Abstract
Functional brain connectivity (FBC), or areas that are anatomically separate but temporally synchronized in their activation, represent a sensitive biomarker for monitoring dementia progression. It is unclear whether frailty is associated with FBC in those at higher risk of progression to dementia (e.g., mild cognitive impairment -MCI-) and if sex plays a role. We used baseline data from the SYNERGIC trial, including participants with MCI that received brain MRI. In this cross-sectional analyses (n = 100), we measured frailty using a deficit accumulation frailty index. Using the CONN toolbox, we assessed FBC of networks and regions of interest across the entire connectome. We used Pearson’s correlation to investigate the relationship between FBC and frailty index in the full sample and by sex. We also divided the full sample and each sex into tertiles based upon their frailty index score and then assessed between-tertile differences in FBC. The full sample (cluster: size = 291 p-FDR < 0.05) and males (cluster: size = 993 and 451 p-FDR < 0.01) demonstrated that increasing (stronger) connectivity between the right hippocampus and clusters in the temporal gyrus was positively correlated with increasing (worse) frailty. Males also demonstrated between-tertile differences in right hippocampus connectivity to clusters in the lateral occipital cortex (cluster: size = 289 p-FDR < 0.05). Regardless of frailty status, females demonstrated stronger within-network connectivity of the Default-Mode (p = 0.024). Our results suggest that increasing (worse) frailty was associated with increasing (stronger) connectivity between regions not typically linked, which may reflect a compensation tactic by the plastic brain. Furthermore, the relationship between the two variables appears to differ by sex. Our results may help elucidate why specific individuals progress to a dementia syndrome. NCT02808676. https://www.clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT02808676
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s11357-022-00702-4.
Keywords: fMRI, Functional brain connectivity, Mild cognitive impairment, Frailty, Older adults, Cross-sectional
Introduction
Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) represents a prodromal stage between expected age-related cognitive decline and dementia, but more than half of those classified remain stable or recovered [1–4]. Identifying which individuals will eventually progress to a dementia syndrome, including Alzheimer’s disease, is an important research goal [5]. Frailty is common in older adults with Alzheimer’s disease [6], and it moderates the relationship between Alzheimer’s disease pathology and clinical symptoms; individuals with low levels of Alzheimer’s pathology but a high degree of frailty appear to be at greater risk for dementia than those with low levels of Alzheimer’s pathology and a low degree of frailty [7]. Similar observations have been made regarding the relationship between biomarkers and dementia, and genetic risk and its clinical disease expression [8]. Ultimately, frailty may partially explain the progression or lack thereof to Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias amongst individuals with MCI [9].
Conceptually, frailty is a graded state of age-related decreasing physiological reserve that gives rise to vulnerability to adverse health outcomes or stressors [10], such as the novel COVID-19 virus [11]. Frailty is a distinct [12] multidimensional [13] entity, often including deficits in cognitive [14] and physical function [15], as well as social and affective domains [16]. Like cognitive decline, frailty is dynamic [17] as people can transition between varying states [18, 19]. Changes in frailty status may occur concurrently with dementia-related changes in neural substrates, as frailty has been shown to predict dementia [20]. Recently, there has been a call for an investigation into the common etiology of frailty and dementia [21].
Researchers have shown frailty to be negatively associated with global brain volume [22], as well as the microstructure of gray and white matter [23]. The relationship between frailty and brain function has garnered less attention. Functional brain connectivity (FBC) refers to brain areas that are spatially separated but temporally synchronized in their activation [24]. FBC is believed to enable efficient information processing and the completion of complex functions [25]. FBC can be measured using functional magnetic resonance imaging and is considered a sensitive biomarker in those at risk of progression to dementia as changes precede structural atrophy and occur years before clinical manifestation [26]. We know of only two studies that have examined FBC and frailty status, using magnetoencephalography [27] and functional magnetic resonance imaging [28]. Both studies classified their cognitively healthy sample using the Cardiovascular Health Study–Frailty Phenotype, which focuses largely on physical frailty domains [29] and restricted their investigation of FBC to motor areas [27, 28]. We found no studies that examined the relationship between frailty and FBC in individuals with cognitive impairment or how a more multidimensional measure of frailty status, such as the frailty index (FI) [30], is associated with FBC in motor areas and beyond.
This cross-sectional study investigated the relationship between frailty status, assessed using the FI, and FBC in individuals clinically classified with MCI. We hypothesized that frailty would be associated with FBC in individuals with MCI. Males and females demonstrate differences in cerebral function [31] and blood flow [32]. Additionally, males are at a greater risk of developing MCI [33], but frailty is more common in females [34]. Therefore, we also conducted a sub-analysis based on the hypothesis that males and females would differ in their association between frailty and FBC.
Methods
Design and participants
The SYNchronizing Exercises, Remedies in GaIt and Cognition (SYNERGIC) trial [35] (NCT02808676) was a multi-site, randomized, phase II, fractional factorial, double-blind controlled study evaluating the effect of combined physical exercise separately and synergistically with cognitive training and/or high-dose vitamin D3 supplementation in older adults (60 to 85 years) with MCI. All SYNERGIC sites were in Canada and included Western University (London, ON; lead site), University of Waterloo (Waterloo, ON), Wilfrid Laurier University (Waterloo, ON), University of Montreal (Montreal, QC), and University of British Columbia (Vancouver, BC). SYNERGIC participants completed three in-person assessments, including baseline or pre-intervention (T0), post-intervention (T6), and follow-up (T12). T0 and T6 occurred immediately before and after a 20-week intervention, while T12 occurred 6 months after T6. Given the cross-sectional nature of the present study, pre-intervention baseline data or T0 is the only time point used in our analyses (Supplemental Material A).
Potential participants were diagnosed with MCI following existing guidelines [36]. The inclusion criteria also required proficiency in English or French (Montreal site), ability to ambulate at least 10 m independently, possessing (corrected) normal vision, in sufficient health according to the Physical Activity Readiness Questionnaire-Plus (PAR-Q +) [37], and ability to comply with trial procedures. The present study’s exclusion criteria was identical to the parent trial except for the following additions: (1) did not complete an MRI assessment at baseline; and (2) participants consider their left hand to be dominant. SYN recruited potential participants from the community and clinics serving MCI populations from September 2016 to March 2020; the trial was terminated early due to the COVID-19 pandemic. Subjects’ consent was obtained according to the Declaration of Helsinki, and all institutions received approval from their local ethics board.
