Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2022 Dec 30;17(12):e0279868. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0279868

Altered visual cortex excitability in premenstrual dysphoric disorder: Evidence from magnetoencephalographic gamma oscillations and perceptual suppression

Viktoriya O Manyukhina 1,2, Elena V Orekhova 1,*, Andrey O Prokofyev 1, Tatiana S Obukhova 1, Tatiana A Stroganova 1
Editor: Thiago P Fernandes3
PMCID: PMC9803314  PMID: 36584199

Abstract

Premenstrual dysphoric disorder (PMDD) is a psychiatric condition characterized by extreme mood shifts during the luteal phase of the menstrual cycle (MC) due to abnormal sensitivity to neurosteroids and unbalanced neural excitation/inhibition (E/I) ratio. We hypothesized that in women with PMDD in the luteal phase, these factors would alter the frequency of magnetoencephalographic visual gamma oscillations, affect modulation of their power by excitatory drive, and decrease perceptual spatial suppression. Women with PMDD and control women were examined twice–during the follicular and luteal phases of their MC. We recorded visual gamma response (GR) while modulating the excitatory drive by increasing the drift rate of the high-contrast grating (static, ‘slow’, ‘medium’, and ‘fast’). Contrary to our expectations, GR frequency was not affected in women with PMDD in either phase of the MC. GR power suppression, which is normally associated with a switch from the ‘optimal’ for GR slow drift rate to the medium drift rate, was reduced in women with PMDD and was the only GR parameter that distinguished them from control participants specifically in the luteal phase and predicted severity of their premenstrual symptoms. Over and above the atypical luteal GR suppression, in both phases of the MC women with PMDD had abnormally strong GR facilitation caused by a switch from the ‘suboptimal’ static to the ‘optimal’ slow drift rate. Perceptual spatial suppression did not differ between the groups but decreased from the follicular to the luteal phase only in PMDD women. The atypical modulation of GR power suggests that neuronal excitability in the visual cortex is constitutively elevated in PMDD and that this E/I imbalance is further exacerbated during the luteal phase. However, the unaltered GR frequency does not support the hypothesis of inhibitory neuron dysfunction in PMDD.

Introduction

Female steroid hormones not only support reproduction but also regulate excitability of the neural cells. These hormones enter the brain by crossing the blood-brain barrier or are synthesized in the brain [13] and affect emotions, perception, and memory [4]. In most females of reproductive age hormone fluctuations during the menstrual cycle (MC) cause only mild changes in physical state and mood a few days before menstruation, known as premenstrual symptoms (PMS) [5, 6]. However, in 2–5% of women, these fluctuations result in disabling psychiatric problems, also known as premenstrual dysphoric disorder (PMDD) [7, 8]. Over the past decades, PMDD has attracted the attention of both clinicians and the scientific community [9], as it is associated with substantial disruption of women’s everyday activities, inability to work and high risks of suicide [1012].

Normal as well as pathological changes in the brain during the MC to a large extent depend on the varying concentration of two steroid hormones–progesterone and estradiol. Although the mechanisms of their action are not fully understood, there is evidence that progesterone and estradiol either directly or indirectly modulate neuronal excitability and affect excitation-to-inhibition (E/I) balance in the brain [13].

There is evidence that PMDD symptoms are related to an altered neural inhibition which results from abnormal sensitivity to neurosteroids rather than their altered levels in the brain [1418]. The neuroactive effects of progesterone associated with PMS are thought to be mediated by its neurosteroid metabolite allopregnanolone (ALLO) [14, 16, 19]. ALLO is a positive allosteric modulator of GABAA receptors (GABAARs) that primarily facilitates tonic inhibition mediated by δ subunit-containing GABAARs (δGABAARs) [20]. However, under certain conditions, it can also produce a paradoxical excitatory effect [2124]. Estradiol also affects neuronal excitability via several pathways [2532]. In general, both progesterone and estradiol produce complex effects on neuronal excitability that depend on the brain area, neurosteroid levels, and other factors.

Symptoms of PMDD, such as depressed mood, anger, irritability, sleep problems, etc. appear to be primarily associated with functional abnormalities in the brain regions involved in emotional regulation [3335]. However, changes associated with PMDD are not limited to these regions [3640]. In particular, the activity of the visual cortex is affected by neurosteroids [4144] and may reflect MC-related abnormalities in the E/I balance regulation in women with PMDD. Primary cortical areas, including visual cortex, display high concentrations of GABAergic receptors [45] and neurons [46] and their GABAergic tone is sensitive to fluctuations in ALLO [44]. Estradiol is another potent modulator of visual cortex excitability [41].

Evidence for the sensitivity of neural activity in the visual cortex to steroid hormones during the MC also comes from several electrophysiological studies. Amplitude and latency of cortical visual evoked potentials [4750], as well as magnitude of the transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) paired-pulse suppression of visual evoked potentials [43] change with phase of the MC. Parameters of the visual alpha rhythm in women without severe PMS are also affected by phase of the MC [5154]. Besides, in a recent study Sumner and colleagues have shown that frequency of gamma oscillations induced by static and moving visual gratings also changes during the MC in women without severe PMS [55].

Functional changes in the visual cortex during the MC may reflect modulatory effects of neurosteroids on excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmission. Gamma oscillations may be particularly sensitive to these modulatory effects, as they closely reflect local cortical interaction of excitatory and inhibitory neurons and are sensitive to the balance of their activity (E/I balance) [5658]. These oscillations are effectively induced by high-contrast regular patterns, such as gratings, and can be reliably recorded in humans using magnetoencephalography (MEG) [59, 60].

Our previous MEG studies suggest that individual variations in regulation of the E/I balance are better captured by stimulation-related changes of oscillatory gamma response (GR) rather than by GR amplitude or frequency measured in a single experimental condition [6163]. Strong and predictable modulations of GR power and frequency can be achieved when modulating the excitatory drive to the visual cortex by increasing drift rate of a large high contrast visual grating: while the frequency of gamma oscillations increases nearly linearly with increasing drift rate, the GR power usually increases with transition from static to slowly drifting grating and then decreases with further increase of the drift rate. As we discussed elsewhere [6265], this pattern of changes in GR power may reflect efficiency of neuronal inhibition: a certain level of activity of inhibitory interneurons is necessary for synchronization of gamma oscillations [66, 67], but their ‘too strong’ activation–which in our experimental paradigm was associated with ‘medium’ and ‘fast’ drift rates [65]–may lead to a disruption of synchronization between principal cells and interneurons resulting in reduced amplitude of gamma oscillations [68, 69]. Following this logic, a higher neural E/I ratio would lead to less suppression of GR power for the same level of excitatory drive (i.e., at the same drift rate of a grating), because in this case more inhibition is required to desynchronize strongly excited principal cells [68]. We therefore predicted that the presumably elevated E/I ratio in the visual cortex in women with PMDD during the symptomatic premenstrual period would result in relatively weaker GR power suppression with increasing drift rate.

Next, we predicted that MC phase-related changes in E/I ratio in women with PMDD would affect the frequency of their gamma oscillations. Animal studies suggest that gamma frequency strongly depends on the tonic excitability of inhibitory neurons, which is regulated through δGABAARs, so that the lower the tonic inhibition of the inhibitory interneurons, the higher the gamma frequency [66, 70]. The δGABAARs are highly sensitive to neurosteroids, and their numbers increase following an increase in progesterone levels [7173]. However, Sumner et al. [55] found that in women without severe PMS, the frequency of visual gamma oscillations was higher during high-progesterone-and-ALLO luteal phase than during low-progesterone-and-ALLO follicular phase. The authors hypothesized that these changes might be caused by developing tolerance to ALLO during late luteal phase. Since abnormal sensitivity to ALLO has previously been suggested to be one of the mechanisms of PMDD [18], we hypothesized that it may manifest as a change in GR frequency during the luteal phase.

Thirdly, we expected that abnormal neural inhibition in women with PMDD during their symptomatic luteal phase would affect visual perception functions that are highly sensitive to the strength of neural inhibition in the visual cortex. To test this prediction, we assessed in our participants perceptual spatial suppression–the capacity of the visual system to suppress the perception of large background-like motion [74]. Spatial suppression is reflected in an increase in the time it takes to discriminate the direction of motion of regularly patterned visual objects as their size increases [75, 76]. This perceptual phenomenon can be explained by an increase of surround inhibition associated with stimulation of the ‘far surround’ of the neurons’ receptive fields [77, 78]. Indeed, elderly people [79, 80] or individuals with disorders associated with weakened neural inhibition [8184] demonstrated decreased spatial suppression.

Herein, to test these predictions, we investigated 1) visual MEG gamma oscillations and 2) perceptual spatial suppression in women with PMDD and women without severe PMS during the early-to-mid follicular and the mid-to-late luteal phases of the MC, when the PMS are least and most pronounced, respectively. For this purpose, we invited the same women twice, counterbalancing the phases of the first visit (follicular and luteal) between the participants.

Materials & methods

Participants

The participants from the PMDD group were recruited via media advertisements (social networks and publications in the media). The control women were recruited among members of the internet ‘healthy life style’ group and students. The criteria for exclusion, common for both groups, included (a) pregnancy or lactation, (b) irregular MC, (c) intake of hormones (contraceptive pills, thyroid hormones, etc.) or psychoactive drugs (nootropics, antidepressants, tranquilisers, etc.). Control women also had to have no personal or family history of neurological or psychiatric disorders. Women with PMDD who had been diagnosed with depression at some point in their lives (two of twenty women) were still included in the study because PMDD is often comorbid with this psychiatric condition [8587].

The potential subjects of the clinical group reported severe changes in mood associated with the MC, which had a significant impact on their lives, relationships, daily activities, and job. To test whether these symptoms met criteria for PMDD, we used Carolina Premenstrual Assessment Scoring System (C-PASS, [88]). The C-PASS measures severity of PMS according to DSM-5 PMDD criteria [89]. It includes 20 statements related to 11 symptoms, which should be rated from 1 (‘not at all’) to 6 (‘extreme symptoms’) according to their severity. All volunteers of the PMDD group were asked to fill in the C-PASS scales daily for two or three MCs. They were included in the study only if they met the C-PASS criteria for PMDD in at least two MCs. All but two PMDD subjects continued to fill in the C-PASS during the period when the MEG and psychophysical investigation were performed, which allowed us to assess the severity of their PMDD symptoms on the day of examination.

To estimate the symptom severity on the day of examination (hereafter ‘same-day PMS scores’), we averaged scores obtained on this day for the symptoms that were reliably associated with PMDD according to the C-PASS (i.e., the symptoms which were strong enough and varied during the MC; see [88]). The same-day PMS scores were available in 18 out of 20 participants with PMDD.

We expected that using the C-PASS among control participants could greatly increase the dropout. It could also lead to a preponderance of control volunteers with certain personality traits (e.g., those who are persistent enough to follow the requirements without being interested in the C-PASS results). Therefore, to ensure that control participants did not have severe PMS, we used a less time-consuming premenstrual tension syndrome scale (PMTS-VAS, [90]). In PMTS-VAS, subjects need to retrospectively assess their symptoms on a visual analogue scale for one week after the start of their last menstruation (early follicular phase) and for a week before their last menstruation (late luteal phase), when the PMS are expected to be least and most pronounced, respectively. PMTS-VAS consists of 12 statements that satisfy the DSM-4 criteria for PMDD and should be rated from 0 (‘not at all’) to 100 (‘extreme symptoms’) across a continuum of values. In each control participant, we estimated PMS score as a difference between PMTS-VAS scores for the luteal and follicular phases [90, 91]. The resulting PMS score could thus vary from 0 (no PMS) to 100 (maximal symptom severity). In control subjects, the resulting PMS score averaged over 12 symptoms varied from 0 to 33.3 (mean ± standard deviation (S.D.): 16.59 ± 10.41), and PMS score averaged over core mood symptoms (depression, anxiety, irritability, and lability of mood) varied from 0 to 37.5 (mean ± S.D.: 18.53 ± 13.87), which is below the cut-off for severe PMS according to Steiner et al. [91].

Neurophysiological (MEG) and behavioral (psychophysical) data were collected from 20 women who fulfilled the criteria for PMDD (PMDD group) and 27 age-matched control women. PMDD and control subjects did not differ in age (Student’s t-test, t(45) = 0.81, p = 0.42), the average length of MC (Student’s t-test, t(45) = 0.03, p = 0.98) or day of MC during the visit (follicular: Mann-Whitney U = 241.0, p = 0.27; luteal: Mann-Whitney U = 267.0, p = 0.48). The day of the MC normalized by the average MC length also did not differ between the groups (p’s>0.26). For PMDD participants, we also got information about the day when their next menstruation started after their ‘luteal phase visit’. Detailed information about the participants is summarized in Table 1.

Table 1. Characteristics of the experimental groups.

Characteristic PMDD group (N = 20) Mean (S.D.) [range] Control group (N = 27) Mean (S.D.) [range] Group difference
Student’s T P-value
Age (years) 29.05 (5.38) [1839] 27.70 (5.82) [1840] 0.81 0.42
Average reported length of MC 28.50 (2.74) [2434] 28.48 (2.01) [2533] 0.03 0.98
Day of MC, follicular phase visit 5.35 (2.56) [210] 4.81 (2.08) [210] 0.79 0.43
Day of MC, luteal phase visit 23.30 (2.79) [1830] 23.33 (2.22) [2029] -0.05 0.96
Luteal phase visit: days before the next menstruation 5.60 (2.72) [110]

N–number of subjects; S.D.—standard deviation; MC–menstrual cycle.

To test whether subjects from the two groups differed in their level of trait anxiety and their level of anxiety during the laboratory visit, participants were asked to complete the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) questionnaire [92] right before the MEG experiment. Additionally, participants with PMDD filled in the screening tests for bipolar disorder (Bipolar Spectrum Diagnostic Scale, BSDS [93]; Mood Disorder Questionnaire, MDQ, [94]; Hypomania Check List, HCL-32 [95]), and for depressive disorder (Beck’s Depression Inventory, BDI [96]). The bipolar disorder questionnaires were completed during the asymptomatic follicular phase, and the BDI was completed twice: during the follicular and symptomatic late luteal phases.

All the participants got blood tests for hormones and underwent MEG recording and psychophysical testing on the same day, once during the early-to-mid follicular (hereafter ‘follicular’) and once during mid-to-late luteal (hereafter ‘luteal’) phases of the MC. The phase of the 1st visit to the laboratory (follicular or luteal) was counterbalanced between participants. The study has been approved by the Ethical Committee of the Moscow State University of Psychology and Education. Subjects were not paid for their participation. All subjects gave written informed consent.

Blood samples

The blood samples used to confirm the phase of the MC and to analyze the effect of steroid hormone levels on visual gamma oscillations were collected in the morning. Plasma levels of estradiol and progesterone were measured using a solid phase chemiluminescence immunoassay. The analyzes were performed by INVITRO, a certified commercial laboratory that performs a wide range of medical tests for clinical and diagnostic purposes (https://www.invitro.ru).

MEG paradigm

Participants watched a sequence of large high-contrast circular gratings (18°, 100% contrast, spatial frequency 1.66 cycles per degree) that either drifted with one of three velocities (‘slow’: 1.2°/s, ‘medium’: 3.6°/s, ‘fast’: 6.0°/s) or remained static (see Fig 1, upper panel). The presentation time for each stimulus ranged randomly from 1.2 to 3 seconds. After this period, the moving grating stopped or the static grating began to move. Participants were instructed to press a button as soon as this change occurred. Each new trial started with a fixed 1.2 s prestimulus interval, continued with the presentation of the stimulus, and ended immediately after the button press. To reduce fatigue and boredom, participants were shown short (3–6 s) cartoon animations after every 2–5 gratings. For each participant, 90 gratings of each type were randomized and presented in three sessions. For additional details, see Orekhova et al. [62, 64].

Fig 1.

Fig 1

Schematic representation of experimental procedures and analysis of MEG (upper panel) and psychophysical data (lower panel). For a detailed description of the experimental procedures and analysis pipelines, see the Methods section. The procedures were identical between the two visits. ISI–inter-stimulus interval.

MRI data acquisition and processing

All participants underwent magnetic resonance imaging (MRI; 1.5 T, voxel size 1 mm x 1 mm x 1 mm). The T1-weighted images were processed with the FreeSurfer’s (v.6.0.0) default ‘recon-all’ pipeline. Its main included steps were motion correction, spatial normalization, skull stripping, gray/white matter segmentation.

MEG data acquisition, preprocessing, and analysis

MEG data were acquired at the Center for Neurocognitive Research (MEG-center) of the Moscow State University of Psychology and Education using an Electa VectorView Neuromag 306-channel MEG detector array (Helsinki, Finland) consisting of 102 magnetometers and 204 planar gradiometers.

The MEG signal was registered with 0.03 Hz high-pass and 300 Hz low-pass in-built filters and sampled at 1000 Hz. Temporal signal-space separation method (tSSS) [97], a temporal extension of SSS, with 0.9 correlation limit and with movement compensation implemented in MaxFilter software (v.2.2), was applied to raw MEG signal in order to reduce interference of external artifact sources and compensate for movement-related distortions.

All subsequent processing steps were performed using MNE python software (v.0.22.0). Raw data were down-sampled to 500 Hz and independent component analysis (ICA) was applied in order to detect and remove ICs that corresponded to biological artifacts (eye blinks and heartbeats). The number of removed ICs did not differ between the groups and/or phases of the MC (all p’s>0.08). The data were then filtered using 30 Hz high-pass and 115 Hz low-pass finite impulse response filter with default parameters (symmetric, with Hamming window; the window length and width of the transition band at the low/high cut-off was set to ‘auto’) and epoched from -1 s to 1.2 s relative to the stimulus onset. Epochs contaminated by instrumental and myogenic artifacts were excluded based on visual inspection of unfiltered epochs. The number of resulting artifact-free epochs did not differ significantly between the groups for any of the four velocity conditions and/or phases of the MC (all p’s>0.19). Average number of artifact-free epochs for ‘static’, ‘slow’, ‘medium’, and ‘fast’ conditions in the control group was 83.41, 83.56, 83.11, 83.56 in the follicular phase vs. 81.78, 82.78, 81.62, 82.52 in the luteal phase; and in the PMDD group was 84.05, 84.25, 84.40, 83.40 in the follicular phase vs. 83.35, 83.55, 82.70, 82.85 in the luteal phase.

MEG source analysis

MEG data were co-registered with the structural MRI of the head, and a single-layer boundary element model was constructed. We then created surface-based source space with 4096 vertex sources in each hemisphere and estimated the forward solution. Noise covariance matrices and data covariance matrices were estimated based on the prestimulus (-1–0 s, relative to the stimulus onset) and stimulation (0–1.2 s, relative to the stimulus onset) time intervals, respectively, of all conditions combined. Linearly constrained minimum variance (LCMV; [98]) beamformer spatial filters with a source orientation that maximizes power and a regularization coefficient 0.05 were created separately for each stimulus type and were applied individually to each epoch. As we expected to observe gamma oscillations in the visual cortex [59, 6265], the inverse solution was limited to visual and adjacent cortical areas, same as in Manyukhina et al. [61].

Time-frequency analysis of the MEG data at the source level

The approach to the analysis of visual gamma parameters used in the present study is described in detail by Manyukhina et al. [61]. The time-frequency analysis was performed for the signal at each vertex source using multi-taper method, separately for the four velocity conditions and for the prestimulus and stimulation time intervals. The following parameters were used: bandwidth = 10 Hz, frequency resolution ~ 1.11 Hz, time step = 2 ms. Then, at each vertex source we estimated the normalized response power as (Stim–PreStim)/PreStim, where PreStim and Stim are the spectral powers estimated at -0.9–0 s and 0.3–1.2 s time intervals relative to the stimulus onset, respectively. Among all the four conditions, we then selected individually for each participant the 26 vertices with maximum response power in gamma range (40–80 Hz), and averaged the spectra over these vertices, separately for each condition. Visual inspection of localization results has shown that for all participants the selected vertices were spatially contiguous in the surface mesh of each hemisphere. See Fig 1 (upper panel) for a brief review of MEG data analysis steps.

For each velocity condition, the GR power was calculated as the average of those spectrum values that exceeded 2/3 of the peak value in the frequency range 35–90 Hz. The GR frequency was estimated as a center of gravity of the spectrum values used to calculate the GR power. The GR was considered reliable if, according to Wilcoxon signed-rank test, the probability of no difference in the frequency corresponding to the peak gamma power in prestimulus and stimulation time intervals was below p = 0.0001 (see, e.g., [63, 64]). In all our participants and velocity conditions, the GRs met this reliability criterion.

Psychophysical perceptual spatial suppression test

To assess perceptual suppression associated with increasing stimulus size, we used a modification of the spatial suppression paradigm suggested by Tadin and colleagues [75, 76]. The similar experimental paradigm was used in our previous studies [63, 99].

Visual stimuli were presented using PsychoToolbox for Matlab (MathWorks). Subjects sat at 60 cm distance from the monitor (Benq XL2420T, 24′′W LED, 1920 × 1080 resolution, 120 Hz). Before the testing, all participants completed a training session.

Inter-trial interval was 500 ms. In the beginning of each trial a central dot flickered at the screen (50 ms on, 50 ms off, 250 ms on, 150 ms off) followed by the stimulus presentation. The stimuli were 12°, 2.5°, and 1° vertical high-contrast sinusoidal gratings moving at a constant rate of 4°/s in each trial (Fig 1, lower panel). Direction of motion (left or right) was determined randomly for each trial. The participants were asked to indicate the direction of the visual motion by pressing the corresponding (right or left) button. No response time limit was given. The stimulus presentation time started from 150 ms and was adjusted in the following trials using ‘one-up two-down’ staircases procedure with the initial step of 16.7 ms that decreased to 8.3 ms after the first two reversals. Separate staircases were used for the three types of the stimuli. The block continued until all staircases completed at least 7 reversals. The subject completed two blocks during each visit. The duration threshold was computed by averaging the presentation times over all the reversals excluding the first two, and then over the two blocks. For the purpose of the present study, only responses to the large (12°) and small (1°) stimuli were analyzed. As a result, for each visit and stimulus size we estimated the minimal exposure time required for the subject to discriminate the direction of motion of the small and large grating (in each visit: ThresholdSMALL and ThresholdLARGE).

To estimate strength of perceptual suppression, we calculated the spatial suppression index (SSI) as:

SSI = log10(ThresholdLARGE)–log10(ThresholdSMALL).

For a brief review of all the steps of psychophysical data analysis, see Fig 1 (lower panel).

Statistical analyses

To analyze group and phase-related differences in gamma parameters, we used General Linear Models (GLM) implemented in the Rstatix and Stats packages in R 4.0.3 [100]. GR power values were log10-transformed to normalize the distributions. According to the Shapiro-Wilk test, distributions of the GR frequency and GR log10-transformed power did not differ significantly from normal (all p’s>0.05). Since the preliminary analysis showed that the GR power and frequency depend on age (see below), standardized Age was included as a factor in the linear models for GR frequency and log10-transformed GR power. The between-group factors were Group (control, PMDD), Visit-Order (1st visit during the follicular phase, 1st visit during the luteal phase). The repeated measures factors were Phase (follicular, luteal) and Velocity (4 levels). We tested for the effects of Group, Visit-Order, Group x Visit-Order, Age, and their interactions with repeated measures factors Phase and Velocity. When appropriate, the Greenhouse-Geisser correction was used to adjust for the lack of sphericity. Planned comparisons were used to analyze the origin of significant repeated measures Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) effects.

To quantify changes in GR power from ‘static’ to ‘slow’ and from ‘slow’ to ‘medium’ conditions (see Results to substantiate the use of these contrasts), we also calculated ‘GR facilitation’ and ‘GR suppression’ as ratios:

GR suppression = (1—GR PowerMedium/GR PowerSlow)*100%

GR facilitation = (1—GR PowerStatic/GR PowerSlow)*100%

A ratio metric can exaggerate between-group difference, especially if groups differ in initial value [101], which in this case corresponds to GR power in the ‘slow’ condition. Besides, a ratio yields reliable results only if the relationship between numerator and denominator is a straight line through the origin for at least one of the two groups being compared [101, 102]. These two requirements–no statistically significant difference in initial value and a zero intercept–were fulfilled for both GR indexes (see S1 Fig for the detailed group comparison of GR power). Therefore, the use of GR suppression and GR facilitation ratio metrics was statistically justified.

Other steps of statistical analysis were performed using the standard Python 3.8.5 libraries Numpy [103] and Scipy [104]. The data were tested for normality using Shapiro-Wilk test, homogeneity of variance was evaluated using Levene’s test. When assumptions for parametric testing were violated, non-parametric tests were implemented, i.e., Wilcoxon signed-rank test for within- and Mann-Whitney U test for between-samples comparisons. Alternatively, parametric Student’s t-test for related or independent groups was used.

Pearson correlation coefficient was estimated to assess the relationship between the normally distributed variables. When assumptions of normality were violated, Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient was estimated. Partial Pearson’s and Spearman’s rank correlation coefficients were used wherever necessary to control for confounding variables.

The Benjamini-Hochberg false discovery rate (FDR) correction with threshold 0.05 was applied to the p-values to control for multiple tests.

Results

Premenstrual symptom severity in women with PMDD

Fig 2 shows the daily symptom severity scores in women with PMDD, averaged over two or three MCs. For the subjects to meet criteria for PMDD on the C-PASS, at least five out of 20 symptoms need to meet the PMDD criteria, i.e., the symptoms need to be strong enough and be reliably associated with the MC. The curves in Fig 2 represent averages over all 20 symptoms. It can be seen that symptom severity substantially increased during the week preceding the onset of menstruation (luteal phase; the score > = 3 of maximum 6) and then gradually decreased during the early follicular phase. According to a recent study of the PMDD subtypes [105], these average symptom scores indicate severe rather than mild PMDD in most participants in our clinical group.

Fig 2. Daily symptom severity scores (from 1 = ‘Not at all’ to 6 = ‘Extreme’) averaged over 20 symptoms (C-PASS questionnaire [88]) and two or three menstrual cycles in participants with PMDD.

Fig 2

The thin lines show individual participants; the thick line is the group average. The days are numbered relative to the start of menstruation (day 1).

Psychometric characteristics of women with PMDD

According to the BSDS (but not MDQ) questionnaire, 11 of our 20 participants with PMDD tested positive for bipolar disorder. According to the HCL-32 questionnaire, 13 of 20 participants with PMDD demonstrated hypomanic features. According to the BDI questionnaire, 6 out of 20 women with PMDD scored positive for mild-to-moderate depression in the follicular phase, whereas the frequency and severity of the depressive symptoms in the PMDD group strongly increased in the luteal phase. For more information on bipolar disorder and depression questionnaires, see S1 Table and S1 Text in S2 File.

According to the STAI questionnaire, the ‘trait anxiety’ was higher in women with PMDD than in control women (Mann-Whitney U test, p = 0.011, Table 2). The ‘state anxiety’ was higher in the PMDD than control group during the luteal phase (Mann-Whitney U test, p = 0.0002, Table 2), while no group differences were found during the follicular phase (Mann-Whitney U test, p = 0.22, Table 2). Also, women with PMDD had higher state anxiety when they visited the laboratory during the luteal compared to the follicular phase (Wilcoxon signed-rank test, Z = 40.0, p = 0.014), which was not the case in control women.

Table 2. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) results in PMDD and control groups.
Scales PMDD group Mean (S.D.) [range] Control group Mean (S.D.) [range] Group difference
Mann-Whitney’s U P-value
STAI, trait NPMDD/NControl = 20/27 47.23 (10.04) [3072] 39.83 (9.37) [2762] 163.5 0.011
STAI, state (follicular) NPMDD/NControl = 20/26 37.85 (8.09) [2553] 34.92 (10.83) [2262] 234.0 0.22
STAI, state (luteal) NPMDD/NControl = 20/27 47.6 (11.56) [2671] 34.33 (9.46) [2056] 105.5 0.0002

N–number of subjects; S.D.–standard deviation.

We expected our participants to be more anxious during their first visit to the laboratory, irrespective of the MC phase. Indeed, in the combined group of subjects, state anxiety was higher during the first visit compared to the second one (Mann-Whitney U = 319.5, p = 0.025). In the individual groups, however, the difference in the state anxiety between the first and second visits did not reach the level of significance (both p’s>0.12).

Plasma levels of steroids and PMDD symptom severity

In all participants, plasma level of progesterone increased from the follicular to the luteal phase (Fig 3A and 3B). Estradiol plasma level also increased from the follicular to the luteal phase in the majority of participants (Fig 3C and 3D). Thus, the hormone levels were in the range consistent with the follicular and luteal phases at each visit.

Fig 3.

Fig 3

(a-d) Plasma levels of estradiol and progesterone in the control (a, c) and PMDD (b, d) groups in two phases of the menstrual cycle (follicular, luteal). (e-g) Violin plots show estradiol and progesterone concentrations and estradiol to progesterone ratios in the luteal phase of the menstrual cycle in control subjects and subjects with PMDD. *p<0.05; **p<0.01.

The luteal progesterone plasma level was reduced in women with PMDD (Student’s t-test, t(45) = -2.53, p = 0.015; Fig 3F), while their luteal estradiol did not differ from that in the control group (Student’s t-test, t(45) = 0.65, p = 0.52; Fig 3E). The ratio of estradiol to progesterone concentrations in the luteal phase was significantly elevated in the PMDD group (Mann-Whitney U = 150.0, p = 0.005; Fig 3G).

To test for the link between steroid hormone levels and symptom severity in PMDD, we estimated Pearson correlation between the hormone concentrations and the same-day PMS scores (number of days prior menstruation onset was partial out of the correlation). The correlation was not significant (NPMDD = 18; estradiol: rpartial = 0.32, p = 0.22; progesterone: rpartial = 0.25, p = 0.33).

MEG results

Behavioral performance in MEG task

The percentage of epochs with omission (reaction time<150 ms) and commission (no response or reaction time> = 1000 ms) errors did not differ significantly between the groups and MC phases (all p’s>0.19). The reaction time was significantly longer in women with PMDD than in control participants during the luteal phase of the MC (Mann-Whitney U = 1995, p = 0.035), but not during the follicular phase (Mann-Whitney U = 2252.0, p = 0.23).

Dependence of the GR parameters on age

In line with the previous studies [106108], GR frequency, averaged over phases, was negatively correlated with age in control women (Table 3). In women with PMDD, none of the correlations reached the level of significance. However, there was no significant difference in the correlation coefficients between the groups (all p’s>0.15, two-sided). Phase-averaged GR power increased with age in both groups of participants and in all velocity conditions (Table 3).

Table 3. Pearson’s correlations between age and gamma response (GR) power (log10-transformed) and frequency in the two groups of participants (control and PMDD).

Gamma parameters were averaged over the two phases of the menstrual cycle.

Grating’s motion velocity Control group (N = 27) PMDD group (N = 20)
GR power
Static, 0°/s r = 0.62, p = 0.005 r = 0.40, p = 0.11
Slow, 1.2°/s r = 0.56, p = 0.008 r = 0.46, p = 0.08
Medium, 3.6°/s r = 0.65, p = 0.005 r = 0.49, p = 0.07
Fast, 6.0°/s r = 0.66, p = 0.005 r = 0.54, p = 0.03
GR frequency
Static, 0°/s r = -0.33, p = 0.13 r = -0.06, p = 0.80
Slow, 1.2°/s r = -0.38, p = 0.09 r = -0.29, p = 0.26
Medium, 3.6°/s r = -0.47, p = 0.03 r = -0.23, p = 0.38
Fast, 6.0°/s r = -0.34, p = 0.11 r = 0.07, p = 0.80

N—number of subjects; p-values are FDR-corrected for multiple comparisons.

