ABSTRACT
The prevalent paradigm governing bacterial two-component signaling systems (TCSs) is specificity, wherein the histidine kinase (HK) of a TCS exclusively activates its cognate response regulator (RR). Cross talk, where HKs activate noncognate RRs, is considered evolutionarily disadvantageous because it can compromise adaptive responses by leaking signals. Yet cross talk is observed in several bacteria. Here, to resolve this paradox, we propose an alternative paradigm where cross talk can be advantageous. We envisioned programmed environments, wherein signals appear in predefined sequences. In such environments, cross talk that primes bacteria to upcoming signals may improve adaptive responses and confer evolutionary benefits. To test this hypothesis, we employed mathematical modeling of TCS signaling networks and stochastic evolutionary dynamics simulations. We considered the comprehensive set of bacterial phenotypes, comprising thousands of distinct cross talk patterns competing in varied signaling environments. Our simulations predicted that in programmed environments phenotypes with cross talk facilitating priming would outcompete phenotypes without cross talk. In environments where signals appear randomly, bacteria without cross talk would dominate, explaining the specificity widely seen. Additionally, a testable prediction was that the phenotypes selected in programmed environments would display one-way cross talk, ensuring priming to future signals. Interestingly, the cross talk networks we deduced from available data on TCSs of Mycobacterium tuberculosis all displayed one-way cross talk, which was consistent with our predictions. Our study thus identifies potential evolutionary underpinnings of cross talk in bacterial TCSs, suggests a reconciliation of specificity and cross talk, makes testable predictions of the nature of cross talk patterns selected, and has implications for understanding bacterial adaptation and the response to interventions.
IMPORTANCE Bacteria use two-component signaling systems (TCSs) to sense and respond to environmental changes. The prevalent paradigm governing TCSs is specificity, where signal flow through TCSs is insulated; leakage to other TCSs is considered evolutionarily disadvantageous. Yet cross talk between TCSs is observed in many bacteria. Here, we present a potential resolution of this paradox. We envision programmed environments, wherein stimuli appear in predefined sequences. Cross talk that primes bacteria to upcoming stimuli could then confer evolutionary benefits. We demonstrate this benefit using mathematical modeling and evolutionary simulations. Interestingly, we found signatures of predicted cross talk patterns in Mycobacterium tuberculosis. Furthermore, specificity was selected in environments where stimuli occurred randomly, thus reconciling specificity and cross talk. Implications follow for understanding bacterial evolution and for interventions.
KEYWORDS: Mycobacterium tuberculosis, cross talk, evolutionary dynamics, mathematical modeling, specificity, two-component regulatory systems
INTRODUCTION
Bacteria sense and respond to environmental cues predominantly via two-component signaling systems (TCSs) (1). The first component of a TCS is the transmembrane histidine kinase (HK). The HK detects the stimulus, which typically is a chemical ligand, and gets autophosphorylated. The phosphorylated HK (HK-P) binds to and transfers its phosphoryl group to the response regulator (RR), the second component of the TCS. Phosphorylated RR (RR-P) typically dimerizes and triggers changes in downstream gene expression, mounting a response to the stimulus (1, 2). Cognate HK-RR pairs, which belong to a TCS, are generally coexpressed under a single promoter in an operon (3) and are often upregulated as part of the response to the stimulus (1, 2).
Bacteria can have many tens of distinct TCSs, each performing a different function (1). Evolutionary pressure is thought to have rendered TCSs specific: the HK of a TCS rarely phosphorylates the RR of another TCS (4). Cross talk between TCSs, defined as phosphotransfer from the HK of one TCS to the RR of another TCS, is considered disadvantageous because it dissipates the signal, decreasing the concentration of the cognate RR-P and thereby weakening the response (4). Moreover, unwanted responses due to gene expression downstream of noncognate RR-Ps might get triggered. Bacteria typically acquire novel TCSs through gene duplication (5), which would naturally allow cross talk before diversification of the TCSs into distinct pathways (6, 7). Several experimental and modeling studies have argued that despite the extensive homology between TCS proteins, there is strong evolutionary pressure for these paralogs to be specific (5, 8–13). For instance, cross talk between TCSs can be abrogated by as few as two mutations, indicative of the evolutionary pressure favoring specificity (8). Further, during the evolution of new TCSs post-gene duplication, bacteria have been predicted to eliminate cross talk before new TCS functionalities can arise (9). The sequence space occupied by the paralogs is thought to be sparse, allowing easy establishment of such specificity (12).
Yet cross talk between bacterial TCSs continues to be observed and, in some bacteria, in significant measure. Approximately 3% of the 850 interactions between TCS proteins in Escherichia coli, for instance, were between noncognate HK-RR pairs (14). A substantially larger fraction, ~50% of the 23 interactions, were between noncognate pairs in Mycobacterium tuberculosis (15). Given the evolutionary advantages of specificity together with the relative ease of establishing it, the observed cross talk is puzzling. Indeed, in some organisms, such as Caulobacter crescentus (16) and Myxococcus xanthus (17), no cross talk has been observed among hundreds of interactions. The observed cross talk may thus not be attributable to chance and may instead have evolutionary underpinnings. Unraveling potential evolutionary advantages of cross talk is expected to have important implications for our understanding of bacterial adaptation, survival, and response to interventions (1, 15, 18, 19).
Here, we conceived of an evolutionary paradigm in which cross talk could be beneficial. We hypothesized that in programmed environments, where signals consistently appear in a predefined sequence, cross talk between TCSs that would prime the bacterium to upcoming signals might confer an evolutionary advantage. To test this hypothesis, we constructed a mechanistic mathematical model of generalized multi-TCS signaling networks and performed extensive evolutionary dynamics simulations. We challenged model predictions with available experimental observations and found evidence in support of our hypothesis. Additionally, we arrived at a plausible synthesis of the seemingly conflicting observations of specificity and cross talk in bacterial TCS systems.
RESULTS
Cross talk can confer a fitness advantage in programmed environments.
We first considered a hypothetical environment involving N = 2 signals, denoted I1 and I2, recognized by two TCSs of a bacterium, TCS1 and TCS2, made up of the proteins HK1 and RR1 and HK2 and RR2, respectively. Depending on the nature of interactions between the TCSs, four phenotypes could exist (Fig. 1a): (i) with no cross talk (phenotype 1), (ii) with cross talk between HK1 and RR2 (phenotype 2), (iii) with cross talk between HK2 and RR1 (phenotype 3), and (iv) with bidirectional cross talk (phenotype 4). We developed a detailed model of signal transduction in a TCS network, allowing for all possible cross talk patterns between the TCSs (Materials and Methods). The model builds on previous models of TCS signaling (9, 15, 20, 21), generalizing them to multi-TCS networks with cross talk. The novelty of our approach lies in recognizing and incorporating the role of the environment. We applied our model to each of the four phenotypes. We first considered the scenario representing a programmed environment. Specifically, we let the signal I1 be followed by I2. For simplicity, we let the signals be identical except for the time of their onset (Fig. 1b). We also assumed the signals to be square pulses arriving in quick succession, mimicking the typical way environments impose stresses (22); we considered alternative signal types below. Using the model, we predicted the concentrations of RR1-P and RR2-P over time (Fig. 1b, top panel) as a proxy for the responses of the bacteria to the two stimuli. Further, we estimated the fitness, ϕ1 and ϕ2, of the bacteria associated with the responses of the two TCSs and the overall fitness, ‹ϕ›, combining the two (Fig. 1b, bottom panel). The fitness was determined by the strength of the cognate responses to the individual stimuli (Materials and Methods).
For phenotype 1, where TCSs are insulated, our model predicted that the responses to the two signals were, expectedly, identical except for a shift in time (black curves in Fig. 1b). When I1 arrived, bacterial fitness dropped sharply, indicating a changed environment to which the bacterium was yet to adapt. The bacterium mounted an adaptive response, improving its fitness with time. As RR1-P increased, the fitness, ϕ1, recovered. The same phenomenon was observed upon the arrival of I2. The absence of cross talk implied that the responses to I1 and I2 were independent. Although the fitness was nearly fully restored eventually, the time-averaged overall fitness, ‹ϕ›, was lower than unity, indicative of the vulnerability of the bacterium during adaptation to the changed environment.
For phenotype 2, with HK1→RR2 cross talk (red curves in Fig. 1b), our model predicted that before the arrival of I2, signal leakage to TCS2 resulted in lower RR1-P and, hence, ϕ1 than for phenotype 1. The signal leakage, however, triggered TCS2. The resulting RR2-P upregulated HK2 and RR2. When I2 came up, the bacterium responded faster and better than phenotype 1; RR2-P and ϕ2 were higher than for phenotype 1. The overall fitness, ‹ϕ›, increased beyond that of phenotype 1. Thus, the bacterium was predicted to be more sensitive and responsive to the upcoming stimulus due to cross talk, increasing its fitness. This scenario was illustrative of the possible advantage of cross talk in a programmed environment.
For phenotype 3, with HK2→RR1 cross talk, in our model predictions, the needless signal dissipation to RR1 following the onset of I2 induced a fitness loss (blue curves in Fig. 1b). Finally, for phenotype 4, with bidirectional cross talk, RR1-P was like phenotype 2 due to dissipation before the arrival of I2. The subtle difference with phenotype 2 arose because of the phosphatase activity of HK2. Cross talk implied that HK2 could exert phosphatase activity on RR1-P, because of which the level of RR1-P was slightly lower and that of RR2-P slightly higher for phenotype 4 than phenotype 2. Thus, immediately upon the arrival of I2, the fitness loss was the least for phenotype 4. However, the advantage of priming was lost due to the HK2→RR1 cross talk after the arrival of I2, resulting in an overall fitness loss (green curves in Fig. 1b). The predicted time-averaged fitness loss, 1 – ‹ϕ›, of the four phenotypes over the entire signal-response period highlights the advantage of phenotype 2, which has a cross talk pattern that mirrors the signal sequence, over the other phenotypes (Fig. 1b, inset).
Next, we examined how the fitness advantage would depend on the strength of cross talk using our model. We defined the selection coefficient, σ, for any phenotype as the difference between the time-averaged fitness of the phenotype and that of phenotype 1, the latter without any cross talk. We quantified the strength of cross talk using γ, the ratio of the efficiencies of phosphotransfer to noncognate and cognate RRs (Materials and Methods). The larger the value of γ, the greater was the extent of cross talk. We found from our predictions that for all the values of γ studied, phenotype 2 had positive σ, whereas the other phenotypes had negative σ (Fig. 1c), consistent with the results described above. Further, for phenotype 2, σ displayed a maximum at intermediate γ (Fig. 1d), specifically at γ = 0.26. Increasing γ increased priming and improved the response to I2, increasing fitness. Beyond a point, however, the advantage of priming diminished, but the response to I1 continued to be compromised, lowering the overall fitness (Fig. 1d, inset). Thus, according to our model, limited cross talk offered a fitness advantage to phenotype 2.
Specificity is advantageous in random environments.
Using the same phenotypes as described above, we applied our model to estimate σ in a random environment, where there was no defined sequence of signals (Materials and Methods). Now, phenotype 1 had the highest estimated fitness; σ was negative for all the other phenotypes (Fig. 1e). Because the upcoming signal was not prespecified, priming conferred no advantage. The detrimental effects of cross talk then decreased fitness regardless of the cross talk pattern. Thus, σ was equal for phenotypes 2 and 3, which had one cross talk interaction each, and lower for phenotype 4, which had two cross talk interactions. Moreover, the greater the value of γ, the lower was the value of σ in the random environment. Thus, in the absence of a consistent sequence of stimuli, our model predicted that evolutionary pressure may select for specificity.
Using sensitivity analysis, we found that the inferences described above were robust to variations in parameter values (see Fig. S1 in the supplemental material). Furthermore, our findings were robust to the fitness construct employed (Text S1; Fig. S2) and the nature of the signals; we tested both square pulses and exponentially decaying signals (Fig. S3). Our model also predicted that with decaying signals, the fitness advantage of cross talk ceased when the interval between the signals was either too small or too large (Fig. S3). When the interval was too small, the second signal appeared before significant priming could happen, whereas when the interval was too large, the priming faded away before the second signal could arrive. These latter predictions were consistent with observations in E. coli (23), where priming conferred a significant fitness advantage, manifested as enhanced growth rate, only for a range of time gaps between signals.
Programmed environments favor one-way cross talk.
For the minimal case of N = 2, phenotype 2 alone could anticipate I2 following I1 and thus was predicted to have the highest fitness in our model. For bacteria with more than two TCSs, the fittest phenotype is not obvious, as such anticipation is possible with multiple phenotypes. For instance, the phenotype with the cross talk interactions HK1→RR2 and HK2→RR3 as well as the phenotype with HK1→RR2 and HK1→RR3 could anticipate the sequence I1→I2→I3. The number of phenotypes grows exponentially with N. A bacterium with N TCSs will have N cognate and up to N(N − 1) noncognate interactions. Depending on whether each of the latter interactions is realized or not, a total of 2N(N–1) phenotypes can exist, each representing a distinct cross talk pattern. For N = 3, this would amount to 26 = 64 phenotypes, and for N = 4, it would amount to 212 = 4,096 phenotypes. Identifying the fittest phenotype would thus require a comprehensive assessment of each of these phenotypes. We performed this assessment next.
We considered N = 3. We numbered the phenotypes from 1 to 64, starting with the phenotype with no cross talk and ending with the phenotype with all cross talk interactions realized (Fig. 2a). We subjected each phenotype to a programmed environment with the signal sequence I1→I2→I3. We also allowed the signals to have different durations, more realistically mimicking natural environments. For each scenario, we applied our model to predict signal-response characteristics and estimated the resulting fitness.
When the signals were all of the same duration, our model predicted that the phenotype that was the fittest depended on the strength of cross talk, γ. When γ was small, phenotype 12, which had HK1→RR2, HK2→RR3, and HK1→RR3 interactions, was the fittest (Fig. 2b). Its fitness was only slightly higher than that of phenotype 10, which had HK1→RR2 and HK2→RR3 interactions. Note that both these phenotypes anticipated upcoming signals and were fitter than phenotype 1, which had no cross talk. As γ increased, phenotype 10 became fitter than phenotype 12 in our predictions. Interestingly, the fitness of the latter decreased beyond a threshold γ and eventually dropped below that of phenotype 1. Phenotype 10, however, remained fitter than phenotype 1 throughout. We understood these trends as follows. When γ was low, the cost of signal dissipation was small. Thus, the gain from cross talk by HK1 with both RR2 and RR3 and by HK2 with RR3 more than compensated for the fitness loss due to leakage. However, as γ increased, the latter cost increased and limiting cross talk became advantageous. Accordingly, our model predicted that cross talk between HK1 and RR2 and between HK2 and RR3, which ensured the requisite anticipation of upcoming signals, was retained, resulting in an overall fitness gain, whereas the redundant cross talk between HK1 and RR3 was eliminated in the fittest phenotype.
We next increased the duration of I2 6-fold (Fig. 2c). When γ was small, phenotype 2, which had the HK1→RR2 interaction alone, was the fittest in our predictions. As γ increased, phenotype 10, which had HK1→RR2 and HK2→RR3 interactions, became the fittest. With weak cross talk, the advantage of priming to I3 through the entire duration of I2 was not enough to compensate for the loss of response to I2. Phenotype 2, which did not have the HK2→RR3 interaction, was therefore the fittest. On the other hand, when cross talk was stronger, the priming from both HK1→RR2 and HK2→RR3 compensated for any signal dissipation, rendering phenotype 10 the fittest in our predictions.
We also considered the effect of shortening the duration of I2 (Fig. 2d and e). When the duration was shortened by 50%, phenotypes 12 and 10 were predicted to be the fittest, depending on γ, in a manner similar to when the signals were all of the same duration (Fig. 2b and d). The shortening of the duration by 50% thus did not affect the cost-benefit analysis substantially. Shortening the duration 5-fold, however, made a difference, with phenotypes 3 and 11 now the fittest (Fig. 2e). As describe above, when γ was small, phenotype 11, with the cross talk interactions HK1→RR3 and HK2→RR3, both anticipating the upcoming signal I3, was the fittest in our model. This was because at low values of γ, priming to I3 while I2 was present did not add significantly to the cost due to signal dissipation, as I2 was present for a short while. However, as γ increased, phenotype 3, which had the single cross talk interaction HK1→RR3, was the fittest. The cost of dissipation, although I2 was short-lived, was no longer affordable. The phenotype that let I1 prime the bacterium to the next major signal, I3, was thus the fittest. Finally, as with the N = 2 scenario, the results were similar when exponentially decaying signals were used instead of square pulses (Fig. 2f).
In all these cases, an intriguing feature of the fittest phenotypes is directed, one-way cross talk. If we denote the signal sequence as I1→I2→I3→…, then the fittest phenotypes had cross talk of the type HKi→RRj with j > i. In other words, the cross talk that enabled priming to upcoming signals was favored. Reverse signal flow, where j < i, resulted in phenotypes that suffered fitness loss. In the interaction matrices, the fittest phenotypes all had nonzero entries in the upper triangular portions and never in the lower triangular portions (Fig. 2a). To test the robustness of this prediction, we adopted two strategies. We performed extensive evolutionary dynamics simulations to examine whether the fitness advantage predicted by the calculations described above would lead to the selection of the corresponding phenotypes with the one-way cross talk patterns. Second, we sought evidence of these predictions in available experimental data.
Evolutionary simulations predict selection of phenotypes with one-way cross talk patterns mirroring signal sequences.
Using the descriptions mentioned above of the responses of different phenotypes to stimuli, we performed stochastic, discrete generation, Wright-Fisher evolutionary simulations (24) (Fig. 3a; Materials and Methods) to determine which phenotypes would get selected in different environments. We now considered N = 4 TCSs, increasing the complexity to a total of 4,096 phenotypes, making it even more difficult to predict the fittest phenotypes intuitively. We performed simulations with two types of initial conditions: (ii) the homogeneous condition, where a single phenotype existed and (ii) the mixed condition, where all the phenotypes were equally represented. With each initial condition, we considered both random and programmed environments. With N = 4, we had four types of signals, one for each of the TCSs. We let each bacterium be stimulated four times. In the random environment, each stimulus was chosen randomly from the four possible signals. In the programmed environment, the signals followed a predetermined sequence, where the signals all appeared once and in a fixed order. We computed the fitness of each of the 4,096 species in each of these environments. In each generation, we allowed every bacterium to be selected with a probability proportional to its fitness. The selected bacteria were duplicated to replace lost bacteria and ensure a constant bacterial population. The bacteria were then subjected to mutations. A mutation involved a change in the cross talk network of the bacterium, resulting in an altered phenotype. Specifically, we allowed each of the N(N − 1) = 12 potential cross talk interactions within a bacterium to be flipped (from existent to nonexistent and vice versa) with a probability μ, the mutation rate, in each generation. The resulting pool of bacteria formed the substrate for evolution in the next generation. We repeated this process over 10,000 generations, which ensured fixation of the fittest phenotype, and performed 50 realizations, for reliable statistics (Materials and Methods).
In the random environment, our simulations predicted that the phenotype without any cross talk dominated the population (Fig. 3b). For the homogeneous condition, we initiated simulations with the species containing all cross talk interactions. Gradually, phenotypes with fewer cross talk interactions emerged. Eventually, the phenotype with no cross talk emerged and dominated the population. With the mixed condition, the latter species began to dominate the population from the early stages and was soon fixed in the population. These observations agree with the prevalent paradigm of TCS signaling favoring specificity (5, 8, 9, 12). Also, rank-ordering phenotypes by their fitness values (Fig. 3b, inset) revealed that phenotypes with an increasing number of cross talk interactions had decreasing fitness. To illustrate this, we present the cross talk patterns of the top five and bottom five fittest phenotypes (Fig. 3b). The former have zero or one cross talk interaction and the latter have all or one less cross talk interactions.
In the programmed environment, which followed the signal sequence I1→I2→I3→I4, the phenotype with the cross talk pattern mirroring this signal sequence dominated the population (Fig. 3c). For the homogeneous condition, we used the species without cross talk to initiate simulations. Gradually, mutants with cross talk emerged and grew, causing the initial species to decline. Eventually, the phenotype with the cross talk pattern mirroring the signal sequence emerged and dominated the population. For the mixed condition, the latter phenotype grew from the early stages and was rapidly fixed. Arranging the fitness values in descending order (Fig. 3c, inset) displays the benefit of priming for upcoming stimuli. The five fittest phenotypes all had cross talk interactions in the upper triangle of their interaction matrices, indicating one-way cross talk patterns that prime bacteria to upcoming signals (Fig. 3c). The least fit phenotypes had the lower triangle of the interaction matrices populated, indicating cross talk that had signal flows opposite to the sequence of stimuli.
These results were not restricted to N = 4 TCSs. With N = 2 (Fig. S4) and N = 3 TCSs (Fig. S5) as well, the phenotype with no cross talk was selected in random environments, and the phenotype with the cross talk pattern mirroring the sequence of signals was selected in programmed environments.
These simulations thus point to environments where cross talk may be evolutionarily favored. It is possible that such programmed environments may have been the reasons for the selection of the cross talk that is observed in some bacteria. Our model and simulations go beyond offering a plausible explanation of the origins of such cross talk and predict that the cross talk selected is likely to be one-way. We next sought evidence of one-way cross talk patterns in available experimental data.
Evidence of one-way cross talk in TCSs of M. tuberculosis.
In a recent study, cross talk between the TCSs of M. tuberculosis has been mapped using in vitro assays of phosphotransfer from HKs to all cognate and noncognate RRs (15). Significant cross talk was observed (Fig. 4a), which allowed us to assess signal flows through extended TCS networks. Using the cross talk interactions, we identified all possible signal flows, or cascades, in the TCSs of M. tuberculosis as follows. We considered the HK PhoR, for instance, which showed cross talk with the RR DevR (Fig. 4a). DevS, the cognate HK of DevR, further showed cross talk with the RR NarL. NarS, the cognate HK of NarL, did not engage in any cross talk. Thus, when PhoR gets activated, it can transmit a portion of the signal to DevR. Similarly, cross talk of DevS with NarL would transmit some portion of the signal from DevS-DevR to the NarS-NarL system, at which point the signal flow would be terminated. Hence, PhoR-PhoP, DevS-DevR, and NarS-NarL form a cascade of signal flow via cross talk. In this cascade, the signal is not transmitted either to PhoP from DevS or NarS or to DevR from NarS, making the flow one-way.
Following the procedure described above, we started with each of the TCSs of M. tuberculosis and traced the resulting cascades. We found 13 such cascades (Fig. 4b). The longest cascade involved 4 TCSs. There were 5 cascades involving 3 TCSs each and 7 cascades involving 2 TCSs each. (Representative interaction matrices for all these cases are presented at the bottom of Fig. 4.) Note that all the cascades had one-way cross talk, with the patterns resembling the fittest phenotypes in our simulations above.
By superimposing the cascades above, we can obtain additional one-way cross talk patterns, reflective of the patterns identified in our simulations. Two such patterns are depicted in Fig. 4c. For instance, the cross talk pattern involving MtrB-MtrA, PhoR-PhoP, and TcrY-TcrX (Fig. 4c, top panel) was equivalent to phenotype 12 in the N = 3 case discussed above (Fig. 2b). Similarly, the pattern involving KdpD-KdpE, DevS-DevR, and NarS-NarL (Fig. 4c, bottom panel) was equivalent to phenotype 11 in the N = 3 case discussed above (Fig. 2b). Remarkably, we could not find any cross talk pattern that was not one-way. This evidence of exclusive one-way cross talk in the TCSs of M. tuberculosis offered support for the predictions of our model and simulations. To assess whether the cross talk could have evolutionarily underpinnings, we sought signatures of evolutionary pressures against diversification post-gene duplication in the sequences of the TCS proteins using bioinformatics analysis (Text S1). Although the analysis could only be conducted on a subset of the TCSs, the results suggested that this evolutionary pressure may have been less for the TCSs involved in cross talk than for the TCSs that were specific, offering further support to the notion that the observed cross talk may have been evolutionarily favored (Text S1, Fig. S6 and S7, Table S2).
DISCUSSION
Despite the strong evolutionary arguments favoring specificity in bacterial TCSs (4, 5), cross talk between TCSs has been observed (14, 15). Here, we present an alternative evolutionary paradigm where cross talk would be advantageous. Using modeling of TCS signaling networks and extensive evolutionary dynamics simulations, we predicted that in programmed environments, where stimuli arrive in a predetermined sequence, cross talk that would prime bacteria to upcoming signals would confer an evolutionary benefit. Thus, specific cross talk patterns that mirror the sequences of stimuli could get selected in bacteria living in such environments. Analyzing recent in vitro data (15), we found that potential cross talk networks involving the TCSs of M. tuberculosis all displayed one-way signal flow, consistent with the notion of priming and selection in programmed environments. This new evolutionary paradigm is not in conflict with the paradigm underlying specificity. Our modeling and simulations predicted that when no predetermined sequence of stimuli existed, specificity was evolutionarily favored. Our study thus offers a conceptual framework that synthesizes specificity and cross talk in bacterial TCS systems. They appear to be two sides of the same coin; they are both outcomes of the same evolutionary forces, but in environments that present signals differently. Programmed environments may be rarer, resulting in the lower prevalence of cross talk.
Independent evidence exists of one-way cross talk aiding bacterial adaptation in programmed environments. In E. coli, evolutionary experiments showed how anticipation, facilitated by cross talk, is selected for when the environment displays a specified pattern of carbon source switching (22). Furthermore, the complex structure of environments can become ingrained in in silico biochemical networks in order to predict environmental changes preemptively (25). In agreement, this adaptive behavior was evident in E. coli, where a match between the covariation of transcriptional responses and the sequence of temperature and oxygen stresses triggering them was observed (25). Evidence also exists of pathogenic bacteria evolving cross talk to adapt to their hosts. For instance, mutations in the TCS BfmS-BfmR of Pseudomonas aeruginosa in individuals with cystic fibrosis were recently found to alter, facilitated via cross talk by the noncognate HK GtrS, regulation of downstream gene expression in order to promote biofilm formation and chronic infection (26). Similarly, in Alphaproteobacteria, multiple HKs of the HWE/HisKA-2 family can control the phosphorylation of the same response regulators in a coordinated manner and tune downstream gene expression (27).
Based on the signaling cascades we deduced from the in vitro TCS cross talk interactions of M. tuberculosis, it would be interesting to identify corresponding sequences of stimuli, potentially unveiling information of the environments to which M. tuberculosis may have adapted. The ligands/stimuli that many of the TCSs sense, however, remain unknown, precluding such analysis (28). However, specific instances suggesting such adaption could be identified from the cascades. For example, the TCS PrrB-PrrA is reported to be involved in the early replication steps of M. tuberculosis inside macrophages (29). The TCS MprB-MprA has been argued to be essential for establishing persistent infection (30), a state of slower or halted replication from which the bacterium can be reactivated to establish active infection (31). Disruption of mprA affected processes required for survival during the persistence and subsequent infection stages (30). One could thus argue that cross talk from PrrB-PrrA to MprB-MprA may be favorable because it would prime the bacterium to activate the processes necessary for establishing persistent infection, a key feature of successful tuberculosis infection (32), once entry is gained into a macrophage. Indeed, this one-way cross talk was observed in the in vitro cascades (15). Future experiments may assess the advantage of such cross talk in vivo.
Our study has focused on cross talk between HKs and RRs. We recognize that cross talk could also occur at the level of stimuli, where the same stimulus may activate multiple HKs. For instance, the HKs NarX and NarQ of E. coli both sense nitrate ions in the environment (33). The extent of the prevalence of such shared stimulation, however, is unknown, as stimuli for many TCSs still remain uncharacterized (28, 34, 35). Nonetheless, although beyond the scope of the present study, our mathematical model can be readily adapted to analyze cross talk arising at the level of stimuli.
Cross talk is not limited to bacterial TCSs. Examples of cross talk exist in human growth factor signaling networks (36), mitogen-activated protein kinase (MAPK) networks of yeast (37), and between TOR and CIP pathways in Schizosaccharomyces pombe (38). The evolutionary underpinnings of these cross talk interactions may be more difficult to unravel because of the more involved regulatory structures in these organisms than in the simpler bacterial TCS systems. Yet controlled evolutionary experiments suggest selection of cross-regulation patterns in broad agreement with our predictions. For instance, the yeast Saccharomyces cerevisiae, which is commonly used in the fermentation industry, is subjected to heat, ethanolic stress, and oxidative stress, in that order, in the industrial process (22). The related regulatory networks were observed to have the following cross talk interactions: heat ethanolic stress, heat oxidative stress, and ethanolic stress oxidative stress (22). This is similar to the phenotype 12 in the N = 3 case in our model (Fig. 2a). Furthermore, when the organism was artificially exposed to these stresses in the reverse order, the cross talk interactions switched their directions (22). These scenarios, together with our proposed paradigm, point to the possible evolutionary advantages of cross talk.
Our findings have implications for the design of signaling systems in synthetic biology. Bacterial TCSs offer promising routes to engineering signaling systems in synthetic biology constructs (39). For instance, they have been used to engineer E. coli to sense light (40). Synthetic biology constructs are being designed to sense and integrate multiple stimuli (39). The different TCSs used for such designs are typically assumed to be insulated. However, if the constructs are to be employed in environments that see programmed sequences of the stimuli, then with time, phenotypes that favor cross talk between the TCSs may be selected, potentially affecting the robustness of the constructs. Conversely, where integration of well-defined sequences of stimuli is sought, accounting for the potential selection of phenotypes with cross talk may lead to more robust signaling system designs.
Because of its evolutionary advantages, cross talk may be a potential target of intervention. With pathogenic bacteria, cross talk may sharpen the already sophisticated strategies to evade host immune responses and promote virulence (28, 41). Bacterial HKs offer promising targets of intervention (1, 18). Where cross talk may aid bacterial survival and adaptation, as suggested, for instance, with M. tuberculosis (15), targeting HKs engaged in cross talk could prove a more potent strategy than targeting specific HKs. It would not only block the cognate response of the targeted HK, but also compromise the responses of the TCSs that would otherwise have been primed by the targeted HK via cross talk.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Mathematical model of TCS signaling with cross talk.
We developed a mathematical model to describe bacterial signal transduction via TCSs. We considered the scenario in which a bacterium contains N distinct TCSs, which can be engaged in cross talk (Fig. 5a). We built the model by envisioning the set of events associated with the ith TCS engaged in cross talk with the jth TCS (), listed below as reactions.
(1) |
(2) |
(3) |
(4) |
(5) |
(6) |
(7) |
(8) |
(9) |
(10) |
(11) |
(12) |
(13) |
(14) |
(15) |
Here, the subscript i refers to the ith TCS. We recognize that HKi can be activated reversibly at some basal level, i.e., in the absence of any input signal, to its active form, (equation 1) (42). The input, Ii, can bind reversibly to HKi or to yield the complexes IiHKi or , respectively (equations 2 and 3). IiHKi can lead to the activated complex at a rate higher than the basal rate above (equation 4). can bind and activate it via phosphotransfer, yielding HKi and (equation 5). An analogous reaction occurs with binding to RRj (equation 6). Note that in these reactions, j = i would imply cognate interactions. HKi can bind to and exert phosphatase activity (equation 7), consistent with the bifunctional nature of typical HKs, which act as both kinases and phosphatases (1, 9, 43). The latter activity can also be triggered by IiHKi (equation 8). The reversible binding of Ii to the intermediate HK-RR complexes is also possible (equations 9 and 10). Thus, we assumed that RR binding to HK does not influence ligand binding to HK. The difference between the efficiencies of activation of cognate and noncognate RRs by a given HK could come from differences in the association rates, dissociation rates, and/or phosphotransfer rates involved. These latter differences are all rarely quantified, although binding affinities and phosphotransfer rates in some select cases have recently been reported (44, 45). Here, for simplicity, we subsumed the differences into the difference in the association rate constants of the HKs with the cognate and noncognate RRs. Specifically, recognizing that the activation rates of noncognate RRs are lower than those of their cognate counterparts, we let the binding rate constants of noncognate partners (kf,ij,phtrf and kf,ij,phtse) be lower than those of the cognate partners (kf,ii,phtrf and kf,ii,phtse), with the difference quantified by the attenuation factor . dimerizes and binds to the corresponding promoter Pj (equation 11). This binding enhances transcription compared to its basal level (equations 12 and 13), i.e., ktpn > kbtpn, where kbtpn and ktpn correspond to basal and activated transcription rate constants, respectively. Transcription produces mRNA, denoted by m, which is then translated with the rate constant ktrn, with the HK and RR translated in the ratio λ:1 (equation 14). Here, we recognize that the response also typically upregulates the corresponding TCS proteins (2, 46). Input signals degrade with rate constant kdeg,input (equation 15). All the other entities present in the network are assumed to degrade with a rate constant kdeg (not written explicitly for convenience).
Next, we estimated the rate of synthesis of HK and RR proteins by assuming that the DNA binding reactions are fast compared to transcription and translation reactions (15, 20). Let PT be the total concentration of promoter binding sites present on the bacterial genome, with fb and ff the fractions of promoter sites in the bound and free states, respectively. We assumed pseudoequilibrium between DNA binding reactions, yielding
(16) |
If is the equilibrium dissociation constant for equation 11, we get
(17) |
Because fb + fb = 1, it follows that
(18) |
and
(19) |
We now have the concentration of promoters in the basal and active states. Equations 11 to 13 estimate the rate of upregulation of the corresponding TCS as follows. From equations 12 and 13, the change of mRNA concentration would be
(20) |
Applying the pseudoequilibrium approximation to mRNA dynamics, i.e., , gives
(21) |
By substituting equations 18 and 19 into equation 21, we obtain
(22) |
These mRNA molecules translate at the rate ktrn to produce HKj and RRj molecules in the ratio λ:1.
(23) |
(24) |
Substituting and , we get the synthesis rates of HK and RR by mRNA translation as
(25) |
(26) |
The rate equations for equations 1 to 15 can be written following standard mass action terms and by utilizing equations 25 and 26 as follows.
(27) |
(28) |
(29) |
(30) |
(31) |
(32) |
(33) |
(34) |
(35) |
(36) |
(37) |
(38) |
(39) |
The rate constants involved were obtained from the literature (9, 20, 47) (Table S1). The rate equations were integrated in MATLAB using the routine ode15s and with chosen initial conditions (Table S1). In all our simulations, the above-described equations were first solved in the absence of stimuli for a sufficiently long time so that the basal autophosphorylation reactions balanced the degradation reactions and all the proteins reached a steady state. Using the latter as the prestimulus state of the bacterium, the above-described equations were solved in the presence of stimuli. The solution depended on the phenotype, described next.
Interaction matrix.
For a bacterium with N TCSs, different phenotypes are possible depending on the presence or absence of specific cross talk interactions. An interaction matrix defines the identity of each phenotype (Fig. 5b). The ijth element in the matrix represents the strength of the cross-interaction between HKi and RRj relative to the cognate interaction. The cognate interactions are all assumed to be equally strong and occupy the diagonal entries. The cross-interactions are also assumed to be of the same relative intensity, γ, whenever they exist. The nondiagonal entities are thus either 0 or γ. Since there are N(N – 1) nondiagonal elements present, with 2 state values possible for each of them, we get 2N(N–1) different phenotypes.
Fitness formulation.
We constructed a fitness variable based on the response of a TCS to a time-dependent input. We defined the fitness corresponding to the ith TCS as
(40) |
where follows from equation 19 above. The term −Ii(t)/Im reflects the inverse relationship between the fitness and input intensity. Im is taken as the maximum (or peak) input value. Thus, as Ii increases, it reflects an increasing change in the environment, inducing a more significant fitness loss until the bacterium responds and adapts. The recovery of fitness following the response is determined by the second entity in the fitness variable, 1 − fb, where fb denotes the fraction of promoters bound by RR*. (We recall that K1 is the dissociation constant of .) As this fraction increases, the magnitude of the response also rises, leading to greater fitness given the signal. This formulation of fitness makes sure that ϕi lies between 0 and 1. TCSs are assumed to contribute independently to fitness. Thus, for a bacterium with N TCSs, the total instantaneous fitness is the product of individual fitness values:
(41) |
In the absence of any signal, ϕ = 1. Similarly, with a perfect response, i.e., with fb = 1, ϕ is again 1. We also considered an alternative fitness formulation and found no qualitative differences in our results (Text S1).
Stochastic evolutionary simulations.
We performed Wright-Fisher simulations to describe the competition between different phenotypes in random and programmed environments. Such simulations have been used widely to study evolutionary dynamics, including to describe viral diversification and the development of drug resistance (48, 49) and the development of antibody responses following vaccination (50, 51). We considered discrete generations with a fixed population of bacteria. Our simulations had these steps:
- We initialized the population in one of two ways:
- Homogeneous population, comprising a colony of a single, chosen phenotype
- Mixed population, comprising equal numbers of all possible phenotypes
- We computed the fitness of bacteria as follows:
- In a programmed environment, we employed the sequence of stimuli I1→I2→…→IN. The fitness of each phenotype was the time-average of the fitness ϕ(t) when all the N signals were elicited once:
Here, T was chosen to be the time when the last signal faded away.(42) - In a random environment, the signals were elicited in a random sequence. Thus, NN signal sequences were possible, allowing the signals to repeat. The fitness of each phenotype was then the mean of its time-averaged fitness estimated separately for each of the NN possible sequences:
(43) (44)
- We next estimated control fitness, measuring the fitness in the absence of any response, using:
This has the same expression as ϕi, but without the fb term.(45) Fitness selection happens on the bacteria in a generation. For each bacterium, we examined whether the fitness ‹ϕ› was larger than ϕcontrol + (1 − ϕcontrol) × r, where was a random number from a uniform distribution. The latter choice accounted for any stochastic variations in environmental factors and associated selection forces. If ‹ϕ› was larger, the bacterium survived. Otherwise, it was removed. This formalism ensured that bacteria that mounted no responses were not selected and that the rest survived with probabilities proportional to their fitness.
From the survivors, we randomly selected, using a uniform random distribution, some bacteria and duplicated them to replace lost bacteria and maintain the population constant. This process assumes that surviving bacteria all have the same ability to multiply.
We mutated the resulting bacteria. In our simulations, a mutation toggled a potential cross talk interaction between on and off. For instance, for a bacterium with cross talk between HKi and RRj, mutation would turn the corresponding kf,ij,phtrf and kf,ij,phtse from γ × 10−3 nM−1 s−1 to 0. Every bacterium was checked for the possibility of mutation with probability μ at each of the 2N(N–1) cross talk interactions possible.
We repeated the above procedure from step 4.
One generation in our simulation time frame was typically T = N × 500 s, with N signals elicited in each generation. This made sure that all the TCSs could be triggered in principle. We performed simulations over 10,000 generations and over 50 realizations for each parameter setting, which ensured reliable statistics.
Data availability.
The MATLAB codes used to estimate the fitness values and perform Wright-Fisher simulations and the codon and amino acid sequence files, domain information, alignment files, and raw data for the resulting phylogenetic trees employed for evolution analyses are available at the GitHub repository (https://github.com/narendradixit/TCS_crosstalk_evolution).
Supplementary Material
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank Sandhya Visweswariah and Supreet Saini for comments and Gaurav Sankhe for input and discussions.
B.V., A.V.P., and N.M.D. designed the study. B.V. and A.V.P. developed the models and codes and performed calculations. B.V., A.V.P., D.K.S., and N.M.D., analyzed the data. B.V. and N.M.D. wrote the paper. B.V., A.V.P., D.K.S., and N.M.D. edited the paper.
We declare that we do not have any competing interests.
Contributor Information
Narendra M. Dixit, Email: narendra@iisc.ac.in.
Vanni Bucci, University of Massachusetts Medical School.
REFERENCES
- 1.Stock AM, Robinson VL, Goudreau PN. 2000. Two-component signal transduction. Annu Rev Biochem 69:183–215. doi: 10.1146/annurev.biochem.69.1.183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Goulian M. 2010. Two-component signaling circuit structure and properties. Curr Opin Microbiol 13:184–189. doi: 10.1016/j.mib.2010.01.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Agrawal R, Narayan VH, Saini DK. 2013. Two-component signalling systems of M. tuberculosis: regulators of pathogenicity and more, p 79–109. In Sree Hari Rao V, Durvasula R (ed), Dynamic models of infectious diseases. Springer, New York, NY. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Laub MT, Goulian M. 2007. Specificity in two-component signal transduction pathways. Annu Rev Genet 41:121–145. doi: 10.1146/annurev.genet.41.042007.170548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Capra EJ, Laub MT. 2012. Evolution of two-component signal transduction systems. Annu Rev Microbiol 66:325–347. doi: 10.1146/annurev-micro-092611-150039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Alm E, Huang K, Arkin A. 2006. The evolution of two-component systems in bacteria reveals different strategies for niche adaptation. PLoS Comput Biol 2:e143. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.0020143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Salazar ME, Laub MT. 2015. Temporal and evolutionary dynamics of two-component signaling pathways. Curr Opin Microbiol 24:7–14. doi: 10.1016/j.mib.2014.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Capra EJ, Perchuk BS, Skerker JM, Laub MT. 2012. Adaptive mutations that prevent crosstalk enable the expansion of paralogous signaling protein families. Cell 150:222–232. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2012.05.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Rowland MA, Deeds EJ. 2014. Crosstalk and the evolution of specificity in two-component signaling. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 111:5550–5555. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1317178111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Podgornaia AI, Laub MT. 2013. Determinants of specificity in two-component signal transduction. Curr Opin Microbiol 16:156–162. doi: 10.1016/j.mib.2013.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Groban ES, Clarke EJ, Salis HM, Miller SM, Voigt CA. 2009. Kinetic buffering of cross talk between bacterial two-component sensors. J Mol Biol 390:380–393. doi: 10.1016/j.jmb.2009.05.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.McClune CJ, Alvarez-Buylla A, Voigt CA, Laub MT. 2019. Engineering orthogonal signalling pathways reveals the sparse occupancy of sequence space. Nature 574:702–706. doi: 10.1038/s41586-019-1639-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Agrawal R, Sahoo BK, Saini DK. 2016. Cross-talk and specificity in two-component signal transduction pathways. Future Microbiol 11:685–697. doi: 10.2217/fmb-2016-0001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Yamamoto K, Hirao K, Oshima T, Aiba H, Utsumi R, Ishihama A. 2005. Functional characterization in vitro of all two-component signal transduction systems from Escherichia coli. J Biol Chem 280:1448–1456. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M410104200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Agrawal R, Pandey A, Rajankar MP, Dixit NM, Saini DK. 2015. The two-component signalling networks of Mycobacterium tuberculosis display extensive cross-talk in vitro. Biochem J 469:121–134. doi: 10.1042/BJ20150268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Skerker JM, Prasol MS, Perchuk BS, Biondi EG, Laub MT. 2005. Two-component signal transduction pathways regulating growth and cell cycle progression in a bacterium: a system-level analysis. PLoS Biol 3:e334. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Willett JW, Tiwari N, Muller S, Hummels KR, Houtman JC, Fuentes EJ, Kirby JR. 2013. Specificity residues determine binding affinity for two-component signal transduction systems. mBio 4:e00420-13. doi: 10.1128/mBio.00420-13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Bem AE, Velikova N, Pellicer MT, Baarlen P, Marina A, Wells JM. 2015. Bacterial histidine kinases as novel antibacterial drug targets. ACS Chem Biol 10:213–224. doi: 10.1021/cb5007135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Tiwari S, Jamal SB, Hassan SS, Carvalho P, Almeida S, Barh D, Ghosh P, Silva A, Castro TLP, Azevedo V. 2017. Two-component signal transduction systems of pathogenic bacteria as targets for antimicrobial therapy: an overview. Front Microbiol 8:1878. doi: 10.3389/fmicb.2017.01878. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Tiwari A, Balazsi G, Gennaro ML, Igoshin OA. 2010. The interplay of multiple feedback loops with post-translational kinetics results in bistability of mycobacterial stress response. Phys Biol 7:e036005. doi: 10.1088/1478-3975/7/3/036005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Batchelor E, Goulian M. 2003. Robustness and the cycle of phosphorylation and dephosphorylation in a two-component regulatory system. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100:691–696. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0234782100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Mitchell A, Romano GH, Groisman B, Yona A, Dekel E, Kupiec M, Dahan O, Pilpel Y. 2009. Adaptive prediction of environmental changes by microorganisms. Nature 460:220–224. doi: 10.1038/nature08112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Mitchell A, Pilpel Y. 2011. A mathematical model for adaptive prediction of environmental changes by microorganisms. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 108:7271–7276. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1019754108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Hartl DL, Clark AG, Clark AG. 1997. Principles of population genetics. Sinauer, Sunderland, MA. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Tagkopoulos I, Liu YC, Tavazoie S. 2008. Predictive behavior within microbial genetic networks. Science 320:1313–1317. doi: 10.1126/science.1154456. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Cao Q, Yang N, Wang Y, Xu C, Zhang X, Fan K, Chen F, Liang H, Zhang Y, Deng X, Feng Y, Yang CG, Wu M, Bae T, Lan L. 2020. Mutation-induced remodeling of the BfmRS two-component system in Pseudomonas aeruginosa clinical isolates. Sci Signal 13:eaaz1529. doi: 10.1126/scisignal.aaz1529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Herrou J, Crosson S, Fiebig A. 2017. Structure and function of HWE/HisKA2-family sensor histidine kinases. Curr Opin Microbiol 36:47–54. doi: 10.1016/j.mib.2017.01.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Bretl DJ, Demetriadou C, Zahrt TC. 2011. Adaptation to environmental stimuli within the host: two-component signal transduction systems of Mycobacterium tuberculosis. Microbiol Mol Biol Rev 75:566–582. doi: 10.1128/MMBR.05004-11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Ewann F, Jackson M, Pethe K, Cooper A, Mielcarek N, Ensergueix D, Gicquel B, Locht C, Supply P. 2002. Transient requirement of the PrrA-PrrB two-component system for early intracellular multiplication of Mycobacterium tuberculosis. Infect Immun 70:2256–2263. doi: 10.1128/IAI.70.5.2256-2263.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Zahrt TC, Deretic V. 2001. Mycobacterium tuberculosis signal transduction system required for persistent infections. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:12706–12711. doi: 10.1073/pnas.221272198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Ehrt S, Schnappinger D, Rhee KY. 2018. Metabolic principles of persistence and pathogenicity in Mycobacterium tuberculosis. Nat Rev Microbiol 16:496–507. doi: 10.1038/s41579-018-0013-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Gomez JE, McKinney JD. 2004. M. tuberculosis persistence, latency, and drug tolerance. Tuberculosis (Edinb) 84:29–44. doi: 10.1016/j.tube.2003.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Francis VI, Porter SL. 2019. Multikinase networks: two-component signaling networks integrating multiple stimuli. Annu Rev Microbiol 73:199–223. doi: 10.1146/annurev-micro-020518-115846. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Kundu M. 2018. The role of two-component systems in the physiology of Mycobacterium tuberculosis. IUBMB Life 70:710–717. doi: 10.1002/iub.1872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Parish T. 2014. Two-component regulatory systems of Mycobacteria. Microbiol Spectr 2:2.1.17. doi: 10.1128/microbiolspec.MGM2-0010-2013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Zielinski R, Przytycki PF, Zheng J, Zhang D, Przytycka TM, Capala J. 2009. The crosstalk between EGF, IGF, and Insulin cell signaling pathways: computational and experimental analysis. BMC Syst Biol 3:88. doi: 10.1186/1752-0509-3-88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Lee B, Jeong SG, Jin SH, Mishra R, Peter M, Lee CS, Lee SS. 2020. Quantitative analysis of yeast MAPK signaling networks and crosstalk using a microfluidic device. Lab Chip 20:2646–2655. doi: 10.1039/d0lc00203h. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Madrid M, Vazquez-Marin B, Franco A, Soto T, Vicente-Soler J, Gacto M, Cansado J. 2016. Multiple crosstalk between TOR and the cell integrity MAPK signaling pathway in fission yeast. Sci Rep 6:37515. doi: 10.1038/srep37515. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Ninfa AJ. 2010. Use of two-component signal transduction systems in the construction of synthetic genetic networks. Curr Opin Microbiol 13:240–245. doi: 10.1016/j.mib.2010.01.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Levskaya A, Chevalier AA, Tabor JJ, Simpson ZB, Lavery LA, Levy M, Davidson EA, Scouras A, Ellington AD, Marcotte EM, Voigt CA. 2005. Synthetic biology: engineering Escherichia coli to see light. Nature 438:441–442. doi: 10.1038/nature04405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Cambier CJ, Falkow S, Ramakrishnan L. 2014. Host evasion and exploitation schemes of Mycobacterium tuberculosis. Cell 159:1497–1509. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.11.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Sankhe GD, Dixit NM, Saini DK. 2018. Activation of bacterial histidine kinases: insights into the kinetics of the cis autophosphorylation mechanism. mSphere 3:e00111-18. doi: 10.1128/mSphere.00111-18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Casino P, Rubio V, Marina A. 2009. Structural insight into partner specificity and phosphoryl transfer in two-component signal transduction. Cell 139:325–336. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2009.08.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Sankhe GD, Raja R, Dixit NM, Saini DK. 2021. Sequestration of histidine kinases by non-cognate response regulators establishes a threshold level of stimulation for bacterial two-component signaling. bioRxiv. doi: 10.1101/2021.12.30.474508. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 45.Singh KK, Bhardwaj N, Sankhe GD, Udaykumar N, Singh R, Malhotra V, Saini DK. 2019. Acetylation of response regulator proteins, TcrX and MtrA in M. tuberculosis tunes their phosphotransfer ability and modulates two-component signaling crosstalk. J Mol Biol 431:777–793. doi: 10.1016/j.jmb.2019.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Ray JC, Igoshin OA. 2010. Adaptable functionality of transcriptional feedback in bacterial two-component systems. PLoS Comput Biol 6:e1000676. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Qin L, Yoshida T, Inouye M. 2001. The critical role of DNA in the equilibrium between OmpR and phosphorylated OmpR mediated by EnvZ in Escherichia coli. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:908–913. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.3.908. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Gadhamsetty S, Dixit NM. 2010. Estimating frequencies of minority nevirapine-resistant strains in chronically HIV-1-infected individuals naive to nevirapine by using stochastic simulations and a mathematical model. J Virol 84:10230–10240. doi: 10.1128/JVI.01010-10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Tripathi K, Balagam R, Vishnoi NK, Dixit NM. 2012. Stochastic simulations suggest that HIV-1 survives close to its error threshold. PLoS Comput Biol 8:e1002684. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Garg AK, Desikan R, Dixit NM. 2019. Preferential presentation of high-affinity immune complexes in germinal centers can explain how passive immunization improves the humoral response. Cell Rep 29:3946–3957.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2019.11.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Garg AK, Mittal S, Padmanabhan P, Desikan R, Dixit NM. 2021. Increased B cell selection stringency in germinal centers can explain improved COVID-19 vaccine efficacies with low dose prime or delayed boost. Front Immunol 12:776933. doi: 10.3389/fimmu.2021.776933. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The MATLAB codes used to estimate the fitness values and perform Wright-Fisher simulations and the codon and amino acid sequence files, domain information, alignment files, and raw data for the resulting phylogenetic trees employed for evolution analyses are available at the GitHub repository (https://github.com/narendradixit/TCS_crosstalk_evolution).