Abstract
The diurnal mosquitoes Aedes aegypti are vectors of several arboviruses, including dengue, yellow fever, and Zika viruses. To find a host to feed on, they rely on the sophisticated integration of olfactory, visual, thermal, and gustatory cues emitted by the hosts. If detected by their target, this latter may display defensive behaviors that mosquitoes need to be able to detect and escape in order to survive. In humans, a typical response is a swat of the hand, which generates both mechanical and visual perturbations aimed at a mosquito. Here, we used programmable visual displays to generate expanding objects sharing characteristics with the visual component of an approaching hand and quantified the behavioral response of female mosquitoes. Results show that Ae. aegypti is capable of using visual information to decide whether to feed on an artificial host mimic. Stimulations delivered in a LED flight arena further reveal that landed Ae. aegypti females display a stereotypical escape strategy by taking off at an angle that is a function of the direction of stimulus introduction. Altogether, this study demonstrates that mosquitoes landed on a host mimic can use isolated visual cues to detect and avoid a potential threat.
Subject terms: Behavioural ecology, Sensory processing, Motion detection, Animal behaviour, Entomology
Introduction
Mosquitoes are responsible for transmitting disease-causing pathogens that kill more than 700,000 people every year1. The underlying motivation for adult females of epidemiologically problematic mosquito species is their need for nutrients, including proteins, contained in vertebrate blood to produce progeny. In addition to physiological stresses associated with the ingestion of blood, accessing a resource that is hidden under the skin of mobile and defensive hosts is not free of any risks. Hosts display defensive behaviors aimed at deterring or killing mosquitoes2. The reproductive fitness of female mosquitoes is thus not only directly linked to their ability to detect, locate and feed on a host but also to survive interactions with a larger and potentially defensive organism.
In humans, a typical response to biting insects is swatting. From the insect’s perspective, a swat corresponds to a rapidly approaching appendage on an interception course, most often a hand. When approaching, the appendage induces rapid air displacement (i.e., mechanical component) and the rapid expansion of an object in the mosquito visual field (i.e., visual component). Previous studies investigating the role of visual cues in triggering escape behaviors in lizards3, frogs4, mice5, larval zebrafish6, crabs7,8, locusts9,10, cricket11, sand flies12 employed variations of looming stimuli, some designed to closely mimic their main predator while others were designed with specific emphasis on characteristics of the looming object itself, such as its angular velocity and direction13,14. The results from these studies found that the likelihood of a prey’s escape response was contingent on specific characteristics of the looming stimuli. For example, fiddler crabs rely heavily on the retinal speed of the stimulus to trigger a “home run” or a full escape where they sprint back to their burrow15. In fruit flies, dropping a physical black disk onto a landed fly revealed that escape take-offs rely on a different neuroethological pathway than voluntary, spontaneous take-offs, where the insect pushes on its legs to increase the speed of its response16. High-speed videography further revealed that these jumps, mediated by giant fiber neurons that link brain visual areas to wing muscles13,17–19, pushed flies directly away from the looming stimulus20.
Using a mammalian tail simulator, Matherne et al.21, showed that the combined visual cues and airflow generated by the swinging of mammals’ tails reduced the proportion of Aedes aegypti landing by 50%. In free flight, both Ae. aegypti and the nocturnal malaria vector Anopheles coluzzii, displayed rapid escape maneuvers when stimulated with an artificial swatter that induces both airflow and visual cues22,23, hinting at the role of visual cues in signaling threats. Furthermore, some mosquito species display a “pre-biting” rest24 or “pre-attack” resting25 behavior when the mosquito density increases around a target host26, which is known to heighten host defensive behaviors27,28. With an increase in host defenses but ultimately an endless supply of blood, it would appear adaptive for the mosquito to wait until the host activity calms. However, what sensory cues mosquitoes use to gauge their hosts’ defensiveness remains unclear.
Here, we hypothesize that vision contributes to mediating mosquito escape behaviors. To test this hypothesis, we describe an experimental paradigm geared towards characterizing mosquitoes' responses to isolated visual threats (i.e., fully decoupled from mechanical cues). Specifically, to investigate the escape responses of Ae. aegypti mosquitoes landed on a host in response to a visually perceived threat, we simulated a predator-like looming (expanding) stimulus that mimicked the visual properties of a slapping hand since humans are Ae. aegypti’s preferred host29,30. In a first series of experiments, we used artificial blood-feeding assays to quantify visual threats' effects on mosquitoes' propensity to land and feed on a host mimic. Next, we relied on free-flight assays to define the response characteristics of visually-induced escape responses.
Results
Extracting visual features of a human swat
To present ecologically relevant stimuli, we video-tracked a slapping hand on a collision course with a fictive mosquito located at the camera’s position (GoPro Hero 6 in “linear” recording mode at 60 fps, GoPro, San Mateo, CA, USA). Video tracking the hand’s width at the widest point below knuckle height showed an average duration of 335 ± 82 ms with an exponentially increasing expansion rate peaking at 874.9°.s−1 before covering the entire field of view of the camera (n = 10; Fig. 1).
Side view recordings of ten slaps, showed that the peak instantaneous velocity of the slap was of 8.01 ± 0.66 m.s−1, which is slower, but within the same order of magnitude as the recorded speed of Olympic boxers’ punches (i.e., ~ 9.14 ± 2 m.s−131). The duration of the swat was used to program a dark looming square displayed to the mosquitoes on either an LCD monitor or on a programmable LED arena (Figs. 2A,3A, respectively). The expansion velocity of the fictive swats used throughout the experiment was kept constant at 28.9 horizontal degrees.s−1 (LCD monitor) and 183.6 horizontal degrees.s−1 (LED arena) as seen from the center of the mosquito container. These virtual swats correspond to the expansion velocity of a hand at 80 ms and 40 ms before contact, respectively. The rationale behind this choice was to simulate earlier phases of the swat in the feeding assays and later moments (i.e., closer to impact) in the take-off experiments, as the distance from the display was greater in the feeding assays than in the take-off experiments (300 mm versus 118.5 mm, respectively).
Visual motion reduces feeding, but not landing, on a warm source of blood
To analyze whether visual, threat-like stimuli could impact mosquitoes’ ability to blood-feed, groups of adult females were provided access to a host mimic positioned in front of a visual display (Fig. 2A). On average, 69.6 ± 3.97% of mosquitoes fed to repletion in front of the green background (Fig. 2B). Similarly, 73.1 ± 1.11% of mosquitoes fed in front of a field of static small squares, 74.1 ± 3.93% in front of translating small squares, and 73.4 ± 3.14% in front of a single repeatedly regressing square (Generalized linear model: p > 0.356; Fig. 2B). However, this proportion was significantly reduced when expanding squares were introduced at regular intervals (38.7 ± 4.38%; Generalized linear model: p < 0.001; Fig. 2B).
In the presence of a uniform green background (negative control), an average of 204 ± 25.2 total landings on the feeder were observed over the course of each trial (n = 8; 1226 total landings). When multiple static squares were randomly displayed on the screen, 277 ± 32.2 landings per trial were counted (n = 7; 1940 total landings), comparable to 280 ± 48.6 landings when randomly positioned squares followed a translational “random walk” on the screen (n = 8; 2240 total landings). When repeatedly presented with a single regressing square 212 ± 18.1 landings were counted per trial (n = 8; 1695 total landings), but only 174 ± 17.8 landings per trial were observed when expanding squares were repeatedly presented to the mosquitoes (n = 7, 1042 total landings). Although there was a tendency for, overall, fewer mosquitoes to land when exposed to looming squares, this effect was only marginally significant (Generalized linear model: p = 0.0514; Fig. 2C). Furthermore, while significantly more mosquitoes landed during the early phases of the experiments (Generalized linear model: p < 0.032; Fig. 2C), the difference between stimuli was not significant (Generalized linear model: p > 0.288; Fig. 2C).
Mosquitoes’ response to visual threats is a function of the distance and direction of the stimulus
To better understand the role of looming visual objects in the context of threat avoidance, mosquitoes were stimulated at regular intervals, with the expanding stimulus being introduced at random positions around an LED-based arena (sensu32) consisting of an array of 96 × 16 LEDs subtending 360° horizontally and 54° vertically from the center of the arena. Because mosquitoes were free to fly (or not), this randomization led to a large number of randomized directions of stimulus introduction relative to the mosquito, and random distances between the location of the mosquito and the point of stimulus introduction on the arena (Fig. 3B–G). Mosquitoes’ responses to expanding stimuli introduced at all directions were sampled homogeneously, with stimulations right behind the mosquito (> 140°) being sampled relatively more than the rest (Supplementary Table 1). Likewise, the mosquitoes’ responses as a function of their distance to the expanding stimuli were sampled homogeneously except for positions where the mosquitoes were either too close (< 3 cm in distance) to or too far (> 12 cm in distance) from the stimuli (Supplementary Table 2).
In the absence of any stimulation, the probability of take-offs was 0.02 (dotted red line; Fig. 3B,E). This number significantly increased to 0.26 when an expanding stimulus was introduced (including stimuli delivered from all directions and distances; Binomial Exact test: p < 0.001). However, mosquitoes’ escape probability varied significantly as a function of their distance to the stimulus, with peaks for stimuli delivered between 3 and 4 cm away from the mosquito (escape probability = 0.52; Generalized linear model, p < 0.001). It is worth noting that the lower escape probability observed for the closest distance bin (2 cm) could be due to the lower sampling of this distance bin. While the effect of the angle at which the expanding square was introduced was not significant (Generalized linear model, p = 0.056), the probability of escape tended to peak at angles between 60° and 90°, i.e., laterally to the mosquito.
To confirm that mosquitoes were indeed evading the expanding square, we compared the spatial occupancy of the arena between mosquitoes that were in flight but not stimulated and mosquitoes that took off following stimulus introduction. Because of the randomization of the stimulus introduction, data were rotated to align all stimuli introductions at 0°. While non-stimulated mosquitoes avoided the center of the arena, they explored the entire periphery of the device (Kuiper’s one sample test of uniformity: n.s.; test statistic = 1.6443, critical value at α = 0.05: 1.747; Fig. 4A). On the other hand, stimulated mosquitoes not only avoided the center of the arena, but also flew to the sectors opposed to where the stimulus was introduced, and avoided the fictive line of interception with the virtual stimulus (Kuiper’s one sample test of uniformity: p < 0.05; test statistic = 1.8243 critical value at α = 0.05: 1.747; Fig. 4B).
To further characterize the escape response displayed by mosquitoes, we analyzed the ‘flight direction’ (denoted as β, i.e., the take-off direction relative to the mosquito’s body orientation) and ‘escape direction’ (denoted as λ, i.e., the direction of escape relative to the stimulus) as a function of both the ‘approach angle’ (denoted as θ, i.e., the angle at which the stimulus is introduced relative to the mosquito’s body orientation) and the distance to the stimulus (Supplementary Figs. S4, S5).
Regardless of the approach angle and distance to the stimulus, mosquitoes did not exhibit a preferred flight direction relative to their body orientation (Rao’s Spacing Test of Uniformity; p > 0.05; Fig. 4C). However, responding mosquitoes significantly escaped away from the direction of the stimulus (Rao’s Spacing Test of Uniformity; p < 0.05; Fig. 4D, Supplementary Figs. S3, S4, S5). This is explained by the significant effect of the angle of approach on the direction of escape (Generalized linear model; p < 0.001), where mosquitoes predominantly escaped by flying towards their back when the stimulus was introduced in front of them, i.e., moving away from the direction of the stimulus (closer to 0°, green bars; Fig. 4C, Supplementary Video 3). Mosquitoes that had stimuli introduced laterally (around 61°–150°) escaped by performing a slight turn relative to the direction of the stimulus, and mosquitoes that had stimuli introduced behind them (< 150°) escaped by flying forward, i.e., orienting the most away from the origin of the stimuli (Fig. 4D; Supplementary Figs. S4, S5). Although there was a tendency for mosquitoes to go further away when the stimulus was introduced closer to them (blue vectors in Supplementary Fig. S5A), the distance to the point of stimulus introduction had no significant effect on the displacement after take-off, regardless of the approach angle θ at which the stimulus was introduced relative to the mosquito’s body orientation (Supplementary Fig. S5B).
Analyzing four aspects of the escape response (i.e., velocity, angular velocity, flight direction, and escape direction; Fig. 5A) we found that the velocity and escape direction (λ) were significantly influenced by the distance to the point of stimulus introduction (Generalized linear models: p = 0.003, p = 0.138, p = 0.265, and p = 0.042 for the velocity, angular velocity, beta, and lambda, respectively; Fig. 5B–E). When analyzed as a function of the angle of stimulus introduction, only the flight direction (β) was significantly affected (Generalized linear models: p = 0.374, p = 0.851, p < 0.001 and p = 0.401 for the velocity, angular velocity, β, and λ, respectively; Fig. 5F–I). In Fig. 5D,H, a β angle of 0° indicates a forward flying mosquito and 180° indicates a backward flying mosquito. In Fig. 5E,I, a λ of 0° indicate flying toward the stimulus and 180° away from the stimulus. These results show that mosquitoes escaped away from the looming stimulus, regardless of the approach angle, by flying away from the direction of the stimulus.
Remarkably, take-offs were even observed even for stimulus introductions immediately behind the mosquito (135°–180° angles), highlighting landed mosquitoes’ capacity to detect threats coming from all directions along the azimuth. Finally, analyzing the response latency of mosquitoes, we found that the time taken by the mosquitoes to take off in response to stimulus introduction was not influenced by the approach angle θ of the stimulus and mosquitoes’ distance to the stimulus. The response latency of the 142 escape responses observed in this study typically ranged between 0 and 0.53 s and was non-normally distributed (Fig. 6; Shapiro–Wilk test; p < 0.001). The cumulative probability of take-off started to plateau at around 350 ms, which is similar to previous observations in D. melanogaster20, although the probability tended to ramp up earlier in mosquitoes (Fig. 6D).
Discussion
Our results demonstrate that landed mosquitoes can use isolated visual cues to detect and respond to potential threats, i.e., even in the absence of other sensory information (e.g., air displacement due to the host’s movements). In our feeding assays, we found that the presence of looming visual cues did not impact the number of landings on the artificial feeder. However, the proportion of mosquitoes fed to repletion was reduced by nearly 50%. Interestingly, other types of visual motion (i.e., translation, regression) did not impact landing and feeding proportions (Fig. 2). This indicates that while the presence of rapidly moving objects in the visual field of flying mosquitoes does not impair their ability to navigate toward a source of food, once landed, mosquitoes specifically respond to looming visual objects by displaying an escape response (Fig. 2B). Although the identity of each mosquito was not maintained in our analysis, the large number of landings relative to the number of feeding events suggests that the same mosquitoes made several landing attempts and that several mosquitoes landed (several times) but did not feed.
Using an LED-based flight arena, we were able to further characterize this response in landed mosquitoes by introducing expanding squares at randomized distances and directions relative to the focal mosquito. Results confirmed that looming squares trigger mosquitoes to take off at a higher probability than chance (i.e., compared to the take-off probability of landed mosquitoes randomly sampled in the absence of visual stimulation: 0.02). The peak escape probability (0.52) observed here was comparable to the frequency of successful escapes from a mechanical swatter by in-flight Ae. aegypti females22,33. The closer the mosquito to the stimulus, the more likely it was to take off, and although all directions of introduction induced take-offs, threats originating laterally tended to elicit more escapes (Fig. 3). In particular, mosquitoes were most responsive to looming stimuli that came from approximately 60°, 90°, and 100°. While the heterogeneity of the mosquito retina34 has been described, it is too early to conclude about the spatial specialization for detecting threats. Further studies will be required to determine the range of elevations above the plane of the substrate at which mosquitoes can detect threats, especially because the field of view available to a mosquito actively engaged in blood-feeding may differ from its resting position (e.g., different head angle, substrate orientation). Such a spatial specialization has, however, been found in tethered Drosophila melanogaster, where the expansion of centrally versus laterally positioned objects elicited different response profiles: frontal objects induced strong leg and wing-beat frequency responses but minimal changes in wing-beat amplitudes while lateral objects elicited stronger changes in wing-beat amplitude and transient increases in wing-beat frequency, but did not evoke leg responses35,36.
Remarkably, the escape responses we observed were not random: mosquitoes rapidly positioned themselves out of the line of interception with the trajectory of the fictive threat (Fig. 4B). Analysis of the trajectories displayed after take-off shows that the flight direction is indeed directly influenced by the angle of stimulus introduction (Figs. 4,5H), which allows mosquitoes to follow an escape direction oriented away from the point of introduction of the stimulus (Figs. 4C, 5I). A similar behavior had been observed in flight in D. melanogaster where looming targets triggered visually-directed banked turns that reoriented the fly’s path within a handful of wingbeats12. In addition, studies focusing on the take-off behavior of D. melanogaster showed the same relationship between the approach angle of a visual threat and the direction of the jump flies performed as the first step of their escape strategy20. It is worth noting that in flies, D. melanogaster displays two types of take-off strategies as a function of the context (i.e., a long mode and short mode take-off) while in mosquitoes only one strategy has been observed during voluntary and escape take-offs16,18,37. Specifically, the analysis of the take-off behavior of blood-fed An. coluzzii showed that mosquitoes reach comparable take-off speeds as D. melanogaster, but with a fraction of the push-off forces37,38. This suggests that mosquitoes may have adapted to escaping their hosts without alerting them, but since no visual threats were introduced in these studies, a fine-scale analysis of landed mosquitoes escaping a visual threat will be required to determine whether they adjust their take-off strategy when threatened. In particular, we observed that the closer the mosquito was to the point of introduction of the threat, the faster it escaped (Fig. 5B). Although the distance to the stimulus and its angle of approach were randomized, it is worth noting that the cumulative probability of escape we observed was overall similar to what has been previously described in fruit flies (Fig. 6D). Future studies providing direct comparisons between flies and mosquitoes will be required for a truly quantitative assessment of mosquitoes’ adaptations to feeding on a defensive host.
It is also possible that the response probability to the looming stimuli was actually higher than what we observed by only quantifying take-offs, as responses may have taken other forms, such as a freeze response to threats. Indeed, other animals have been shown to make a decision to either freeze or flee when a threat is presented5. Given that freezing responses could not be identified and characterized, we focused on landed mosquitoes to analyze the fleeing responses. In addition, because of the size of the clear acrylic cage the mosquitoes were contained in, escape responses could only be examined over a short period of time, as mosquitoes would rapidly reach the wall of the container and redirect their flight trajectory (Supplementary Video 3). However, despite this limitation and while quantifying responses in 2-dimensions only, our results show clear visual responses to isolated visual stimuli: similarly to flies16,20, mosquitoes displayed a tight relationship between the angle at which the stimulus is introduced and the flight direction. Within 1/6th of a second, mosquitoes did not just randomly scatter. Instead, they flew away at an angle directly influenced by the approach angle of the stimulus to escape on a trajectory mostly away and at a slight angle from the stimulus (Fig. 5H,I).
Altogether, results from the present study provide some insights into the “pre-biting’ or “pre-attack” resting behavior previously observed in several anopheline and culicine mosquito species26. Because this “pre-biting” rest was more frequently observed at higher mosquito densities, one could hypothesize that mosquitoes possessed the ability to evaluate the level of defensiveness from afar, possibly using vision in this context. In this hypothetical scenario, mosquitoes would land near the host and wait for its defensive behaviors to decrease before approaching, thus increasing the probability of successful blood-feeding. However, in our artificial feeding assays, the number of landings on a host mimic was not affected by visual threats, only the proportion of successful feedings on an otherwise defenseless feeder. This suggests that the pre-biting behavior would require mosquitoes to land on the host and experience its defensive behavior, which differs from other studies conducted on tabanid flies, where stripes present on the fur of the host made it much more difficult for the flies to land on them39. In this case, visual cues directly impaired the insects’ landing ability, and, as a result, horses with solid color experienced more fly landings.
In nature, as mentioned above, a critical contributor to mosquitoes’ feeding success is the density of female mosquitoes around the host27,28,40,41. Indeed, as the density of mosquitoes increases, so does the level of defensiveness of the host, which negatively correlates with the proportion of females that successfully blood-feed28. Consequently, mosquitoes prefer to feed on the least defensive or least active host over more defensive ones40,42. While our results suggest that Ae. aegypti gauges the defensiveness of a host only after landing on it, it is worth highlighting that only visual cues were available in our experiments. But while mechanical cues (i.e., air displacement) generated by a mammal tail simulator significantly prevented mosquitoes from landing21, whether they can trigger the pre-biting resting behavior remains to be determined. In a previous study, when a mechanical vibration calibrated to mimic an average swat was repeatedly paired with host olfactory cues, Ae. aegypti females learned the association between the mechanical stimulus and the odor and subsequently avoided the trained odor43. Mechanical cues thus contribute to host selection processes2,43,44 and, by disturbing the ability of mosquitoes to land on the host, most likely contribute to a mosquito’s evaluation of the host’s defensiveness. This relationship between mosquito density, host defensiveness, and feeding success is central to the vectorial capacity of mosquitoes, and the results of the present study suggest that visual cues may be used by landed mosquitoes to gauge the defensiveness of a host.
Beyond their role when presented in isolation, sensory cues from multiple modalities (e.g., vision and olfaction) are integrated by the mosquito brain45–47. In the context of evading host defenses, Cribellier et al.22 showed that, in flight, Ae. aegypti females were most successful at evading a mechanical swatter in bright light conditions, i.e., when both visual and mechanical cues were available. Interestingly, night-active An. coluzzii were most successful at escaping in the dark, suggesting that nocturnal mosquitoes maximize their escape performance by adjusting the parameters of their flight behavior itself, such as adopting a more stochastic (i.e., protean) flight33.
In the present study, we provide an experimental paradigm that permits the presentation of visual cues in isolation from mechanical perturbations. Our Results show that visual threat-like stimuli alone are sufficient to disrupt Ae. aegypti’s blood-feeding behavior and trigger a stereotypical escape response in landed females, influenced by the distance and direction of the threat. This work opens new research avenues for improving our understanding of the role of vision in mosquito biology and developing control strategies that target this sensory modality. For example, the experimental paradigm presented here could be adapted to spatially (and temporarily) decouple visual and mechanosensory cues to deepen our understanding of multimodal sensory integration processes in the context of threat avoidance.
Methods
Mosquitoes
Wild type Aedes aegypti mosquitoes (Rockefeller strain, MR-734, MR4, ATCC®, Manassas, VA, USA) were used throughout the experiments. The colony was maintained in a climatic chamber set at 25 ± 1 °C, 60 ± 10% relative humidity (RH), and under a 12-12 h light–dark cycle. Cages of adults were fed weekly using an artificial feeder (D.E. Lillie Glassblowers, Atlanta, Georgia; 2.5 cm internal diameter) with heparinized bovine blood (Lampire Biological Laboratories, Pipersville, PA, USA) heated at 37 °C using a circulating water-bath. Between blood-meals, mosquitoes were fed ad libitum with 10% sucrose. No vertebrate hosts were used to rear mosquitoes or during the experiments. Eggs were collected from blood-fed females and hatched in deionized water. Larvae were reared in groups of 200 in covered pans (26 × 35x4 cm) containing deionized water and fed ad libitum with fish food (Hikari Tropic 382 First Bites—Petco, San Diego, CA, USA). Pupae, in groups of 100, were isolated in 16 oz containers (Mosquito Breeder Jar, Bioquip Products, Rancho Dominguez, CA, USA) until emergence.
For all the experiments, 6–8 days old female mosquitoes were used. They were kept in the presence of males, fed ad libitum with 10% sucrose until 24 h prior to the experiments, and never blood-fed. This gave mosquitoes the time to mate in the containers before the experiments (dissection of randomly selected females revealed that 95% of them had oocytes) and ensured high motivation to seek hosts for blood feeding48,49. All experiments were performed during the last four hours of the mosquitoes’ subjective day, i.e., Zeitgeber Time (ZT) 8–12 50,51.
Expanding stimuli
Tracking of the hand’s width of the first author over ten replicates provided an average duration and expansion velocity profile of a slap (ImageJ; National Institutes of Health, USA; Fig. 1). This information was used to program the symmetrical expansion of a dark square over a bright green background. A square was selected over other shapes as behavioral responses to expanding squares have been shown in tethered Ae. aegypti females47, and the response characteristics of classical models such as Drosophila melanogaster have been extensively analyzed36. Furthermore, in the programmable LED arena, a square allowed for the expansion of the stimulus along both dimensions with a higher step resolution than, for example, a disk.
Artificial blood-feeding assays
Procedure
Visual patterns generated with PsychoPy v3.0 52 were displayed on a 29-in. (73 cm diagonal) LED-backlit LCD monitor (UltraSharp 29 Ultrawide Monitor—U2917W, Dell, Round Rock, TX, USA). Specifically, five patterns were generated:
A uniform green screen (negative control).
A “square field” composed of fifty static squares randomly positioned on the screen, without overlap. Altogether, the 50 squares occupy the same area of the screen as the looming square at its maximum expansion.
An animated square field where 50 squares similar to (2) are randomly positioned on the screen and follow a translational motion (i.e., “random walk” without overlap), at a velocity of 5.4 pixels/s.
Regressive squares identical to the expanding squares (see 5) in all respect but the direction of motion.
Expanding squares programmed to have the same duration as a human swat (Fig. 2A, Supplementary Video 1) and introduced every 0.8 s for a duration of 0.3 s.
The positions of the regressing and looming squares were randomly determined for each experiment trial to account for potential spatial bias. For this purpose, PsychoPy used an array of cartesian coordinates randomized using the rand function in Microsoft Excel.
Green, with a peak irradiance at 535 nm, was chosen as a background color to allow comparisons with the conditions in the LED arena (peak irradiance: 571 nm) (Supplementary Fig. S1).
Mosquitoes (21 < n < 35) were isolated into clear glass cylinders (10 × 10 cm), starved from sucrose for 24 h before the experiment, and tested during the last 2 h of the photophase (ZT 10–12). In the first hour of the experiment, the screen was kept uniformly green to allow mosquitoes to acclimate to the experimental conditions. At the onset of the second hour, a membrane blood feeder was placed on the fabric mesh-lined top side of the glass cylinder. The feeder was warmed to 37 ℃ using a circulating water bath thirty minutes prior to the onset of the experiment. The warmed feeder was filled with ~ 5 ml of heparinized bovine whole blood (Lampire Biological Laboratories, Pipersville, PA, USA) fifteen minutes prior to the beginning of the experiment to allow the blood to heat up. The fabric mesh-lined side of the glass cylinder allowed mosquitoes to sense the heat and see the visual contrast of the feeder. The number of mosquitoes that landed and blood-fed on the feeder was recorded using a camera (Logitech C920, Logitech, Lausanne, Switzerland).
In the negative control (2 consecutive hours of green background), the baseline levels of landing and feeding were quantified in front of a uniform green screen. In the first hour, mosquitoes were allowed to acclimatize to the experimental conditions, including the uniform green background. In the second hour, after placing the blood feeder on a glass cylinder enclosure, the number of landings and feedings on the feeder were quantified. In the treatment groups, mosquitoes isolated in the glass cylinder enclosure were acclimatized to the uniform green screen in the first hour, following which the visual stimuli, i.e., either static square field, moving square field, regressing squares, or expanding squares were introduced.
Statistical analysis
After every trial, the number of engorged females was quantified via visual inspection of their abdomen for the presence of blood, and the number of landings was quantified from the video recordings. These numbers were then compared between treatments as proportions (categorical fixed predictors with two levels: uniform background and looming squares). For the analysis, we used a Generalized Linear Model assuming a quasibinomial error distribution for the proportion of feeding and a Poisson error distribution for the number of landings per mosquito. The analysis was performed in R (version 3.6.2) using the packages lme4 (version 1.1–27.153) and multcomp (version 1.4–1754).
Free-flight LED arena
Procedure
Individual mosquitoes were cold anesthetized on ice and separated in clear acrylic cylinders with a clear acrylic lid on the top and a fabric mesh lining at the bottom. These containers were kept at 26 °C for at least two hours to allow the mosquitoes to recover from the cold anesthesia. Containers were next moved to the experimental room (23 ± 1 °C, 45 ± 5% RH) thirty minutes before the start of the experiment to allow the mosquitoes to acclimatize to the ambient conditions. In every experimental trial that lasted for thirty minutes, a container was placed inside the arena, with the expanding stimulus being introduced every minute at a randomized angle around the arena (Fig. 3A, Supplementary Video 2). The distance and the direction to the point of stimulus introduction were a function of both the mosquito’s position in the arena and the randomized angle of introduction. The behavior of mosquitoes was recorded at 30 fps with a camera (Logitech C920, Logitech, Lausanne, Switzerland).
Video analysis
Mosquitoes that did not move throughout the duration of the experiment (~ 7%) were determined “not active” and discarded from the analysis. Because of the study’s focus on take-off responses, trials in which mosquitoes were in flight before the introduction of the stimulus were discarded from the analysis (n = 63). Based on these criteria, a total of 551 stimuli introductions were conserved, and videos of landed mosquitoes were trimmed 30 s before and 30 s after each stimuli introduction. These one-minute trimmed videos were then converted into image sequences and imported into Fiji ImageJ (National Institutes of Health) for manual continuous tracking of the entire mosquito trajectory using the Manual Tracking plugin. The head of each mosquito was tracked before, during, and after stimulus introduction. The stimuli center points were also tracked for each stimulus introduction. The mosquito-stimulus distance was measured using the measuring tool in Fiji ImageJ and measuring the distance between the center of the stimulus to the mosquito head. The angle of stimulus introduction was measured using the same tool by measuring from the center of the stimulus to the head of the mosquito to the tip of the abdomen. Distances were measured in pixels and the conversions to cm provided throughout the manuscript were obtained by reporting the measured diameter of the arena to its measurement on the videos.
Statistical analysis
To calculate mosquitoes’ escape probability, only individuals that took off within 15 frames post-stimulus introduction (i.e., 0.5 s) were considered as “responsive” but, overall, we observed that 98.6% of mosquitoes that responded to the stimulus took off within 0.4 s of the stimulus presentation. Take-offs past the ½ of a second time frame were not considered responses as mosquitoes would have been intercepted by the fictive object beyond that time.
A baseline probability of take-off was determined by repeating the experiments with the exception that no visual looming stimuli were introduced. All LEDs of the arena were kept ON for thirty minutes and the behavior of individualized mosquitoes (n = 10) was recorded with the same video camera as the treatment group. For each mosquito, ten time points were randomly selected within these 30 min, using the sample function in R (i.e., N = 100). At each of these times and for up to five seconds afterward, spontaneous take-offs were quantified and used to calculate the baseline take-off probability in the absence of visual stimuli.
All data were saved as .csv and imported in R (version 4.1.3). Trajectories were rotated so that all stimuli introductions were fictively re-positioned at 0°. All the points before the stimulus introduction were considered pre-stimulus, while all the points after the stimulus introduction were considered post-stimulus. In the post-stimulus points, we synchronized the escape responses (N = 142) by discounting mosquitoes’ latency to respond to the expanding stimulus (i.e., trial-to-trial variations in the timing of the take-off response). We opted to analyze the first 5 frames of these synchronized responses as our frame-by-frame analysis revealed that this duration (i.e., 1/6 of a second) effectively captured the escape (take-off) responses of mosquitoes while excluding the effects of mosquitoes hitting the walls of the arena (Supplementary Video 3). From this data, five response variables were calculated by subtracting the pre-stimulus period from the post-stimulus period (to account for mosquitoes walking before stimulus introduction): linear and angular velocity, flight direction ‘β’, escape direction ‘λ’, and displacement: (i) the linear velocity was determined by the distance between consecutive points divided by the sampling interval (cm.s−1); (ii) the angular velocity was calculated as the change in heading between consecutive points divided by the sampling interval (degrees.s−1); (iii) the flight direction ‘β’ was calculated as the direction of escape relative to the mosquito’s body orientation (degrees); (iv) the escape direction ‘λ’ was calculated as the direction of escape relative to the stimulus and (v) the displacement (distance travelled) was quantified by measuring differences between the mosquito’s location at time of stimulus introduction and its location 5 frames (0.17 s) after the take-off triggered by the introduction of the stimulus. The effects of the distance to stimulus and the approach angle ‘θ’ on linear velocity, angular velocity, displacement, and response latency were assessed by means of generalized linear models (GLMs) using the R package lme4 53. The response variables flight direction ‘β’ and escape direction ‘λ’were analyzed to assess the uniformity and periodicity of circular data using Rao’s spacing test and Rayleigh’s test respectively using the R package circular55.
In order to quantify the amount of digitizing error introduced by manual tracking of the mosquitoes, three experimenters tracked the same 2-s-long sequence (59 frames), once a day during 3 consecutive days. This provided an estimate of the inter- and intra-individual errors in the digitized cartesian coordinates, quantified here as the Root Square Mean Error (RMSE), calculated according to Eq. (1):
1 |
On average, the tracked location of the mosquito deviated by 7.86 pixels (Supplementary Fig. S2) from the mean of the locations measured by all three experimenters. This corresponds to 43.9% of the body length of an average-sized mosquito landed at the bottom of the arena (17.88 ± 0.47 pixels or 4.87 ± 0.12 mm). In this context, and to minimize the influence of errors introduced during the digitization process, changes in velocity and angular velocity were calculated after smoothing the raw data using a low-pass butterworth filter (pass.filt(), R package dplR 1.7.2 56) with a cut-off frequency of 0.25 57. The four response variables thus quantified were visualized as a function of the mosquitoes’ distance from the point of stimulus introduction (Fig. 5B–E) and the angle of stimulus introduction (Fig. 5F–I) with a trendline (± 95% confidence interval) from a single-term local regression model fit summarizing the relationship (geom_smooth(method = ‘loess’), R package ggplot2 3.3.5 58).
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
We thank Drs. Chloé Lahondère, Jake Tu, and Jake Socha for their feedback and valuable insights on the experimental design and the manuscript. We also thank Danny Eanes and Steve Lowe for their help with technical details, Chanel Hsu for her help with preliminary data management and visualization, and Shajaesza Diggs and Darren Dougharty for their help with mosquito rearing. Finally, we would like to thank the anonymous reviewers and the editor for their comments on the manuscript.
Author contributions
Conceptualization: N.W., K.C, C.V.; Methodology: N.W., K.C., C.V.; Software: K.C., C.V.; Validation: N.W.; Formal analysis: K.C., C.V.; Investigation: N.W., L.F.; Resources: C.V.; Data Curation: N.W., K.C.; Writing—original draft preparation: N.W., K.C.; Writing—review and editing: N.W., K.C., C.V.; Visualization: N.W., K.C., C.V.; Supervision: C.V.; Project administration: N.W., C.V.; Funding acquisition: C.V.
Funding
This material is based upon work supported by the US Department of Agriculture National Institute of Food and Agriculture under Hatch project # 1017860 to C.V.
Data availability
The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available in the Mendeley Data repository, https://data.mendeley.com/datasets/hhkz8bspsw/1.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-022-25461-2.
References
- 1.World Health Organization. World Health Statistics 2018. (WHO, 2018).
- 2.Wynne NE, Lorenzo MG, Vinauger C. Mechanism and plasticity of vectors’ host-seeking behavior. Curr. Opin. Insect Sci. 2020;40:1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.cois.2020.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Carlile PA, Peters RA, Evans CS. Detection of a looming stimulus by the Jacky dragon: Selective sensitivity to characteristics of an aerial predator. Anim. Behav. 2006;72:553–562. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.10.027. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Ingle DJ. Visually elicited evasive behavior in frogs. Bioscience. 1990;40:284–291. doi: 10.2307/1311265. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Yilmaz M, Meister M. Rapid innate defensive responses of mice to looming visual stimuli. Curr. Biol. 2013;23:2011–2015. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.08.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Temizer I, Donovan JC, Baier H, Semmelhack JL. A visual pathway for looming-evoked escape in larval zebrafish. Curr. Biol. 2015;25:1823–1834. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Scarano F, Tomsic D, Sztarker J. Direction selective neurons responsive to horizontal motion in a crab reflect an adaptation to prevailing movements in flat environments. J. Neurosci. 2020 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0372-20.2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Scarano F, Tomsic D. Escape response of the crab Neohelice to computer generated looming and translational visual danger stimuli. J. Physiol. Paris. 2014;108:141–147. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2014.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Santer RD, Rind FC, Stafford R, Simmons PJ. Role of an identified looming-sensitive neuron in triggering a flying locust’s escape. J. Neurophysiol. 2006;95:3391–3400. doi: 10.1152/jn.00024.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Simmons PJ, Rind FC, Santer RD. Escapes with and without preparation: The neuroethology of visual startle in locusts. J. Insect Physiol. 2010;56:876–883. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2010.04.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Dupuy F, Casas J, Body M, Lazzari CR. Danger detection and escape behaviour in wood crickets. J. Insect Physiol. 2011;57:865–871. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2011.03.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Muijres FT, Elzinga MJ, Melis JM, Dickinson MH. Flies evade looming targets by executing rapid visually directed banked turns. Science. 2014;344:172–177. doi: 10.1126/science.1248955. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Ache JM, et al. Neural basis for looming size and velocity encoding in the Drosophila giant fiber escape pathway. Curr. Biol. 2019;29:1073–1081.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.01.079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Domenici P, Booth D, Blagburn JM, Bacon JP. Cockroaches keep predators guessing by using preferred escape trajectories. Curr. Biol. 2008;18:1792–1796. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.09.062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Smolka J, Zeil J, Hemmi JM. Natural visual cues eliciting predator avoidance in fiddler crabs. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2011;278:3584–3592. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.2746. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Card G, Dickinson M. Performance trade-offs in the flight initiation of Drosophila. J. Exp. Biol. 2008;211:341–353. doi: 10.1242/jeb.012682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Sun YA, Wyman RJ. Neurons of the Drosophila giant fiber system: I. Dorsal longitudinal motor neurons. J. Comp. Neurol. 1997;387:157–166. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19971013)387:1<157::AID-CNE13>3.0.CO;2-R. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.von Reyn CR, et al. Feature integration drives probabilistic behavior in the Drosophila escape response. Neuron. 2017;94:1190–1204.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.05.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Fotowat H, Fayyazuddin A, Bellen HJ, Gabbiani F. A novel neuronal pathway for visually guided escape in Drosophila melanogaster. J. Neurophysiol. 2009;102:875–885. doi: 10.1152/jn.00073.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Card G, Dickinson MH. Visually mediated motor planning in the escape response of Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 2008;18:1300–1307. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.07.094. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Matherne ME, Cockerill K, Zhou Y, Bellamkonda M, Hu DL. Mammals repel mosquitoes with their tails. J. Exp. Biol. 2018;221:178905. doi: 10.1242/jeb.178905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Cribellier A, et al. Diurnal and nocturnal mosquitoes escape looming threats using distinct flight strategies. Curr. Biol. 2022;32:1232–1246.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2022.01.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Cribellier, A., Spitzen, J., Straw, A. D., van Leeuwen, J. L. & Muijres, F. T. Escape flight performances of night-active malaria mosquitoes: the role of visual and airflow cues of an approaching object. in Integrative and Comparative Biology. Vol. 61. E170–E171 (Oxford University Press Inc Journals Dept, 2021).
- 24.Reid, J. A. Anopheline Mosquitoes of Malaya and Borneo. Studies from the Institute for Medical Research, Malaysia. (1968).
- 25.Clements AN. The Biology of Mosquitoes. Volume 2: Sensory Reception and Behaviour. CABI Publishing; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Tuno N, Tsuda Y, Takagi M, Swonkerd W. Pre- and postprandial mosquito resting behavior around cattle hosts. J. Am. Mosq. Control Assoc. 2003;19:211–219. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Day JF, Edman JD. Mosquito engorgement on normally defensive hosts depends on host activity Patterns. J. Med. Entomol. 1984;21:732–740. doi: 10.1093/jmedent/21.6.732. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Edman JD, Webber LA, Kale HW. Effect of mosquito density on the interrelationship of host behavior and mosquito feeding success. Am. J. Trop. Med. Hyg. 1972;21:487–491. doi: 10.4269/ajtmh.1972.21.487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Christophers, S. R. Aedes aegypti: The Yellow Fever Mosquito. (1960).
- 30.Ponlawat A, Harrington LC. Blood feeding patterns of Aedes aegypti and Aedes albopictus in Thailand. J. Med. Entomol. 2005;42:844–849. doi: 10.1093/jmedent/42.5.844. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Walilko TJ, Viano DC, Bir CA. Biomechanics of the head for Olympic boxer punches to the face. Br. J. Sports Med. 2005;39:710–719. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2004.014126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Reiser MB, Dickinson MH. A modular display system for insect behavioral neuroscience. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2008;167:127–139. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.07.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Cribellier, A. Biomechanics of Flying Mosquitoes During Capture and Escape. Doctoral Dissertation. (Wageningen University, 2021).
- 34.Hu X, Leming MT, Whaley MA, O’Tousa JE. Rhodopsin coexpression in UV photoreceptors of Aedes aegypti and Anopheles gambiae mosquitoes. J. Exp. Biol. 2014;217:1003–1008. doi: 10.1242/jeb.096347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Tammero LF, Frye MA, Dickinson MH. Spatial organization of visuomotor reflexes in Drosophila. J. Exp. Biol. 2004;207:113–122. doi: 10.1242/jeb.00724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Tammero LF, Dickinson MH. Collision-avoidance and landing responses are mediated by separate pathways in the fruit fly, Drosophila melanogaster. J. Exp. Biol. 2002;205:2785–2798. doi: 10.1242/jeb.205.18.2785. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Muijres FT, et al. Escaping blood-fed malaria mosquitoes minimize tactile detection without compromising on take-off speed. J. Exp. Biol. 2017;220:3751–3762. doi: 10.1242/jeb.163402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.van Veen WG, van Leeuwen JL, Muijres FT. Malaria mosquitoes use leg push-off forces to control body pitch during take-off. J. Exp. Zool. A Ecol. Integr. Physiol. 2020;333:38–49. doi: 10.1002/jez.2308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Caro T, et al. Benefits of zebra stripes: Behaviour of tabanid flies around zebras and horses. PLoS ONE. 2019;14:e0210831. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0210831. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Edman JD, Webber LA, Schmid AA. Effect of host defenses on the feeding pattern of Culex nigripalpus when offered a choice of blood sources. J. Parasitol. 1974;60:874–883. doi: 10.2307/3278923. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Walker ED, Edman JD. The influence of host defensive behavior on mosquito (Diptera: Culicidae) biting persistence1. J. Med. Entomol. 1985;22:370–372. doi: 10.1093/jmedent/22.4.370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Warnes ML, Finlayson LH. Effect of host behaviour on host preference in Stomoxys calcitrans. Med. Vet. Entomol. 1987;1:53–57. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2915.1987.tb00322.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Vinauger C, et al. Modulation of host learning in Aedes aegypti mosquitoes. Curr. Biol. 2018;28:333–344.e8. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.12.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Wolff GH, Riffell JA. Olfaction, experience and neural mechanisms underlying mosquito host preference. J. Exp. Biol. 2018;221:157131. doi: 10.1242/jeb.157131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Alonso San Alberto D, et al. The olfactory gating of visual preferences to human skin and visible spectra in mosquitoes. Nat. Commun. 2022;13:1–14. doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-28195-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.van Breugel F, Riffell J, Fairhall A, Dickinson MH. Mosquitoes use vision to associate odor plumes with thermal targets. Curr. Biol. 2015;25:2123–2129. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.06.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Vinauger C, et al. Visual-olfactory integration in the human disease vector mosquito, Aedes aegypti. Curr. Biol. 2019;29:2509–2516.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.06.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Grant AJ, O’Connell RJ. Age-related changes in female mosquito carbon dioxide detection. J. Med. Entomol. 2007;44:617–623. doi: 10.1093/jmedent/44.4.617. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Tallon AK, Hill SR, Ignell R. Sex and age modulate antennal chemosensory-related genes linked to the onset of host seeking in the yellow-fever mosquito, Aedes aegypti. Sci. Rep. 2019;9:43. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-36550-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Eilerts DF, VanderGiessen M, Bose EA, Broxton K, Vinauger C. Odor-specific daily rhythms in the olfactory sensitivity and behavior of Aedes aegypti mosquitoes. Insects. 2018;9:147. doi: 10.3390/insects9040147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Taylor B, Jones MD. The circadian rhythm of flight activity in the mosquito Aedes aegypti (L). The phase-setting effects of light-on and light-off. J. Exp. Biol. 1969;51:59–70. doi: 10.1242/jeb.51.1.59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Peirce J, et al. PsychoPy2: Experiments in behavior made easy. Behav. Res. Methods. 2019;51:195–203. doi: 10.3758/s13428-018-01193-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. arXiv [stat.CO] (2014).
- 54.Hothorn T, Bretz F, Westfall P. Simultaneous inference in general parametric models. Biom. J. 2008;50:346–363. doi: 10.1002/bimj.200810425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Lund, U., & Agostinelli, C. Package “Circular”. Repository CRAN (2017).
- 56.Bunn AG. A dendrochronology program library in R (dplR) Dendrochronologia. 2008;26:115–124. doi: 10.1016/j.dendro.2008.01.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Walker JA. Estimating velocities and accelerations of animal locomotion: A simulation experiment comparing numerical differentiation algorithms. J. Exp. Biol. 1998;201:981–995. doi: 10.1242/jeb.201.7.981. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Wickham H. ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. Springer; 2009. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available in the Mendeley Data repository, https://data.mendeley.com/datasets/hhkz8bspsw/1.