Effects of diagnosis, MC phase, and visit order on GR frequency

Results of GLM with factors Group (control, PMDD), Visit-Order (1st visit during the follicular phase, 1st visit during the luteal phase), repeated measures factors Phase (follicular, luteal) and Velocity (4 levels), and Age as a covariate are presented in Tables 4 and 5 for GR frequency and GR power, respectively. S1 Fig shows violin plots of individual GR frequency values for each group, condition, and MC phase.

Table 4. ANCOVA results for GR frequency.
Effect F df G-G epsilon Generalized eta squared P / adj. p
Group 0.159 1, 42 0.003 0.69
Visit-Order 0.142 1, 42 0.003 0.71
Group * Visit-Order 0.520 1, 42 0.009 0.47
Phase 8.259 1, 42 0.004 0.006
Phase * Group 0.000 1, 42 3.7*10−8 0.99
Phase * Visit-Order 17.968 1, 42 0.009 0.0001
Phase * Group * Visit-Order 0.105 1, 42 0.00005 0.75
Velocity 410.088 3, 126 0.61 0.63 2.5*10 −40
Velocity * Group 0.185 3, 126 0.61 0.0008 0.81
Velocity * Visit-Order 0.859 3, 126 0.61 0.004 0.42
Velocity * Group * Visit-Order 0.747 3, 126 0.61 0.003 0.47
Phase * Velocity 0.347 3, 126 0.58 0.0003 0.68
Phase * Velocity * Group 0.078 3, 126 0.58 0.00006 0.90
Phase * Velocity * Visit-Order 2.845 3, 126 0.58 0.002 0.072
Phase * Velocity * Group * Visit-Order 0.248 3, 126 0.58 0.0002 0.75

Significant effects are highlighted in bold.

Table 5. ANCOVA results for log10-transformed GR power.
Effect F df G-G epsilon Generalized eta squared P / adj. p
Group 0.833 1, 42 0.014 0.37
Visit-Order 0.608 1, 42 0.010 0.44
Group * Visit-Order 3.502 1, 42 0.055 0.068
Phase 0.070 1, 42 0.0002 0.79
Phase * Group 0.155 1, 42 0.0004 0.70
Phase * Visit-Order 0.909 1, 42 0.002 0.35
Phase * Group * Visit-Order 2.714 1, 42 0.007 0.11
Velocity 152.058 3, 126 0.71 0.375 3.5*10 −30
Velocity * Group 3.464 3, 126 0.71 0.013 0.033
Velocity * Visit-Order 1.661 3, 126 0.71 0.007 0.19
Velocity * Group * Visit-Order 0.618 3, 126 0.71 0.007 0.55
Phase * Velocity 1.615 3, 126 0.76 0.0008 0.20
Phase * Velocity * Group 2.441 3, 126 0.76 0.001 0.085
Phase * Velocity * Visit-Order 3.020 3, 126 0.76 0.001 0.047
Phase * Velocity * Group * Visit-Order 0.317 3, 126 0.76 0.0002 0.76

Significant effects are highlighted in bold.

There was a highly significant effect of Velocity: GR frequency almost linearly increased with increasing grating’s motion velocity (Table 4, Fig 4A and 4B). There was also a significant effect of Phase (luteal>follicular), which however can be better understood by inspecting the highly significant interaction between Phase and Visit-Order (F(1,42) = 18.0, p = 0.0001). This result shows that the difference in GR frequency between the luteal and follicular phases was present only if the luteal phase occurred during the first visit to the MEG laboratory (Fig 4C and 4D, right panels), but not during the second visit (Fig 4C and 4D, left panels). Notably, the effect of Phase and Visit-Order interaction–higher GR frequency during the luteal phase if it coincided with the first visit–was the same in both PMDD and control groups (Fig 4C and 4D). Indeed, the Phase and Visit-Order interaction remained highly significant when tested separately in the control and PMDD groups (Control: F(1,24) = 16.3, p = 0.0005; PMDD: F(1,17) = 11.5, p = 0.003). The effect of Group and interactions of Group with Phase, Visit-Order, or Velocity were all not significant.

Fig 4. Effect of the phase of the menstrual cycle on the weighted peak frequency of gamma response (GR).

Fig 4

The data are shown for four motion velocity conditions (‘static’: 0°/s, ‘slow’: 1.2°/s, ‘medium’: 3.6°/s, ‘fast’: 6.0°/s). (a) and (b) show comparison of the GR weighted peak frequency in the luteal and follicular phases separately for all control (a) and all PMDD (b) subjects. (c, d) show comparison of the GR weighted peak frequency in the luteal and follicular phases separately for those subjects, who came for the first investigation during their follicular (c, d left panel) or luteal (c, d right panel) phase. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. *p<0.05; **p<0.01.

Effects of diagnosis and MC phase on GR power

The group average GR power spectra for participants from the control and PMDD groups are shown in Fig 5. S1 Fig shows violin plots of individual GR power values for each group, condition, and MC phase.

Fig 5. Grand average spectra of gamma response (GR) ([stimulation-prestimulus] / prestimulus).

Fig 5

The spectra are shown for four motion velocity conditions (‘static’: 0°/s, ‘slow’: 1.2°/s, ‘medium’: 3.6°/s, ‘fast’: 6.0°/s), two phases of the menstrual cycle (follicular, luteal), and two groups of subjects (control (a), PMDD (b)). Shaded areas represent 95% confidence intervals.

For GR power, there was a highly significant effect of Velocity (Table 5): the power increased from the ‘static’ to the ‘slow’ condition and then decreased with further increase of grating’s drift rate (Fig 5). There were no main effects of Phase or Group. However, there was a significant Velocity and Group interaction (F(3, 126) = 3.5, epsilon = 0.71, adj. p = 0.033). There was also a tendency for Phase * Velocity * Group interaction (F(3, 126) = 2.4, epsilon = 0.76, adj. p = 0.085). Since we had a clear prediction of a reduction of GR suppression in women with PMDD during the luteal phase and of the association between this reduction and PMS severity, we further analyzed this prediction even though the Phase * Velocity * Group interaction was only a trend.

Modulations of GR power by visual motion velocity

To investigate group differences in velocity-related modulations of GR power, we used post-hoc planned comparisons. There were no group differences in GR powers in any MC phase or velocity condition (all p’s>0.1). However, suppression of the GR power from the ‘slow’ to the ‘medium’ condition was lower in women with PMDD than in control women during the luteal phase (F(1, 42) = 5.1, p = 0.03; Fig 6A, right panel). This group difference was absent during the follicular phase (F(1, 42) = 0.01, p = 0.9; Fig 6A, left panel). Fig 6C shows this GR suppression in percent of GR power in the ‘slow’ condition, which induced maximal power of GR at the group level; GR suppression = (1—GR PowerMedium/GR PowerSlow)*100%. Larger values indicate stronger GR power decrease under higher sensory load. S2 Fig shows violin plots of individual GR suppression scores for each group and MC phase.

Fig 6. Group differences in gamma response (GR) power.

Fig 6

(a) Log10-transformed GR power modulation in four velocity conditions (‘static’: 0°/s, ‘slow’: 1.2°/s, ‘medium’: 3.6°/s, ‘fast’: 6.0°/s) in the follicular (left panel) and luteal (right panel) phases of the menstrual cycle. (b) Group differences in velocity-related GR power facilitation from the ‘static’ to the ‘slow’ velocity condition, estimated in percent of the GR power in the ‘slow’ condition, which induced maximal GR at the group level: (1—GR PowerStatic/GR PowerSlow)*100%. (c) Group differences in velocity-related GR power suppression from the ‘slow’ to the ‘medium’ velocity condition, estimated in percent of the GR power in the ‘slow’ condition: (1—GR PowerMedium/GR PowerSlow)*100%. (d) Correlation between luteal GR power suppression and premenstrual symptom (PMS) severity measured on the same day in women with PMDD. Error bars and shaded areas represent 95% confidence intervals.

The GR suppression increased from the follicular to the luteal phase in control women (Student’s t-test, t(26) = 2.2, p = 0.041; Fig 6C), but not in women with PMDD (Student’s t-test, t(19) = -1.5, p = 0.16; Fig 6C), in which the direction of change was even inverted. As a result, in the luteal phase women with PMDD had lower GR suppression than control participants (Student’s t-test, t(45) = -2.5, p = 0.017; Fig 6C). The lower luteal GR suppression in women with PMDD correlated with higher severity of their same-day PMS scores (NPMDD = 18, Pearson’s r = -0.49, p = 0.04; Fig 6D).

We then tested for the presence of group differences in GR facilitation, i.e., an increase of GR power from the ‘static’ to the ‘slow’ condition, which likely has a different mechanism from that of GR suppression (see [65] and Discussion section). The increase of GR power from the ‘static’ to the ‘slow’ condition was steeper in PMDD than in control participants in both MC phases (follicular: F(1, 43) = 4.2, p = 0.047; luteal: F(1, 43) = 7.0, p = 0.01; combined phases: F(1, 43) = 6.1, p = 0.02; Fig 6A). This means that women with PMDD show a stronger facilitation of GR power from the ‘static’ to the ‘slow’ condition regardless of the MC phase. Fig 6B presents GR facilitation in percent of GR power in the ‘slow’ condition, which induced maximal power of GR at the group level: GR facilitation = (1—GR PowerStatic/GR PowerSlow)*100%. Larger values indicate stronger GR facilitation with a moderate increase of sensory load. S2 Fig shows violin plots of individual GR facilitation scores for each group and MC phase. Similarly to the planned comparison analysis, this measure indicates greater GR facilitation in PMDD than in control subjects during both follicular (Student’s t-test, t(45) = 2.2, p = 0.034) and luteal (Student’s t-test, t(45) = 2.9, p = 0.006) phases of the MC (Fig 6B). The magnitude of GR facilitation did not change between MC phases in either the control (Student’s paired t-test, t(52) = 0.16, p = 0.87) or PMDD group (Student’s paired t-test, t(38) = 0.62, p = 0.54).

To sum up, we found that the modulation of GR power by velocity differed in women with PMDD and control participants. GR facilitation from the ‘static’ to the ‘slow’ condition (i.e., on the ascending branch of the GR power modulation curve) was greater in women with PMDD than in control women during both phases of the MC. On the other hand, GR suppression from the ‘slow’ to the ‘medium’ velocity condition (i.e., at the descending branch of the GR power modulation curve) was reduced in PMDD participants specifically during the luteal phase, and correlated with the severity of PMS on the day of the visit.

Relationship between GR power and GR frequency modulations

We have previously reported a significant correlation between an increase in GR frequency and suppression of GR power from the ‘slow’ to the ‘medium’ visual motion velocity condition [64].

Consistently with the previous findings, we found in control participants a correlation between the ‘slow’/‘medium’ GR power ratio and an increase in GR frequency from the ‘slow’ to the ‘medium’ condition (NControl = 27, follicular: Pearson’s r = 0.39, p = 0.04; luteal: Pearson’s r = 0.56, p = 0.002). This correlation was absent in the PMDD sample (NPMDD = 20, follicular: Pearson’s r = -0.15, p = 0.5; luteal: Pearson’s r = -0.08, p = 0.7). The between-group difference in correlation coefficients was significant in the luteal phase (p = 0.03) and approached significance level in the follicular phase (p = 0.09).

Relationship between GR parameters and plasma levels of steroid hormones

To test for the link between plasma levels of steroid hormones and GR parameters, we estimated partial Spearman’s correlation coefficients between GR frequency or GR power in the corresponding phase of the MC and (A) follicular estradiol, (B) luteal estradiol, or (C) luteal progesterone separately in the control and PMDD groups (see S1 Table). Age was partialled out of the correlations because it affected the GR parameters (see Table 3). Correlations with follicular progesterone were not assessed because the poor sensitivity of commercial immunoassay systems to low progesterone concentrations (<5 nmol/L) precludes its reliable measurement [109]. In control participants, there were positive correlations between follicular estradiol and GR frequency in the ‘medium’ and ‘fast’ velocity conditions. None of these correlations, however, survived correction for multiple comparisons.

Results of the psychophysical experiment: Directional sensitivity to motion.

Spatial suppression was estimated in 19 of 20 subjects with PMDD and in 26 of 27 control subjects. Two women (1 control, in both phases; 1 PMDD, in the follicular phase) demonstrated a persistent illusion of reverse motion when presented with large moving grating, which did not allow to reliably estimate motion direction discrimination thresholds (hereafter, ‘duration thresholds’), in these participants.

In both groups and during both phases of the MC, subjects demonstrated reliable perceptual spatial suppression: it took them longer to discriminate direction of motion of a large than of a small visual grating (all F’s>9.0, p’s<0.0001; Fig 7A). Based on duration thresholds, we calculated the spatial suppression index (SSI; SSI = log10(ThresholdLARGE/ThresholdSMALL)) and tested for the Group and Phase differences in spatial suppression using repeated measures ANOVA. This analysis revealed the main effect of Phase (F(1,43) = 4.37, p = 0.043) and Group * Phase interaction (F(1,43) = 4.10, p = 0.049), which is illustrated in Fig 7B. In the PMDD group, SSI decreased from the follicular to the luteal phase (Student’s t-test, t(18) = 2.49, p = 0.022). Importantly, this decrease was mainly due to an improvement in discriminating the direction of motion for the large stimulus during the luteal phase (mean follicular ThresholdLARGE = 127 ms, mean luteal ThresholdLARGE = 110 ms; Student’s t-test, t(18) = 2.02, p = 0.06), rather than a change in the duration threshold for the small one (mean follicular ThresholdSMALL = 39 ms, mean luteal ThresholdSMALL = 44 ms; Student’s t-test, t(18) = 1.3, p = 0.19). There was also a tendency for stronger spatial suppression in women with PMDD than in control women during the follicular phase (Student’s t-test, t(18) = 1.8, p = 0.08; Fig 7B).

Fig 7. Dependence of motion direction distinction on the phase of the menstrual cycle (follicular, luteal phase) in control and PMDD participants.

Fig 7

(a) duration thresholds for the small and large stimuli (b) and spatial suppression index (SSI). Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. *p<0.05; **p<10−8.

Relationship between perceptual spatial suppression and GR power suppression

In control women, the percent of GR suppression from the ‘slow’ to the ‘medium’ condition (Fig 6C) correlated with the SSI during the follicular phase (NControl = 26; Pearson’s r = 0.53, p = 0.005). During the luteal phase, the correlation was in the same direction, although not significant (NControl = 26; Pearson’s r = 0.31, p = 0.12). The difference between correlation coefficients in the follicular and luteal phases in the control group was not significant. This direction of correlations is in line with results of our previous study in a smaller sample of neurotypical women (13 of them were included in the control sample in this study) where we estimated GR suppression as a ‘gamma suppression slope’ based on three velocity conditions [63]; for the same analysis of the present data see the S3 Table and S2 Text in S3 File). In the PMDD group, this correlation was absent (NPMDD = 19; follicular Pearson’s r = -0.12, p = 0.6; luteal Pearson’s r = -0.25, p = 0.3). The group difference in correlation coefficients was significant in the follicular phase (p = 0.035, two-sided) but not in the luteal phase (p = 0.084, two-sided).

Discussion

We hypothesized that in women with PMDD, the paradoxical response to neurosteroids, which results in changes of neural E/I balance during the luteal phase of the MC, would affect parameters of visual gamma oscillations and visual perception that are highly dependent on E/I balance, and that these abnormalities would be specific to the symptomatic luteal phase. To test this hypothesis, in women with and without PMDD we measured visual gamma responses (GRs) to drifting gratings with MEG and, in a separate psychophysical experiment, estimated perceptual spatial suppression. By using drift rate of a visual grating as a proxy of excitatory drive and changing it from ‘static’ to ‘slow’ and further to ‘medium’ and ‘fast’ [65], we probed the dynamics of gamma response, which strongly depends on the neural E/I balance.

