males to produce homozygous Rbfox1fl/fl/Synapsin1-Cre+/− (KO) offspring. Rbfox1fl/fl/ and Rbfox1fl/+/Synapsin-Cre−/− mice were used as controls. While we cannot fully rule out the possibility that such breeding scheme results in Rbfox1 deletion in non-neuronal cells (e.g., in the glial cells), the genetic make-up of this transgenic mouse line and the stability of Cre-expression being limited to neurons [34] makes this very unlikely. Male C57Bl/6J mice were used as social stimuli in the social interaction test and intruders in the aggression testing paradigm. All breeding and experimental procedures were conducted in accordance with the Directive of the European Communities Council of 24 November 1986 (86/609/EEC) and German animal welfare laws (TierSchG and TSchV) and were approved by the Darmstadt regional council (approval ID: FK/1126).
Quantitative PCR (qPCR)
Tissue punches (1 mm, n = 12–18 samples per group) from different brain regions (Nucleus accumbens, striatum, ACC, septum, PVN, hippocampus, amygdala, thalamus) were isolated from coronal sections (250 µm thickness) made at −22 °C using a cryostat (Leica CM 3050 S) of brains from three Rbfox1-KO and four CTRL mice and stored at −80 °C. RNA was isolated using the RNeasy Plus Micro Kit (Qiagen) according to the manufacturer’s instructions. Further information on the qPCR procedure can be found in the Supplementary Methods. Expression data were calculated relative to the expression of Sdha, which was selected as the most stable of the four reference genes by analysis with Normfinder or Syn1, to normalize against a neuronal marker. Data were calculated relative to the average of the CTRL group and converted to log2.
Mouse behavioural experiments
The experiments were conducted in three cohorts, and the behavioural batteries for each cohort are described in Supplementary Fig. 6. For habituation purposes, mice were transported to the behavioural testing room at least 45 min before testing. Experiments were performed between 9:00–14:00, with animals tested in randomised order by an experimenter blinded to the genotype (however, full blinding was not possible due to the robust phenotype of the KO mice). Behavioural apparatuses were cleaned before testing and between animals using Aerodesin 2000 (Lysoform Dr Hans Rosemann GmbH, Berlin, Germany). Detailed information on the mouse behavioural tests, including the open field (OF) test and novel object investigation, light-dark box (LDB) test, touchscreen pairwise visual discrimination task, spontaneous alternation task, pre-pulse inhibition (PPI) of the acoustic startle reflex, cued fear conditioning and extinction test, resident-intruder test, social interaction test, and marble burying test, can be found in the Supplementary Methods. Mouse behavioural data were analysed using GraphPad Prism 8.0 (GraphPad Software, San Diego, USA) and Jamovi (Version 2.2.5.0, Sydney, Australia). Unless described otherwise, data were analysed using t-tests, one- or two-way ANOVA, with repeated measures where appropriate, followed by Bonferroni post hoc tests. In case ANOVA assumptions of variance homogeneity and/or normality were violated, non-parametric tests (Welch’s t-test, Mann-Whitney U-test, Friedman’s test) were used. All values are presented as mean ± S.E.M.
Results
Genome-wide and gene-level associations between RBFOX1 and psychiatric disorders
Genome-wide associations between single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in RBFOX1 and major depressive disorder (MDD; 38 SNPs), risk tolerance (RT; 4 SNPs), and the cross-disorder meta-analysis (CD-MA; 42 SNPs) were found in these studies (Supplementary Table 1 for sample description, data in Supplementary Table 2). At the gene level, RBFOX1 was found to be associated with several psychiatric conditions, obtaining again gene-wide significance for MDD (p = 8.62e-17), RT (p = 5.6e-12), and CD-MA (p = 1.2e-10), but also for schizophrenia (SCZ; p = 7.2e-08) (Fig. 1A). Interestingly, genes associated with these disorders were significantly enriched for RBFOX1 targets (MDD, p = 0.016; SCZ, p = 0.042; RT, p = 0.010; CD-MA, p = 0.019) (Supplementary Table 3), as it was previously shown for aggression (p = 3.4e-05) [35]. In line with these findings, significant associations in RBFOX1 with other psychiatric traits or disorders, such as neuroticism, depressive symptoms, alcohol dependence, fed-up feelings or well-being spectrum, were also found using PheWAS (https://atlas.ctglab.nl/PheWAS; search terms: “RBFOX1”, domain: “psychiatric”, results sorted by P-value). The above evidence, therefore, highlights RBFOX1 as a robust, replicated cross-disorder risk gene with pleiotropic effects.
Copy number variations spanning RBFOX1 in patients with psychiatric conditions
Next, we browsed CNVs spanning RBFOX1 reported in patients with psychiatric conditions, identifying CNVs for six disorders/traits (in total 124 losses and 34 gains). The vast majority of CNVs were found in patients with ASD (112 CNVs), and in patients with SCZ (24 CNVs) (Fig. 1B, Supplementary Fig. 1 and Supplementary Tables 4 and 5), probably due to the larger number of CNV studies for these disorders. Across all six disorders, CNVs appeared to be 2.3 times more frequent in cases than in controls, with significant enrichment in ASD (ratio = 5:1). A significantly increased burden of CNVs was observed across studies of ADHD and ASD (Supplementary Table 5). Most of these CNVs probably affect RBFOX1 function in distinct ways; while half of them span particular exons or even the whole gene, affecting the coding sequence, many CNVs in introns overlap regions with transcription factors binding activity (such as gains in intron 2 and losses in intron 3) containing putative regulatory elements (Fig. 1B, Supplementary Figs. 1, 2, and Supplementary Tables 4, 6) and potentially altering RBFOX1 expression. In line with this strong evidence that genetically driven variation of RBFOX1 expression is associated with mental disorders, we synthesised knowledge of brain expression of RBFOX1 from existing studies on post-mortem brain samples. Reanalysis of data from those studies showed significantly decreased RBFOX1 mRNA levels in frontal and temporal cortices of ASD patients and prefrontal cortices of SCZ patients (Supplementary Table 7). These brain regions converge with those where the expression of RBFOX1 is highest (Supplementary Figs. 3, 4). Taken together, common genetic variation in RBFOX1 is robustly associated with a variety of mental disorders and behavioural traits, while rare genetic variation and reduced brain expression appear most strongly linked to neurodevelopmental disorders with onset in childhood and adolescence.
The effects of RBFOX1 rs6500744 on neural activation during emotion processing and executive functioning
Given the role of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) in integrating cognition with emotion [36–38], its link with mental disorders [39], and the high level of RBFOX1 expression in this brain area (Supplementary Fig. 3), we assessed the effects of rs6500744 on dorsal ACC (dACC) activation during implicit emotion processing and executive functioning. Region-of-interest (ROI)-analyses in 313 healthy volunteers showed an increased response of the dACC for matching fearful as well as angry faces (compared to matching geometric forms) for C-allele carriers compared to T/T carriers (Fig. 2A), suggesting increased reactivity to emotional stimuli in the target brain area. At a stringent whole-brain significance threshold, no other brain area showed a significant genotype effect during implicit emotion processing (peak-voxel family-wise error-corrected p < 0.05). In 324 healthy controls, ROI analyses did not reveal any significant effect of rs6500744 on dACC activation during executive functioning as measured with the Flanker/Go-NoGo task. However, whole-brain analyses revealed that C-allele carriers compared to T/T carriers showed a reduced left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dLPFC) response during cognitive control (contrast [incongruent & nogo] < [congruent & neutral]) (Fig. 2B). This reduced dLPFC activation during executive functioning suggests less efficient prefrontal processing during cognitive and impulse control [40, 41] that might contribute to increased impulsivity. Altered brain activation during implicit emotion processing and executive functioning as influenced by the effects of RBFOX1 genotype may therefore underlie the increased risk for mental disorders characterised by increased emotional reactivity (e.g., MDD), impaired impulse control (e.g., ADHD, ASD, risk tolerance), and aggression, all of which are associated with RBFOX1.
The effects of RBFOX1 rs6500744 on neural activation during fear conditioning
In an independent dataset, we tested whether rs6500744 influences the neural activation in the dACC and amygdala during fear conditioning (Fig. 2C) in a sample of 47 panic disorder and agoraphobia (PD/AG) patients, again using fMRI. Compared to T/T carriers, C/C carriers revealed a significant activation enhancement in the dACC (Fig. 2C and Supplementary Table 9) for simple fear learning and a significant reduction of activation in the dACC for CS+ after fear extinction (Fig. 2C and Supplementary Table 9). ROI analyses with a threshold of p < 0.001 within the amygdala did not find any significant genotype differences. Since the dACC is crucial for fear appraisal [42] and expression [43–45], our findings suggest that rs6500744 C/C genotype carriers display enhanced fear expression after fear conditioning and more fear reduction after extinction training compared to T-allele carriers, which fail to demonstrate fear conditioning- and extinction-related changes in neural processing. Notably, the patients with the C/C genotype also had significantly increased depression (BDI-II) and anxiety (ASI) scores compared to T-allele carriers (Supplementary Tables 10 and 11).
The effects of RBFOX1 rs6500744 on fear behaviour in panic disorder/agoraphobia patients
To further investigate the effect of rs6500744 on fear behaviour, we examined its effect on avoidance during the BAT, where a behavioural and autonomous response to a fear-inducing situation is measured, in 333 PD/AG patients (Table 1). The rs6500744 C-allele was significantly and dose-dependently associated with a high frequency of avoidance behaviour (linear trend: p = 0.022; Table 1). This result was concurrent with observed differences between genotypes according to everyday life avoidance behaviour, assessed by clinical expert ratings (Clinical Global Index): again, avoidance increased linearly with the number of C-alleles (linear trend: p = 0.04, Table 1). In the 106 BAT non-avoiding patients who reported at least moderate fear during the task, the heart rate during both the anticipation and exposure phase was significantly increased relative to the recovery phase with an increasing number of C-alleles (linear trend BAT phase × genotype: p = 0.031, Supplementary Table 12) indicating increased autonomic threat processing. Importantly, T/T allele homozygotes did not show any heart rate modulation during the BAT. Together with the fMRI data, this suggests that rs6500744 C-allele carriers show more avoidance behaviour due to better fear learning and improved stimulus discrimination.
Table 1.
C/C (n = 119) | C/T (n = 156) | T/T (n = 58) | F/Chi2 | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Age in years | 36.00 ± 10.44 | 35.46 ± 10.93 | 34.57 ± 10.80 | 0.35 |
Female gender | 87 (73%) | 107 (69%) | 44 (76%) | 1.34 |
Years of education | 2.04 | |||
≤8 | 18 | 19 | 8 | |
9─11 | 67 | 87 | 28 | |
≥12 | 34 | 50 | 22 | |
CGI total | 5.04 ± 0.74 | 5.15 ± 0.80 | 5.03 ± 0.90 | 0.75 |
CGI avoidance | 4.65 ± 1.03 | 4.59 ± 1.10 | 4.31 ± 1.29 | 2.13 |
SIGH-A | 23.34 ± 7.44 | 22.54 ± 7.68 | 22.19 ± 8.19 | 0.56 |
MI alone | 2.91 ± 0.84 | 2.92 ± 0.85 | 2.72 ± 0.86 | 1.13 |
PAS | 27.19 ± 9.72 | 26.76 ± 9.59 | 25.17 ± 10.80 | 0.84 |
ASI | 32.82 ± 11.19 | 30.03 ± 11.63 | 30.14 ± 10.34 | 2.29 |
BDI-II | 16.71 ± 9.17 | 15.55 ± 7.56 | 14.66 ± 8.93 | 1.31 |
Patients showing avoidance behaviour during the BAT | 44 (37%) | 39 (25%) | 13 (22%) | 6.13 |
ASI Anxiety Sensitivity Index, BDI-II Beck Depression Inventory-II, CGI Clinical Global Impression, MI Mobility Inventory, PAS Panic and Agoraphobia-Scale, SIGH-A Structured Interview Guide for the Hamilton Anxiety Scale. Due to missing values MI alone was available in 312 patients only (C/C: 108; C/T: 149; T/T: 55). CGI and SIGH-A scores were not available for one patient with C/C genotype. ASI score was not available for one patient with C/T genotype.
Behavioural effects of neuron-specific deletion of Rbfox1 in mice
Neuronal-specific deletion of Rbfox1 resulted in a pronounced downregulation of Rbfox1 relative to the neuronal marker Synapsin 1 compared to more moderate downregulation relative to the housekeeping gene Sdha (i.e., also normalizing expression relative to all non-neuronal cells) without concomitant compensatory changes in either Rbfox2 or Rbfox3 (Supplementary Fig. 5A), and a reduction in body weight (Supplementary Fig. 5B). We observed persistent and pronounced hyperactivity in the KO mice in the open field and light-dark box (Fig. 3A–C), and marble burying tests (Supplementary Fig. 5C) compared to CTRL and HET. Interestingly, the hyperactivity was coupled with thigmotaxis, as the KO spent twice as much time as CTRL and HET moving adjacent to the maze walls (Supplementary Fig. 5D). This behaviour confounded the typical measures of anxiety in these tests. However, when a subset of the mice was tested again in the open field at the age of 8–9 months and was exposed to a novel object placed in the centre of the open field, the KOs spent three times longer than CTRL investigating it (Fig. 3B), suggesting an increase in the exploratory drive and the persistence of the hyperactive phenotype with age. In the pre-pulse inhibition test, conducted to assess the sensorimotor gating of startle response [46], KO mice showed a deficit in the acoustic startle response (Fig. 3D), but their ability to suppress their startle reflex when the startling stimulus was preceded by a sub-threshold pre-pulse stimulus (pre-pulse inhibition) was not impaired.
Next, we moved to more cognitively demanding tests. Here, we found that HET and KO mice had impairments in both fear acquisition and extinction in the auditory cued fear conditioning and extinction test (Fig. 3E). Compared to KO, HET mice were able to acquire fear conditioning like littermate controls but were unable to retain fear memory as evident from their reduced freezing in the extinction stage. Although it has been reported that some proposed rodent autism models show enhanced fear conditioning and impaired extinction [47], several transgenic mouse models for ASD display deficits in fear conditioning or no change from controls [48]. The deficit observed in our study was specific for cued fear learning, as neither associative learning in a touchscreen pairwise visual discrimination task (Fig. 3F; Supplementary Fig. 5E) nor spatial working memory (spontaneous alternations in the Y-maze) were impaired (Fig. 3G).
Finally, given the genetic association of RBFOX1 with ASD and previous reports of a role in aggression, we assessed social interaction as well as male-male aggression. We observed significantly less social interest in the KO mice (Fig. 3H), which also manifested in a complete lack of aggressive behaviour (Fig. 3I). Thus, neuron-specific Rbfox1 depletion in mice leads to hallmark features of ASD: repetitive-stereotyped and hyperactive locomotor behaviour, abnormalities in the fear circuitry, and impaired social interactions [49]. Such pronounced effects of neuron-specific loss of Rbfox1 might thus also occur in human carriers of rare loss-of-function variants with high penetrance, underscoring the relevance of this gene for neurodevelopment.
Discussion
The evidence from the genetic studies accrued here suggests that common genetic variation in RBFOX1 goes along with a wide spectrum of psychiatric phenotypes, while rare CNVs in this gene contribute especially to ASD and SCZ, although this might be biased by the low number of studies investigating CNVs in other psychiatric disorders. The molecular-cellular effects of common genetic variation in RBFOX1 are however yet elusive and likely include the regulation of gene expression. This may be operative only in certain cell types or developmental stages, as some of the major roles of RBFOX1 occur during early brain maturation [50], where it orchestrates downstream genetic networks implicated in neuronal development [51] via direct regulation of post-transcriptional programs. These gene networks are markedly inter-connected and enriched for genes relevant for cortical development and ASD [15] as well as MDD and SCZ susceptibility (Supplementary Table 3). On target transcripts, RBFOX1 regulates alternative splicing of tissue-specific exons [52] by binding to mRNA GCAUG motifs in the nucleus and affecting mRNA stability in the cytosol and thus has different roles in those intracellular compartments. Importantly, RBFOX1 promotes interneuron-specific connectivity in the developing neocortex [16] by regulating cell-type-specific splicing (parvalbumin [PV] vs. somatostatin [SST] interneurons). Loss of RBFOX1 in inhibitory interneurons causes significantly reduced synaptic transmission [53], by affecting membrane excitation and neurotransmission [54], resulting in reduced inhibition of the postsynaptic neuron and leading to excitatory/inhibitory (E/I) imbalance, a key feature of ASD. As PV + interneurons are regulators of E/I balance [55], this might link dysregulation of RBFOX1 to E/I dysbalance and ASD susceptibility.
With respect to common genetic variation, RBFOX1 is associated with all disorders combined, SCZ, MDD, and RT. Our neuroimaging data argue for an effect of RBFOX1 genetic variation on the networks controlling emotional-associative learning, executive functioning, and emotional processing. Although our fMRI samples were relatively small and replications are necessary, we showed that rs6500744 risk genotype carriers display higher reactivity to emotional stimuli and reduced DLPFC activation during cognitive control, which are both linked to these mental disorders. Increased aggression found in C-allele carriers [14] is thus likely to be interpreted as reactive-impulsive, but not proactive, aggression. It must be considered that genetic variants in RBFOX1 with small effect sizes in a polygenic scenario interact with many other variants to increase the risk towards mental disorders in a quasi-stochastic manner, probably explaining the broad psychopathological phenotype. In contrast, more penetrant CNVs with presumably stronger molecular effects may result in a more specific chronic neurodevelopmental behavioural syndrome.
While we cannot yet finally determine the functional consequences of RBFOX1 genetic variation in humans, combining data from human and rodent experiments may suggest an increase in expression in MDD, anxiety, and (reactive) aggression. Up- and downregulation of RBFOX1 however are likely to have different effects on the regulated gene networks [56], and human post-mortem data argues for reduced RBFOX1 expression at least in ASD and SCZ. In line with this hypothesis, the remarkable behavioural phenotype of neuron-specific Rbfox1 knockout mice suggests that loss-of-function of RBFOX1 causes a behavioural syndrome characterised by hyperactivity, stereotypies, and specific cognitive and social impairments typical for ASD. A limitation of our study is that we only conducted behavioural experiments in male mice, but the findings will be extended to female animals in the future. As for the clinical phenotype in human RBFOX1 CNV carriers, extending beyond “pure” ASD, we proposed that it is additionally shaped by genetic background and environmental factors. Given that our Rbfox1 KO mouse line shows both high construct and face validity of ASD—as it is characterized by downregulation of Rbfox1 in the brain and displays several ASD-related behaviours, we consider it an excellent animal model for ASD with an unprecedentedly robust behavioural phenotype.
Differential consequences of common and rare genetic variation, as we observe them for RBFOX1, may be a general principle in psychiatric genetics, where common variation in a gene might underly more generalized vulnerability, while rare, highly penetrant variation causes more specific phenotypes. In either case, it becomes clear that current diagnostic boundaries do not adequately reflect corresponding biological disease types. Given that approaches to modify RBFOX1 expression are already at hand, which might be used in the sense of personalised mental health, this calls for mechanistic rather than theoretical, operationalised definitions of mental disorders.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank Jürgen Deckert (University of Würzburg, Germany) and Sylvia Helbig-Lang (University of Hamburg, Germany) for their contribution to the PANIC-Net study and Maximillian Rechberg for his assistance with the animal experiments. This project received funding from the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme for research, technological development and demonstration under grant agreement No. 602805 (Aggressotype) (AR, BF, FF, AOL), European Union H2020 Programme [H2020/2014-2020] under grant agreement No. 643051 (MiND) (AR, BF), German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (Project No. 01GV0615) Mechanisms of Action in CBT (MAC) (ALG, AOH, GWA, TK, AS, TL), Heidelberg University (Olympia-Morata Program) and the German Academic Exchange Service (GG), Spanish ‘Ministerio de Ciencia, Innovación y Universidades’ (RTI2018-100968-B-100) (BC), ‘AGAUR/Generalitat de Catalunya’ (2017-SGR-738) (BC), ECNP network “ADHD across the lifespan” (NFC, BC, BF, AR), the European Union H2020 Program [H2020/2014-2020] under grant agreement n° 667302 (CoCA) (NFC, BC, BF, AR), the European Union H2020 Program [H2020/2014-2020] under grant agreement n°728018 (Eat2beNICE) (NFC, JCD, BC, BF, AR), ‘Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Enfermedades Raras’ (CIBERER) (NFC), German Ministry for Education and Research (BMBF) grants NGFNplus MooDS (01 GS 08148) (NRS), PROTECT-AD (BS, TK) and PANIC-NET (TK), the Hessisches Ministerium für Wissenschaft und Kunst (HMWK; project ‘The Adaptive Mind’; TK, BS, YY) and Estonian Research Council (PRG1213) (AOL).
Author contributions
Conceptualization: AR, NFC, EAG, JCD, DB, BC, AH, AH, AML, AS, ALG, BF, CPF, ES, HT, HW, JR, JIS, KS, LF, MN, NRS, SW, TF, TL, TK, UL, VA, AOL, DAS, FF; Formal analysis: NFC, EAG, JCD, DB, BC, AY, BC, BS, DAS, EAG, ES, FF, GG, HT, HW, JR, JCD, JC, MR, NRM, NFC, RM, TMK, YY; Investigation: AH, AH, JR, JIS, KS, LG, NRS, TK, AOL, DAS, FF; Writing—original draft: AOL, AR; Writing—review & editing: all authors.
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
Competing interests
BF has received educational speaking fees from Medice. AR has served on advisory boards and/or received speaker’s honoraria from Medice, Shire/Takeda, Janssen, SAGE/Biogen, Boehringer Ingelheim, and Cyclerion. All other authors declare no conflicts of interest.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: Aet O’Leary, Noèlia Fernàndez-Castillo, Gabriela Gan, Yunbo Yang.
Supplementary information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41380-022-01722-4.
References
- 1.Manolio TA, Collins FS, Cox NJ, Goldstein DB, Hindorff LA, Hunter DJ, et al. Finding the missing heritability of complex diseases. Nature. 2009;461:747–53. doi: 10.1038/nature08494. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Lee PH, Anttila V, Won H, Feng Y-CA, Rosenthal J, Zhu Z, et al. Genomic relationships, Novel Loci, and Pleiotropic mechanisms across eight psychiatric disorders. Cell. 2019;179:1469–1482.e1411. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2019.11.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Sullivan PF, Agrawal A, Bulik CM, Andreassen OA, Borglum AD, Breen G, et al. Psychiatric genomics: an update and an agenda. Am J Psychiatry. 2018;175:15–27. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.17030283. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Coleman JRI, Gaspar HA, Bryois J, Breen G. The genetics of the mood disorder spectrum: genome-wide association analyses of more than 185,000 cases and 439,000 controls. Biol Psychiatry. 2020;88:169–84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 5.Davies MN, Verdi S, Burri A, Trzaskowski M, Lee M, Hettema JM, et al. Generalised anxiety disorder-a twin study of genetic architecture, genome-wide association and differential gene expression. PloS One. 2015;10:e0134865. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0134865. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Wray NR, Ripke S, Mattheisen M, Trzaskowski M, Byrne EM, Abdellaoui A, et al. Genome-wide association analyses identify 44 risk variants and refine the genetic architecture of major depression. Nat Genet. 2018;50:668–81. doi: 10.1038/s41588-018-0090-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Nagel M, Jansen PR, Stringer S, Watanabe K, de Leeuw CA, Bryois J, et al. Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies for neuroticism in 449,484 individuals identifies novel genetic loci and pathways. Nat Genet. 2018;50:920–7. doi: 10.1038/s41588-018-0151-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Bacchelli E, Cameli C, Viggiano M, Igliozzi R, Mancini A, Tancredi R, et al. An integrated analysis of rare CNV and exome variation in Autism Spectrum Disorder using the Infinium PsychArray. Sci Rep. 2020;10:3198. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-59922-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Turner TN, Hormozdiari F, Duyzend MH, McClymont SA, Hook PW, Iossifov I, et al. Genome sequencing of autism-affected families reveals disruption of putative noncoding regulatory DNA. Am J Hum Genet. 2016;98:58–74. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2015.11.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Griswold AJ, Dueker ND, Van Booven D, Rantus JA, Jaworski JM, Slifer SH, et al. Targeted massively parallel sequencing of autism spectrum disorder-associated genes in a case-control cohort reveals rare loss-of-function risk variants. Mol Autism. 2015;6:43. doi: 10.1186/s13229-015-0034-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kanduri C, Kantojarvi K, Salo PM, Vanhala R, Buck G, Blancher C, et al. The landscape of copy number variations in Finnish families with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Res: Off J Int Soc Autism Res. 2016;9:9–16. doi: 10.1002/aur.1502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Zhao WW. Intragenic deletion of RBFOX1 associated with neurodevelopmental/neuropsychiatric disorders and possibly other clinical presentations. Mol Cytogenet. 2013;6:26. doi: 10.1186/1755-8166-6-26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Harich B, van der Voet M, Klein M, Čížek P, Fenckova M, Schenck A, et al. From rare copy number variants to biological processes in ADHD. Am J psychiatry. 2020;177:855–66. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.19090923. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Fernandez-Castillo N, Gan G, van Donkelaar MMJ, Vaht M, Weber H, Retz W, et al. RBFOX1, encoding a splicing regulator, is a candidate gene for aggressive behavior. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2020;30:44–55. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2017.11.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Lee JA, Damianov A, Lin CH, Fontes M, Parikshak NN, Anderson ES, et al. Cytoplasmic Rbfox1 regulates the expression of synaptic and autism-related genes. Neuron. 2016;89:113–28. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Wamsley B, Jaglin XH, Favuzzi E, Quattrocolo G, Nigro MJ, Yusuf N, et al. Rbfox1 mediates cell-type-specific splicing in cortical interneurons. Neuron. 2018;100:846–859.e847. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.09.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Richter J, Hamm AO, Pané-Farré CA, Gerlach AL, Gloster AT, Wittchen HU, et al. Dynamics of defensive reactivity in patients with panic disorder and agoraphobia: implications for the etiology of panic disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;72:512–20. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.03.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Pezawas L, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Drabant EM, Verchinski BA, Munoz KE, Kolachana BS, et al. 5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-amygdala interactions: a genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:828–34. doi: 10.1038/nn1463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Schweiger JI, Bilek E, Schafer A, Braun U, Moessnang C, Harneit A, et al. Effects of BDNF Val(66)Met genotype and schizophrenia familial risk on a neural functional network for cognitive control in humans. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2019;44:590–7. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0248-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Tost H, Alam T, Geramita M, Rebsch C, Kolachana B, Dickinson D, et al. Effects of the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism on white matter microstructure in healthy adults. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013;38:525–32. doi: 10.1038/npp.2012.214. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Meyer-Lindenberg A, Kohn PD, Kolachana B, Kippenhan S, McInerney-Leo A, Nussbaum R, et al. Midbrain dopamine and prefrontal function in humans: interaction and modulation by COMT genotype. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:594–6. doi: 10.1038/nn1438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Braun U, Schafer A, Bassett DS, Rausch F, Schweiger JI, Bilek E, et al. Dynamic brain network reconfiguration as a potential schizophrenia genetic risk mechanism modulated by NMDA receptor function. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2016;113:12568–73. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1608819113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Schneider M, Walter H, Moessnang C, Schafer A, Erk S, Mohnke S, et al. Altered DLPFC-Hippocampus connectivity during working memory: independent replication and disorder specificity of a putative genetic risk phenotype for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2017;43:1114–22. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Cao H, Bertolino A, Walter H, Schneider M, Schafer A, Taurisano P, et al. Altered functional subnetwork during emotional face processing: a potential intermediate phenotype for schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry. 2016;73:598–605. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.0161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Grimm O, Heinz A, Walter H, Kirsch P, Erk S, Haddad L, et al. Striatal response to reward anticipation: evidence for a systems-level intermediate phenotype for schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry. 2014;71:531–9. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Harneit A, Braun U, Geiger LS, Zang Z, Hakobjan M, van Donkelaar MMJ, et al. MAOA-VNTR genotype affects structural and functional connectivity in distributed brain networks. Hum Brain Mapp. 2019;40:5202–12. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Hariri AR, Tessitore A, Mattay VS, Fera F, Weinberger DR. The amygdala response to emotional stimuli: a comparison of faces and scenes. Neuroimage. 2002;17:317–23. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Meyer-Lindenberg A, Buckholtz JW, Kolachana B, Hariri AR, Pezawas L, Blasi G, et al. Neural mechanisms of genetic risk for impulsivity and violence in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006;103:6269–74. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0511311103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage. 2002;15:273–89. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Reif A, Richter J, Straube B, Höfler M, Lueken U, Gloster AT, et al. MAOA and mechanisms of panic disorder revisited: from bench to molecular psychotherapy. Mol Psychiatry. 2014;19:122–8. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Ridderbusch IC, Wroblewski A, Yang Y, Richter J, Hollandt M, Hamm AO, et al. Neural adaptation of cingulate and insular activity during delayed fear extinction: A replicable pattern across assessment sites and repeated measurements. NeuroImage. 2021;237:118157. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Marek S, Tervo-Clemmens B, Calabro FJ, Montez DF, Kay BP, Hatoum AS, et al. Reproducible brain-wide association studies require thousands of individuals. Nature. 2022;603:654–60. doi: 10.1038/s41586-022-04492-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Yang Y, Lueken U, Richter J, Hamm A, Wittmann A, Konrad C, et al. Effect of CBT on biased semantic network in panic disorder: a multicenter fMRI study using semantic priming. Am J Psychiatry. 2020;177:254–64. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.19020202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Zhu Y, Romero MI, Ghosh P, Ye Z, Charnay P, Rushing EJ, et al. Ablation of NF1 function in neurons induces abnormal development of cerebral cortex and reactive gliosis in the brain. Genes Dev. 2001;15:859–76. doi: 10.1101/gad.862101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Zhang-James Y, Fernàndez-Castillo N, Hess JL, Malki K, Glatt SJ, Cormand B, et al. An integrated analysis of genes and functional pathways for aggression in human and rodent models. Mol Psychiatry. 2019;24:1655–67. doi: 10.1038/s41380-018-0068-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Vogt BA. Pain and emotion interactions in subregions of the cingulate gyrus. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6:533–44. doi: 10.1038/nrn1704. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Laird AR, Fox PM, Eickhoff SB, Turner JA, Ray KL, McKay DR, et al. Behavioral interpretations of intrinsic connectivity networks. J Cogn Neurosci. 2011;23:4022–37. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Fullana MA, Harrison BJ, Soriano-Mas C, Vervliet B, Cardoner N, Àvila-Parcet A, et al. Neural signatures of human fear conditioning: an updated and extended meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Mol Psychiatry. 2016;21:500–8. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Yucel M, Wood SJ, Fornito A, Riffkin J, Velakoulis D, Pantelis C. Anterior cingulate dysfunction: implications for psychiatric disorders? J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2003;28:350–4. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Badre D. Cognitive control, hierarchy, and the rostro-caudal organization of the frontal lobes. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008;12:193–200. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Swick D, Ashley V, Turken AU. Left inferior frontal gyrus is critical for response inhibition. BMC Neurosci. 2008;9:102. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-9-102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Etkin A, Egner T, Kalisch R. Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15:85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Milad MR, Quirk GJ, Pitman RK, Orr SP, Fischl B, Rauch SL. A role for the human dorsal anterior cingulate cortex in fear expression. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:1191–4. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.04.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Milad MR, Quirk GJ. Fear extinction as a model for translational neuroscience: ten years of progress. Annu Rev Psychol. 2012;63:129–51. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131631. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Sehlmeyer C, Schöning S, Zwitserlood P, Pfleiderer B, Kircher T, Arolt V, et al. Human fear conditioning and extinction in neuroimaging: a systematic review. PloS One. 2009;4:e5865. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005865. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Swerdlow NR, Caine SB, Braff DL, Geyer MA. The neural substrates of sensorimotor gating of the startle reflex: a review of recent findings and their implications. J Psychopharmacol. 1992;6:176–90. doi: 10.1177/026988119200600210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Markram K, Rinaldi T, La Mendola D, Sandi C, Markram H. Abnormal fear conditioning and amygdala processing in an animal model of autism. Neuropsychopharmacol: Off Publ Am Coll Neuropsychopharmacol. 2008;33:901–12. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Ey E, Leblond CS, Bourgeron T. Behavioral profiles of mouse models for autism spectrum disorders. Autism Res: Off J Int Soc Autism Res. 2011;4:5–16. doi: 10.1002/aur.175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Silverman JL, Yang M, Lord C, Crawley JN. Behavioural phenotyping assays for mouse models of autism. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11:490–502. doi: 10.1038/nrn2851. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Jacko M, Weyn-Vanhentenryck SM, Smerdon JW, Yan R, Feng H, Williams DJ, et al. Rbfox Splicing Factors Promote Neuronal Maturation and Axon Initial Segment Assembly. Neuron. 2018;97:853–868 e856. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.01.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Fogel BL, Wexler E, Wahnich A, Friedrich T, Vijayendran C, Gao F, et al. RBFOX1 regulates both splicing and transcriptional networks in human neuronal development. Hum Mol Genet. 2012;21:4171–86. doi: 10.1093/hmg/dds240. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Li YI, Sanchez-Pulido L, Haerty W, Ponting CP. RBFOX and PTBP1 proteins regulate the alternative splicing of micro-exons in human brain transcripts. Genome Res. 2015;25:1–13. doi: 10.1101/gr.181990.114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Vuong CK, Wei W, Lee JA, Lin CH, Damianov A, de la Torre-Ubieta L, et al. Rbfox1 regulates synaptic transmission through the inhibitory neuron-specific vSNARE Vamp1. Neuron. 2018;98:127–141.e127. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.03.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Gehman LT, Stoilov P, Maguire J, Damianov A, Lin CH, Shiue L, et al. The splicing regulator Rbfox1 (A2BP1) controls neuronal excitation in the mammalian brain. Nat Genet. 2011;43:706–11. doi: 10.1038/ng.841. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Ferguson BR, Gao WJ. PV Interneurons: critical regulators of E/I balance for prefrontal cortex-dependent behavior and psychiatric disorders. Front Neural Circuits. 2018;12:37. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2018.00037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Tomassoni-Ardori F, Fulgenzi G, Becker J, Barrick C, Palko ME, Kuhn S, et al. Rbfox1 up-regulation impairs BDNF-dependent hippocampal LTP by dysregulating TrkB isoform expression levels. Elife. 2019;8:e49673. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.