Frailty
All in-person assessments included collecting demographic information and a battery of tests (Supplemental Material B). Therefore, it permitted a secondary retrospective analysis of participants’ frailty status via a FI. The FI is a health state measure that reflects vulnerability to adverse health outcomes or, put more simply, a cumulative deficit model where “the more individuals have wrong with them, the more likely they are to be frail [38].” The FI is calculated as:
A person with 9 of 30 potential deficits has an FI of (9/30) 0.30 and is considered “more frail” than an individual with 4 of 30 potential deficits (4/30 = 0.13). Compared to other tools, the FI is unidimensional and has been suggested to have high predictive value in community settings and for adverse outcomes [39]. All variables in the present study’s FI have been previously utilized in other FIs [40, 41] and are listed in Supplemental Material C. The included FI variables were grouped into one of the following nine domains: physical, functional, exhaustion, nutrition, neuropsychiatric, falling, comorbidities, vital signs, and medications. Given the demographic of interest, we excluded measures of cognitive function from our FI as such variables were expected to be impaired; previous research has done the same for cardiovascular outcomes in a cardiac rehabilitation demographic [40]. Notably, the total number of variables included in the FI is inconsequential as long as there are at least 30 [42].
MRIs
MRIs were collected according to version 3.8 of the Canadian Dementia Imaging Protocol [43], but only T1W and resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging scans were used here. We visually inspected data for overall quality and then organized according to the brain imaging data structure [44], preprocessed via fMRIPrep (version 20.2.0) [45] (Supplemental Material D), skull-stripped using FMRIB Software Library (version 6.0.4) [46] Brain Extraction Tool [47], and then uploaded to the CONN Functional Connectivity Toolbox (version 20.b) [48]. Once in CONN, we denoised and analyzed data using both a region of interest (ROI)-to-ROI (ROI-ROI) and seed-to-voxel (S-V) approach; the rationale to conduct multiple analyses was based upon the study’s exploratory nature, and that previous researchers have conducted multiple analyses within the same study [49, 50]. ROIs included the Default-Mode [51], Dorsal Attention [52], Salience [53], Frontoparietal [54], and Sensorimotor [55] networks, while the S-V analysis used both the left and right hippocampus as seeds (Supplemental Material E–F). Finally, we exported significant clusters into both Multi-image Analysis GUI (MANGO; version 4.1) [56] and xjView (version 9.7) [57] to review the overlap of cluster coordinates and consistency in anatomical labeling, respectively.
Notably, CONN’s quality assurance plots, including variables related to motion, global signal change, and valid scans, should score ≥ 95% [58]. After completing the original denoising, the quality assurance plots did not achieve the 95% goal. Therefore, we extracted voxel-wise standardization (DVARS) and mean framewise displacement values, which reflect signal change and motion, via MRI Quality Control [59]; similar to fMRIPrep, MRI Quality Control is another application [60] available to datasets organized according to the brain imaging data structure. Subsequently, DVARS and framewise displacement values were imported into the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS version 27; IBM Canada Ltd. Markham, Ontario) to identify and remove participants classified as extreme (± 3 times the interquartile range) outliers. Quality assurance plots then achieved the recommended 95% goal (Supplemental Material G).
Statistical analyses
Demographic information
Except for sex reported as sample size, we summarized the demographic characteristics using means and standard deviations. To identify trends in whom or potentially why specific individuals chose to forgo MRIs, we also compared the characteristics of participants who completed baseline imaging versus those who did not. In SPSS, an independent samples t-test assessed between-group (i.e., sex and MRI completion status) differences in participant characteristics.
FBC and frailty in ROI-ROI analysis
We applied no cluster or connection threshold to the ROI-ROI analysis as we aimed to ascertain the average connectivity score for every within-network connection for every participant after controlling for the covariates of age, sex, and years of education. We then calculated the average within-network connectivity for each participant by averaging the individual connectivity scores. For example, CONN’s Default-Mode network includes four ROIs (medial prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, and lateral parietal cortex left and right). We calculated the connectivity of the six possible connections for each participant and then averaged the six connections for a Default-Mode connectivity score. We then imported each participant’s average within-network connectivity score into SPSS, where we completed a Pearson correlation with the FI z-score. We converted FI to a z-score via standardized linear regression residuals to control for the same covariates used in CONN (age, sex, and years of education).
FBC and frailty in S-V analysis
Unlike ROI-ROI, we imported raw FI values into CONN to test the existence of significant associations between connectivity and FI after controlling for the same covariates (age, sex, and years of education). As such, we utilized standard S-V thresholds (cluster threshold, p < 0.05 cluster-size p-FDR corrected; voxel threshold, p < 0.001 p-uncorrected) [61]. We did not follow the same procedure for the ROI-ROI analysis due to (1) no interest in a particular connection with the seed and (2) the likely overwhelming number of results produced from eliminating cluster and voxel thresholds.
Sex
The above analyses were repeated in males and females separately, keeping age and years of education as covariates.
Tertiles
We divided our entire sample and each sex into FI tertiles (cut-points = 0.16 and 0.23). Tertiles permit the grouping and, thus, comparison of FBC across a more narrow frailty spectrum (i.e., low, medium, and high). Previous work in frailty and brain health also utilized a tertiles strategy [21]. Average FBC was compared between the three tertiles after controlling for sex, age, and self-reported years of education; sex was removed as a covariate when conducting tertile analyses in males and females. Tertile characteristics were compared in SPSS using a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) to determine if any differences other than the mean FI value existed.
Data availability
The data supporting this study’s findings are available from the corresponding authors upon reasonable request.
Results
Demographic information
SYN randomized 183 participants. One hundred twenty completed a baseline MRI, but we removed 20 from the present study’s analysis due to left-handedness, n = 8; MRI artifacts, n = 2; incomplete dataset, n = 5; and identified as an outlier based upon DVARS and/or framewise displacement values, n = 5 (Fig. 1). Therefore, the final analysis included 100 participants (females, n = 48).
Individuals that completed an MRI were significantly older than those who did not (Supplemental Material H; p = 0.047). The baseline characteristics of the participants included in the present study’s analysis (n = 100) generally reflected those of the original (n = 120) sample (Supplemental Material I). However, and as expected for sex comparisons within this demographic, height (p < 0.001), weight (p < 0.001), and years of education (p = 0.047) differed (Table 1). The average FI score for the full sample (n = 100) was 0.19. Females were frailer than males, but not significantly so. However, females demonstrated statistically higher or stronger within-network connectivity of the Default-Mode network (p = 0.024). The full sample also showed a significant difference in body mass index when comparing the low and medium tertiles (p = 0.023; Table 2). There were no significant between-tertile differences in baseline characteristics for each sex (Tables 3 and 4).
Table 1.
Characteristic | Total (n = 100) | Males (n = 52) | Females (n = 48) | p-value |
---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 73.79 ± 6.24 | 74.27 ± 6.19 | 73.27 ± 6.31 | 0.426 |
# of comorbidities | 4.73 ± 2.54 | 4.65 ± 2.57 | 4.81 ± 2.53 | 0.757 |
Years of education | 15.10 ± 3.67 | 15.79 ± 4.08 | 14.33 ± 3.04 | 0.047 |
Height (cm) | 167.33 ± 10.05 | 173.81 ± 7.21 | 160.31 ± 7.70 | < 0.001 |
Weight (kg) | 74.94 ± 14.89 | 82.56 ± 13.40 | 66.69 ± 11.77 | < 0.001 |
Body mass index | 26.65 ± 4.20 | 27.29 ± 3.94 | 25.96 ± 4.41 | 0.114 |
MoCA | 22.88 ± 3.06a | 22.94 ± 2.89 | 22.81 ± 3.27b | 0.829 |
Frailty index value | 0.19 ± 0.07 | 0.19 ± 0.07 | 0.20 ± 0.07 | 0.594 |
Default-Mode connectivity | 0.5283 ± 0.1712 | 0.4915 ± 0.1659 | 0.5682 ± 0.1696 | 0.024 |
Sensorimotor connectivity | 0.5784 ± 0.2462 | 0.6085 ± 0.2405 | 0.5458 ± 0.2505 | 0.205 |
Salience connectivity | 0.3974 ± 0.1490 | 0.3785 ± 0.1497 | 0.4179 ± 0.1470 | 0.188 |
Dorsal Attention connectivity | 0.4458 ± 0.1674 | 0.4695 ± 0.1836 | 0.4201 ± 0.1456 | 0.141 |
Frontoparietal connectivity | 0.4875 ± 0.1730 | 0.4693 ± 0.1834 | 0.5072 ± 0.1605 | 0.275 |
All values are mean ± standard deviation. Independent samples t-test used in analysis of males versus females. MoCA Montreal Cognitive Assessment, # number, cm centimeters, kg kilograms; an = 99; bn = 47. Bolded p-values indicate statistical significance
Table 2.
Characteristic | Low (n = 39) | Medium (n = 31) | High (n = 30) | p-value |
---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 72.62 ± 6.39 | 74.16 ± 7.04 | 74.93 ± 4.97 | 0.289 |
Years of education | 14.94 ± 3.02 | 15.73 ± 4.86 | 14.63 ± 3.00 | 0.486 |
Height (cm) | 169.11 ± 10.71 | 168.68 ± 9.37 | 163.62 ± 9.11 | 0.052 |
Weight (kg) | 72.59 ± 14.06 | 79.03 ± 15.45 | 73.79 ± 14.97 | 0.175 |
Body mass index | 25.22 ± 3.39 | 27.69 ± 4.59 | 27.44 ± 4.36 | 0.023* |
MoCA | 22.69 ± 3.33 | 23.19 ± 2.96 | 22.79 ± 2.87a | 0.784 |
Default-Mode connectivity | 0.5340 ± 0.1818 | 0.5418 ± 0.1518 | 0.5070 ± 0.1796 | 0.710 |
Sensorimotor connectivity | 0.5580 ± 0.2777 | 0.6420 ± 0.2232 | 0.5394 ± 0.2186 | 0.215 |
Salience connectivity | 0.4034 ± 0.166 | 0.3957 ± 0.1354 | 0.3914 ± 0.1436 | 0.945 |
Dorsal Attention connectivity | 0.4240 ± 0.1970 | 0.4609 ± 0.1256 | 0.4585 ± 0.1658 | 0.587 |
Frontoparietal connectivity | 0.4750 ± 0.1899 | 0.5081 ± 0.1791 | 0.4825 ± 0.1450 | 0.719 |
All values are mean ± standard deviation. One-way ANOVA used for analysis. MoCA Montreal Cognitive Assessment, # number, cm centimeters, kg kilograms; an = 29. *, low significantly different than intermediate
Table 3.
Characteristic | Low (n = 18) | Medium (n = 12) | High (n = 18) | p-value |
---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 71.44 ± 5.88 | 74.17 ± 9.05 | 74.5 ± 4.09 | 0.302 |
Years of education | 14.81 ± 3.11 | 14.79 ± 3.58 | 13.56 ± 2.55 | 0.397 |
Height (cm) | 160.99 ± 8.56 | 162.67 ± 8.56 | 158.06 ± 5.8 | 0.25 |
Weight (kg) | 63.53 ± 11.22 | 70.52 ± 15.14 | 67.31 ± 9.36 | 0.275 |
Body mass index | 24.43 ± 3.43 | 26.67 ± 5.67 | 27.02 ± 4.15 | 0.174 |
MoCA | 22.61 ± 3.57 | 22.92 ± 3.85 | 22.94 ± 2.63a | 0.95 |
Default-Mode connectivity | 0.6134 ± 0.1411 | 0.5520 ± 0.1588 | 0.5338 ± 0.1993 | 0.353 |
Sensorimotor connectivity | 0.5586 ± 0.3090 | 0.5399 ± 0.2661 | 0.5370 ± 0.1788 | 0.968 |
Salience connectivity | 0.4093 ± 0.1435 | 0.4122 ± 0.1720 | 0.4303 ± 0.1405 | 0.905 |
Dorsal Attention connectivity | 0.3988 ± 0.1619 | 0.4338 ± 0.0919 | 0.4323 ± 0.1617 | 0.741 |
Frontoparietal connectivity | 0.4892 ± 0.1843 | 0.5322 ± 0.1879 | 0.5086 ± 0.1167 | 0.778 |
All values are mean ± standard deviation. One-way ANOVA used for analysis. MoCA Montreal Cognitive Assessment; # number, cm centimeters, kg kilograms; an = 17. No p-values reached statistical significance
Table 4.
Characteristic | Low (n = 21) | Medium (n = 19) | High (n = 12) | p-value |
---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 73.62 ± 6.76 | 74.16 ± 5.70 | 75.58 ± 6.20 | 0.686 |
Years of education | 15.05 ± 3.01 | 16.32 ± 5.54 | 16.25 ± 2.99 | 0.569 |
Height (cm) | 176.07 ± 6.73 | 172.47 ± 7.91 | 171.98 ± 6.31 | 0.176 |
Weight (kg) | 80.35 ± 11.47 | 84.40 ± 13.38 | 83.52 ± 16.84 | 0.618 |
Body mass index | 25.91 ± 3.28 | 28.33 ± 3.79 | 28.08 ± 4.77 | 0.11 |
MoCA | 22.76 ± 3.19 | 23.37 ± 2.34 | 22.58 ± 3.29 | 0.72 |
Default-Mode connectivity | 0.4659 ± 0.188 | 0.5353 ± 0.1513 | 0.4668 ± 0.144 | 0.359 |
Sensorimotor connectivity | 0.5574 ± 0.2556 | 0.7065 ± 0.1686 | 0.5429 ± 0.2768 | 0.080 |
Salience connectivity | 0.3984 ± 0.1865 | 0.3853 ± 0.1104 | 0.3331 ± 0.1328 | 0.478 |
Dorsal Attention connectivity | 0.4457 ± 0.2244 | 0.4780 ± 0.1426 | 0.4977 ± 0.1713 | 0.721 |
Frontoparietal connectivity | 0.4628 ± 0.1982 | 0.4929 ± 0.1768 | 0.4433 ± 0.1778 | 0.755 |
All values are mean ± standard deviation. One-way ANOVA used for analysis. MoCA Montreal Cognitive Assessment, # number, cm centimeters, kg kilograms. No p-values reached statistical significance
FBC and frailty in ROI-ROI analysis
There were no significant correlations between within-network connectivity (Default-Mode, Sensorimotor, Salience, Dorsal Attention, and Frontoparietal) and FI values for the full sample nor males and females. All effect sizes were considered insignificant or small (|r|< 0.3; Table 5 and Supplemental Material J-K).
Table 5.
Group | Variables included in correlation analysis | Correlation score (r) | Significance (2-tailed; p) | |
---|---|---|---|---|
All subjects (n = 100) | Frailty index | Default-Mode connectivity | − 0.098 | 0.334 |
Sensorimotor connectivity | 0.040 | 0.692 | ||
Salience connectivity | − 0.014 | 0.892 | ||
Dorsal Attention connectivity | 0.178 | 0.076 | ||
Frontoparietal connectivity | − 0.035 | 0.727 | ||
Females (n = 48) | Frailty index | Default-Mode connectivity | − 0.210 | 0.153 |
Sensorimotor connectivity | 0.000 | 0.998 | ||
Salience connectivity | 0.129 | 0.382 | ||
Dorsal Attention connectivity | 0.155 | 0.294 | ||
Frontoparietal connectivity | 0.029 | 0.845 | ||
Males (n = 52) | Frailty index | Default-Mode connectivity | 0.007 | 0.963 |
Sensorimotor connectivity | 0.076 | 0.590 | ||
Salience connectivity | − 0.146 | 0.302 | ||
Dorsal Attention connectivity | 0.200 | 0.156 | ||
Frontoparietal connectivity | − 0.095 | 0.504 |
No p-values reached statistical significance
FBC and frailty in S-V analysis
After controlling for covariates, the full sample demonstrated that the FI score was positively correlated with connectivity between the right hippocampus and a cluster located in the left inferior and middle temporal gyrus (Fig. 2/Table 6; cluster: size = 291 p-FDR < 0.05). Females showed no significant association between FI and FBC within this analysis. Conversely, males demonstrated that their FI score was positively correlated with connectivity between the right hippocampus and clusters from bilateral regions of the inferior and middle temporal gyrus (Fig. 3/Table 6; cluster: size = 993 and 451 p-FDR < 0.01) even after controlling for covariates; one cluster demonstrated overlap with the cluster from the full sample (Supplemental Material L). The significant associations within the full sample and males mean that higher (worse) FI values were associated with greater FBC.
Table 6.
Demographic | Seed | Direction of connectivity | Cluster | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
x, y, z | Size | Size p-FDR | Peak p-unc | Anatomical area | % | Voxels | |||
Full (n = 100) | Right hippocampus | ↑ | − 48 + 2 − 44 | 291 | 0.029812 | 0.000107 | Inferior temporal gyrus, anterior division left | 44 | 127 |
Temporal pole left | 13 | 37 | |||||||
Middle temporal gyrus, anterior division left | 11 | 31 | |||||||
Not-labeled | 33 | 96 | |||||||
Male (n = 52) | Right hippocampus | ↑ | − 44 + 4 − 46 | 993 | 0.000021 | 0.000004 | Inferior temporal gyrus, anterior division left | 26 | 262 |
Inferior temporal gyrus, posterior division left | 18 | 174 | |||||||
Temporal pole left | 11 | 105 | |||||||
Middle temporal gyrus, anterior division left | 9 | 91 | |||||||
Middle temporal gyrus, posterior division left | 2 | 15 | |||||||
Temporal fusiform cortex, anterior division left | 0 | 2 | |||||||
Temporal fusiform cortex, posterior division left | 0 | 1 | |||||||
Not-labeled | 35 | 343 | |||||||
↑ | 66 − 28 − 26 | 451 | 0.00351 | < 0.000001 | Inferior temporal gyrus, posterior division right | 49 | 219 | ||
Middle temporal gyrus, posterior division right | 20 | 88 | |||||||
Inferior temporal gyrus, temporooccipital part right | 0 | 2 | |||||||
Not-labeled | 31 | 142 | |||||||
Male tertiles (n = 52) Intermediate > low and high |
Right hippocampus | ↑ | − 26 − 70 + 16 | 289 | 0.015491 | 0.000006 | Lateral occipital cortex, inferior division left | 6 | 18 |
Lateral occipital cortex, superior division left | 2 | 5 | |||||||
Not-labeled | 92 | 266 |
Bolded p-values indicate statistical significance
Only males demonstrated a significant between-tertile difference in connectivity between the right hippocampus and a cluster located in the lateral occipital cortex. Specifically, low demonstrated less connectivity than the medium tertile but more than the high tertile (Fig. 4/Table 6; cluster: size = 289 p-FDR < 0.05). Notably, the significant difference for low versus medium was at a more liberal (p < 0.05) voxel threshold. Anatomical labeling of all significant connections according to both CONN and xjView are available in Supplemental Material M.
Discussion
In this cross-sectional study of older adults with MCI, we examined the relationship between frailty status, assessed using the FI, and FBC throughout the brain. In support of our hypothesis, we found significant associations between FI scores and FBC in the full sample. Also supporting our hypothesis, we found significant sex-specific differences in the relationship between FI and FBC.
The ROI-ROI analysis included five networks (Default-Mode, Sensorimotor, Salience, Dorsal Attention, and Frontoparietal) with a total of 42 different connections. The main finding from the ROI-ROI analysis was that females possess greater connectivity within the Default-Mode network, regardless of frailty status and despite all participants being classified with an identical cognitive status (i.e., MCI). Notably, no ROI-ROI networks demonstrated a significant association between connectivity and FI values in the full sample or by sex. The S-V analysis used the right and left hippocampus as a seed, but only the right demonstrated connections significantly associated with FI values. When analyzing the full sample and males continuously, the right hippocampus increased connectivity to the left and bilateral regions of the inferior and middle temporal gyrus. Such results suggest that males are likely driving the significant connection to the left inferior/middle temporal gyrus in the full sample.
The connectivity coefficients that were significantly associated with FI demonstrated a positive correlation; this means that higher or worse FI values were associated with greater or increasing FBC. The significant connections appear between regions not typically linked or belonging to a single network. Admittedly, this is difficult to confirm given the broad (temporal lobe) anatomically labeling applied to the cluster as per the imaging programs utilized (i.e., CONN and xjView). We speculate that this connection may reflect compensation or an attempt by the plastic brain to maintain homeostasis by increasing connectivity via alternative circuits or regions not typically connected. Such adjustments reflect the “scaffolding” theory [62] and have been previously demonstrated with the Default-Mode [63]. Further support for this compensatory behavior is reflected in the lack of significant negative associations between FI scores and within-network connectivity. Such compensatory behavior, however, does not appear to exist in perpetuity according to our male tertile results.
The male low tertile possessed less connectivity than the medium tertile but more than the high tertile. Such findings conflict with the correlation observed in our continuous data and may suggest that individuals from the low and medium tertiles are driving the significant positive correlations between FI and FBC. More importantly, these findings indicate that as the individual becomes more frail or moves into the high tertile, the brain is incapable of maintaining the new compensatory connection. Such a trajectory may mean that the high tertile is at the greatest risk of progression to Alzheimer’s disease, despite receiving an identical cognitive classification (i.e., MCI) as the medium and low tertiles. Stated differently, if cognitive status does indeed reflect brain function, then we would expect our tertiles to have insignificant differences in FBC, but this is not the case, and increasing or worse frailty appears to play a role. Future research, inclusive of associations with behavioral or clinical implications (i.e., cognitive test scores), as well as a larger sample size with a more diverse frailty spectrum, should test such theories.
Our findings both support and refute previous research. Only two studies have examined the relationship between FBC and frailty status [27, 28]. Both used the Cardiovascular Health Study–Frailty Phenotype [29], only included cognitively normal older adults, and restricted their analysis to motor regions. Only Lammers and colleagues used functional magnetic resonance imaging and performed scans with eyes closed [28]. They found a significant negative relationship between frailty and the Supplementary Motor Area but not the pre-SMA [28]. Conversely, we found no significant associations between the Sensorimotor Network and FI nor any of our other pre-defined networks. The discrepancy between studies may reflect methodological differences as we utilized an FI that included variables from various domains, focused on clinically impaired older adults, and performed functional magnetic resonance imaging scans with eyes open; visual status is an essential consideration for neural function [64]. Network-specific associations to an identical health outcome are further supported by previous work examining the relationship between FBC and gait parameters [65]. Evidently, more research is needed on FBC and its relationship with frailty.
Our results suggest that sex is essential in understanding the association between frailty and FBC. Lammer and colleagues did not conduct a separate analysis by sex, but other researchers have demonstrated sex-specific differences in brain function [31] and brain blood flow [32]. Furthermore, sex-specific differences in frailty are well established as females become frail earlier and at any given age are more frail than their male counterparts but manage to live longer; such a phenomenon is known as “the male–female health survival paradox” [34, 66] and is an active area of research [67]. Females’ greater within-network Default-Mode connectivity and lack of what we identified as compensatory connectivity may reflect better brain function and further support females’ resilience or their ability to handle neurodegeneration. Sex hormones likely play a role, but a 2017 review highlighted two biological theories for the frailty-sex discrepancy: (1) males possess less physiological reserve so, at the same FI score, they are at a greater risk of mortality; and (2) females tend to accumulate less severe deficits or deficits associated with lower risk of mortality, highlighting an issue with collecting the number, but not the nature of the deficits [68]. Notably, sex differences may have convoluted the full sample and, thus, be responsible for the lack of more meaningful findings in the present study. Ultimately, more research is needed on the relationships between FBC, frailty, and sex.
In addition to the suggestions already put forth, future studies should consider frailty scores and sex when conducting FBC analyses in clinical groups as it may impact findings. Similar to research in frailty and exercise interventions [18, 19], future research should analyze frailty status using the Frailty Phenotype and FI to determine if they produce divergent findings. Furthermore, such analyses should be simultaneously conducted in cognitively healthy and impaired older adults to help elucidate how dementia-related neurodegeneration alters the FBC-frailty relationship. Along the dementia spectrum, researchers may even want to consider the stage or classification of MCI (i.e., amnestic vs. non-amnestic [69, 70], early vs. late [71], and single vs. multi-domain [72, 73]) as this is potentially another factor impacting findings. Researchers should not restrict their analysis to a single region as some networks are more susceptible to neurodegeneration, which subsequently affects within and between-network connectivity [74]. Moreover, the “neural context” hypothesis suggests that the functional relevance of a brain area depends on the status of other connected areas [75]. Therefore, alterations in one region do not necessarily have the same implications as alterations in another. Only by examining the entire connectome will we better understand global and local alterations and their potential downstream consequences on behavioral outcomes. Tracking the longitudinal relationship between FBC and frailty will provide the greatest insight into many of the suggestions offered above while also creating an opportunity for early intervention.
We conducted the first study to cross-sectionally analyze the relationship between FBC and frailty status using the FI in individuals with MCI, but it is not without limitations. We classified most but not all of our participants with amnestic MCI. Therefore, different sub-types may be convoluting our findings. As is typical with FBC, the present study merely reflects two analyses available to researchers. Our previous systematic reviews [76, 77] and other works [78] have demonstrated that researchers can take different methodological approaches to answer the same question. Our sample may be considered less frail than other work in frailty and brain health as our maximum value was the equivalent of another study’s upper tertile cut-point [21]; this is reflective of Neyman bias [79] or when more sick individuals are erroneously excluded. Similarly, our sex tertile results should be interpreted cautiously, given their small sample size (< 20). No cognitively healthy comparator makes drawing interpretations for biological or normal aging difficult. Finally, cross-sectional studies inherently create several limitations, including the inability to make causal inferences and the “snapshot” nature.
The present study examined the cross-sectional association between FBC throughout the brain, and frailty status, as per a FI. Individuals with worse frailty scores demonstrated increased connectivity of the right hippocampus to broadly labeled clusters within the temporal gyrus. We believe such changes reflect compensation by the brain to maintain homeostasis via an increase in between-network connectivity or regions that are not typically linked. Outcomes differed by sex as only males demonstrated significant correlations between frailty and FBC, but females showed greater within-network Default-Mode connectivity than males regardless of frailty status. Overall, our findings add to the growing literature on how frailty impacts males’ and females’ (brain function) differently and suggest why only some individuals may progress to a dementia syndrome.
Supplementary Information
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank Dr. Yanina Sarquis-Adamson and Nellie Kamkar for their help with this manuscript.
Abbreviations
- FBC
Functional brain connectivity
- FI
Frailty index
- MCI
Mild cognitive impairment
- ROI
Region of interest
- S-V
Seed-to-voxel
- SYN
SYNchronizing Exercises, Remedies in GaIt and Cognition
- T0
Pre-intervention
- T6
Post-intervention
- T12
Follow-up
Author contribution
Nick W. Bray, conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, investigation, project administration, visualization, roles/writing—original draft; Frederico Pieruccini-Faria, formal analysis, investigation, roles/writing—original draft; Suzanne Witt, formal analysis, investigation, roles/writing—original draft; Kenneth Rockwood, conceptualization, formal analysis, writing—review and editing; Robert Bartha, conceptualization, supervision, writing—review and editing; Timothy J. Doherty, conceptualization, supervision, writing—review and editing; Lindsay S. Nagamatsu, conceptualization, supervision, writing—review and editing; Quincy J. Almeida, conceptualization, writing—review and editing; Teresa Liu-Ambrose, conceptualization, writing—review and editing; Laura E. Middleton, conceptualization, writing—review and editing; Louis Bherer, conceptualization, writing—review and editing; Manuel Montero-Odasso, conceptualization, formal analysis, funding acquisition, supervision, writing—review and editing.
Funding
The SYNERGIC Trial is funded by Canadian Consortium on Neurodegeneration in Aging (CCNA Grant# “FRN” CNA 137794). The CCNA is supported by a grant from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research with funding from several partners. Dr Nick W. Bray was supported by a 2018–2021 Ontario Graduate Scholarship (OGS); the “Fellowship in Care of the Older Adult” from the St. Joseph’s Healthcare Foundation, Parkwood Institute, Division of Geriatric Medicine, University of Western Ontario; and the Canadian Consortium on Neurodegeneration in Aging (FRN CNA 137794). Teresa Liu-Ambrose is a Canada Research Chair (Tier I) in Healthy Aging. Dr. Montero-Odasso’s program in Gait and Brain Health is supported by grants from the Canadian Institute of Health Research (MOP 211220; PJT 153100), the Ontario Ministry of Research and Innovation (ER11–08–101), the Ontario Neurodegenerative Diseases Research Initiative (OBI 34739), the Canadian Consortium on Neurodegeneration in Aging (FRN CNA 137794), and Department of Medicine Program of Experimental Medicine Research Award (POEM 768915), University of Western Ontario. He is the first recipient of the Schulich Clinician-Scientist Award.
Declarations
Conflict of interest
The authors declare no competing interests.
Disclaimer
The sponsors had no role in the study design; in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; and in the decision to submit the article for publication.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Nick W. Bray, Email: nicholas.bray@ucalgary.ca
Frederico Pieruccini-Faria, Email: frederico.faria@sjhc.london.on.ca.
Suzanne T. Witt, Email: switt4@uwo.ca
Kenneth Rockwood, Email: kenneth.rockwood@dal.ca.
Robert Bartha, Email: rbartha@uwo.ca.
Timothy J. Doherty, Email: tim.doherty@lhsc.on.ca
Lindsay S. Nagamatsu, Email: lnagamat@uwo.ca
Quincy J. Almeida, Email: qalmeida@wlu.ca
Teresa Liu-Ambrose, Email: teresa.ambrose@ubc.ca.
Laura E. Middleton, Email: laura.middleton@uwaterloo.ca
Louis Bherer, Email: louis.bherer@umontreal.ca.
Manuel Montero-Odasso, Email: mmontero@uwo.ca.
References
- 1.Petersen RC, Lopez O, Armstrong MJ, Getchius TSD, Ganguli M, Gloss D, et al. Practice guideline update summary: mild cognitive impairment: report of the guideline development, dissemination, and implementation subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology. 2018;90(3):126–135. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000004826. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Ward A, Tardiff S, Dye C, Arrighi HM. Rate of conversion from prodromal Alzheimer’s disease to Alzheimer’s dementia: a systematic review of the literature. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord Extra. 2013;3(1):320–332. doi: 10.1159/000354370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Mitchell AJ, Shiri-Feshki M. Rate of progression of mild cognitive impairment to dementia–meta-analysis of 41 robust inception cohort studies. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2009;119(4):252–265. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2008.01326.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Canevelli M, Arisi I, Bacigalupo I, Arighi A, Galimberti D, Vanacore N, et al. Biomarkers and phenotypic expression in Alzheimer’s disease: exploring the contribution of frailty in the Alzheimer’s disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Geroscience. 2021;43(2):1039–1051. doi: 10.1007/s11357-020-00293-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.2020 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2020;16(3):391–460. 10.1002/alz.12068. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 6.Kojima G, Liljas A, Iliffe S, Walters K. Prevalence of frailty in mild to moderate Alzheimer’s disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2017;14(12):1256–1263. doi: 10.2174/1567205014666170417104236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Wallace LMK, Theou O, Godin J, Andrew MK, Bennett DA, Rockwood K. Investigation of frailty as a moderator of the relationship between neuropathology and dementia in Alzheimer’s disease: a cross-sectional analysis of data from the Rush Memory and Aging Project. Lancet Neurol. 2019;18(2):177–184. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(18)30371-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Ward DD, Ranson JM, Wallace LMK, Llewellyn DJ, Rockwood K. Frailty, lifestyle, genetics and dementia risk. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2022;93(4):343–350. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2021-327396. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Canevelli M, Bruno G, Valletta M, Cesari M. Editorial: Could there Be Frailty in the discrepancy between lesions and symptoms of Alzheimer’s disease? J Frailty Aging. 2022;11(3):248–249. doi: 10.14283/jfa.2022.43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Clegg A, Young J, Iliffe S, Rikkert MO, Rockwood K. Frailty in elderly people. Lancet. 2013;381(9868):752–762. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)62167-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Hewitt J, Carter B, Vilches-Moraga A, Quinn TJ, Braude P, Verduri A, et al. The effect of frailty on survival in patients with COVID-19 (COPE): a multicentre, European, observational cohort study. Lancet Public Health. 2020;5(8):e444–e451. doi: 10.1016/S2468-2667(20)30146-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Fried LP, Ferrucci L, Darer J, Williamson JD, Anderson G. Untangling the concepts of disability, frailty, and comorbidity: implications for improved targeting and care. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2004;59(3):M255–M263. doi: 10.1093/gerona/59.3.M255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Langlois F, Vu TTM, Kergoat M-J, Chassé K, Dupuis G, Bherer L. The multiple dimensions of frailty: physical capacity, cognition, and quality of life. Int Psychogeriatr. 2012;24(9):1429–1436. doi: 10.1017/S1041610212000634. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Montero-Odasso MM, Barnes B, Speechley M, Muir Hunter SW, Doherty TJ, Duque G, et al. Disentangling cognitive-frailty: results from the gait and brain study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2016;71(11):1476–1482. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glw044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Dent E, Morley JE, Cruz-Jentoft AJ, Woodhouse L, Rodríguez-Mañas L, Fried LP, et al. Physical frailty: ICFSR international clinical practice guidelines for identification and management. J Nutr Health Aging. 2019;23(9):771–787. doi: 10.1007/s12603-019-1273-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Gobbens RJJ, van Assen MALM, Luijkx KG, Wijnen-Sponselee MT, Schols JMGA. Determinants of frailty. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2010;11(5):356–364. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2009.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Howlett SE, Rutenberg AD, Rockwood K. The degree of frailty as a translational measure of health in aging. Nature Aging. 2021;1(8):651–665. doi: 10.1038/s43587-021-00099-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Bray NW, Jones GJ, Rush KL, Jones CA, Jakobi JM. Practical implications for strength and conditioning of older pre-frail females. J Frailty Aging. 2020;9(2):118–121. doi: 10.14283/jfa.2020.15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Bray NW, Jones GJ, Rush KL, Jones CA, Jakobi JM. Multi-component exercise with high-intensity, free-weight, functional resistance training in pre-frail females: a quasi-experimental, pilot study. J Frailty Aging. 2020;9(2):111–117. doi: 10.14283/jfa.2020.13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Rogers NT, Steptoe A, Cadar D. Frailty is an independent predictor of incident dementia: Evidence from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Sci Rep. 2017;7(1):15746. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-16104-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Wallace L, Hunter S, Theou O, Fleming J, Rockwood K, Brayne C. Frailty and neuropathology in relation to dementia status: the Cambridge City over-75s cohort study. Int Psychogeriatr. 2021:1–9. 10.1017/S1041610220003932. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 22.López-Sanz D, Suárez-Méndez I, Bernabé R, Pasquín N, Rodríguez-Mañas L, Maestú F, et al. Scoping review of neuroimaging studies investigating frailty and frailty components. Front Med. 2018;5:284. doi: 10.3389/fmed.2018.00284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Tian Q, Williams OA, Landman BA, Resnick SM, Ferrucci L. Microstructural neuroimaging of frailty in cognitively normal older adults. Front Med. 2020;7:546344. doi: 10.3389/fmed.2020.546344. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Damoiseaux JS, Beckmann CF, Arigita EJS, Barkhof F, Scheltens P, Stam CJ, et al. Reduced resting-state brain activity in the “default network” in normal aging. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18(8):1856–1864. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Bressler SL, Menon V. Large-scale brain networks in cognition: emerging methods and principles. Trends Cogn Sci. 2010;14(6):277–290. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Sheline YI, Raichle ME. Resting state functional connectivity in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiat. 2013;74(5):340–347. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.11.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Suárez-Méndez I, Doval S, Walter S, Pasquín N, Bernabé R, Gallo EC, et al. Functional connectivity disruption in frail older adults without global cognitive deficits. Front Med. 2020;7:322. doi: 10.3389/fmed.2020.00322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Lammers F, Zacharias N, Borchers F, Mörgeli R, Spies CDCD, Winterer G. Functional connectivity of the supplementary motor network is associated with Fried’s modified frailty score in the elderly. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2020;75(12):2239–2248. doi: 10.1093/GERONA/GLZ297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Fried LP, Tangen CM, Walston J, Newman AB, Hirsch C, Gottdiener J, et al. Frailty in older adults: evidence for a phenotype. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2001;56(3):M146–M157. doi: 10.1093/gerona/56.3.M146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Mitnitski AB, Mogilner AJ, Rockwood K. Accumulation of deficits as a proxy measure of aging. Sci World J. 2001;1:323–336. doi: 10.1100/tsw.2001.58. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Ritchie SJ, Cox SR, Shen X, Lombardo MV, Reus LM, Alloza C, et al. Sex differences in the adult human brain: evidence from 5216 UK Biobank Participants. Cereb Cortex. 2018;28(8):2959–2975. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Alisch JSR, Khattar N, Kim RW, Cortina LE, Ac R, Qian W, et al. Sex and age-related differences in cerebral blood flow investigated using pseudo-continuous arterial spin labeling magnetic resonance imaging. Aging. 2021;13(4):4911-4925. 10.18632/AGING.202673. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 33.Petersen RC, Roberts RO, Knopman DS, Geda YE, Cha RH, Pankratz VS, et al. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment is higher in men. The Mayo Clinic Study of Aging. Neurology. 2010;75(10):889–97. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181f11d85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Collard RM, Boter H, Schoevers RA, Oude Voshaar RC. Prevalence of frailty in community-dwelling older persons: a systematic review. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2012;60(8):1487–1492. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2012.04054.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Montero-Odasso M, Almeida QJ, Burhan AM, Camicioli R, Doyon J, Fraser S, et al. SYNERGIC TRIAL (SYNchronizing Exercises, Remedies in Gait and Cognition) a multi-centre randomized controlled double blind trial to improve gait and cognition in mild cognitive impairment. BMC Geriatrics. 2018;18(1):93. doi: 10.1186/s12877-018-0782-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Albert MS, DeKosky ST, Dickson D, Dubois B, Feldman HH, Fox NC, et al. The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(3):270–279. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Warburton DER, Bredin SSD, Jamnik VK, Gledhill N. Validation of the PAR-Q+ and ePARmed-X+ Health Fit J Can. 2011;4(2):38–46. doi: 10.14288/hfjc.v4i2.151. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Rockwood K, Mitnitski A. Frailty in relation to the accumulation of deficits. J Gerontol - Ser A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2007;62(7):722–727. doi: 10.1093/gerona/62.7.722. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Widagdo IS, Pratt N, Russell M, Roughead EE. Construct validity of four frailty measures in an older Australian population: a Rasch analysis. J Frailty Aging. 2016;5(2):78–81. doi: 10.14283/jfa.2016.83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Kimber D, Kehler D, Lytwyn J, Boreskie K, Jung P, Alexander B, et al. Pre-operative frailty status is associated with cardiac rehabilitation completion: a retrospective cohort study. J Clin Med. 2018;7(12):560. doi: 10.3390/jcm7120560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Kehler DS, Giacomantonio N, Firth W, Blanchard CM, Rockwood K, Theou O. Association between cardiac rehabilitation and frailty. Can J Cardiol. 2020;36(4):482–489. doi: 10.1016/j.cjca.2019.08.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Searle SD, Mitnitski A, Gahbauer EA, Gill TM, Rockwood K. A standard procedure for creating a frailty index. BMC Geriatrics. 2008;8:24. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-8-24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Duchesne S, Chouinard I, Potvin O, Fonov VS, Khademi A, Bartha R, et al. The Canadian dementia imaging protocol: harmonizing national cohorts. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2019;49(2):456–465. doi: 10.1002/jmri.26197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Gorgolewski KJ, Auer T, Calhoun VD, Craddock RC, Das S, Duff EP, et al. The brain imaging data structure, a format for organizing and describing outputs of neuroimaging experiments. Sci Data. 2016;3(1):160044. doi: 10.1038/sdata.2016.44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Esteban O, Markiewicz CJ, Goncalves M, DuPre E, Kent JD, Salo T, et al. fMRIPrep: a robust preprocessing pipeline for functional MRI. Nat Methods. 2020. 10.5281/ZENODO.4055773. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 46.Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TEJ, Johansen-Berg H, et al. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage. 2004;23:S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2004.07.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Smith SM. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002;17(3):143–155. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Nieto-Castanon A. Conn: A functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connect. 2012;2(3):125–141. doi: 10.1089/brain.2012.0073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Flodin P, Jonasson LS, Riklund K, Nyberg L, Boraxbekk CJ. Does aerobic exercise influence intrinsic brain activity? An aerobic exercise intervention among healthy old adults. Front Aging Neurosci. 2017;9:267. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00267. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Li R, Zhu X, Yin S, Niu Y, Zheng Z, Huang X, et al. Multimodal intervention in older adults improves resting-state functional connectivity between the medial prefrontal cortex and medial temporal lobeâ€. Front Aging Neurosci. 2014;6:39. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2001;98(2):676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Fox MD, Corbetta M, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Raichle ME. Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006;103(26):10046–10051. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0604187103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, Kenna H, et al. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J Neurosci. 2007;27(9):2349–2356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Shi L, Sun J, Xia Y, Ren Z, Chen Q, Wei D, et al. Large-scale brain network connectivity underlying creativity in resting-state and task fMRI: cooperation between default network and frontal-parietal network. Biol Psychol. 2018;135:102–111. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Londei A, D’Ausilio A, Basso D, Sestieri C, Gratta CD, Romani G-L, et al. Sensory-motor brain network connectivity for speech comprehension. Hum Brain Mapp. 2010;31(4):567–580. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20888. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Lancaster JL, Martinez MJ. Mango – short for Multi-image Analysis GUI. Date accessed: 03 April 2021. Available from: http://rii.uthscsa.edu/mango/index.html.
- 57.Cui X, Li J, Song X, Ma Z. xjView. Date accessed: 03 April 2021. Available from: https://www.alivelearn.net/xjview/.
- 58.Nieto-Castanon A. Handbook of functional connectivity magnetic resonance imaging methods in CONN. Hilbert Press; 2020.
- 59.Esteban O, Birman D, Schaer M, Koyejo OO, Poldrack RA, Gorgolewski KJ. MRIQC: Advancing the automatic prediction of image quality in MRI from unseen sites. PLOS ONE. 2017;12(9):e0184661-e. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0184661. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Gorgolewski KJ, Alfaro-Almagro F, Auer T, Bellec P, Capotă M, Chakravarty MM, et al. BIDS apps: improving ease of use, accessibility, and reproducibility of neuroimaging data analysis methods. PLoS Comput Biol. 2017;13(3):e1005209-e. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Worsley KJ, Marrett S, Neelin P, Vandal AC, Friston KJ, Evans AC. A unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of cerebral activation. Hum Brain Mapp. 1996;4(1):58–73. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1996)4:1<58::AID-HBM4>3.0.CO;2-O . [DOI] [PubMed]
- 62.Park DC, Reuter-Lorenz P. The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009;60(1). 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093656. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 63.Grady CL, Springer MV, Hongwanishkul D, McIntosh AR, Winocur G. Age-related changes in brain activity across the adult lifespan. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006;18(2):227–241. doi: 10.1162/089892906775783705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Weng Y, Liu X, Hu H, H H, S Z, Q C, et al. Open eyes and closed eyes elicit different temporal properties of brain functional networks. NeuroImage. 2020;222. 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2020.117230. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 65.Lo O-Y, Halko MA, Zhou J, Harrison R, Lipsitz LA, Manor B. Gait speed and gait variability are associated with different functional brain networks. Front Aging Neurosci. 2017;9:390. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00390. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Hubbard RE, Rockwood K. Frailty in older women. Maturitas. 2011;69(3):203–207. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2011.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Stephan A-J, Schwettmann L, Meisinger C, Ladwig K-H, Linkohr B, Thorand B, et al. Living longer but less healthy: the female disadvantage in health expectancy. Results from the KORA-Age study. Exp Gerontol. 2021;145:111196. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2020.111196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Gordon EH, Peel NM, Samanta M, Theou O, Howlett SE, Hubbard RE. Sex differences in frailty: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Exp Gerontol. 2017;89:30–40. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2016.12.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Au B, Dale-McGrath S, Tierney MC. Sex differences in the prevalence and incidence of mild cognitive impairment: a meta-analysis. Ageing Res Rev. 2017;35:176–199. doi: 10.1016/J.ARR.2016.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Welstead M, Luciano M, Muniz-Terrera G, Taylor AM, Russ TC. Prevalence of Mild Cognitive Impairment in the Lothian Birth Cohort 1936. medRxiv. 2020:2020.10.08.20209130–2020.10.08. 10.1101/2020.10.08.20209130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 71.Aisen PS, Petersen RC, Donohue MC, Gamst A, Raman R, Thomas RG, et al. Clinical core of the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative: progress and plans. Alzheimer’s Dementia. 2010;6(3):239–246. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2010.03.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Petersen RC, Doody R, Kurz A, Mohs RC, Morris JC, Rabins PV, et al. Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol. 2001;58(12):1985–1992. doi: 10.1001/archneur.58.12.1985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med. 2004;256(3):183–194. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Palmqvist S, Schöll M, Strandberg O, Mattsson N, Stomrud E, Zetterberg H, et al. Earliest accumulation of β-amyloid occurs within the default-mode network and concurrently affects brain connectivity. Nat Commun. 2017;8(1):1214. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-01150-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.McIntosh AR. Contexts and catalysts: a resolution of the localization and integration of function in the brain. Neuroinformatics. 2004;2(2):175–182. doi: 10.1385/NI:2:2:175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Bray NW, Pieruccini-Faria F, Bartha R, Doherty TJ, Nagamatsu LS, Montero-Odasso M. The effect of physical exercise on functional brain network connectivity in older adults with and without cognitive impairment. A systematic review. Mech Ageing Dev. 2021;196:111493. doi: 10.1016/j.mad.2021.111493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Titus J, Bray NW, Kamkar N, Camicioli R, Nagamatsu LS, Speechley M, et al. The role of physical exercise in modulating peripheral inflammatory and neurotrophic biomarkers in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Mech Ageing Dev. 2021;194:111431. doi: 10.1016/J.MAD.2021.111431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Botvinik-Nezer R, Holzmeister F, Camerer CF, Dreber A, Huber J, Johannesson M, et al. Variability in the analysis of a single neuroimaging dataset by many teams. Nature. 2020;582(7810):84–88. doi: 10.1038/s41586-020-2314-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Levin KA. Study design III: Cross-sectional studies. Evid Based Dent. 2006;7(1):24–25. doi: 10.1038/sj.ebd.6400375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data supporting this study’s findings are available from the corresponding authors upon reasonable request.