Our predictions were only partially confirmed. The only parameter that distinguished women with PMDD from women with the healthy MC specifically during the luteal phase was the attenuated suppression of GR from the ‘slow’ condition optimal for GR generation to the ‘medium’ one. This attenuation indicates an abnormally decreased inhibition during the luteal phase in women with PMDD compared to control women. Reduced GR suppression during the luteal phase in women with PMDD correlated with greater severity of their clinical symptoms assessed on the same day. Another characteristic feature of the PMDD group was an atypically strong facilitation of GR power from the ‘suboptimal’ for gamma generation ‘static’ to the ‘optimal’ ‘slow’ condition. This excessive GR facilitation, however, was observed in both MC phases, indicating a constitutively elevated E/I ratio in the visual cortex in women with PMDD regardless of the effect of neurosteroid hormones.

Contrary to our expectations, GR frequency was not affected in women with PMDD. Their perceptual spatial suppression was not altered in the expected direction (i.e., was not decreased). On the contrary, in the follicular phase, spatial suppression in women with PMDD tended to be stronger than in control women, but it significantly decreased from the follicular to the luteal phase. In the discussion that follows, we will argue that in women with PMDD, neural excitability in the visual cortex is constitutively elevated and that the E/I balance shifts even more toward excitability during the luteal phase, possibly due to tolerance to ALLO.

Representativeness of the PMDD sample

It has been suggested that the often inconsistent findings in PMDD may be due, at least in part, to inconsistent diagnostic practices and varying symptom severity in patients [88, 110, 111]. We therefore start the discussion with the characterization of our PMDD sample.

To confirm the presence of PMDD and assess its severity, we used a questionnaire in which participants reported their symptoms daily for two or three MCs. This approach provides a more reliable assessment of PMDD than questionnaires based on retrospective PMS reports [88, 112]. Consistent with the previously described PMDD symptomatology [88, 113, 114], the severity of self-reported symptoms in participants of the clinical group increased from the ovulatory to the late luteal phase and decreased after the onset of menstruation (Fig 2). In a similar vein, according to the STAI and BDI questionnaires, their state anxiety and depressive symptoms, which are diagnostic features of PMDD [89], also exacerbated during the luteal phase. The state anxiety scores of women with PMDD were higher than those of control participants specifically during the luteal phase (Table 2). In addition to MC-dependent mood changes, high scores on trait anxiety, depression, and bipolar disorder questionnaires (according to their cut-off scores) in the PMDD sample agree well with previous reports of a high frequency of respective comorbid disorders in this clinical group [86].

A comparison of our participants’ daily PMS scores with those in the Eisenlohr-Moul et al’s study suggests that the majority of participants in our clinical sample had severe rather than mild form of PMDD (compare Fig 2 in the present paper and Fig 1 in [105]). Abnormal luteal hormone levels in women with PMDD compared with age-matched control participants are also consistent with the presence of severe PMDD in the clinical group. Indeed, there is evidence that decreased luteal progesterone, together with an elevated luteal estradiol/progesterone ratio in the mid-luteal phase of the MC (Fig 3F and 3G) characterize women with severe rather than mild PMS [111].

Thus, we discuss below the results obtained in a sample of women with severe PMDD and age-matched control women.

Altered modulation of MEG visual GR power by excitatory drive in PMDD

Phase-dependent changes in GR suppression in control women and in those with PMDD

In control women, changes in the visual GR power caused by the increase of excitatory drive were affected by the MC phase on the descending branch of the GR power modulation curve: during transition from the ‘slow’ to the ‘medium’ velocity (Fig 6C). The suppression of GR with an increase in excitatory drive from the ‘slow’ to the ‘medium’ and then to the ‘fast’ drift rate most likely reflects desynchronization of gamma oscillations as a result of compensatory ‘excessive’ activation of inhibitory neurons ([68, 115, 116]; see [65] for a detailed discussion). Indeed, animal studies have shown that increasing excitatory drive to the visual cortex above a certain limit leads to a greater increase in the inhibitory than excitatory current on the principal neurons, so the overall E/I ratio shifts toward inhibition [115]. Hence, a steeper slope of GR suppression at stimulation intensities exceeding the ‘optimal’ one for gamma generation may indicate a transition to a stronger inhibition of the principal neurons.

Despite the presence of phase-related changes in GR suppression in women with a healthy MC, their absolute GR power did not differ between the follicular to the luteal phases. The later result is consistent with that of Sumner et al. [55], and underscores the functional relevance of velocity-related modulation of power of gamma oscillations as a measure of the E/I ratio in the visual cortex. Considering the presumed role of strong inhibition in GR suppression, the increase in GR suppression in the luteal phase compared to the follicular phase of the MC in control women is consistent with enhanced inhibitory transmission. The increase in inhibition associated with the luteal phase may result from potentiation of the tonic GABAA conductance in principal neurons by ALLO, the neuroactive metabolite of progesterone. ALLO concentration follows that of progesterone [117] and is elevated during the luteal phase. ALLO targets the GABAARs family [118], but especially strongly potentiates δGABAARs (δGABAARs) [1921]. These receptors are located extrasynaptically and produce a tonic form of inhibition [20], which plays an important role in controlling neuronal excitability [119] and gamma generation [116, 120]. Furthermore, the elevated concentration of ALLO during the luteal phase affects GABAARs expression pattern: it potentiates the expression of δGABAARs and therefore further augments the effect of ALLO on tonic inhibitory neurotransmission [30, 121].

The putative enhancement of inhibitory neurotransmission from the follicular (i.e., low ALLO) to the luteal (i.e., high ALLO) phase of the MC is consistent with studies in rodents that demonstrate a potentiating effect of physiological levels of ALLO on inhibitory neurotransmission during the estrous cycle [30, 121, 122]. Evidence in favor of increased inhibitory tone during high-ALLO mid-luteal phase also comes from TMS studies in the motor cortex of naturally cycling women [40, 123, 124] and from reports of decreased seizure susceptibility during this phase of the MC [13]. However, the effect of ALLO on net excitability may vary between brain regions and species [125127]. As for the human visual cortex, the results by Epperson et al. [42] are particularly relevant to our finding. In a magnetic resonance spectroscopy study, these authors showed a significant decrease of GABA concentration in the occipital cortex from the follicular to the mid-luteal phase of the healthy MC and interpreted this result in terms of homeostatic regulation, which partially compensates for the increased sensitivity of GABAARs to ALLO by depressing GABA synthesis.

Given the scarce and inconsistent literature on the role of neurosteroid hormones in the human visual cortex [43, 128], our results pointing to increased inhibitory tone in gamma-generating visual circuitry during the luteal phase in healthy MC are particularly important. They strengthen the evidence linking the physiological effect of ALLO with increased tonic inhibition in the visual cortex of healthy naturally cycling women.

In contrast to control participants, women with PMDD did not show strengthening of GR suppression from the follicular to the luteal phase of the MC (Fig 6C). The absence of this normal change in the GR suppression suggests the lack of normal enhancement of neural inhibition in the visual cortex in women with PMDD during their symptomatic luteal phase. This result resembles that obtained by Smith and colleagues in the motor cortex of women with strong PMS [40]. Using TMS, Smith et al. found in these women a luteal phase-specific deficit in cortical inhibition and explained it by the lack of the normal increase in inhibition during the luteal phase.

Since our results in women with PMDD suggest the lack of normal luteal increase in GR suppression (indicative of unchanged inhibition between the follicular and luteal phases) rather than luteal decrease in GR suppression (which would be indicative of reduced inhibition in the luteal phase), they are more compatible with developing tolerance to ALLO [17] than with its paradoxical excitatory effect [21]. This tolerance may reflect an abnormally attenuated action of ALLO on δGABAARs on principal neurons, possibly due to an abnormal or insufficient plasticity of the δGABAARs in response to the postovulatory increase in ALLO [17, 19]. Indeed, reduced sensitivity to the sedative effects of GABAARs agonists, such as benzodiazepines, ethanol, and GABA-active steroids (pregnanolone), was previously demonstrated in women with PMDD during the luteal phase, when ALLO concentration is high, but not during the follicular phase, when the levels of GABA-active steroids are low [16, 129131].

Although changes in visual circuitry are unlikely to be directly related to the core PMDD symptoms (i.e., depression, anxiety, irritability, and mood lability), the luteal phase-specific deficit in visual GR suppression in PMDD was associated with more severe clinical symptoms assessed on the same day (Fig 6D). This correlation can be explained by abnormal action of progesterone-derived neurosteroids, i.e., ALLO, on δGABAARs in multiple brain structures, which affects gamma oscillations via altering inhibitory neurotransmission in the visual cortex and is manifested in clinical symptoms of PMDD via altering inhibitory neurotransmission in the brain regions involved in the control of mood and anxiety (see, e.g., [132134]).

To sum up, our results indicate that the GR suppression associated with increasing excitatory drive to the visual cortex reflects a luteal phase-specific and symptom severity-related deficit in neural inhibition in PMDD.

GR facilitation in control women and in those with PMDD

The GR facilitation caused by a moderate increase in excitatory drive (i.e., from static to slowly drifting grating) most probably has a different mechanism than the GR suppression caused by its strong increase (when drift rate of the grating changes from ‘slow’ to ‘medium’ or ‘fast’) (see [61, 65] for discussion). While GR suppression is thought to be associated with an ‘excessive’ activation of inhibitory neurons [68], GR facilitation reflects increased activation of the excitatory principal cells and their synchronization [57, 135]. When sensory input increases to a certain level, more excited cortical circuitries produce stronger gamma oscillations in both animals [136, 137] and humans [138, 139].

In contrast to GR suppression, GR facilitation showed no MC-related changes in either control women or women with PMDD. The dissociation between the GR facilitation and GR suppression in their sensitivity to cyclic changes in neurosteroids suggests a difference in their mechanisms. Yet, the enhanced GR facilitation differentiated women with PMDD from control participants in both phases of the MC. Although this result was not predicted, it suggests the existence of additional MC-independent mechanisms involved in PMDD.

Both principal cells and inhibitory interneurons play pivotal roles in generation of gamma [57, 135]. However, normal peak frequency of the GR in women with PMDD (Table 4) speaks against an apparent deficit in functioning of the inhibitory neurons (see later in this Discussion). Therefore, the atypically high GR facilitation in PMDD is rather due to an elevated excitability of the principal cells and their enhanced propensity to engage in synchronous oscillations in response to visual stimulation (i.e., ‘increased gain’). A similar deficit in gain regulation was observed in the visual cortex of patients with idiopathic generalized epilepsy using the steady-state visually evoked potentials (SSVEP) paradigm. These patients demonstrated an abnormally strong increase of SSVEPs associated with increasing contrast of a flickering grating [140142]. The authors concluded that the altered regulation of gain control in these patients is the result of hyperexcitability of local neuronal ensembles and enhanced lateral spread of the excitation.

Because GR facilitation did not change between the follicular and luteal phases in either control or PMDD women, but was elevated in PMDD women during both of these MC phases, it is unlikely to depend on neurosteroid changes in MC and may reflect constitutively elevated excitability of the principal neurons in women with PMDD.

Our finding of a neurofunctional abnormality that is not limited to the symptomatic luteal phase in women with PMDD is not unique. Several studies that applied a range of different methods and paradigms reported in women with PMDD neurofunctional abnormalities during asymptomatic period of the MC [16, 33, 37, 38, 42, 129131, 143145]. Significantly decreased GABA concentration in the visual cortex was found in women with PMDD during the follicular phase [42]. There is also evidence on altered activation in the dorsolateral prefrontal-cingulate cortex [38] and enhanced amygdala response to negative stimuli [145] in women with PMDD regardless of the MC phase. Thus, our results provide further support for the idea that pathophysiology of PMDD is not limited to the postovulatory luteal phase events, and that E/I imbalance in PMDD is present even during the asymptomatic follicular phase, but is further exacerbated during the premenstrual period, presumably due to altered sensitivity to GABA-active neurosteroids.

Because our study is limited to a relatively small sample of women with PMDD, some of whom may have had undiagnosed comorbid neuropsychiatric conditions (e.g., depression), it does not allow us to conclude whether their elevated GR facilitation, which was observed in both luteal and follicular phases, is specific for PMDD. Various neuropsychiatric disorders are thought to be associated with constitutively altered regulation of E/I balance in visual cortex [146, 147], which in turn can contribute to visual perceptual abnormalities frequently observed in these disorders [81, 148152]. Further studies that include the control groups of patients with these conditions without past or present PMDD are therefore needed.

Frequency of GR and first scan effect

The neural mechanisms regulating peak frequency of gamma oscillations are substantially different from those regulating their power. While power of gamma oscillations reflects synchronized activity of pyramidal cells, their frequency is predominantly controlled by tonic excitability of parvalbumin-containing (PV+) inhibitory neurons [66, 70, 153, 154]. Difference in the mechanisms that regulate GR frequency and power may explain their different dynamics associated with increasing excitatory drive [64, 136, 137].

Similarly to Sumner et al. [55], we found that in women with healthy MC, frequency of GR induced by static and slowly moving gratings increased from the follicular to the luteal phase (Fig 4A). However, a closer investigation showed that this effect was exclusively driven by those subjects who first came for the MEG experiment during the luteal phase (Fig 4C, right) and was absent in those who first came during the follicular phase (Fig 4C, left). Although Sumner et al. counterbalanced phases of the first visit (luteal 1st vs. follicular 1st), they did not test the effect of the order of visits, which, in our study, appears to be an important factor associated with the luteal phase-specific increase in gamma frequency.

Remarkably, control participants and those with PMDD showed the same dependency of GR frequency on the order of visits (Fig 4). Furthermore, no differences were found between the PMDD and control groups in either the GR frequency or its modulation by the drift rate and MC phase. Thus, contrary to our prediction, frequency of visual gamma oscillations seems to be normal in PMDD.

The effect of the visit order on the GR frequency is interesting by itself. It shows that modulation of gamma frequency by a hormonal status depends on some situational factor. Changes in attention and/or learning are unlikely to explain this result, as neither reaction time, nor percent of omission and commission errors differed between the first and second visit in subjects first investigated during the luteal or follicular phase of the MC (all p’s>0.11). On the other hand, according to the STAI questionnaire, the first visit to the MEG laboratory was associated in our participants with significantly higher situational anxiety than the second visit. We, therefore, suggest that a mild acute stress associated with being a study participant for the first time is a probable factor interfering with the hormone levels and contributing to our gamma frequency results. In line with this suggestion, several studies do show increased stress sensitivity in the luteal phase in both women without severe PMS [133, 155, 156] and in women with PMDD [19, 157161].

The factors associated with the 1st visit led to an increase in GR frequency only if they coincided with the luteal phase of the MC. During this phase, the δGABAARs-mediated inhibition is potentiated by ALLO (see the previous section). On the other hand, the δGABAARs-mediated inhibition is further increased in response to acute stress [162164]. The increase in δGABAARs-mediated tonic inhibition is expected to dampen excitability of PV+ inhibitory neurons, whose tonic inhibition is exclusively mediated by δGABAARs [165, 166]. This dampened tonic activity of PV+ inhibitory neurons is expected to decrease GR frequency [66, 167]. Our results, however, strongly indicate the opposite.

Although speculative, the impact of mild acute stress on gamma frequency during the luteal phase may be explained by the paradoxical excitatory effect of GABA-mediated currents on inhibitory neurons. It has been demonstrated that experimentally increased tonic GABAARs conductance, mimicking its natural increase during the luteal phase, has a depolarizing effect on adult hippocampal inhibitory neurons [116]. While this excitatory effect, mediated by tonic GABA currents, may not be sufficient to cause a measurable increase in gamma frequency in the luteal phase, mild acute stress, e.g., associated with the first visit, may further increase interneuron excitability. Indeed, acute stress rapidly increases plasma and cortical ALLO levels, which, in turn, leads to upregulation of δGABAARs [7173]. That is, when acute stress coincides with the luteal phase, δGABAARs reach their highest density. Apart from the potentiating effect of ALLO, other stress-derived neurosteriods have also been shown to increase the burst firing of inhibitory neurons [72, 168]. The increased excitability of inhibitory neurons due to the additive effects of the luteal phase and mild acute stress could thus explain the increased frequency of gamma oscillations in women whose first visit to the laboratory coincided with the luteal phase of their MC.

