Abstract
Plants play a very important role in landscape construction. In order to explore whether different living environment will affect people’s preference for the structural features of plant organs, this study examined 26 villagers and 33 college students as the participants, and pictures of leaves, flowers and fruits of plants as the stimulus to conduct eye-tracking and EEG detection experiments. We found that eye movement indicators can explain people’s visual preferences, but they are unable to find differences in preferences between groups. EEG indicators can make up for this deficiency, which further reveals the difference in psychological and physiological responses between the two groups when viewing stimuli. The final results show that the villagers and the students liked leaves best, preferring aciculiform and leathery leaves; solitary, purple and capitulum flowers; and medium-sized, spathulate, black and pear fruits. In addition, it was found that the overall attention of the villagers when watching stimuli was far lower than that of the students, but the degree of meditation was higher. With regard to eye movement and EEG, the total duration of fixations is highly positively correlated with the number of fixations, and the average pupil size has a weak negative correlation with attention. On the contrary, the average duration of fixations has a weak positive correlation with meditation. Generally speaking, we believe that Photinia×fraseri, Metasequoia glyptostroboides, Photinia serratifolia, Koelreuteria bipinnata and Cunninghamia lanceolata are superior landscape building plants in rural areas and on campuses; Pinus thunbergii, Myrica rubra, Camellia japonica and other plants with obvious features and bright colours are also the first choice in rural landscapes; and Yulania biondii, Cercis chinensis, Hibiscus mutabilis and other plants with simple structures are the first choice in campus landscapes. This study is of great significance for selecting plants for landscape construction and management according to different environments and local conditions.
Introduction
Environmental quality plays an important role in our life, which affects appeal to people’s aesthetic sense, and their physiological and psychological health [1]. People can perceive and evaluate the environment through sight, hearing, smell and touch, but visual perception is the most important means [2]. Some scholars use the term “visual environment” to describe people’s visual perception of the environment [3, 4], and many existing studies also use the term “visual landscape” to highlight the visual attributes of landscape [2, 5]. The visual preference of the landscape means the environment is conveyed by visual perception, and people form inner perceptual feelings based on personal experience, needs, expectations and psychological state [6, 7]. The range of landscape is very wide, encompassing everything from the macro- to the micro-scale, from urban parks to plants [8]; plant organs can also be called environment.
Some scholars have focused on the landscape and explored people’s preference for different landscape types or features. They have selected cities [9], parks [10], courtyards [11], green spaces [12] and the river bank [13], among others, on as the research objects, and summarized nine visual concepts [14] used to analyze the visual landscape features: naturalness, interference, complexity, consistency, historicity, management work, horizon, image and time variation. In addition to the above landscape types, increasing numbers of scholars have done much meaningful research on plant landscape preference. Plants are an important element in the landscape, with the capacity to improve people’s mental and physical health. Any vegetation contributes to visual improvement [15], and is also an important element of environmental perception and preference [16], so it cannot be ignored. People often start from individual plants [15], communities or plant configuration combinations [17], and study them from a macro perspective. The structural features and systems of plants have an important influence on how people interpret plants [18], so there are also some researchers who cut in from a deeper direction to study the public’s preference for plant organs. Rahnema et al. [19] selected eight ornamental plants with flowers or leaves, and used questionnaires to reveal the preference and emotional perception of ornamental plants from the perspective of plant organ features. Tarakci-Eren and Duzenli [16] took photos of designated angiosperms in different seasons, and studied the preference level and cognitive differences of the colour changes of stems, leaves, flowers and fruits of plants on the Karadeniz Technical University campus. However, it is worth noting that these scholars have not undertaken this study of plant organs as the core of their research, so they have not systematically integrated people’s preferences when organs are used as plant landscapes.
As early as several decades ago, scholars proposed that most people viewing a landscape would determine the advantages and disadvantages of the landscape [20], and thought that different people would have different views when viewing the same landscape [21, 22]. Therefore, in addition to the above-mentioned research on landscape preferences, some scholars pay attention to the differences in the preferences of different individuals or groups for particular landscapes. Most are based on age and gender [23], cultural background [24], professional background [25] and familiarity with the research location [26, 27] as individual or group features. In addition, landscape preference is not only influenced by people’s natural self-awareness [28], but also impacted by the growing environment to a certain extent, so the living environment is also a very important group characteristic. Yet there are currently few research results. The differences in living environment span a wide range and can exist in different countries. Some scholars have investigated the differences in preferences of people who live in different countries and have different living environments for urban square design elements [29]. There may also be differences in living environment within the same country. For example, Luo et al. [30] took China as an example and compared local high school students in Xiamen and Xinjiang. Through the implementation of landscape livability and aesthetic preference tests, the landscape preference differences and their features and the assimilation trend between the two groups of people were discussed. In some studies, the concept of “prototype landscape” was used to distinguish people from different living environments. This concept is the experience summary of settlers’ previous living landscapes [31]; Gong et al. [32] discuss the influence of different original landscape types on the perception of the urban plant landscape.
The rural residential environment is an important part of livable and sustainable rural areas all over the world. Planting trees can change the perception of landscape, and different tree configurations will produce different responses [33]. With the development of social technology and the changing rural economy, the configuration of the rural landscape is being redrawn [34]. A campus is a miniature of the general trend of urban forest development. The College of Botany, Landscape Architecture and Urban Design of The University of Pennsylvania in Philadelphia in the United States once made a comprehensive landscape plan for its campus, and advocated the planting and cultivation of campus trees [35]. Hami and Abdi [36] also emphasize the importance of vegetation, rest areas and waterscape for a campus landscape, because students like campuses with natural foundations, landscape elements and places where they can sit and rest. Study areas should be designed with vertical natural elements. However, the best plants for landscape construction in the two environments have not been studied extensively. We therefore decided to explore whether differences exist in people’s preferences for particular plant landscapes in rural and campus living environments. Villagers and students are the biggest landscape audiences in rural and campus environments respectively. They spend most of their time in rural and campus environments, making them the best candidates to study in relation to the two environments; we therefore chose to make villagers and students the research objects of this study.
People’s attitudes, perceptions and preferences are closely related to the features of eye movement, and people’s cognitive behavior can be analyzed using eye movement [37]. Eye movement is the movement of the eyes when they receive external information, which mainly includes three basic forms: fixation, saccade and pursuit movement [38]. Among these, fixation refers to aiming the central fossa of the eyeball at the stimulate target in order to get the clearest image. However, the eyeball is not completely stationary when staring, and there are still very small saccade movements, such as drift, tremor and involuntary saccade. The saccade is the extremely fast movement of the fixation point, thus keeping the object to be fixed in the central fovea area, in order to search for the stimulation target and obtain clear vision. Pursuit movement refers to the movement of people’s eyes following a moving object [39]. People usually think they control eye movements, but in fact there are many eye movements that are difficult for individuals to control or even detect. The above three kinds of eye movements always exist in people’s daily life consciously or unconsciously. According to statistics, 80 to 90 per cent of the information people get from the outside world comes from vision [40], so eye tracking has wide application value in many fields [41–45]. This method has gradually matured and is widely used in the study of visual landscape preference, especially in the forest [46, 47], urban parks [10, 48] and rural landscapes [49]. As mentioned above, however, few studies exist on visual preference using eye tracking experiments for individual plants and their organs at a closer distance. For example, Zheng et al. [18] used the pictures of ornamental bamboo stems and leaves as their stimulus materials for eye tracking experiments, and discussed people’s preference types from stem colours to stem stripes, leaf stripes, stem variation types and growth habits.
In addition to eye tracking, EEG technology has also been applied to landscape evaluation and preference. The development of neuroscience over the last century has greatly enriched people’s understanding of the bioelectrical signals emitted by brain neurons [50]. EEG is a physiological signal that uses physiological methods to measure nerve signals on the surface of the scalp and record them. The patterns and frequencies of these bioelectric signals can be measured by sensors placed on the scalp [51]. Different neural activities produce different brain wave patterns, and different brain wave patterns emit EEG with different frequencies, thus showing different brain states. The following are the frequency band divisions and the mental state of the brain reflected by different types of EEG:
Delta wave: 1–3 Hz, in deep sleep, non-rapid eye sleep, unconscious state
Theta wave: 4–7 Hz, in the state of creation, memory, fantasy, imagination, light sleep state
Alpha wave: 8–12 Hz, in a relaxed but not sleepy, calm and conscious state
Low-frequency Beta wave: 12–15 Hz, in a motor-sensory rhythm—that is, a state of relaxation, concentration and coordination
Intermediate-frequency Beta wave: 16–20 Hz, in a state of thinking and clear awareness of self and surrounding environment
High-frequency Beta wave: 21–30 Hz, in a state of alertness and excitement.
Many different psychological indicators can be obtained using this technique, which may infer attention from the electrical signals of the brain [41]. EEG response reveals the difference of electrical activity in the human brain [52]. The application of EEG mostly focuses on clinical diagnosis and research in medicine [53, 54]. In recent years, scholars have also applied this technology to other fields, such as driving a motor vehicle [55, 56], click intention of network users [57], consumer preference [58, 59] and landscape evaluation [60, 61], but the influence of plants on psychophysiological response has not been explored thoroughly [62]. When combined with other instruments, EEG can provide more accurate results for participants’ response under stimulation [63, 64]. Therefore, some researchers have combined eye tracking and EEG technology to discover people’s preferences and physiological responses to the landscape more scientifically and effectively [51, 65]. On the whole, however, the number of related studies remains small.
This study combines visual and neural science technologies–eye tracking and EEG–to study the preferences and differences of plant features between villagers and students living in different environments to provide a basis for a discussion of the similarities and differences of plant application in rural and campus landscape construction. We take plant organs as our research core, choose leaves, flowers and fruits as plant landscapes, and extract their colour, shape, size, type and other characteristics for research into preferences. The aims were to provide scientific reference suggestions for the selection of plants in landscape construction, explore some new perspectives for landscape preference, and provide more reference data for the combination of eye movement and EEG and the relationship between them.
Methods
Participants
There are two groups of participants in this experiment. One group comprises 26 villagers (Mage = 47.23, SDage = 10.359) in Changkou Village, Sanming City, Fujian Province, with education levels of high school and below. They have lived in Changkou Village for at least two years and represent the group living in the rural environment. The other group consists of 33 undergraduates and postgraduates (Mage = 21.79, SDage = 2.176) in Central South University of Forestry and Technology, Changsha City, Hunan Province. They are sophomores and above–that is, they have lived in the university for at least one year, representing the group living in the campus environment. The villagers in Changkou Village were organized to participate in the experiment by the staff of the village committee with the consent of the participants themselves, while the students in the university were recruited by the researchers through social media software on WeChat to publish recruitment information in the university via group chats. A total of 59 people participated in the experiment, including 20 men and 39 women, aged between 18 and 63 years. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, no colour blindness or weak colour recognition, and all of them passed the eye movement calibration in the experiment, meeting the experimental requirements. The experiment obtained the informed consent of all subjects. The study was carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations, and approved by the academic committee of Central South University of Forestry and Technology. The data were analyzed anonymously.
Stimuli
It is considered effective to experiment with pictures as a medium [66–68]. The participants need to watch a total of 123 colour pictures, of which three are warm-up pictures set to prevent interference of the primary effect. The three warm-up pictures are first shown to the participants in the experiment, but their data are not included in any statistical analysis, and the remaining 120 are experimental pictures to obtain valid experimental data. Each picture uses depth-of-field mode of camera or fuzzy processing to ensure that one of the leaves, flowers or fruits of a certain plant can be clearly and intensively displayed. These pictures reflect some specific attributes of various plant organs, such as colour, shape, inflorescence, size, and so on (Fig 1). For the sake of explaining the size of fruit, this study called fruit with a diameter greater than 5 cm “large fruit”, that with a diameter of 3–5 centimetres “medium fruit”, that with a diameter of 1–3 centimetres “small fruit” and that with a diameter of less than 1 centimeter “micro fruit”. There are 52 species and 25 families of plants involved in the experiment, which were selected under the guidance of botanical experts (Table 1). Images were taken by researchers or downloaded from official websites related to plants, including the Institute of Botany, Chinese Academy of Sciences (http://www.ibcas.ac.cn), and Chinese Union of Botanical Gardens (https://www.cubg.cn). In order to reduce the interference of different tones on vision, Photoshop CS6 software was used to unify the tones of the pictures without changing the colours of plants.
Table 1. The species, families and numbers of plants used in this study.
Number of family | Family | Number of species | Species |
---|---|---|---|
1 | Cupressaceae | 6 | Fokienia hodginsii |
50 | Metasequoia glyptostroboides | ||
51 | Cunninghamia lanceolata | ||
2 | Cannabaceae | 20 | Celtis sinensis |
3 | Aquifoliaceae | 29 | Ilex rotunda |
4 | Fabaceae | 38 | Cercis chinensis |
5 | Elaeocarpaceae | 30 | Elaeocarpus glabripetalus |
6 | Pittosporaceae | 8 | Pittosporum tobira |
7 | Juglandaceae | 5 | Pterocarya stenoptera |
8 | Buxaceae | 12 | Buxus sinica |
9 | Hamamelidaceae | 4 | Liquidambar formosana |
31 | Distylium racemosum | ||
43 | Loropetalum chinense | ||
10 | Malvaceae | 45 | Hibiscus mutabilis |
11 | Calycanthaceae | 13 | Chimonanthus praecox |
12 | Magnoliaceae | 3 | Yulania liliiflora |
9 | Michelia chapensis | ||
15 | Yulania biondii | ||
23 | Michelia maudiae | ||
42 | Magnolia grandiflora | ||
47 | Liriodendron chinense | ||
49 | Michelia figo | ||
13 | Oleaceae | 11 | Fraxinus hubeiensis |
16 | Ligustrum lucidum | ||
19 | Osmanthus fragrans | ||
48 | Jasminum mesnyi | ||
14 | Anacardiaceae | 17 | Choerospondias axillaris |
15 | Lythraceae | 25 | Punica granatum |
39 | Lagerstroemia indica | ||
16 | Rosaceae | 2 | Cerasus campanulata |
26 | Malus halliana | ||
27 | Amygdalus persica | ||
36 | Chaenomeles speciosa | ||
37 | Photinia×fraseri | ||
52 | Photinia serratifolia | ||
17 | Theaceae | 34 | Camellia sasanqua |
41 | Camellia transarisanensis | ||
46 | Camellia japonica | ||
18 | Ebenaceae | 14 | Diospyros rhombifolia |
19 | Pinaceae | 10 | Pinus thunbergii |
44 | Pinus massoniana | ||
20 | Sapindaceae | 7 | Koelreuteria bipinnata |
22 | Acer buergerianum | ||
32 | Sapindus saponaria | ||
21 | Araliaceae | 1 | Fatsia japonica |
22 | Berberidaceae | 18 | Berberis thunbergii |
21 | Nandina domestica | ||
24 | Mahonia fortunei | ||
23 | Myricaceae | 40 | Myrica rubra |
24 | Ginkgoaceae | 33 | Ginkgo biloba |
25 | Lauraceae | 28 | Cinnamomum japonicum |
35 | Fokienia hodginsii |
Apparatus
The eye movement data are recorded by Tobii Pro Glasses 2 wearable eye tracker, which has a wireless real-time observation function; its super wide-angle scene camera ensures that we can investigate the natural visual observation behavior. The eye tracker uses pupil corneal reflection and binocular dark pupil acquisition technology to track the eye movement of the participant, and supports slip compensation–that is, the data error compensation when the capitulum-mounted module moves slightly during the test–so the data quality will not be affected. With the slip compensation technology and the sampling rate of 100Hz (four eye-tracking cameras, each with a sampling rate of 100Hz), more accurate data will be obtained. EEG data are collected by Mind Wave Mobile capitulumset equipment of the NeuroSky Company, which is connected to smartphone through Bluetooth. EEG data collected by equipment are received and recorded by eegID software, with a recording frequency of once every second, and output in CSV format file. The equipment uses ThinkGear technology to amplify the original EEG signal and filter out the interference caused by environmental noise and pulse and muscle movement, to ensure a clear EEG signal can be collected. It measures EEG signals through a sensor placed on the forehead and a reference electrode contact placed on the ear, and obtains δ, θ, α, β, and γ signal data, which are processed by an integrated chip. It then calculates people’s current mental state in a digital exponential way by using the eSenseblem algorithm, and obtains the quantized eSense indicators value, including attention and meditation. The specific calculation formula is as follows:
Where Pa is attention; γ, β, and α are, respectively, the percentages of γ wave, β wave, and α wave in EEG energy; and m, n, and t are the weight coefficients of γ wave, β wave, and α wave, respectively;
where Pm is meditation; θ, δ, and α are, respectively, the percentage of θ wave, δ wave, and α wave in the energy of the EEG signal; and x, y, and z represent the weight coefficients of θ wave, δ wave, and α wave, respectively.
Methodology
Experiments were carried out in the laboratory of the Tourism College of Central South University of Forestry and Technology and in the meeting room of Changkou Village Committee. To avoid inaccurate eye tracking and EEG acquisition caused by the participants’ nervousness due to their unfamiliarity with laboratory instruments and the environment, and the influence of external factors, before the experiment the participants sat in a comfortable chair and wore the eye tracker and EEG detector at the same time under the guidance of researchers. They then adapted and rested for a certain period of time until the discomfort was eliminated. In addition, the experimental environment was controlled to be quiet, dimly lit with suitable for temperature and humidity [69]. In the experiment, eye movement calibration was carried out first, with the researcher instructing the participants to hold eye movement calibration cards for calibration; the experiment was carried out only after the calibration was completed. The experimental pictures were displayed in random order, and each picture was played for 10 seconds, so the participants only needed to watch freely according to their daily viewing habits [49]. The participants did not know the specific purpose of the experiment [70], and the experiment process was lengthy, allowing the participants to pause at any time and rest for five to 10 seconds before continuing [22, 71]. After the experiment, the participants need to fill in their personal details forms, and the researcher gave each participant a red envelope containing cash to express gratitude.
Indicator selection
After the experiment, a large amount of eye movement and EEG related data were obtained, and the indicator data suitable for this study needed to be selected according to the research needs. In terms of eye movement, taking into account the consistent viewing time of each image and the clearly defined area of interest (AOI), five eye movement indicators were selected in this study, including total duration of fixations (TDF), number of fixations (NF), average duration of fixations (ADF), average pupil size (APS) and average amplitude of saccades (AAS). Among them, total duration of fixations and number of fixations can be regarded as a measure of attractiveness [72, 73] and can reflect the visual preference of participants [74]. The greater the attraction, the greater the value of the two indicators. The average duration of fixations conveys to researchers the time taken for the participants to acquire and process the information displayed by the stimulus. The larger the average of fixations, the greater the information conveyed by the stimulus, and the more time needed by participants to understand [75, 76]. The average pupil size reflects the visual sensitivity of participants to stimuli [18]. The average amplitude of saccades reflects the range of information obtained. The larger the amplitude, the clearer the picture features, meaning the participants could directly reach the target area [77].
In terms of EEG, two indicators of attention and meditation were selected. The attention indicators show the intensity of participants’ mental concentration, and mental states such as distraction, trance, inattention and anxiety will reduce the attention indicators [78]. Meditation indicates the mental relaxation degree of the participants, and there is a clear correlation between increased levels of meditation and decreased brain activity. Mental states such as anxiety, agitation and sensory stimulation will reduce the meditation value [78].
Data analysis
After the experiment, the data of participants with low eye tracking sampling rate were deleted, and 24 valid data of villagers and 30 valid data of students were obtained, totalling 54. By deleting the data with incomplete EEG data records or too many pauses, 17 valid data of villagers and 25 valid data of students were obtained, totalling 42. The eye movement data were processed in Tobii Pro Lab software based on leaves, flowers and fruits as AOI, and then the required eye movement data was exported. For EEG data, first the data of the first 30s was deleted when watching the warm up pictures, then the data of each experimental picture for 10s was intercepted, and the average value of all tested data per second was calculated for subsequent analysis. SPSS 20.0 was used for statistical analysis, nonparametric testing and correlation analysis.
Results
Eye movement data
Organs, species and families
According to the results of the Mann-Whitney U-test, on the whole there are significant differences between villagers and students in five eye movement indicators respectively (p<0.05). The students’ total duration of fixations, number of fixations, average pupil size and average amplitude of saccades is much higher than those of villagers, but the average duration of fixations is the reverse (Table 2). Multivariate analysis of variance showed that there was no significant interaction between different environmental background and plant organs and their features in eye movement indicators (p>0.05).
Table 2. Mean value of eye movement indicators of villagers and students as a whole.
Varieties | TDF | NF | ADF | APS | AAS | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | |
Villagers | 3.872 | 2.442 | 11.32 | 7.519 | 0.371 | 0.348 | 3.950 | 0.990 | 4.250 | 2.020 |
Students | 5.109 | 2.517 | 16.89 | 8.267 | 0.321 | 0.181 | 4.264 | 0.725 | 5.425 | 2.119 |
TDF: Total duration of fixations; NF: Number of fixations; ADF: Average duration of fixations; APS: Average pupil size; AAS: Average amplitude of saccades.
With respect to plant organs, the eye movement indicators of villagers and students have the same trend, and Kruskal-Wallis H-test results show that there are significant differences in their total duration of fixations, number of fixations and average duration of fixations (p<0.05), but no differences in their average pupil size or average amplitude of saccades. Both groups had the longest total duration of fixations and the biggest number of fixations when watching leaves, followed by flowers and fruits, while the order of average duration of fixations was completely opposite, and fruits had the longest. On the whole, all the indicators of students are much higher than those of villagers except for the average duration of fixations (Table 3).
Table 3. Mean results of eye movement indicators of two populations on different plant organs.
Groups | Varieties | TDF | NF | ADF | APS | AAS |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Villagers | Leaf | 4.743 | 14.837 | 0.347 | 3.958 | 4.328 |
Flower | 3.715 | 10.400 | 0.381 | 3.941 | 4.215 | |
Fruit | 3.152 | 8.710 | 0.384 | 3.950 | 4.207 | |
Students | Leaf | 6.091 | 21.300 | 0.303 | 4.296 | 5.452 |
Flower | 5.057 | 16.310 | 0.329 | 4.229 | 5.516 | |
Fruit | 4.179 | 13.050 | 0.330 | 4.268 | 5.305 |
TDF: Total duration of fixations; NF: Number of fixations; ADF: Average duration of fixations; APS: Average pupil size; AAS: Average amplitude of saccades.
In terms of species, there are significant differences between villagers and students in their total duration of fixations, number of fixations and average amplitude of saccades respectively (p<0.05), and the trends are basically the same. For the first two indicators, Photinia×fraseri has the highest value, Metasequoia glyptostroboides, Photinia serratifolia, Koelreuteria bipinnata and Cunninghamia lanceolata are also at the forefront. Meanwhile, villagers have higher values of the indicators on Pinus thunbergii, Myrica rubra and Camellia japonica, while students have higher values of the indicators on Yulania biondii, Cercis chinensis and Hibiscus mutabilis, and larger saccades on Pinus massoniana, Acer buergerianum, Cunninghamia lanceolata, Jasminum mesnyi, Choerospondias axillaris, Camellia japonica, and Sapindus saponaria. In addition, there are differences in average duration of fixations among villagers (p = 0.000<0.05) with higher values of Chimonanthus praecox, Pinus thunbergii and Diospyros rhombifolia, and differences in average pupil size among students (p = 0.000<0.05) with larger average pupil size on Pinus massoniana, Photinia×fraseri and Malus halliana (Fig 2).
Like species, there are significant differences in families in total duration of fixations, number of fixations and average amplitude of saccades between the two groups (p<0.05). Fabaceae, Myricaceae, Malvaceae, Araliaceae, Cupressaceae, Pinaceae and Theaceae are all in the forefront. Both groups have larger saccades in Anacardiaceae, Juglandaceae, Ginkgoaceae, Sapindaceae, Cupressaceae, Araliaceae and Theaceae. There are differences in average duration of fixations among villagers (p = 0.000<0.05), with higher values in Calycanthaceae, Ebenaceae and Pinaceae. There are also differences in the average pupil size of students (p = 0.014<0.05). The pupil sizes of Pinaceae, Ebenaceae and Calycanthaceae are larger (Fig 3).
Leaves
There are significant differences in total duration of fixations and number of fixations between the two groups in different leaf shapes (p<0.05). The highest average value of the two indicators is aciculiform, followed by strip, cordiform and sector. The difference is that the average value of the student group is also higher in the jacket-shaped leaves. In addition, the average pupil size of different leaf shapes is different among students (p = 0.004<0.05), and the average values of aciculiform, strip and sector are higher still (Fig 4). In terms of leaf texture, there is a significant difference in the total duration of fixations of villagers (p = 0.004<0.05), and the average value of leather leaves is slightly higher than that of paper leaves. The average pupil size of students is different (p = 0.001<0.05), and the leathery leaves are higher than the paper leaves (Fig 5). There is also a significant difference among students in the average pupil size of leaves with or without leaf lobes (p = 0.027<0.05), and the results show that the average value of leaves without leaf lobes is higher.
Flowers
There are significant differences between villagers and students in total duration of fixations and number of fixations for different inflorescences respectively (p<0.05), with the highest values of each indicator in capitulum, followed by solitary flower and panicle. The difference is that villagers have higher fixation time and number on cymes, while students have a higher number of fixations on corymb. The average amplitude of saccades of villagers is different (p = 0.039<0.05), with the saccade of corymb, umbel and solitary flower larger than that of other inflorescences. Correspondingly, villagers and students have the longest total fixation time when watching solitary flowers, followed by limited inflorescences and infinite inflorescences (Fig 6). There are differences between the two groups in the total duration of fixations and frequency, and the average amplitude of saccades of different colours respectively (p<0.05). The average arrangement of the first two indicators of the two groups is basically the same: purple is first, followed by white and yellow. But there are differences between the two groups in saccade amplitude. Except for yellow and red, the villagers have the largest saccade amplitude when they look at green, while the students have a larger saccade amplitude when they look at pink (Fig 7).
Fruits
On the whole, compared with students, the features of fruit have a greater influence on villagers. In terms of fruit size, there are significant differences among the five indicators of villagers (p<0.05). The total duration of fixations of medium fruit and micro fruit is the highest, while the average duration of fixations of large and medium fruit is longer, and the average pupil size and saccade amplitude of large and small fruit are larger. In the student group, there is significant difference (p<0.05) in other indicators except for the average duration of fixations. Similarly, the total duration of fixations and number of fixations of medium and micro fruit are at the forefront, and the average pupil size of large and small fruit is larger. But the difference is that the average eye saccade of students to small and micro fruit is larger.
For fruit shapes, there are significant differences in total duration of fixations and number of fixations between the two groups (p<0.05). The values of spathulate and reniform are the highest, followed by the values of wing and spherical. There are also differences in the average amplitude of saccades of villagers (p = 0.001<0.05), and the saccade amplitude of wing, spathulate and spindle is larger. With regard to fruit types, there are differences in all the indicators of eye movement among villagers (p<0.05), but there is no difference in average duration of fixations and average pupil size among students (p>0.05). Villagers, as well as students, have the most total duration of fixations and number of fixations when watching pomes, pods, samaras and berries; the average duration of fixations and the pupil size of achene and aggregate fruit are higher; and the average eye saccade of nuts, pomes and pods is higher. In terms of colour, there are differences between villagers and students in average duration of fixations and frequency respectively(p<0.05). Both groups have the same average result of colour, with the highest values of black and red. There are also differences in average duration of fixations of villagers (p = 0.001<0.05), and brown and green have the longest fixation time (Fig 8).
EEG data
The digital indicator eSense, which is calculated and processed by eSenseblem algorithm, represents people’s attention level and meditation level with specific values between 1 and 100. In this study, the values of the two indicators of the two groups are between 40 and 60, indicating that their indicators values are in the general range–that is, the baseline range value. According to the results of Mann-Whitney U-test, on the whole there are significant differences between villagers and students in two EEG indicators respectively (p<0.05), and students’ attention level is much higher than that of villagers, while their meditation degree is lower (Table 4).
Table 4. Mean value of EEG indicators and non-parametric test results of villagers and students as a whole.
Groups | Attention | Meditation | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | p | Mean | SD | p | |
Villagers | 46.48 | 4.895 | 0.000 | 56.56 | 4.645 | 0.038 |
Students | 50.23 | 4.294 | 56.20 | 3.212 |
Organs, species and families
On the two EEG indicators, the interaction effect between different living environments and different organ types, species and families are very significant (p<0.05). Separately, there are significant differences (p<0.05) between villagers and students in organ types, species and families, except that there is no difference in students’ meditation degree on different organs (p = 0.280>0.05).
The results of Kruskal-Wallis H-test show that villagers have the highest attention (M = 47.38) and meditation (M = 57.37) when watching flowers, followed by fruits and leaves, while students are the opposite: their highest attention lies in leaves (M = 50.68), followed by fruits and flowers. As to species, both villagers and students pay more attention to Magnolia grandiflora and Fokienia hodginsii, while villagers also pay more attention to Choerospondias axillaris, Liquidambar formosana and Pinus thunbergii, and students pay more attention to Camellia japonica, Ginkgo biloba and Yulania liliiflora. There is no overlap in meditation degree between the two groups. Villagers have a higher degree of meditation on Yulania biondii, Koelreuteria bipinnata, Diospyros rhombifolia, Pinus thunbergii and Acer buergerianum, while students have a higher degree of meditation on Magnolia grandiflora, Metasequoia glyptostroboides, Sapindus saponaria, Myrica rubra and Jasminum mesnyi (Fig 9).
For families, students have higher attention and meditation on Myricaceae and Buxaceae, with Ginkgoaceae, Pittosporaceae and Elaeocarpaceae also at the forefront, while villagers have the highest attention and meditation on Anacardiaceae, Pinaceae and Ebenaceae (Fig 10).
Leaves
Multivariate analysis of variance shows that there are differences (p<0.05) on attention in the interaction effects of different living environment with shape, colour and texture of leaves, but there are no differences in other features or in meditation. For leaf shape, there are significant differences (p<0.05) between villagers and students. Both groups have higher attention on jacket-shape and cordiform, and have the highest degree of meditation on jacket-shape and strip. Villagers also have higher attention on aciculiform and strip, while students have higher attention on sector and lanceolar. As to leaf colour, villagers have significant differences (p<0.05) in attention and meditation. Red leaves have the highest mean value, followed by yellow and green leaves. Students only have differences (p = 0.015<0.05) in attention, which are followed by yellow, green and red. On leaf texture, there is a significant difference (p = 0.004<0.05) in students’ attention. The paper leaf (M = 51.45) is higher than the leather leaf (M = 50.30), contrary to the results for the villagers. But there is no difference (p = 0.566>0.05) between the two leaf textures. There is a significant difference (p = 0.017<0.05) in villagers’ attention on leaf lobes. The average value of leaves with leaf lobes (M = 47.05) is higher than that of leaves without leaf lobes (M = 45.30), which is consistent with students, but there is no difference (p = 0.260>0.05) in students’ attention (Fig 11).
Flowers
The interaction effect between different living environment and all features of flowers with regard to attention and meditation is significant (p = 0.000<0.05). In different inflorescences, there are significant differences between villagers and students in the two indicators respectively (p<0.05). Both groups have the highest attention on umbels, and the villagers have higher average values for catkins, spicas and solitary flowers, while students have higher attention on racemes, capitulums and cymes. In terms of meditation degree, the average value is higher in spica and catkin. Villagers and students also have a high degree of meditation in panicles and corymbs respectively. Furthermore, for the inflorescence types, there is only a significant difference in meditation degree between the two populations (p<0.05), and the infinite inflorescence has the highest value, followed by solitary flower and limited inflorescence. With regard to flower colours, there are significant differences between villagers and students in the two indicators respectively (p<0.05). Villagers are more focused on pink, green and yellow, while the students are more focused on red, purple and green. In meditation, the villagers have the highest values of green, pink and white, while students have the highest values for white, red and green in turn (Fig 12).
Fruit
Except for fruit colour, the interaction effect between different living environment and other features of fruit is very significant (p<0.05), and there are significant differences (p<0.05) between villagers and students in most features respectively. In terms of fruit size, large fruit has the largest average value of the two indicators among the two groups. In terms of attention, villagers pay more attention to medium fruit, followed by small and micro fruits, and students pay more attention to medium, micro and small fruits. In terms of meditation degree, villagers are highest on small fruit, followed by medium and micro fruits, while students are highest on micro, medium and small fruits. With regard to fruit shapes, villagers and students are more focused and relaxed on oval and spindle; third is wing shape for villagers and reniform for students. For fruit types, villagers pay higher attention to cones, nuts and aggregate fruits, and the meditation degree of samaras is the highest. Students have the highest values of aggregate fruits and cones, and their attention and meditation degree of pods and samaras are also at the forefront. In terms of fruit colours, there is no difference in the meditation degree of villagers, but they focus more on brown, yellow and green. Students are similar to villagers, focusing more on brown, yellow and black. In meditation degree, the values of brown, black and red are higher for students (Fig 13).
Correlation analysis
The Pearson correlation analysis between the five eye movement indicators and the two EEG indicators showed that there was a strong positive correlation (p = 0.000, Pr = 0.96) between total duration of fixations and number of fixations, a significant weak negative correlation (p = 0.015, Pr = –0.221) between average pupil size and attention, and a significant weak positive correlation(p = 0.028, Pr = 0.200) between average duration of fixations and meditation degree.
Discussion
Visual preference and difference through eye movement
There is no significant interaction between living environment and local plant features in indicators of eye movement–that is, living environment will not affect people’s eye movement features when watching different plant pictures, which is consistent with the research results of Luo et al. [30], who points out that the aesthetic preferences of Xiamen local high school students and Xinjiang high school students in Xiamen for new and old landscapes tend to be consistent. Besides living environment, Paraskevopoulou et al. [23] used eye tracking technology to study the influence of seasonal colour changes of plants on patients with mental disorders, and also found that age and gender did not affect the preferences of participants. We believe it may be difficult to discover the specific impact of the environment on people only by using eye movement indicators.
People prefer attractive stimuli, which lead to a longer fixation time and a larger number of fixations [79]. At the same time, this study found that the two indicators have a strong positive correlation, so they can jointly reflect the same phenomenon: the preference level of participants [80]. As for species, Photinia×fraseri, Metasequoia glyptostroboides, Photinia serratifolia, Koelreuteria bipinnata and Cunninghamia lanceolata have higher visual appeal, while for families, both groups prefer Fabaceae, Myricaceae, Malvaceae, Araliaceae, Cupressaceae, Pinaceae and Theaceae. In terms of organ types, both groups are most easily attracted by leaves, followed by flowers and fruits. This is different from the research results of Rahnema et al. [19] and Yin et al. [81], which indicate that flowers are more attractive than leaves because the visual attention of participants is driven by many different factors that have nothing to do with subjective and declarative preferences [41]. For example, Lindemann-Matthies and Bose [82] found that the participants indicated in the questionnaire that they liked plants with large or colourful flowers, but when they were asked to choose 25 kinds of local wild plants from 779 kinds to create their favorite lawn patch, only one-third of the flowering plants were selected. Among the different features of leaves, aciculiform, strip, cordiform and sector shapes have higher preference rates. Lindemann-Matthies and Bose [82] also point out that leaf shape has a significant impact on people’s preferences. Villagers prefer leather leaves a with stiff texture and strong lustre, while students have no obvious preference between leather leaves and paper leaves. Among the different features of flowers, capitulums, solitary flowers and panicles attract the attention of both groups. Furthermore, they both prefer solitary flowers, followed by finite inflorescences and infinite inflorescences. In terms of flower colours, purple, white, yellow and red have higher preference rates. This discovery supports the results of other studies [19], which show that red, purple and yellow have the highest preference rates. Among the different types of fruits, the two groups prefer medium and micro fruits, and spathulate, reniform, wing and spherical fruits have higher visual appeal. Compared with ellipse and oval shapes, they are more prominent and unique in shape, and tend to arouse curiosity, which impacts visual appeal [41]. Fruit types such as pomes, pods, samaras and berries have a higher preference rate, and black and red colours are attractive to the two groups. Red can promote people’s optimistic mood, while dark colours can produce a relaxed mood [83].
On the whole, students’ average duration of fixation is shorter than that of villagers, and they can grasp the information displayed by the pictures more quickly, which may be influenced by their educational background [27, 47]. There is no difference in species and families among students, but Chimonanthus praecox, Pinus thunbergii, Diospyros rhombifolia, Calycanthaceae, Ebenaceae and Pinaceae show a lot of information to villagers, and they need to spend more time to understand it. In terms of plant organs, although leaves have a high preference, the most prominent feature of plants is the differentiation of flowers and fruits in different seasons [16], so the various features of leaves will not affect the processing time of the information they convey, and require considerable time. However, while the features of fruit convey much information, this only affects villagers, to whom large and medium fruits convey the most information, and achene and aggregate fruits need more time to analyse, as brown and green fruits do.
The average pupil size of students is larger than that of villagers as a whole, indicating that students’ visual response to stimulation is stronger. Different features of leaves have different effects on students’ visual stimulation, and they are more sensitive to aciculiform, leather leaves and leaves without leaf lobes. Various features of fruit have a greater influence on villagers. Achene and aggregate fruits are highly stimulating and, like students, villagers are sensitive to large and small fruits.
The average amplitude of students’ saccade is much larger than that of villagers–that is, students can find and lock the AOI more easily, and have a more sensitive response to the stimulation of plants, which corresponds to the difference between experts and non-experts discussed by Pihel et al. [47]. Specifically, there are obvious differences among villagers’ eye saccade. In terms of flowers, corymbs, umbels and solitary flowers are more obvious to them. Yellow and red are more likely to attract the attention of the two groups, because red can stimulate and distract visual attention [83], while yellow flowers can be used to create pleasant places [84]. In terms of fruit, the features of wing, spathulate and spindle shapes are obvious to villagers. At the same time, like students, nuts, pomes and pods are more prominent.
Physiological characteristics and differences through EEG
We found a weak negative correlation between average pupil size and attention: when the participants are stimulated by vision, the pupil response increases [85], while the mental attention level decreases to some extent. This is different from the research results of some scholars [57]. The results also show a weak positive correlation between the average duration of fixations and the meditation value, which means that when the participants need more time to receive and process a large amount of information transmitted by experimental stimulus materials, their average duration of fixations will increase [76], while the visual stimulus will decrease and the meditation value will increase. It is understandable that the correlation between eye movement indicators and EEG indicators is weak. Garcia-Madariaga et al. [86] mentions that no connection exists between explicit preference measured by eye tracking and attention measured by EEG, while Vettori et al. [87] reached a completely opposite conclusion, pointing out that eye tracking and EEG measurement are strongly correlated. Therefore, the correlation between them needs further research.
On the whole, students’ mental attention level when watching plant stimulation is much higher than that of villagers, which may be due to the influence of educational background [27]. Students might do some plant recognition activities during watching, while villagers simply browse the pictures. Accordingly, students are engaged in more brain activities, and the mental meditation degree is lower than that of villagers. There is a very significant interaction effect between different living environment and various native plant features in EEG indicators. It can be seen from the visual indicators alone that there is no difference between students and villagers when watching plant pictures, and the addition of EEG further supplements and reveals the real situation [76]. Therefore, there are in fact very different physiological reactions between the two groups when watching. This is similar to the findings of other scholars. In the eye tracking data, there is no interaction between the group and the stimulus types, but the EEG indicators are the opposite, and the difference between the two groups can clearly be seen [87]. As to organ types, when villagers watch flowers, the two EEG indicators are the highest, followed by fruit and leaves, while students have completely different attention. For leaf colours, red leaves can increase villagers’ attention and meditation, and green leaves have the lowest corresponding value, but green leaves can improve students’ attention, while red leaves have the lowest effect. In terms of flowers, besides umbels, catkins, spicas and solitary flowers also can promote villagers’ attention, while racemes, capitulums and cymes can attract students’ attention. In this sense, there is a significant difference between these two groups. It can be seen that obvious features and bright colours can relieve the spirit of villagers [75], and improve their attention and meditation, such as flowers with catkins or spicas and red leaves. On the contrary, relatively less prominent plant features and simpler structure, such as large areas of green leaves and flowers with racemes and capitulum, can increase students’ EEG values.
This study introduced two physiological technologies, eye tracking and EEG, which transcended the limitations of the previous studies only from the perspective of ecology or using subjective measurement methods. At the same time, it was found that different environments would have certain influences on people’s viewing of plants. These not only provide an objective technical support for the selection of suitable plants in different landscape construction, which makes the plant selection more rule-based and can even enable generalization, but also provide a new research perspective for landscape preference-related fields.
Limitations and prospects
However, this study has some limitations. First, we have not completely covered all the features of plant organs in the study. Future studies need to further improve the selection of experimental plants, in order to achieve full coverage of features as much as possible. On this basis, it is necessary to use plant individuals, populations or communities as experimental stimulation materials to make research closer to people’s natural observation habits. At the same time, it is necessary to enrich more group samples in different living environments, to provide scientific suggestions for the use of plants in the process of creating more different landscapes. In addition, the EEG detection instrument used in this study is portable and simple, and its accuracy and synchronization with eye tracking need to be improved. More advanced EEG data instruments should be used, which can monitor 14, 64 or 128 channels, and can be directly connected with the eye tracker to synchronize more accurately in further research.
Conclusion
In this study, villagers and students were taken as research groups, and their preferences and differences on plant organ features were explored by using eye tracking and EEG technology. According to the eye movement indicators, there is no obvious difference in preferences between villagers and students. Both groups prefer leaves, especially aciculiform and leather leaves, followed by flowers and fruits. Solitary flowers, capitulums and purple flowers have the highest preference rates, and medium-sized fruits with diameters of 3–5 centimeters, spathulate, black and pome fruits have the highest visual appeal. However, the application of EEG technology further reveals significant physiological and psychological differences between villagers and students when they watch the stimuli. On the whole, students’ attention is much higher than that of villagers, while their meditation degree is the opposite. This study has certain significance for plant selection in landscape construction in rural areas and campuses in China. We think Photinia×fraseri, Metasequoia glyptostroboides, Photinia serratifolia, Koelreuteria bipinnata and Cunninghamia lanceolata are the preferred plants in both environments. For rural areas, Pinus thunbergii, Myrica rubra and Camellia japonica are also highly recommended, and plants with obvious features and bright colours are also suitable for rural landscape construction. On campus, Yulania biondii, Cercis chinensis and Hibiscus mutabilis are recommended species, and plants without too-prominent characteristics and with simple structures are also suitable for campus landscape construction.
Acknowledgments
We sincerely thank all the subjects who participated in our experiment, enabling us to successfully complete this research. We also need to appreciate all the students and teachers participating in the research, who helped the authors to process data and perform this research.
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.
Funding Statement
ND, YZ and JL received support from the Ministry of Science and Technology of The People's Republic of China grants 2019YFD1100400. ND received support from the Central South University of Forestry and Technology grants CX202102098, SZ received support from the Hunan Provincial Department of Education grants 19B582. HX received support from the Hunan Natural Science Foundation grants 2021JJ60014. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Velarde MD, Fry G, Tveit M. Health effects of viewing landscapes–landscape types in environmental psychology. Urban Forestry and Urban Greening. 2007;6: 199–212. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Daniel TC. Whither scenic beauty? Visual landscape quality assessment in the 21st century. Landscape and Urban Planning. 2001; 54: 267–281. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Ribe RG. Perceptions of forestry alternatives in the US Pacific Northwest: Information effects and acceptability distribution analysis. Journal of Environmental Psychology. 2006;26: 100–115. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Yumin Y, Xiaodong Z, Yingbi X, Haiyan Y, Xiang S. Assessing the visual quality of urban waterfront landscapes: The case of Hefei, China. Acta Ecologica Sinica. 2012;32: 5836–5845. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Schmid WA. The emerging role of visual resource assessment and visualisation in landscape planning in Switzerland. Landscape and Urban Planning. 2001; 54: 213–221. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Zube EH, Sell JL, Taylor JG. Landscape perception: Research, application and theory. Landscape Planning. 1982;9: 1–33. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Zube EH, Pitt DG. Cross-cultural perceptions of scenic and heritage landscapes. Landscape Planning. 2015;8: 69–87. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Naveh Z, Lieberman AS. Landscape ecology: Theory and application. Heidelberg: Springer, 1994. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Pazhouhanfar M, Kamal MS. Effect of predictors of visual preference as characteristics of urban natural landscapes in increasing perceived restorative potential. Urban Forestry and Urban Greening. 2014;13: 145–151. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2013.08.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Gholami Y, Taghvaei SH, Norouzian-Maleki S, Mansouri Sepehr R. Identifying the stimulus of visual perception based on eye-tracking in urban parks: Case study of Mellat Park in Tehran. Journal of Forest Research. 2021;26: 91–100. doi: 10.1080/13416979.2021.1876286 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Stehouwer R, Visscher J, Iverson N et al. Homeowner preferences for wooded front yards and backyards: Implications for carbon storage. Landscape and Urban Planning. 2016;146: 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2015.09.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Li J, Zhang Z, Jing F, Gao J, Ma J, Shao G, et al. An evaluation of urban green space in Shanghai, China, using eye tracking. Urban Forestry and Urban Greening. 2020;56: 126903. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2020.126903 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Hu S, Yue H, Zhou Z. Preferences for urban stream landscapes: Opportunities to promote unmanaged riparian vegetation. Urban Forestry and Urban Greening. 2018;38: 114–123. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2018.12.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Tong QI, Wang Y, Wang W. A review on visual landscape study in foreign countries. Progress in Geography. 2013;32: 975–983. doi: 10.11820/dlkxjz.2013.06.015 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Zhao J, Xu W, Li R. Visual preference of trees: The effects of tree attributes and seasons. Urban Forestry and Urban Greening. 2017;25: 19–25. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2017.04.015 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Tarakci-Eren E, Duzenli T. Determination of the visual preference levels and perceptual differences in the appearance of certain taxa in different seasons. Fresenius Environmental Bulletin. 2017;26: 14. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Rafi ZN, Kazemi F, Tehranifar A. Public preferences toward water-wise landscape design in a summer season. Urban Forestry and Urban Greening. 2019;48: 126563. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2019.126563 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Zheng J, Tarin WKM, Jiang D, Li M, Ye J, Chen L et al. Which ornamental features of bamboo plants will attract the people most? Urban Forestry and Urban Greening. 2021;61: 127101. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2021.127101 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Rahnema S, Sedaghathoor S, Allahyari MS, Damalas CA, Bilali HE. Preferences and emotion perceptions of ornamental plant species for green space designing among urban park users in Iran. Urban Forestry and Urban Greening. 2019;39: 98–108. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2018.12.007 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Tang XM, Wang XR. Landscape visual environment assessment (LVEA): Concept, origin and development. Journal of Shanghai Jiaotong University(Agricultural Science). 2007, http://en.cnki.com.cn/Article_en/CJFDTOTAL-SHNX200703000.htm [Google Scholar]
- 21.Conrad L, Christie M, Fazey I. Incorporating people’s perception into landscape planning: Ethical challenges in dealing with diversity of opinion within a community. Nephrology Dialysis Transplantation. 2009;27: 3516–23. doi: 10.1093/ndt/gfs128 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Dupont L, Antrop M, Eetvelde VV. Does landscape related expertise influence the visual perception of landscape photographs? Implications for participatory landscape planning and management. Landscape and Urban Planning. 2015;141: 68–77. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2015.05.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Paraskevopoulou AT, Kamperi E, Demiris N, Economou M, Papageorgiou C. The impact of seasonal colour change in planting on patients with psychotic disorders using biosensors. Urban Forestry and Urban Greening. 2018;36: 50–56. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Ren X. Consensus in factors affecting landscape preference: A case study based on a cross-cultural comparison. Journal of Environmental Management. 2019;252: 109622. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvman.2019.109622 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Strobele M, Hunziker M. Are suburbs perceived as rural villages? Landscape-related residential preferences in Switzerland. Landscape and Urban Planning. 2017;163: 67–79. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2017.02.015 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Fu XY. Analysis of landscape preference characteristics of low familiarity users in urban public open space. Urban Problems. 2018; 10(11): 1128. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Keskin M, Ooms K, Dogru AO, De Maeyer P. Exploring the cognitive load of expert and novice map users using EEG and eye tracking. International Journal of Geo-Information. 2020;9: 429. doi: 10.3390/ijgi9070429 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kaplan R, Herbert EJ. Cultural and sub-cultural comparisons in preferences for natural settings. Landscape and Urban Planning. 1987;14: 281–293. doi: 10.1016/0169-2046(87)90040-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Kim S, Kim J, Kim B. Immersive virtual reality-aided conjoint analysis of urban square preference by living environment. Sustainability. 2020;12: 6440. doi: 10.3390/su12166440 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Luo T, Yang F, Huang L, Min XU. Where goes the homesickness? Contemplation on the results of a comparative study on landscape preference between high school students from Xiamen and Xinjiang. Chinese Landscape Architecture. 2019; 35:98–103. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Wang ZJ, Hu LH, Li SH. Investigation on influences of prototype landscape on people’s environmental perceptions. Chinese Landscape Architecture. 2010; 26:46–48. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Gong P, Zhang L, Gu X. Influences of prototype landscape on people’s perceptions of urban plant landscape. Scientia Silvae Sinicae. 2016. doi: 10.11707/j.1001-7488.20160908 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Huang S, van den Bosch CK, Fu W, Qi J, Chen Z, Zhu Z, et al. Does adding local tree elements into dwellings enhance individuals’ homesickness? Scenario-visualisation for developing sustainable rural landscapes. Sustainability. 2018;10: 3943. doi: 10.3390/su10113943 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Domon G. Landscape as resource: Consequences, challenges and opportunities for rural development. Landscape and Urban Planning. 2011;100: 338–340. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2011.02.014 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Roman LA, Fristensky JP, Eisenman TS, Greenfield EJ, Lundgren RE, Cerwinka CE et al. Growing canopy on a college campus: Understanding urban forest change through archival records and aerial photography. Environmental Management. 2017;60: 1042–1061. doi: 10.1007/s00267-017-0934-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Hami A, Abdi B. Students’ landscaping preferences for open spaces for their campus environment. Indoor and Built Environment. 2021;30: 87–98. doi: 10.1177/1420326X19887207 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Deng Z., Theories techniques and applied researches about eye-movement psychology. Journal of Nanjing Normal University (Social Science Edition). 2005; 90–95. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Chen QR, Wang MJ, Liu HN, Tan DL, Deng Z, Xu XD. Combination of eye movement and ERP methods in language cognition: Theory, method, and application. Advances in Psychological Science. 2011;19: 264–273. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Riggs LA, Ratliff F, Cornsweet JC, Cornsweet TN. The disappearance of steadily fixated visual test objects. Journal of the Optical Society of America. 1953;43: 495–501. doi: 10.1364/josa.43.000495 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Chen GP, Hu AX, Li YH, Yu HZ. Review of linguistics understanding based on eye tracking system. Journal of Northwest University for Nationalities (Natural Science). 2011; 32:49–55. doi: 10.14084/j.cnki.cn62-1188/n.2011.02.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.García-Madariaga J, Blasco López M-F, Burgos IM, Virto NR. Do isolated packaging variables influence consumers’ attention and preferences? Physiology & Behavior. 2019;200: 96–103. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.04.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Khan MQ, Lee S. Gaze and eye tracking: Techniques and applications in ADAS. Sensors. 2019;19: 5540. doi: 10.3390/s19245540 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Krieber M, Bartl-Pokorny KD, Pokorny FB, Einspieler C, Langmann A, Körner C et al. The relation between reading skills and eye movement patterns in adolescent readers: Evidence from a regular orthography. PLOS ONE. 2016;11: e0145934. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0145934 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.McGowan VA, White SJ, Paterson KB. The effects of interword spacing on the eye movements of young and older readers. Journal of Cognitive Psychology. 2015;27: 609–621. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2014.988157 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Stevens E, Johnson AL, Leshner G, Sun F, Kim S, Eleanor LSL, et al. People in e-cigarette ads attract more attention: An eye-tracking study. Tobacco Regulatory Science. 2020;6: 105–117. doi: 10.18001/trs.6.2.3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Dupont L, Antrop M, Eetvelde VV. Eye-tracking analysis in landscape perception research: Influence of photograph properties and landscape characteristics. Landscape Research. 2014;39: 417–432. doi: 10.1080/01426397.2013.773966 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Pihel J, Ode Sang Å, Hagerhall C, Nyström M. Expert and novice group differences in eye movements when assessing biodiversity of harvested forests. Forest Policy and Economics. 2015;56: 20–26. doi: 10.1016/j.forpol.2015.04.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Franěk M, Petružálek J, Šefara D. Eye movements in viewing urban images and natural images in diverse vegetation periods. Urban Forestry and Urban Greening. 2019;46: 126477. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2019.126477 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Dupont L, Ooms K, Antrop M, Van Eetvelde V. Comparing saliency maps and eye-tracking focus maps: The potential use in visual impact assessment based on landscape photographs. Landscape and Urban Planning. 2016;148: 17–26. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2015.12.007 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Thomas Z, Ramsy N, Michael I. A consumer neuroscience study of conscious and subconscious destination preference. Scientific Reports. 2019;9: 15102–15102. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-51567-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Ke LI, Gong C. Application of brain–computer interface in evaluation of trail’s landscape. Central South Forest Inventory and Planning. 2015; 34: 34–38+48. doi: 10.16166/j.cnki.cn43-1095.2015.02.009 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Alexandra L, Monica R, Aleksandra S, Michael E. Pattern classification of EEG signals reveals perceptual and attentional states. PLOS ONE. 2017;12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Kim K, Duc NT, Min C, Lee B. EEG microstate features for schizophrenia classification. PLOS ONE. 2021;16. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0251842 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Nadja R, Kay J, Thomas K, Mara K, Martinus H, Werner S et al. Shifted coupling of EEG driving frequencies and fMRI resting state networks in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. PLOS ONE. 2013;8: e76604. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076604 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Shahbakhti M, Beiramvand M, Rejer I, Augustyniak P, Marozas V. Simultaneous eye blink characterization and elimination from low-channel prefrontal EEG signals enhances driver drowsiness detection. IEEE Journal of Biomedical and Health Informatics. 2021. doi: 10.1109/JBHI.2021.3096984 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Tuncer T, Dogan S, Ertam F, Subasi A. A dynamic center and multi threshold point based stable feature extraction network for driver fatigue detection utilizing EEG signals. Cognitive Neurodynamics. 2021. doi: 10.1007/s11571-020-09601-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Slanzi G, Balazs JA, Velásquez JD. Combining eye tracking, pupil dilation and EEG analysis for predicting web users’ click intention. Information Fusion. 2017;35: 51–57. doi: 10.1016/j.inffus.2016.09.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Aldayel M, Ykhlef M, Al-Nafjan A. Recognition of consumer preference by analysis and classification EEG signals. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2021. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.604639 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Balconi M, Venturella I, Sebastiani R, Angioletti L. Touching to feel: Brain activity during in-store consumer experience. Frontiers in Psychology. 2021. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.653011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Deng L, Li X, Luo H, Fu E-K, Ma J, Sun L-X, et al. Empirical study of landscape types, landscape elements and landscape components of the urban park promoting physiological and psychological restoration. Urban Forestry and Urban Greening. 2020;48. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2019.126488 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Yuting W, Shujian W, Ming X. The function of colour and structure based on EEG features in landscape recognition. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021;18. doi: 10.3390/IJERPH18094866 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Jiang M, Hassan A, Chen Q, Liu Y. Effects of different landscape visual stimuli on psychophysiological responses in Chinese students. Indoor and Built Environment. 2020;29: 1006–1016. doi: 10.1177/1420326X19870578 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Leighton J, Dalvit S. The branded mind: What neuroscience really tells us about the puzzle of the brain and the brand. International Journal of Advertising. 2011;30: 723. doi: 10.2501/IJA-30-4-723-725 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Makin A, Poliakoff E, Ackerley R, El-Deredy W. Covert tracking: A combined ERP and fixational eye movement study. PLOS ONE. 2012;7. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038479 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Akiba HT, Costa MF, Gomes JS, Oda E, Simurro PB, Dias AM. Neural correlates of preference: A transmodal validation study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2019;13: 73. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00073 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Coeterier JF. A photo validity test. Journal of Environmental Psychology. 1983;3: 315–323. [Google Scholar]
- 67.Dachary-Bernard J, Rambonilaza T. Choice experiment, multiple programmes contingent valuation and landscape preferences: How can we support the land use decision making process? Land Use Policy. 2012;29: 846–854. [Google Scholar]
- 68.Meitner MJ. Scenic beauty of river views in the Grand Canyon: Relating perceptual judgments to locations. Landscape and Urban Planning. 2004. doi: 10.1016/S0169-2046(03)00115-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Hollenstein N, Rotsztejn J, Troendle M, Pedroni A, Zhang C, Langer N. ZuCo, a simultaneous EEG and eye-tracking resource for natural sentence reading. Scientific Data. 2018;5: 180291. doi: 10.1038/sdata.2018.291 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Ding X, Yue X, Zheng R, Bi C, Li D, Yao G. Classifying major depression patients and healthy controls using EEG, eye tracking and galvanic skin response data. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2019;251: 156–161. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.03.058 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Orden KV, Jung T, Makeig S. Combined eye activity measures accurately estimate changes in sustained visual task performance. Biological Psychology. 2000;52: 221–240. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(99)00043-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Behe BK, Huddleston PT, Childs KL, Chen J, Muraro IS. Seeing through the forest: The gaze path to purchase. PLOS ONE. 2020;15: e0240179. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0240179 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Behe BK, Bae M, Huddleston PT, Sage L. The effect of involvement on visual attention and product choice. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services. 2015;24: 10–21. [Google Scholar]
- 74.Zamani H, Abas A, Mamin MK. Eye tracking application on emotion analysis for marketing strategy. Journal of Telecommunication, Electronic and Computer Engineering(JTEC). 2016;8: 87–91. [Google Scholar]
- 75.Guo F, Li M, Hu M, Li F, Lin B. Distinguishing and quantifying the visual aesthetics of a product: An integrated approach of eye-tracking and EEG. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics. 2019;71: 47–56. doi: 10.1016/j.ergon.2019.02.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Scharinger C, Schüler A, Gerjets P. Using eye-tracking and EEG to study the mental processing demands during learning of text-picture combinations. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2020;158: 201–214. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2020.09.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Guo S, Zhao N, Zhang J, Xue T, Liu P, Shuai XU, et al. Landscape visual quality assessment based on eye movement:college student eye-tracking experiments on tourism landscape pictures. Resources Science. 2017; 39:1137–1147. [Google Scholar]
- 78.Hang H, Dong Y. An empirical study of deep learning content and its resource representation. Distance Education in China. 2017; 57–63+80. doi: 10.13541/j.cnki.chinade.2017.08.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Charlotte H, Todd H, Kun G. Differential gaze behavior towards sexually preferred and non-preferred human figures. The Journal of Sex Research. 2011. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2010.521899 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Yadava M, Kumar P, Saini R, Roy PP, Dogra DP. Analysis of EEG signals and its application to neuromarketing. Multimedia Tools & Applications. 2017;76: 1–25. doi: 10.1007/s11042-017-4580-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Yin F, Bai ZY, He HC. A research into visitors’ landscape preference upon wetland plant community: The case of Yanghu Wetland Park. Journal of Central South University of Forestry and Technology (Social Sciences). 2013; 7:37–41. doi: 10.14067/j.cnki.1673-9272.2013.02.028 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Lindemann-Matthies P, Bose E. Species richness, structural diversity and species composition in meadows created by visitors of a botanical garden in Switzerland. Landscape and Urban Planning. 2007;79: 298–307. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2006.03.007 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Townshend TG. Therapeutic landscapes: An evidence-based approach to designing healing gardens and restorative outdoor spaces. Herd. 2015;40: 1–2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Jang HS, Kim J, Kim KS, Pak CH. Human brain activity and emotional responses to plant colour stimuli. Colour Research and Application. 2014;39. doi: 10.1002/col.21788 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Steinhauer SR, Siegle GJ, Condray R, Pless M. Sympathetic and parasympathetic innervation of pupillary dilation during sustained processing. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2004;52: 77–86. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2003.12.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Garcia-Madariaga J, Blasco Lopez M-F, Moya Burgos I, Recuero Virto N. Do isolated packaging variables influence consumer’s attention and preferences? Physiological Behaviour. 2019;200: 96–103. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.04.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Vettori S, Dzhelyova M, Van der Donck S, Jacques C, Van Wesemael T, Steyaert J, et al. Combined frequency-tagging EEG and eye tracking reveal reduced social bias in boys with autism spectrum disorder. Cortex. 2020;125: 135–148. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.12.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.