Skip to main content
Elsevier - PMC COVID-19 Collection logoLink to Elsevier - PMC COVID-19 Collection
. 2021 May 11;279:114025. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2021.114025

Understanding at-the-moment stress for parents during COVID-19 stay-at-home restrictions

Bridget Freisthler a,, Paul J Gruenewald b, Erin Tebben a, Karla Shockley McCarthy a, Jennifer Price Wolf c
PMCID: PMC9756775  PMID: 34004571

Abstract

Rationale

In spring 2020, many states in the United States enacted stay-at-home orders to limit the spread of COVID-19 and lessen effects on hospitals and health care workers. This required parents to act in new roles without much support. Although studies have asked parents about stress before and during the pandemic, none have examined how stress may have fluctuated throughout the day and the characteristics related to those daily changes.

Objective

Our study assesses how time-varying (e.g., presence of a focal child) and day-varying (e.g., weekend vs. weekday) factors were related to parents’ level of stress.

Methods

We use Ecological Momentary Assessment to examine stress three times a day (10 a.m., 3 p.m., and 9 p.m.) for 14 days. We include two different dates hypothesized to be related to parents’ stress levels: (1) when Ohio announced schools would go virtual for the rest of the academic year and (2) when most retail businesses were allowed to re-open. Our sample of 332 individuals, recruited via Facebook, Craigslist, and word of mouth, completed 13,360 of these brief surveys during April–May 2020. Data were analyzed using generalized ordered logit models.

Results

Parents report lower levels of stress when completing the 9 p.m. survey, but higher levels when they were at work, during weekdays (compared to weekends) or when they were with the focal child. COVID-19 milestone dates were not related to stress levels.

Conclusions

Parents need some form of respite (e.g. child care, child-only activities) to reduce stress, especially during the week when parents are juggling their outside employment and their child(ren)'s schooling. Providing parents with skills and tools to identify and reduce stress, such as apps monitoring heart rate or providing deep breathing techniques, may be one way of helping parents cope with extremely stressful situations.

Keywords: Ecological momentary assessment, Stress, Parents, COVID-19, Generalized ordered logit models

1. Introduction

The coronavirus (COVID-19) pandemic has caused widespread psychosocial stress and social and economic disruption. In spring 2020, it disrupted life. Government and public health officials in a majority of states in the United States enacted shelter-in-place or stay-at-home orders to limit the spread of the virus and lessen the effects on hospitals and health care workers. These extraordinary measures required parents to act in a variety of new roles without much support. During this time, parents felt higher levels of stress due to forced changes in their routines, including through loss of childcare and the switch to virtual K–12 school—often while trying to work from home (American Psychological Association [APA], 2020a; Hiraoka and Tomoda, 2020). Essential workers, including cashiers at local grocery and big box stores, had to navigate childcare center closures and making new arrangements with pandemic childcare centers (Patrick et al., 2020). Extracurricular activities for children were cancelled, leaving children with few outlets for engagement or entertainment outside of the home. Yet, while there is indication that parental stress may be increased during the COVID-19 pandemic, little is known about how that stress may fluctuate over time and circumstances. Identifying the situations and contexts where parents experience the most stress could help the development of targeted interventions to promote family well-being.

In Ohio, the first cases of COVID-19 were detected on March 9, 2020, with the first death occurring 11 days later on March 20, 2020. K–12 schools were initially shut down on March 17 and ultimately shut down for the rest of the academic year on April 20, 2020. The first stay-at-home order began on midnight March 24, 2020. Stay-at-home restrictions ultimately lasted until May 29, 2020; however, a phased roll out of businesses re-opening began May 1, 2020. Most consumer and retail businesses were able to open on May 12, 2020 (Bischoff and Spicker, 2020).

Stress, Health, and Well-Being among Parents. Parenting stress is not unique to the COVID-19 pandemic. Parents generally report feeling more stressed (Umberson et al., 2010) and less happy (Glass et al., 2016) than comparable groups of non-parents throughout the developed world. Some of this stress is attributable to the act of parenting itself (Musick et al., 2016; Nelson et al., 2014; Skreden et al., 2012) and is variable depending on children's gender, age, and number of children (Barroso et al., 2018). Mothers of boys, for example, have been found to experience higher levels of parenting stress than mothers of girls (Vierhaus et al., 2013). Child age has also been associated with parental stress, with findings suggesting that stress is particularly high among parents of infants and toddlers (Neece et al., 2012; Nelson et al., 2014; Skreden et al., 2012), decreases into the preschool and early school age years (Williford et al., 2007) and then increases as children enter adolescence (Meier et al., 2018). The addition of subsequent children to a family also increases the stress felt by parents (Nomaguchi and Fettro, 2018; Skreden et al., 2012). A preponderance of research evidence demonstrates that the act of parenting, regardless of the individual characteristics of children, is stressful.

These pressures experienced by parents have been associated with a number of parental and family characteristics as well. Stressors experienced by parents are lessened by having adequate financial resources (Glass et al., 2016; Pollman-Schult, 2014; Puff and Renk, 2014) and higher levels of educational attainment independent of financial well-being (Respler-Herman et al., 2012), though Parkes et al. (2015) found increased stress among parents with both high and low levels of education compared to parents with intermediate levels and Nomaguchi and Brown (2011) also found increased stress among highly-educated parents. Parents who are parenting within the context of a partnered relationship generally have lower stress compared to single parents; this pattern holds for children of a variety of ages (Anderson, 2008; Cairney et al., 2003; Copeland and Harbaugh, 2005; Parkes et al., 2015).

Parents' stress is likely heightened by the interaction between their role as parent and other life domains. Parents experience significant financial pressures (Glass et al., 2016; Pollman-Schult, 2014) that increases their overall experiences of life stress (Puff and Renk, 2014). Employed parents also experience strain as a result of conflict between their role as parent and their role as employee (Braunstein-Bercovitz et al., 2012; Khan, 2014) and spillover of work stress into their parenting life (Malinen et al., 2017; Repetti and Wood, 1997). Increased relationship conflict (Nelson et al., 2014; Twenge et al., 2003) and feelings of social isolation (Skreden et al., 2012) have further been theorized to be associated with increased feelings of stress and decreased well-being for parents.

Many of these stressors became commonplace during the COVID-19 stay-at-home restrictions (Lee et al., 2021; Patrick et al., 2020). For example, parents who were sheltering in place may experience social isolation, while those that are juggling both working from home and assisting with children who are distance learning may feel enhanced role conflict. This stress could vary throughout the day, with spikes at particular times, such as during work and distance learning hours. In the current study, we were able to assess how parents’ stress levels changed throughout the day and those characteristics that varied with stress levels. Parents may find some relief or respite when they are able to spend even small amounts of time away from the home.

Stress and Parenting: COVID-19, Natural and Macroeconomic Disasters. The impact of other community-level disrupters—natural and macroeconomic disasters—may provide important insight into parental behavior when families are thus challenged. Natural disasters lead to closing schools and businesses, shifts in routines, decreased resources, and increased stress (Campbell et al., 2020); and macroeconomic events can lead to job loss and recession, similar to the pandemic event. Natural disasters and macroeconomic events contributed to parental disengagement and avoidance behavior such as refusal to talk about the situation (Cobham et al., 2016; Garfin et al., 2014; Kilic et al., 2003), use of substances to self-soothe (Kelley et al., 2010), and inability to monitor and support children (Hafstad et al., 2012; Motti-Stefanidi and Asendorph, 2017). Natural disasters, regardless of the type or location, were linked to fear-oriented maladaptive parenting strategies such as excessive parental control, reduction in child autonomy, increased protectiveness, and hypervigilance (Bokszczanin, 2008; Cobham and McDermott, 2014; Kelley et al., 2010; McFarlane, 1987; Pynoos, 1994). Macroeconomic events were associated with decreased parenting satisfaction and increased authoritative mothering (Leionen et al., 2002), and maternal harsh parenting (Lee et al., 2013). Family-level factors can mitigate the consequences of disasters. Education level was associated with coping and resource utilization, contributing to lower negative impacts (Muttarak and Lutz, 2014) and family resiliency (Hackbarth et al., 2012). Intimate partners were found to be both contributors to and alleviators of family stress (Reid and Reczek, 2011). Taken together, this research indicates that significant disruptions to work, school, and economic institutions such as those during the COVID-19 pandemic, may negatively impact parenting behaviors, potentially through increased parental stress. This suggests that children throughout the United States may be experiencing COVID-19 related maladaptive parenting. A more nuanced understanding of the factors associated with parental stress during the COVID-19 pandemic could reveal potential opportunities for preventive intervention.

Emerging research indicates that parent perceptions of the impact of COVID-19 are associated with increased parenting stress (Chung et al., 2020). The attempts to mitigate the transmission of COVID-19 have created challenges of financial insecurity, caregiver burden, and isolation that are likely to increase parent stress (Brown et al., 2020; Prime et al., 2020). Cumulative stressors resulting from COVID-19 were significantly and positively related to perceived stress (Brown et al., 2020), and poor work-family balance, economic uncertainty, and reduced social support experienced due to COVID-19 containment measures were linked to higher parenting stress (Carroll et al., 2020; Chung et al., 2020). Although mothers (Bikmazer et al., 2020) were at higher risk for distress, both parents in co-parenting households showed increased depression, anxiety, and stress (Achterberg et al., 2021; Calvano et al., 2021). Parents world-wide reported higher levels of stress after school closures (Calvano et al., 2021; Carroll et al., 2020; Davis et al., 2020; Hiraoka and Tomoda, 2020). Poor work-family balance may lead to higher levels of stress during the week when these role conflicts are likely to be highest with virtual schooling. Notably, these effects of increasing stress associated with COVID-19 appear independent of characteristics such as child's age (Chung et al., 2020; Spinelli et al., 2020) and the number of children in a household (Brown et al., 2020; Chung et al., 2020).

Current Study. Although we know parents have experienced significant stress during the COVID-19 pandemic, no studies to date have examined how parents’ stress levels fluctuate throughout the day and factors associated with that fluctuation. Examining this variation may help us identify small changes parents can make in their routine to help them better manage those stress levels. The research question for the current study is: How do time-varying, day-varying, and individual-level varying characteristics relate to at-the-moment stress during COVID-19 stay-at-home restrictions in Ohio? Based on the work presented here, we hypothesize (1) higher levels of stress later in the day when role conflict is likely to be highest; (2) higher levels of stress for those parents who spent time at work; (3) higher levels of stress in the time period after the April 20, 2020 announcement that K–12 schooling would remain virtual through the end of the 2019–20 academic year; and (4) lower stress (possibly due to reduced social isolation) after most retail businesses are allowed to be open in some fashion on May 12, 2020.

We use ecological momentary assessment (EMA) to identify factors related to at-the-moment stress for parents during a two-week time period during COVID-19 stay-at-home restrictions in Central Ohio. EMA is a research technique that pushes out brief surveys multiple times a day to understand how microcontexts might affect behavior (Freisthler et al., 2014). These factors are time-varying (e.g., time of day, location, presence of focal child), day-varying (e.g., day of week), or individual-varying (e.g., app version downloaded). We include measures that track specific COVID-19 restrictions, to assess their relationship to stress experience by parents.

2. Methods

Study Design. An ecological momentary assessment (EMA) study was conducted with parents of at least one child between the ages of 2 and 12 in Central Ohio during COVID-19 stay-at-home orders. As part of this study, parents responded to survey prompts three times a day (10 a.m., 3 p.m., 9 p.m.) for a total of 14 days. The 3–5 min brief surveys asked questions about level of stress, location where the survey was being taken, and whether the focal child had been with the parent since the last survey. Parents were recruited online through advertisements on Facebook and Craigslist and word of mouth. We posted those recruitment advertisements and began assessing initial eligibility via online surveys on April 11, 2020.

Eligibility was determined using a Qualtrics survey that assessed participant age (≥18 years), number of children between the ages of 2 and 12 (must have at least one), primary residence (must be in Central Ohio), availability of a smartphone to be used to for the research team to push out the brief surveys, and their contact information. Once eligibility was determined, the study team contacted the potential participant via email to set up a time to obtain verbal consent and provide the next steps of the study. Verbal consent was obtained for all participants. We enrolled our first participants on April 13th, about a month after Ohio's stay-at-home restrictions were ordered. Our first EMA surveys were conducted on April 14, 2020, with data collection ending on May 27, 2020.

As an incentive, participants received $1 for every brief survey completed, an extra $2 per day if all three EMA surveys were completed on a particular day, and an additional $10 for completing at least 90% (at least 37) of the surveys. The average number of surveys completed for those who provided verbal consent and completed at least one EMA survey, was 40.4 (range 18–42). IRB approval was obtained [BLINDED FOR REVIEW]. Participants were also asked to complete a baseline survey with information on demographics. These data are used to understand the characteristics of parents included in the sample. However, as they are not time-varying characteristics, we do not include them in the multivariate analyses.

Sample. Our final sample includes 332 individuals, who completed 13,360 brief surveys. We had 629 individuals begin the screening survey. 150 were found to be ineligible or did not complete the screening survey (e.g., dropped off when required to provide a first name), with the remaining 479 successfully screening into the survey. Of those that screened in, 342 consented to participate in the study, with 332 of those who consented completing at least one EMA brief survey. Our sample is primarily female (>90%), highly educated with greater than 40% of respondents having a graduate degree, and high income (30.8% of households had incomes with greater than $120,000). Focal children in our sample were primarily white (77.3%), with slightly more boys (56.5%) than girls (see Table 1).

Table 1.

Characteristics of analytic sample for study participants (N = 332).

Mean (SD) or % (n)
Parent Characteristics
 Parent Biological Sex
  Female 92.7 (306)
  Male 7.3 (24)
 Parent Age 37.5 (5.9)
  Marital Status
  Married or Living in a Marriage-Like Relationship 86.1 (285)
  Single/Widowed/Divorced 13.9 (46)
 Parent Education
  ≤ High School Diploma 3.3 (11)
  Some College 20.3 (67)
  Bachelor Degree 34.5 (114)
  Graduate Degree 41.8 (138)
Child Characteristics
 Child Biological Sex
  Female 43.3 (144)
  Male 56.5 (187)
 Focal Child Age 6.2 (3.0)
 Child Race/Ethnicity
Caucasian/White 77.3 (256)
African American/Black 12.7 (42)
  Other Race or Ethnicity 10.0 (33)
Family Characteristics
 Income
  ≤ $40,000 11.2 (36)
  $40,001 - $80,000 27.7 (89)
  $80,001 - $120,000 30.2 (97)
  > $120,000 30.8 (99)
 Number of Adults 2.0 (0.5)
 Number of Children 2.1 (1.0)

Measures. Our dependent variable—stress—was measured using a one-item question asking participants to rate their level of stress from 1 (low) to 10 (high) since the last survey, adapted from a study conducted by the American Psychological Association (2014). The specific question asks “How would you rate your stress since the last survey?” The one-item measure has face validity. Comparing this measure with the Perceived Stress Scale (10-items; Cohen et al., 1983), the one-item measure was highly positively correlated (ρ = 0.682; APA, 2014). The original responses were poorly distributed for Gaussian models, making the most efficient approach to using these data through an ordinal categorical model. We recoded the variables to five categories (see Table 2 ) in order to aid in interpretation and model convergence. On average, parents reported that their stress level was 3.25 out of 10.

Table 2.

Descriptive statistics for dependent and independent variables.

Variable Name Mean (SD) or % n
Stress
 Levels 1–2 45.7 6106
 Levels 3–4 30.3 4049
 Levels 5–6 14.2 1902
 Levels 7–8 7.3 980
 Levels 9 - 10 2.4 324
 Average Stress Level 3.25 (2.14) 13,321
Weekdays
 Sunday 14.4 1918
 Monday 14.3 1910
 Tuesday 14.3 1905
 Wednesday 14.2 1896
 Thursday 14.4 1920
 Friday 14.3 1906
 Saturday 14.3 1909
Locations
 Home 86.7 11,581
 Work 6.2 822
 Store 1.1 142
 Child Activities 0.8 107
 Traveling Between Places 2.1 284
 Other 3.1 423
With Focal Child during Survey Period
 Yes 91.4 12,209
 No 8.6 1151
Focal Child Age
 5 through 9 35.0 4658
 10 through 12 18.8 2498
Focal Child Biological Sex
 Female 43.3 5771
 Male 56.7 7558
 Number of Children 2.1 13,321
Trigger Time of Day
 10:00 a.m. 33.5 4476
 3:00 p.m. 33.3 4451
 9:00 p.m. 33.2 4434
Week of Assessment
 Week of April 12 3.0 395
 Week of April 19 22.8 3048
 Week of April 26 41.9 5595
 Week of May 3 26.7 3564
 Week of May 10 5.2 691
 Week of May 17 0.5 62
 Week of May 24 0.1 11
Application Version
 244 27.3 3647
 245 28.6 3819
 245.1 44.1 5895

In addition to stress, the time-varying characteristics used as independent variables included location where the parent was completing the survey, the trigger time for the survey, and whether a focal child was with the parent since the last survey. Locations included home (used as referent category in our study), work, store, child's activities (including at child's school), traveling between locations, or other (including restaurants). The majority of daily EMAs were completed with participants at home (86.7%, n = 11,581) and when the child had been with the parent during the survey time period (91.4%, n = 12,209). Daily characteristics included the day of the week (with Sunday as the reference group) and week of the study (reference group was week of April 12, 2020). The only participant-varying characteristic was the version of the app installed on the participant's cellular telephone. During the duration of our study, the research app we used released two versions to fix patches and provide additional functionality. Although we did not expect this variable to be statistically significant, we include it as a control variable where Version 4:04.0 is used as the reference group. Finally, we include two different dates we hypothesized to be related to parents' stress levels. Ohio Governor DeWine announced on April 20, 2020 that K–12 schooling would remain virtual through the end of the 2019–20 academic year. The second date, May 12, 2020, was the date most retail businesses were allowed to open with social distancing requirements in place, indicating greater freedom for families.

We also include specific information about the child and family as covariates in this model: age of the focal child, his or her biological sex, number of children in the family, number of adults in the family, and education of the parent. We recoded child age into three categories: ages 2 to 5; ages 6 to 9; and ages 10 to 12.

Data Analysis. For this study, we had observations (Level 1) nested within days (Level 2), nested within participants (Level 3). We used generalized ordered logit models (Williams, 2016) to account for clustering due to nesting of observations within days and participants and assess proportional effects for each covariate on the ordered outcome. This procedure incorporates a robust variance estimator that accounts for nesting of observations within days within participants. A key assumption of ordered logit models is that covariates have proportional effects across all levels of the dependent measure's ordered categories; in our case this is the five levels of stress. When this assumption is violated covariates may nevertheless be found to have effects related to one level of the ordinal outcome, but not others. To address this, we tested for non-parallel effects for all covariates with p < .05. If significant, we present tests for effects at each level within that block of measures, again assessing significance at p < .05.

We also conducted missing data analyses to assess how our results may differ due to missing data. By design, the 332 respondents included in the final analyses were to provide 3 assessments per day over 14 days (42 total) for a study total of 13,944 assessments. We obtained 13,360 such assessments (95.8% of the total expected). For the missing assessments, 49.2% of the respondents were missing one or more assessments and 27% were missing two or more assessments. The median number of missed assessments among those missing assessments was 6. There were few missing data among variables measured within assessments (less than 0.5% for any respondent) and no missing data within assessments for the 332 respondents included in the reported analyses. Missing data arose from missing assessments, not from missing responses within assessments.

Based on this information, we examined how our results differed among respondents who completed all their daily assessments vs. those who did not complete all of their assessments. However, it remains possible that systematic biases existed between respondents who did and did not miss assessments which could bias assessments of covariate relationships. To be certain, we conducted logistic regression analyses of missingness across assessments. We present our findings from these analyses in the results section below.

3. Results

As shown in Table 3 , Wald chi-square tests indicated that statistically significant relationships were observed between measured days of the week, parent locations, the presence of the focal child, one of the two COVID-19 orders, the trigger time of the survey, and the week the assessments occurred. A non-significant Wald test was found for the version of the app that was used by the participant. Daily stress varied by day of the week with parents reporting significantly more stress Monday through Friday (compared to Sunday). Parents completing the 9 p.m. survey (which could be completed between 9 p.m. and 1 a.m. the following day) reported significantly less stress compared to the 10 a.m. survey. Being at work (compared to being at home) was related to significantly higher levels of stress among our sample. Across all parents, stress levels increased progressively throughout the period, peaking in the last week observed.

Table 3.

Ordered logit models with tests for disproportional odds assessing relationships of five levels of stress with time- and situational-varying characteristics.

Covariate: Non-Parallel
Parameter Estimates:
Block Tests:
Effects:a b se p Wald χ2: P
Weekdays (Sunday ref) 70.53 <0.001
 Monday 0.325 0.061 <0.001
 Tuesday 0.362 0.073 <0.001
 Wednesday 0.440 0.080 <0.001
 Thursday 0.364 0.084 <0.001
 Friday 0.333 0.060 <0.001
 Saturday −0.026 0.049
Locations (Home ref) 15.72 0.008
 Work 0.464 0.153 0.002
 Store 0.093 0.200
 Child Activities −0.397 0.204
 Between Places −0.083 0.137
 Other 0.223 0.207
With Focal Child during Survey Period 1 0.411 0.147 0.005
2 0.201 0.144
3 −0.023 0.162
4 0.211 0.292
Focal Child Age (2–4 years ref) 12.19 0.002
 5–9 years −0.145 0.158
 10–12 years −0.759 .218 <0.001
Focal Child Biological Sex (female ref)
 Male 0.182 0.146
 Number of Children −0.066 0.077
Number of Adults in Household (Two Parents ref) 27.76 <0.001
 One Adult 1 .484 .282
2 1.052 .287 <0.001
3 1.229 .298 <0.001
4 1.488 .318 <0.001
 More than Two Adults .088 .366
Parents' Education (High School Grad or Less ref) 4.62
 Some College .482 .465
 Bachelor Degree .347 .454
 Graduate Degree .637 .450
COVID-19 Orders 9.68
 April 20, 2020: School Virtual 1 0.116 0.146
2 −0.163 0.163
3 −0.312 0.197
4 −0.095 0.343
 May 12, 2020: Retail Business Opens −0.124 0.197
Trigger Time of Day (10:00 a.m. ref) 33.21 <0.001
 3:00 p.m. 0.025 0.030
 9:00 p.m. −0.181 0.040 <0.001
Week of Assessment (April 12, 2020 ref) 46.16 <0.001
 Week of April 19 0.388 0.349
 Week of April 26 0.345 0.386
 Week of May 3 0.215 0.428
 Week of May 10 0.112 0.467
 Week of May 17 0.942 0.693
 Week of May 24 1 1.257 0.534 0.019
2 1.256 0.611 0.040
3 1.535 0.724 0.034
4 2.137 0.860 0.013
Application Version (244 ref) 0.24
 245 −0.092 0.191
 245.1 −0.072 0.193
Constants
 1 −0.540 0.503
 2 −1.451 0.496 0.003
 3 −2.200 0.514 <0.001
 4 −4.112 0.545 <0.001
a

Model Components with proportional odds/parallel lines assumption violated; Wald χ2 = 228.17, p < 0.001; effects indexed by ordered categories.

As also shown in Table 3, presence of the focal child and the occurrence of the early April COVID order both had disproportionate effects across the ordered set of stress categories. Presence of the focal child during the survey window was related to greater stress, but only at the lowest categories of stress (1 and 2). Stress was somewhat greater when the participant was with the child during the timeframe, but only among parents reporting relatively low levels of stress. The first of the two COVID-19 specific milestones (announcing schools will remain virtual through the remainder of the academic year) exhibited disproportional odds and an overall significant Wald test, but no specific effect was significant. There was some indication that this order affected low levels of stress once again.

Importantly, each successive week of the study (compared to the first week) was related to higher stress among parents; with this being statistically significant for the final weeks of the study. Note that this corresponds to when the Ohio Governor had begun to ease stay-at-home restrictions, including opening retail stores, bars, and restaurants for general public use. Those parents whose focal child was 10–12 years old reported lower levels of at-the-moment stress compared to parents with a focal child 2–4 years of age. Having one adult in the home is related to higher stress than two adults. The number of children under 18 years of age in the family, biological sex of the child, and parent's education was not related to at-the-moment stress for parents.

Missing Data Analysis Results. As described above, we assessed covariate relationships among respondents with one or more missing assessments, which may differ from those with no missing assessments. We conducted a complete case analysis, using only data from those respondents with all 42 assessments, to see how they may differ from the results including all respondents. While substantially reduced in power, with N = 169 instead of 332, the results of this complete case analysis showed the same well-supported effects with one exception: Statistical support for the age effect was weak, shifting to p = .1344. Nevertheless, age effect parameters from the complete case model were much the same as those from the full analysis; greatest stress was reported among parents of the youngest children.

It remains possible that systematic biases were to be found between respondents who did and did not miss assessments and these could bias assessments of covariate relationships. Logistic regression analyses of missingness across assessments revealed three differences: Missing assessments were more likely when reporting “between places,” earlier in the evening, and earlier in the day. A supplementary analysis of the full dataset, once reweighted using propensity scores for missingness at the assessment level, again showed that the same effects remained statistically well-supported.

4. Discussion

Our study sought to identify what at-the-moment situational characteristics were related to stress among parents during the COVID-19 pandemic. Although it is generally agreed that parents were likely under much higher levels of stress (APA, 2020a), we do not have much information on ‘real-time’ factors that might be affecting stress levels. In this study, we focused on situational (e.g., with child, location), time-varying (e.g., time of day), and day-varying (e.g., day of week) characteristics to assess when and where higher stress levels among parents occurred. Notably, our survey of parents reported lower levels of stress (average = 3.25 out of 10) than a study conducted by the APA (2020a), which found a mean of 6.7 among parents. One major difference in these studies is we asked parents about their level of stress three times a day, while the APA study asked parents to rate their stress for the past month. In our study, being with the child during the survey window was also related to higher levels of stress during COVID-19, but only at very low levels of stress.

Further, we found that specific locations of where the parent was when completing the brief surveys were related to stress levels. If parents were working, then they reported higher stress levels, which mathes prior work that suggests parents often experience stress due to conflicting roles as caregiver and employee (Braunstein-Bercovitz et al., 2012; Khan, 2014). The role of teacher during this time—as Ohio switched to virtual K–12 school—is likely to have only increased this conflict between roles. Given the unique circumstances of COVID-19, a portion of our parents could be working at home where their children are participating in virtual school. This would be the ultimate case of work-life spillover, which is known to increase stress for parents (Malinen et al., 2017; Repetti and Wood, 1997). If so, this location might be capturing the stress of trying to work at home with children present and, in many cases, assisting them in completing their schoolwork. If parents are not working from home when they completed this item, then the significant relationship with stress may indicate that being at work, during these unprecedented times may increase stress. This pattern may be especially true if our parents are essential workers, regardless of whether they are health care professionals, first respondents, or retail workers at grocery or big box stores. It may also be that work for parents in our study is inherently stressful and that this relationship would be found under more ‘usual’ parenting circumstances. However, other locations, such as being at a child's activity or traveling between locations were unrelated to stress or reduced stressed (as evidenced by the Wald test showing that there are well-supported differences in stress across locations).

Mirroring the findings from above, parents reported higher levels of stress on weekdays, compared to the weekends, in general. These findings are not surprising given the stressed experienced by parents to support and virtually teach their children, in the absence of daily on-site learning opportunities for schooling (APA, 2020a). On weekends, parents have greater freedom to arrange schedules or activities for their children that focus on enjoyable activities, rather than ensuring specific learning milestones occurred or homework was completed. We also found that parents' stress varied based on the time of day. We theorized that parents would report higher levels of stress later in the day, due to stress related to role conflict. We found no difference between stress at the morning and mid-day surveys. Our final survey was deployed at 9 p.m. and parents had until 1 a.m. the following day to complete it. Parents reported significantly less stress during that time period. Parents are likely to have been completing these surveys at the end of the day as parents are relaxing after their children have gone to bed. These lower levels of stress may be indicative of the relief that comes with having quiet time or extra time to one's self.

We should note, however, that these relationships of day of the week and time of day may exist even when parenting is not occurring during a pandemic. Work by Hibel et al. (2014), for example, documents a pattern of increased stress for parents working outside of the home on workdays compared to non-work days. Muscik et al. (2016) also demonstrate a pattern of mothers rating their time with their children as generally stressful albeit more enjoyable, than time without children present. Both results are likely to represent decreased stress as a result of a reduction in the daily hassles of parenting (Crnic and Greenberg, 1990). Additional research is needed in both crisis and non-crisis times on patterns of parental stress both across the course of their parenting days, including time with and away from their children, and across a parenting week, including school/non-school days.

We hypothesized that specific milestones during this COVID-19 time period would increase a parent's stress level. However, we did not notice an increase in stress after the Governor of Ohio announced that schools would not re-open and learning would remain virtual through the end of the academic year, as had been found in Japan (Hiraoka and Tomoda, 2020). Nor did we see a decrease in stress as parents had more freedom to go to retail stores, despite our hypothesis that this increased ability to move around might decrease social isolation among parents. As the pandemic continued to restrict movement and activities of families, we observed that stress levels increased compared to the first week we conducted the study (week of April 12, 2020). In particular, we see the highest levels of reported stress during the final week of our data collection period. This pattern may be a cumulative effect of having little to no respite from parenting duties, general concerns about staying safe during re-opening (APA, 2020b), or concern about keeping children safe as some activities began to resume, as suggested by Brown et al. (2020).

We find no difference in levels of stress for parents who reported on male vs. female focal child or based on the number of children in the family. These findings are similar to other COVID-19 studies examining parent stress (Brown et al., 2020; Chung et al., 2020). We did find that parents who had an older child, aged 10–12 years old, as the focal child had significantly lower stress than parents where the focal child was 2–4 years old. Here, our findings differ from those recent COVID-19 studies that show stress was not related to child age (Chung et al., 2020; Spinelli et al., 2020). Yet, our findings are similar to other studies that show higher stress among families with toddlers and younger children (Neece et al., 2012; Nelson et al., 2014; Skreden et al., 2012). The difference may be due to the daily nature of at-the-moment stress where parents are providing real-time assessments. In our study, daily interactions with younger children are likely to be at the forefront of a parent's mind as he or she completes the survey compared to a global measure of stress where parents are retrospectively thinking about a past month. These results should be interpreted with caution due to the results of one of our missing data analyses. We found no difference in stress by parents education level, which differs from previous work (cf. Nomaguchi and Brown, 2011; Parkes et al., 2015). Having only one adult was also related to higher levels of stress. This is not surprising as parents had to adapt to act in multiple roles, made more complicated if there is no one else to share responsibilities during the pandemic. Historically, single parents have higher levels of stress compared to partnered parents (Anderson, 2008; Cairney et al., 2003; Copeland and Harbaugh, 2005; Parkes et al., 2015).

Limitations. Our study is limited in a number of ways. We did not ask about specific stressors related to COVID-19, such as whether a parent had lost a job or faced reduced hours, whether they had to work from home or going in to work, or whether a family member had been diagnosed with the COVID-19. Instead, we had to rely on the government response to try and identify significant factors in that response that might be related to parents' at-the-moment stress level. As a convenience sample of families recruited primarily through web-based ads and word of mouth in central Ohio, these results may not be generalizable beyond this setting. Similarly, our sample is largely white, female, and highly educated. These parents are likely to have more financial means at their disposal to buffer some of the financial effects of the pandemic. Studies examining whether the time-varying situational contexts found to be important predictors of stress in this study remain important under ‘usual’ parenting circumstances. Future work should examine whether these same time-varying situational contexts are related to stress in parents or if different contexts are more stressful for parents. In order to keep the daily surveys brief, we used one item to assess stress level of parents. Although this item has face validity, a multi-item scale or more robust measures of stress may provide better information on a parent's stress level. We also only assessed a small number of situational characteristics (e.g., time of day, location of parent) and did not get an assessment during homework and dinnertime hours. Future efforts may want to incorporate additional characteristics that may increase stress for parents and add additional measurement time periods. Given the lack of diversity in our sample, we are unable to fully assess whether populations are differentially affected by the situational characteristics presented here. Conducting the study with participants who have different characteristics, possibly recruited through non-web strategies, would allow us to assess a broader range of situational characteristics that might affect parenting when under extreme stress.

One potential source of stress not measured here is reliable access to internet and availability of devices (e.g., computer, laptop, tablet) for children to obtain and complete assignments. Finally, we did not tease out whether parents who said they were at work during a survey time period were physically at work or working from home when the assessment was completed. Different stressors and stress levels during the pandemic might be present for parents who are conducting employment activities and monitoring virtual schooling, than those who are performing paid work outside of the home.

Practice Implications. Through technological advances, we demonstrated that parent stress levels can be assessed in real-time, which offers several potential possibilities for intervention. As parents were able to engage with the smartphone-based research study, it is likely that they would be amenable to potential smartphone app interventions. Providing parents with skills and tools to identify and reduce stress, such as apps monitoring heart rate or providing deep breathing techniques, may be one way of helping parents cope with extremely stressful situations. These interventions could assess stress and provide resources when parents report high scores and use geodata to notify parents of local resources or support services as they pass nearby.

Our findings also present implications for the continuing COVID-19 pandemic, or other future disasters that require sheltering-in-place. For example, parents need some form of respite (e.g. childcare, child-only activities) in order to reduce stress, especially during the week when parents are juggling their outside employment and their children's schooling. Locations or programs that would usually provide group activities (e.g., YMCA, youth sports leagues) might invest in identifying programming that maintains physical distance requirements and use of protective personal equipment (e.g., masks) for children. These activities could provide parents with a break and reduce their overall levels of stress. Foundations or other funding agencies (e.g., Department of Education) could subsidize these activities for low income and vulnerable children, youth, and families. Anecdotally, parents who have the financial ability to do so, are examining the possibility of using nannies during the school hours to help with virtual schooling to free up parents' time (Horn, 2020). This practice may further educationally harm children living in working class or poor homes that do not have the funds for this option. This may be exacerbated by whether the family has access to reliable internet (Stelitano et al., 2020). In a more equitable solution than private “pods,” public schools or non-profit agencies could form small groups of families to assist each other with childcare or distance learning, potentially providing a virtual staffed weekly “check-in” that would allow parents to process stress and emotions arising from sheltering-in-place with children. Expansion of the free lunch program to all students has been enacted in Ohio. This helps all families at least reduce the stress of having additional food on hand and is helpful for those families who are just above the income threshold for qualifying for free lunch. As stress can lead to a variety of health problems (McEwen, 2008; Health Resources and Services Administration Maternal and Child Health Bureau, 2018; Giallo et al., 2011; Skreden et al., 2012) and result in harsh parenting (Lorber, 2012; Malinen et al., 2017; Mikolajczak et al., 2019), identifying real-time situational contexts and developing strategies to change the environment that leads to these contexts may promote overall well-being in families.

5. Conclusions

Given the current context, much remains unknown about the immediate effects of the pandemic on families. Our work identifies a variety of factors that are related to higher levels of stress in parents. The parents and families in our study are not considered ‘at-risk’ and do not appear to be dealing with the same socio-economic challenges that many other families are facing. Yet, our study shows that even these relatively advantaged parents have been experiencing stress that is likely the result of change in routines and social connections due to COVID-19 restrictions (APA, 2020a; Chung et al., 2020). Although the specific milestones related to Ohio's stay-at-home orders were not related to higher levels of stress, we do identify situational characteristics that may serve to increase stress (e.g., being with the focal child). As such, this study provides a baseline of stress-levels found during the pandemic that can be compared to situational characteristics related to stress when life returns to something viewed as more ‘normal’ parenting conditions.

Credit author statement

Bridget Freisthler: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Data Creation, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. Paul J. Gruenewald: Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing. Erin Tebben: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. Karla Shockley McCarthy: Writing – original draft, review, & editing. Jennifer Price Wolf: Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Declaration of competing interest

None.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by The Ohio State University's Office of Research COVID-19 Seed Grant program.

References

  1. Achterberg M., Dobbelaar S., Boer O.D., Crone E.A. Perceived stress as mediator for longitudinal effects of the COVID-19 lockdown on well-being of parents and children. Sci. Rep. 2021;11(1):1–14. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-81720-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. American Psychological Association [APA] 2014. Stress in AmericaTM : Are Teens Adopting Adults' Stress Habits?https://www.apa.org/news/press/releases/stress/2013/stress-report.pdf [Google Scholar]
  3. American Psychological Association [APA] vol. 1. American Psychological Association; 2020. (Stress in America 2020: Stress in the Time of COVID-19). [Google Scholar]
  4. American Psychological Association [APA] vol. 2. American Psychological Association; 2020. (Stress in America 2020: Stress in the Time of COVID-19). [Google Scholar]
  5. Anderson L.S. Predictors of parenting stress in a diverse sample of parents of early adolescents in high-risk communities. Nurs. Res. 2008;57(5):340–350. doi: 10.1097/01.NNR.0000313502.92227.87. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Barroso N.E., Mendez L., Graziano P.A., Bagner D.M. Parenting stress through the lens of different clinical groups: a systematic review & meta-analysis. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2018;46(3):449–461. doi: 10.1007/s10802-017-0313-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bıkmazer A., Kadak M.T., Görmez V., Doğan U., Aslankaya Z.D., Bakır F., Öztürk M. Parental psychological distress associated with COVID-19 outbreak: a large-scale multicenter survey from Turkey. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatr. 2020 doi: 10.1177/0020764020970240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bischoff L.A., Spicker K. Dayton Daily News; 2020. Coronavirus Timeline: A Look at the Orders Changing Life in Ohio.https://www.daytondailynews.com/news/local/timeline-coronavirus-prompts-orders-changing-everyday-life-ohio/gpnVSADPxZxMltlDVyqKEP/ [Google Scholar]
  9. Bokszczanin A. Parental support, family conflict, and overprotectiveness: predicting PTSD symptom levels of adolescents 28 months after a natural disaster. Hist. Philos. Logic. 2008;21(4):325–335. doi: 10.1080/10615800801950584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Braunstein-Bercovitz H., Frish-Burstein S., Benjamin B.A. The role of personal resources in work–family conflict: implications for young mothers' well-being. J. Vocat. Behav. 2012;80(2):317–325. [Google Scholar]
  11. Brown S.M., Doom J.R., Lechuga-Peña S., Watamura S.E., Koppels T. Stress and parenting during the global COVID-19 pandemic. Child Abuse Negl. 2020;110:104699. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2020.104699. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cairney J., Boyle M., Offord D.R., Racine Y. Stress, social support and depression in single and married mothers. Soc. Psychiatr. Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2003;38(8):442–449. doi: 10.1007/s00127-003-0661-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Campbell A.M. An increasing risk of family violence during the COVID-19 pandemic: strengthening community collaborations to save lives. Forensic Sci. Int.: Report. 2020;2:100089. doi: 10.1016/j.fsir.2020.100089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Carroll N., Sadowski A., Laila A., Hruska V., Nixon M., Ma D.W.L., Haines J. The impact of covid-19 on health behavior, stress, financial and food security among middle to high income Canadian families with young children. Nutrients. 2020;12(8):1–14. doi: 10.3390/nu12082352. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Calvano C., Engelke L., Di Bella. J., Kindermann J., Renneberg B., Winter S.M. Families in the covid-19 pandemic: parental stress, parent mental health and the occurrence of adverse childhood experiences-results of a representative survey in germany. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2021 doi: 10.1007/s00787-021-01739-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Chung G., Chan X.W., Lanier P., Ju P.W.Y. Associations between work-family balance, parenting stress, and marital conflicts during COVID-19 pandemic in Singapore. OSF Preprints. 2020 doi: 10.1007/s10826-022-02490-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cobham V., McDermott B. Perceived parenting change and child posttraumatic stress following a natural disaster. J. Child Adolesc. Psychopharmacol. 2014;24(1):18–23. doi: 10.1089/cap.2013.0051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cobham V., McDermott B., Haslam D., Sanders M. The role of parents, parenting and their family environment in children's post-disaster mental health. Child Family Disaster Psychiat. 2016;18(53):1–4. doi: 10.1007/s11920-016-0691-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cohen S., Kamarck T., Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J. Health Soc. Behav. 1983;24:386–396. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Copeland D., Harbaugh B.L. Differences in parenting stress between married and single first time mothers at six to eight weeks after birth. Comprehen.Child Adolescent Nurs. 2005;28(3):139–152. doi: 10.1080/01460860500227556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Crnic K.A., Greenberg M.T. Minor parenting stresses with young children. Child Dev. 1990;61(5):1628–1637. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02889.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Davis C.R., Grooms J., Ortega A., Rubalcaba J.A-A., Vargas E. Distance learning and parental mental health during COVID-19. Educ. Res. 2020;50:61–64. [Google Scholar]
  23. Freisthler B., Lipperman-Kreda S., Bersamin M., Gruenewald P.J. Tracking the when, where, and with whom of alcohol use: integrating ecological momentary assessment and geospatial data to examine risk for alcohol-related problems. Alcohol Res. Curr. Rev. 2014;36:29–38. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Garfin D.R., Silver R.C., Gil-Rivas V., Guzmán J., Murphy J.M., Cova F., Rincón P.P., Squicciarini A.M., George M., Guzmán M.P. Children's reactions to the 2010 Chilean earthquake: the role of trauma exposure, family context, and school-based mental health programming. Psychol. Trauma: Theor. Res. Pract. Pol. 2014;6(5):563–573. [Google Scholar]
  25. Giallo R., Wade C., Cooklin A., Rose N. Assessment of maternal fatigue and depression in the postpartum period: support for two separate constructs. J. Reprod. Infant Psychol. 2011;29(1):69–80. [Google Scholar]
  26. Glass J., Simon R.W., Andersson M.A. Parenthood and happiness: effects of work-family reconciliation policies in 22 OECD countries. Am. J. Sociol. 2016;122(3):886–929. doi: 10.1086/688892. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hackbarth M., Pavkov T., Wetchler J., Flannery M. Natural disasters: an assessment of family resiliency following Hurricane Katrina. J. Marital Fam. Ther. 2012;38(2):340–351. doi: 10.1111/j.1752-0606.2011.00227.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hafstad G.S., Haavind H., Jensen T.K. Parenting after a natural disaster: a qualitative study of Norwegian families surviving the 2004 tsunami in Southeast Asia. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2012;21(2):293–302. doi: 10.1007/s10826-011-9474-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Health Resources and Services Administration Maternal and Child Health Bureau . 2018. National Survey of Children's Health.https://www.childhealthdata.org/ [Google Scholar]
  30. Hibel L.C., Trumbell J.M., Mercado E. Work/non-workday differences in mother, child, and mother-child morning cortisol in a sample of working mothers and their children. Early Hum. Dev. 2014;90(1):1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2013.11.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hiraoka D., Tomoda A. Relationship between parenting stress and school closures due to the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychiatr. Clin. Neurosci. 2020;74(9):497–498. doi: 10.1111/pcn.13088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Horn M.B. 2020. The Rapid Rise of Pandemic Pods. Education Next.https://www.educationnext.org/rapid-rise-pandemic-pods-will-parent-response-covid-19-lead-to-lasting-changes/ [Google Scholar]
  33. Kelley M.L., Self-Brown S., Le B., Bosson J.B., Hernandez B.C., Gordon A.T. Predicting posttraumatic stress symptoms in children following Hurricane Katrina: a prospective analysis of the effect of parental distress and parenting practices. J. Trauma Stress. 2010;25:582–590. doi: 10.1002/jts.20573. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Khan N. Family to work conflict among working mothers in UAE. Eur. Sci. J. 2014;10(20):205–216. [Google Scholar]
  35. Kilic E.Z., Ozenguven H.D., Sayil I. The psychological effects of parent mental health on children experiencing disaster: the experience of the Bolu Earthquake in Turkey. Fam. Process. 2003;42:485–495. doi: 10.1111/j.1545-5300.2003.00485.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Lee D., Brooks-Gunn J., McLanahan S.S., Notterman D., Garfinkel I. The great recession, genetic sensitivity, and maternal harsh parenting. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2013;110(34):13780–13784. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1312398110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Lee S.J., Ward K.P., Chang O.D., Downing K.M. Parenting activities and the transition to home-based education during the COVID-19 pandemic. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 2021;122:105585. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105585. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Leinonen J., Solantaus T., Punamäki R. The specific mediating paths between economic hardship and the quality of parenting. IJBD (Int. J. Behav. Dev.) 2002;26(5):423–435. [Google Scholar]
  39. Lorber M.F. The role of maternal emotion regulation in overreactive and lax discipline. J. Fam. Psychol. 2012;26(4):642–647. doi: 10.1037/a0029109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Malinen K., Rönkä A., Sevón E., Schoebi D. The difficulty of being a professional, a parent, and a spouse on the same day: daily spillover of workplace interactions on parenting, and the role of spousal support. J. Prev. Interv. Community. 2017;45(3):156–167. doi: 10.1080/10852352.2016.1198121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. McEwen B.S. Central effects of stress hormones in health and disease: understanding the protective and damaging effects of stress and stress mediators. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 2008;583(Issues 2–3):174–185. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2007.11.071. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. McFarlane A.C. Family functioning and overprotection following a natural disaster: the longitudinal effects of posttraumatic morbidity. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatr. 1987;21:210–218. doi: 10.3109/00048678709160914. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Meier A., Musick K., Fischer J., Flood S. Mothers' and fathers' well‐being in parenting across the arch of child development. J. Marriage Fam. 2018;80(4):992–1004. doi: 10.1111/jomf.12491. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Mikolajczak M., Gross J.J., Roskam I. Parental burnout: what is it, and why does it matter? Clinic. Psychol. Sci. 2019;7(6):1319–1329. [Google Scholar]
  45. Motti-Stefanidi F., Asendorpf J.B. Adaptation during a great economic recession: a cohort study of Greek and immigrant youth. Child Dev. 2017;88(4):1139–1155. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Musick K., Meier A., Flood S. How parents fare. Am. Socio. Rev. 2016;81(5):1069–1095. [Google Scholar]
  47. Muttarak R., Lutz W. Is Education a key to reducing vulnerability to natural disasters and hence unavoidable climate change? Ecol. Soc. 2014;19(1) doi: 10.5751/ES-06476-190142. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  48. Neece C.L., Green S.A., Baker B.L. Parenting stress and child behavior problems: a transactional relationship across time. Am. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 2012;117(1):48–66. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-117.1.48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Nelson S.K., Kushlev K., Lyubomirsky S. The pains and pleasures of parenting: when, why, and how is parenthood associated with more or less well-being? Psychol. Bull. 2014;140(3):846–895. doi: 10.1037/a0035444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Nomaguchi K.M., Brown S.L. Parental strains and rewards among mothers: the role of education. J. Marriage Fam. 2011;73(3):621–636. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2011.00835.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Nomaguchi K., Fettro M.N. Cohort differences in mothers' perceptions of neighborhood quality, child well‐being, and parental strain, 1976–2002. Fam. Relat. 2018;67(4):449–466. doi: 10.1111/fare.12327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Parkes A., Sweeting H., Wight D. Parenting stress and parent support among mothers with high and low education. J. Fam. Psychol. 2015;29(6):907–918. doi: 10.1037/fam0000129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Patrick S.W., Henkhaus L.E., Zickafoose J.S., Lovell K., Halvorsen A., Loch S., Letterie M., Davis M.M. Well-being of parents and children during the COVID-19 pandemic: a national survey. Pediatrics. 2020;146 doi: 10.1542/peds.2020-016824. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Pollmann-Schult M. Parenthood and life satisfaction: why don't children make people happy? J. Marriage Fam. 2014;76(2):319–336. [Google Scholar]
  55. Prime H., Wade M., Browne D.T. Risk and resilience in family well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic. Am. Psychol. 2020;75(5):631–643. doi: 10.1037/amp0000660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Puff J., Renk K. Relationships among parents' economic stress, parenting, and young children's behavior problems. Child Psychiatr. Hum. Dev. 2014;45(6):712–727. doi: 10.1007/s10578-014-0440-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Pynoos R.S. Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: A Clinical Review. Sidran Press; 1994. Traumatic stress and developmental psychopathology in children and adolescents; pp. 65–98. [Google Scholar]
  58. Reid M., Reczek C. Stress and support in family relationships after Hurricane Katrina. J. Fam. Issues. 2011;32(10):1397–1418. doi: 10.1177/0192513X11412497. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  59. Repetti R.L., Wood J. Effects of daily stress at work on mothers' interactions with preschoolers. J. Fam. Psychol. 1997;11(1):90–108. [Google Scholar]
  60. Respler-Herman M., Mowder B.A., Yasik A.E., Shamah R. Parenting beliefs, parental stress, and social support relationships. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2012;21(2):190–198. [Google Scholar]
  61. Skreden M., Skari H., Malt U.F., Pripp A.H., Björk M.D., Faugli A., Emblem R. Parenting stress and emotional wellbeing in mothers and fathers of preschool children. Scand. J. Publ. Health. 2012;40(7):596–604. doi: 10.1177/1403494812460347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Spinelli M., Lionetti F., Pastore M., Fasolo M. Parents' stress and children's psychological problem in families facing the COVID-19 outbreak in Italy. Front. Psychol. 2020;11:1713. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01713. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Stelitano L., Doan S., Woo A., Diliberti M., Kaufman J.H., Henry D. 2020. The Digital Divide and COVID-19: Teachers' Perceptions of Inequality and Participation in Remote Learning.https://www.rand.org/pubs/research_reports/RRA134-3.html Retrieved from. [Google Scholar]
  64. Twenge J.M., Campbell W.K., Foster C.A. Parenthood and marital satisfaction: a meta-analytic review. J. Marriage Fam. 2003;65(3):574–583. [Google Scholar]
  65. Umberson D., Pudrovska T., Reczek C. Parenthood, childlessness, and well-being: a life course perspective. J. Marriage Fam. 2010;72(3):612–629. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2010.00721.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Vierhaus M., Lohaus A., Schmitz A.-K., Schoppmeier M. Relationships between maternal parenting stress and reports on children's internalizing and externalizing problems: a cross-lagged structural equation model. J. Educ. Dev. Psychol. 2013;3(1):39–50. [Google Scholar]
  67. Williams R. Understanding and interpreting generalized ordered logit models. J. Math. Sociol. 2016;40:7–20. [Google Scholar]
  68. Williford A.P., Calkins S.D., Keane S.P. Predicting change in parenting stress across early childhood: child and maternal factors. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2007;35:251–263. doi: 10.1007/s10802-006-9082-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Social Science & Medicine (1982) are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

RESOURCES