Whatever the exact mechanism leading to the luteal phase-specific effect of the first MEG scanning on the visual GR frequency, it did not differ in women with and without PMDD. Overall, the absence of PMDD-specific differences in the frequency of visual GR as well as in its modulation by either excitatory drive (i.e., grating’s drift rate), visit order, or MC phase suggests that activity of PV+ interneurons in the visual cortex is not altered in PMDD.

Although GR frequency was not affected in women with PMDD, we did find in this group an altered relationship between GR frequency and strength of GR suppression. In control women, we reproduced the previously reported correlation [64]: greater increase in gamma frequency correlated with greater gamma suppression caused by a strong excitatory drive to the visual cortex. This correlation suggests that in the healthy brain, the inhibitory down-regulation of the E/I balance evidenced by GR power suppression is proportional to excitation of inhibitory PV+ neurons, which define frequency of gamma oscillations (see [64] for thorough discussion). In women with PMDD, GR frequency did not correlate with the GR suppression, and the difference in the correlation coefficients between PMDD and control groups was significant in the luteal phase. This finding suggests that in women with PMDD the presynaptic inhibitory neurons fail to control synchronized activity of overexcited principal cells in an optimal way.

Perceptual spatial suppression in PMDD

Contrary to our original hypothesis, which was based on a presumed link between perceptual spatial suppression and cortical inhibition [75, 83], spatial suppression was not reduced in women with PMDD. During the follicular phase, our participants with PMDD tended to have even stronger spatial suppression than control women. This trend is opposite to the effects found in elderly individuals [79, 80] and people with neuropsychiatric disorders that are associated with a disturbed balance between excitation and inhibition [8184, 99, 169] (but for results similar to ours, see [150]), and is not consistent with an assumed reduction of neural inhibition. One possible reason for this unexpected result is that perceptual spatial suppression is a complex phenomenon, and, as Tadin [75] notes, its association with inhibitory dysfunction is not as straightforward as suggested in some studies cited above. Animal studies of neural surround suppression–a decrease in firing rate of cortical neurons with increasing visual stimulus size–have demonstrated the complexity of this phenomenon, which relies on E/I interactions in the primary visual cortex involving multiple inhibitory cell subtypes, as well as on their top-down regulation by higher-tier cortical areas [77, 170172]. The balance of neural mechanisms underlying surround suppression is essential for normal perception, while its disruption may lead to an altered perceptual spatial suppression in clinical populations [172].

Given the critical role of top-down influences in surround suppression [170, 173] and perceptual suppression itself [174, 175], our results rather indicate preserved or even elevated top-down control in the asymptomatic follicular phase in women with PMDD. However, the complex hierarchical organization of excitatory-inhibitory interactions underlying perceptual spatial suppression makes it difficult to separate initially elevated inhibition from homeostatic compensation for its deficit (see, e.g., [146]). Interestingly, such a putative compensatory mechanism may also present in patients with migraine, who are thought to have an elevated neural excitability in the visual cortex [176, 177], but at the same time demonstrate increased perceptual spatial suppression [178, 179].

While perceptual spatial suppression did not depend on the MC phase in the control participants, it significantly decreased from the follicular to the luteal phase in women with PMDD (Fig 7B). This decrease may reflect diminished effectiveness of the top-down compensatory influences in women with PMDD during the symptomatic luteal phase. This suggestion is broadly consistent with MRI findings of decreased anterior cingulate cortex control over the amygdala response to negative social stimulation in women with PMDD during the luteal phase compared to the follicular phase ([180]; but see [35, 38]), and with animal studies showing a facilitating effect of anterior cingulate cortex activation on surround inhibition [181].

The atypical regulation of perceptual spatial suppression in women with PMDD is further supported by the lack of the neuro-behavioral correlation found in the control group. Consistent with our previous results in typically developing children [63], visual GR suppression predicted spatial suppression in control women, with stronger gamma suppression corresponding to greater perceptual suppression (see Results section and S3 Table and S2 Text in S3 File for additional analysis). This relationship was absent in women with PMDD, indicating that some atypical compensatory processes may result in apparently normal perceptual spatial suppression in this clinical group.

Limitations and future direction of research

One limitation of the present study is the relatively small sample size. Although being comparable to that in the majority of neurophysiological studies into PMS/PMDD pathogenesis, it did not allow us to investigate the subtypes, which likely exist within the PMDD nosological category [105] and which may be associated with distinct neuro-functional deficits. According to the literature, about 50% of women with PMDD have comorbid conditions [8587]. It would be important to investigate if the constitutively elevated cortical excitability characterizes all women with PMDD, or rather certain patients with specific symptom patterns and/or comorbidities.

Further, our current design contrasts the early-to-mid follicular phase with the mid-to-late luteal phase, while it misses events in the middle of the MC. The inclusion of the ovulatory period where many women with PMDD are already starting to develop adverse symptoms, would help to better understand the relationship between MC-related changes in the E/I balance and PMS onset.

In the future, it would be interesting to model the effects of excitatory drive, acute stress, and PMDD diagnosis on gamma parameters using computer models of gamma generation in the visual cortex.

Finally, our work has been restricted to the occipital cortex, which is not usually involved in the pathogenesis of affective disorders. One cannot rule out the possibility that E/I balance in other, ‘core’ for PMDD, brain regions will be differently affected by neurosteroids and other factors associated with PMDD.

Conclusion

Although the main symptoms of PMDD are emotional rather than sensory-related, the neuro-functional abnormalities associated with this disorder are present even in the visual cortex. Our results showing atypical modulation of gamma power as a function of excitatory drive suggest that neuronal excitability is constitutively elevated in women with PMDD, which may be related to the high frequency of comorbid neuropsychiatric conditions in this clinical group. The E/I imbalance in women with PMDD is further exacerbated during the luteal phase, possibly as a result of their atypical sensitivity to neurosteroids. The mechanisms of elevated neural excitability and E/I imbalance in PMDD remain to be elucidated, but these abnormalities are rather unlikely to be caused by deficits in inhibitory neurons. Indeed, the peak frequency of oscillatory gamma response, which is primarily regulated by tonic excitability of parvalbumin-containing interneurons, was normal in PMDD. Moreover, women with PMDD and control women demonstrated the same luteal phase-specific effect of ‘the first scan’ on the gamma peak frequency. The presence of normal or even slightly increased (in the follicular phase) perceptual spatial suppression in PMDD also indicates that there is no major deficit in activity of inhibitory neurons, at least in the visual cortex. Therefore, our results suggest that the changes in neural E/I balance in the visual cortex in women with PMDD are most likely explained by increased tonic excitability of the principal cells and/or impaired regulation of their excitability at the synaptic levels. Overall, our results contribute to elucidating the mechanisms of PMDD and provide new insights into the functional correlates of the visual gamma rhythm.

Supporting information

S1 Fig

Violin plots for gamma response (GR) power (upper panel) and GR frequency (lower panel). Note that the group differences were not significant for all of these GR parameters (t-test, all p’s>0.12; uncorrected for multiple comparisons).

(DOCX)

S2 Fig. Violin plots of gamma response (GR) suppression and GR facilitation scores.

(DOCX)

S1 File

(DOCX)

S2 File. Results of bipolar disorder and depression questionnaires in women with PMDD.

(DOCX)

S3 File. Gamma suppression slope and perceptual spatial suppression.

(DOCX)

S1 Table. Partial Spearman’s correlations between steroid hormone levels (estradiol, progesterone) and gamma response (GR) power and frequency adjusted for age in the two groups of participants (control, PMDD).

Correlations with p<0.05 (uncorrected for multiple comparisons) are highlighted in bold.

(DOCX)

S1 Data. The data used for statistical analysis.

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

We sincerely thank all of the women who participated in this study.

The study was conducted at the unique research facility "Center for Neurocognitive Research (MEG-Center)" of MSUPE.

Data Availability

All relevant data are available in the Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

This study was funded by Moscow State University of Psychology and Education (MSUPE). The funder had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Azcoitia I, Yague JG, Garcia-Segura LM. Estradiol synthesis within the human brain. Neuroscience. 2011;191:139–47. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.02.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Bixo M, Backstrom T, Winblad B, Selstam G, Andersson A. Comparison between pre- and postovulatory distributions of oestradiol and progesterone in the brain of the PMSG-treated rat. Acta Physiol Scand. 1986;128(2):241–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-1716.1986.tb07972.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Micevych P, Sinchak K. Estradiol regulation of progesterone synthesis in the brain. Mol Cell Endocrinol. 2008;290(1–2):44–50. doi: 10.1016/j.mce.2008.04.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Farage MA, Osborn TW, MacLean AB. Cognitive, sensory, and emotional changes associated with the menstrual cycle: A review. Arch Gynecol Obstet. 2008;278(4):299–307. doi: 10.1007/s00404-008-0708-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Biggs WS, Demuth RH. Premenstrual syndrome and premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Am Fam Physician. 2011;84(8):918–24. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Direkvand-Moghadam A, Sayehmiri K, Delpisheh A, Satar K. Epidemiology of premenstrual syndrome, a systematic review and meta-analysis study. J Clin Diagnostic Res. 2014;8(2):106–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Halbreich U, Borenstein J, Pearlstein T, Kahn LS. The prevalence, impairment, impact, and burden of premenstrual dysphoric disorder (PMS/PMDD). Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2003;28:1–23. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(03)00098-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Tschudin S, Bertea PC, Zemp E. Prevalence and predictors of premenstrual syndrome and premenstrual dysphoric disorder in a population-based sample. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2010;13(6):485–94. doi: 10.1007/s00737-010-0165-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Gao M, Gao D, Sun H, Cheng X, An L, Qiao M. Trends in Research Related to Premenstrual Syndrome and Premenstrual Dysphoric Disorder From 1945 to 2018: A Bibliometric Analysis. Front Public Heal. 2021;9:1–14. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.596128 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Chan JH, Lo C, Hsu CD, Chiu CC, Huang MC, Liao SC, et al. Premenstrual dysphoric symptoms and lifetime suicide experiences in patients with mood disorder. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 2021;71(7):82–7. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2021.04.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Wikman A, Sacher J, Bixo M, Hirschberg AL, Kopp Kallner H, Epperson CN, et al. Prevalence and correlates of current suicidal ideation in women with premenstrual dysphoric disorder. BMC Womens Health. 2022;22(1):1–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Yang Q, Sjölander A, Li Y, Viktorin A, Bertone-Johnson ER, Ye W, et al. Clinical indications of premenstrual disorders and subsequent risk of injury: a population-based cohort study in Sweden. BMC Med. 2021;19(1):1–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Finocchi C, Ferrari M. Female reproductive steroids and neuronal excitability. Neurol Sci. 2011;32(1):31–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Bäckström T, Haage D, Löfgren M, Johansson IM, Strömberg J, Nyberg S, et al. Paradoxical effects of GABA-A modulators may explain sex steroid induced negative mood symptoms in some persons. Neuroscience. 2011;191:46–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.03.061 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Dubey N, Hoffman JF, Schuebel K, Yuan Q, Martinez PE, Nieman LK, et al. The ESC/E(Z) complex, an effector of response to ovarian steroids, manifests an intrinsic difference in cells from women with premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Mol Psychiatry. 2017;22(8):1172–84. doi: 10.1038/mp.2016.229 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Timby E, Bäckström T, Nyberg S, Stenlund H, Wihlbäck ACN, Bixo M. Women with premenstrual dysphoric disorder have altered sensitivity to allopregnanolone over the menstrual cycle compared to controls—a pilot study. Psychopharmacology. 2016;233(11):2109–17. doi: 10.1007/s00213-016-4258-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Turkmen S, Backstrom T, Wahlstrom G, Andreen L, Johansson IM. Tolerance to allopregnanolone with focus on the GABA-A receptor. Br J Pharmacol. 2011;162(2):311–27. doi: 10.1111/j.1476-5381.2010.01059.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Schmidt PJ, Nieman LK, Danaceau MA, Adams LF, Rubinow DR. With and in Those Without Premenstrual Syndrome. N Engl J Med. 1998;338(4):209–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hantsoo L, Epperson CN. Allopregnanolone in premenstrual dysphoric disorder (PMDD): Evidence for dysregulated sensitivity to GABA-A receptor modulating neuroactive steroids across the menstrual cycle. Neurobiol Stress. 2020;12:100213. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2020.100213 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Reddy DS. Neurosteroids: Endogenous role in the human brain and therapeutic potentials. Prog Brain Res. 2010;186:113–37. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53630-3.00008-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Bäckström T, Bixo M, Johansson M, Nyberg S, Ossewaarde L, Ragagnin G, et al. Allopregnanolone and mood disorders. Prog Neurobiol. 2014;113:88–94. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.07.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Barth AMI, Ferando I, Mody I. Ovarian cycle-linked plasticity of δ-GABAA receptor subunits in hippocampal interneurons affects γ oscillations in vivo. Front Cell Neurosci. 2014;8:1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Lovick TA. Sex determinants of experimental panic attacks. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;46:465–71. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Wohlfarth KM, Bianchi MT, Macdonald RL. Enhanced neurosteroid potentiation of ternary GABAA receptors containing the δ subunit. J Neurosci. 2002;22(5):1541–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Balthazart J, Ball GF. Is brain estradiol a hormone or a neurotransmitter? Trends Neurosci. 2006;29(5):241–9. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2006.03.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Clemens AM, Lenschow C, Beed P, Li L, Sammons R, Naumann RK, et al. Estrus-Cycle Regulation of Cortical Inhibition. Curr Biol. 2019;29(4):605–615.e1–e6. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.01.045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Kow LM, Easton A, Pfaff DW. Acute estrogen potentiates excitatory responses of neurons in rat hypothalamic ventromedial nucleus. Brain Res. 2005;1043(1–2):124–31. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.02.068 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Schroeder A, Hudson M, Du X, Wu YWC, Nakamura J, van den Buuse M, et al. Estradiol and raloxifene modulate hippocampal gamma oscillations during a spatial memory task. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2017;78:85–92. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.01.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Schwarz JM, Liang SL, Thompson SM, McCarthy MM. Estradiol Induces Hypothalamic Dendritic Spines by Enhancing Glutamate Release: A Mechanism for Organizational Sex Differences. Neuron. 2008;58(4):584–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.03.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Wu X, Gangisetty O, Carver CM, Reddy DS. Estrous cycle regulation of extrasynaptic d-containing GABAA receptor-mediated tonic inhibition and limbic epileptogenesis. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2013;346(1):146–60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Wu YWC, Du X, Van den Buuse M, Hill RA. Sex differences in the adolescent developmental trajectory of parvalbumin interneurons in the hippocampus: A role for estradiol. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2014;45:167–78. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.03.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Huang Y, Zhou R, Wu M, Wang Q, Zhao Y. Premenstrual syndrome is associated with blunted cortisol reactivity to the TSST. Stress. 2015;18(2):160–8. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2014.999234 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Dan R, Reuveni I, Canetti L, Weinstock M, Segman R, Goelman G, et al. Trait-related changes in brain network topology in premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Horm Behav. 2020;124:104782. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2020.104782 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Dubol M, Epperson CN, Lanzenberger R, Sundström-Poromaa I, Comasco E. Neuroimaging premenstrual dysphoric disorder: A systematic and critical review. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2020;57:100838. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2020.100838 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Petersen N, Ghahremani DG, Rapkin AJ, Berman SM, Wijker N, Liang L, et al. Resting-state functional connectivity in women with PMDD. Transl Psychiatry. 2019;9(1). doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-0670-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Baller EB, Wei SM, Kohn PD, Rubinow DR, Alarcón G, Schmidt PJ, et al. Abnormalities of dorsolateral prefrontal function in women with premenstrual dysphoric disorder: A multimodal neuroimaging study. Am J Psychiatry. 2013;170(3):305–14. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12030385 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Bannbers E, Gingnell M, Engman J, Morell A, Comasco E, Kask K, et al. The effect of premenstrual dysphoric disorder and menstrual cycle phase on brain activity during response inhibition. J Affect Disord. 2012;142(1–3):347–50. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.04.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Comasco E, Hahn A, Ganger S, Gingnell M, Bannbers E, Oreland L, et al. Emotional fronto-cingulate cortex activation and brain derived neurotrophic factor polymorphism in premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35(9):4450–8. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22486 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Niederhofer H. Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation for Premenstrual Dysphoric Disorder: A Case Report. Physicians Postgrad Press. 2009;11(2):84–5. doi: 10.4088/pcc.08l00657 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Smith MJ, Adams LF, Schmidt PJ, Rubinow DR, Wassermann EM. Abnormal luteal phase excitability of the motor cortex in women with premenstrual syndrome. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;54(7):757–62. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01924-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Jeong JK, Tremere LA, Burrows K, Majewska AK, Pinaud R. The mouse primary visual cortex is a site of production and sensitivity to estrogens. PLoS One. 2011;6(5). doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020400 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Epperson NC, Haga K, Mason GF, Sellers E, Gueorguieva R, Zhang W, et al. Cortical γ-aminobutyric acid levels across the menstrual cycle in healthy women and those with premenstrual dysphoric disorder: A proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59(9):851–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Schloemer N, Lenz M, Tegenthoff M, Dinse HR, Höffken O. Parallel modulation of intracortical excitability of somatosensory and visual cortex by the gonadal hormones estradiol and progesterone. Sci Rep. 2020;10(1):1–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Sergeeva EG, Espinosa-Garcia C, Atif F, Pardue MT, Stein DG. Neurosteroid allopregnanolone reduces ipsilateral visual cortex potentiation following unilateral optic nerve injury. Exp Neurol. 2018;306:138–48. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2018.05.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Zilles K, Palomero-Gallagher N, Grefkes C, Scheperjans F, Boy C, Amunts K, et al. Architectonics of the human cerebral cortex and transmitter receptor fingerprints: Reconciling functional neuroanatomy and neurochemistry. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2002;12(6):587–99. doi: 10.1016/s0924-977x(02)00108-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Craig MM, Misic B, Pappas I, Adapa RM, Menon DK, Stamatakis EA. Propofol sedation-induced alterations in brain connectivity reflect parvalbumin interneurone distribution in human cerebral cortex. Br J Anaesth. 2021;126(4):835–44. doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2020.11.035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Avitabile T, Longo A, Caruso S, Gagliano C, Amato R, Scollo D, et al. Changes in visual evoked potentials during the menstrual cycle in young women. Curr Eye Res. 2007;32(11):999–1003. doi: 10.1080/02713680701679006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.De Lima Resende LA, Silva MD, Impemba F, Achôa NB, Schelp AO. Multimodal evoked potentials and the ovarian cycle in young ovulating women. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2000;58(2-B):418–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Lusk BR, Carr AR, Ranson VA, Bryant RA, Felmingham KL. Early visual processing is enhanced in the midluteal phase of the menstrual cycle. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2015;62:343–51. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.08.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Yilmaz H, Erkin EF, Mavioǧlu H, Sungurtekin Ü. Changes in pattern reversal evoked potentials during menstrual cycle. Int Ophthalmol. 1998;22(1):27–30. doi: 10.1023/a:1006165126702 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Bazanova OM, Kondratenko A V., Kuzminova OI, Muravlyova KB, Petrova SE. EEG alpha indices depending on the menstrual cycle phase and salivary progesterone level. Hum Physiol. 2014;40(2):140–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Brötzner CP, Klimesch W, Kerschbaum HH. Progesterone-associated increase in ERP amplitude correlates with an improvement in performance in a spatial attention paradigm. Brain Res. 2015;1595:74–83. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.11.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Haraguchi R, Hoshi H, Ichikawa S, Hanyu M, Nakamura K, Fukasawa K, et al. The Menstrual Cycle Alters Resting-State Cortical Activity: A Magnetoencephalography Study. Front Hum Neurosci. 2021;15:1–16. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.652789 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Lin IM, Tsai YC, Peper E, Yen CF. Depressive mood and frontal alpha asymmetry during the luteal phase in premenstrual dysphoric disorder. J Obstet Gynaecol Res. 2013;39(5):998–1006. doi: 10.1111/jog.12020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Sumner RL, McMillan RL, Shaw AD, Singh KD, Sundram F, Muthukumaraswamy SD. Peak visual gamma frequency is modified across the healthy menstrual cycle. Hum Brain Mapp. 2018;39(8):3187–202. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24069 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Bartos M, Vida I, Jonas P. Synaptic mechanisms of synchronized gamma oscillations in inhibitory interneuron networks. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8(1):45–56. doi: 10.1038/nrn2044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Buzsáki G, Wang XJ. Mechanisms of Gamma Oscillations. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2008;35:1–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Ray S, Maunsell JHR. Do gamma oscillations play a role in cerebral cortex? Trends Cogn Sci. 2015;19(2):78–85. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.12.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Hoogenboom N, Schoffelen JM, Oostenveld R, Parkes LM, Fries P. Localizing human visual gamma-band activity in frequency, time and space. Neuroimage. 2006;29(3):764–73. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.043 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Muthukumaraswamy SD. High-frequency brain activity and muscle artifacts in MEG/EEG: A review and recommendations. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Manyukhina VO, Rostovtseva EN, Prokofyev AO, Obukhova TS, Schneiderman JF, Stroganova TA, et al. Visual gamma oscillations predict sensory sensitivity in females as they do in males. Sci Rep. 2021;11(1):1–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Orekhova E V., Stroganova TA, Schneiderman JF, Lundström S, Riaz B, Sarovic D, et al. Neural gain control measured through cortical gamma oscillations is associated with sensory sensitivity. Hum Brain Mapp. 2019;40(5):1583–93. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24469 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Orekhova E V., Rostovtseva EN, Manyukhina VO, Prokofiev AO, Obukhova TS, Nikolaeva AY, et al. Spatial suppression in visual motion perception is driven by inhibition: Evidence from MEG gamma oscillations. Neuroimage. 2020;213:116753. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116753 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Orekhova E V., Sysoeva O V., Schneiderman JF, Lundström S, Galuta IA, Goiaeva DE, et al. Input-dependent modulation of MEG gamma oscillations reflects gain control in the visual cortex. Sci Rep. 2018;8(1):1–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Orekhova E V., Prokofyev AO, Nikolaeva AY, Schneiderman JF, Stroganova TA. Additive effect of contrast and velocity suggests the role of strong excitatory drive in suppression of visual gamma response. PLoS One. 2020;15(2):1–23. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0228937 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Mann EO, Mody I. Control of hippocampal gamma oscillation frequency by tonic inhibition and excitation of interneurons. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13(2):205–12. doi: 10.1038/nn.2464 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Rodriguez R, Kallenbach U, Singer W, Munk MHJ. Short- and long-term effects of cholinergic modulation on gamma oscillations and response synchronization in the visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2004;24(46):10369–78. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1839-04.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Börgers C, Kopell N. Effects of noisy drive on rhythms in networks of excitatory and inhibitory neurons. Neural Comput. 2005;17(3):557–608. doi: 10.1162/0899766053019908 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Cannon J, Mccarthy MM, Lee S, Lee J, Börgers C, Whittington MA, et al. Neurosystems: Brain rhythms and cognitive processing. Eur J Neurosci. 2014;39(5):705–19. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12453 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Ferando I, Mody I. In vitro gamma oscillations following partial and complete ablation of δ subunit-containing GABAA receptors from parvalbumin interneurons. Neuropharmacology. 2015;88:91–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Barbaccia ML, Concas A, Serra M, Biggio G. Stress and neurosteroids in adult and aged rats. Exp Gerontol. 1998;33(7–8):697–712. doi: 10.1016/s0531-5565(98)00042-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Mody I, Maguire J. The reciprocal regulation of stress hormones and GABA A receptors. Front Cell Neurosci. 2012;6:1–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Purdy RH, Morrow AL, Moore PH, Paul SM. Stress-induced elevations of γ-aminobutyric acid type a receptor-active steroids in the rat brain. PNAS. 1991;88(10):4553–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Tadin D, Park WJ, Dieter KC, Melnick MD, Lappin JS, Blake R. Spatial suppression promotes rapid figure-ground segmentation of moving objects. Nat Commun. 2019;10(1):1–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Tadin D. Suppressive mechanisms in visual motion processing: From perception to intelligence. Vision Res. 2015;115:58–70. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2015.08.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Tadin D, Lappin JS, Gilroy LA, Blake R. Perceptual consequences of centre-surround antagonism in visual motion processing. Nature. 2003;424(6946):312–5. doi: 10.1038/nature01800 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Angelucci A, Bijanzadeh M, Nurminen L, Federer F, Merlin S, Bressloff PC. Circuits and Mechanisms for Surround Modulation in Visual Cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2017;40:425–51. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-072116-031418 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Nurminen L, Angelucci A. Multiple components of surround modulation in primary visual cortex: Multiple neural circuits with multiple functions? Vision Res. 2014;104:47–56. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2014.08.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Betts LR, Taylor CP, Sekuler AB, Bennett PJ. Aging reduces center-surround antagonism in visual motion processing. Neuron. 2005;45(3):361–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Betts LR, Sekuler AB, Bennett PJ. Spatial characteristics of motion-sensitive mechanisms change with age and stimulus spatial frequency. Vision Res. 2012;53(1):1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.11.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Golomb JD, McDavitt JRB, Ruf BM, Chen JI, Saricicek A, Maloney KH, et al. Enhanced visual motion perception in major depressive disorder. J Neurosci. 2009;29(28):9072–7. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1003-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Linares D, Amoretti S, Marin-Campos R, Sousa A, Prades L, Dalmau J, et al. Spatial suppression and sensitivity for motion in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull Open. 2020;1(1):1–76. [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Tadin D, Kim J, Doop ML, Gibson C, Lappin JS, Blake R, et al. Weakened center-surround interactions in visual motion processing in schizophrenia. J Neurosci. 2006;26(44):11403–12. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2592-06.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Zhuang X, Chen Y, Zhuang X, Xing T, Chen T, Jiang G, et al. Impaired center-surround suppression in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimer’s Dis. 2017;55(3):1101–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Cirillo PC, Passos RBF, Bevilaqua MC do N, López JRRA, Nardi AE. Bipolar disorder and premenstrual syndrome or premenstrual dysphoric disorder comorbidity: A systematic review. Rev Bras Psiquiatr. 2012;34(4):467–79. doi: 10.1016/j.rbp.2012.04.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.de Carvalho AB, Cardoso T de A, Mondin TC, da Silva RA, Souza LD de M, Magalhães PV da S, et al. Prevalence and factors associated with Premenstrual Dysphoric Disorder: A community sample of young adult women. Psychiatry Res. 2018;268:42–5. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.06.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Kim DR, Gyulai L, Freeman EW, Morrison MF, Baldassano C, Dubé B. Premenstrual dysphoric disorder and psychiatric co-morbidity. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2004;7(1):37–47. doi: 10.1007/s00737-003-0027-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Eisenlohr-Moul TA, Girdler SS, Schmalenberger KM, Dawson DN, Surana P, Johnson JL, et al. Toward the reliable diagnosis of DSM-5 premenstrual dysphoric disorder: The Carolina premenstrual assessment scoring system (C-PASS). Am J Psychiatry. 2017;174(1):51–9. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.15121510 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Psychiatric Association American. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). 2013;
  • 90.Steiner M, Peer M, MacDougall M, Haskett R. The premenstrual tension syndrome rating scales: An updated version. J Affect Disord. 2011;135(1–3):82–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.06.058 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Steiner M, Streiner DL. Validation of a revised visual analog scale for premenstrual mood symptoms: Results from prospective and retrospective trials. Can J Psychiatry. 2005;50(6):327–32. doi: 10.1177/070674370505000607 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Spielberger CD. Manual for the State-Trait Inventory STAI (Form Y). Vol. 29, CA: Mind Garden. 1983. 348–353 p. [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Ghaemi SN, Miller CJ, Berv DA, Klugman J, Rosenquist KJ, Pies RW. Sensitivity and specificity of a new bipolar spectrum diagnostic scale. J Affect Disord. 2005;84(2–3):273–7. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(03)00196-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Hirschfeld RMA. The Mood Disorder Questionnaire: A simple, patient-rated screening instrument for bipolar disorder. Prim Care Companion J Clin Psychiatry. 2002;4(1):9–11. doi: 10.4088/pcc.v04n0104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Vieta E, Sánchez-Moreno J, Bulbena A, Chamorro L, Ramos JL, Artal J, et al. Cross validation with the mood disorder questionnaire (MDQ) of an instrument for the detection of hypomania in Spanish: The 32 item hypomania symptom check list (HCL-32). J Affect Disord. 2007;101(1–3):43–55. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.09.040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Beck AT, Steer RA, Carbin MG. Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clin Psychol Rev. 1988;8(1):77–100. [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Taulu S, Simola J. Spatiotemporal signal space separation method for rejecting nearby interference in MEG measurements. Phys Med Biol. 2006;51(7):1759–68. doi: 10.1088/0031-9155/51/7/008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Van Veen BD, Van Drongelen W, Yuchtman M, Suzuki A. Localization of brain electrical activity via linearly constrained minimum variance spatial filtering. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 1997;44(9):867–80. doi: 10.1109/10.623056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Sysoeva OV., Galuta IA, Davletshina MS, Orekhova E V., Stroganova TA. Abnormal size-dependent modulation of motion perception in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Front Neurosci. 2017;11:1–13. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00164 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Found Stat Comput; Vienna, Austria. 2013; [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Curran-Everett D, Williams CL. Explorations in statistics: The analysis of change. Adv Physiol Educ. 2015;39(1):49–54. doi: 10.1152/advan.00018.2015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Curran-Everett D. Explorations in statistics: The analysis of ratios and normalized data. Adv Physiol Educ. 2013;37(3):213–9. doi: 10.1152/advan.00053.2013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Harris CR, Millman KJ, van der Walt SJ, Gommers R, Virtanen P, Cournapeau D, et al. Array programming with NumPy. Nature. 2020;585(7825):357–62. doi: 10.1038/s41586-020-2649-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Virtanen P, Gommers R, Oliphant TE, Haberland M, Reddy T, Cournapeau D, et al. SciPy 1.0: fundamental algorithms for scientific computing in Python. Nat Methods. 2020;17(3):261–72. doi: 10.1038/s41592-019-0686-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Eisenlohr-Moul TA, Kaiser G, Weise C, Schmalenberger KM, Kiesner J, Ditzen B, et al. Are there temporal subtypes of premenstrual dysphoric disorder?: Using group-based trajectory modeling to identify individual differences in symptom change. Psychol Med. 2020;50(6):964–72. doi: 10.1017/S0033291719000849 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Gaetz W, Roberts TPL, Singh KD, Muthukumaraswamy SD. Functional and structural correlates of the aging brain: Relating visual cortex (V1) gamma band responses to age-related structural change. Hum Brain Mapp. 2012;33(9):2035–46. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21339 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Muthukumaraswamy SD, Singh KD, Swettenham JB, Jones DK. Visual gamma oscillations and evoked responses: Variability, repeatability and structural MRI correlates. Neuroimage. 2010;49(4):3349–57. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Orekhova E V., Butorina A V., Sysoeva O V., Prokofyev AO, Nikolaeva AY, Stroganova TA. Frequency of gamma oscillations in humans is modulated by velocity of visual motion. J Neurophysiol. 2015;114(1):244–55. doi: 10.1152/jn.00232.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Shankara-Narayana N, Zawada S, Walters KA, Desai R, Marren A, Handelsman DJ. Accuracy of a Direct Progesterone Immunoassay. J Appl Lab Med. 2016;1(3):294–9. doi: 10.1373/jalm.2016.020123 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Amiel Castro RT, Ehlert U, Fischer S. Variation in genes and hormones of the hypothalamic-pituitary-ovarian axis in female mood disorders–A systematic review and meta-analysis. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2021;62:100929. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2021.100929 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Munday MR, Brush MG, Taylor RW. Correlations between progesterone, oestradiol and aldosterone levels in the premenstrual syndrome. Clin Endocrinol (Oxf). 1981;14(1):1–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2265.1981.tb00359.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Bosman RC, Albers CJ, De Jong J, Batalas N, Aan Het Rot M. No Menstrual Cyclicity in Mood and Interpersonal Behaviour in Nine Women with Self-Reported Premenstrual Syndrome. Psychopathology. 2018;51(4):290–4. doi: 10.1159/000489268 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Hammarbäck S, Damber JE, Bäckström T. Relationship between symptom severity and hormone changes in women with premenstrual syndrome. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1989;68(1):125–30. doi: 10.1210/jcem-68-1-125 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Seippel L, Bäckström T. Luteal-phase estradiol relates to symptom severity in patients with premenstrual syndrome. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1998;83(6):1988–92. doi: 10.1210/jcem.83.6.4899 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Adesnik H. Synaptic Mechanisms of Feature Coding in the Visual Cortex of Awake Mice. Neuron. 2017;95(5):1147–59. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.08.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Pavlov I, Savtchenko LP, Song I, Koo J, Pimashkin A, Rusakov DA, et al. Tonic GABAA conductance bidirectionally controls interneuron firing pattern and synchronization in the CA3 hippocampal network. PNAS. 2014;111(1):504–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1308388110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Timby E, Balgård M, Nyberg S, Spigset O, Andersson A, Porankiewicz-Asplund J, et al. Pharmacokinetic and behavioral effects of allopregnanolone in healthy women. Psychopharmacology. 2006;186(3):414–24. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0148-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.MacKenzie G, Maguire J. The role of ovarian hormone-derived neurosteroids on the regulation of GABAA receptors in affective disorders. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2014;231(17):3333–42. doi: 10.1007/s00213-013-3423-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Stell BM, Brickley SG, Tang CY, Farrant M, Mody I. Neuroactive steroids reduce neuronal excitability by selectively enhancing tonic inhibition mediated by δ subunit-containing GABAA receptors. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100(24):14439–44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Héja L, Nyitrai G, Kékesi O, Dobolyi Á, Szabó P, Fiáth R, et al. Astrocytes convert network excitation to tonic inhibition of neurons. BMC Biol. 2012;10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Maguire JL, Stell BM, Rafizadeh M, Mody I. Ovarian cycle-linked changes in GABAA receptors mediating tonic inhibition alter seizure susceptibility and anxiety. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8(6):797–804. doi: 10.1038/nn1469 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Devall AJ, Santos JM, Fry JP, Honour JW, Brandão ML, Lovick TA. Elevation of brain allopregnanolone rather than 5-HT release by short term, low dose fluoxetine treatment prevents the estrous cycle-linked increase in stress sensitivity in female rats. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2015;25(1):113–23. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2014.11.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Smith MJ, Keel JC, Greenberg BD, Adams LF, Schmidt PJ, Rubinow DA, et al. Menstrual cycle effects on cortical excitability. Neurology. 1999;53(9):2069–72. doi: 10.1212/wnl.53.9.2069 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Smith MJ, Adams LF, Schmidt PJ, Rubinow DR, Wassermann EM. Effects of ovarian hormones on human cortical excitability. Ann Neurol. 2002;51(5):599–603. doi: 10.1002/ana.10180 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Engin E, Treit D. The anxiolytic-like effects of allopregnanolone vary as a function of intracerebral microinfusion site: The amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex, or hippocampus. Behav Pharmacol. 2007;18(5–6):461–70. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e3282d28f6f [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Lovick TA, Griffiths JL, Dunn SMJ, Martin IL. Changes in GABAA receptor subunit expression in the midbrain during the oestrous cycle in Wistar rats. Neuroscience. 2005;131(2):397–405. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.11.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Rossetti MF, Cambiasso MJ, Holschbach MA, Cabrera R. Oestrogens and Progestagens: Synthesis and Action in the Brain. J Neuroendocrinol. 2016;28(7):1–11. doi: 10.1111/jne.12402 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Sy JL, Tomarken AJ, Patel V, Blake R. The time course of binocular rivalry during the phases of the menstrual cycle. J Vis. 2016;16(15):1–19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Nyberg S, Wahlström G, Bäckström T, Poromaa IS. Altered sensitivity to alcohol in the late luteal phase among patients with premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2004;29(6):767–77. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(03)00121-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Sundstrom I, Ashbrook D, Backstrom T. Reduced benzodiazepine sensitivity in patients with premenstrual syndrome: a pilot study. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1997;22(1):25–38. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(96)00035-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Sundström I, Bäckström T. Patients with premenstrual syndrome have decreased saccadic eye velocity compared to control subjects. Biol Psychiatry. 1998;44(8):755–64. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00012-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Liu B, Wang G, Gao D, Gao F, Zhao B, Qiao M, et al. Alterations of GABA and glutamate-glutamine levels in premenstrual dysphoric disorder: A 3T proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. Psychiatry Res—Neuroimaging. 2015;231(1):64–70. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.10.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Ossewaarde L, Hermans EJ, van Wingen GA, Kooijman SC, Johansson IM, Bäckström T, et al. Neural mechanisms underlying changes in stress-sensitivity across the menstrual cycle. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35(1):47–55. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.08.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Van Wingen G, Van Broekhoven F, Verkes RJ, Petersson KM, Bäckström T, Buitelaar J, et al. How progesterone impairs memory for biologically salient stimuli in healthy young women. J Neurosci. 2007;27(42):11416–23. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1715-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Vinck M, Womelsdorf T, Fries P. Gamma-band synchronization and information transmission. Princ Neural Coding. 2013;449–70. [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Jia X, Xing D, Kohn A. No consistent relationship between gamma power and peak frequency in macaque primary visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2013;33(1):17–25. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1687-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Salelkar S, Somasekhar GM, Ray S. Distinct frequency bands in the local field potential are differently tuned to stimulus drift rate. J Neurophysiol. 2018;120(2):681–92. doi: 10.1152/jn.00807.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Hermes D, Miller KJ, Wandell BA, Winawer J. Stimulus dependence of gamma oscillations in human visual cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2015;25(9):2951–9. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu091 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Perry G, Hamandi K, Brindley LM, Muthukumaraswamy SD, Singh KD. The properties of induced gamma oscillations in human visual cortex show individual variability in their dependence on stimulus size. Neuroimage. 2013;68:83–92. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.043 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Porciatti V, Bonanni P, Fiorentini A, Guerrini R. Lack of cortical contrast gain control in human photosensitive epilepsy. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3(3):259–63. doi: 10.1038/72972 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Tsai JJ, Norcia AM, Ales JM, Wade AR. Contrast gain control abnormalities in idiopathic generalized epilepsy. Ann Neurol. 2011;70(4):574–82. doi: 10.1002/ana.22462 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Won D, Kim W, Chaovalitwongse WA, Tsai JJ. Altered visual contrast gain control is sensitive for idiopathic generalized epilepsies. Clin Neurophysiol. 2017;128(2):340–8. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2016.12.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Kask K, Gulinello M, Bäckström T, Geyer MA, Sundström-Poromaa I. Patients with premenstrual dysphoric disorder have increased startle response across both cycle phases and lower levels of prepulse inhibition during the late luteal phase of the menstrual cycle. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33(9):2283–90. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301599 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 144.Monteleone P, Luisi S, Tonetti A, Bernardi F, Genazzani AD, Luisi M, et al. Allopregnanolone concentrations and premenstrual syndrome. Eur J Endocrinol. 2000;142(3):269–73. doi: 10.1530/eje.0.1420269 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Gingnell M, Morell A, Bannbers E, Wikström J, Sundström Poromaa I. Menstrual cycle effects on amygdala reactivity to emotional stimulation in premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Horm Behav. 2012;62(4):400–6. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.07.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Nelson SB, Valakh V. Excitatory/Inhibitory Balance and Circuit Homeostasis in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Neuron. 2015;87(4):684–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.07.033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 147.Sohal VS, Rubenstein JLR. Excitation-inhibition balance as a framework for investigating mechanisms in neuropsychiatric disorders. Mol Psychiatry. 2019;24(9):1248–57. doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0426-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 148.Miller SM, Gynther BD, Heslop KR, Liu GB, Mitchell PB, Ngo TT, et al. Slow binocular rivalry in bipolar disorder. Psychol Med. 2003;33(4):683–92. doi: 10.1017/s0033291703007475 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 149.Ngo TT, Mitchell PB, Martin NG, Miller SM. Psychiatric and genetic studies of binocular rivalry: An endophenotype for bipolar disorder? Acta Neuropsychiatr. 2011;23(1):37–42. [Google Scholar]
  • 150.Norton DJ, Ryan KM, Pizzagalli DA, Cronin-Golomb A, Chen Y. Dysregulation of visual motion inhibition in major depression. Psychiatry Res. 2016;249:214–21. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.04.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 151.Schallmo MP, Sponheim SR, Olman CA. Reduced contextual effects on visual contrast perception in schizophrenia and bipolar affective disorder. Psychol Med. 2015;45(16):3527–37. doi: 10.1017/S0033291715001439 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 152.Yang E, Tadin D, Glasser DM, Hong SW, Blake R, Park S. Visual context processing in schizophrenia. Clin Psychol Sci. 2013;1(1):5–15. doi: 10.1177/2167702612464618 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 153.Ferando I, Mody I. Altered gamma oscillations during pregnancy through loss of δ subunit-containing GABAA receptors on parvalbumin interneurons. Front Neural Circuits. 2013;7(144):1–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 154.Towers SK, Gloveli T, Traub RD, Driver JE, Engel D, Fradley R, et al. α5 subunit-containing GABAA receptors affect the dynamic range of mouse hippocampal kainate-induced gamma frequency oscillations in vitro. J Physiol. 2004;559(3):721–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 155.Hastrup JL, Light KC. Sex differences in cardiovascular stress responses: modulation as a function of menstrual cycle phases. J Psychosom Res. 1984;28(6):475–83. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(84)90081-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 156.Sato N, Miyake S. Cardiovascular reactivity to mental stress: Relationship with menstrual cycle and gender. J Physiol Anthropol Appl Human Sci. 2004;23(6):215–23. doi: 10.2114/jpa.23.215 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 157.Beddig T, Reinhard I, Kuehner C. Stress, mood, and cortisol during daily life in women with Premenstrual Dysphoric Disorder (PMDD). Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2019;109:104372. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.104372 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 158.Epperson CN, Pittman B, Czarkowski KA, Stiklus S, Krystal JH, Grillon C. Luteal-phase accentuation of acoustic startle response in women with premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32(10):2190–8. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301351 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 159.Hantsoo L, Epperson CN. Premenstrual Dysphoric Disorder: Epidemiology and Treatment. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2015;17(11). doi: 10.1007/s11920-015-0628-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 160.Logue CM, Moos RH. Perimenstrual symptoms: Prevalence and risk factors. Psychosom Med. 1986;48(6):388–414. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198607000-00002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 161.Parry BL, Javeed S, Laughlin GA, Hauger R, Clopton P. Cortisol circadian rhythms during the menstrual cycle and with sleep deprivation in premenstrual dysphoric disorder and normal control subjects. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;48(9):920–31. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00876-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 162.Bali A, Jaggi AS. Multifunctional aspects of allopregnanolone in stress and related disorders. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacology Biol Psychiatry. 2014;48:64–78. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2013.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 163.Biggio G, Pisu MG, Biggio F, Serra M. Allopregnanolone modulation of HPA axis function in the adult rat. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2014;231(17):3437–44. doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3521-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 164.Bortolato M, Coffey BJ, Gabbay V, Scheggi S. Allopregnanolone: The missing link to explain the effects of stress on tic exacerbation? J Neuroendocrinol. 2022;34(2):1–14. doi: 10.1111/jne.13022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 165.Ferando I, Mody I. Interneuronal GABAA receptors inside and outside of synapses. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2014;26:57–63. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.12.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 166.Lee V, Maguire J. Impact of inhibitory constraint of interneurons on neuronal excitability. J Neurophysiol. 2013;110(11):2520–35. doi: 10.1152/jn.00047.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 167.Barth C, Villringer A, Sacher J. Sex hormones affect neurotransmitters and shape the adult female brain during hormonal transition periods. Front Neurosci. 2015;9:1–20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 168.Shin SY, Han TH, Lee SY, Han SK, Park JB, Erdelyi F, et al. Direct corticosteroid modulation of GABAergic neurons in the anterior hypothalamic area of GAD65-eGFP mice. Korean J Physiol Pharmacol. 2011;15(3):163–9. doi: 10.4196/kjpp.2011.15.3.163 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 169.Serrano-Pedraza I, Romero-Ferreiro V, Read JCA, Diéguez-Risco T, Bagney A, Caballero-González M, et al. Reduced visual surround suppression in schizophrenia shown by measuring contrast detection thresholds. Front Psychol. 2014;5:1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 170.Angelucci A, Bressloff PC. Contribution of feedforward, lateral and feedback connections to the classical receptive field center and extra-classical receptive field surround of primate V1 neurons. Prog Brain Res. 2006;154:93–120. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)54005-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 171.Nurminen L, Merlin S, Bijanzadeh M, Federer F, Angelucci A. Top-down feedback controls spatial summation and response amplitude in primate visual cortex. Nat Commun. 2018;9(1). doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-04500-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 172.Rubin DB, Van Hooser SD, Miller KD. The stabilized supralinear network: A unifying circuit motif underlying multi-input integration in sensory cortex. Neuron. 2015;85(2):402–17. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.12.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 173.Vangeneugden J, van Beest EH, Cohen MX, Lorteije JAM, Mukherjee S, Kirchberger L, et al. Activity in Lateral Visual Areas Contributes to Surround Suppression in Awake Mouse V1. Curr Biol. 2019;29(24):4268–75. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.10.037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 174.Schach S, Surges R, Helmstaedter C. Visual surround suppression in people with epilepsy correlates with attentional-executive functioning, but not with epilepsy or seizure types. Epilepsy Behav. 2021;121:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2021.108080 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 175.Melnick MD, Harrison BR, Park S, Bennetto L, Tadin D. A strong interactive link between sensory discriminations and intelligence. Curr Biol. 2013;23(11):1013–7. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.04.053 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 176.Mulleners WM, Chronicle EP, Palmer JE, Koehler PJ, Vredeveld JW. Visual cortex excitability in migraine with and without aura. Headache. 2001;41(6):565–72. doi: 10.1046/j.1526-4610.2001.041006565.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 177.Wickmann F, Stephani C, Czesnik D, Klinker F, Timäus C, Chaieb L, et al. Prophylactic treatment in menstrual migraine: A proof-of-concept study. J Neurol Sci. 2015;354(1–2):103–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2015.05.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 178.Battista J, Badcock DR, McKendrick AM. Center-surround visual motion processing in migraine. Investig Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2010;51(11):6070–6. doi: 10.1167/iovs.10-5290 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 179.Battista J, Badcock DR, McKendrick AM. Migraine increases centre-surround suppression for drifting visual stimuli. PLoS One. 2011;6(4). doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018211 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 180.Gingnell M, Ahlstedt V, Bannbers E, Wikström J, Sundström-Poromaa I, Fredrikson M. Social stimulation and corticolimbic reactivity in premenstrual dysphoric disorder: A preliminary study. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord. 2014;4(1):1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 181.Zhang S, Xu M, Kamigaki T, Do JPH, Chang WC, Jenvay S, et al. Long-range and local circuits for top-down modulation of visual cortex processing. Science. 2014;345(6197):660–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Thiago P Fernandes

28 Oct 2022

PONE-D-22-26872Altered visual cortex excitability in premenstrual dysphoric disorder: evidence from magnetoencephalographic gamma oscillations and perceptual suppressionPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Orekhova,

Thank you for submitting your study.

This is a very interesting and worthwhile proposal.

Also you will notice that reviewers enjoyed your study, but required some amendments.

Please respond to all comments AND highlight the tracked changes.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Thiago P. Fernandes, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1.  Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf  and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Partly

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The authors of this manuscript focus on a timely and interesting research question of how the menstrual-related neurosteroids influence excitation and inhibition in sensory cortex, in both the normal menstrual cycle and in the context of premenstrual dysphoric disorder (PMDD). They focus on the visual cortex, with behavioural measures including induced MEG gamma oscillations, and the behavioural measure of surround suppression.

The neural and behavioural metrics, and paradigms in which these are tested, are well-characterised, with considerable scientific underpinnings known. As such, they make excellent choices for addressing the authors' hypotheses. On that note, the hypotheses are clear and well-articulated.

An interesting pattern of results is seen, with the notable differences being:

PMDD is associated with:

Reduced luteal phase progesterone levels

Slower luteal phase reaction time

Increased gamma power facilitation from static to slow drifting gratings in both menstrual phases

Decreased gamma power suppression from slow to medium drifting gratings in the luteal phase only

Decreased perceptual surround suppression from follicular to luteal phase

Findings are interpreted as PMDD being associated with:

A luteal phase specific deficit in cortical inhibition

A phase-general increase in cortical excitation

Overall comments:

In short, I think this is an excellent scientific manuscript throughout. The research area, aims and hypotheses are interesting and novel. The overall paradigm and approach is well-chosen, and implemented with rigorous attention to methodological detail and elimination of potential confounds. MEG data analysis is soundly conducted, and in line with best practice.

The results are interesting, and likely to be of broad relevance across a large numbers of basic and clinical neuroscience. The interpretation given by the authors is well-argued and seems very plausible, and avoids excessive speculation.

I have only a few minor comments, and do not feel that further peer review is necessary to check the revisions arising from these:

Line 180: ‘liability’ should be ‘lability’

Line 196: ‘Besides’ should be ‘Additionally’

Line 539: ‘menstrual’ should be ‘menstrual cycle’

Line 673: it is stated that perceptual surround suppression was not affected in PMDD, but lines 620-627 in the Results section state and discuss that a significant difference was seen. Please clarify.

Reviewer #2: Manyukhina and colleagues present a study of visual functioning in women with premenstrual dysphoric disorder (PMDD) versus controls. They hypothesized that the balance of excitatory and inhibitory (E/I) functions would be altered in PMDD participants due to disrupted hormone sensitivity. They sought to test this hypothesis using two visual paradigms: 1) gamma-band responses (GR) to static and moving gratings measured with MEG, and 2) spatial suppression of motion discrimination (duration thresholds), measured in a behavioral task.

Overall, this study is very thorough. The authors paid careful attention to potential confounds (visit day, age) and provided good justification for the statistical tests that were used. The question of whether E/I imbalance disrupts visual perception in PMDD is an interesting one. However, there are a number of issues that limit my enthusiasm for the current manuscript.

Major points:

• Throughout the manuscript, the authors assert that their measure of spatial suppression is explained by inhibition in visual cortex, but this point appears somewhat controversial within the literature, and empirical support is equivocal. In contrast to the work that is cited, a number of studies in both humans and animal models have failed to find a strong link between visual center-surround suppression and inhibitory functioning (Liu et al., 2018; Ozeki et al., 2009; Ozeki et al., 2004; Read et al., 2015; Sato et al., 2016; Schach et al., 2021; Schallmo et al., 2018; Schallmo et al., 2020). Some of these have proposed alternative models, wherein withdrawal of recurrent excitation plays a primary role in the suppression phenomenon, with inhibition playing a secondary or supporting role. To some extent, this seems like an issue about the semantic distinction between ‘inhibition’ (i.e., GABAergic hyperpolarization) and ‘suppression’ (i.e., reduced perceptual functioning). Although the authors’ theory regarding the link between motion suppression and inhibition is interesting, it would be prudent to clarify that this is an area of active research, rather than a settled point in the literature.

• The issue of co-morbidity between PMDD and bipolar disorder or depression is important, as visual disruptions, including in center-surround tasks, have been observed in these conditions (Golomb et al., 2009; Miller et al., 2003; Ngo et al., 2011; Norton et al., 2016; Schallmo et al., 2015; Yang et al., 2013). This topic is touched on briefly in the limitations section, but is not fully addressed. How might the authors differentiate between specific effects of PMDD versus effects of co-morbid mood disorders?

• Regarding the GR suppression metric, I am concerned that expressing this value as a ratio distorts the pattern of results. It appears that the PMDD group has higher GR power in both the slow and medium conditions vs. controls, as supported by the significant group * velocity interaction. This overall difference could induce a spurious difference between these two groups when a ratio metric is used. Instead, a subtraction metric appears warranted. See (Curran-Everett, 2013). This comment applies equally to GR facilitation.

• According to the text in the Results, the authors calculate their suppression index in a way that is not standard in the field; the standard metric is the log of the difference in threshold values (Tadin, 2003; Foss-Feig, 2013), rather than the ratio. Using the current metric makes it difficult to compare the results of this study to the wider literature. However, the methods state that the standard difference metric was used. Please clarify.

• The manuscript is overly-long in general, but frustratingly, appears to leave out important details and justification in places (some examples are provided in the Minor points below). I believe the manuscript would benefit from greater use of supplemental materials. Some tables and analyses (e.g., sections 3.1 – 3.3) are less relevant to the main hypotheses than others and could be briefly summarized in the main text, with details in the supplemental text.

Minor points:

• Line 38 – please clarify that suppression refers to GR power, not perception.

• Lines 106 & 110 – please specify GR power rather than frequency.

• Please provide an explanation for why controls did not also complete the C-PASS.

• Which ethics body specifically reviewed the study, and how were participants compensated for their time?

• Why were 26 vertices used, and are these in the same position for all participants? Were they required to be contiguous?

• Why not include motion discrimination data from the 2.5 degree size condition? No justification is given, and these data might be expected to aid the interpretation of the pattern of results, as the pattern of spatial suppression across stimulus sizes is well established in healthy adults.

• Data points from individual participants are shown in Figures 2 and 3, which is appreciated, but this is not the case for later figures.

• It appears the correlations in Table 3 were not corrected for multiple comparisons. This should be done, or at least included in addition to uncorrected p-values, given the large numbers of tests reported (16).

• Line 641 – The authors say "In control women, the percent of GR suppression from the ‘slow’ to the ‘medium’ condition (Fig 6c) correlated with the SSI" but the correlation is only significant for the follicular phase, please clarify.

• Line 647 – Is there a significant group difference in the GR suppression and SSI correlations themselves?

References

Curran-Everett, D. (2013). Explorations in statistics: the analysis of ratios and normalized data. Advances in Physiology Education, 37, 213-219.

Golomb, J. D., McDavitt, J. R. B., Ruf, B. M., Chen, J. I., Saricicek, A., Maloney, K. H., Hu, J., Chun, M. M., & Bhagwagar, Z. (2009). Enhanced visual motion perception in major depressive disorder. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29(28), 9072-9077.

Liu, L., Miller, K. D., & Pack, C. C. (2018). A unifying motif for spatial and directional surround suppression. The Journal of Neuroscience, 38(4), 989-999.

Miller, S. M., Gynther, B. D., Heslop, K. R., Liu, G. B., Mitchell, P. B., Ngo, T. T., Pettigrew, J. D., & Geffen, L. B. (2003). Slow binocular rivalry in bipolar disorder. Psychological Medicine, 33(4), 683-692.

Ngo, T. T., Mitchell, P. B., Martin, N. G., & Miller, S. M. (2011). Psychiatric and genetic studies of binocular rivalry: an endophenotype for bipolar disorder? Acta Neuropsychiatrica, 23(1), 37-42.

Norton, D. J., McBain, R. K., Pizzagalli, D. A., Cronin-Golomb, A., & Chen, Y. (2016). Dysregulation of visual motion inhibition in major depression. Psychiatry Research, 240, 214-221.

Ozeki, H., Finn, I. M., Schaffer, E. S., Miller, K. D., & Ferster, D. (2009). Inhibitory stabilization of the cortical network underlies visual surround suppression. Neuron, 62, 578-592.

Ozeki, H., Sadakane, O., Akasaki, T., Naito, T., Shimegi, S., & Sato, H. (2004). Relationship between excitation and inhibition underlying size tuning and contextual response modulation in the cat primary visual cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 24(6), 1428-1438.

Read, J. C. A., Georgiou, R., Brash, C., Yazdani, P., Whittaker, R., Trevelyan, A. J., & Serrano-Pedraza, I. (2015). Moderate acute alcohol intoxication has minimal effect on surround suppression measured with a motion direction discrimination task. Journal of Vision, 15, 5-5.

Sato, T. K., Haider, B., Haüsser, M., & Carandini, M. (2016). An excitatory basis for divisive normalization in visual cortex. Nature neuroscience, 1-3.

Schach, S., Surges, R., & Helmstaedter, C. (2021). Visual surround suppression in people with epilepsy correlates with attentional-executive functioning, but not with epilepsy or seizure types. Epilepsy & Behavior, 121, 108080.

Schallmo, M.-P., Kale, A. M., Millin, R., Flevaris, A. V., Brkanac, Z., Edden, R. A. E., Bernier, R. A., & Murray, S. O. (2018). Suppression and facilitation of human neural responses. eLife, 7, e30334.

Schallmo, M.-P., Kolodny, T., Kale, A. M., Millin, R., Flevaris, A. V., Edden, R. A. E., Gerdts, J., Bernier, R. A., & Murray, S. O. (2020). Weaker neural suppression in autism. Nature communications, 11(1), 2675.

Schallmo, M.-P., Sponheim, S. R., & Olman, C. A. (2015). Reduced contextual effects on visual contrast perception in schizophrenia and bipolar affective disorder. Psychological Medicine, 45(16), 3527-3537.

Yang, E., Tadin, D., Glasser, D. M., Hong, S. W., Blake, R., & Park, S. (2013). Visual context processing in bipolar disorder: A comparison with schizophrenia. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(569), 1-12.

Reviewer #3: The authors investigate the difference in the E-I balance between post-menstrual dysphoric disorder (PMDD) subjects and healthy controls using the changes in gamma-band power to the drifting annular gratings of varying velocity and perceptual suppression task. The way gamma power and frequency vary with drift rate has been proposed to be an indicator of E/I balance. Therefore, the authors hypothesize that during the symptomatically intensive luteal phase, the differences in E-I balance is prominent and should be reflected in the gamma frequency, suppression of gamma power with drift rate and performance of perceptual spatial suppression task. While the authors did not find the expected results, they did find some differences between PMDD and control subjects for some drift rates.

First, I think irrespective of the results, the study has been done very well. The study design is thorough, statistics are sound, literature survey is thorough and excellent. It is also clear that the authors have worked very hard to write a strong Discussion section (probably based on comments in previous submissions) to address a variety of questions that could have come up while reading the paper.

My only major comment is to perhaps tone down the Abstract a little bit and explicitly state that the main effect that they were looking for is not present (i.e., it is a null result as far as the main prediction is concerned). This does not take anything away from the work. The reasons provided in the Discussion are thorough and could lead to more refined studies in future. Overall, I endorse the publication of this manuscript.

Minor:

1) Line 75-77: There is an error in qualifying the concentration of GABAA receptors in visual cortex. Authors state “Visual cortex displays the highest concentration of GABAA and GABAB receptors among other brain areas in humans” but the figure 7 and figure 8 of the cited reference [45] show the highest concentration of GABAA in the auditory cortex.

2) Figure 1: Does MEG task panel in the figure depict a single trial? If so, it doesn’t match the description of the task in the methods. In the method (line 225), it is described as “Each new trial started with a fixed 1.2 s prestimulus interval…” but in the figure the trial seems to start with the stimulus followed by a prestimulus period. Also, in the figure it seems as if two stimuli are shown in a trial but it’s not the case according to the methods. An arrow indicating the direction of time can be helpful here.

3) The psychophysical task is not very clear from the figure or from the methods. Little more details on the task, especially the structure of a trial in both methods and figure will be helpful. I was unable to find reference [98] online, which is cited for the details of the task. From the figure it seems as if stimuli of different sizes are presented in a trial and response is sought only for the one presented at the end? Is that the case?

Reference [98]:

Manyukhina VO, Rostovtseva EN, Prokofyev AO, Stroganova TA, Orekhova E V. Menstrual cycle-linked changes in neural oscillations measured by MEG may provide biomarker for premenstrual dysphoric disorder. 2020;(2010):2020.

4) Line 618: There seems to be a mistake in the SSI formula. The ratio of the threshold should have been large/small.

5) Line 645: Three is misspelled as tree.

6) Line 818-820: Authors argue that GR facilitation in PMDD is unlikely to depend on MC phase related fluctuations in neurosteroids and the effect of neurosteroids on GABAA mediated inhibition because it enhanced in both the phases. However, this alone is not sufficient to rule out the effect of neurosteroid on GABAA mediated tonic inhibition in case there is a tolerance to ALLO in the luteal phase as the authors hypothesize in section 4.2.1. The evidence for unlikely dependence of facilitation on the effects of neurosteroid on GABAA receptor comes from the control group where the facilitation is maintained despite the absence of the tolerance to ALLO.

7) Table 3: Correlations were significant for control women but not for PMDD subjects. Any reason why?

8) Line 256: Is it ‘Humming’ or Hamming window?

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: William Sedley

Reviewer #2: No

Reviewer #3: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Decision Letter 1

Thiago P Fernandes

19 Dec 2022

Altered visual cortex excitability in premenstrual dysphoric disorder: evidence from magnetoencephalographic gamma oscillations and perceptual suppression

PONE-D-22-26872R1

Dear Dr. Orekhova,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Thiago P. Fernandes, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Please check your data disposition acorrding to the PLOS policies, also check your references and a few issues on grammar when in typesetting. Thank you for your valuable subsmission. Hope you can consider PLOS again.

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: From my perspective, and my points raised, I am satisfied that this manuscript is ready for publication.

That said, my comments were already minor and limited, and the bigger question is whether the other two reviewers are satisfied with the responses to their comments and questions. At a glance, these seem to have been satisfactorily addressed, but the other two reviewers will be in a better position to judge this than I am.

Reviewer #2: The authors have made an admirable effort to address the concerns raised by myself and the other reviewers. I thank them, and I have no further comments.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: William Sedley

Reviewer #2: No

**********

Acceptance letter

Thiago P Fernandes

22 Dec 2022

PONE-D-22-26872R1

Altered visual cortex excitability in premenstrual dysphoric disorder: evidence from magnetoencephalographic gamma oscillations and perceptual suppression

Dear Dr. Orekhova:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Thiago P. Fernandes

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Fig

    Violin plots for gamma response (GR) power (upper panel) and GR frequency (lower panel). Note that the group differences were not significant for all of these GR parameters (t-test, all p’s>0.12; uncorrected for multiple comparisons).

    (DOCX)

    S2 Fig. Violin plots of gamma response (GR) suppression and GR facilitation scores.

    (DOCX)

    S1 File

    (DOCX)

    S2 File. Results of bipolar disorder and depression questionnaires in women with PMDD.

    (DOCX)

    S3 File. Gamma suppression slope and perceptual spatial suppression.

    (DOCX)

    S1 Table. Partial Spearman’s correlations between steroid hormone levels (estradiol, progesterone) and gamma response (GR) power and frequency adjusted for age in the two groups of participants (control, PMDD).

    Correlations with p<0.05 (uncorrected for multiple comparisons) are highlighted in bold.

    (DOCX)

    S1 Data. The data used for statistical analysis.

    (XLSX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: PONErev_responses_rev1.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are available in the Supporting Information files.


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES