Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2022 Dec 20;11:e77806. doi: 10.7554/eLife.77806

Personality traits are consistently associated with blood mitochondrial DNA copy number estimated from genome sequences in two genetic cohort studies

Richard F Oppong 1, Antonio Terracciano 2,3, Martin Picard 4, Yong Qian 1, Thomas J Butler 1, Toshiko Tanaka 1, Ann Zenobia Moore 1, Eleanor M Simonsick 1, Krista Opsahl-Ong 1, Christopher Coletta 5, Angelina R Sutin 6, Myriam Gorospe 5, Susan M Resnick 3, Francesco Cucca 7, Sonja W Scholz 8,9, Bryan J Traynor 9,10, David Schlessinger 5, Luigi Ferrucci 1,, Jun Ding 1,
Editors: Jonathan Flint11, Ma-Li Wong12
PMCID: PMC9767459  PMID: 36537669

Abstract

Background:

Mitochondrial DNA copy number (mtDNAcn) in tissues and blood can be altered in conditions like diabetes and major depression and may play a role in aging and longevity. However, little is known about the association between mtDNAcn and personality traits linked to emotional states, metabolic health, and longevity. This study tests the hypothesis that blood mtDNAcn is related to personality traits and mediates the association between personality and mortality.

Methods:

We assessed the big five personality domains and facets using the Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R), assessed depressive symptoms with the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D), estimated mtDNAcn levels from whole-genome sequencing, and tracked mortality in participants from the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Results were replicated in the SardiNIA Project.

Results:

We found that mtDNAcn was negatively associated with the Neuroticism domain and its facets and positively associated with facets from the other four domains. The direction and size of the effects were replicated in the SardiNIA cohort and were robust to adjustment for potential confounders in both samples. Consistent with the Neuroticism finding, higher depressive symptoms were associated with lower mtDNAcn. Finally, mtDNAcn mediated the association between personality and mortality risk.

Conclusions:

To our knowledge, this is the first study to show a replicable association between mtDNAcn and personality. Furthermore, the results support our hypothesis that mtDNAcn is a biomarker of the biological process that explains part of the association between personality and mortality.

Funding:

Support for this work was provided by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institute on Aging (Z01-AG000693, Z01-AG000970, and Z01-AG000949) and the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke of the National Institutes of Health. AT was also supported by the National Institute on Aging of the National Institutes of Health Grant R01AG068093.

Research organism: Human

eLife digest

Cells are powered by internal structures called mitochondria which have their own DNA molecules. How many copies of mitochondrial DNA blood cells contain is one aspect of mitochondrial health and is considered to provide a good indication of an individual’s ability to convert glucose into energy. Consequently, changes in the amount of mitochondrial DNA in the blood are linked to conditions like diabetes and cancer, and have also been associated with aging and mortality.

A set of well-classified personality traits known as ‘the Big Five’ have also been shown to affect energy levels and the longevity of individuals. However, it remained unclear if there is a relationship between these characteristics and the number of copies of mitochondrial DNA in the blood.

To investigate, Oppong et al. used a specialized test to assess the personality traits of participants from two separate cohorts: Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Ageing and the SardiNIA Project. The genomic sequence of each person was then analyzed to calculate the amount of mitochondrial DNA in their blood, and their mortality was recorded based on whether they were alive or dead multiple years later.

Oppong et al. found that low levels of mitochondrial DNA were linked with high scores in neuroticism (a trait typically associated with anxiety, depression, and self-doubt). Further statistical tests revealed that mitochondrial DNA levels mediate the relationship between a person’s personality and their risk of death.

These findings suggest that personality traits impact the number of mitochondrial DNA molecules in a person’s blood, which, in turn, influences how long they are likely to live. However, further work is needed to find out what causes this effect.

Introduction

Mitochondria are cellular organelles essential for the life of eukaryotic cells (Duchen, 2004). They produce adenosine 5'-triphosphate (ATP), the main energetic currency for biological function, by oxidative phosphorylation (OXPHOS) of a number of substrates (Attardi and Schatz, 1988). Mitochondria harbor their own circular DNA molecules (mtDNA), which lack histone proteins and bear genes lacking introns (Attardi and Schatz, 1988). The proximity of mtDNA to OXPHOS production and its lack of histone protection make it more susceptible to ROS damage, threatening healthy cellular function (Clay Montier et al., 2009). The number of copies of mtDNA (mtDNAcn) in tissues and blood leukocytes is considered an imperfect proxy measure of mitochondrial mass that may reflect energetic resilience or hematopoiesis (Picard, 2021).

Consequently, reduced mtDNAcn in peripheral blood has been associated with various health outcomes, including renal cell carcinoma (Xing et al., 2008), hypertension (Hägg et al., 2021), neurodegenerative disease (Yang et al., 2021), and diabetes-related conditions, including insulin sensitivity (Gianotti et al., 2008; Song et al., 2001), fasting plasma glucose level (Xu et al., 2012), and diabetic nephropathy (Al-Kafaji et al., 2018). High levels of mtDNAcn in tissue and the blood can also occur in conditions of stress and illness such as diabetes (Moore et al., 2018) and peripheral artery disease (McDermott et al., 2018), suggesting that in some situations of stress, mtDNAcn can be elevated to compensate for low cellular energy (Picard, 2021; Giordano et al., 2014) and stress adaptation (Picard and McEwen, 2018a). For major depression, however, the findings are mixed, with some studies reporting that mtDNAcn has a positive (Cai et al., 2015), negative (Kim et al., 2011), or null association with depression (de Sousa et al., 2014; He et al., 2014; Lindqvist et al., 2018; Verhoeven et al., 2018; Vyas et al., 2020).

Since specific personality traits affect individuals’ susceptibility to environmental stress (Ebstrup et al., 2011) and the development of pathology and premature death, we hypothesized that blood mtDNAcn is a biomarker of stress burden and might mediate the association between personality traits and excess mortality (Chapman et al., 2020). To test this hypothesis, we tested the relationship between personality and levels of whole-blood mtDNAcn estimated by whole-genome sequencing and whether mtDNAcn mediated the relation between personality and mortality. To ensure replicability and robustness, we tested these associations in two cohorts, one from the United States and the other from Italy.

To assess personality, we used participants’ profiles of the big five domains, including the six facets of each domain, measured with the Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R), a personality measure validated extensively in multiple populations and languages (Costa and McCrae, 2008). Personality traits measured with the NEO-PI-R have been associated with stressors such as anxiety and depression (Hayward et al., 2013; Kotov et al., 2010), cognitive decline (Caselli et al., 2016; Luchetti et al., 2016), metabolic dysfunction (Jokela et al., 2014; Stephan et al., 2016), energy levels (Terracciano et al., 2013), and physical illness (Smith and MacKenzie, 2006). Facets of personality have also been associated with mortality risk (Chapman et al., 2020). Compared with behavioral pathways to mortality risk (Turiano et al., 2015), there has been less attention on the physiological mechanisms that explain the well-known association between personality and mortality risk. As mentioned above, lower mtDNAcn has been associated with both stressful situations and higher mortality (Mengel-From et al., 2014). We thus address the critical novel question by testing whether mtDNAcn mediates the association between personality traits and mortality risk.

The goal of this study was to investigate the association between blood mtDNAcn and personality traits and facets assessed by the NEO-PI-R in two epidemiological studies: the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging (BLSA) (Kuo et al., 2020; Stone and Norris, 1966) and the SardiNIA study (Pilia et al., 2006). Using mediation analysis, we further tested the hypothesis that mtDNAcn is a biomarker in the biological process that connects personality characteristics with mortality.

Methods

Study cohorts’ description

Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging

The BLSA is an ongoing scientific study that aims to understand risk factors and pathways that cause a decline in physical and cognitive function with aging. The study began in 1958 and is run by the National Institute on Aging Intramural Research Program and enrolls healthy volunteers. Blood samples were obtained from 955 individuals, and DNA was extracted from a red-cell-free buffy coat and sequenced on a HiSeq X Ten sequencer using 150 base-pair, paired-end cycles (version 2.5 chemistry, Illumina), as described elsewhere (Chia et al., 2021). For genetic homogeneity, we retained only participants with white ancestry for further analysis downstream. We further excluded participants who had developed Alzheimer’s disease (AD) from further analysis because their NEO-PI-R profiles are likely changed by clinical dementia (Islam et al., 2019). Therefore, 722 participants (mean age 75, ranging from 48 to 100, 48% were women; Table 1) remained for downstream analysis. All participants gave written informed consent, with protocols approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Intramural Research Program of the National Institutes of Health (protocol number 03-AG-0325).

Table 1. Demography of study cohorts.
BLSA (n = 722) SardiNIA (n = 587)
n/%/mean (SD) n/%/mean (SD)
Female 48.1% 62.2%
Age (years) 74.88 (10.86) 57.28 (13.24)
Education (years) 17.68 (2.50) 7.33 (3.96)
Big five personality traits
Neuroticism 45.35 (8.25) 55.46 (8.36)
Extraversion 50.60 (10.10) 46.80 (8.04)
Openness 52.59 (10.55) 44.35 (9.08)
Agreeableness 51.88 (9.51) 47.55 (8.55)
Conscientiousness 51.91 (9.31) 49.20 (8.79)
Measures of depressive symptoms
CES-D continuous 5.47 (5.66) 11.66 (7.79)
CES-D binary cutoff 16 6.6% 23.8%
CES-D binary cutoff 20 2.6% 13.9%

BLSA, Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging; CES-D, Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale.

SardiNIA Longitudinal Study

SardiNIA (Pilia et al., 2006) is a longitudinal study of human aging and genetics of quantitative traits that has followed a genetically isolated population on the Italian island of Sardinia since 2000. The data used for this study consist of 2077 participants who had their DNA from whole blood sequenced using Illumina Genome Analyzer IIx and Illumina HiSeq 2000 instruments. We further excluded one of each pair of participants with a genetic relationship above the level of second-degree cousins. A total of 587 participants (mean age 57, ranging from 15 to 96, 62% women; Table 1) remained for further downstream analysis. All participants gave written informed consent, with protocols approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Intramural Research Program of the National Institutes of Health (protocol number 04-AG-N317).

Phenotype measures and definition

Personality was assessed using the NEO-PI-R (Costa and McCrae, 2008). The NEO-PI-R is an inventory of 240 items that examines an individual’s thoughts, feelings, and behaviors (Costa and McCrae, 2008). Each item is answered on a five-point Likert scale ranging from ‘strongly disagree’ to ‘strongly agree.’ Responses to these items are scored to assess the big five personality traits (the five domains of personality traits): Neuroticism (N), Extraversion (E), Openness (O), Agreeableness (A), and Conscientiousness (C). In addition to the five domains, the NEO-PI-R also assesses six facets within each domain, for a total of 30 facets (e.g., for the Neuroticism domain, the following six facets are assessed: Anxiety [N1], Angry Hostility [N2], Depression [N3], Self-Consciousness [N4], Impulsiveness [N5], Vulnerability [N6]). Scores are standardized using a normative population to generate standardized T scores (mean = 50, SD = 10) (Costa and McCrae, 2008). These T scores were used in the analysis.

BLSA participants completed the NEO-PI-R at each clinic visit (four assessment points on average), and we used the average across all assessment points for the downstream analysis. We did this because barring onset and progression of clinical dementia, which causes a drastic change in NEO-PI-R profiles (Terracciano and Sutin, 2019), NEO-PI-R scores are quite stable in adults over long periods (Costa and McCrae, 1988). Averaging the NEO-PI-R scores over multiple visits improves the BLSA analysis by reducing the standard deviation of the effects estimates and therefore increases statistical power. SardiNIA participants completed the NEO-PI-R once, and we used this one-time measure for downstream analysis. An extensive literature supports the validity and reliability of the NEO-PI-R scales in the United States and other countries, including Italy (McCrae and Terracciano, 2005; Terracciano, 2003). In both the BLSA and SardiNIA, the five traits have high internal consistency (alpha > 0.80), and in the BLSA, the test–retest stability of the five traits ranges from 0.78 to 0.83 over a follow-up of 10 years (Costa et al., 2007; Terracciano et al., 2006; Appendix 1—figure 5). The correlations between the NEO-PI-R traits are shown in Appendix 1—figure 6.

We also analyzed in both studies the measure of depressive symptoms using the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) (Radloff, 1977). The CES-D is a 20-item instrument assessing depressive mood and behavior over the past week. Responses to these statements are graded on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from ‘rarely or none of the time’ to ‘most or all of the time.’ The responses are scored from 0 to 3 and summed across the 20 items to assess depressive symptoms (range 0–60). In general populations, a CES-D score of 16 is the clinical cutoff used to classify depressive symptoms (Beekman et al., 1997; Delitala et al., 2016). However, among older populations, a cutoff score of 20 is used to improve the accuracy of the diagnosis of major depression (Milaneschi et al., 2011; Penninx et al., 1998). We, therefore, analyzed two binary clinical cutoffs of CES-D (16 and 20 for significant depressive symptoms) and continuous scores in both studies. CES-D in both studies was assessed at a single time point.

mtDNA copy number estimation

The whole-blood mtDNAcn of each study participant was estimated from whole-genome sequence information using fastMitoCalc (Qian et al., 2017). This software implements the formula below, as described by Ding et al., 2015.

mtDNAcopynumberpercell=mtDNAaveragecoverageautosomalDNAaveragecoverage×2

The above formula was derived from the fact that average sequencing coverage should be proportional to DNA copy number for autosomal DNA and mtDNA. Since there are two copies of autosomal DNA in each cell, the mtDNAcn is inferred as a ratio of average mtDNA coverage to autosomal DNA coverage multiplied by two. The average coverage of mtDNA and autosomal DNA (Appendix 1—figure 7) is obtained using sequence alignment with SAMtools (Li et al., 2009).

Statistical analysis

We performed initial simple linear regression of mtDNAcn values on variables such as age, sex, sequence coverage (the average autosomal coverage for each study participant), platelet count, and white blood cell (WBC) parameters, including WBC count, percentages of the major leukocytes: lymphocytes, neutrophils, eosinophils, monocytes, and basophils. These initial assessments helped us determine which variables should be included as covariates in subsequent multiple linear regression models of mtDNAcn on NEO-PI-R traits.

As expected from previous findings reviewed in Picard, 2021, age, sex, WBC count, platelet count, and percentages of neutrophils, lymphocytes, and basophils were significantly associated with mtDNAcn (p-value<0.0001). Sequence coverage and percentage of eosinophils were nominally associated with mtDNAcn (p-value<0.05), while percentage of monocytes was marginally associated with mtDNAcn (p-value=0.0821). Therefore, all regression analyses relating mtDNAcn with personality traits or depression were adjusted for age, sex, WBC and platelet count, percent neutrophils, lymphocytes, basophils, and eosinophils, and sequence coverage.

Standardized values were used in regression analyses to make effect sizes more comparable across the two study cohorts. Therefore, all reported NEO-PI-R associations are expressed as the number of standard deviations change in mtDNAcn in response to higher NEO-PI-R trait values. A random-effect meta-analysis was used to obtain the combined estimates of effects and 95% confidence interval (CI) of each NEO-PI-R/ CES-D trait on mtDNAcn using the standardized effect sizes obtained from the two study cohorts.

Personality, mtDNAcn, and mortality risk

In previous studies, the vulnerability facet of Neuroticism, the activity facet of Extraversion, and the self-discipline and competence facets of Conscientiousness were associated with mortality risk, with vulnerability (increased risk) and self-discipline (reduced risk) showing the strongest associations (Chapman et al., 2020). To confirm these findings in the literature, we classified BLSA and SardiNIA participants according to vulnerability and self-discipline scores below and above the median level, and cross-classified participants into four groups by combinations of these two dichotomous variables (see Appendix 1—figure 8 for more details). We tested their association with mtDNAcn using the low-vulnerability/high self-discipline group as a reference. We also used the Cox proportional hazards model to test for their association with mortality, adjusting for age and sex.

In subsequent analyses, we computed a personality-mortality index (PMI) using data from the four NEO-PI-R facets (vulnerability, activity, self-discipline, and competence) that have been previously shown as significantly associated with mortality by Chapman et al., 2020. For each of the four traits, we assigned a score of 0 or 1 to each individual based on the median value and the direction of the association of the trait with mortality. For example, individuals with vulnerability measures lower than the median received a score of 1 for the trait, and individuals with self-discipline measures higher than the median received a score of 1. The final PMI is the sum of the four scores for the four traits. The index ranged between 0 and 4, where a score of 0 means an individual has a high vulnerability, low self-discipline, low activity, and low competence (i.e., a poor PMI and potentially highest mortality risk). A score of 4 represents low vulnerability, high self-discipline, high activity, and high competence, indicating a favorable PMI and potentially lowest mortality risk. Scores of 1, 2, and 3 are various levels of improved PMI. We compared the mtDNAcn among the five indices (0, 1, 2, 3, and 4) of the PMI. We also tested for an association between PMI as a continuous variable and mtDNAcn, and death (a binary variable that classified study participants as dead or alive).

Finally, we used R package lavaan (version 0.6-7) to perform a mediation analysis to test whether mtDNAcn mediates the association between personality (i.e., PMI) and mortality risk (a binary variable describing the dead or alive status of study participants). In general, there are three steps in a mediation analysis to show a potential mediator variable mediates the relationship between an independent variable and a dependent variable. Briefly, the three steps in our specific analysis are (1) a total effect model, which regresses mortality (dependent variable) on PMI (independent variable) to confirm that PMI is a significant predictor of mortality; (2) a mediator model, which regresses mtDNAcn (mediator) on PMI to confirm that PMI is a significant predictor of mtDNAcn; and (3) an outcome model, which regresses mortality on both mtDNAcn and PMI to confirm that (a) mtDNAcn is a significant predictor of mortality and (b) the strength of the previously significant effect of PMI in step (1) is now greatly reduced (i.e., the effect of PMI on mortality is largely indirect via mtDNAcn). In our analysis, the significance of the indirect effect was estimated using 5000 bootstrap samples.

Results

Relationship of sex, age, and WBC parameters with mtDNAcn

In both cohorts, mtDNAcn was negatively associated with age and male biological sex (Appendix 1—table 1). Furthermore, higher mtDNAcn was associated with lower WBC count and lower neutrophil percentage and was also associated with higher platelet count, lymphocyte percentage, eosinophil percentage, monocyte percentage, and basophil percentage (Appendix 1—table 1), consistent with previous findings (Picard, 2021). After adjusting for the effect of sex, sequence coverage (mean sequence coverage of 35.8× and 3.8×, respectively, for BLSA and SardiNIA), platelet count, and WBC parameters, there remained a significant, albeit modest, inverse association of mtDNAcn with age in both study cohorts (Figure 1 and Appendix 1—table 1). For the BLSA cohort, on average, every 1-year increase in age was associated with a 0.014 standard deviation decrease in mtDNAcn (about 3.89% decrease in mtDNAcn per decade of life, p-value =2.5×10-5), while in the SardiNIA cohort, there was a 0.0065 standard deviation decline in mtDNAcn for every year increase in age (about 1.36% decrease in mtDNAcn per decade of life, p-value=0.028). After adjusting for all the other covariates, women still had a significantly higher mtDNAcn than men (Figure 1 and Appendix 1—table 1).

Figure 1. Association of mitochondrial DNA copy number (mtDNAcn) with age, and sex differences.

Figure 1.

Panel (A) is the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging (BLSA) cohort, and panel (B) is the SardiNIA cohort. Plots (i) are regression of mtDNAcn residuals on age after adjusting for sex, sequence coverage, platelets count, and white blood cell parameters. Plots (ii) are comparisons of mtDNAcn residuals after adjusting for age, sequence coverage, platelets count, and white blood cell parameters between males (M) and females (F), with t-test p-values shown.

Association of personality traits with mtDNA copy number

To test the association between NEO-PI-R traits and mtDNAcn in each study cohort, we regressed mtDNAcn values on each personality trait, adjusting for age, sex, sequence coverage, platelet count, and WBC parameters. In both samples, mtDNAcn was associated with lower Neuroticism and higher Extraversion, Openness, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness (Figure 2). We then performed a random-effects meta-analysis and used a false discovery rate (FDR)-corrected p-value threshold of 0.01. Among the 35 personality traits (5 domains, 30 facets) tested, 9 (5 facets in the Neuroticism domain and 4 from Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness) were associated significantly with mtDNAcn even after the stringent FDR correction. Table 2 shows the individual study and meta-analysis results for the Neuroticism domain, while Appendix 1—table 2 provides detailed results for all 35 NEO-PI-R traits.

Figure 2. Association of Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) traits with mitochondrial DNA copy number (mtDNAcn).

Figure 2.

Standardized/pooled effects with 95% CI of NEO-PI-R traits after adjusting for the effects of age, sex, sequence coverage, platelets count, and white blood cell parameters. Color coding is green for positive personality types, red for negative personality types, and blue for the composite scores for the big five NEO-PI-R domains. * Traits with false discovery rate (FDR)-corrected meta-analysis p-values≤0.01.

Table 2. Association of Neuroticism domain with mitochondrial DNA copy number (mtDNAcn).

Domain and facets BLSA β (p-value) SardiNIA β (p-value) Pooled β (p-value)* I2 % [p of Q]
Neuroticism –0.106 (0.001) –0.116 (0.01) –0.11 (0.0015) 0% [0.87]
Anxiety –0.082 (0.01) –0.086 (0.08) –0.08 (0.010) 0% [0.94]
Angry hostility –0.078 (0.02) –0.090 (0.054) –0.08 (0.010) 0% [0.84]
Depression –0.122 (0.0002) –0.070 (0.1) –0.11 (0.0015) 0% [0.37]
Self-consciousness –0.060 (0.08) –0.116 (0.01) –0.08 (0.013) 0% [0.33]
Impulsiveness –0.067 (0.046) –0.023 (0.6) –0.05 (0.099) 0% [0.45]
Vulnerability –0.080 (0.02) –0.097 (0.04) –0.09 (0.0087) 0% [0.77]
Measures of depressive symptoms
CES-D continuous –0.122 (0.0002) –0.077 (0.13) –0.11 (0.00008) 0% [0.46]
CES-D binary cutoff 16 –0.260 (0.051) –0.180 (0.13) –0.22 (0.015) 0% [0.65]
CES-D binary cutoff 20 –0.591 (0.005) –0.343 (0.02) –0.42 (0.0004) 0% [0.34]
PMI
Personality-mortality index 0.092 (0.005) 0.124 (0.007) 0.10 (0.0001) 0% [0.57]

BLSA, Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging; CES-D, Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale; PMI, personality-mortality index; FDR, false discovery rate.

*

Pooled effect p-values of Neuroticism domain and facets are FDR-corrected for 35 tests for 35 personality traits. Information for other personality domains and facets are provided in Appendix 1—table 2.

The Neuroticism domain showed the most significant associations with mtDNAcn. The overall Neuroticism domain and four out of six facets were associated significantly (FDR p-value≤0.01) with mtDNAcn (Table 2). The four facets were anxiety, angry hostility, depression, and vulnerability. Of note, facet self-consciousness was associated with mtDNAcn with an FDR-corrected p-value of 0.013. Notably, all neuroticism traits were associated negatively with mtDNAcn, with higher trait values corresponding to lower mtDNAcn values. Furthermore, the higher-order Neuroticism domain was the personality trait associated with mtDNAcn with the most significant p-value (FDR p-value=0.0015). The Neuroticism domain also showed highly consistent associations between the two study cohorts, with I2 (a measure of inconsistency among individual studies) equal to 0 for Neuroticism and its facets. Appendix 1—figure 1 shows the effect sizes for the Neuroticism domain and facets.

The overall Extraversion domain, the positive emotions facet in the Extraversion domain, the altruism facet in the Agreeableness domain, and the dutifulness facet in the Conscientiousness domain were associated with mtDNAcn with FDR-corrected p-values<0.01 (Appendix 1—table 2). These are all positive associations, with higher trait values corresponding to higher mtDNAcn values. No facets in the Openness domain were significantly associated with mtDNAcn.

Appendix 1—figure 2 compares the standardized effect sizes of the NEO-PI-R traits between the two study cohorts. The results show remarkable concordance between the two study cohorts, with the data points clustering close to the diagonal line. In a sensitivity analysis evaluating the robustness of these findings to covariate adjustments, we compared the standardized effect sizes of the personality traits using models with and without adjustment for platelet count and WBC parameters (Appendix 1—figure 3). As most of the points were closely aligned to the diagonal line, we concluded that adjusting for platelet count and WBC parameters did not significantly impact the results.

Association of CES-D with mtDNAcn

In both cohorts, we also regressed mtDNAcn on both continuous and binary CES-D values, adjusting for the effect of other covariates. Higher CES-D scores, measured as a continuous variable, were associated with lower mtDNAcn. Participants with depressive symptoms that exceeded the CES-D cutoff of either 16 or 20 had 6.06 or 11.57% lower mtDNAcn than those with no depressive symptoms, respectively (Table 2). Remarkably, in both cohorts, the effect sizes almost doubled when moving from moderate depressive symptoms (CES-D binary cutoff 16) to significant depressive symptoms (CES-D binary cutoff 20). The random-effects meta-analysis supported both the magnitude and direction of the effects with highly significant association p-values. There was also high consistency across the two cohorts for the continuous and binary CES-D traits, with I2 equal to 0.

Personality, mtDNAcn, and mortality risk

We further studied the relationship between mtDNAcn and personality facets known to be associated with mortality risk. Appendix 1—figure 4 compares the mtDNAcn among four distinct personality types (see ‘Methods’ for details) based on the vulnerability facet of Neuroticism and self-discipline facet of Conscientiousness, which were the two individual facets most strongly associated with mortality (Chapman et al., 2020). Comparing the mtDNAcn between the four personality types in the BLSA cohort, we observed that the personality type with the perceived highest mortality risk (high vulnerability low self-discipline [HVLD]) had the lowest mtDNAcn among the four personality types. The HVLD personality type had a mean mtDNAcn value 0.23 standard deviations lower than the type with a perceived lowest mortality risk ( low vulnerability high self-discipline [LVHD]) (p-value<0.0065), indirectly supporting an association between blood mtDNAcn and mortality risk. A more detailed description and interpretation of the results can be found in Appendix 1.

mtDNAcn mediates the association between personality and mortality risk

The above preliminary observation that mtDNAcn was associated with mortality-related personality traits led us to a more detailed investigation of the relationship between personality, blood mtDNAcn, and mortality. In particular, we further extended our analysis to include four facets (vulnerability, self-discipline, activity, and competence) by creating a PMI (see ‘Methods’ for details) and performed a mediation analysis.

First, we performed a Cox proportional hazards analysis for the four NEO-PI-R facets (Appendix 1—table 3). The analysis showed that after adjusting for age and sex, all four facets are significantly associated with increased risk of mortality (vulnerability) or reduced risk of mortality (activity, self-discipline, and competence). For the PMI analysis, we observed an increasing mtDNAcn when moving from index 0 (worst PMI) to index 4 (excellent PMI) in both study cohorts (Figure 3). Analyzing the PMI as a continuous variable showed that higher trait values were associated with higher mtDNAcn in both cohorts (Table 2). We confirmed the association between PMI and mortality by finding that the participants had significantly longer survival time with increasing PMI from 0 to 4 (Figure 4).

Figure 3. Comparison of mitochondrial DNA copy number (mtDNAcn) values among the five personality-mortality indices created from the vulnerability facet of Neuroticism, activity facet of Extraversion, and self-discipline and competence facets of Conscientiousness.

Figure 3.

Panel (A) is Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging (BLSA) cohort, and panel (B) is SardiNIA cohort. The personality-mortality index with the lowest mortality risk is 4, and the index with the highest mortality risk is index 0. p-Values of linear regression of mtDNAcn and the personality-mortality index are shown on the plots.

Figure 4. The effect of personality-mortality index (PMI) on mortality is significantly mediated via mitochondrial DNA copy number (mtDNAcn).

Figure 4.

Panel (A) is a Kaplan–Meier plot of time to death of Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging (BLSA) participants in the five PMIs. Panel (B) shows the regression coefficients between PMI (predictor), mortality (outcome), and mtDNAcn (mediator) for the BLSA study. The effects of age and sex are adjusted for in the models. The indirect effect is (4.89) × (–0.02) = –0.097. We tested for the significance of the indirect effect using 5000 bootstrap samples and it was statistically significant (p-value=0.002). See Table 3 for more details.

Given the significant associations of mtDNAcn and specific personality traits with mortality, we then tested the hypothesis that the association between PMI and mortality may be mediated through mtDNAcn. We used structural equation modeling to test the indirect effect of PMI on mortality via mtDNAcn. The results are shown in Table 3 and Figure 4. We observed that for BLSA, PMI was a significant predictor of mortality. The indirect effect estimate showed that the effect of PMI on mortality was fully mediated via mtDNAcn. We tested the significance of the indirect effect using 5000 bootstrap samples, which yielded a significant p-value=0.002 (estimate = –0.097). Similar results were obtained for SardiNIA, although they were not statistically significant, likely due to limited power because the Sardinian cohort is younger, and the mortality data are limited compared to the BLSA cohort. Also, the Sardinian analysis had reduced statistical power compared to the BLSA analysis because the personality measures are averaged across multiple visits for BLSA, which decreases the standard deviation (increasing the reliability) of effect estimates.

Table 3. Testing the direct and indirect effects of personality-mortality index and mitochondrial DNA copy number (mtDNAcn) on mortality.

BLSA (n = 721) SardiNIA (n = 395)
Total effect Estimate (p-value) Estimate (p-value)
Personality-mortality index –0.149 (0.001) –0.022 (0.808)
Full effects
Personality-mortality index –0.052 (0.260) –0.019 (0.895)
mtDNAcn –0.020 (0.000) –0.001 (0.966)
Indirect effect Estimate [nonparametric bootstrap 95% CI] Estimate [nonparametric bootstrap 95% CI]
mtDNAcn –0.097 [–0.143; –0.03] –0.004 [–0.169; 0.189]

BLSA, Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging.

Discussion

An extensive literature has reported the association of blood mtDNAcn with various psychological outcomes such as major depression (Cai et al., 2015; Kim et al., 2011), autism (Giulivi et al., 2010), bipolar disorder (Chang et al., 2014), and stress (Bersani et al., 2016; Boeck et al., 2016; Tyrka et al., 2016). Our study adds new information to this literature by reporting an association between mtDNAcn and personality traits. Our results show that higher mtDNAcn is significantly associated with a lower level of Neuroticism and higher levels of Extraversion, Openness, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness in study participants. We demonstrate the robustness of these associations by showing that the direction and size of the effects are robust to adjustment for potential confounders such as age, sex, platelet count, and WBC parameters and are replicated across two cohorts. Consistent with Neuroticism, participants with more depressive symptoms had lower mtDNAcn than those without depressive symptoms. Finally, we report an effect of personality on mortality and demonstrate that this effect is partially mediated via mtDNAcn.

mtDNAcn is a biomarker of energetic health and cellular aging (Picard and McEwen, 2018a). The mechanism that controls the levels of mtDNAcn in tissues, blood, and plasma is not fully understood, and it is unclear whether the change of blood mtDNAcn reflects mitochondrial biogenesis, immune health, and/or hematopoiesis in the bone marrow (Picard, 2021). Increased mtDNAcn have been shown to have a different and potentially opposite indication depending on the underlying health condition (Moore et al., 2018). In population-based cohorts such as those involved in this study, increased mtDNAcn is generally positively correlated with mitochondria mass and thus is generally associated with higher oxidative capacity. Having a large pool of well-functioning mitochondria can positively influence health and generate signals that contribute to stress adaptation (Picard and McEwen, 2018a). Consistent with this interpretation, we found that the mtDNAcn levels were higher in younger participants, and our primary findings indicated that individuals with higher mtDNAcn levels had lower Neuroticism and were at lower risk of depression and lower risk of mortality. In adverse health conditions, however, it should be noted that increased mtDNAcn may not correlate with function (Moore et al., 2018; Wei et al., 2001). Rather, it may indicate a compensatory upregulation of mtDNAcn (Picard, 2021), a phenomenon where mtDNAcn is increased to compensate for low-energy cellular environment due to poor mitochondrial energetics (Picard, 2021; Giordano et al., 2014).

Personality traits influence behaviors, emotional states, and how individuals deal with stress. For example, personality traits are associated with how individuals assess stressful life circumstances (Ebstrup et al., 2011) and what coping responses to the stressors (i.e., neuroendocrine responses, immune functioning and inflammation, cardiovascular responses) are elicited (Schneiderman et al., 2005). Given these associations with stress and health-related behaviors, it is not surprising that personality traits are also related to better health, function, and longevity (Hayward et al., 2013; Caselli et al., 2016; Smith and MacKenzie, 2006; Terracciano and Sutin, 2019; Bogg and Roberts, 2004; Sutin et al., 2019; Canada et al., 2021). We observed a positive association between mtDNAcn and personality traits that are also related to health-relevant habits and conditions, such as nonsmoking (Malouff et al., 2006), healthy diet and obesity (Jokela et al., 2013; Mõttus et al., 2013), exercise and physical function (Kekäläinen et al., 2020), and preventive medical screenings (Aschwanden et al., 2019). Thus, indirect pathways may exist to link personality and health-related behaviors to changes in mitochondrial biology. Our finding of an association between mtDNAcn and personality expands this broader literature on personality traits and health.

Reduced mtDNAcn may reflect reduced mitochondrial mass or loss of mtDNA due to mitochondrial dysfunction (Picard, 2021). Dysfunctional mitochondria that are not appropriately removed by mitophagy produce excess ROS, which causes oxidative stress. As a result of damage caused by oxidative stress, mitochondria release mitochondrial antigenic molecules called damage-associated molecular patterns (DAMPs) into the cytoplasm (Picard and McEwen, 2018a; Zampino et al., 2020). These released DAMPs contain mtDNA, which are unmethylated and thus act as antigenic fragments and elicit an innate immune response through toll-like receptors (Zhang et al., 2010). This triggers an inflammatory response through cytokine release (Picard and McEwen, 2018a; Zampino et al., 2020). Prolonged inflammation, when unresolved, can lead to damage to cells and tissues and has been associated with a number of chronic diseases, including cardiovascular diseases (Lopez-Candales et al., 2017), depression (Maydych, 2019), and neurodegeneration (Missiroli et al., 2020). Our study shows that low mtDNAcn is associated with higher levels of Neuroticism and facets of Neuroticism, including depression. Of note, data from the SardiNIA sample indicates that Neuroticism is associated with higher interleukin-6 (IL-6) and C-reactive protein (CRP) (Sutin et al., 2010). We observed a similar relationship between mtDNAcn and CES-D (depressive symptoms), and the effect becomes even more pronounced (almost doubling in effect size) when considering significant depressive symptoms (setting CES-D binary cutoff at 20 instead of at 16). Kim et al., 2011 report a similar negative association between mtDNAcn and depression. However, other studies like Cai et al., 2015 and Tyrka et al., 2016 report a positive association of mtDNAcn with major depression, and several studies have found null associations (de Sousa et al., 2014; He et al., 2014; Lindqvist et al., 2018; Verhoeven et al., 2018; Vyas et al., 2020). One explanation for these inconsistencies could be the heterogeneity of the depression phenotyping: the depression facet of the Neuroticism domain is a trait measure, and the CES-D measures symptoms, but neither is a diagnostic instrument for major depression. It should also be noted that although some past studies assayed for mtDNAcn from whole blood, they did not adjust for the effect of platelets, WBCs, and other leukocyte subgroups on mtDNAcn as we do in this study. mtDNAcn estimates differ among the various blood cell types (Rausser et al., 2021), and this significantly impacts whole-blood mtDNAcn estimates and should be adjusted for in association models (also see discussion below).

mtDNAcn and personality are both predictors of mortality (Chapman et al., 2020; Mengel-From et al., 2014; Ashar et al., 2015), thus making our finding of an association between the two even more intriguing. Understanding which variable affects the other in driving the effect on mortality is a significant step forward, but alternative models are similarly plausible. To test the hypothesis that mtDNAcn mediates the association between personality and mortality, we performed a causal mediation analysis. In this article, we provide the first evidence of mtDNAcn significantly mediating the association between personality (including three facets capturing positive psychosocial states [activity, self-discipline, and competence] and an emotional stressor [vulnerability]) and mortality. Advances in mitochondrial research have presented the mitochondrion as a target of stress (Picard and McEwen, 2018b), and more recently, as a potential biological mediator of the stress-disease cascade (Picard and McEwen, 2018a). Picard and McEwen, 2018a provide a conceptual model for mitochondrial stress pathophysiology, outlining how mitochondria interact with psychosocial factors and stressors to influence health and lifespan outcomes. The underlying mechanism for this mediation is still an area of active research and is not fully understood, but the significance of our results remains clear: mtDNAcn is one mechanism in the pathway that connects personality and mortality.

This study also shows that mtDNAcn was negatively associated with age and male biological sex, which is consistent with previous reports (Ding et al., 2015). Also, higher mtDNAcn was significantly associated with higher levels of platelet count and percentages of lymphocytes, eosinophils, monocytes, and basophils. However, higher mtDNAcn was associated with lower WBC count and lower neutrophil percentage. Neutrophils have relatively lower mtDNAcn compared to other leukocytes (Rausser et al., 2021), and they form the largest proportion of WBC, which may explain the negative association of WBC count with mtDNAcn. A similar negative association between mtDNAcn and WBC count was observed by Liu et al., 2020. This study and other recent publications Hurtado-Roca et al., 2016; Knez et al., 2016; Shim et al., 2020 have highlighted the importance of adjusting for WBC parameters and platelet count when testing the association between traits and mtDNAcn measured in whole blood.

The primary strength of this study is the use of two independent study cohorts, one in the United States and the other in Europe: the top associations not only had significant p-values in both studies but also had very similar estimated effect sizes. Furthermore, in both cohorts, the associations of the personality traits and mtDNAcn ranked high when put in a broader context of mtDNAcn associations with other traits. The consistency of the results with the previous literature also clearly demonstrates the reproducibility of this study. The way mtDNAcn is estimated using whole-genome sequence information from the two study cohorts is also seen as the gold standard going forward in this line of research (Filograna et al., 2021), with major mitochondria consortia such as TopMed (Liu et al., 2020) employing the same approach. A limitation of this study was the one-time assessment of mtDNAcn, which precluded a longitudinal analysis of changes in mtDNAcn. Personality was assessed only once in the SardiNIA cohort, but personality is relatively stable in adults over long periods (Costa and McCrae, 1988; Terracciano et al., 2006); therefore, not much information may be lost in a cross-sectional analysis of personality. Additionally, we note that the formula for mtDNA copy number calculates a weighted average (weights determined by the proportion of leukocyte populations) of mtDNA copy number per cell across all leukocytes; each cell has two copies of nuclear DNA that provide the reference for the calculation. However, this does not take into account the platelet content of the buffy coat. Because each platelet has about 1.6 copies of mtDNA (Hurtado-Roca et al., 2016; Urata et al., 2008) but does not contain nuclear DNA, the formula somewhat overestimates mtDNA copy number (Rausser et al., 2021). The platelet content of buffy coat may also vary, representing a limitation of this and other studies of mtDNA in blood DNA material (Picard, 2021). Because of this limitation, papers published on mtDNA copy number typically use both platelets count and WBC counts as covariates in any models testing the association between mtDNA copy number and quantitative traits. We also used this conventional approach in our analyses. Another drawback of this study is the difference in the distribution of age and mtDNAcn across the two cohorts. The differences in the average mtDNAcn between the two cohorts can be explained by the gap in the time at which sequencing was done; almost a decade passed between the time the two cohorts were sequenced. Despite these limitations, the associations between mtDNAcn and personality traits are clear and consistent across the two study cohorts.

In this study, we showed a significant association between mtDNAcn and neuroticism traits that were replicated in two independent cohorts. Other larger cohorts (e.g., UK Biobank) have also collected certain personality information (e.g., questions about neuroticism), as well as whole-genome sequencing data (which makes estimating mtDNAcn feasible). Therefore, trying to replicate our findings in such larger cohorts will be a natural extension of this study.

In conclusion, we provide the first evidence of an association between mtDNAcn and personality. These results may provide further evidence of the link between mtDNAcn and psychosocial stress and suggest that blood mtDNAcn may correlate with changes in mitochondrial biology under stress. These initial findings will require further exploration to reveal the potential causal pathways between personality and mtDNAcn. We also provide novel insights into the effect of personality on mortality by showing that this effect is mediated through mtDNAcn.

Acknowledgements

Support for this work was provided by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institute on Aging (Z01-AG000693, Z01-AG000970, and Z01-AG000949) and the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke of the National Institutes of Health. AT was also supported by the National Institute on Aging of the National Institutes of Health Grant R01AG068093.

