Abstract
Human genetics is advancing at an unprecedented pace. Improvements in genotyping technology and rapidly falling costs have accelerated gene discovery. We can now comprehensively scan the genome, testing variation across millions of genetic markers, to identify specific variants associated with any outcome of interest. Large consortia consisting of hundreds of scientists are analyzing data from hundreds of thousands to millions of individuals. Multivariate methods now enable us to identify genes involved in underlying processes, to complement studies focused on specific disorders or traits. There has been an exponential increase in use of direct-to-consumer genetic feedback platforms. These advances are poised to have a widespread effect on medicine and society. However, with such rapid progress will come ethical, social, and legal challenges. Among those challenges is the need for increased efforts to enhance public understanding of the ways genes contribute to complex behavioral outcomes, and for increased diversity in the field of genetics to ensure that all people benefit from advances. Psychologists can play an important role in addressing the inevitable questions that will arise as genetics increasingly becomes mainstream.
Keywords: genetics, behavior genetics, gene identification
Introduction
In 2002, as a newly graduated Ph.D., I published a review article in Current Directions in Psychological Science with my graduate advisor Richard Rose, entitled “Behavior Genetics: What’s New? What’s Next?” (Dick & Rose, 2002). Twenty years later, when asked to provide an update on the field, I went back to that piece to reflect on how far we’ve come. Most of the research directions discussed in that paper have stood the test of time: Characterizing how genetic effects change across the lifespan (Dick et al., 2006) and interact with the environment (Assary et al., 2018) remain active areas of study, as is characterizing the extent to which genetic effects contribute to comorbidity across outcomes (Allegrini et al., 2020). We also forecast advances in gene identification, though it was here that our predictions fell short. We did not foresee the speed and scale at which gene discovery would advance and characterize the recent history of the field. The past decade has been witness to the formation of international consortia consisting of hundreds of scientists, scanning millions of genomes to collaboratively advance gene identification. It has been characterized by the launch of the precision medicine initiative (Collins & Varmus, 2015), and rapid escalation in direct-to-consumer genetic testing (Folkersen et al., 2020).
In our review of 20 years ago, we were also remiss in failing to recognize an insidious problem in the field: the lack of diversity in genetics research, among our participants and research community, and the potential for harm this would create. In this paper, I provide an update on advances that have occurred, and, in the spirit of Karl Popper, I make risky new predictions about the potential applications of genetics in the future, which I believe will be far-reaching, with drastic impact on our lives. Finally, I discuss the ethical implications that have become increasingly pressing as the field has advanced.
Gene Discovery: From There to Here
Behavior genetics is a field that has experienced rapid advancement, the likes of which are unparalleled, with the exception of advances in computer science. The first great triumph of behavior genetics was to create widespread understanding that nearly all behavioral outcomes are under some degree of genetic influence, representing a drastic shift from the first half of the 20th century, when behavioral disorders were viewed largely as environmentally determined. If the first 50 years of the field were dedicated to demonstrating that a wide range of human behavior is genetically influenced, the next 50 years will surely be consumed with mapping those genes and applying this newfound knowledge.
We have entered a period of unprecedented gene discovery, made possible by advances in genotyping technology, coupled with greater knowledge of genetic variation, and rapidly falling genotyping costs. Now we can comprehensively scan the human genome, testing variation across millions of markers, to identify specific variants associated with any outcome of interest. Because scanning millions of genetic markers requires massive multiple testing correction, it became clear that huge samples sizes would be necessary to conduct well-powered studies. Researchers came together and formed consortia, pooling their samples to achieve huge sample sizes. One of the most prominent examples of these international collaborative consortia is the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, which consists of over 800 investigators from >40 countries (Watson et al., 2020). Other large-scale consortia have focused on behavioral outcomes such as alcohol and tobacco use (Liu et al., 2019), and educational attainment (Lee et al., 2018). Several countries and medical systems formed biobanks to collate health information and genotyping for hundreds of thousands of individuals, creating rich resources made broadly available to the field (Bycroft et al., 2018).
The strategy worked. There is compelling evidence that the larger the sample, the more genetic variants are identified (Sullivan & Geschwind, 2019). This has been observed across all psychiatric, substance use, and other complex health conditions and behaviors that have been studied. The Ns required to identify genetic variants vary as a function of the heritability of the trait, and associated complexities, such as the degree to which the environment plays a role. However, the evidence is unambiguous: Bigger is better. Extremely large, well-powered GWAS have led to replicable effects, for both individual genes and aggregate genetic liability scores created by summing associated genetic effects across the genome, in a way that earlier candidate gene, linkage, and smaller association studies did not (Duncan et al., 2019).
In contrast to our expectations in 2002, phenotyping has taken a backseat in this process. It became clear that having a precise or well-measured phenotype was far less relevant than having a huge sample. However, now that the field has a working strategy for gene discovery, the importance of phenotype is re-emerging. There is growing recognition that the genes you find are substantively affected by the phenotype you analyze. A salient example can be found in the substance use field, where GWAS for alcohol consumption and alcohol problems produced surprisingly different results, though they were both considered relevant phenotypes for understanding genetic contributions to alcohol use disorder (Mallard et al., 2021).
Recently, multivariate methods for gene identification have been developed (Grotzinger et al., 2019) that allow one to model genetic covariance (the degree to which there is genetic sharing across outcomes) in a way that parallels factor models of phenotypic covariance. These methods allow researchers to use our understanding of the genetic architecture of behavioral traits to aid in gene identification, capitalizing on decades of twin research that elucidates how genetic influences impact behavioral and psychiatric outcomes.
For example, there is compelling evidence that alcohol and other substance use disorders are influenced by a common set of genes, that also impact other traits related to self-regulation, such as childhood behavior problems and measures of impulsivity (Krueger et al., 2009). This common underlying factor is called externalizing, and is highly heritable. Based on this literature, we conducted a GWAS of an externalizing factor in 1.5 million individuals, identifying 579 genetic variants that influence self-regulation (Karlsson Linnér et al., 2021). The resulting genetic risk scores, tallied for an individual based on how many of the risk variants they carry (Bogdan et al., 2018), predict a wide array of behavioral, social, and medical outcomes related to self-regulation in independent samples. They account for ~10% of the variance in externalizing behavior, commensurate with many central social science constructs, e.g., socioeconomic status, neighborhood disadvantage.
Science Fiction or Science Fact?
So, what will we do with all this information? Gene identification can lead to enhanced understanding of the underlying biology of disorders, facilitating the development of targeted drug treatments. That process takes time, often decades, before knowledge translates into advances in patient care.
But there is another way GWAS findings can be used, which I predict will have more immediate impact. The 1997 movie Gattaca envisions a world where genetic information is mainstream. In an opening scene, a nurse rattles off a baby’s genetic propensity to develop a number of health problems: “Neurological condition: 60% probability; Manic depression: 42% probability; ADHD: 89% probability.” The movie imagines how that information could reshape society and have a profound impact on the lives of individuals who know their genetic proclivities and vulnerabilities from birth.
This represents a drastic shift from current state. At present, people usually go to their treatment providers when something is wrong, and often they wait to see if the problem resolves on its own, leading to a worsening of symptoms before the individual seeks help. The idea of precision medicine (Collins & Varmus, 2015), set forth by President Obama in his state of the union address in 2015, is to convert health care from a treatment-based model to one that is predictive, preventative, and personalized. Because our DNA sequences are present from birth, our genes provide information about outcomes for which we are at elevated risk, and can help us intervene preventively.
We are now approaching a time when we can generate risk predictions of the very sort imagined in Gattaca. Presently, there are wide error bars around those predictions, and most genetics researchers will tell you they aren’t ready for prime time. You won’t see genetic risk scores for complex behavioral outcomes in the clinic any time soon. But medical professionals are no longer the keepers of knowledge. Start-up companies have by-passed traditional gate-keepers across many industries, from taxis to hotels to the medical establishment, to give consumers direct access. The field of genetics is no exception: there has been exponential growth direct-to-consumer genetic testing (Regalado, 2019), and the use of free public websites that allow you to upload raw genetic data to compute genetic risk scores for a variety of conditions. Half of the top twelve most accessed genetic risk scores from one widely used site are substance use or mental health outcomes (Folkersen et al., 2020). People want this information, and they can already get it.
If the idea of genetics drastically changing society sounds unrealistic to you, watch a clip from The Today Show from 1994 in which the hosts are discussing a mysterious new concept called the internet. Finally, a confused Katie Couric asks an off-camera producer, “Can you explain what internet is?” Fast forward just 27 years and most of us carry super-computers in our pockets. The internet has changed the way we communicate, shop, vote. We have an entire generation of children who don’t remember a world without limitless information available at their fingertips. It has radically changed society, for better and worse, and we are struggling to catch up.
These rapid changes are a product of Moore’s law, which refers to the exponential growth that the computing industry experienced over the past decades. It is named after Intel co-founder Gordon Moore, who in 1965 observed that computing capacity was doubling every other year. Exponential growth changes our lives and society at a pace that human brains, evolved to think linearly, cannot comprehend. Now consider that genotyping costs have dropped at a pace that surpasses Moore’s law; we’re just a few decades behind with respect to when we entered our exponential growth period. It took 2.7 billion dollars and 13 years to sequence the first human genome, published in 2003; today, we can sequence a human genome for less than $1000, in a single day. I predict that genetics will be the next major societal revolution. Like advances in computing, it will sneak up on us, and we will find ourselves in a world radically changed, for which we are largely unprepared.
DNA is not Destiny
So how do we get ready? We are spending billions on gene identification, but very little research comparatively is focused on how we will deliver personalized genetic information to individuals in ways that are understandable and empowering. Data indicate that approximately 80% of people report wanting access to their genetic risk scores (Driver, Kuo, Dick, et al., 2020), but over a quarter of people don’t properly understand how complex genetics works. They erroneously believe that genes are determinative and misunderstand the role of the environment (Chapman et al., 2019; Driver, Kuo, & Dick, 2020).
Although our genotypes contain useful information about our risk profile, genes influence behavior in complex ways. There are no genes “for” substance use disorder, depression, anxiety, or other complex behavioral outcomes. Our genetic codes impact the ways our brains are wired, contributing to differences in temperamental traits, the way we respond to risk and reward, our emotionality. These natural tendencies then impact our interactions with the world: the environments we seek out, the way we perceive our environments, the way the world responds to us (Scarr & McCartney, 1983). These processes are called gene-environment correlation, and they create feedback loops that accentuate genetic influence across time. Further, certain environments can constrain or exacerbate the influence of genetic predispositions, a phenomenon known as gene-environment interaction (Dick, 2011).
The complexity of these processes is ignored by GWAS, which agnostically detects any genetic variant associated with outcome regardless of how that variant exerts an effect. Some associated variants directly influence our biology to alter risk. For example, variants in the ADH and ALDH genes impact an individual’s ability to metabolize alcohol, which protects against developing an alcohol use disorder (Edenberg, 2007). Other genes create cascades of influence via social pathways (Raffington et al., 2020). A high sensation-seeking adolescent may seek out bars and risk-taking peers, elevating risk for substance use disorder. Compounding risk via environmental pathways is likely one of the reasons why heritability estimates from twin studies are higher than the variance accounted for by genetic effects identified in GWAS. Our current genetic liability scores are also limited by the methods used to calculate them, which do not take into account gene-gene interactions or fully capture the range of variation that exists across our genomes.
Understanding the pathways by which our genes confer risk, and the limitations inherent in genetic liability scores, highlights why genetic information is not determinative. Gene-outcome associations are correlated with - and importantly, can be interrupted by - environmental processes. Mapping genetic pathways, understanding how associated genes lead to elevated risk, yields information about how to intervene. This information will be critical for precision medicine, so we can help individuals understand what it means to be at elevated genetic risk, and what they can do to reduce risk.
But this is not the genetics most of us are taught in grade school. We learn about peas and Punnett squares, monks and Mendel. We teach children about single gene effects, but not about the ways of complex genetics, whereby thousands of genetic variants come together with the environment to influence important life outcomes.
For individuals to understand and interpret genetic scores, they need to understand the complicated pathways by which our genes influence our lives. This requires researchers to do more to get our findings out of scientific journals and into the hands of people who can use it. I recently wrote a popular press book called The Child Code in an effort to bring behavior genetic research to mainstream parenting – to help parents understand how their child’s behavior is influenced by their genetic code, to reduce some of the pressure and judgement that often surrounds parenting, and to help parents identify their child’s unique needs. We should be able to harness the wealth of new genetic knowledge to improve lives, but it will require us to educate the public (and likely many medical professionals) about the complex ways that genes influence behavior.
The Diversity Problem in Genetics
There is another problem in our field: the profound lack of representation in genetics, both among our participants and among the researchers who are driving the research agenda. Nearly 80% of all gene identification studies are conducted with individuals of European descent (Popejoy & Fullerton, 2016). This is problematic because our genetic predictions are most accurate for individuals whose genetic background matches the research participants used to identify relevant genetic variants (Martin et al., 2017). There are many reasons for this (Lewis & Vassos, 2020; Peterson et al., 2019), but central among them is a basic property of our DNA in which “background” genetic variation is a product of the ancestral history of the migration of people around the world. When we detect associated variants, often we are picking up on nearby correlated variants, rather than causal variants, which will necessarily be connected to an individual’s ancestral background. Differences in social processes and gene environment interaction across different groups also likely impacts the portability of genetic scores.
There are efforts underway to increase diversity in genetic studies, but remedying such extreme imbalance will take time and substantial investment. Parallel to these efforts, we must increase the diversity of researchers in our field, who bring critical perspectives in working with diverse populations and the unique considerations surrounding conducting genetic research within groups that have been historically and systemically disadvantaged and mistreated by the research community (Davis, 2021).
Genetics will likely make it into the clinic and mainstream before these problems are remedied, and this has concerning potential to further perpetuate health disparities. This should serve as a call to action to all of us with a commitment to anti-racism, to educate ourselves about the history and role of genetics in human behavior so we are prepared to address inequities and misinformation.
Conclusions
The field of genetics has entered a period of rapid advancement that promises to revolutionize our society. There is much good that can follow, including more tailored and effective prevention, intervention, and treatment, and a deeper appreciation for our individuality. But there are also complex challenges that will accompany the integration of genetic information into our lives. In our review of 20 years ago, we ended by addressing ethical, legal, and social issues to be confronted once genes conferring susceptibility to disorders are identified: “How should information about the nature and meaning of susceptibility genes be conveyed to the media, the public, and the courts? How can erroneous beliefs about genetic determinism be dispelled effectively? Such issues will be even more salient once dispositional genes for normal behavioral variation are identified.” That time has come, but we’ve made little progress toward answering those questions. We’ve been so excited about advances in gene discovery that we haven’t slowed to address the complex ethical issues that will inevitably follow. My hope is that bringing more widespread attention to these issues will inspire more researchers, from all areas and backgrounds, to enter the exciting, dizzying, and sometimes frightening, realm of human behavioral genetics, so we can work together collectively to address these pressing questions.
Acknowledgments
Dr. Danielle Dick is supported by NIH R01 AA015416, P50 AA022537, R25 AA027402, R34 AA027347, and U10 AA008401 from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA), and by R01 DA050721 and R25 DA051339 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA).
I would like to thank Emily Balcke, MS for her assistance with preparing this manuscript.
References
- Allegrini AG, Cheesman R, Rimfeld K, Selzam S, Pingault J-B, Eley TC, & Plomin R (2020). The p factor: genetic analyses support a general dimension of psychopathology in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61(1), 30–39. 10.1111/jcpp.13113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Assary E, Vincent JP, Keers R, & Pluess M (2018). Gene-environment interaction and psychiatric disorders: Review and future directions. Seminars in Cell and Developmental Biology, 77, 133–143. 10.1016/j.semcdb.2017.10.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bogdan R, Baranger DAA, & Agrawal A (2018). Polygenic Risk Scores in Clinical Psychology: Bridging Genomic Risk to Individual Differences. Annual review of clinical psychology, 14, 119–157. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050817-084847 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bycroft C, Freeman C, Petkova D, Band G, Elliott LT, Sharp K, Motyer A, Vukcevic D, Delaneau O, O’Connell J, Cortes A, Welsh S, Young A, Effingham M, McVean G, Leslie S, Allen N, Donnelly P, & Marchini J (2018). The UK Biobank resource with deep phenotyping and genomic data. Nature, 562(7726), 203–209. 10.1038/s41586-018-0579-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chapman R, Likhanov M, Selita F, Zakharov I, Smith-Woolley E, & Kovas Y (2019). New literacy challenge for the twenty-first century: genetic knowledge is poor even among well educated. Journal of community genetics, 10(1), 73–84. 10.1007/s12687-018-0363-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins FS, & Varmus H (2015). A new initiative on precision medicine. New England Journal of Medicine, 372(9), 793–795. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis LK (2021, March 17, 2021). Human Genetics Needs an Antiracism Plan. Scientific American. [Google Scholar]; *A call to action to understand the history of racism in the study of genetics and combat the problem of racist ideas in science.
- Dick DM (2011). Gene-environment interaction in psychological traits and disorders. Annual review of clinical psychology, 7, 383–409. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032210-104518 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dick DM, Bierut L, Hinrichs A, Fox L, Bucholz KK, Kramer J, Kuperman S, Hesselbrock V, Schuckit M, Almasy L, Tischfield J, Porjesz B, Begleiter H, Nurnberger J, Xuei X, Edenberg HJ, & Foroud T (2006). The Role of GABRA2 in Risk for Conduct Disorder and Alcohol and Drug Dependence across Developmental Stages. Behavior Genetics, 36(4), 577–590. 10.1007/s10519-005-9041-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dick DM, & Rose RJ (2002). Behavior genetics: What’s new? What’s next? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(2), 70–74. [Google Scholar]
- Driver MN, Kuo SI, Dick DM, & Spit for Science Working Group. (2020). Interest in Genetic Feedback for Alcohol Use Disorder and Related Substance Use and Psychiatric Outcomes among Young Adults. Brain sciences, 10(12), 1007–1007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Driver MN, Kuo SI-C, & Dick DM (2020). Genetic feedback for psychiatric conditions: Where are we now and where are we going. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 183(7), 423–432. 10.1002/ajmg.b.32815 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duncan LE, Ostacher M, & Ballon J (2019). How genome-wide association studies (GWAS) made traditional candidate gene studies obsolete. Neuropsychopharmacology : official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 44(9), 1518–1523. 10.1038/s41386-019-0389-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edenberg HJ (2007). The genetics of alcohol metabolism: role of alcohol dehydrogenase and aldehyde dehydrogenase variants. Alcohol Research & Health, 30(1), 5–13. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Folkersen L, Pain O, Ingason A, Werge T, Lewis CM, & Austin J (2020). Impute.me: An Open-Source, Non-profit Tool for Using Data From Direct-to-Consumer Genetic Testing to Calculate and Interpret Polygenic Risk Scores. Frontiers in Genetics, 11, 578. 10.3389/fgene.2020.00578 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fullerton JM & Nurnberger JI (2019). Polygenic scores in psychiatry: Will they be useful for clinicians?. F1000Research, 8, F1000 Faculty Rev-1293. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; *This article provides an overview of PRS in general terms (including a glossary suitable for informed non-geneticists) and discusses the use and applications of PRS.
- Grotzinger AD, Rhemtulla M, de Vlaming R, Ritchie SJ, Mallard TT, Hill WD, Ip HF, Marioni RE, McIntosh AM, Deary IJ, Koellinger PD, Harden KP, Nivard MG, & Tucker-Drob EM (2019). Genomic structural equation modelling provides insights into the multivariate genetic architecture of complex traits. Nature Human Behavior, 3(5), 513–525. 10.1038/s41562-019-0566-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karlsson Linnér R, Mallard TT, Barr PB, Sanchez-Roige S, Madole JW, Driver MN, Poore HE, de Vlaming R, Grotzinger AD, Tielbeek JJ, Johnson EC, Liu M, Rosenthal SB, Ideker T, Zhou H, Kember RL, Pasman JA, Verweij KJH, Liu DJ, … Collaborators C (2021). Multivariate analysis of 1.5 million people identifies genetic associations with traits related to self-regulation and addiction. Nature Neuroscience, 24(10), 1367–1376. 10.1038/s41593-021-00908-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krueger RF, Hicks BM, Patrick CJ, Carlson SR, Iacono WG, & McGue M (2009). Etiologic connections among substance dependence, antisocial behavior, and personality: Modeling the externalizing spectrum. American Psychological Association. 10.1037/11855-003] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee JJ, Wedow R, Okbay A, Kong E, Maghzian O, Zacher M, Nguyen-Viet TA, Bowers P, Sidorenko J, Karlsson Linnér R, Fontana MA, Kundu T, Lee C, Li H, Li R, Royer R, Timshel PN, Walters RK, Willoughby EA, … Cesarini D (2018). Gene discovery and polygenic prediction from a genome-wide association study of educational attainment in 1.1 million individuals. Nature Genetics, 50(8), 1112–1121. 10.1038/s41588-018-0147-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis CM, & Vassos E (2020). Polygenic risk scores: from research tools to clinical instruments. Genome Medicine, 12(1), 44. 10.1186/s13073-020-00742-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; *This review considers the many possible ways that PRS could be useful in clinical care.
- Liu M, Jiang Y, Wedow R, Li Y, Brazel DM, Chen F, Datta G, Davila-Velderrain J, McGuire D, Tian C, Zhan X, Choquet H, Docherty AR, Faul JD, Foerster JR, Fritsche LG, Gabrielsen ME, Gordon SD, Haessler J, … Vrieze S (2019). Association studies of up to 1.2 million individuals yield new insights into the genetic etiology of tobacco and alcohol use. Nature Genetics, 51(2), 237–244. 10.1038/s41588-018-0307-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mallard TT, Savage JE, Johnson EC, Huang Y, Edwards AC, Hottenga JJ, Grotzinger AD, Gustavson DE, Jennings MV, Anokhin A, Dick DM, Edenberg HJ, Kramer JR, Lai D, Meyers JL, Pandey AK, Harden KP, Nivard MG, de Geus EJC, … Sanchez-Roige S (2021). Item-Level Genome-Wide Association Study of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test in Three Population-Based Cohorts. American Journal of Psychiatry, appiajp202020091390. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.20091390 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin AR, Gignoux CR, Walters RK, Wojcik GL, Neale BM, Gravel S, Daly MJ, Bustamante CD, & Kenny EE (2017). Human Demographic History Impacts Genetic Risk Prediction across Diverse Populations. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 100(4), 635–649. 10.1016/j.ajhg.2017.03.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray GK, Lin T, Austin J, McGrath JJ, Hickie IB, & Wray NR (2020). Could polygenic risk scores be useful in psychiatry?: a review. JAMA psychiatry. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; *This review evaluates the potential use of PRS across multiple contexts in psychiatry.
- Peterson RE, Kuchenbaecker K, Walters RK, Chen C-Y, Popejoy AB, Periyasamy S, Lam M, Iyegbe C, Strawbridge RJ, Brick L, Carey CE, Martin AR, Meyers JL, Su J, Chen J, Edwards AC, Kalungi A, Koen N, Majara L, … Duncan LE (2019). Genome-wide Association Studies in Ancestrally Diverse Populations: Opportunities, Methods, Pitfalls, and Recommendations. Cell, 179(3), 589–603. 10.1016/j.cell.2019.08.051 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Popejoy AB, & Fullerton SM (2016). Genomics is failing on diversity. Nature, 538(7624), 161–164. 10.1038/538161a [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raffington L, Mallard T, & Harden KP (2020). Polygenic Scores in Developmental Psychology: Invite Genetics In, Leave Biodeterminism Behind. Annual Review of Developmental Psychology, 2(1), 389–411. 10.1146/annurev-devpsych-051820-123945 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; *This review provides an overview of the use of polygenic scores in psychological research, while also considering ethical, legal, and social concerns.
- Regalado A (2019). More than 26 million people have taken an at-home ancestry test. MIT Technology Review, 11(2), 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Scarr S, & McCartney K (1983). How People Make Their Own Environments: A Theory of Genotype → Environment Effects. Child Development, 54(2), 424–435. 10.2307/1129703 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sullivan PF, & Geschwind DH (2019). Defining the Genetic, Genomic, Cellular, and Diagnostic Architectures of Psychiatric Disorders. Cell, 177(1), 162–183. 10.1016/j.cell.2019.01.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watson HJ, Yilmaz Z, & Sullivan PF (2020). The Psychiatric Genomics Consortium: History, development, and the future. In Baune BT (Ed.), Personalized Psychiatry (pp. 91–101). Academic Press. 10.1016/B978-0-12-813176-3.00009-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]