Abstract
The social isolation and the subsequent, increased use of Social Networking Sites due to the COVID-19 pandemic have had an impact on subjective well-being around the world. The present longitudinal study examined whether changes in psychological distress and well-being during the Italian second wave of the pandemic differ among people with different levels of Problematic Facebook Use (PFU). A total of 493 participants (Mage = 24.55 ± 7.25; 80.3% females) completed measures of passive use of Facebook, social comparison orientation on Facebook, fear of missing out, psychological distress (depressive symptoms and fear of COVID-19 pandemic) and well-being across three waves. Latent class analysis (LCA) categorized participants into three groups with different PFU levels: healthy users, moderate PFU users, and high PFU users. Hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) showed that the between-person level (class membership) accounted for most of the variability in psychological distress and well-being. No significant changes were found in psychological distress and well-being over time, but the High PFU users showed greater levels of psychological distress and lower levels of well-being at each time point. The findings of this study suggest that the relationship between PFU, psychological distress and well-being may reflect trait-like time-invariant differences between individuals rather than state-like changes.
Subject terms: Psychology, Health care
Introduction
The COVID-19 pandemic enhanced the individual’s use of social media and increased the risk of acquiring addictive tendencies1. In the last few years, the restrictions aimed at lessening the spread of the virus have resulted in social distancing, curfews, and shelter-in-place orders across the globe, all of which has led to limited interpersonal and close relationships. During this difficult time, people were overwhelmed by the continual desire to stay connected with others and improve interpersonal communication, and this need was easily satisfied by using social media, such as Facebook2,3. However, there is a debate on the consequences of heavy Facebook use for an individual’s well-being4,5, and whether problematic Facebook use (PFU), defined as a lack of self-regulation in one’s own use of Facebook, leading to problems in the user’s life6,7, can be conceptualized as dysfunctional behavior. Although Facebook might prove valuable in enhancing social contact, receiving positive feedback (there is no dislike button on this platform) and by enhancing social capital8,9, PFU is considered as a dysfunctional use of Facebook; it has been related to clinical impairments in various areas of one’s life, such as increased psychological distress and sense of loneliness, decreased self-esteem and life satisfaction10–13. Although PFU is associated with time spent online14, frequency of Facebook use does not seem to capture the core issues related to PFU15. Prior research adopted the broader construct of Problematic social media use (PSMU), which refers to an enduring preoccupation with social media (e.g., Facebook, Instagram, Tik-Tok) that can lead to impairment in well-being16 and/or nervousness, loneliness and other psychological problems17. However, focusing on specific social media platforms, such as Facebook, more than broad umbrella-constructs can be helpful to understand this complex phenomenon in more depth.
Previous meta-analytic evidence showed that PFU is associated with excessive or problematic Internet use15, but probably boasting distinctive features. For example, the hypothesis for passive social media use (e.g., scrolling through news feeds or looking at other users’ profiles, without engaging in direct social interactions) posits that passive Facebook use can lead to a decline in well-being18,19. Recent reviews and meta-analytic evidence, from both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies, supported a negative association between passive social media use (i.e., content consumption, browsing with low social connection) and well-being outcomes4,5,20.
Passive Facebook use might bring about negative feelings or distress because it may induce upward social comparison regarding dimensions that are important to one’s self-worth and social connectivity21,22. Contents posted by others are usually positively skewed, and social comparison can make readers feel negative about their own lives23,24. The meta-analysis by Yoon and colleagues25 showed that social comparisons on Facebook were more strongly related to depression than was the time actually spent, suggesting that an extensive use of SNSs might lead individuals to compare themselves with other users in a negative way, thus resulting in lower subjective well-being.
Prior research suggested that Fear of Missing Out (FoMO; i.e., “a pervasive apprehension that others might be having rewarding experiences from which one is absent”, [26; p. 1841] may also represent a reinforcement mechanism of PFU27. Problematic Facebook users experiencing unwanted feelings or a sense of loneliness when they use Facebook might report increased levels of FoMO2,28,29. Specifically, individuals who are afraid of being excluded from the world of Facebook and who are in situations of physical isolation might increase widespread use or PFU30.
To sum up, previous research suggested that the mental health consequences of SNSs such as Facebook may critically depend on the way they are used8,31. PFU encompasses different domains (e.g., passive use, social comparison, FoMO) which are related to the individual’s need for relatedness, which may lead to lower well-being or distress. However, early research into PFU-well-being effects relied primarily on cross-sectional data6 and did not take into account how these effects varied in magnitude between individuals32,33. Thus, examining how PFU characteristics co-occur may provide a valuable research option. Prior research has utilized clustering techniques to identify distinct groups and patterns of problematic social media use34–37. In the current study we examine different patterns of PFU by Latent Class analysis (LCA) and whether these empirically-derived subgroups differ on both well-being and psychological distress over time. LCA derives a set of latent variables from a series of observed variables and allocates them to a latent class. Thus, this method can be useful in exploring the multifaceted nature of PFU and revealing its maladaptive patterns.
In the present investigation we focus on the link between patterns of PFU and well-being during the COVID-19 outbreak, which has thus far not received extensive research attention. Although people have been using Facebook heavily for sharing COVID-19 information38, prior studies showed an association between problematic social media or passive use, online social comparison, FoMO with different facets of an individual’s distress during the first wave of the pandemic1,2,22,32,39,40. As previously mentioned, PFU takes place when the individual is characterized by a lack of self-regulation in one’s use, with negative consequences in daily activities, social relationships, and psychological health15. During the pandemic, problematic Facebook use appeared to foster home confinement41 and internet-associated risky behaviors42. Sayeed and colleagues43 identified several risk factors for the development of PFU during the Covid-19 pandemic, such as having relationship break-ups or a history of violence and stressful life events.
However, it is worth noting that no prior longitudinal studies examined the relationship between patterns of PFU, well-being and distress during the second wave of the COVID-19 pandemic.
The present longitudinal study examined (i) whether distinct patterns of PFU could be identified through LCA on the basis of the following indicators: Facebook passive use, social comparison on Facebook, FoMO, and numbers of Facebook friends; and (ii) whether both moment-related evaluations, as well as changes in both psychological distress (i.e. depressive symptoms and Fear of COVID-19 pandemic) and well-being, differ among people with different patterns of PFU. According to prior evidence regarding positive associations between PFU and psychological distress2,4,19,29,44,45, it was hypothesized that the group with a higher dysfunctional pattern of PFU would have greater psychological distress and lower well-being across different stages of the second wave of pandemic.
Methods
Participants and procedure
Participants comprised college students at two large Universities in southern Italy. Four hundred and ninety-four participants consented to participate and completed an online survey at T0 (October 2020, a pandemic time in which new restrictions were implemented because of the spread of the second wave of COVID-19). Of these, two hundred and twenty-four participants (response rate 45.3%) completed the survey at T1 (December 2020, a period characterized by tightened containment measures and “red zones” for the Christmas holidays) and one hundred and ninety-one participants (response rate 38.7%) completed the survey at T2 (February 2021; a period characterized by the easing of restrictions). One participant was identified as a univariate outlier and was subsequently excluded from the analyses (see the Results section), thus 493 (80.3% females; Mage = 24.55 ± 7.25; age range = 18–63), 224 (78.1% females; Mage = 24.49 ± 6.61; age range = 18–57) and 190 (82.1% females; Mage = 25.00 ± 6.97; age range = 18–57) participants were considered for the three waves, respectively. Since we kept missing data points when matching the data for the three waves, the analytical sample included 493 participants. They were recruited through an announcement in the University and via on-line advertisements. Participation was voluntary and participants received no compensation. Information about the objectives of the study was given to the participants, and a prior statement of informed consent to participate was obtained from each participant. The online questionnaire took approximately 15–20 min to be completed. The research was conducted in accordance with the ethical standards of the Italian Psychological Association (AIP), as well as the Declaration of Helsinki. Participants’ demographic information and health-related data, as reported in Table 1.
Table 1.
Variable | Participants (N = 493) |
---|---|
Age, M (SD) | 24.55 (7.25) |
Gender, n (%) | |
Females | 396 (80.3) |
Males | 97 (19.7) |
Educational level, n (%) | |
8 years of education | 12 (2.4) |
13 years of education | 265 (53.8) |
Degree/post-degree | 216 (43.8) |
Marital status, n (%) | |
In a relationship/married | 267 (54.2) |
Single/divorced/widowed | 226 (45.8) |
Personal COVID-19 infection, n (%) yes | 18 (3.7) |
COVID-19 infection among relatives/friends, n (%) yes | 154 (31.2) |
Measures
At each wave, the first part of the questionnaire was used to collect information about participants’ demographic characteristics, including age, gender, educational level and marital status. In the next part, some questions about personal and relatives’ experiences of COVID-19 infection were inserted (in order to describe participants’ exposure to the virus as well as to account for the relationship that these variables may have with the variables of the study, such as depression and fear of COVID-19)46. Finally, data about PFU (i.e., number of Facebook friends, social comparison orientation on Facebook, passive use of Facebook and FoMO), psychological distress (i.e., depressive symptoms and fear of COVID-19 pandemic) and well-being were collected.
Social comparison orientation on Facebook
The Iowa-Netherlands Comparison Orientation Measure (INCOM)47 is an 11-item self-report measure of social comparison orientation (e.g., “I often compare myself with respect to what I have accomplished in life”). For the purposes of the current study, the scale was adapted by asking participants to think about the social interactions and behavior that are established on Facebook (e.g., “When I use Facebook, I often compare myself with respect to what I have accomplished in life”). Items were rated on a 5-point Likert scale, from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 5 (Strongly Agree) with higher overall scores indicating a greater Facebook Social comparison orientation40. The scale demonstrated good internal consistency in the present study at each time-point (Cronbach’s α = 0.84, 0.87 and 0.85 for T0, T1 and T2, respectively).
Passive use of Facebook
The Active and Passive Facebook Use Scale (APUF)18 is a 7-item self-report measure of passive use of Facebook. Participants were asked to rate the frequency of use for some Facebook activities (e.g. “Reading posts”) on a 7-point Likert scale, from 1 (Never to) to 7 (More than once a day) with higher overall scores indicating a greater passive use of Facebook. The scale demonstrated acceptable-to-good internal reliability in the present study at each time-point (Cronbach’s α = 0.81, 0.81 and 0.79 at T0, T1 and T2, respectively).
Fear of missing out
The Fear of Missing Out scale (FoMOs)26,48 is a 10-item self-report measure of fear of missing out (e.g. “I get anxious when I don’t know what my friends are up to”). Items were rated on a 5-point Likert scale, from 1 (Not at all true of me) to 5 (Extremely true of me) with higher overall scores indicating more severe fear of missing out. The scale demonstrated good internal consistency in the present study at each time-point (Cronbach’s α = 0.82, 0.84 and 0.84 at T0, T1 and T2, respectively).
Depressive symptoms
The 7-item Depression subscale (DASS-D) of the Italian adaptation of Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale (DASS-21)49,50 was used to measure depressive symptoms (e.g.“I felt down hearted and blue”). Items were rated on a 4-point Likert scale, from 0 (It's never happened to me) to 3 (It's happened to me most of the time) with higher overall scores indicating more severe depression. The scale demonstrated excellent internal consistency in the present study at each time-point (Cronbach’s α = 0.91, 0.91 and 0.93 for T0, T1 and T2, respectively).
Fear of COVID-19 pandemic
The Multidimensional Assessment of COVID-19-Related Fears (MAC-RF)45 is an 8-item self-report measure of fear of COVID-19 pandemic (e.g., “During the coronavirus pandemic I constantly feel that I have to do something”). Items were rated on a 5-point Likert scale, from 0 (Strongly Disagree) to 4 (Strongly Agree) with higher overall scores indicating more severe fear of the COVID-19 pandemic. In the present study, the scale demonstrated acceptable-to-good internal consistency in the present study at each timepoint (Cronbach’s α = 0.75, 0.80 and 0.77 at T0, T1 and T2, respectively).
Well-being
The Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS)51,52 is a 5-item self-report measure of well-being (e.g., “If I could live my life over, I would change almost nothing”). Items were rated on a 5-point Likert scale, from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 7 (Strongly Agree) with higher overall scores indicating a greater well-being. The scale demonstrated good-to-excellent internal reliability in the present study at each timepoint (Cronbach’s α = 0.88, 0.87 and 0.90 at T0, T1 and T2, respectively).
Statistical analysis
Data analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS (v. 22), Mplus (v. 7.0) and HLM software (v. 8.2). As a preliminary step in the data analysis, attrition analysis was conducted in order to compare participants with complete data with those with missing data at T1 and/or T2. Cronbach’s alphas were computed for all scales in order to assess their internal consistency. The normality of continuous variables was checked examining their skewness and kurtosis values. Descriptive statistics (means and standard deviations for continuous variables and frequencies and percentages for categorical variables) were computed for demographics and variables of interest.
As a first step in the data analysis, Latent Class Analysis (LCA) was conducted in order to classify the participants into different groups according to their PFU (i.e., number of Facebook friends, Passive use of Facebook, Social Comparison Orientation on Facebook and FoMO) at T0. We ranked models containing the one to four latent class to find a more meaningful and parsimonious model. The following fit indicators were examined to determine how many groups should be classified: Bayesian information criterion (BIC), sample size adjusted BIC (aBIC), entropy, Lo-Mendell Rubin likelihood ratio test (LMR LRT), and bootstrap likelihood ratio test (BLRT). The most suitable model had the following fit indices: BIC and aBIC should be lower; entropy should be larger and LMR and BLRT should be significant53. Moreover, the clinical meaning of the latent classes was also considered when selecting the model.
As a second step in the data analysis, we tested for the presence of significant linear changes in psychological distress (i.e., depressive symptoms and fear of COVID-19 pandemic) and well-being, from baseline to 4-months later, using 2-level Hierarchical Linear Models (HLMs). HLMs are considered one of the best statistical techniques for examining longitudinal changes in nested data54. Then, we entered the classes of participants -obtained through LCAs- as predictors of the longitudinal changes in psychological distress and well-being. This allowed us to test whether participants in specific classes experienced different time slopes compared to those from other classes. In addition, we compared the levels of each dependent variable at T1 and T2 across the classes through HLMs, changing how time was coded in our models (i.e., for comparisons at T1, time was coded as “− 1”, “0” and “1” for the three time points, respectively; for comparisons at T2, it was coded as “− 2”, “− 1” and “0”) and testing for significant group differences at the Intercept.
Effect sizes indicating the proportion of within-person variance were accounted for by adding the linear parameter and were assessed and reported using pseudo-R255. Their magnitude was interpreted according to guidelines (0.01 = small, 0.06 = medium, > 0.14 = large)56.
Ethics
The study was conducted in accordance with the ethical standards of the Italian Psychological Association (AIP), as well as the Helsinki Declaration of 1975 and its later amendments. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants to be included in the study.
Institutional review board statement
The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Palermo (Ethic Committee approval code: 37/2021).
Results
Preliminary analyses
At the baseline, no significant differences on demographics (i.e., age, gender and marital status), health-related data (i.e., personal and relatives COVID-19 infection), PFU characteristics (i.e., social comparison on Facebook, Facebook passive use and FoMO), psychological distress (i.e., depressive symptoms and fear of COVID-19 pandemic) or well-being at T0 were found between participants with complete data on all waves and those with missing data at T1 and/or T2. Significant differences were found only for educational level and number of Facebook friends. The normality of continuous variables was checked, and a positive skewed distribution was found for the number of Facebook friends. One univariate outlier was removed, and square root transformation was conducted to improve the normality of this variable. All other variables revealed no substantial violation of normality regarding data distribution at each time point (|Sk|< 1; Ku range: − 1.273 to 1.393).
Latent class analysis of Facebook users
LCA identified three classes of participants. Evaluating one to four class models, the three-class model revealed the best solution (Table 2). Class 3 (n = 143; 29%) had the highest scores on all PFU indicators; therefore, it was defined as the “High PFU users”. Class 2 (n = 28; 6%) had the lowest scores on all indicators; therefore, it was defined as the “Healthy users”. Finally, Class 1 (n = 322; 65%) had indicators’ scores between Class 3 and Class 2; therefore, it was defined as the “Moderate PFU users”. Descriptives across all time points are reported in Table 3 for the whole group and for the three classes, separately. Correlations among the study variables at T0 are reported in Supplementary Table S1 (Supplementary Materials).
Table 2.
Model | BIC | aBIC | Entropy | LMR LRT | BLRT |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
#1 | 13,379.771 | 13,354.379 | – | – | – |
#2 | 13,210.069 | 13,168.807 | 0.722 | − 6665.084*** | − 6665.084*** |
#3 | 13,179.563 | 13,122.431 | 0.760 | − 6564.731*** | − 6564.731*** |
#4 | 13,182.823 | 13,109.821 | 0.790 | − 6533.977 | − 6533.977*** |
LCA latent class analysis, BIC Bayesian information criterion, aBIC sample size adjusted BIC, LMR LRT Lo-Mendell Rubin likelihood ratio test, BLRT bootstrap likelihood ratio test.
#Number of classes.
***p < .001.
Table 3.
Variable | Group | T0 | T1 | T2 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N | Mean (SD) | N | Mean (SD) | N | Mean (SD) | ||
Depressive symptoms | Total | 490 | 9.13 (5.89) | 224 | 8.53 (5.86) | 189 | 9.42 (6.52) |
Healthy users | 28 | 5.46 (5.02) | 12 | 4.25 (4.99) | 9 | 6.33 (6.69) | |
Moderate PFU users | 321 | 8.16 (5.55) | 151 | 7.40 (5.46) | 132 | 8.33 (5.99) | |
High PFU users | 141 | 12.09 (5.67) | 61 | 12.18 (5.31) | 48 | 12.98 (6.67) | |
Fear of COVID-19 pandemic | Total | 493 | 15 (5.82) | 223 | 14.77 (5.91) | 190 | 13.86 (5.77) |
Healthy users | 28 | 12.04 (6.48) | 12 | 11.58 (6.46) | 9 | 9.44 (6.41) | |
Moderate PFU users | 322 | 14.24 (5.39) | 151 | 13.97 (5.54) | 133 | 13.31 (5.42) | |
High PFU users | 143 | 17.29 (5.94) | 60 | 17.42 (5.90) | 48 | 16.23 (5.83) | |
Well-being | Total | 491 | 21.37 (6.51) | 224 | 21.28 (6.22) | 189 | 22.01 (6.61) |
Healthy users | 28 | 24.50 (5.72) | 12 | 24.25 (5.24) | 9 | 24.33 (5.79) | |
Moderate PFU users | 322 | 22.00 (6.49) | 151 | 21.75 (6.31) | 132 | 22.36 (6.54) | |
High PFU users | 141 | 19.3 (6.18) | 61 | 19.54 (5.83) | 48 | 20.63 (6.82) | |
Number of Facebook friends | Total | 415 | 719.82 (622.67) | – | – | – | – |
Healthy users | 17 | 231.24 (215.38) | – | – | – | – | |
Moderate PFU users | 278 | 690.94 (581.23) | – | – | – | – | |
High PFU users | 120 | 855.94 (708.19) | – | – | – | – | |
Passive use of Facebook | Total | 487 | 32.69 (8.16) | 223 | 31.80 (8.01) | 188 | 31.80 (7.49) |
Healthy users | 28 | 14.71 (5.89) | 12 | 18.50 (6.19) | 9 | 14.89 (6.51) | |
Moderate PFU users | 320 | 32.85 (6.52) | 150 | 31.87 (6.43) | 132 | 31.57 (5.96) | |
High PFU users | 139 | 35.92 (7.32) | 61 | 34.26 (9.28) | 47 | 35.68 (6.93) | |
Social comparison on Facebook | Total | 483 | 23.82 (7.93) | 221 | 23.40 (8.42) | 189 | 22.92 (7.82) |
Healthy users | 25 | 15.12 (4.41) | 12 | 13.83 (3.13) | 9 | 14.11 (3.22) | |
Moderate PFU users | 319 | 20.63 (5.28) | 150 | 21.29 (6.96) | 133 | 20.87 (6.24) | |
High PFU users | 139 | 32.71 (5.94) | 59 | 30.71 (7.60) | 47 | 30.43 (7.18) | |
Fear of missing out | Total | 493 | 23.88 (7.40) | 223 | 23.54 (7.51) | 190 | 23.70 (7.32) |
Healthy users | 28 | 14.82 (3.63) | 12 | 16.00 (3.10) | 9 | 15.67 (3.46) | |
Moderate PFU users | 322 | 21.08 (5.25) | 151 | 21.34 (6.29) | 133 | 21.71 (6.09) | |
High PFU users | 143 | 31.95 (5.03) | 60 | 30.60 (5.91) | 48 | 30.71 (5.96) |
PFU problematic Facebook use, T0 October, 2020, T1 December, 2020; T2 February, 2021.
Longitudinal changes in psychological distress and well-being across the three classes
The 2-level HLMs models evidenced non-significant longitudinal changes in measures of psychological distress and well-being, from baseline to 4-months later. That is to say, participants from the entire sample did not experience significant changes in psychological distress nor in well-being over time. The addition of the predictor "Time" at level-1 of all models accounted for 17–33% of the within-patient variance in the dependent variables, with large effects (see Table 4). Moreover, the between-person level accounted for most of the variability in all the dependent variables (range: 0.71–0.78).
Table 4.
Variable | β10 | SE | t-value | df | p-value | R2 | Within-person variance | Between-person variance |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Depressive symptoms | 0.20 | 0.17 | 1.167 | 489 | 0.25 | 0.33 | 0.29 | 0.71 |
Fear of COVID-19 pandemic | − 0.21 | 0.15 | − 1.394 | 492 | 0.19 | 0.17 | 0.27 | 0.73 |
Well-being | 0.29 | 0.16 | 1.858 | 490 | 0.064 | 0.19 | 0.22 | 0.78 |
R2 refers to pseudo-R2 indicating the proportion of within-person variance accounted for by adding the “Time” parameters to the model.
SE standard error of the regression coefficient, df degrees of freedom.
We then added the dummy-coded grouping variables as second-level predictors in our models: at baseline (T0), participants from the three groups reported significantly different levels (all ps < 0.05) in psychological distress (High PFU users > Moderate PFU users > Healthy users) and life satisfaction (High PFU users < Moderate PFU users < Healthy users; see Table 3 for descriptives and Supplementary Table S2 for t- and p-values). The only non-significant comparison was that between “Healthy users” and “Moderate PFU users” for fear of COVID-19. Interestingly, the grouping variable did not predict the longitudinal changes over time in psychological distress and well-being. That is to say, individuals clustered on the basis of their PFU pattern reported significantly different levels of psychological distress and well-being at baseline, which remained consistent over time (i.e., did not change). We further compared the levels of each dependent variable at T1 and T2 across the three classes; at two months follow-up, High PFU users reported greater levels of psychological distress and lower levels of well-being than the other two classes. Similarly, Moderate PFU users reported greater distress than Healthy users, but differences on well-being were not significant. At four months follow-up, High PFU users reported greater levels of psychological distress and lower levels of well-being than individuals belonging to the other two classes. Furthermore, Moderate PFU users reported greater fear of the COVID-19 pandemic than Healthy users, but all other comparisons were non-significant (see Table 3 for descriptives and Supplementary Tables S3 and S4 for t- and p-values).
Analyses re-ran controlling for personal COVID-19 infection and COVID-19 infection among relatives/friends at each time point (i.e., as time-varying covariates) led to almost identical results. Thus, our findings were robust in regard to the effect of these potentially confounding variables (see Supplementary Table S5).
Discussion
The current study showed that a three-class model categorized effectively adults presenting different degrees of problematic Facebook use. The “High PFU users” reported greater passive use of Facebook, higher tendency toward online social comparison on Facebook, a greater number of online friends, and higher levels of FoMO. Healthy users showed the lowest scores on all characteristics of PFU, whereas participants in the “Moderate PFU users” reported mild scores on PFU variables ranging between the High PFU and Healthy users.
Consistently with our hypothesis (i.e., the class with higher level of PFU would have significantly greater psychological distress and lower well-being across the three waves), the findings of the study showed that participants with higher PFU also showed higher levels of psychological distress (i.e., depressive symptoms and fear of COVID-19) as well as lower well-being at each time point. These findings support previous evidence regarding the association between PFU and psychological distress and the negative link between life satisfaction and PFU6. Moreover, the current study adds preliminary evidence that passive Facebook use, online social comparison and FoMO may represent core characteristics of PFU. Prior studies suggested that passive social media use was associated with social comparison, which in turn predicted levels of stress during the pandemic22. It was suggested that passive social media use can negatively affect well-being due to social comparison with those better off than oneself as well as feelings of envy21. Conversely, individuals who report less problematic use of social media (with lower levels of passive use and lower online social comparison) may be less exposed to others' online content31. This may partially account for the higher scores for subjective well-being of users in the Healthy and Moderate PFU classes. The current findings further support the role of FoMO as an important facet of the individual’s impaired control with social media and as a correlate of psychological distress26,57,58.
During the second wave of the COVID-19 pandemic, it is also likely that the ongoing social restrictions may have increased PFU (e.g., through increased passive exposure) and worsened subjective well-being59, which exacerbated the use of SNSs for those who had already been problematic users before the pandemic. These findings may also explain the results regarding significantly lower levels of distress among both the Moderate PFU users and Healthy users during the second wave of the pandemic.
Generally, social media-related activities may have been a major channel in the search for COVID-19-related information during the second wave of the pandemic60. Therefore, a vicious cycle may have been generated and subsequently a positive relationship with the PFU61. This leads to the speculation that people with more severe PFU may be more exposed to COVID-19 relevant information, and it may then result in exaggerated psychological distress62.
The findings of the present study further indicated that the trajectory of psychological distress (i.e., depressive symptoms and fear of COVID-19) and well-being across three stages of the second wave of pandemic remained stable for all the three classes. Thus, participants clustered as “High PFU users” reported significantly higher levels of depressive symptoms and fear of COVID-19 as well as lower levels of well-being, which remained consistent over time. Our results may suggest that the association between the severity of PFU, psychological distress and well-being may be related to stable trait-like and time-invariant differences between individuals (between-person variance) rather than state-like changes (within-person variance) which commonly refers to those that occur from one assessment point to the next one32.
The current study extends our understanding of how PFU is associated with psychological distress and well-being by using a longitudinal design. Proposed implications are especially valuable when the relationship between COVID-19 pandemic, Facebook use, psychological distress and well-being is addressed. In addition, the results of the present study also included a period characterized by the easing of restrictions, which emphasized the importance of the potential need to maintain good mental health, even after the pandemic is over. However, some limitations should be considered when interpreting results. Firstly, the results may not be generalized to other countries due to discrepancies in the stage of COVID-19 infection and different governments’ policies aimed at limiting the spread of the virus. Secondly, the assessment of a non-stratified population with different recruitment procedures does not make these data generalizable. Thirdly, the self-report assessment may also limit conclusions from these results because the accuracy of the participants’ answers might have been affected. Future research needs to use a stratified sample whilst adding objective assessments of PFU.
Conclusion
These results underlined the link between problematic Facebook use during the COVID-19 pandemic, psychological distress and well-being. It should be emphasized that increased time spent on social media was unavoidable during the pandemic when many activities were suspended. Therefore, in interpreting these results, one must be aware that, regardless of people’s usual online habits, the utilization of social media and information acquired through online activities may have triggered the onset of PFU, on top of general, increased internet use during the pandemic.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
We thank all participants.
Author contributions
All authors contributed to the study conception and design. Material preparation and data collection were performed by R.C.B, L.S. and G.L. Analyses were performed by A.B. and L.S. The first draft of the manuscript was written by R.C.B and L.S. and all authors commented on previous versions of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Data availability
The dataset generated for this study are available on request from the corresponding author.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-022-26281-0.
References
- 1.Zhao N, Zhou G. COVID-19 stress and addictive social media use (SMU): Mediating role of active use and social media flow. Front. Psychiatry. 2021;12:635546. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.635546. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Gioia F, Fioravanti G, Casale S, Boursier V. The effects of the fear of missing out on people’s social networking sites use during the COVID-19 pandemic: The mediating role of online relational closeness and individuals’ online communication attitude. Front. Psychiatry. 2021;12:620442. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.620442. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Wiederhold BK. Social media use during social distancing. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2020;23:275–276. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2020.29181.bkw. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Liu D, Baumeister R, Yang C, Hu B. Digital communication media use and psychological well-being: A meta-analysis. J. Comput. Mediat. Comm. 2019;24:259–274. doi: 10.1093/jcmc/zmz013. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Verduyn P, Ybarra O, Résibois M, Jonides J, Kross E. Do social network sites enhance or undermine subjective well-being? A critical review. Soc. Issues Policy Rev. 2017;11:274–302. doi: 10.1111/sipr.12033. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Marino C, Gini G, Vieno A, Spada MM. The associations between problematic Facebook use, psychological distress and well-being among adolescents and young adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Affect. Disord. 2018;15:274–281. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Primi C, Fioravanti G, Casale S, Donati MA. Measuring problematic Facebook use among adolescents and young adults with the Bergen Facebook addiction scale: A psychometric analysis by applying item response theory. Int. J. Environ. Res. 2021;18:2979. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18062979. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Appel M, Marker C, Gnambs T. Are social media ruining our lives? A review of meta-analytic evidence. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2020;24:60–74. doi: 10.1177/1089268019880891. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Liu D, Baumeister RF. Social networking online and personality of self-worth: A meta- analysis. J. Res. Pers. 2016;64:79–89. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2016.06.024. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Chou HTG, Edge N. ‘They are happier and having better lives than I am’’: The impact of using Facebook on perceptions of others’ lives. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2012;15:117–121. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2011.0324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kuss D, Griffiths M. Online social networking and addiction—A review of the psychological literature. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2011;8:3528. doi: 10.3390/ijerph8093528. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Shensa A, et al. Problematic social media use and depressive symptoms among U.S. young adults: A nationally-representative study. Soc. Sci. Med. 2017;182:150–157. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.03.061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Sherlock M, Wagstaff DL. Exploring the relationship between frequency of Instagram use, exposure to idealized images, and psychological well-being in women. Psychol. Pop. Media Cult. 2019;8:482–490. doi: 10.1037/ppm0000182. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Hormes JM, Kearns B, Timko CA. Craving Facebook? Behavioral addiction to online social networking and its association with emotion regulation deficits. Addiction. 2014;109:2079–2088. doi: 10.1111/add.12713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Marino C, Gini G, Vieno A, Spada MM. A comprehensive meta-analysis on problematic Facebook Use. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2018;83:262–277. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.02.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kuss DJ, Grifths MD. Social networking sites and addiction: Ten lessons learned. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2017;14:311. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14030311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Paakkari L, Tynjälä J, Lahti H, Ojala K, Lyyra N. Problematic social media use and health among adolescents. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2021;18:1885. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18041885. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Fioravanti G, Casale S. The active and passive use of Facebook: Measurement and relationship to Facebook addiction. J. Psychopathol. 2020;26:176–182. doi: 10.36148/2284-0249-329. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Verduyn P, et al. Passive Facebook usage undermines affective well-being: Experimental and longitudinal evidence. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2015;144:480–488. doi: 10.1037/xge0000057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Valkenburg PM, van Driel II, Beyens I. The associations of active and passive social media use with well-being: A critical scoping review. New Media Soc. 2022;24:530–549. doi: 10.1177/14614448211065425. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Verduyn P, Gugushvili N, Massar K, Taht K, Kross E. Social comparison on social networking sites. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2020;36:32–37. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Yue Z, Zhang R, Xiao J. Passive social media use and psychological well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic: The role of social comparison and emotion regulation. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2022;127:107050. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2021.107050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Vogel EA, Rose JP, Okdie BM, Eckles K, Franz B. Who compares and despairs? The effect of social comparison orientation on social media use and its outcomes. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2015;86:249–256. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.06.026. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Yang CC. Instagram use, loneliness, and social comparison orientation: Interact and browse on social media, but don’t compare. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2016;19:703–708. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2016.0201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Yoon S, Kleinman M, Mertz J, Brannick M. Is social network site usage related to depression? A meta-analysis of facebook–depression relations. J. Affect. Disord. 2019;248:65–72. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.01.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Przybylski AK, Murayama K, DeHaan CR, Gladwell V. Motivational, emotional, and behavioral correlates of fear of missing out. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2013;29:1841–1848. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.02.014. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Wegmann E, Brand M. A narrative overview about psychosocial characteristics as risk factors of a problematic social networks use. Curr. Addict. Rep. 2019;6:402–409. doi: 10.1007/s40429-019-00286-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Błachnio A, Przepiórka A. Facebook intrusion, fear of missing out, narcissism, and life satisfaction: A cross-sectional study. Psychiatry Res. 2018;259:514–519. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.11.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Dempsey AE, O’Brien KD, Tiamiyu MF, Elhai JD. Fear of missing out (FOMO) and rumination mediate relations between social anxiety and problematic Facebook use. Addic. Behav. Rep. 2019;9:100150. doi: 10.1016/j.abrep.2018.100150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Schimmenti A, Billieuxm J, Starcevic V. The four horsemen of fear: An integrated model of understanding fear experiences during the COVID-19 pandemic. Clin. Neuropsychiatry. 2020;17:41–45. doi: 10.36131/CN20200202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Verduyn P, Gugushvili N, Kross E. The impact of social network sites on mental health: Distinguishing active from passive use. World Psychiatry. 2021;20:133–134. doi: 10.1002/wps.20820. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Di Blasi M, et al. A three-wave panel study on longitudinal relations between problematic social media use and psychological distress during the COVID-19 pandemic. Addict. Behav. 2022;134:107430. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2022.107430. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Parry DA, Fisher JT, Mieczkowski H, Sewall CJR, Davidson BI. Social media and well-being: A methodological perspective. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2022;45:101285. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Dantlgraber M, Wetzel E, Schutzenberger P, Stieger S, Reips UD. Simple construct evaluation with latent class analysis: An investigation of Facebook addiction and the development of a short form of the Facebook Addiction Test (F-AT) Behav. Res. Methods. 2016;48:869–879. doi: 10.3758/s13428-016-0716-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Lee SY, et al. Distinct patterns of Internet and smartphone-related problems among adolescents by gender: Latent class analysis. J. Behav. Addict. 2018;7:454–465. doi: 10.1556/2006.7.2018.28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Lo Coco GL, et al. Empirically-derived subgroups of Facebook users and their association with personality characteristics: A Latent class analysis. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2018;86:190–198. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.04.044. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Shensa A, Sidani JE, Dew MA, Escobar-Viera CG, Primack BA. Social media use and depression and anxiety symptoms: A cluster analysis. Am. J. Health Behav. 2018;42:116–128. doi: 10.5993/AJHB.42.2.11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Malik A, Mahmood K, Islam T. Understanding the Facebook users’ behavior towards COVID-19 information sharing by integrating the theory of planned behavior and gratifications. Inf. Dev. 2021 doi: 10.1177/02666669211049383. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Dong H, Yang F, Lu X, Hao W. Internet addiction and related psychological factors among children and adolescents in China during the Coronavirus Disease 2019 (COVID-19) epidemic. Front. Psychiatry. 2020;11:00751. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00751. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Ruggieri S, Ingoglia S, Bonfanti R, Lo Coco G. The role of Online Social Comparison as a protective factor for psychological wellbeing: A longitudinal study during the COVID-19 quarantine. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2021;171:110486. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110486. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Hossain MT, et al. Social and electronic media exposure and generalized anxiety disorder among people during COVID-19 outbreak in Bangladesh: A preliminary observation. PLoS ONE. 2020;15:8974. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0238974. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Jahan I, et al. How has the COVID-19 pandemic impacted internet use behaviors and facilitated problematic internet use? A Bangladeshi study. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manage. 2021;14:1127–1138. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S323570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Sayeed A, et al. Facebook addiction associated with internet activity, depression and behavioral factors among university students of Bangladesh: A cross-sectional study. Child Youth Serv. Rev. 2020;118:5424. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105424. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44.de Vries DA, Kühne R. Facebook and self-perception: Individual susceptibility to negative social comparison on facebook. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2015;86:217–221. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.05.029. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Schimmenti A, Starcevic V, Giardina A, Khazaal Y, Billieux J. Multidimensional assessment of COVID-19-related fears (MAC-RF): A theory-based instrument for the assessment of clinically relevant fears during pandemics. Front. Psychiatry. 2020;11:748. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00748. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Peng P, et al. The prevalence and risk factors of mental problems in medical students during COVID-19 pandemic: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Affect. Disord. 2022;321:167–181. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2022.10.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Gibbons FX, Buunk BP. Individual differences in social comparison: The development of a scale of social comparison orientation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1999;76:129–142. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.1.129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Casale S, Fioravanti G. Factor structure and psychometric properties of the Italian version of the fear of missing out scale in emerging adults and adolescents. Addict. Behav. 2020;102:106179. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.106179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Bottesi G, et al. The Italian version of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales-21: Factor structure and psychometric properties on community and clinical samples. Compr. Psychiatry. 2015;60:170–181. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2015.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Henry JD, Crawford JR. The short form version of the depression anxiety stress scales (DASS-21): Construct validity and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 2005;44:227–239. doi: 10.1348/014466505X29657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.di Fabio, A., & Palazzeschi, L. The satisfaction with life scale (SWLS): A contribution to Italian validation with adult workers. Counseling: Giornale Italiano di Ricerca e Applicazioni5, 207–215 (2012).
- 52.Diener E, Emmons RA, Larsen RJ, Griffin S. The satisfaction with life scale. J. Pers. Assess. 1985;49:71–75. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Tein JY, Coxe S, Cham H. Statistical power to detect the correct number of classes in latent profile analysis. Struct. Equ. Model. 2013;20:640–657. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2013.824781. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Singer, J. D., & Willett, J. B. Applied Longitudinal Data Analysis: Modeling Change and Event Occurrence (Oxford University Press, 2003).
- 55.Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. Hierarchical Linear Models: Applications and Data Analysis Methods (2nd ed.) (Sage Publications, 2002).
- 56.Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.) (Erlbaum, 1988).
- 57.Fioravanti G, et al. Fear of missing out and social networking sites use and abuse: A meta-analysis. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2021;122:106839. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2021.106839. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Blackwell D, Leaman C, Tramposch R, Osborne C, Liss M. Extraversion, neuroticism, attachment style and fear of missing out as predictors of social media use and addiction. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2017;116:69–72. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.04.039. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Fang Y, et al. The prevalence of psychological stress in student populations during the COVID-19 epidemic: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sci. Rep. 2022;12:12118. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-16328-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Statista. Social media use during COVID-19 worldwide—statistics & facts. https://www.statista.com/ (2022).
- 61.Sun Y, et al. Brief report: Increased addictive internet and substance use behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic in China. Am. J. Addict. 2020;29:268–270. doi: 10.1111/ajad.13066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Ahmad AR, Murad HR. The impact of social media on panic during the COVID-19 pandemic in Iraqi Kurdistan: Online questionnaire study. J. Med. Internet Res. 2020;22:e19556. doi: 10.2196/19556. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The dataset generated for this study are available on request from the corresponding author.