Appendix 1

Supplementary information text

Personality, mtDNAcn, and mortality risk

We studied the relationship between mtDNAcn and personality facets known to be associated with mortality risk. Appendix 1—figure 8 compares the mtDNAcn among four distinct personality types (see ‘Methods’ for details) created from the vulnerability facet of Neuroticism and self-discipline facet of Conscientiousness, which were the two individual facets most strongly associated with mortality. When comparing the mtDNAcn between the four personality types in the BLSA cohort, we observed that the personality type with the perceived highest mortality risk (HVLD) had the lowest mtDNAcn among the four personality types. The type with a perceived lowest mortality risk (LVHD) had a mean mtDNAcn value that is 0.23 standard deviations higher than the highest mortality type (HVLD) (p-value<0.0065). As a comparison of effect size relative to the slope of mtDNAcn and age after adjusting for all covariates, this difference in mtDNAcn between personality types corresponds to the difference in mtDNAcn observed across 0.6 years (about 7 months) of life. A similar mtDNAcn difference was observed in the SardiNIA cohort: the mean mtDNAcn difference between the two personality types was 0.21 standard deviations. This difference corresponds to the difference in mtDNAcn observed across 0.3 years (a little over 3 months) of life.

PMI does not mediate the association between mtDNAcn and mortality risk

We used causal mediation analysis to test the indirect effect of mtDNAcn on mortality via PMI as a mediator. The indirect effect estimate showed that the effect of mtDNAcn on mortality is not mediated through PMI. We tested for the significance of the indirect effect using 5000 bootstrap samples, and it was not statistically significant (p-values=0.355 and 0.963, respectively, for BLSA and SardiNIA). See Appendix 1—table 4 for more details.

Menopause status does not affect the association between mtDNAcn and personality

Given the mean age of 57 years in the SardiNIA cohort, we investigated menopausal status as a potential confounder in the association between mtDNAcn and personality. We found that there was no significant difference between the mtDNAcn of premenopausal and menopausal women (p-value=0.56) (Appendix 1—figure 9). The results show that menopause had no effect on mtDNAcn in the SardiNIA cohort. Also, we ran two association models between mtDNAcn and NEO--PI-R traits in women, with and without adjustment for the effect of menopause status (both models adjusted for the effect of age, sequence coverage, WBC, platelet count, and percentages of major types of leukocytes). The results are shown in Appendix 1—table 5. The results show that menopause status does not affect the association between mtDNAcn and personality: the effect sizes and p-values were almost the same in both models for all the traits.

mtDNAcn is not influenced by mitochondrial haplogroups

We investigated the effect of the major mitochondrial haplogroups present in our dataset. There were 17 haplogroups in total, and we excluded 7 haplogroups that were extremely rare (present in less than five study participants). The most common haplogroup was H (frequency of about 42%). Appendix 1—figure 10 shows the relative mtDNAcn in each haplogroup. mtDNAcn has a similar distribution among the different haplogroups, although slight variations in mtDNAcn were observed among them. We further investigated whether these mtDNAcn variations among the haplogroups were statistically significant by testing for the association of each haplogroup versus all other haplogroups with mtDNAcn. The results are shown in Appendix 1—table 6. Among the 10 haplogroups present in more than five study participants, none was significantly associated with mtDNAcn. This finding suggests that none of the haplogroups present in our study participants will confound our results.

Appendix 1—figure 1. Random-effect meta-analysis of Neuroticism domain of Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R).

Appendix 1—figure 1.

Appendix 1—figure 2. Comparison of effect sizes of Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) traits between the two cohorts.

Appendix 1—figure 2.

Regression model adjusted for the effects of age, sex, sequence coverage, and white blood cell parameters. Color coding is green for positive personality types, red for negative personality types, and blue for the composite scores for the big five NEO-PI-R domains.

Appendix 1—figure 3. Comparison of effect sizes of Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) traits between models with white blood cell (WBC) parameters adjustments and with no adjustments.

Appendix 1—figure 3.

Panel (A) is Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging (BLSA) cohort, and panel (B) is SardiNIA cohort. Color coding is green for positive personality types, red for negative personality types, and blue for the composite scores for the big five NEO-PI-R domains.

Appendix 1—figure 4. Comparison of mitochondrial DNA copy number (mtDNAcn) values among the four distinct personality types created from the vulnerability facet of Neuroticism and self-discipline facet of Conscientiousness.

Appendix 1—figure 4.

Panel (A) is Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging (BLSA) cohort, and panel (B) is SardiNIA cohort. The personality type with lowest mortality risk (low vulnerability high self-discipline [LVHD]) has a significantly higher mean mtDNAcn value than the highest mortality risk type (high vulnerability low self-discipline [HVLD]).

Appendix 1—figure 5. Stability of Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) traits across multiple visits.

Appendix 1—figure 5.

(A) Test–retest stability of the NEO-PI-R traits. Red dash line is Pearson’s correlation of 0.8. All the traits have a high test–retest reliability. (B) The stability of two randomly selected personality traits (Deliberation and Activity) across multiple measurement visits. Plot color represents starting trait values; blue shows individual’s lower initial trait values, and red shows higher initial trait values. The plot shows that subsequent trait measurement tends to stabilize around the initial measurement values.

Appendix 1—figure 6. Correlation between Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) traits.

Appendix 1—figure 6.

The color intensity and size of circles are indications of the size and significance of the correlation.

Appendix 1—figure 7. Histograms of autosomal and mitochondrial DNA (mtDNA) sequencing coverages for (A) Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging (BLSA) and (B) SardiNIA.

Appendix 1—figure 7.

Appendix 1—figure 8. Four distinct personality types are created from the vulnerability facet of Neuroticism and the self-discipline facet of Conscientiousness.

Appendix 1—figure 8.

Appendix 1—figure 9. Mitochondrial DNA copy number (mtDNAcn) comparison between menopausal and premenopausal women in SardiNIA.

Appendix 1—figure 9.

Appendix 1—figure 10. Mitochondrial DNA copy number (mtDNAcn) comparison between the major mitochondrial haplogroups present in more than five study participants.

Appendix 1—figure 10.

Appendix 1—table 1. Association of age, sex, and white blood cell (WBC) parameters with mitochondrial DNA copy number (mtDNAcn).
β (p-value) of model adjusted
No adjustment Age, sex, coverage, and WBC parameters
Trait BLSA SardiNIA BLSA SardiNIA
Age –0.025 (2.85e-13) –0.0093 (0.003) –0.014 (2.52e-5) –0.0065 (0.028)
Sex –0.61 (8.14e-17) –0.25 (0.004) –0.36 (1.21e-6) –0.13 (0.11)
Platelet count 0.004 (1.31e-7) 0.0007 (0.30) 0.003 (3.71e-6) 0.001 (0.063)
WBC count –0.13 (2.35e-10) –0.14 (1.37e-8) –0.097 (1.48e-6) –0.08 (0.002)
Lymphocytes percent 0.04 (5.5e-25) 0.042 (5.19e-16) –0.033 (0.23) 0.073 (0.55)
Neutrophils percent –0.041 (1.47e-28) –0.041 (6.19e-18) –0.063 (0.021) 0.036 (0.77)
Eosinophils percent 0.037 (0.031) 0.043 (0.06) –0.045 (0.16) 0.076 (0.55)
Monocytes percent 0.022 (0.082) 0.062 (0.002) –0.014 (0.57) 0.082 (0.52)
Basophils percent 0.44 (8.54e-5) 0.11 (0.38) 0.044 (0.69) –0.076 (0.55)

BLSA, Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging.

Appendix 1—table 2. Association of Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) domains with mitochondrial DNA copy number (mtDNAcn).
NEO-PI-R domains BLSA β (p-value) SardiNIA β (p-value) Meta-analysis
Pooled β Raw p-value FDR adj. p* I2 % [p of Q]
Neuroticism –0.106 (0.001) –0.116 (0.01) –0.11 0.00006 0.0015 0% [0.87]
Anxiety –0.082 (0.01) –0.086 (0.08) –0.08 0.002 0.010 0% [0.94]
Angry hostility –0.078 (0.02) –0.090 (0.054) –0.08 0.003 0.010 0% [0.84]
Depression –0.122 (0.0002) –0.070 (0.1) –0.11 0.00008 0.0015 0% [0.37]
Self-consciousness –0.060 (0.08) –0.116 (0.01) –0.08 0.004 0.013 0% [0.33]
Impulsiveness –0.067 (0.046) –0.023 (0.6) –0.05 0.06 0.099 0% [0.45]
Vulnerability –0.080 (0.02) –0.097 (0.04) –0.09 0.002 0.0087 0% [0.77]
Extraversion 0.083 (0.01) 0.117 (0.01) 0.09 0.0005 0.0036 0% [0.56]
Warmth 0.064 (0.06) 0.162 (0.0004) 0.11 0.03 0.051 66% [0.09]
Gregariousness 0.040 (0.2) 0.047 (0.3) 0.04 0.1 0.16 0% [0.90]
Assertiveness 0.066 (0.048) –0.007 (0.9) 0.04 0.3 0.36 38% [0.21]
Activity 0.106 (0.002) 0.037 (0.4) 0.08 0.02 0.040 30% [0.23]
Excitement seeking 0.011 (0.7) 0.067 (0.2) 0.03 0.3 0.36 0% [0.35]
Positive emotions 0.087 (0.01) 0.122 (0.01) 0.10 0.0004 0.0036 0% [0.54]
Openness 0.066 (0.06) 0.076 (0.1) 0.07 0.01 0.032 0% [0.86]
Fantasy –0.006 (0.9) 0.093 (0.051) 0.04 0.4 0.48 65% [0.09]
Aesthetics 0.015 (0.7) 0.086 (0.06) 0.04 0.2 0.27 34% [0.22]
Feelings 0.045 (0.2) –0.015 (0.8) 0.02 0.4 0.47 2% [0.31]
Actions 0.063 (0.07) 0.039 (0.4) 0.05 0.051 0.094 0% [0.68]
Ideas 0.079 (0.02) 0.070 (0.1) 0.08 0.005 0.014 0% [0.88]
Values 0.078 (0.02) –0.014 (0.8) 0.04 0.4 0.46 59% [0.12]
Agreeableness 0.058 (0.1) 0.141 (0.003) 0.09 0.02 0.047 49% [0.16]
Trust 0.085 (0.014) 0.065 (0.2) 0.08 0.005 0.014 0% [0.73]
Straightforwardness 0.059 (0.08) 0.095 (0.04) 0.07 0.009 0.024 0% [0.54]
Altruism 0.077 (0.02) 0.133 (0.005) 0.10 0.0005 0.0036 0% [0.34]
Compliance 0.005 (0.9) 0.039 (0.4) 0.02 0.5 0.57 0% [0.57]
Modesty –0.018 (0.6) 0.080 (0.09) 0.03 0.6 0.62 65% [0.09]
Tendermindedness 0.030 (0.4) 0.089 (0.053) 0.05 0.07 0.12 7% [0.30]
Conscientiousness 0.084 (0.009) 0.040 (0.4) 0.07 0.008 0.022 0% [0.43]
Competence 0.115 (0.0004) 0.024 (0.6) 0.08 0.09 0.14 61% [0.11]
Order –0.003 (0.9) 0.036 (0.4) 0.01 0.7 0.69 0% [0.49]
Dutifulness 0.097 (0.003) 0.066 (0.2) 0.09 0.001 0.0064 0% [0.57]
Achievement striving 0.093 (0.005) 0.018 (0.7) 0.06 0.09 0.14 42% [0.19]
Self-discipline 0.056 (0.08) 0.016 (0.7) 0.04 0.1 0.14 0% [0.48]
Deliberation 0.069 (0.03) 0.009 (0.8) 0.05 0.09 0.14 11% [0.29]
Personality and mortality BLSA Md (p-value) SardiNIA Md (p-value) Pooled Md Raw p-value FDR adj. p I2 % [p of Q]
HVLD vs. LVHD 0.234 (0.006) 0.209 (0.09) 0.226 0.0013 - 0% [0.87]

BLSA, Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging; HVLD, high vulnerability low self-discipline; LVHD, low vulnerability high self-discipline; FDR, false discovery rate.

*

Entries with FDR-corrected p-values≤0.01 are labeled as bold.

Appendix 1—table 3. Cox proportional hazards analysis of four Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) traits used to compute personality-mortality index (PMI).

Models adjusted for the effect of age and sex.

Coefficient Hazard ratio p-Value
Vulnerability 0.037 1.038 0.0006
Activity –0.036 0.946 0.0004
Self-discipline –0.028 0.972 0.002
Competence –0.041 0.959 2.28e-5
Appendix 1—table 4. Testing the direct and indirect effects of mitochondrial DNA copy number (mtDNAcn) and personality-mortality index (PMI) on mortality.
BLSA (n = 721) SardiNIA (n = 395)
Total effect Estimate (p-value) Estimate (p-value)
 mtDNAcn –0.010 (0.001) 0.003 (0.574)
Full effects
 mtDNAcn –0.010 (0.000) 0.003 (0.673)
 PMI –0.118 (0.340) –0.028 (0.959)
Indirect effect Estimate (nonparametric bootstrap p-value) Estimate (nonparametric bootstrap p-value)
 PMI 0.000 (0.355) 0.000 (0.963)

BLSA, Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging.

Appendix 1—table 5. Association of Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) domains with mitochondrial DNA copy number (mtDNAcn) in SardiNIA women, with and without adjustment for menopause status.

Both models adjusted for the effect of age, sequence coverage, white blood cell (WBC), platelet count, and percentages of major leukocytes.

β (p-value) of model
NEO-PI-R domains Not adjusted for menopause status Adjusted for menopause status
Neuroticism –0.013 (0.074) –0.013 (0.077)
Anxiety –0.007 (0.316) –0.007 (0.323)
Angry hostility –0.010 (0.123) –0.010 (0.127)
Depression –0.008 (0.236) –0.008 (0.240)
Self-consciousness –0.012 (0.056) –0.012 (0.056)
Impulsiveness 0.002 (0.794) 0.002 (0.802)
Vulnerability –0.011 (0.059) –0.011 (0.063)
Extraversion 0.017 (0.022) 0.017 (0.021)
Warmth 0.021 (0.0004) 0.021 (0.0004)
Gregariousness 0.005 (0.483) 0.005 (0.476)
Assertiveness 0.002 (0.828) 0.001 (0.862)
Activity 0.001 (0.857) 0.001 (0.864)
Excitement seeking 0.010 (0.183) 0.010 (0.165)
Positive emotions 0.014 (0.020) 0.015 (0.017)
Openness 0.014 (0.035) 0.014 (0.036)
Fantasy 0.017 (0.016) 0.017 (0.016)
Aesthetics 0.011 (0.098) 0.011 (0.102)
Feelings 0.007 (0.319) 0.007 (0.312)
Actions 0.008 (0.195) 0.008 (0.192)
Ideas 0.009 (0.189) 0.009 (0.186)
Values 0.001 (0.887) 0.001 (0.910)
Agreeableness 0.017 (0.010) 0.017 (0.011)
Trust 0.006 (0.313) 0.006 (0.321)
Straightforwardness 0.012 (0.050) 0.012 (0.055)
Altruism 0.012 (0.042) 0.012 (0.041)
Compliance 0.008 (0.175) 0.008 (0.179)
Modesty 0.005 (0.471) 0.005 (0.499)
Tendermindedness 0.013 (0.025) 0.013 (0.024)
Conscientiousness 0.006 (0.409) 0.005 (0.428)
Competence 0.001 (0.846) 0.001 (0.857)
Order 0.003 (0.606) 0.003 (0.637)
Dutifulness 0.006 (0.325) 0.006 (0.346)
Achievement striving 0.005 (0.471) 0.005 (0.442)
Self-discipline 0.001 (0.854) 0.001 (0.908)
Deliberation 0.005 (0.414) 0.005 (0.417)
Appendix 1—table 6. Effect of mitochondrial haplogroups on mitochondrial DNA copy number (mtDNAcn).
Haplogroups Frequency (%) Beta SE p-Value
N 1.94 –0.45 0.27 0.095
H 41.83 –0.12 0.08 0.103
T 9.42 0.20 0.13 0.122
X 1.94 –0.31 0.27 0.258
K 9.70 0.12 0.13 0.361
W 2.49 0.19 0.24 0.418
V 3.88 –0.13 0.19 0.500
U 12.60 0.07 0.11 0.504
I 2.77 –0.09 0.23 0.703
J 11.50 0.03 0.12 0.795

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Luigi Ferrucci, Email: ferruccilu@grc.nia.nih.gov.

Jun Ding, Email: jun.ding@nih.gov.

Jonathan Flint, University of California, Los Angeles, United States.

Ma-Li Wong, State University of New York Upstate Medical University, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Institute on Aging Intramural Research Program to Bryan J Traynor, Bryan J Traynor.

  • National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Intramural Research Program to Sonja W Scholz.

  • National Institute on Aging Grant R01AG068093 to Antonio Terracciano.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

No competing interests declared.

participates as a Scientific advisory board member for Lewy body dementia association. The author has no other competing interests to declare.

has indicated several grants, patents, positions and interests in his individual ICMJE form, but he has done it in the interest of transparency and none of his disclosures is related to the content of the current manuscript.

is a consultant for Elixirgen Therapeutics, Inc The author has no other competing interests to declare.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft.

Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft.

Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft.

Software, Writing – review and editing.

Software, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Writing – review and editing.

Formal analysis, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Writing – original draft.

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft.

Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Supervision, Validation, Methodology, Writing – original draft.

Ethics

All participants of the two cohort studies gave written informed consent, with protocols approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Intramural Research Program of the National Institutes of Health (protocol numbers 03-AG-0325 and 04-AG-N317 for BLSA and SardiNIA, respectively).

Additional files

Transparent reporting form
Reporting standard 1. A checklist of items that should be included in reports of observational studies.
Source code 1. R code for the analysis on mtDNAcn and personality.
elife-77806-code1.zip (88.2KB, zip)

Data availability

Researchers interested in using BLSA data (e.g., sequencing or phenotypic data) need to submit an analysis plan to a review committee organized by the National Institute on Aging (A detailed guideline for the submission of an analysis plan can be found at https://www.blsa.nih.gov/how-apply). Researchers will get the access once the proposed analysis plans are reviewed and approved. Aggregated genetic and phenotypic data from the SardiNIA study are deposited in the European Genome-phenome Archive (EGA) (https://ega-archive.org/studies/phs000338). In accord with European Union regulations, disaggregated data, including sequence/genotyping data of individuals, cannot be made public, but investigators can request specific analyses by sending a one-page proposal description to Dr. F. Cucca (fcucca@uniss.it), the P.I. of the study. After screening by an internal committee, the requested analyses are customarily done by staff of IRGB and the results provided to the requestor. The R code for the analyses performed in this paper is provided as a Supplementary file (source code file: R code for the analysis on mtDNAcn and personality).

References

  1. Al-Kafaji G, Aljadaan A, Kamal A, Bakhiet M. Peripheral blood mitochondrial DNA copy number as a novel potential biomarker for diabetic nephropathy in type 2 diabetes patients. Experimental and Therapeutic Medicine. 2018;16:1483–1492. doi: 10.3892/etm.2018.6319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aschwanden D, Gerend MA, Luchetti M, Stephan Y, Sutin AR, Terracciano A. Personality traits and preventive cancer screenings in the health retirement study. Preventive Medicine. 2019;126:105763. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2019.105763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Ashar FN, Moes A, Moore AZ, Grove ML, Chaves PHM, Coresh J, Newman AB, Matteini AM, Bandeen-Roche K, Boerwinkle E, Walston JD, Arking DE. Association of mitochondrial DNA levels with frailty and all-cause mortality. Journal of Molecular Medicine. 2015;93:177–186. doi: 10.1007/s00109-014-1233-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Attardi G, Schatz G. Biogenesis of mitochondria. Annual Review of Cell Biology. 1988;4:289–333. doi: 10.1146/annurev.cb.04.110188.001445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Beekman AT, Deeg DJ, Van Limbeek J, Braam AW, De Vries MZ, Van Tilburg W. Criterion validity of the center for epidemiologic studies depression scale (CES-D): results from a community-based sample of older subjects in the Netherlands. Psychological Medicine. 1997;27:231–235. doi: 10.1017/s0033291796003510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bersani FS, Morley C, Lindqvist D, Epel ES, Picard M, Yehuda R, Flory J, Bierer LM, Makotkine I, Abu-Amara D, Coy M, Reus VI, Lin J, Blackburn EH, Marmar C, Wolkowitz OM, Mellon SH. Mitochondrial DNA copy number is reduced in male combat veterans with PTSD. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry. 2016;64:10–17. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2015.06.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Boeck C, Koenig AM, Schury K, Geiger ML, Karabatsiakis A, Wilker S, Waller C, Gündel H, Fegert JM, Calzia E, Kolassa I-T. Inflammation in adult women with a history of child maltreatment: the involvement of mitochondrial alterations and oxidative stress. Mitochondrion. 2016;30:197–207. doi: 10.1016/j.mito.2016.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bogg T, Roberts BW. Conscientiousness and health-related behaviors: a meta-analysis of the leading behavioral contributors to mortality. Psychological Bulletin. 2004;130:887–919. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cai N, Chang S, Li Y, Li Q, Hu J, Liang J, Song L, Kretzschmar W, Gan X, Nicod J, Rivera M, Deng H, Du B, Li K, Sang W, Gao J, Gao S, Ha B, Ho H-Y, Hu C, Hu J, Hu Z, Huang G, Jiang G, Jiang T, Jin W, Li G, Li K, Li Y, Li Y, Li Y, Lin Y-T, Liu L, Liu T, Liu Y, Liu Y, Lu Y, Lv L, Meng H, Qian P, Sang H, Shen J, Shi J, Sun J, Tao M, Wang G, Wang G, Wang J, Wang L, Wang X, Wang X, Yang H, Yang L, Yin Y, Zhang J, Zhang K, Sun N, Zhang W, Zhang X, Zhang Z, Zhong H, Breen G, Wang J, Marchini J, Chen Y, Xu Q, Xu X, Mott R, Huang G-J, Kendler K, Flint J. Molecular signatures of major depression. Current Biology. 2015;25:1146–1156. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.03.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Canada B, Stephan Y, Fundenberger H, Sutin AR, Terracciano A. Cross-Sectional and prospective association between personality traits and IADL/ADL limitations. Psychology and Aging. 2021;36:309–321. doi: 10.1037/pag0000502. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Caselli RJ, Dueck AC, Locke DEC, Henslin BR, Johnson TA, Woodruff BK, Hoffman-Snyder C, Geda YE. Impact of personality on cognitive aging: a prospective cohort study. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2016;22:765–776. doi: 10.1017/S1355617716000527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chang CC, Jou SH, Lin TT, Liu CS. Mitochondrial DNA variation and increased oxidative damage in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 2014;68:551–557. doi: 10.1111/pcn.12163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Chapman BP, Elliot A, Sutin A, Terraciano A, Zelinski E, Schaie W, Willis S, Hofer S. Mortality risk associated with personality facets of the big five and interpersonal circumplex across three aging cohorts. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2020;82:64–73. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000756. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chia R, Sabir MS, Bandres-Ciga S, Saez-Atienzar S, Reynolds RH, Gustavsson E, Walton RL, Ahmed S, Viollet C, Ding J, Makarious MB, Diez-Fairen M, Portley MK, Shah Z, Abramzon Y, Hernandez DG, Blauwendraat C, Stone DJ, Eicher J, Parkkinen L, Ansorge O, Clark L, Honig LS, Marder K, Lemstra A, St George-Hyslop P, Londos E, Morgan K, Lashley T, Warner TT, Jaunmuktane Z, Galasko D, Santana I, Tienari PJ, Myllykangas L, Oinas M, Cairns NJ, Morris JC, Halliday GM, Van Deerlin VM, Trojanowski JQ, Grassano M, Calvo A, Mora G, Canosa A, Floris G, Bohannan RC, Brett F, Gan-Or Z, Geiger JT, Moore A, May P, Krüger R, Goldstein DS, Lopez G, Tayebi N, Sidransky E, American Genome Center. Norcliffe-Kaufmann L, Palma J-A, Kaufmann H, Shakkottai VG, Perkins M, Newell KL, Gasser T, Schulte C, Landi F, Salvi E, Cusi D, Masliah E, Kim RC, Caraway CA, Monuki ES, Brunetti M, Dawson TM, Rosenthal LS, Albert MS, Pletnikova O, Troncoso JC, Flanagan ME, Mao Q, Bigio EH, Rodríguez-Rodríguez E, Infante J, Lage C, González-Aramburu I, Sanchez-Juan P, Ghetti B, Keith J, Black SE, Masellis M, Rogaeva E, Duyckaerts C, Brice A, Lesage S, Xiromerisiou G, Barrett MJ, Tilley BS, Gentleman S, Logroscino G, Serrano GE, Beach TG, McKeith IG, Thomas AJ, Attems J, Morris CM, Palmer L, Love S, Troakes C, Al-Sarraj S, Hodges AK, Aarsland D, Klein G, Kaiser SM, Woltjer R, Pastor P, Bekris LM, Leverenz JB, Besser LM, Kuzma A, Renton AE, Goate A, Bennett DA, Scherzer CR, Morris HR, Ferrari R, Albani D, Pickering-Brown S, Faber K, Kukull WA, Morenas-Rodriguez E, Lleó A, Fortea J, Alcolea D, Clarimon J, Nalls MA, Ferrucci L, Resnick SM, Tanaka T, Foroud TM, Graff-Radford NR, Wszolek ZK, Ferman T, Boeve BF, Hardy JA, Topol EJ, Torkamani A, Singleton AB, Ryten M, Dickson DW, Chiò A, Ross OA, Gibbs JR, Dalgard CL, Traynor BJ, Scholz SW. Genome sequencing analysis identifies new loci associated with Lewy body dementia and provides insights into its genetic architecture. Nature Genetics. 2021;53:294–303. doi: 10.1038/s41588-021-00785-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Clay Montier LL, Deng JJ, Bai Y. Number matters: control of mammalian mitochondrial DNA copy number. Journal of Genetics and Genomics = Yi Chuan Xue Bao. 2009;36:125–131. doi: 10.1016/S1673-8527(08)60099-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Costa PT, McCrae RR. Personality in adulthood: a six-year longitudinal study of self-reports and spouse ratings on the neo personality inventory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988;54:853–863. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.5.853. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Costa PT, Terracciano A, Uda M, Vacca L, Mameli C, Pilia G, Zonderman AB, Lakatta E, Schlessinger D, McCrae RR. Personality traits in sardinia: testing founder population effects on trait means and variances. Behavior Genetics. 2007;37:376–387. doi: 10.1007/s10519-006-9103-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Costa P, McCrae R. The revised NEO personality inventory (NEO-PI-R) SAGE. 2008;2:179–198. [Google Scholar]
  19. Delitala AP, Terracciano A, Fiorillo E, Orrù V, Schlessinger D, Cucca F. Depressive symptoms, thyroid hormone and autoimmunity in a population-based cohort from Sardinia. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2016;191:82–87. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.11.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. de Sousa RT, Uno M, Zanetti MV, Shinjo SMO, Busatto GF, Gattaz WF, Marie SKN, Machado-Vieira R. Leukocyte mitochondrial DNA copy number in bipolar disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry. 2014;48:32–35. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2013.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Ding J, Sidore C, Butler TJ, Wing MK, Qian Y, Meirelles O, Busonero F, Tsoi LC, Maschio A, Angius A, Kang HM, Nagaraja R, Cucca F, Abecasis GR, Schlessinger D. Assessing mitochondrial DNA variation and copy number in lymphocytes of ~2,000 Sardinians using tailored sequencing analysis tools. PLOS Genetics. 2015;11:e1005306. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1005306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Duchen MR. Mitochondria in health and disease: perspectives on a new mitochondrial biology. Molecular Aspects of Medicine. 2004;25:365–451. doi: 10.1016/j.mam.2004.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Ebstrup JF, Eplov LF, Pisinger C, Jørgensen T. Association between the five factor personality traits and perceived stress: is the effect mediated by general self-efficacy? Anxiety, Stress, and Coping. 2011;24:407–419. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2010.540012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Filograna R, Mennuni M, Alsina D, Larsson NG. Mitochondrial DNA copy number in human disease: the more the better? FEBS Letters. 2021;595:976–1002. doi: 10.1002/1873-3468.14021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gianotti TF, Sookoian S, Dieuzeide G, García SI, Gemma C, González CD, Pirola CJ. A decreased mitochondrial DNA content is related to insulin resistance in adolescents. Obesity. 2008;16:1591–1595. doi: 10.1038/oby.2008.253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Giordano C, Iommarini L, Giordano L, Maresca A, Pisano A, Valentino ML, Caporali L, Liguori R, Deceglie S, Roberti M, Fanelli F, Fracasso F, Ross-Cisneros FN, D’Adamo P, Hudson G, Pyle A, Yu-Wai-Man P, Chinnery PF, Zeviani M, Salomao SR, Berezovsky A, Belfort R, Ventura DF, Moraes M, Moraes Filho M, Barboni P, Sadun F, De Negri A, Sadun AA, Tancredi A, Mancini M, d’Amati G, Loguercio Polosa P, Cantatore P, Carelli V. Efficient mitochondrial biogenesis drives incomplete penetrance in leber’s hereditary optic neuropathy. Brain. 2014;137:335–353. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Giulivi C, Zhang Y-F, Omanska-Klusek A, Ross-Inta C, Wong S, Hertz-Picciotto I, Tassone F, Pessah IN. Mitochondrial dysfunction in autism. JAMA. 2010;304:2389–2396. doi: 10.1001/jama.2010.1706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hägg S, Jylhävä J, Wang Y, Czene K, Grassmann F. Deciphering the genetic and epidemiological landscape of mitochondrial DNA abundance. Human Genetics. 2021;140:849–861. doi: 10.1007/s00439-020-02249-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hayward RD, Taylor WD, Smoski MJ, Steffens DC, Payne ME. Association of five-factor model personality domains and facets with presence, onset, and treatment outcomes of major depression in older adults. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2013;21:88–96. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2012.11.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. He Y, Tang J, Li Z, Li H, Liao Y, Tang Y, Tan L, Chen J, Xia K, Chen X. Leukocyte mitochondrial DNA copy number in blood is not associated with major depressive disorder in young adults. PLOS ONE. 2014;9:e96869. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0096869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hurtado-Roca Y, Ledesma M, Gonzalez-Lazaro M, Moreno-Loshuertos R, Fernandez-Silva P, Enriquez JA, Laclaustra M. Adjusting mtDNA quantification in whole blood for peripheral blood platelet and leukocyte counts. PLOS ONE. 2016;11:e0163770. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0163770. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Islam M, Mazumder M, Schwabe-Warf D, Stephan Y, Sutin AR, Terracciano A. Personality changes with dementia from the informant perspective: new data and meta-analysis. Journal of the American Medical Directors Association. 2019;20:131–137. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2018.11.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Jokela M, Hintsanen M, Hakulinen C, Batty GD, Nabi H, Singh-Manoux A, Kivimäki M. Association of personality with the development and persistence of obesity: a meta-analysis based on individual-participant data. Obesity Reviews. 2013;14:315–323. doi: 10.1111/obr.12007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Jokela M, Elovainio M, Nyberg ST, Tabák AG, Hintsa T, Batty GD, Kivimäki M. Personality and risk of diabetes in adults: pooled analysis of 5 cohort studies. Health Psychology. 2014;33:1618–1621. doi: 10.1037/hea0000003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kekäläinen T, Terracciano A, Sipilä S, Kokko K. Personality traits and physical functioning: a cross-sectional multimethod facet-level analysis. European Review of Aging and Physical Activity. 2020;17:20. doi: 10.1186/s11556-020-00251-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kim MY, Lee JW, Kang HC, Kim E, Lee DC. Leukocyte mitochondrial DNA (mtdna) content is associated with depression in old women. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics. 2011;53:e218–e221. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2010.11.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Knez J, Winckelmans E, Plusquin M, Thijs L, Cauwenberghs N, Gu Y, Staessen JA, Nawrot TS, Kuznetsova T. Correlates of peripheral blood mitochondrial DNA content in a general population. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2016;183:138–146. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwv175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kotov R, Gamez W, Schmidt F, Watson D. Linking “ big ” personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 2010;136:768–821. doi: 10.1037/a0020327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kuo P-L, Schrack JA, Shardell MD, Levine M, Moore AZ, An Y, Elango P, Karikkineth A, Tanaka T, de Cabo R, Zukley LM, AlGhatrif M, Chia CW, Simonsick EM, Egan JM, Resnick SM, Ferrucci L. A roadmap to build a phenotypic metric of ageing: insights from the Baltimore longitudinal study of aging. Journal of Internal Medicine. 2020;287:373–394. doi: 10.1111/joim.13024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Li H, Handsaker B, Wysoker A, Fennell T, Ruan J, Homer N, Marth G, Abecasis G, Durbin R, 1000 Genome Project Data Processing Subgroup The sequence alignment/map format and samtools. Bioinformatics. 2009;25:2078–2079. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btp352. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Lindqvist D, Wolkowitz OM, Picard M, Ohlsson L, Bersani FS, Fernström J, Westrin Å, Hough CM, Lin J, Reus VI, Epel ES, Mellon SH. Circulating cell-free mitochondrial DNA, but not leukocyte mitochondrial DNA copy number, is elevated in major depressive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2018;43:1557–1564. doi: 10.1038/s41386-017-0001-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Liu X, Longchamps RJ, Wiggins K, Raffield LM, Bielak LF, Zhao W, Pitsillides A, Blackwell T, Yao J, Guo X, Kurniansyah N, Thyagarajan B, Pankratz N, Rich SS, Taylor KD, Peyser PA, Heckbert SR, Seshadri S, Cupples LA, Boerwinkle E, Grove ML, Larson N, Smith JA, Vasan RS, Sofer T, Fitzpatrick AL, Fornage M, Ding J, Correa A, Abecasis G, Psaty BM, Wilson JG, Levy D, Rotter JI, Bis JC, Satizabal CL, Arking DE, Liu C. Association of Mitochondrial DNA Copy Number with Cardiometabolic Diseases in a Large Cross-Sectional Study of Multiple Ancestries. bioRxiv. 2020 doi: 10.1101/2020.04.20.20016337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  43. Lopez-Candales A, Hernández Burgos PM, Hernandez-Suarez DF, Harris D. Linking chronic inflammation with cardiovascular disease: from normal aging to the metabolic syndrome. Journal of Nature and Science. 2017;3:e341. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Luchetti M, Terracciano A, Stephan Y, Sutin AR. Personality and cognitive decline in older adults: data from a longitudinal sample and meta-analysis. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2016;71:591–601. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbu184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Malouff JM, Thorsteinsson EB, Schutte NS. The five-factor model of personality and smoking: a meta-analysis. Journal of Drug Education. 2006;36:47–58. doi: 10.2190/9EP8-17P8-EKG7-66AD. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Maydych V. The interplay between stress, inflammation, and emotional attention: relevance for depression. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2019;13:384. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00384. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. McCrae RR, Terracciano A. Personality profiles of cultures project, universal features of personality traits from the observer’s perspective: data from 50 cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2005;88:547–561. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.3.547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. McDermott MM, Peterson CA, Sufit R, Ferrucci L, Guralnik JM, Kibbe MR, Polonsky TS, Tian L, Criqui MH, Zhao L, Stein JH, Li L, Leeuwenburgh C. Peripheral artery disease, calf skeletal muscle mitochondrial DNA copy number, and functional performance. Vascular Medicine. 2018;23:340–348. doi: 10.1177/1358863X18765667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Mengel-From J, Thinggaard M, Dalgård C, Kyvik KO, Christensen K, Christiansen L. Mitochondrial DNA copy number in peripheral blood cells declines with age and is associated with general health among elderly. Human Genetics. 2014;133:1149–1159. doi: 10.1007/s00439-014-1458-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Milaneschi Y, Bandinelli S, Penninx BW, Vogelzangs N, Corsi AM, Lauretani F, Kisialiou A, Vazzana R, Terracciano A, Guralnik JM, Ferrucci L. Depressive symptoms and inflammation increase in a prospective study of older adults: a protective effect of a healthy (mediterranean-style) diet. Molecular Psychiatry. 2011;16:589–590. doi: 10.1038/mp.2010.113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Missiroli S, Genovese I, Perrone M, Vezzani B, Vitto VAM, Giorgi C. The role of mitochondria in inflammation: from cancer to neurodegenerative disorders. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2020;9:E740. doi: 10.3390/jcm9030740. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Moore AZ, Ding J, Tuke MA, Wood AR, Bandinelli S, Frayling TM, Ferrucci L. Influence of cell distribution and diabetes status on the association between mitochondrial DNA copy number and aging phenotypes in the inchianti study. Aging Cell. 2018;17:12683. doi: 10.1111/acel.12683. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Mõttus R, McNeill G, Jia X, Craig LCA, Starr JM, Deary IJ. The associations between personality, diet and body mass index in older people. Health Psychology. 2013;32:353–360. doi: 10.1037/a0025537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Penninx BW, Guralnik JM, Ferrucci L, Simonsick EM, Deeg DJ, Wallace RB. Depressive symptoms and physical decline in community-dwelling older persons. JAMA. 1998;279:1720–1726. doi: 10.1001/jama.279.21.1720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Picard M, McEwen BS. Psychological stress and mitochondria: a conceptual framework. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2018a;80:126–140. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000544. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Picard M, McEwen BS. Psychological stress and mitochondria: a systematic review. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2018b;80:141–153. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000545. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Picard M. Blood mitochondrial DNA copy number: what are we counting? Mitochondrion. 2021;60:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.mito.2021.06.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Pilia G, Chen W-M, Scuteri A, Orrú M, Albai G, Dei M, Lai S, Usala G, Lai M, Loi P, Mameli C, Vacca L, Deiana M, Olla N, Masala M, Cao A, Najjar SS, Terracciano A, Nedorezov T, Sharov A, Zonderman AB, Abecasis GR, Costa P, Lakatta E, Schlessinger D. Heritability of cardiovascular and personality traits in 6,148 Sardinians. PLOS Genetics. 2006;2:e132. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.0020132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Qian Y, Butler TJ, Opsahl-Ong K, Giroux NS, Sidore C, Nagaraja R, Cucca F, Ferrucci L, Abecasis GR, Schlessinger D, Ding J. FastMitoCalc: an ultra-fast program to estimate mitochondrial DNA copy number from whole-genome sequences. Bioinformatics. 2017;33:1399–1401. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btw835. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. SAGE. 1977;1:100306. doi: 10.1177/014662167700100306. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  61. Rausser S, Trumpff C, McGill MA, Junker A, Wang W, Ho S-H, Mitchell A, Karan KR, Monk C, Segerstrom SC, Reed RG, Picard M. Mitochondrial phenotypes in purified human immune cell subtypes and cell mixtures. eLife. 2021;10:e70899. doi: 10.7554/eLife.70899. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Schneiderman N, Ironson G, Siegel SD. Stress and health: psychological, behavioral, and biological determinants. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2005;1:607–628. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.144141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Shim HB, Arshad O, Gadawska I, Côté HCF, Hsieh AYY. Platelet mtDNA content and leukocyte count influence whole blood mtDNA content. Mitochondrion. 2020;52:108–114. doi: 10.1016/j.mito.2020.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Smith TW, MacKenzie J. Personality and risk of physical illness. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2006;2:435–467. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.2.022305.095257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Song J, Oh JY, Sung YA, Pak YK, Park KS, Lee HK. Peripheral blood mitochondrial DNA content is related to insulin sensitivity in offspring of type 2 diabetic patients. Diabetes Care. 2001;24:865–869. doi: 10.2337/diacare.24.5.865. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Stephan Y, Sutin AR, Luchetti M, Terracciano A. Allostatic load and personality: a 4-year longitudinal study. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2016;78:302–310. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Stone JL, Norris AH. Activities and attitudes of participants in the Baltimore longitudinal study. Journal of Gerontology. 1966;21:575–580. doi: 10.1093/geronj/21.4.575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Sutin AR, Terracciano A, Deiana B, Naitza S, Ferrucci L, Uda M, Schlessinger D, Costa PT., Jr High neuroticism and low conscientiousness are associated with interleukin-6. Psychological Medicine. 2010;40:1485–1493. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709992029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Sutin AR, Stephan Y, Terracciano A. Personality and metabolic dysfunction in young adulthood: a cross-sectional study. Journal of Health Psychology. 2019;24:495–501. doi: 10.1177/1359105316677294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Terracciano A. The Italian version of the neo PI-R: conceptual and empirical support for the use of targeted rotation. Personality and Individual Differences. 2003;35:1859–1872. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00035-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Terracciano A, Costa PT, McCrae RR. Personality plasticity after age 30. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin. 2006;32:999–1009. doi: 10.1177/0146167206288599. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Terracciano A, Schrack JA, Sutin AR, Chan W, Simonsick EM, Ferrucci L. Personality, metabolic rate and aerobic capacity. PLOS ONE. 2013;8:e54746. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0054746. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Terracciano A, Sutin AR. Personality and Alzheimer’s disease: an integrative review. Personality Disorders. 2019;10:4–12. doi: 10.1037/per0000268. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Turiano NA, Chapman BP, Gruenewald TL, Mroczek DK. Personality and the leading behavioral contributors of mortality. Health Psychology. 2015;34:51–60. doi: 10.1037/hea0000038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Tyrka AR, Parade SH, Price LH, Kao H-T, Porton B, Philip NS, Welch ES, Carpenter LL. Alterations of mitochondrial DNA copy number and telomere length with early adversity and psychopathology. Biological Psychiatry. 2016;79:78–86. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.12.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Urata M, Koga-Wada Y, Kayamori Y, Kang D. Platelet contamination causes large variation as well as overestimation of mitochondrial DNA content of peripheral blood mononuclear cells. Annals of Clinical Biochemistry. 2008;45:513–514. doi: 10.1258/acb.2008.008008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Verhoeven JE, Révész D, Picard M, Epel EE, Wolkowitz OM, Matthews KA, Penninx BWJH, Puterman E. Depression, telomeres and mitochondrial DNA: between- and within-person associations from a 10-year longitudinal study. Molecular Psychiatry. 2018;23:850–857. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Vyas CM, Ogata S, Reynolds CF, Mischoulon D, Chang G, Cook NR, Manson JE, Crous-Bou M, De Vivo I, Okereke OI. Lifestyle and behavioral factors and mitochondrial DNA copy number in a diverse cohort of mid-life and older adults. PLOS ONE. 2020;15:e0237235. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0237235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Wei YH, Lee CF, Lee HC, Ma YS, Wang CW, Lu CY, Pang CY. Increases of mitochondrial mass and mitochondrial genome in association with enhanced oxidative stress in human cells harboring 4,977 BP-deleted mitochondrial DNA. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2001;928:97–112. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05640.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Xing J, Chen M, Wood CG, Lin J, Spitz MR, Ma J, Amos CI, Shields PG, Benowitz NL, Gu J, de Andrade M, Swan GE, Wu X. Mitochondrial DNA content: its genetic heritability and association with renal cell carcinoma. Journal of the National Cancer Institute. 2008;100:1104–1112. doi: 10.1093/jnci/djn213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Xu FX, Zhou X, Shen F, Pang R, Liu SM. Decreased peripheral blood mitochondrial DNA content is related to hba1c, fasting plasma glucose level and age of onset in type 2 diabetes mellitus. Diabetic Medicine. 2012;29:e47–e54. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-5491.2011.03565.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Yang SY, Castellani CA, Longchamps RJ, Pillalamarri VK, O’Rourke B, Guallar E, Arking DE. Blood-Derived mitochondrial DNA copy number is associated with gene expression across multiple tissues and is predictive for incident neurodegenerative disease. Genome Research. 2021;31:349–358. doi: 10.1101/gr.269381.120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Zampino M, Brennan NA, Kuo P-L, Spencer RG, Fishbein KW, Simonsick EM, Ferrucci L. Poor mitochondrial health and systemic inflammation? test of a classic hypothesis in the Baltimore longitudinal study of aging. GeroScience. 2020;42:1175–1182. doi: 10.1007/s11357-020-00208-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Zhang Q, Raoof M, Chen Y, Sumi Y, Sursal T, Junger W, Brohi K, Itagaki K, Hauser CJ. Circulating mitochondrial DAMPs cause inflammatory responses to injury. Nature. 2010;464:104–107. doi: 10.1038/nature08780. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Editor's evaluation

Jonathan Flint 1

This paper makes a comprehensive survey of the relationship between mtDNAcn and the personality dimensions, as well as how and whether they mediate the relationships between personality dimensions and mortability as well as other behavioural measures that may lead to mortality.

Decision letter

Editor: Jonathan Flint1
Reviewed by: Na Cai2

Our editorial process produces two outputs: (i) public reviews designed to be posted alongside the preprint for the benefit of readers; (ii) feedback on the manuscript for the authors, including requests for revisions, shown below. We also include an acceptance summary that explains what the editors found interesting or important about the work.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Personality traits are consistently associated with blood mitochondrial DNA copy number estimated from genome sequences in two genetic cohort studies" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by 2 peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and Ma-Li Wong as the Senior Editor. The following individual involved in review of your submission has agreed to reveal their identity: Na Cai (Reviewer #2).

The reviewers have discussed their reviews with one another, and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this to help you prepare a revised submission.

Essential revisions:

1. mtDNAcn quantification: What are the WGS coverage for BLSA and SardiNIA respectively? It would be great to see a distribution of WGS coverage (over autosomes and over mtDNA, separately) in both studies, to get a sense of whether they are comparable, since quantification of mtDNAcn will be more accurate with higher WGS coverage. I suggest including these plots as additional supplementary figures and stating the coverage in main text.

2. Phenotype measures and definition: the authors wrote that they averaged across all four assessments of NEO-PI-R scores for BLSA participants as this "improves the BLSA analysis by reducing the standard deviation of the effects estimates and therefore increases statistical power". I suggest the authors show test-retest stability of these measures in a supplementary table or figure, drawing special attention to the age progression between the four assessments.

3. Personality mortality index (PMI) and mortality: In methods, the authors wrote that they computed a PMI based on "four NEO PI-R facets (vulnerability, activity, self-discipline, and competence) that have been previously shown as significantly associated with mortality", and "assigned a score of 0 or 1 to each individual based on the median value and the direction of the association of the trait with mortality". Is the association of the trait with mortality referred to here from reference 25? The authors also write in the main text that "we confirmed the association between PMI and mortality by finding that the participants had significantly longer survival time with increasing PMI from zero to four (Figure 4)". In Figure 4, I don't understand what the X axis means – it is labelled "Time" and goes from 0 to 6000, what units are these? Also, this means the authors must have derived a measure of mortality in BLSA and SardiNIA data, as they use it subsequently to perform mediation analysis between PMI, mtDNAcn and mortality. Is this mortality measure in survival time (ie. age at death, in the units shown in Figure 4)? Please explain what is "mortality" in all analysis that uses this measure, I can't find its derivation in the methods section. Also, please include a figure or table on the associations between each of the four NEO PI-R facets (vulnerability, activity, self-discipline, and competence) with "mortality" defined in BLSA and SardiNIA.

4. Mediation between PMI and mortability by mtDNAcn: Figure 4 legend writes "effect of PMI on mortality is fully mediated via mtDNAcn". I do not think this is true, as after regressing out mtDNAcn there is still significant direct effect of PMI to mortality. I suggest the authors change the phrasing of this to simply "mtDNAcn significantly mediates relationship between PMI and mortability".

5. Mediation of PMI on the relationship between mtDNAcn and mortality: I would have rather liked to see the mediation analysis performed the other way round. The current analysis tests for the indirect effect of PMI on mortality through mtDNAcn. I would like to see what are the direct and indirect effects of mtDNAcn on mortality, the latter controlling for PMI. This would allow us to better understand the relationship between PMI, mtDNAcn and mortality, and better infer the underlying biological mechanisms.

6. Previous studies have shown that mtDNAcn are potentially mediated by hormonal levels and thus menopause. Given the mean age of 57 in the SardiNIA cohort, the authors should investigate in more detail the potential confounding effects of menopause in women.

7. The only personality trait (out of the big five) available in the UK Biobank is neuroticism. Since the authors found that most of their associations are significant for this complex, I would strongly suggest they try to replicate their findings in patients from the UK Biobank which have both, genome-wide sequencing data as well the summary score of neuroticism (Data-Field 20127).

8. The amount of mtDNA varies across populations and across different haplogroups. The authors should therefore compute the major haplogroups present in Europeans and adjust/account for those variables in the correlation and mortality analyses.

9. The current study used buffy cells for sequencing, therefor the estimation of mtDNA per cell is not possible. Hence, the formula needs to be changed accordingly to indicate that the overall mtDNA abundance in blood is measured.

10. It is not clear which sequence coverage the authors want to include in their models. Autosomal, mitochondrial coverage or the total number of reads for each individual? Please specify.

11. It is not clear which regression method was used to assess mortality. I assume it was Cox regression adjusted for the most important covariates associated with mortality? This needs to be clearly stated.

12. Figure 4: Which regression coefficients are shown here? Those from a linear regression model? In this Figure, you also should show the 95% CI of the effect sizes and the P-Values as actual P-values not just asterisks (which are also not mentioned what they mean in the Figure legends).

Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

Methods:

– mtDNA copy number estimation. The current study used buffy cells for sequencing, therefor the estimation of mtDNA per cell is not possible. Hence, the formula needs to be changed accordingly to indicate that the overall mtDNA abundance in blood is measured.

– Statistical analyses. It is not clear which sequence coverage the authors want to include in their models. Autosomal, mitochondrial coverage or the total number of reads for each individual? Please specify.

– Statistical analyses. It is not clear which regression method was used to assess mortality. I assume it was Cox regression adjusted for the most important covariates associated with mortality? This needs to be clearly stated.

Results:

– Figure 4: Which regression coefficients are shown here? Those from a linear regression model? In this Figure, you also should show the 95% CI of the effect sizes and the P-Values as actual P-values not just asterisks (which are also not mentioned what they mean in the Figure legends).

Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

1. mtDNAcn quantification: What are the WGS coverage for BLSA and SardiNIA respectively? It would be great to see a distribution of WGS coverage (over autosomes and over mtDNA, separately) in both studies, to get a sense of whether they are comparable, since quantification of mtDNAcn will be more accurate with higher WGS coverage. I suggest including these plots as additional supplementary figures, and stating the coverage in main text.

2. Phenotype measures and definition: the authors wrote that they averaged across all four assessments of NEO-PI-R scores for BLSA participants as this "improves the BLSA analysis by reducing the standard deviation of the effects estimates and therefore increases statistical power". I suggest the authors show test-retest stability of these measures in a supplementary table or figure, drawing special attention to the age progression between the four assessments.

3. Personality mortality index (PMI) and mortality: In methods, the authors wrote that they computed a PMI based on "four NEO PI-R facets (vulnerability, activity, self-discipline, and competence) that have been previously shown as significantly associated with mortality", and "assigned a score of 0 or 1 to each individual based on the median value and the direction of the association of the trait with mortality". Is the association of the trait with mortality referred to here from reference 25? The authors also write in the main text that "we confirmed the association between PMI and mortality by finding that the participants had significantly longer survival time with increasing PMI from zero to four (Figure 4)". In Figure 4, I don't understand what the X axis means – it is labelled "Time" and goes from 0 to 6000, what units are these? Also, this means the authors must have derived a measure of mortality in BLSA and SardiNIA data, as they use it subsequently to perform mediation analysis between PMI, mtDNAcn and mortality. Is this mortality measure in survival time (ie. age at death, in the units shown in Figure 4)? Please explain what is "mortality" in all analysis that uses this measure, I can't find its derivation in the methods section. Also, please include a figure or table on the associations between each of the four NEO PI-R facets (vulnerability, activity, self-discipline, and competence) with "mortality" defined in BLSA and SardiNIA.

4. Mediation between PMI and mortability by mtDNAcn: Figure 4 legend writes "effect of PMI on mortality is fully mediated via mtDNAcn". I do not think this is true, as after regressing out mtDNAcn there is still significant direct effect of PMI to mortality. I suggest the authors change the phrasing of this to simply "mtDNAcn significantly mediates relationship between PMI and mortability".

5. Mediation of PMI on the relationship between mtDNAcn and mortality: I would have rather liked to see the mediation analysis performed the other way round. The current analysis tests for the indirect effect of PMI on mortality through mtDNAcn. I would like to see what are the direct and indirect effects of mtDNAcn on mortality, the latter controlling for PMI. This would allow us to better understand the relationship between PMI, mtDNAcn and mortality, and better infer the underlying biological mechanisms.

eLife. 2022 Dec 20;11:e77806. doi: 10.7554/eLife.77806.sa2

Author response


Essential revisions:

1. mtDNAcn quantification: What are the WGS coverage for BLSA and SardiNIA respectively? It would be great to see a distribution of WGS coverage (over autosomes and over mtDNA, separately) in both studies, to get a sense of whether they are comparable, since quantification of mtDNAcn will be more accurate with higher WGS coverage. I suggest including these plots as additional supplementary figures and stating the coverage in main text.

We thank the reviewer for this comment. The average WGS coverage for BLSA and SardiNIA participants is 35.8X and 3.8X, respectively (because BLSA performed high coverage sequencing while SardiNIA did low-pass sequencing). We have included as Appendix 1-figure 7, four histograms describing the autosomal and mtDNA coverage for BLSA and SardiNIA. We have also modified the sentence in line 251 as shown below to include the sequence coverage in the main text.

“After adjusting for the effect of sex, sequence coverage (mean sequence coverage of 35.8X and 3.8X, respectively for BLSA and SardiNIA), platelet count, and white blood cell parameters, there remained a significant, albeit modest, inverse association of mtDNAcn with age in both study cohorts (Figure 1 and Appendix 1-table 1).”

2. Phenotype measures and definition: the authors wrote that they averaged across all four assessments of NEO-PI-R scores for BLSA participants as this "improves the BLSA analysis by reducing the standard deviation of the effects estimates and therefore increases statistical power". I suggest the authors show test-retest stability of these measures in a supplementary table or figure, drawing special attention to the age progression between the four assessments.

We thank the reviewer for the suggestion. For the BLSA cohort, there were four personality assessment points on average per participant. We used the average across all assessment points. We had stated the test-retest stability for the five personality domains for BLSA in the main text (line 162). Now, motivated by the reviewer’s suggestion, we have performed the test-retest stability test for all the facets and included the bar plot of the results as Appendix 1 -figure 5.

3. Personality mortality index (PMI) and mortality: In methods, the authors wrote that they computed a PMI based on "four NEO PI-R facets (vulnerability, activity, self-discipline, and competence) that have been previously shown as significantly associated with mortality", and "assigned a score of 0 or 1 to each individual based on the median value and the direction of the association of the trait with mortality". Is the association of the trait with mortality referred to here from reference 25?

Yes. We have made it clear in the main text as below (line 217):

“In subsequent analyses, we computed a personality-mortality index (PMI) using data from the four NEO PI-R facets (vulnerability, activity, self-discipline, and competence) that have been previously shown as significantly associated with mortality by Chapman et al. (25).”

The authors also write in the main text that "we confirmed the association between PMI and mortality by finding that the participants had significantly longer survival time with increasing PMI from zero to four (Figure 4)". In Figure 4, I don't understand what the X axis means – it is labelled "Time" and goes from 0 to 6000, what units are these?

The X-axis labeled Time had units in days, so the range of the time on the X-axis was from 0 to 6,000 days (0 – 16.4 years). We realized that using “day” as the unit is confusing and less interpretable. We have now updated the plot in figure 4 to use “year” as the time unit.

Also, this means the authors must have derived a measure of mortality in BLSA and SardiNIA data, as they use it subsequently to perform mediation analysis between PMI, mtDNAcn and mortality. Is this mortality measure in survival time (ie. age at death, in the units shown in Figure 4)?

We agree with the reviewer that we had not made this clear. Mortality used in the mediation analysis is a binary variable describing the dead or alive status of study participants. We have made this clearer in the main text in line 230:

“Finally, we used R package lavaan (version 0.6-7) to perform a mediation analysis to test whether mtDNAcn mediates the association between personality (i.e., PMI) and mortality risk (a binary variable describing the dead or alive status of study participants).”

Please explain what is "mortality" in all analysis that uses this measure, I can't find its derivation in the methods section. Also, please include a figure or table on the associations between each of the four NEO PI-R facets (vulnerability, activity, self-discipline, and competence) with "mortality" defined in BLSA and SardiNIA.

We thank the reviewer for this comment. We used mortality data to derive time-to-event in the Kaplan-Meier plot shown in figure 4. Mortality in subsequent analyses is a binary variable describing the dead or alive status of study participants. We had stated this in the main text line 228.

Also, as suggested by the reviewer, we have performed a Cox Proportional Hazards analysis for the four NEO PI-R facets and included the results as Appendix 1-table 3. We have also summarized the results in the main text in line 325:

“First, we performed a Cox Proportional Hazards analysis for the four NEO PI-R facets (Appendix 1-table 3). The analysis showed that after adjusting for age and sex, all four facets are significantly associated with increased risk of mortality (vulnerability) or reduced risk of mortality (activity, self-discipline, and competence).”

4. Mediation between PMI and mortability by mtDNAcn: Figure 4 legend writes "effect of PMI on mortality is fully mediated via mtDNAcn". I do not think this is true, as after regressing out mtDNAcn there is still significant direct effect of PMI to mortality. I suggest the authors change the phrasing of this to simply "mtDNAcn significantly mediates relationship between PMI and mortability".

We appreciate this comment, and have changed the wording of that figure legend to reflect the reviewer’s comment:

“The effect of Personality-mortality Index (PMI) on mortality is significantly mediated via mtDNAcn.”

5. Mediation of PMI on the relationship between mtDNAcn and mortality: I would have rather liked to see the mediation analysis performed the other way round. The current analysis tests for the indirect effect of PMI on mortality through mtDNAcn. I would like to see what are the direct and indirect effects of mtDNAcn on mortality, the latter controlling for PMI. This would allow us to better understand the relationship between PMI, mtDNAcn and mortality, and better infer the underlying biological mechanisms.

We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We have performed a mediation analysis that treats PMI as the mediator and tests the direct and indirect effect of mtDNAcn on mortality. We have included the results as Appendix 1-table 4. We have also summarized the results in the supplementary information:

“We used causal mediation analysis to test the indirect effect of mtDNAcn on mortality via PMI as a mediator. The indirect effect estimate showed that the effect of mtDNAcn on mortality is not mediated through PMI. We tested for the significance of the indirect effect using 5000 bootstrap samples and it was not statistically significant (p-values = 0.355 and 0.963, respectively for BLSA and SardiNIA). See Appendix 1-table 4 for more details.”

6. Previous studies have shown that mtDNAcn are potentially mediated by hormonal levels and thus menopause. Given the mean age of 57 in the SardiNIA cohort, the authors should investigate in more detail the potential confounding effects of menopause in women.

We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. In response, we have investigated menopausal status as a potential confounder on mtDNAcn. We found that there was no significant difference between the mtDNAcn of pre-menopausal and menopausal women (p-value = 0.56; a plot as shown in Appendix 1-figure 9). The results show that menopause had no effect on mtDNAcn in the SardiNIA cohort. Also, we ran two association models between mtDNAcn and NEO PI-R traits in women, with and without adjustment for the effect of menopause status (both models adjusted for the effect of age, sequence coverage, WBC, platelet count, and percentages of major types of leukocytes). The results are shown in the supplementary information in Appendix 1-table 5. The results show that menopause status does not affect the association between mtDNAcn and personality: the effect sizes and p-values were almost the same in both models for all the traits.

7. The only personality trait (out of the big five) available in the UK Biobank is neuroticism. Since the authors found that most of their associations are significant for this complex, I would strongly suggest they try to replicate their findings in patients from the UK Biobank which have both, genome-wide sequencing data as well the summary score of neuroticism (Data-Field 20127).

We thank the reviewer and agree that a further extension of our findings to a larger cohort like the UK Biobank would be a natural next step for this study. However, the current study already provides replication of the results in two independent cohorts, and thus we prefer to retain the focus on these cohorts. The reasons include:

1) We have found the association between mtDNAcn and neuroticism traits in two independent cohorts for which neuroticism traits were measured in exactly the same way (using the NEO PI-R Inventory with 48 items). In contrast, the UK Biobank participants were assessed for neuroticism with 12 items, and the measurements were therefore different. Furthermore, the UKB did not assess the other personality factors or facets, and we could not perform most of the analyses presented in this manuscript.

2) An analysis of UK Biobank data in this current study is a logistical challenge. In addition to gaining access to UK Biobank data, we would also have to calculate the mtDNAcn for UK Biobank participants with whole-genome sequencing data (currently for 200,000 participants) – which would be computationally expensive and require considerable time and effort by staff who are now differently tasked.

Therefore, we believe that the replication in two independent cohorts meets the criterion for publication of a first report.

Still, we strongly agree with the Reviewer that replication in the UK Biobank is important, and we have added the following paragraph in the Discussion section line 465:

“In our current study, we showed a significant association between mtDNAcn and neuroticism traits that replicated in two independent cohorts. Other larger cohorts (e.g., UK Biobank) have also collected certain personality information (e.g., questions about neuroticism) as well as whole-genome sequencing data (which makes estimating mtDNAcn feasible). Therefore, trying to replicate our findings in such larger cohorts will be a natural extension of this study.”

8. The amount of mtDNA varies across populations and across different haplogroups. The authors should therefore compute the major haplogroups present in Europeans and adjust/account for those variables in the correlation and mortality analyses.

We thank the reviewer for this comment. We have computed the major haplogroups present in our dataset. There were 17 haplogroups in total, and we excluded seven haplogroups that were extremely rare (present in less than 5 study participants). The most common haplogroup was H (Frequency of about 42%). The boxplot shows the relative mtDNAcn in each haplogroup. mtDNAcn has a similar distribution among the different haplogroups, although slight variations in mtDNAcn were observed amongst them. We further investigated if these mtDNAcn variations amongst the haplogroups were statistically significant by testing for the association of each haplogroup versus all other haplogroups with mtDNAcn. Amongst the 10 haplogroups present in more than 5 study participants, none was significantly associated with mtDNAcn. This finding suggests that none of the haplogroups present in our study participants will impact our results. Our results are also consistent with the literature. For example, Cai and colleagues stated in their paper (Cai et al., Front Genet. 2020) that “Testing each haplogroup against all others for association with mtDNA copy number (Table 1), we found that mtDNA copy number is significantly higher in Haplogroup L, and lower in Haplogroups A, B, C, D, and F, none of which occur at high frequencies in Europe.” Indeed, the 6 haplogroups that were significantly associated with mtDNA copy number were also very rare in our study: L present in 4 participants; A present in 2 participants; B present in 1 participant; C present in 2 participants; D present in 3 participants; and F present in 0 participants. We have included the figure and table as supplementary information (Appendix 1-figure 10 and Appendix 1-table 6).

9. The current study used buffy cells for sequencing, therefor the estimation of mtDNA per cell is not possible. Hence, the formula needs to be changed accordingly to indicate that the overall mtDNA abundance in blood is measured.

We thank the reviewer for this important comment. We indeed used DNA extracted from buffy coat, which is the most common DNA source for sequencing, to do the whole-genome sequencing analyzed here. Buffy coat contains leukocytes and platelets, so the reviewer is certainly right that the mtDNA copy number is not estimated for a specific cell type.

Without considering platelets, the formula for mtDNA copy number calculates a weighted average (weights determined by the proportion of the leukocytes populations) of mtDNA copy number per cell across all leukocytes; each cell has two copies of nuclear DNA that provide the reference for the calculation. The platelet content of the buffy coat then makes the calculation somewhat more complicated and less precise. Because platelets have mtDNA but do not contain nuclear DNA, the formula overestimates the mtDNA copy number (Rausser et al. eLife 2021). One platelet contains on average 1.6 molecules of mtDNA (Urata et al. Ann Clin Biochem 2008; Hurtado-Roca et al. PLoS One 2016). And there are on average 40 platelets per leukocyte in whole blood (Urata et al. Ann Clin Biochem 2008), though that number is likely to be substantially lower in the buffy coat (we have no precise measurements of the amounts). Thus, the impact of platelets on buffy coat mtDNA copy number is likely small but certainly not negligible. This represents a limitation of both our own and every other study of mtDNA in blood DNA material (Picard M. Mitochondrion 2022).

Because of this limitation, papers published in the field typically use both platelet count, and white blood cell counts as covariates in any models testing the association between mtDNA copy number and quantitative traits. We also used this conventional approach in all of our analyses.

In response to the important comment of the Reviewer, we have added the following paragraph to the Discussion section line 450:

“Additionally, we note that the formula for mtDNA copy number calculates a weighted average (weights determined by the proportion of leukocyte populations) of mtDNA copy number per cell across all leukocytes; each cell has two copies of nuclear DNA that provide the reference for the calculation. However, this does not take into account the platelet content of the buffy coat. Because each platelet has about 1.6 copies of mtDNA (Urata et al. Ann Clin Biochem 2008; Hurtado-Roca et al. PLoS ONE 2016) but does not contain nuclear DNA, the formula somewhat overestimates mtDNA copy number (Rausser et al. eLife 2021). The platelet content of buffy coat may also vary, representing a limitation of this and other studies of mtDNA in blood DNA material (Picard M. Mitochondrion 2022). Because of this limitation, papers published on mtDNA copy number typically use both platelets count, and white blood cell counts as covariates in any models testing the association between mtDNA copy number and quantitative traits. We also used this conventional approach in our analyses.”

10. It is not clear which sequence coverage the authors want to include in their models. Autosomal, mitochondrial coverage or the total number of reads for each individual? Please specify.

We have now made it clear in the methods that we use autosomal coverage for each individual in the regression models by adding this sentence in line 187:

“…sequence coverage (the average autosomal coverage for each study participant),”

11. It is not clear which regression method was used to assess mortality. I assume it was Cox regression adjusted for the most important covariates associated with mortality? This needs to be clearly stated.

Yes, we used the Cox Proportional Hazards model and adjusted for age and sex. We have made it clearer in the main text by adding this sentence in line 213:

We also used the Cox Proportional Hazards model to test for their association with mortality, adjusting for age and sex.”

12. Figure 4: Which regression coefficients are shown here? Those from a linear regression model? In this Figure, you also should show the 95% CI of the effect sizes and the P-Values as actual P-values not just asterisks (which are also not mentioned what they mean in the Figure legends).

The regression coefficients shown in figure 4 are the coefficients from the causal mediation analysis. We have updated the figure to show the 95% CI and p-values of the coefficients and have also been explicit in the figure legend.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Transparent reporting form
    Reporting standard 1. A checklist of items that should be included in reports of observational studies.
    Source code 1. R code for the analysis on mtDNAcn and personality.
    elife-77806-code1.zip (88.2KB, zip)

    Data Availability Statement

    Researchers interested in using BLSA data (e.g., sequencing or phenotypic data) need to submit an analysis plan to a review committee organized by the National Institute on Aging (A detailed guideline for the submission of an analysis plan can be found at https://www.blsa.nih.gov/how-apply). Researchers will get the access once the proposed analysis plans are reviewed and approved. Aggregated genetic and phenotypic data from the SardiNIA study are deposited in the European Genome-phenome Archive (EGA) (https://ega-archive.org/studies/phs000338). In accord with European Union regulations, disaggregated data, including sequence/genotyping data of individuals, cannot be made public, but investigators can request specific analyses by sending a one-page proposal description to Dr. F. Cucca (fcucca@uniss.it), the P.I. of the study. After screening by an internal committee, the requested analyses are customarily done by staff of IRGB and the results provided to the requestor. The R code for the analyses performed in this paper is provided as a Supplementary file (source code file: R code for the analysis on mtDNAcn and personality).


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES