Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2022 Dec 21;17(12):e0279366. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0279366

What Should I do and Who’s to blame? A cross-national study on youth’s attitudes and beliefs in times of COVID-19

Elisabeth L De Moor 1, Ting-Yu Cheng 1, Jenna E Spitzer 2, Christian Berger 3, Alexia Carrizales 4, Claire F Garandeau 5, Maria Gerbino 6, Skyler T Hawk 7, Goda Kaniušonytė 8, Asiye Kumru 9, Elisabeth Malonda 10, Anna Rovella 11, Yuh-Ling Shen 12, Laura K Taylor 13, Maarten van Zalk 14, Susan Branje 1, Gustavo Carlo 15, Laura Padilla Walker 16, Jolien Van der Graaff 1,*
Editor: Helena R Slobodskaya17
PMCID: PMC9770422  PMID: 36542632

Abstract

The COVID-19 crisis has had a major impact on youth. This study examined factors associated with youth’s attitudes towards their government’s response to the pandemic and their blaming of individuals from certain risk groups, ethnic backgrounds, and countries or regions. In a sample of 5,682 young adults (Mage = 22) from 14 countries, lower perceived burden due to COVID-19, more collectivistic and less individualistic values, and more empathy were associated with more positive attitudes towards the government and less blaming of individuals of certain groups. Youth’s social identification with others in the pandemic mediated these associations in the same direction, apart from the COVID-19 burden on attitudes, which had a positive indirect effect. No evidence of country-level moderation was found.

Introduction

The COVID-19 crisis has had a major impact on the lives of youth across the globe. Although their physical health risks from the virus were relatively low, young adults experienced disproportionally high social and financial costs due to the restrictions imposed by governments to curb the spread of the virus [1], as well as costs for their mental health [2].

Yet, despite these expressions of negative attitudes towards governments and others, collective crises like the COVID-19 pandemic are also known to increase positive attitudes [3]. Individuals can experience collective identity based on their membership in certain groups [4, 5], and emergencies sometimes create new groups to identify with; that is, all others who are affected by the same adversity [36]. These feelings of shared social identity among those affected may result in positive and supportive attitudes towards other individuals and the government on the one hand. On the other hand, they may result in an increased sense of “we versus them” [4, 5], and as such increase the tendency to blame others for experienced adversities. Personal experiences during a crisis (i.e., the perceived burden), as well as personal characteristics (i.e., value orientation and empathy) are thought to play a role in individuals’ tendency to identify with others [711], and as such may link directly to youth’s attitudes towards the government and other citizens during the pandemic, but also indirectly via social identification.

In the present study, we examined among young adults from 14 countries 1) their attitudes towards their government’s approach to handling the pandemic and 2) the extent to which they blamed individuals from certain risk groups, ethnic backgrounds, and countries or regions for their negative experiences during the pandemic, which reflect their attitudes towards the institutions and individuals in a society and may also predict their behavior. COVID-19-related and more general individual factors were examined to explain individual differences in youth’s attitudes. In particular, we studied whether youth’s perceived burden from the pandemic, their collectivistic and individualistic values, and their empathic tendencies predicted their attitudes. We further investigated whether youth’s social identification with others during the pandemic could (partly) explain these associations. Finally, even though previous research has shown links between person characteristics and youth’s prosocial attitudes to be consistent across cultures (e.g. [12]), it is important to take differences in country-level contextual factors into account. For instance, the number of COVID-19 cases varied greatly across countries at the time of this study (e.g., very low in Taiwan and high in Chile), and the approach that governments took in response to the pandemic also varied. As a result, citizens experienced many more restrictions in one country (e.g., Argentina) than they did in another (e.g., Finland). Therefore, we explored differences across countries in the strength of predictors based on cumulative COVID-19 cases, individualistic orientation of the country, and the strictness of the national COVID-19 approach at the time of assessment.

Perceived burden as a consequence of the COVID-19 pandemic

Individual differences between youth in the extent to which they perceived a burden due to the COVID-19 pandemic may help understand differences in their attitudes and beliefs. Individuals living through the pandemic may have faced a myriad of changes in their social, medical, and financial situation [1], either as a direct consequence of the virus (e.g., becoming sick yourself or seeing others get sick), or as a consequence of the restrictions put in place to regulate the spread of the virus (e.g., closing of businesses resulting in lay-offs, social distancing resulting in less social contact). These restrictions may have been especially impactful for this age group, as young adults are more likely to live alone than mid- and late-adults and thus to become isolated [13], and they tend to be in a less secure job situation [14, 15], which may have contributed to a sense of distress [15].

The personal experience of the COVID-19 pandemic may have affected youth’s attitudes towards the government and others in the society, as they may have shifted blame to the government and to other citizens to alleviate their own stress and relieve responsibility (Murray, [unpublished]). In particular, those individuals who perceived the burden to be high may have had more negative attitudes towards the government approach, given the role of the government in putting in place restrictions that were part of the burden that they experienced or that were ineffective at decreasing it. For instance, individuals from the UK who felt that the satisfaction of their need for connectedness was thwarted by the restrictive measures, and thus perceived a greater burden, reported lower trust in their government [16]. Also, youth who perceived a high burden may also have been more likely to blame certain groups for their negative experiences [17]. Experiencing stress may deplete individuals’ cognitive and emotional resources that are necessary to attend to another’s situation and to respond in a prosocial way (see [18]). Indeed, individuals who had contracted COVID-19 and who, as a result, experienced a lot of distress were found to blame others for their non-supportiveness during their suffering, such as doctors, neighbors, and the community at large [19]. Thus, we expected youth who perceived a higher COVID-19-related burden to be less positive about the government’s approach and to be more likely to blame certain other groups for their distress.

Collectivistic and individualistic values

Youth’s collectivistic and individualistic values may also relate to their attitudes towards the government and other groups in society. Although the individualism-collectivism spectrum has traditionally been used to classify and contrast countries [20], it is important to note that within countries, individuals also differ in the extent to which they hold individualistic (i.e., distinctiveness from others, high self-reliance, and self-promotion) and collectivistic (i.e., similarity to others, interdependence, community-promotion) values. Individuals who hold more individualistic values are less supportive of government interventions on national welfare, state ownership, and market competition [21]. In contrast, individuals with more collectivistic values tend to feel more responsibility towards, and greater identification with, their community [2224]. They also tend to show greater compliance with, and adherence to, social norms set forth by that community on average [25]. In the context of the COVID-19 pandemic, individuals with more collectivistic values have indeed been found to be more inclined to comply with the cautionary measures [26, 27]. Therefore, youth who held more collectivistic and less individualistic values can be expected to have had more positive attitudes towards the government approach to combat the pandemic.

Regarding attitudes toward other groups, however, having more collectivistic and less individualistic values may be related with more blaming of certain groups that are not considered part of one’s community [28]. Having higher collectivistic values has indeed been associated with a stronger differentiation between in- and outgroup [29]. In line with this finding, during the COVID-19 pandemic youth with collectivistic values tended to be more afraid and worried than youth with less collectivistic values of being infected by others, such as foreigners (outgroup), or something unknown, like a novel virus [27]. Youth with higher levels of collectivistic values and less individualistic values might thus have more negative attitudes towards–and even blame–groups in the society that they see as responsible for the spread of COVID-19 or for the restrictive measures.

Empathy

Youth’s empathy may also be associated with their attitudes towards the government and towards others during the COVID-19 pandemic. Although definitions of empathy vary, empathy can be viewed as a person’s responses to other’s experience, including both cognitive processes like understanding other’s perspective and emotional processes like feeling concern for others [30]. As highly empathic individuals are generally sensitive to the needs of others and are understanding of others’ situation, they tend to show prosocial attitudes and behaviors [31]. Higher empathy has indeed been related to a wide range of prosocial tendencies (e.g., [32, 33]).

Indeed, in the context of the pandemic, levels of empathy may explain differences in youth’s attitudes and blaming. Regarding attitudes towards the government approach, more empathic individuals have been found to be more positive towards government intervention [34] and to hold more social and liberal political views in general [35]. Thus, youth with higher levels of empathy may have also held more positive views regarding the national approach to combatting COVID-19. With regard to blaming of certain groups, higher levels of empathy have also been associated, developmentally and experimentally, to lower prejudice and lower bias towards outgroups (e.g., [3639]). During the COVID-19 pandemic, youth who were more empathic may therefore have had a lower tendency to blame certain groups.

The mediating role of social identification

Youth’s social identification with others who were affected by the COVID-19 pandemic may in part mediate the above-mentioned pathways. That is, youth who identified with other individuals who were affected may have shown greater understanding for restrictions imposed by the government and have been less likely to blame others. This is in line with work showing that greater identification with the community predicted more understanding and prosociality during previous crises (e.g., [3, 40]) and during the COVID-19 pandemic (i.e., providing support and adherence to regulations; [41]). Individuals who feel more connected to others and, as such, experience a greater sense of “we-ness” or identification with their ingroup may more highly value the benefits to be reaped by society.

Both collectivism and empathy have been related to social identification [810], with collectivistic and empathic individuals generally having a stronger sense of social identification. Furthermore, in line with the notion of “altruism born of suffering” (e.g., [42, 43]), we may expect that youth who experienced a greater COVID-19-related burden identified more with others that were affected by the pandemic [7, 11]. Thus, we expected that youth who had higher levels of empathy, collectivism and COVID-19-related burden, and lower levels of individualism experienced greater social identification with those affected by the COVID-19 pandemic and, in turn, exhibited more positive attitudes towards the government approach and less blaming of certain groups.

The current study

Following the idea that the COVID-19 crisis may have impacted youth’s relations with others and their social identities, the present study investigated whether perceived COVID-19 burden, collectivistic and individualistic values, and empathy could help us understand individual differences in youth’s attitudes and beliefs during the COVID-19 pandemic. We examined social identification as a mediator and explored moderation at the country level to uncover potential cross-country differences in these associations. Specifically, we examined moderation by the cumulative COVID-19 cases per 100,000 inhabitants, the strictness of the national approach as factors of COVID-19-related country differences, and the national-level individualistic orientation as a more general country difference factor, as the association between individual-level factors may depend on both the specific COVID-19 situation and general values in a country (see Fig 1 for a complete conceptual overview of the tested associations).

Fig 1. Conceptual model of the studied relations.

Fig 1

We expected that youth who reported a greater perceived burden due to COVID-19 would report less positive attitudes towards the government approach and would be more likely to blame others for their distress. Youth who held more collectivistic and less individualistic values were expected to report more positive attitudes towards the government approach and more blaming of groups they perceive as an outgroup. We also expected youth with greater empathy to express more positive attitudes towards the government approach and less blaming of certain groups. Furthermore, we hypothesized indirect effects via social identification with others during the COVID-19 pandemic in the same direction for collectivistic and individualistic values and empathy. For perceived burden, we expected a higher burden to be associated with higher social identification and thus indirectly associated with more positive attitudes and less blaming of certain groups. Finally, we did not form any specific hypotheses regarding country-level moderation. The present study’s research questions, hypotheses, methods, and analyses were pre-registered at https://osf.io/et2nb.

Materials and methods

Participants and procedures

The faculty ethics review board of the Faculty of Social and Behavioral Sciences of Utrecht University approved the study (20–298). Informed consent was obtained in written form from all participants. Participants were 5,682 young adults aged 18–25 years (67.1% female; Mage = 21.51 years, SDage = 2.13) from 14 countries: Argentina (n = 253), Chile (n = 146), China (n = 286), Finland (n = 465), France (n = 506), Germany (n = 375), Ireland (n = 495), Italy (n = 502), Lithuania (n = 346), Spain (n = 179), Taiwan (n = 169), the Netherlands (n = 472), Turkey (n = 372), and the United States (n = 1,116). Participants were mostly of medium to high socioeconomic status (SES), with 76.1% reporting some form of postsecondary education (23.8% reported secondary education, 1 participant reported primary education) and 38.0% indicating to live comfortably on the present household income (44.9% coping on present income, 13.8% having some difficulties with the present income, 3.3% having many difficulties on the present income). These youth participated in #Young in Times of COVID-19, a study evaluating prosocial behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic among youth. Participants were primarily recruited via the networks of faculty collaborators in each country. However, participants from the Netherlands and the United States were also recruited using survey panel methods. Respondents received no or minimal (no more than 5 euros) compensation. Participants completed an online survey throughout the summer and fall of 2020 in the primary language of their county. For several of the questionnaires included, versions in the primary languages were already available; questionnaires for which no primary language version existed were translated by collaborators from each of these countries. The survey took approximately 20 minutes to complete. Only participants who completed the survey and passed the validation checks (e.g., “If you are reading this, select ‘a lot’”) were included in the final dataset. See S1 Table for participants’ demographic information by country.

Measures

Outcome variables

Attitude towards government approach. Participants reported on their attitudes towards their country’s government approach to COVID-19 using a 3-item scale developed for this research. Participants expressed the extent to which they agreed that the government in their country “takes effective measures to combat COVID-19”, “protects all citizens against the negative effects of COVID-19”, and “explains its decisions about the approach to combat COVID-19”. Responses were rated on a Likert scale of 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) with higher scores reflecting a more positive attitude towards their government approach. Importantly, these items were not included in the Chinese questionnaire. The scale exhibited good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = .84).

Blaming of certain groups for COVID-19. Participants reported on their negative outgroup beliefs using 4 items that were created for the current study. Respondents answered the following question, “To what extent do you blame the following groups for the negative consequences you experience from the COVID-19 pandemic?” for the following four groups: “Citizens from certain ethnic groups”, “Citizens who do not follow the recommendations to prevent the spread of the virus”, “Citizens who belong to so-called risk groups (e.g., elderly, people with pre-existing health conditions)”, and “Citizens of certain countries or regions”. Responses were rated on a Likert scale from 1 (do not blame them at all) to 4 (blame them completely), with higher values reflecting greater blame. The complete scale exhibited questionable internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = .57). Therefore, and following our pre-registered plan, we removed one item, which pertained to the second item described above. This led to an acceptable internal consistency of Cronbach’s α = .69. The three-item scale was used in further analyses. To test for robustness, we also ran our analyses using the original four-item scale. Those findings did not meaningfully differ from the findings reported in the manuscript. After excluding the worst-fitting blaming item, however, the effect of the perceived burden on blaming was somewhat weaker and the effect of empathy on blaming somewhat stronger, although both effects were significant in the analyses with and without the worst-fitting item.

Predictors

Perceived COVID-19 burden. Participants reported on the extent to which they were burdened by the COVID-19 pandemic using 9 items composed specifically for this study (see S2 Table). The scale covered several aspects of burden, including health (2 items; e.g., “being or become infected with or having symptoms of COVID-19”), social situation (4 items; e.g., “being restricted in visiting friends or family”), and finance (3 items; e.g., “being at an increased risk for lower financial means”). Participants rated the items on a Likert scale of 1 (not at all burdensome) to 5 (very burdensome), with higher scores reflecting greater perceived burden. The scale exhibited good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = .76).

Collectivistic and individualistic values. Orientations towards collectivism and individualism were measured using two subscales (with 8 items each) from the validated Culture Orientation Scale [44]. Respondents rated the extent to which various statements about culture orientation reflected their values using a Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree), with higher scores reflecting greater individualism or greater collectivism. Sample items include, “If a coworker gets a prize, I would feel proud” (collectivistic values), and “I often do ‘my own thing’” (individualistic values). The scale exhibited good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = .70 for individualistic values, .72 for collectivistic values).

Empathy. Participants reported on two empathy-related constructs, namely, empathetic concern and perspective taking, using 10 items adapted from the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI; [30]). Responses were based on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (does not describe me at all) to 5 (describes me greatly). Sample items include, “I would describe myself as a pretty soft-hearted person” and, “I sometimes find it difficult to see things from the other person’s point of view”. Items were reverse coded when necessary, such that higher scores indicated more empathy. Scores from both subscales were averaged to create one empathy score. The empathy scale exhibited good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = .75), and the IRI has procured ample evidence of psychometric validity and reliability (see [45] for an overview).

Social identification with others during COVID-19. Participants reported on their social identification with others during the COVID-19 pandemic using 4 items adapted from Doosje et al. [46]. On a Likert scale of 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree), participants responded to items such as “During the COVID-19 crisis… I identified with the other people affected”. Higher scores reflect greater social identification, and the scale exhibited good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = .78).

Country-level moderators

Cumulative cases of COVID-19 per 100,000 inhabitants. The cumulative number of COVID-19 cases, collected from the World Health Organization’s official Coronavirus Dashboard [47], was used to characterize the extent of the outbreak in each country at the end of data collection. For Taiwan, data were collected from the Taiwan Centers for Disease Control official website [48]. To make meaningful cross-national comparisons, we computed the number of cumulative cases per 100,000 inhabitants, based on the population numbers of the United Nations Population Division estimates [49].

Strictness of COVID-19 approach. We used the Stringency Index of the Oxford COVID-19 Government Response Tracker to characterize the level of action each government undertook in response to the pandemic [50]. This measure assigns a level of strictness on a scale from 0 to 100 based on aggregated indicators of government responses, such as school closures and travel restrictions, plotted against the coronavirus cases and deaths in each country. We computed the strictness of each government’s response by averaging the daily strictness ratings for each country from the start to the end of data collection in each country.

National level individualistic orientation. We assessed individualistic values at the national level using the Hofstede’s Index [51]. The individualism scale reflects the extent to which each society prioritizes the pursuit of individual goals over collective goals. Higher ratings indicate a stronger emphasis on individualistic values.

Statistical plan

Prior to registering our study, a power analysis was conducted to estimate the required sample size to detect effects in the mediation model and the extended moderated mediation model. With our complete sample of 5,682 participants, a power of .80, and significance at α = .05, effects of f2 = .002 (very small effect size) could be detected for the mediation model. In the country-level multigroup models which had subgroup sizes of at least 1,116 participants (for the “high” group of national-level individualistic orientation), effects of at least f2 = .011 (small effect size) could be detected.

Direct effects and mediation models

To test our research questions, we tested a series of models in Mplus [52]. The analysis scripts and anonymized data are made available on the project OSF page: https://osf.io/738z5/. We first estimated a model with four predictors (i.e., perceived burden, collectivistic values, individualistic values, and empathy) and two outcomes (i.e., attitude towards government approach and blaming of certain groups). This model was estimated for the participants from all countries together. Then, we added social identification as a mediator of all predictive pathways from independent to dependent variables to the model. An alpha of .05 was used to determine significance of effects. Effect size was evaluated using rules of thumb by Funder & Ozer [53] and the R2 statistic.

Country-level moderation models

Next, to examine moderation by country-level variables, we divided the sample into groups of low, medium, and high on each of the country-level factors. As preregistered, this was done because the number of countries was too small to test a multilevel mediation model, for which a minimum number of 50 clusters is suggested when used in a Structural Equation Framework with Maximum Likelihood (ML) estimation [54, 55]. For cumulative cases per 100,000 inhabitants and for the strictness of the COVID-19 approach, we used percentile scores to divide the countries into three categories because there were, to the best of our knowledge, no informative grounds at the time to base this classification on in the broader literature. We separated the countries into three categories of “low” (1st to 33th percentile), “medium” (34th to 66th percentile), and “high” (67th to 100th percentile). For national level individualistic orientation, we used the cutoffs described for this Hofstede dimension on the Hofstede Insights website [51, 56]. These cutoffs describe “low” (below 50), “high” (above 60), and “very high” values (above 80). For the present study, we adapted this scale to cover all possible values, such that “low” was 0–50, “medium” was 51–80, and “high” was 81–100.

Then, to examine moderation by these country-level variables, we estimated a multigroup model for each of the country-level factors. The three scores on the country-level variables (i.e., low, medium, and high) were used to divide the different countries into three groups. We then tested a model for each country-level variable in which the predictive paths were constrained to be equal across groups. If model fit was appropriate, we released groups of predictive parameters in steps, to see whether allowing the predictive paths to vary between groups would significantly improve the fit to the data. Originally, we planned to test two models per country-level variable: one with the predictive pathways constrained and one fully unconstrained, and to compare their model fit. However, the unconstrained model was fully saturated and therefore no model fit statistics were available to be compared with the constrained model. Instead parameters were released per outcome variable, so that we tested three partially unconstrained models: one with unconstrained pathways towards attitudes towards the government approach, one with unconstrained pathways towards blaming of certain groups, and one with unconstrained pathways towards social identification. If one of the partially unconstrained models fitted significantly better than the constrained one, this was taken as evidence for country-level moderation. If this was the case, we further examined for this model on which predictive pathways the differences existed by releasing the constraints of the individual paths one at a time. Model fit of CFA ≥ .900 and RMSEA of ≤ .080 was deemed acceptable [57, 58]. Change of ΔCFI ≥ .005, supplemented by ΔRMSEA ≥ -.010 was seen as indicative of significant change in model fit [59], indicating that the (partially) unconstrained model fit better than the constrained model.

Results

Descriptive statistics

Descriptive statistics of, and correlations between, the study variables are provided in Table 1, respectively (see S1 Table for a breakdown of the descriptive statistics per country). On average, participants scored above the middle of the scale on all variables, except blaming of certain groups, which was reported comparatively little. Furthermore, most correlations were significant, with effects ranging from small to medium in size. Most of the correlations were in the expected direction, although notably, higher social identification was associated with more blaming of certain groups. Moreover, there were no significant correlations between having individualistic values and social identification, and between individualistic values and the attitude towards the government approach. Moreover, having collectivistic values was not significantly associated with the blaming of certain groups.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of and correlations between the study variables.

1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7.
1. COVID-19 burden
2. Individualistic values .14*
3. Collectivistic values .25* .09*
4. Empathy .20* -.07* .40*
5. Social identification .20* .02 .36* .29*
6. Attitude towards government approach -.05* -.01 .11* .08* .19*
7. Blaming of certain groups .13* .21* -.02 -.20* .04* .01
Mean 3.16 4.54 5.18 3.83 3.18 3.43 1.37
SD 0.79 0.87 0.82 0.57 0.92 1.06 0.55

Main results

Direct effects model

We first tested a main effects model (Table 2), which was fully saturated and thus yielded no interpretable model fit statistics (i.e., CFI = 1.000, RMSEA = .000). Perceiving a greater burden due to COVID-19 was related to a less positive attitude, whereas having more collectivistic values and being more empathic was related to a more positive attitude towards the government approach. Having more individualistic values was not significantly related to youth’s attitudes. Perceiving a greater COVID-19 burden and having stronger individualistic values were associated with more blaming of certain groups, whereas higher empathy was related to less blaming. Having collectivistic values was not associated with youth’s blaming of certain groups. All significant regression coefficients fell in the range of relatively small effects (around β = .10; [53]). Total explained variance in the dependent variables was also limited (Table 2).

Table 2. Direct effects regression model with attitude towards government approach and blaming of certain groups regressed on COVID-19 burden, individualistic values, collectivistic values, and empathy.
b SE(β) β p
Predictors of attitude towards government approach
COVID-19 burden -0.12 .01 -0.09 < .001
Individualistic values -0.01 .01 -0.01 .587
Collectivistic values 0.14 .02 0.11 < .001
Empathy 0.10 .02 0.05 < .001
Predictors of blaming of certain groups
COVID-19 burden 0.11 .01 0.15 < .001
Individualistic values 0.11 .01 0.17 < .001
Collectivistic values 0.01 .01 0.02 .288
Empathy -0.21 .01 -0.22 < .001
Covariances
Attitude towards government approach × Blaming of certain groups 0.02 .01 0.03 .011

Note. R2 for attitude towards government approach = 2.0%

R2 for blaming of certain groups = 9.8%

Indirect effects model

Next, we included social identification in the model as a mediating variable (Table 3). Again, the resulting model was fully saturated, and did not allow us to gauge model fit. The direct effects of COVID-19 burden, individualistic values, collectivistic values, and empathy did not differ meaningfully from those in the direct effects model. In addition, social identification was also a significant predictor of a more positive attitude towards the government approach and, surprisingly, of more blaming of certain groups. Moreover, youth’s perceived COVID-19 burden, collectivistic values, and empathy were significantly related to more social identification. Individualistic values were not significantly associated with social identification. As for the direct effects model, most effect sizes were relatively small.

Table 3. Mediation model with attitude towards government approach and blaming of certain groups regressed on COVID-19 burden, individualistic values, collectivistic values, and empathy, mediated by social identification.
Direct effects Indirect effects
b SE(β) β p b SE(β) β p
Predictors of attitude towards government approach
COVID-19 burden -0.14 .01 -0.11 < .001 0.03 < .01 0.02 < .001
Individualistic values < -0.01 .01 < -0.01 .970 < -0.01 < .01 < -0.01 .575
Collectivistic values 0.08 .02 0.06 < .001 0.07 .01 0.05 < .001
Empathy 0.04 .02 0.02 .142 0.05 < .01 0.03 < .001
Social identification 0.21 .01 0.19 < .001
Predictors of blaming of certain groups
COVID-19 burden 0.10 .01 0.14 < .001 0.01 < .01 0.01 < .001
Individualistic values 0.11 .01 0.17 < .001 < 0.01 < .01 < -0.01 .576
Collectivistic values < 0.01 .02 -0.01 .731 0.01 < .01 0.02 < .001
Empathy -0.22 .01 -0.23 < .001 0.01 < .01 0.01 < .001
Social identification 0.04 .01 0.07 < .001
Predictors of social identification
COVID-19 burden 0.12 .01 0.10 < .001
Individualistic values -0.01 .01 -0.01 .574
Collectivistic values 0.31 .01 0.27 < .001
Empathy 0.25 .01 0.16 < .001
Covariances
Attitude towards government approach × Blaming of certain groups 0.01 .01 0.03 .071

Note. R2 for attitude towards government approach = 4.9%, R2 for blaming of certain groups = 16.5%, R2 for social identification = 10.2%

Regarding indirect effects, we found a positive indirect effect of empathy on attitude towards the government approach via social identification, which was in line with the direct effect. We also found a positive indirect for perceived burden, whereas the direct effect was negative. This indicates that in addition to perceived COVID-19 burden being directly associated with a less positive attitude towards the government approach, higher perceived burden was also associated with more social identification, which in turn was associated with a more positive attitude. Youth’s perceived COVID-19 burden, their collectivistic values, and their empathy were also indirectly related to blaming of certain groups via social identification. The indirect effect of COVID-19 burden was in the same (positive) direction as the direct effects, but the indirect effect of empathy was not. Specifically, although youth’s empathy was directly related to lower blaming, indirectly it was related to more blaming due to the positive association of social identification with blaming of certain groups. For collectivistic values, there was no direct association with blaming of certain groups, but there was a positive indirect association via social identification. All significant indirect effects were very small in size (around β = .05; [53]), and total explained variance of the dependent variables was limited (Table 3).

Country-level moderation models

To examine moderation by country-level variables, we fitted multigroup models using each of the country-level variables as a grouping variable to group participants of the 14 countries into a low, medium, or high group. We first fitted the model using cumulative COVID-19 cases as a grouping variable, constraining all predictive paths including indirect paths to be equal across groups. This model fit the data well (CFI = .932, RMSEA = .051; see S3 Table for the predictive pathway parameters of this model). To examine whether a partially unconstrained model would fit the data better, we then tested three subsequent models: one in which all direct paths on youth’s attitudes towards the national approach were allowed to vary between groups, one in which all direct paths on blaming of certain groups was allowed to vary, and one in which all direct paths on social identification were allowed to vary. All of the partially unconstrained models fit the data well (CFI = .959, RMSEA = .050; CFI = .961, RMSEA = .049; CFI = .946, RMSEA = .055, respectively), but did not significantly improve model fit.

Then, we examined the strictness of the COVID-19 approach as a potential moderator by fitting a multigroup model in which all predictive pathways were constrained to be equal across strictness groups. This model fit the data acceptably (CFI = .932, RMSEA = .052; see S4 Table for the predictive parameters). We once more tested three partially unconstrained models, with predictive pathways released for attitudes towards national approach, blaming of certain groups, and social identification. All of these models fit the data well (CFI = .947, RMSEA = .057; CFI = .964, RMSEA = .047; CFI = .951, RMSEA = .052, respectively), but none significantly improved the fit of the constrained model.

Finally, we explored national level individualistic orientation as a moderator, again splitting our sample into three groups of low, middle, and high, and comparing these groups in a multigroup model. The constrained model fit the data well (CFI = .943, RMSEA = .045; see S5 Table for the predictive parameters). Models with all prospective pathways unconstrained on attitudes towards the government approach, blaming of certain groups, and social identification also fit the data well (CFI = .969, RMSEA = .041; CFI = .965, RMSEA = .044; CFI = .951, RMSEA = .049, respectively), but did not significantly improve model fit. Thus, we concluded that country-level differences in the cumulative COVID-19 cases, the strictness of the national approach, and the national level individualistic orientation did not significantly moderate the examined associations.

Discussion

The COVID-19 pandemic and the restrictions put in place to combat it have had a great impact on young adults’ daily lives, and may have impacted the ways in which youth relate to others in society [6]. The present study aimed to help us explain differences in youth’s attitudes towards their national government’s approach to combatting the virus and their tendency to blame individuals in risk groups or from certain ethnic backgrounds, countries, or regions. Our findings show that youth’s perceived burden due to COVID-19, collectivistic and individualistic values, and empathy indeed played a role. The extent to which youth identified with other people during the pandemic mediated these associations, which was consistent across different countries.

Perceived burden as a consequence of the COVID-19 pandemic

In line with our expectations, youth who perceived a greater burden had, on average, more negative attitudes towards the national approach to combatting the spread of the virus. This result is in support of the notion that experiencing stress depletes individuals’ cognitive and emotional resources that are necessary to attend to others’ needs [18]. As such, youth who perceived high burden during the COVID-19 pandemic may have been less able to attend to the consequences of the pandemic for other. As a result, they may have been less supportive of the government’s approach to handle these collective consequences, in particular when the costs for themselves were high. Another possibility is that they reported a more negative attitude towards the government’s approach because they shifted blame for their burden to the government in an attempt to alleviate COVID-19 related distress (Murray, [unpublished]). Our result is in line with previous research that also showed that higher perceived burden was related to more negative attitudes toward the government [16].

Further, in support of our hypothesis, youth who experienced a higher burden were also more likely to blame certain groups for the origin or spread of the disease and for the restrictions. Similar processes as for the link with negative attitudes towards the government have likely played a role here. That is, blaming others may have been used by youth as a way of coping with their own distress (Murray, [unpublished]), and the depletion of cognitive and emotional resources due to the burden they experienced may have diminished their tendency to see things from others’ point of view [18], which may have been a ground for negative attitudes towards others. It further supports previous research also showing an association between burden as a consequence of COVID-19 and individuals’ negative attitudes towards others (e.g., [19]).

Regarding the indirect effects via social identification, perceived burden was also indirectly related to more blaming of certain groups. However, in contrast to the direct effect, perceiving a greater burden as a consequence of the COVID-19 pandemic was indirectly associated with more positive attitudes towards the government approach via social identification. This finding is in line with the notion of “altruism born of suffering” [11, 43] and with previous empirical research on the role of stress in prosocial attitudes [7, 42, 60, 61]. This work has showed that experiencing stress can increase one’s identification with others who experienced the same stress, and this may facilitate one’s motivation to help relieving others’ stress. Our results indeed suggest that, if youth’s higher perceived burden went together with higher levels of identification with others during the COVID-19 pandemic, this facilitated their understanding and supportiveness of the measures that governments had to take to diminish the consequences of the pandemic for all.

Thus, perceived burden may play both a positive and negative role in youth’s attitudes, depending on whether it is paired with a greater sense of identification with the people around them. It is also possible that the way burden is related to one’s attitudes depends on the type of burden. For instance, experiencing a burden to one’s health (e.g., getting ill) may be tied to greater social identification, whereas experiencing a social burden (e.g., not being able to visit friends) may be tied to less social identification. In the future, it would be interesting to examine the differential effects of these different types of burden on attitudes and behavior. For policy makers, it may be important to keep in mind the negative effect experiencing a burden may have on youth’s attitudes towards the government and other members of society, and on their behavior during a pandemic more generally.

Collectivistic and individualistic values

In line with our hypotheses and earlier work [26, 27], having collectivistic values was directly and indirectly related to more positive attitudes towards the national approach. Thus, youth who held more collectivistic values were more likely to see the benefits of their government’s approach to combatting the virus. However, and contrary to our expectations, individualistic values were not related to these attitudes. Although having individualistic values is generally associated with less support for government intervention [21], it is possible that the same does not hold for COVID-19 related interventions. That is, although youth with individualistic values may still not like their government to impose strict rules on them, they may feel personally responsible to contribute to combating this global disease [62], and as such be more supportive of a government approach that is in line with that goal. For future research, it would be helpful to include information on youth’s sense of personal responsibility and their opinions regarding what would be the best approach towards handling the COVID-19 pandemic.

In contrast to our hypothesis, our findings showed that holding collectivistic values was not directly related to blaming of certain groups. However, holding stronger individualistic values was directly related to more blaming. This might be explained by individuals with individualistic values placing more value on personal responsibility, both for themselves and for others [62]. As such, they may view others as personally responsible for contributing to the pandemic. For instance, they may have considered vulnerable groups responsible for the unwanted government regulations [21]. That holding collectivistic values was not directly related to blaming might be due to the specific groups that were seen by youth as ingroups or outgroups and those that were included in the blaming measure. Whereas some of the items of the measures referred to outgroups (e.g., people from other countries), other items referred to groups that may have been considered ingroups (e.g., people from risk groups). The COVID-19 pandemic may have actually increased identification with these groups [6]. As stronger collectivistic values have been associated with a stronger differentiation between in- and outgroup [29], this may explain how having higher levels of collectivistic values was indirectly related via social identification to more blaming of certain groups. Thus, our findings suggest that creating a sense of “we-ness” rather than a focus on individualistic needs may be important during a global pandemic to foster positive attitudes towards the national approach to combat the spreading of the disease and to limit negative views towards other members of society, but also caution that greater identification with one group may come at the risk of increasing negative views towards other groups. Thus, policy makers during a pandemic should carefully consider the sense of social identity that they promote, and who is and who is not included in this identity.

Empathy

As expected, higher levels of empathy were directly and indirectly related to more positive attitudes towards the government approach to combatting the disease and directly to less blaming of certain groups. This suggests that empathy promoted more positive attitudes towards the government and others in society during the COVID-19 pandemic. This is in line with the notion that individuals high in empathy are sensitive to the needs of others and are understanding of others’ situations, and as such they are more likely to engage in prosocial attitudes [31]. Moreover, it corroborates findings showing that empathy is associated with a more positive view of government intervention [34] and lower prejudice towards outgroups [3639, 63, 64]. Our finding that empathy was positively linked to social identification (which in turn was related to more positive attitudes towards the government) is also in accordance with previous work showing that individuals high in empathy have a higher tendency to identify with others (e.g., [65]). These findings suggest that fostering empathy could potentially be an important pathway towards more prosocial attitudes and behaviors during a global pandemic.

The role of social identification

In addition to the direct and indirect paths described above, social identification was also positively associated with youth’s attitudes towards the government approach and blaming of certain groups. Youth with higher levels of social identification reported more positive attitudes towards the government approach but, surprisingly, also more blaming. This might be the case because the individuals youth identified with (e.g., “other people affected”) may not overlap fully with the groups one holds responsible for the origin/spread of the disease or the restrictions put in place to combat the disease. Thus, identifying more with individuals may make them feel more strongly for their suffering and, as a result, make them more likely to blame others who they see as responsible for that suffering. Again, in the future it would be helpful to get a better sense of who belongs to young adults’ ingroup and outgroup. Regarding mediation effects on blaming of other groups, we found indirect effects for the perceived burden, collectivistic values, and empathy which were opposed to the direct effects and against our expectations. This is probably due to the positive direct association of social identification with blaming described above.

Country-level moderation

Our exploration of the role of country-level moderators revealed no significant results, suggesting that the studied associations did not differ between countries depending on the cumulative number of COVID-19 cases, the strictness of the national approach, or the national level individualistic orientation. The associations thus seem to be robust across the various countries, which is in line with previous research revealing consistent results on the predictors of prosocial tendencies across different cultures (e.g., [66, 67]). That country-level factors did not moderate the associations in our study can also be explained by the fact that our participants were all in the same developmental stage (i.e., young adulthood) and many of them were college students. According to Jensen’s cultural-developmental approach [68], during young adulthood the similarities in individuals’ attitudes due to the shared developmental phase, in which all youth strive for increasing independence, are larger than the differences due to variations across cultures and countries. Moreover, due to the COVID-19 pandemic, the challenges that individuals faced were highly similar, which may also have increased similarity across cultures.

Alternatively, although there were no country-level differences depending on the cumulative number of COVID-19 cases, the strictness of the national approach, or the national level of individualistic orientation, it is possible that other factors at the country-level may indicate differences in youth’s attitudes towards their government’s approach to combatting the spread of the disease and to their blaming of individuals from certain risk groups, ethnic groups, and countries or regions. For instance, the present study focused on individual and country-level differences in collectivistic and individualistic values, but other cultural dimensions may also play a role (e.g., [20, 56]). As a case in point, differences in the uncertainty avoidance dimension (i.e., discomfort with and resistance to unfamiliar phenomena; [56]) have been associated at the country level with spread of the disease and with greater mortality risk [69, 70]. In addition to health outcomes, higher uncertainty avoidance has been related at the country-level to less gathering of people in public places [71] and more consumer stockpiling [72], and at an individual level to greater beliefs in COVID-19 conspiracy theories [73]. Thus, it may be interesting to examine individual- and country-level values also in relation to youth’s attitudes towards their government’s approach to combatting the spread of the disease and to their blaming of risk groups and individuals from certain ethnic groups and countries or regions.

Limitations

The present study had some limitations. First, it should be noted that our sample was likely somewhat biased towards high-SES, highly educated individuals due to recruitment having occurred mostly via the researchers’ personal networks. Second, the data were cross-sectional. Whereas the present data helped us understand differences between youth in their attitudes towards the government approach and blaming of certain groups, they do not allow us to predict these differences. What is more, we cannot be sure that the directionality examined in this study reflects reality, and it could be that instead youth’s attitudes and blaming are predictors of perceived burden, values, and empathy, or that all examined variables are associated in a non-directional way (e.g., by all being explained by a third variable). Thus, the present study reflects only a first explorative step, and more follow-up research is needed in a representative longitudinal sample.

Third, individuals’ responses are influenced by the environments they are in–a consideration especially relevant for the present study, as we examined context-specific experiences and attitudes. The data collection for the study ran for several months in the summer and fall of 2020, and the COVID-19 situation changed across that period, as well as before and after it. As a result, the number of cases and the strictness of the national approach was not the same when different individuals filled in the questionnaire. In the future, it would be useful to include a measure of the COVID-19 situation at the exact moment that each person participated in the data collection, as a better proxy of the environment in which young adults were embedded.

Finally, for all effects, even those highly significant, it is important to note that effect sizes were small [53], and that explained variance in the outcome variables was limited. Other, perhaps more situational factors such as the strictness of the government approach at each moment may have been at play that contribute to understanding individual differences in youth’s attitudes towards the government approach and blaming of certain groups. It is likely that these fluctuating situational and other contextual factors may have played a larger role in a situation that is highly non-normative than stable, more distant factors such as personal values and traits.

Conclusion

The present study examined individual differences in youth’s attitudes towards their government and other social groups during times of COVID-19. We found evidence that a lower perceived burden due to COVID-19, higher collectivistic and lower individualistic values, and a higher level of empathy were each related to more positive attitudes towards the government approach and less blaming of certain groups, and that these associations were invariant across countries. Moreover, burden was indirectly related to less positive attitudes via more social identification. Burden, collectivistic values, and empathy were indirectly related to more blaming. The findings demonstrate support for social identity theory, which posits that identification with others may be a mechanism for individual attitudes and behavior. The findings provide a foundation for further research that can examine the specific groups that young adults identify with, and the specific groups that they might blame in future pandemics and when they experience pandemic-related government policies.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Descriptive statistics and demographics per country.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Newly designed perceived COVID-19 burden measure used in the present study.

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Constrained multigroup mediation model with attitude towards government approach and blaming of certain groups regressed on COVID-19 burden, individualistic values, collectivistic values, and empathy, mediated by social identification; grouping on cumulative COVID-19 cases per 100,000 inhabitants.

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Constrained multigroup mediation model with attitude towards government approach and blaming of certain groups regressed on COVID-19 burden, individualistic values, collectivistic values, and empathy, mediated by social identification; grouping on strictness of the national COVID-19 approach.

(DOCX)

S5 Table. Constrained multigroup mediation model with attitude towards government approach and blaming of certain groups regressed on COVID-19 burden, individualistic values, collectivistic values, and empathy, mediated by social identification; grouping on national level individualistic values.

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We want to thank all international collaborators for their help in collecting the data. We additionally thank all participants who contributed their unique experiences during the COVID-19 pandemic to this project.

Data Availability

The anonymized data is made available on the project OSF page: https://osf.io/738z5/. If researchers want to use the data, they are asked to submit a data request to the project executive team (Jolien Van der Graaff, Laura Padilla-Walker, Gustavo Carlo, Susan Branje). If a project proposes to use data from a single country, approval must be obtained from the lead PIs from that country. If the project requires data from multiple countries, the proposed research needs to be approved by the lead PIs in those countries, as well as the project executive team. A full list of project contributors can be found at: https://youngincoronatimes.sites.uu.nl/international-collaborators/.

Funding Statement

JvdG received a grant from the Faculty of Social Sciences of Utrecht University to fund data collection (no grant number available). URL: https://www.uu.nl/en/organisation/faculty-of-social-and-behavioural-sciences ELdM and SB were supported by a grant from the European Research Council (ERC-CoG INTRANSITION-773023). URL: https://erc.europa.eu/ The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Keyworth C., Epton T., Byrne-Davis L., Leather J. Z., & Armitage C. J. (2021). What challenges do UK adults face when adhering to COVID-19-related instructions? Cross-sectional survey in a representative sample. Preventive Medicine, 147, 106458. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2021.106458 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Vacchiano M., (2022). How the first COVID-19 lockdown worsened younger generations’ mental health: Insights from network theory. Sociological Research Online. 10.1177/13607804221084723 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Drury J., Brown R., González R., & Miranda D. (2016). Emergent social identity and observing social support predict social support provided by survivors in a disaster: Solidarity in the 2010 Chile earthquake. European Journal of Social Psychology, 46, 209–223. 10.1002/ejsp.2146 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Taifel H. (1978). Differentiation between social groups: Studies in the social psychology of intergroup relations. London, UK: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Taijfel H., & Turner J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In Austin W. G. & Worchel S. (Eds.), The Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations (pp. 33–47). Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Drury J., Carter H., Ntontis E., & Guven S. Public behaviour in response to the COVID-19 pandemic: Understanding the role of group processes (2021). BJPsych Open, 7, e11. 10.1192/bjo.2020.139 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Davis A. N., Luce H., & Davalos N. (2018). Associations among life events, empathic concern, and adolescents’ prosocial and aggressive behaviors toward specific targets. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 179, 183–197. doi: 10.1080/00221325.2018.1468314 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Deaux K., & Reid A. (2000). Contemplating collectivism. In Stryker S., Owens T. J., & White R. W. (Eds.), Self, identity, and social movements (Vol. 13, pp. 172–190). Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Gallese V. (2009) Mirror neurons, embodied simulation, and the neural basis of social identification, Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 19, 519–536. 10.1080/10481880903231910 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Hamer K., McFarland S., & Penczek M. (2019). What lies beneath? Predictors of identification with all humanity. Personality and Individual Differences, 141, 258–267. 10.1016/j.paid.2018.12.019 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Staub E., & Vollhardt J. (2008). Altruism born of suffering: The roots of caring and helping after victimization and other trauma. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 7, 267–280. doi: 10.1037/a0014223 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Carlo G., Streit C., & Crockett L. (2018). Generalizability of a traditional social cognitive model of prosocial behaviors to U.S. Latino/a youth. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 24, 596–604. doi: 10.1037/cdp0000188 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Luchetti M., Lee J. H., Aschwanden D., Sesker A., Strickhouser J. E., Terracciano A., et al. (2020). The trajectory of loneliness in response to COVID-19. American Psychologist, 75, 897–908. doi: 10.1037/amp0000690 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Liu C. H., Zhang E., Wong G. T. F., & Hyun S. (2020). Factors associated with depression, anxiety, and PTSD symptomatology during the COVID-19 pandemic: Clinical implications for US young adult mental health. Psychiatry Research, 290, 113–172. 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113172 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Shanahan L., Steinhoff A., Bechtiger L., Murray A.L., Nivette A., Hepp U., et al. (2020). Emotional distress in young adults during the COVID-19 pandemic: Evidence of risk and resilience from a longitudinal cohort study. Psychological Medicine. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1017/S003329172000241X [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lamarche V. M. (2021). Socially connected and COVID-19 prepared: The influence of sociorelational safety on perceived importance of COVID-19 precautions and trust in government responses. Social Psychological Bulletin, 15, 1–15. 10.32872/spb.4409 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Barreneche S. M. (2020). Somebody to blame: On the construction of the other in the context of the COVID-19 outbreak. Society Register, 4, 19–32. 10.14746/sr.2020.4.2.02 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Lazarus R. S., & Folkman S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. Springer publishing company. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Guo Q., Zheng Y., Shi J., Wang J., Li G., Li C., et al. (2020). Immediate psychological distress in quarantined patients with COVID-19 and its association with peripheral inflammation: A mixed-method study. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 88, 17–27. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2020.05.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Hofstede G. (1984). Culture’s consequences: International differences in work-related values. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Pitlik H., & Rode M. (2017). Individualistic values, institutional trust, and interventionist attitudes. Journal of Institutional Economics, 13, 575–598. https://doi-org.proxy.library.uu.nl/10.1017/S1744137416000564 [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Bao G., Zhang Z., & Chen J. (2015). The mediation of in-group identification between collectivism and knowledge sharing. Innovation: Organization and Management, 17, 341–363. 10.1080/14479338.2015.1054605 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Chen C. C., Peng M. W., & Saparito P. A. (2002). Individualism, collectivism, and opportunism: A cultural perspective on transaction cost economics. Journal of Management, 28, 567–583. 10.1177/014920630202800405 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Triandis H. C. (2001). Individualism-collectivism and personality. Journal of Personality, 69, 907–924. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.696169 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Kim U., Triandis H. C., Kâğitçibaşi Ç., Choi S.-C., & Yoon G. (1994). Cross-cultural research and methodology series, Vol. 18. Individualism and collectivism: Theory, method, and applications. Michigan, MI: Sage Publications, Inc. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Biddlestone M., Green R., & Douglas K. M. (2020). Cultural orientation, power, belief in conspiracy theories, and intentions to reduce the spread of COVID-19. British Journal of Social Psychology, 59, 663–673. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12397 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Germani A., Buratta L., Delvecchio E., & Mazzeschi C. (2020). Emerging adults and COVID-19: The role of individualism-collectivism on perceived risks and psychological maladjustment. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17, 3497. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17103497 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Han G., Park B. (1995). Children’s choice in conflict: Application of the theory of individualism-collectivism. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 26, 298–313. 10.1177/0022022195263005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Triandis H. C. (1991). Cross-cultural differences in assertiveness/competition vs. group loyalty/cooperation. In Hinde R. A. & Groebel J. (Eds.), Cooperation and prosocial behavior (pp. 78–88). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Davis M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 113–126. 10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Eisenberg N., Eggum-Wilkens N. D., & Spinrad T. L. (2015). The development of prosocial behavior. In Schroeder D. A. & Graziano W. G. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of prosocial behavior (pp. 114–136). Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.FeldmanHall O., Dalgleish T., Evans D., & Mobbs D. (2015). Empathic concern drives costly altruism. NeuroImage, 105, 347–356. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.10.043 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Skoe E. E. A. (2010). The relationship between empathy‐related constructs and care‐based moral development in young adulthood. Journal of Moral Education, 39, 191–211. 10.1080/03057241003754930 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Wagaman M. A., & Segal E. A. (2014). The relationship between empathy and attitudes toward government intervention. Journal of Sociology and Social Welfare, 41, 91–122. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Franke V. C., & Elliot C. N. (2021). Optimism and social resilience: Social isolation, meaninglessness, trust, and empathy in times of COVID-19. Societies, 11, 35. 10.3390/soc11020035 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Batson C. D., Chang J., Orr R., & Rowland J. (2002). Empathy, attitudes, and action: Can feeling for a member of a stigmatized group motivate one to help the group? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 105–118. doi: 10.1177/014616702237647 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Bäckström M., & Björklund F. (2002). Structural modeling of generalized prejudice. Journal of Individual Differences, 28, 10–17. 10.1027/1614-0001.28.1.10 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Miklikowska M. (2018). Empathy trumps prejudice: The longitudinal relation between empathy and anti-immigrant attitudes in adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 54, 703–717. doi: 10.1037/dev0000474 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Vanman E. J. (2016). The role of empathy in intergroup relations. Current Opinion in Psychology: Intergroup Relations, 11, 59–63. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.06.007 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Alnabulsi H., Drury J., & Templeton A. (2018). Predicting collective behaviour at the Hajj: Place, space and the process of cooperation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 373, 20170240. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0240 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Stevenson C., Wakefield J., Drury J., & Felsner I. (2020). Collectively Coping with Coronavirus: Local Community Identification Predicts Giving Support and Lockdown Adherence During the COVID-19 Pandemic. Unpublished manuscript. 10.31219/osf.io/2uyw7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Qin Y., Zhou Y., Fan F., Chen S., Huang R., Cai R., et al. (2016). Developmental trajectories and predictors of prosocial behavior among adolescents exposed to the 2008 Wenchuan earthquake. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 29, 80–87. doi: 10.1002/jts.22064 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Vollhardt J. R. (2009). Altruism born of suffering and prosocial behavior following adverse life events: A review and conceptualization. Social Justice Research, 22, 53–97. 10.1007/s11211-009-0088-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Triandis H. C. & Gelfland M. J. (1998). Converging measurement of horizontal and vertical individualism and collectivism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 118–128. 10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.118 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Keaton S. A. (2017). Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI). In (Eds. Worthington D. L. & Bodie G. D.), The Sourcebook of Listening Research. 10.1002/9781119102991.ch34 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Doosje B., Branscombe N. R., Spears R., & Manstead A. S. (1998). Guilty by association: When one’s group has a negative history. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 872–886. 10.1037/0022-3514.75.4.872 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.World Health Organization. (2020, October 23). WHO COVID-19 Global Data [Data set]. Retrieved from https://covid19.who.int/table [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Taiwan Centers for Disease Control (2020, October 23). COVID-19 data. Retrieved from https://www.cdc.gov.tw/En [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Worldometer (2021, April 20). Countries in the World by Population. Retrieved from https://www.worldometers.info/world-population/population-by-country/ [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Hale T., Noam A., Cameron-Blake E., Hallas L., Kira B., Majumdar S., et al. (2020). Oxford COVID-19 Government Response Tracker [Data set]. Blavatnik School of Government. Retrieved from https://covidtracker.bsg.ox.ac.uk/about-api [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Hofstede G., Hofstede G., & Minkov M. (2010). Cultures and organizations: Software of the mind (3rd ed.). McGraw-Hill. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Muthén L. K., & Muthén B. O. (1998–2017). Mplus User’s Guide. Eighth Edition. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Funder D. C., & Ozer D. J. (2019). Evaluating effect size in psychological research: sense and nonsense. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 2, 156–168. doi: 10.1177/2515245919847202 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Hox J. J., Moerbeek M., Kluytmans A., & Van de Schoot R. (2014). Analyzing indirect effects in cluster randomized trials. The effect of estimation method, number of groups and group sizes on accuracy and power. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 78. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00078 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Meuleman B., & Billiet J. (2009). A Monte Carlo sample size study : How many countries are needed for accurate multilevel SEM? Survey Research Methods, 3, 45–58. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Hofstede G. (2011). Dimensionalizing cultures: The Hofstede model in context. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture, 2, 2307–0919. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Hu L., & Bentler P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Hooper D., Coughlan J., & Mullen M. R. (2008). Structural equation modelling: Guidelines for determining model fit. The Electronic Journal of Business Methods, 6, 53–60. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Chen F. F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indices to lack of measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 14, 464–504. doi: 10.1080/1705510701301834 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Taylor L. K., Merrilees C. E., Goeke-Morey M. C., Shirlow P., Cairns E., & Cummings E. M. (2014). Political violence and adolescent outgroup attitudes and prosocial behaviors: Implications for positive intergroup relations. Social Development, 23, 840–859. doi: 10.1111/sode.12074 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Taylor L. K., & Hanna J. R. (2018). Altruism born of suffering among emerging adults in Northern Ireland. Journal of Aggression, Conflict and Peace Research, 10, 157–169. 10.1108/JACPR-01-2017-0271 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Realo A., Koido K., Ceulemans E., & Allik J. (2002). Three components of individualism. European Journal of Personality, 16, 163–184. 10.1002/per.437 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Taylor L. K., O’Driscoll D., Dautel J., & McKeown S. (2020). Empathy to action: Child and adolescent outgroup attitudes and prosocial behaviors in a setting of intergroup conflict. Social Development, 29, 461–477. 10.1111/sode.12421 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Taylor L. K., & McKeown S. (2021). Adolescent outgroup helping, collective action, and political activism in a setting of protracted conflict. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 85, 37–46. 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2021.09.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Deng X. (2021). Identification with all humanity and willingness to help people in COVID-19 affected countries: Testing a moderated mediation model. Personality and Individual Differences, 181, 111012. 10.1016/j.paid.2021.111012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Barrett D. W., Wosinka W., Butner J., Petrova P., Gornik-Durose M., & Cialdini R. B. (2004). Individual differences in the motivation to comply across cultures: the impact of social obligation. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 19–31. 10.1016/j.paid.2003.08.024 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Knight T. (2015). A Psychometric Examination of Prosocial Behavior Across Cultural Contexts (Doctoral dissertation, Duquesne University). Retrieved from https://dsc.duq.edu/etd/759 [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Jensen L. A. (2008). Through two lenses: A cultural-developmental approach to moral psychology. Developmental Review, 28, 289–315. 10.1016/j.dr.2007.11.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Erman A., & Medeiros M. (2021). Exploring the effect of collective cultural attributes on Covid-19-related public health outcomes. Frontiers in Psychology, 12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.627669 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Voegel J., & Wachsman Y. (2021). The effect of culture in containing a pandemic: The case of COVID-19. Journal of Risk Research. Advanced online publication. 10.1080/13669877.2021.1986566 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Huynh T. L. D. (2020). Does culture matter social distancing under the COVID-19 pandemic? Safety Science, 130, 104872. doi: 10.1016/j.ssci.2020.104872 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Ahmadi I., Habel J., Jia M., Lee N., & Wei S. (2022). Consumer stockpiling across countries during the COVID-19 pandemic. Journal of International Marketing, 30, 28–37. 10.1177/1069031X211037590 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Alper S., Bayrak F., & Yilmaz O. (2021). Psychological correlates of COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs and preventive measures: Evidence from Turkey. Current Psychology, 40, 5708–5717. doi: 10.1007/s12144-020-00903-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Helena R Slobodskaya

13 Jul 2022

PONE-D-22-10911What should I do and who's to blame? A cross-national study on youth's attitudes and beliefs in times of COVID-19PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. De Moor,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

As you can see, the reviews are in general favourable. However, both reviewers suggest some changes to your manuscript. After considering the reviews and reading the paper myself, I offer a number of points to address in a potential revision, as well.

1. Please explain how the scales were translated into the language of the participants’ country? It is also unclear whether the measures of culture orientation, empathy and social identification have been validated for use in the 14 study countries.

2. Although individualism/collectivism is perhaps the most commonly applied construct in explaining cultural effects, other dimensions of culture identified by Hofstede, e.g. power distance, masculinity-femininity uncertainty avoidance, long- vs. short-term orientation and indulgence-restraint, may also be relevant. It would be useful if the authors acknowledge the need for further cross-cultural research on this topic.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Aug 27 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Helena R. Slobodskaya, M.D., Ph.D., D.Sc.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf  and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2.  Peer review at PLOS ONE is not double-blinded (https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/editorial-and-peer-review-process). For this reason, authors should include in the revised manuscript all the information removed for blind review.

3. Thank you for stating the following in the Acknowledgments Section of your manuscript:

“Elisabeth de Moor and Susan Branje were supported by a grant from the European Research Council (ERC-CoG INTRANSITION-773023). Jolien Van der Graaff received a grant from the COVID-19 fund of the Faculty of Social Sciences of Utrecht University.”

We note that you have provided additional information within the Acknowledgements Section that is not currently declared in your Funding Statement. Please note that funding information should not appear in the Acknowledgments section or other areas of your manuscript. We will only publish funding information present in the Funding Statement section of the online submission form.

Please remove any funding-related text from the manuscript and let us know how you would like to update your Funding Statement. Currently, your Funding Statement reads as follows:

“JvdG received a grant from the Faculty of Social Sciences of Utrecht University to fund data collection (no grant number available). URL: https://www.uu.nl/en/organisation/faculty-of-social-and-behavioural-sciences

ELdM and SB were supported by a grant from the European Research Council (ERC-CoG INTRANSITION-773023). URL: https://erc.europa.eu/

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.”

Please include your amended statements within your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

4. In your Data Availability statement, you have not specified where the minimal data set underlying the results described in your manuscript can be found. PLOS defines a study's minimal data set as the underlying data used to reach the conclusions drawn in the manuscript and any additional data required to replicate the reported study findings in their entirety. All PLOS journals require that the minimal data set be made fully available. For more information about our data policy, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability.

Upon re-submitting your revised manuscript, please upload your study’s minimal underlying data set as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and include the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers within your revised cover letter. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories. Any potentially identifying patient information must be fully anonymized.

Important: If there are ethical or legal restrictions to sharing your data publicly, please explain these restrictions in detail. Please see our guidelines for more information on what we consider unacceptable restrictions to publicly sharing data: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions. Note that it is not acceptable for the authors to be the sole named individuals responsible for ensuring data access.

We will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide in your cover letter.

5. We note that you have referenced (Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Utah and Unpublished manuscript ) which has currently not yet been accepted for publication. Please remove this from your References and amend this to state in the body of your manuscript: (ie “Bewick et al. [Unpublished]”) as detailed online in our guide for authors

http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-reference-style

6. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Partly

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: N/A

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: the work is interesting and well written. Some aspects that can improve the work are emphasized.

An in-depth study of country-specific literature in the construction of the theoretical part with respect to the topics under investigation, including the part on the restrictions of different governments.

It would be important to test the power of the overall sample and individual countries.

In addition, the application implications of the work should be developed

Reviewer #2: This article discusses a relevant topic in the current context: what are the determinants (or some of them) of young people's attitudes to public health measures put in place during the pandemic. The topic is of global interest and appropriate for a multidisciplinary journal such as PLOS ONE.

ABSTRACT:

The authors identify their research object rather clearly in the abstract, although I suggest describing the results of their model in greater detail and precision. Also, '(...) blaming certain groups' seems too vague phrasing to me. The authors need to be more specific about which "groups" they refer to in the abstract and the text in general (and in the model). Ideally, it would also be helpful to discuss (along the manuscript) what is the meaning of blaming these groups in the youth context of these countries. I also recommend adding a sentence to put the theory of "social identification" in context, so that it is made more understandable in an interdisciplinary context.

Keywords should be words, not sentences.

INTRODUCTION:

It seems to me that the authors in this section offer an adequate amount of information, but that it should be better organized. First, the authors seem to arrive at Figure 1 too quickly. This conceptual model should be better justified (the identification of these factors and the mediating mechanisms) and it would also help to make the hypotheses derived or underlying them more explicit. Some hypotheses are made but not formally highlighted.

I would thus reorganize the section so that the reader is first explained the rationale for the identification of the determinants and hypotheses. After that, I would offer the conceptual model (which could possibly also be moved to the methodological part as an analysis model).

One recent reference that could help to add in this section on young people:

Vacchiano, M., (2022). How the First COVID-19 Lockdown Worsened Younger Generations' Mental Health: Insights from Network Theory. Sociological Research Online, https://doi.org/10.1177/13607804221084723

METHODS:

The methodological part is sufficiently detailed. One fundamental doubt:

I am not sure it can be said 100% that a level 2 of 14 units does not need a multilevel analysis. I know there is debate on the issue, but as far as I know the best way to measure effects at the country level is through multilevel analysis. According to Austin (2010) even models with 10 level-2 clusters could produce reliable statistical inference (see also Bryan and Jenkins 2016 on this controversial issue).

I have no expertise to evaluate the strategy that the authors have set up to test the effect of countries. I am not saying it is wrong, I simply don't know it and fully understand it. Personally, I think a multilevel analysis would be sounder.

RESULTS, DISCUSSION, LIMITATIONS:

On the rest of the manuscript, I have little to add. It seems to me that the paper brings out some interesting results and that the authors identify well the limitations of their work and the data.

In general, the paper is publishable with appropriate modifications. First, (1) reorganizing the introductory section and making explicit in a formal way the hypotheses (and so justifying the conceptual model). Second, the (2) authors need to better justify why multilevel analysis is not performed (here a paper https://doi.org/10.1093/esr/jcv059 that may help). Multilevel analysis could still be necessary in this paper. Also, in the discussion the differences between countries are not mentioned at all, which casts more doubt on the adequacy of the strategy pursued by the authors to capture country effects. I am sure the authors can offer more interesting arguments on this country differences.

All this would certainly benefit the quality and impact of this article.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Decision Letter 1

Helena R Slobodskaya

7 Dec 2022

What should I do and who's to blame? A cross-national study on youth's attitudes and beliefs in times of COVID-19

PONE-D-22-10911R1

Dear Dr. De Moor,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Helena R. Slobodskaya, M.D., Ph.D., D.Sc.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #2: Partly

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #2: N/A

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #2: The authors have substantially improved the original version of the manuscript and responded to my comments, resolving where possible the problems highlighted.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #2: No

**********

Acceptance letter

Helena R Slobodskaya

12 Dec 2022

PONE-D-22-10911R1

What Should I do and Who's to Blame? A Cross-National Study on Youth’s Attitudes and Beliefs in Times of COVID-19

Dear Dr. De Moor:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Helena R. Slobodskaya

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Table. Descriptive statistics and demographics per country.

    (DOCX)

    S2 Table. Newly designed perceived COVID-19 burden measure used in the present study.

    (DOCX)

    S3 Table. Constrained multigroup mediation model with attitude towards government approach and blaming of certain groups regressed on COVID-19 burden, individualistic values, collectivistic values, and empathy, mediated by social identification; grouping on cumulative COVID-19 cases per 100,000 inhabitants.

    (DOCX)

    S4 Table. Constrained multigroup mediation model with attitude towards government approach and blaming of certain groups regressed on COVID-19 burden, individualistic values, collectivistic values, and empathy, mediated by social identification; grouping on strictness of the national COVID-19 approach.

    (DOCX)

    S5 Table. Constrained multigroup mediation model with attitude towards government approach and blaming of certain groups regressed on COVID-19 burden, individualistic values, collectivistic values, and empathy, mediated by social identification; grouping on national level individualistic values.

    (DOCX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers_COVID-19_R1.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    The anonymized data is made available on the project OSF page: https://osf.io/738z5/. If researchers want to use the data, they are asked to submit a data request to the project executive team (Jolien Van der Graaff, Laura Padilla-Walker, Gustavo Carlo, Susan Branje). If a project proposes to use data from a single country, approval must be obtained from the lead PIs from that country. If the project requires data from multiple countries, the proposed research needs to be approved by the lead PIs in those countries, as well as the project executive team. A full list of project contributors can be found at: https://youngincoronatimes.sites.uu.nl/international-collaborators/.


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES