Abstract
In two studies using samples representative of the US adult population with regard to age, sex and ethnicity, we investigated relationships between loving-kindness and compassion-based practices with speciesism, animal solidarity and desire to help animals. In a cross-sectional study (Study 1, N = 2,822), results showed that past 30 days practice and estimated lifetime number of hours of lovingkindness or compassion meditation were associated with more animal solidarity and greater desire to help animals. Past 30 days practice was also associated with less speciesism, but only when adjusting for sociodemographic characteristics. In an experimental study (Study 2, N = 1,102), results showed that participants randomized to a befriending meditation (a practice similar to loving-kindness and compassion meditation) condition scored higher on animal solidarity and desire to help animals than participants randomized to a control condition. No significant difference was observed on speciesism, but mediation analyses suggested that effects on all three outcomes were mediated through perceived commonality with animals.
Introduction
The nature of the relationship between humans and animals has consequences not only for animal welfare. For instance, the treatment of animals has been shown to influence the incidence and spread of animal diseases [1], which can have downstream effects on human health (but see [2]). Other research has also shown that the consumption of meat and other animal products contributes to global heating and biodiversity loss [3,4]. It is therefore important, even if only for anthropocentric reasons, to better understand how more positive human-animal relations can be encouraged.
Previous research has investigated the effects of various psychological interventions on human-animal relations. For example, imagining positive contact with a specific individual animal has been found to increase perceived commonality with the animal’s subgroup and induce greater prosocial intentions toward animals in general [5] (see also [6]). The imagined intergroup contact approach has been shown to reduce intergroup bias generally [7,8], with some evidence to suggest that perceived commonality with the outgroup may be a key mediator [9,10]. However, it may be difficult to scale such interventions in real-world settings and have large numbers of people voluntarily choose to imagine positive interactions with animals. Other psychological interventions that might be easily accessible and more commonly used should therefore be investigated.
Loving-kindness and compassion-based practices such as befriending meditation [11] might be an alternative to imagined intergroup contact. Such practices are similar to imagined intergroup contact in that different types of people are typically brought to mind. However, imagined intergroup contact involves imagining positive interactions with outgroup members while loving-kindness and compassion-based practices typically involve directly cultivating a sense of kindness and friendliness toward whoever has been brought to mind [12,13]. The evidence to date suggests that these types of practices can reduce political intergroup bias [14] (see also [15]), with effects mediated by perceived commonality with the political outgroup [16]. Other research has also shown that such practices may increase social and nature connectedness [17–19], but the relationship between loving-kindness and compassion-based practices and human-animal relations remain largely unexplored.
In two studies using online samples representative of the US adult population with regard to age, sex and ethnicity, we investigated relationship between loving-kindness and compassion-based practices and three measures related to human-animal relations: speciesism, animal solidarity and desire to help animals. In Study 1 (N = 2,822), we conducted a cross-sectional study to assess the associations between lifetime and current engagement with loving-kindness or compassion meditation and speciesism, animal solidarity and desire to help animals. We hypothesized that lifetime and current engagement with loving-kindness or compassion meditation would be negatively associated with speciesism and positively associated with solidarity with animals and desire to help animals. In Study 2 (N = 1,102), we conducted a pre-registered randomized controlled trial to investigate the effects of a brief befriending meditation on speciesism, animal solidarity and desire to help animals. We hypothesized that participants randomized to listen to a brief befriending meditation (a practice similar to loving-kindness and compassion meditation) would score lower on speciesism and higher on solidarity with animals and desire to help animals than participants randomized to listen to an active control condition. We also hypothesized that the effect of befriending meditation on speciesism, animal solidarity and desire to help animals would be mediated by perceived commonality with animals.
Study 1
Materials and methods
In Study 1, the sample was comprised of US residents (18 years or older) who were recruited in October 2021 through Prolific Academic (https://app.prolific.co), which is a platform that allows researchers to upload study advertisements and recruit participants for research. The platform offers a representativeness function–which was used in this study–that uses proportionate stratification on three census-matched factors–age (18–27, 28–37, 38–47, 48–57, 58+), sex (male, female), and ethnicity (White, Mixed, Asian, Black, Other)–to reflect the US adult population’s demographic distribution.
Design and procedure
All participants gave informed consent digitally (“Please click ‘Next’ to consent to participate in this study”) and responded to questions about sociodemographic characteristics (e.g., educational attainment, annual household income), speciesism [20] (α = .87 in Study 1), animal solidarity [21] (α = .96 in Study 1), and desire to help animals [22] (α = .77 in Study 1). Participants were also asked to report whether they had ever tried any type of meditation. Those who reported lifetime meditation use were asked to report whether they had ever tried loving-kindness or compassion meditation, including Metta, Compassion Cultivation Training, and Cognitively-Based Compassion Training. Participants who reported having been exposed to loving-kindness or compassion meditation were asked to report, on average, over the past 30 days how many days per week they engaged with loving-kindness or compassion meditation (1 = 0 days, 2 = 1 day, 3 = 2 days, 4 = 3 days, 5 = 4 days, 6 = 5 days, 7 = 6 days, 8 = 7 days) and estimated lifetime number of hours of loving-kindness or compassion meditation (1 = 0–10, 2 = 11–100, 3 = 101–500, 4 = 501–1000, 5 = 1001–5000, 6 = 5001+). Participants who reported no exposure to loving-kindness or compassion meditation were coded as 0 for both past 30 days practice and estimated lifetime number of hours. If the participant completed the study, they received $2.20. Study procedures were determined to be exempt from review by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Wisconsin-Madison. All procedures performed involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Statistical analyses
We used three linear regression models to evaluate associations between meditation-related variables and human-animal relations (i.e., speciesism, animal solidarity and desire to help animals). Model 1 did not adjust for covariates; Model 2 controlled for sociodemographic characteristics: age in years (18–25, 26–34, 35–49, 50–64 or 65 or older), gender (male, female, transgender/non-binary), ethnoracial identity (non-Hispanic White, non-Hispanic African American, non-Hispanic Native American/Alaska Native, non-Hispanic Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander, non-Hispanic Asian, non-Hispanic more than one race, Hispanic), sexual orientation (heterosexual, bisexual, gay/lesbian, other), educational attainment (some high school or less, high school graduate or equivalent, some college/community college degree, Bachelor’s degree or higher), annual household income (less than US$20,000, US$20,000–49,999, US$50,000–74,999, US$75,000 or more), marital status (married/living with a partner/in a long-term relationship, widowed, divorced/separated, not married/single); Model 3 controlled for the same covariates as Model 2, plus other variables that may be relevant to human-animal relations: diet (omnivore, pescatarian, vegetarian, vegan), pet ownership (yes, no), political affiliation (Republican, Democrat, Independent, other, none), and nature visits (every day, one to several times per week, one to several times per month, one to several times per year, never).
Results
Table 1 presents results from the three regression models testing the associations between meditation-related variables and human-animal relations. As demonstrated in the table, both past 30 days practice and estimated lifetime number of hours were modestly associated with more animal solidarity and greater desire to help animals in all three models. Past 30 days practice was also mostly associated with less speciesism, but only when adjusting for sociodemographic characteristics.
Table 1. Loving-kindness or compassion meditation practice and human-animal relations.
Speciesism | Animal Solidarity | Desire to Help Animals | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | p | β | p | β | p | |
Model 1 | ||||||
Past 30 days practice | -.02 | .312 | .09 | < .001 | .06 | .001 |
Lifetime practice | -.02 | .389 | .09 | < .001 | .06 | .001 |
Model 2 | ||||||
Past 30 days practice | -.04 | .031 | .09 | < .001 | .07 | < .001 |
Lifetime practice | -.03 | .094 | .10 | < .001 | .06 | .001 |
Model 3 | ||||||
Past 30 days practice | -.02 | .174 | .07 | < .001 | .05 | .006 |
Lifetime practice | -.01 | .497 | .07 | < .001 | .04 | .030 |
β = standardized coefficients. Note: Past 30 days practice and lifetime practice were entered into the models separately (see S1 Table in S1 File for descriptive statistics on past 30 days practice and lifetime practice).
Discussion
In Study 1, results showed that both past 30 days practice and estimated lifetime number of hours were associated with more animal solidarity and greater desire to help animals in all three models. Past 30 days practice was also associated with less speciesism, but only when adjusting for sociodemographic characteristics (Model 2).
There were limitations with the research design in Study 1 that should be considered when interpreting the findings. First, it is possible that the relationships between meditation-related variables and human-animal relations were confounded by variables that were not controlled for in the regression models (e.g., religious beliefs). The generally lower coefficients in Model 3 (as compared to Model 1 and 2) show that the included control variables had a noticeable impact on the results. Second, potential mechanisms linking loving-kindness or compassion meditation with human-animal relations were not investigated. Third, temporal sequence could not be established due to the cross-sectional research design, which means that the data cannot be used to infer causality. We aimed to address these limitations in Study 2. In this experimental study, we investigated the effects of a brief befriending meditation on speciesism, animal solidarity and desire to help animals, in a sample representative of the US adult population with regards to age, sex and ethnicity. We also examined perceived commonality with animals as a candidate mechanism.
Study 2
Materials and methods
In Study 2, the sample was comprised of US residents (18 years or older) who were recruited in July 2022 through Prolific Academic. The representativeness function that was used in Study 1 was also used in Study 2. The design plan, sampling plan, hypothesis and variables for this study were all pre-registered on the Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/rnpag/. Sample size was determined a priori using power analysis. We assumed a small effect size and found that a sample size of 1060 participants would achieve 80% power to detect an effect size of d = 0.20 with an alpha of .0166 (this alpha was chosen based on the Bonferroni correction to adjust for multiple comparisons) using an independent t-test. We therefore aimed to recruit approximately 1100 participants.
Design and procedure
We utilized a posttest-only randomized experimental design to test whether participants randomized into a befriending condition would score lower on speciesism and higher on animal solidarity and desire to help animals than participants randomized into an active control group. After giving their consent to partake in the study, participants were randomized to listen to a 10-minute befriending meditation (befriending condition) or to listen to an expert talking about meditation (control condition). Participants in the befriending condition were first instructed to bring friendship and kindness to themselves by repeating the phrase: “May I be free from suffering, may I be happy and healthy, may I have ease of being.” The participants were then instructed to bring friendship and kindness to a loved one, a stranger, a difficult person, and all living beings. Participants in the control condition were educated about mindfulness meditation, the neuroscience of mindfulness, and the evidence to date on mindfulness-based programs. The audio for both conditions were derived from recordings by Professor Mark Williams.
After listening to the audio recording, participants were presented with an attention check depending on which audio recording they had been assigned to (see Supporting Information for more information). Participants who gave the wrong answer on the attention check (n = 3) were excluded from data analyses. All participants also completed a manipulation check to ensure that the befriending condition had the desired effects. As a manipulation check, we assessed whether the befriending meditation induced feelings of kindness and good-will toward others more than the active control on a scale from 1 (Not at all) to 5 (Very much; see Supporting Information for more information).
After the attention and manipulation checks, participants were assessed on perceived commonality with animals on a scale from 1 to 7 using a modified version of the Inclusion of the Other in the Self Scale [23] (higher scores mean more perceived commonality; see Supporting Information for more information). Using the same measures described in Study 1, the participants were also assessed on speciesism (α = .88 in Study 2), animal solidarity (α = .97 in Study 2) and desire to help animals (α = .83 in Study 2). Participants were then asked to respond to questions about age, gender, first language, educational attainment, frequency of meditation practice, and equipment used to listen to the audio clip (see Supporting Information for more information) through Qualtrics. If the respondent completed the study, they received $1.74. Study procedures were determined to be exempt from review by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Wisconsin-Madison.
Statistical analyses
As specified in our preregistration, we performed an independent samples t-test to assess whether there were significant differences in human-animal relations (i.e., speciesism, animal solidarity and desire to help animals) scores across the two conditions. We also performed a pre-registered mediation analysis using Hayes’ (2017) [24] PROCESS macro (Model 4) with 5,000 bootstrapped estimates to assess whether perceived commonality with animals mediated the effects of the befriending meditation on speciesism, animal solidarity and desire to help animals.
Results
Although the participants were randomly assigned to one of the two conditions, we first examined whether the two groups were balanced on characteristics that could potentially influence the results. Independent samples t-tests revealed that there were significant differences across conditions in age (t(1100) = - 2.84, p = .005), with participants in the befriending condition reporting higher mean age in years (M = 46.57, SD = 16.09) than participants in the active control condition (M = 43.84, SD = 15.80). Chi-square test revealed that there were no significant differences across conditions in gender (χ2 (5, N = 1102) = 0.80, p = 977), having English as first language (χ2 (1, N = 1102) = 0.02, p = .896), education level (χ2 (7, N = 1102) = 7.97, p = .336), frequency of meditation practice (χ2 (6, N = 1102) = 8.67, p = .193), or the equipment used to listen to the audio clip (χ2 (2, N = 1102) = 4.67, p = .097).
Manipulation check
As a manipulation check, we assessed whether the befriending meditation induced feelings of kindness and good-will toward others more than the active control condition. An independent samples t-test revealed that there was a significant difference between conditions in feelings of kindness and good-will toward others (t(1100) = -10.25, p < .001, d = 0.62), with participants in the befriending condition reporting more kindness and good-will toward others (M = 3.72, SD = 1.00) than participants in the control condition (M = 3.05, SD = 1.15). This suggests that the manipulation had the desired effects.
Perceived commonality with animals
As specified in our preregistration, we assessed whether participants in the befriending condition would score higher on perceived commonality with animals than participants in the active control condition. An independent samples t-test revealed that there was a very small but significant difference between conditions in perceived commonality with animals (t(1100) = -1.98, p = .048, d = 0.12), with participants in the befriending condition reporting more kindness and good-will toward others (M = 4.65, SD = 1.70) than participants in the control condition (M = 4.45, SD = 1.71).
Human-animal relations
As specified in our preregistration, we assessed whether participants in the befriending condition would score lower on speciesism and higher on animal solidarity and desire to help animals than participants in the active control condition. An independent samples t-test revealed that there was no significant difference between conditions in speciesism (t(1100) = 1.89, p = .059, d = 0.11). An independent samples t-test revealed that there was a significant difference between conditions in animal solidarity (t(1100) = -2.81, p = .005, d = 0.17), with participants in the befriending condition reporting more animal solidarity (M = 5.13, SD = 1.49) than participants in the control condition (M = 4.88, SD = 1.54). An independent samples t-test revealed that there was a significant difference between conditions in desire to help animals (t(1100) = -2.77, p = .006, d = 0.17), with participants in the befriending condition reporting more desire to help animals (M = 5.78, SD = 1.07) than participants in the control condition (M = 5.60, SD = 1.17). None of the effects were moderated by demographics or other variables (i.e., age, gender, first language, educational attainment, meditation practice, equipment). The results remain largely unchanged when running linear regression models controlling for demographics and other variables. The effects on animal solidarity and desire to help animals both survived corrections for multiple comparisons.
Mediation
As specified in our preregistration, we performed a mediation analysis using Hayes’ (2017) [24] PROCESS macro (Model 4) with 5,000 bootstrapped estimates to examine whether perceived commonality with animals mediated the effects of the meditation intervention (treatment vs. control) on speciesism, animal solidarity and desire to help animals. A significant indirect effect was detected linking the meditation intervention with all three outcomes mediated through perceived commonality with animals. This was apparent when examining effects on speciesism (indirect effect B = -0.091, 95% bootstrapped confidence interval [CI] = -0.18, -0.0011, representing 56.3% of the total effect), animal solidarity (indirect effect B = 0.14, 95% CI = 0.0016, 0.27, representing 53.9% of the total effect), and desire to help animals (indirect effect B = 0.086, 95% CI = 0.0010, 0.17, representing 45.7% of the total effect).
Discussion
In Study 2, results showed that participants randomized to the befriending condition scored higher on animal solidarity and desire to help animals than participants randomized to the active control condition. No significant difference was observed across conditions on speciesism, but mediation analyses suggested that the effects on all three outcomes (i.e., speciesism, animal solidarity and desire to help animals) were mediated through perceived commonality with animals.
There were limitations with the research design in Study 2 that should be considered when interpreting the findings. First, this study only included posttest measures of human-animal relations. While such a research design can avoid potentially sensitizing participants to study hypotheses, it is possible that posttest differences in speciesism, animal solidarity and desire to help animals were due to pre-test differences. Second, the immediate effects of a brief befriending meditation were only assessed. It might therefore be valuable for future studies to investigate the short- and long-term effects of meditation interventions on human-animal relations. Third, participants in the befriending condition were instructed to bring friendship and kindness to all living being, which may have led to expectancy or demand effects.
General discussion
The present research investigated the links between loving-kindness and compassion-based practices such as befriending meditation and human-animal relations. In Study 1, results showed that both past 30 days practice and estimated lifetime number of hours were associated with more animal solidarity and greater desire to help animals. Past 30 days practice was also associated with less speciesism, but only when adjusting for sociodemographic characteristics. In Study 2, results showed that participants randomized to the befriending condition scored higher on animal solidarity and desire to help animals than participants randomized to the active control condition. No significant difference was observed across conditions on speciesism, but mediation analyses suggested that the effects on all three outcomes were mediated through perceived commonality with animals. Taken together, these results suggests that meditation practices designed to cultivate a sense of kindness and friendliness toward oneself and others can influence human-animals relations, with effects mediated by perceived commonality with animals. This may be especially promising in light of the growing use of meditation [25–27].
The results in this research project correspond with previous meditation studies on intergroup relations [28] and perceived commonality with others [16]. Even though the effects of a brief befriending meditation on human-animal relations may be small, meditation interventions are highly scalable and might therefore have meaningful effects when delivered to large groups of people through, for example, meditation apps, which are the most popular mental health apps [29] and the most common means of learning how to meditate [30]. It is also worth noting that the befriending meditation was a very minimal intervention and effects may be larger with ongoing or more intensive practice. Future research should investigate the relative effects of short and long-term meditation practice and could utilize a naturalistic study design to better understand the potential effects of meditation practice in society.
The findings open the possibility of investigating the effects on human-animal relations of other interventions that dissolve the perceived difference between self and others. For example, classic psychedelics such as psilocybin have recently been examined as a potential treatment for certain psychiatric disorders [31]. Such substances reliably reduce the sense of self at higher doses [32] and have been shown to increase nature connectedness [33], but recent evidence suggests it might also influence human-animal relations [34]. Future research should therefore examine the potential effects on human-animal relations of classic psychedelics and other interventions with similar mechanisms of action.
The present research project utilized two different research designs to evaluate the links between loving-kindness and compassion-based practices such as befriending meditation and human-animal relations. There are, however, limitations to consider when interpreting the findings in both studies. First, even though the samples were representative of the US adult population with regards to age, sex and ethnicity, they might not have been representative on other unmeasured variables. Second, the dependent variables were self-report measures which are vulnerable to response biases. Future research should recruit samples that are representative on more variables and utilize more objective measures of human-animal relations (e.g., willingness to donate money to organizations related to animal rights, behaviors associated with human-animal relations).
Conclusions
In sum, the findings suggest that loving-kindness and compassion-based practices such as befriending meditation can influence human-animal relations, with effects mediated by perceived commonality with animals. The results contribute to a small but growing literature on human-animal relations and indicate that different types of meditation may be a highly scalable means to change attitudes toward animals, which may lead to more sustainable ways of living.
Supporting information
Data Availability
Data and syntax are available at OSF:https://osf.io/u5mc7/.
Funding Statement
OS was supported by Ekhaga Foundation (2021-59). SG was supported by a grant (K23AT010879) from the National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health and by a Defeating Depression Award from the Hope for Depression Research Foundation. This study was funded by FORMAS, Swedish Research Council for Sustainable Development (Grant number: FR-2018/0006), and Haga Foundation. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Magouras I, Brookes VJ, Jori F, Martin A, Pfeiffer DU, Dürr S. Emerging zoonotic diseases: Should we rethink the animal–human interface?. Frontiers in veterinary science. 2020. Oct 22;7:582743. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2020.582743 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Messenger AM, Barnes AN, Gray GC. Reverse zoonotic disease transmission (zooanthroponosis): a systematic review of seldom-documented human biological threats to animals. PloS one. 2014. Feb 28;9(2):e89055. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0089055 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Henry RC, Alexander P, Rabin S, Anthoni P, Rounsevell MD, Arneth A. The role of global dietary transitions for safeguarding biodiversity. Global Environmental Change. 2019. Sep 1;58:101956. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Stoll-Kleemann S, Schmidt UJ. Reducing meat consumption in developed and transition countries to counter climate change and biodiversity loss: a review of influence factors. Regional Environmental Change. 2017. Jun;17(5):1261–77. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Auger B, Amiot CE. The impact of imagined contact in the realm of human-animal relations: Investigating a superordinate generalization effect involving both valued and devalued animals. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2019. Nov 1;85:103872. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Cerrato S, Forestell CA. Meet your meat: The effect of imagined intergroup contact on wanting and liking of meat. Appetite. 2022. Jan 1;168:105656. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2021.105656 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Crisp RJ, Turner RN. The imagined contact hypothesis. Advances in experimental social psychology. 2012. Jan 1;46:125–82. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Miles E, Crisp RJ. A meta-analytic test of the imagined contact hypothesis. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. 2014. Jan;17(1):3–26. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Vezzali L, Birtel MD, Di Bernardo GA, Stathi S, Crisp RJ, Cadamuro A, Visintin EP. Don’t hurt my outgroup friend: A multifaceted form of imagined contact promotes intentions to counteract bullying. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. 2020. Aug;23(5):643–63. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Wojcieszak M, Warner BR. Can interparty contact reduce affective polarization? A systematic test of different forms of intergroup contact. Political Communication. 2020. Nov 1;37(6):789–811. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Williams M, Penman D. Mindfulness: a practical guide to finding peace in a frantic world. Hachette UK; 2011. May 5. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Galante J, Galante I, Bekkers MJ, Gallacher J. Effect of kindness-based meditation on health and well-being: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology. 2014. Dec;82(6):1101. doi: 10.1037/a0037249 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Gu X, Luo W, Zhao X, Chen Y, Zheng Y, Zhou J, et al. The effects of loving‐kindness and compassion meditation on life satisfaction: A systematic review and meta‐analysis. Applied Psychology: Health and Well‐Being. 2022. May 9. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12367 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Simonsson O, Narayanan J, Marks J. Love thy (partisan) neighbor: Brief befriending meditation reduces affective polarization. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. 2022. Sep;25(6):1577–93. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Simonsson O, Bazin O, Fisher SD, Goldberg SB. Effects of an 8-Week Mindfulness Course on Affective Polarization. Mindfulness. 2022. Feb;13(2):474–83. doi: 10.1007/s12671-021-01808-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Simonsson O, Goldberg SB, Marks J, Yan L, Narayanan J. Bridging the (Brexit) divide: Effects of a brief befriending meditation on affective polarization. PloS one. 2022. May 11;17(5):e0267493. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0267493 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Aspy DJ, Proeve M. Mindfulness and loving-kindness meditation: Effects on connectedness to humanity and to the natural world. Psychological reports. 2017. Feb;120(1):102–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Hutcherson CA, Seppala EM, Gross JJ. Loving-kindness meditation increases social connectedness. Emotion. 2008. Oct;8(5):720. doi: 10.1037/a0013237 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Kok BE, Singer T. Effects of contemplative dyads on engagement and perceived social connectedness over 9 months of mental training: A randomized clinical trial. Jama psychiatry. 2017. Feb 1;74(2):126–34. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.3360 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Piazza J, Ruby MB, Loughnan S, Luong M, Kulik J, Watkins HM, et al. Rationalizing meat consumption. The 4 Ns. Appetite. 2015. Aug 1;91:114–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Amiot CE, Bastian B. Solidarity with animals: Assessing a relevant dimension of social identification with animals. PloS one. 2017. Jan 3;12(1):e0168184. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0168184 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Amiot CE, Sukhanova K, Bastian B. Social identification with animals: Unpacking our psychological connection with other animals. Journal of personality and social psychology. 2020. May;118(5):991. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000199 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Aron A, Aron EN, Smollan D. Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1992. Oct;63(4):596. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. Guilford publications; 2017. Oct 30. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Black LI, Barnes PM, Clarke TC, Stussman BJ, Nahin RL. Use of yoga, meditation, and chiropractors among US children aged 4–17 years. US Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Health Statistics; 2018. Nov 1. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Clarke TC, Barnes PM, Black LI, Stussman BJ, Nahin RL. Use of yoga, meditation, and chiropractors among US adults aged 18 and over. US Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Health Statistics; 2018. Nov 1. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Simonsson O, Martin M, Fisher S. Sociodemographic characteristics and health status of mindfulness users in the United States. Mindfulness. 2020. Dec;11(12):2725–9. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Oyler DL, Price-Blackshear MA, Pratscher SD, Bettencourt BA. Mindfulness and intergroup bias: A systematic review. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. 2022. Jun;25(4):1107–38. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Wasil AR, Gillespie S, Patel R, Petre A, Venturo-Conerly KE, Shingleton RM, et al. Reassessing evidence-based content in popular smartphone apps for depression and anxiety: Developing and applying user-adjusted analyses. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2020. Nov;88(11):983. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000604 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Simonsson O, Fisher S, Martin M. Awareness and experience of mindfulness in Britain. Sociological Research Online. 2021. Dec;26(4):833–52. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Nutt D, Carhart-Harris R. The current status of psychedelics in psychiatry. JAMA psychiatry. 2021. Feb 1;78(2):121–2. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.2171 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Nour MM, Evans L, Nutt D, Carhart-Harris RL. Ego-dissolution and psychedelics: validation of the ego-dissolution inventory (EDI). Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2016. Jun 14;10:269. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00269 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Kettner H, Gandy S, Haijen EC, Carhart-Harris RL. From egoism to ecoism: Psychedelics increase nature relatedness in a state-mediated and context-dependent manner. International journal of environmental research and public health. 2019. Dec;16(24):5147. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16245147 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Pöllänen E, Osika W, Stenfors CU, Simonsson O. Classic psychedelics and human–animal relations. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022. Jul 1;19(13):8114. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19138114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Data and syntax are available at OSF:https://osf.io/u5mc7/.