Abstract
Background
Childhood cognitive development depends on neuroimmune interactions. Immunomodulation by early-life microbial exposure may influence neuropsychological function. In this study, we investigate the association between residential indoor microbiota and cognition and behavior among preschoolers.
Results
Indoor-settled dust bacterial and fungal characteristics were assessed using 16S and ITS amplicon sequencing (microbial diversity) and qPCR measurements (microbial loads). Child behavior was assessed using four scales: peer relationship, emotional, conduct, and hyperactivity was assessed by the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ). Cognitive function was assessed using four tasks of the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) software. The first two tasks were designed to assess attention and psychomotor speed (Motor Screening (MOT) and Big/Little Circle (BLC)) and the last two to evaluate the child’s visual recognition/working memory (Spatial Span (SSP) and Delayed Matching to Sample (DMS)). Among the 172 included children (age 4–6 years), we observed a 51% (95%CI;75%;9%) lower odds of children scoring not normal for hyperactivity and a decrease of 3.20% (95%CI, −6.01%; −0.30%) in BLC response time, for every IQR increase in fungal Shannon diversity. Contrarily, microbial loads were directly associated with SDQ scales and response time. For example, a 2-fold increase in Gram-positive bacterial load was associated with 70% (95%CI 18%; 156%) higher odds of scoring not normal for hyperactivity and an increase of 5.17% (95%CI 0.87%; 9.65%) in DMS response time.
Conclusions
Our findings show that early-life exposure to diverse indoor fungal communities is associated with better behavioral and cognitive outcomes, whereas higher indoor microbial load was associated with worse outcomes.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1186/s40168-022-01406-9.
Keywords: Microbiota, Indoor, Diversity, Childhood, Cognition, Behavior
Background
During childhood, environmental factors can readily induce adaptive changes with permanent health effects in later life [1, 2]. Cognition-related outcomes are particularly interesting during childhood, because neural connections are developing and maturing, making them highly receptive to external factors [3, 4]. Considering that children spend most of their time indoors and particularly within their own home environment [5], it is important to investigate the early-life exposure to components present in the child’s home, including the indoor microbiota represented in house dust. There is a good number of studies providing consistent evidence for a connection between indoor dust microbiota and other health outcomes, in particular regarding the development of asthma [6–10]. Immunomodulation by microbiota in the course of early childhood exposure as part of the immune development is proposed as a potential pathway explaining these associations [11–14]. Interestingly, in addition to being a critical period for the maturation of the immune system, childhood is also crucial to neural development and neuroimmune interactions are thought to play a regulating role in synaptic plasticity [15]. This close connection is, for example, apparent by the fact that attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms oftentimes coincide with allergies or asthma symptoms [16–18], and increased inflammation has been linked to poorer cognitive performance [19–21]. Consequently, it is important to explore further the association between indoor microbiota and childhood neuropsychological function.
To date, only few studies [22–25] have investigated the potential role of early-life exposure to indoor microbial communities and their indoor determinants in cognitive function and behavior. Overall, the results of these studies consistently showed that the presence of mold and dampness at home was associated with poorer cognitive function and behavioral problems during childhood [22, 23, 25]. Another study showed that occasional farm animal contact was associated with better cognitive function at the age of 4 years. The authors considered exposure to microbial agents as one potential explanation for this finding [23]. So far, only one study has included actual measures of indoor microbial diversity. In this study [24], it was found that early-life indoor fungal diversity exposure was associated with higher odds of hyperactivity/inattention for 10-year old children but lower odds at the age of 15 years. In the present study, we complement the results of existing studies by investigating the associations of behavioral and cognitive function with indoor microbial exposure, assessed qualitatively and quantitatively, in young children aged 4–6 years, participating in the ENVIRONAGE birth cohort.
Methods
Study design
ENVIRonmental influence ON early AGEing (ENVIRONAGE) is a Belgian birth cohort that started in 2010, with ongoing recruitment for mother-newborn pairs at birth in the East-Limburg Hospital (Genk, Belgium). Complete information on the eligibility and recruitment process is presented elsewhere [26]. A follow-up examination is conducted when the child is 4–6 years, where parents fill out questionnaires to provide lifestyle and socio-demographic information, and the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) to assess the child’s behavior. In addition, during this visit, the child performs cognitive testing using the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) [27]. The study protocol was approved by the ethical committee of Hasselt University and complied with the Helsinki Declaration. Parents gave written informed consent and children verbal permission [26, 28]. This study followed the Strengthening Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) reporting guideline.
In 2017 and 2018, a subset of the ENVIRONAGE participants were asked to participate in home visits. More specifically, we selected households that already participated in the follow-up examination or had the examination planned close to the home visits, did not move in between, and had no indoor renovations planned. In total, 233 of the 284 eligible households were contacted, of which 189 accepted to participate, resulting in a participation rate of 81% (Supplemental Fig. 1). Due to logistical constraints, eight samples were not collected. Additionally, we excluded two samples because of sampling irregularities, two samples with less than 1000 sequences due to insufficient dust and one sample because sampling period exceeded the predetermined maximum of 9 weeks. Of the 176 children with appropriate dust samples, 171 and 172 children completed the SDQ and CANTAB, respectively.
Indoor microbial assessment during home visit
Settled dust was collected using two sterile, open-faced Petri dishes (92x16mm) over a period of minimum four and maximum nine weeks (mean 43.4 days), in spring 2017 and spring 2018, in the household’s living room. They were placed approximately 2 m above floor level, a safe distance from major air flows and heating sources [29]. Upon collection, the Petri dishes were sealed and stored at −20°C to be processed in the summer of 2018, as described previously [30]. After processing, samples were shipped frozen on dry ice to the Finnish Institute for Health and Welfare (Kuopio, Finland), where DNA extraction was conducted as described earlier [30], storing the DNA at −20°C until sequencing.
Extracted DNA from dust and control samples was shipped frozen to the sequencing service partner LGC Genomics (Germany) for library preparation and sequencing. For bacteria, the V4 region of the bacterial 16S rRNA gene was amplified using 515F/806R primers [31]. For fungi, the Internal Transcribed Spacer region 1 (ITS1) was amplified using ITS1F/ITS2 primers [32].
The PCR procedure, sequencing, sequence processing, and bioinformatics analyses are described more in-depth in supplement. In brief, 16S and ITS amplicon data was processed by standard dada2 pipeline version 1.8 [30]. Taxonomy was assigned using SILVA [33] database version 132 for bacteria and UNITE database version 7.2 for fungi [34]. Downstream processing included removal of chimeras, chloroplast, and mitochondria sequences, as well as of potential contaminants utilizing negative controls and Decontam package version 1.2 [35].
QIIME software version 1.9.1 [36] was used to calculate Chao1 richness and Shannon diversity index using rarefaction values of 1495 and 3956 sequences for bacteria and fungi, respectively. The Chao1 index is an abundance-based estimator of species richness. The Shannon diversity index incorporates species evenness, i.e., the homogeneity of species abundance, with species richness.
We used quantitative PCR to calculate the total Gram-negative and Gram-positive bacterial and fungal loads, as described previously [30]. We determined the number of microbial cell equivalents (CE) in the samples using relative quantification, utilizing an internal standard to adjust for the presence of DNA inhibitors and/or variability in DNA extraction efficiency [37]. Results were normalized for sampling surface area and accumulation duration and expressed as CE per m2 settling surface area per day, referred to hereafter as microbial load.
Neuropsychological assessment during follow-up visit
Behavioral outcomes
To assess the child’s behavior, parents filled out the SDQ [38], a validated screening method for psychiatric disorders in children [39–41]. From this questionnaire, four scales (range 0–10) are calculated from five statements each: peer relationship, emotional, conduct, and hyperactivity and were additionally combined to calculate a Total Difficulties Score (range 0–40). We used British cutoff guidelines and categorized SDQ outcomes into discrete variables by grouping borderline and abnormal scores together to define a “not normal” category. Being not normal was defined when scores were equal or above 3 for peer relationship, 4 for emotional, 3 for conduct, 6 for hyperactivity, and 14 for the Total Difficulties Score [42].
Cognitive function outcomes
Cognitive function was assessed via CANTAB [27] software on a touchscreen tablet, reliable for measuring executive functions in young children [43]. In total, each child had to complete four tasks (Fig. 1). Two tasks assessed the attention and psychomotor speed (Motor Screening Task (MOT) and Big/Little Circle (BLC)), and two assessed the visual working memory (Spatial Span (SSP) and Delayed Matching to Sample (DMS)). Detailed information on cognitive assessment is provided in supplement.
The MOT outcome variables included the response time and error. The response time is the average time in milliseconds to select the cross successfully. The error is the average distance in pixel units between the child’s press and the cross’s center on all successful trials. Low values in response time and in error indicate better performance. The BLC outcome variable was response time, being, in this test, the median time in milliseconds to select the right circle successfully, low values indicate better performance. The SSP measured the maximum sequence length the child could correctly recall, further referred to as span length, higher considered better. In the fourth and last test, i.e., DMS, the first outcome variable was the average time, in milliseconds, it took to correctly answer on the first try, further referred to as response time; lower values considered better. We excluded response times calculated on less than 25% of the trials. The last outcome variables were the probability of error if the previous trial was correct, and the total proportion of correct answers on first try, further referred to as percentage correct, both expressed in percentages. A lower probability of error and higher percentage correct are considered better.
Covariables
During the follow-up examination, questionnaires were used to collected lifestyle and clinical information. Maternal education was used to represent socioeconomic status, coded as “low” (no diploma or primary school), “middle” (high school), or “high” (college or university degree). In addition, we obtained information on average daily screen time, defined as watching television, playing computer games and tablet use, categorized as “<1 h per day,” “1–2 h per day,” and “>2 h per day.” The time of examination was used as a continuous variable in the main analyses and categorized in the sensitivity analysis into morning (before 12pm), early afternoon (from 12pm up to 4pm), and late afternoon (after 4pm). Additionally, we obtained information on parental smoking coded dichotomously as non-smoking parents versus one or both parents smoking.
The residential addresses of the households were geocoded and categorized into rural, and suburban or urban, based on population density, employment, location, and spatial planning of statistical sectors (Flemish Government-Department Environment). In addition, we calculated average black carbon (BC) exposure by averaging daily BC concentrations at the residential address over the sampling period, using a spatiotemporal interpolation method [44] as described elsewhere [30]. Additionally, average outdoor temperature (°C), provided by the Belgian Royal Meteorological Institute, was calculated as the daily mean temperatures measured at a representative measuring station (Diepenbeek, Belgium) averaged over the sampling period.
Upon sample collection, we obtained additional household information: number of household members, pet ownership, ventilation type, and sampling duration. Pet ownership was dichotomized into the presence of a furry pet (cat, dog, rabbit, hamster, or guinea pig) or not and ventilation type into the use of passive ventilation or not.
Statistical analysis
For the statistical processing, we used the R environment version 3.6.0 [45]. We screened for outliers using a threshold of more or less than three times the standard deviation away from the mean. We removed two outliers for the bacterial Shannon diversity index. For the Gram-negative, Gram-positive, and fungal load, we removed two, five, and three outliers, respectively, and log-transformed values (base 10) to better comply with linear model assumptions. We identified certain core variables to be included in all of our models, including child’s age, sex, maternal education, urbanicity, and sampling duration, which reflect important clinical, socioeconomical, and technical information regarding the microbial and cognitive assessment.
To examine the associations between microbial exposure and the child’s behavior, we used logistic regression models for the SDQ outcomes, adjusting for the aforementioned core variables (child’s age, sex, maternal education, urbanicity, and sampling duration), as well as the number of household members, which was identified as an additional potential confounder for this analysis. Results regarding microbial diversity are expressed as odds ratios (OR) per interquartile range (IQR) increase in microbial diversity indices or for a 2-fold increase in microbial load.
To investigate microbial exposure in association with cognitive CANTAB outcomes, we log-transformed (base 10) all response times to better comply with assumptions on model linearity. Furthermore, we performed multivariable linear regression models adjusting for the aforementioned core variables (child’s age, sex, maternal education, urbanicity, and sampling duration), as well as the time of examination, reflecting relevant technical information regarding the CANTAB outcomes. Results are expressed as a unit change or a percentage change for the log-transformed response time outcomes (with 95% CI), per IQR increment in microbial diversity or per 2-fold increase in microbial load.
To assess the robustness of our findings, we additionally adjusted for potential confounders: smoking, determinants for indoor microbiota (average outdoor temperature, BC exposure, pet ownership, and ventilation), screen time (as a proxy for prior screen familiarization), and number of household members. We performed two sensitivity analyses for CANTAB models, excluding children who showed any signs of possible disinterest during cognitive testing, based on behavioral remarks and irregular touch patterns (n=1, 4, 7, and 5 for MOT, BLC, SSPM and DMS, respectively). Lastly, because a child’s performance may depend on tiredness, we restricted our analysis to children that performed the cognitive tests before 4pm.
Results
Study population
Descriptive statistics on follow-up examination, household, and child characteristics are presented in Table 1. The majority of follow-up examinations were performed in the afternoon (56.2%), and half of the children were boys and were on average 4.5 years old (± 0.3) and two thirds of the mothers reporting having high education.
Table 1.
Characteristics of the follow-up examination | |
Time of examination | |
Morning (before 12 pm) | 68 (39.3) |
Afternoon (from 12 to 4 pm) | 97 (56.1) |
Late afternoon (after 4 pm) | 8 (4.6) |
Season of examination | |
Spring | 47 (27.2) |
Summer | 44 (25.4) |
Autumn | 43 (24.9) |
Winter | 39 (22.5) |
Characteristics of the child | |
Age at follow-up, years | 4.5 ± 0.3 |
Sex, boy | 87 (50.3) |
Time spent watching television/playing games | |
None to less than 1 hour/day | 57 (32.9) |
1-2 hours/day | 96 (55.5) |
>2 hours/day | 14 (8.1) |
Missing information | 6 |
Characteristics of the mother | |
Age of the mother at follow-up visit, years | 35.1 ± 3.8 |
Educational level | |
Low (no high school diploma) | 5 (2.9) |
Middle (high school diploma) | 38 (22.0) |
High (college degree or higher) | 130 (75.1) |
Characteristics of the household | |
Smoking | |
Non-smoker parents | 128 (74.0) |
One or both parents smoked | 36 (20.8) |
Missing information | 9 |
Number of household members | 4 ± 0.8 |
Presence of furry pets, yes | 82 (47.1) |
Passive ventilation, yes | 140 (80.5) |
Characteristics of the dust sampling | |
Age of child, during sampling | 4.8 ± 0.6 |
Duration of sampling (days) | 43.4 ± 5.6 |
Sampling average outdoor temperature (°C) | 16.7 ± 1.9 |
Sampling average ambient airborne BC concentration (μg/m3) | 0.7 ± 0.2 |
Continuous variables presented as mean ± sd and categorical variables as n (%)
The SDQ and CANTAB outcomes are summarized in Table 2. The largest group of children scoring not normal occurred within the emotional and conduct scales, where approximately a quarter of the children scored not normal, followed by the hyperactivity scale (17.5%). In addition, the response times of the CANTAB tests assessing the attention and psychomotor speed (i.e., MOT and BLC) correlated the strongest (Supplemental Fig. 2).
Table 2.
n (%) | min | P25 | P50 | P75 | max | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
SDQ outcomes | ||||||
Peer relationship scale | ||||||
Normal | 143 (83.6) | |||||
Not normal | 28 (16.4) | |||||
Emotional scale | ||||||
Normal | 126 (73.7) | |||||
Not normal | 45 (26.3) | |||||
Conduct scale | ||||||
Normal | 127 (74.3) | |||||
Not normal | 44 (25.7) | |||||
Hyperactivity scale | ||||||
Normal | 141 (82.5) | |||||
Not normal | 30 (17.5) | |||||
Total difficulties score | ||||||
Normal | 143 (83.6) | |||||
Not normal | 28 (16.4) | |||||
Cantab outcomes | ||||||
Attention and psychomotor speed | ||||||
MOT | ||||||
Response time, ms | 170 | 659.3 | 817.8 | 980.4 | 1161.1 | 2961.8 |
Error, pixel units | 170 | 8.2 | 12.0 | 13.89 | 15.99 | 21.5 |
BLC | ||||||
Response time, ms | 172 | 726 | 898.6 | 985.0 | 1083.6 | 1467.0 |
Visual and working memory | ||||||
SSP | ||||||
Span Length, number of boxes | 172 | 0 | 2 | 3 | 3 | 5 |
DMS | ||||||
Response time on first try, ms | 143 | 1340.0 | 3303.9 | 4169.9 | 5368.7 | 13994.7 |
Error given correct answer, % | 169 | 13.3 | 41.7 | 54.6 | 66.7 | 100.0 |
Percentage correct, % | 169 | 10.0 | 35.0 | 45.0 | 55.0 | 85.0 |
Microbial measurements | ||||||
Microbial diversity | ||||||
Bacteria | ||||||
Chao1 richness | 173 | 111 | 294 | 406 | 501 | 728 |
Shannon diversity | 171 | 4.93 | 6.57 | 7.11 | 7.61 | 8.57 |
Fungi | ||||||
Chao1 richness | 172 | 24 | 92 | 131 | 174 | 300 |
Shannon diversity | 172 | 0.66 | 2.71 | 3.40 | 4.09 | 5.57 |
Microbial load | ||||||
Gram-negative bacterial load | 171 | 9683 | 150635 | 289297 | 486076 | 2786787 |
Gram-positive bacterial load | 168 | 437 | 84846 | 157486 | 273577 | 1010599 |
Fungal load | 170 | 155 | 20332 | 33724 | 62577 | 211855 |
An overview of the microbial variables is also provided in Table 2. Overall, indoor dust bacterial diversity and load were found to be higher than fungal measures. The microbial diversity indices were overall positively and strongly correlated with each other. In contrast, microbial load was overall negatively correlated with the corresponding microbial diversity indices (Supplemental Fig. 3).
Associations between behavior and indoor microbiota
After adjustment, we generally observed tendencies for inverse associations between the SDQ outcomes and microbial diversity indices (Table 3). These associations were stronger for hyperactivity and the Total Difficulties Score and only statistically significant for the fungal Shannon diversity index. More specifically, an IQR increase in the fungal Shannon diversity was associated with a 51% (95%CI 75%; 9%) and a 57% (95%CI 80%; 15%) lower odds of a not normal score for hyperactivity and Total Difficulties Score, respectively. In contrast, microbial loads were directly and significantly associated with hyperactivity and the Total Difficulties Score. A 2-fold increase in bacterial (Gram-negative and Gram-positive) and fungal loads was associated with a significant increase in the odds of scoring not normal for hyperactivity. In addition, a 2-fold increase in Gram-negative and Gram-positive bacterial load was associated with a 55% (95%CI 7%; 130%) and a 46% (95%CI 1%; 120%) increased odds of children scoring not normal for total difficulties.
Table 3.
Diversity indicesb | Microbial loadc | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bacteria | Fungi | Gram-negative load | Gram-positive load | Fungal load | |||
Chao1 | Shannon | Chao1 | Shannon | ||||
Peer relationship scale | 0.86 [0.44;1.64] | 0.91 [0.52;1.63] | 1.16 [0.60;2.21] | 1.03 [0.53;2.00] | 1.32 [0.93;1.92] | 1.39 [0.97;2.05] | 1.11 [0.80;1.59] |
Emotional scale | 0.81 [0.47;1.39] | 1.06 [0.65;1.74] | 1.13 [0.65;1.94] | 0.82 [0.47;1.41] | 0.95 [0.71;1.27] | 0.97 [0.75;1.29] | 0.88 [0.67;1.15] |
Conduct scale | 0.80 [0.47;1.35] | 0.71 [0.44;1.15] | 0.68 [0.40;1.15] | 0.68 [0.40;1.14] | 1.10 [0.84;1.45] | 0.96 [0.74;1.24] | 1.19 [0.92;1.59] |
Hyperactivity scale | 0.66 [0.35;1.22] | 0.82 [0.47;1.44] | 0.61 [0.31;1.14] | 0.49 [0.25;0.91] | 1.54 [1.09;2.22] | 1.70 [1.18;2.56] | 1.67 [1.15;2.53] |
Total difficulties Score | 0.61 [0.31;1.16] | 0.70 [0.40;1.24] | 0.72 [0.35;1.39] | 0.43 [0.20;0.85] | 1.55 [1.07;2.30] | 1.46 [1.01;2.20] | 1.34 [0.94;1.98] |
aAssociations adjusted for child’s sex and age, maternal education, sampling duration, number of household members, and urbanicity. Estimates presented in bold are statistically significant p<0.05
bAssociations with microbial diversity indices are expressed for IQR increments
cAssociations with microbial load are expressed for a 2-fold increment
Associations between cognitive outcomes and indoor microbiota
After adjustment, we found for an IQR increase in indoor fungal Shannon diversity a decrease of 3.2% (95%CI, −6.0%; −0.3%) in the BLC response time, assessing attention and psychomotor speed. In contrast, we found for a 2-fold increase in Gram-negative bacterial, Gram-positive bacterial, and fungal loads, an increase of 5.74% (95%CI 1.04; 10.67), 5.17% (95%CI 0.87; 9.65), and 5.70% (95%CI 1.44; 10.14), respectively, in the DMS response time, assessing visual recognition memory. Other association estimates were not statistically significant. The statistically significant results are presented in Fig. 2, and the exact values of the association estimates are provided in Supplemental Table 1.
Sensitivity analyses
Additionally, adjusting for smoking, screen time, number of household members, or determinants for indoor microbiota did not importantly change the observed associations (data not shown). In addition, excluding children who showed signs of possible disinterest during CANTAB testing (Supplemental Table 2), and children that performed these tests after 4pm did not result in important changes to the observed associations.
Discussion
In this study, we used qualitative and quantitative measures to characterize early-life indoor dust microbial exposure and evaluated associations with child behavior and cognitive function in four to 6-year-old children. One key finding was that higher indoor fungal diversity was associated with a reduction in hyperactive behavioral traits and the Total Difficulties Score. In contrast, exposure to higher quantities, i.e., loads of microbiota indoors were generally associated with worse outcomes in these behavioral assessments. For cognitive function, we obtained similar results. Here, higher fungal diversity was associated with shorter (better) BLC response time, whereas higher indoor microbial loads were associated with longer (worse) DMS response times.
To date, only few studies have investigated associations between behavioral and cognitive outcomes and indoor microbiota measures, with the reported results being inconsistent [22–25]. For example, in a German cohort [24], heterogenous associations were described between the indoor microbial diversity and the development of hyperactive problems during childhood. The authors reported that early-life indoor fungal diversity exposure was associated with higher odds of hyperactivity/inattention for 10-year old children but lower odds at the age of 15 years. Interestingly, similar differences in associations with fungal exposures depending on age have been observed for atopic outcomes, where fungal diversity seems to have a protective effect in early childhood, whereas the effects are attenuated in later life [46]. In addition to improved behavioral outcomes, we found indoor fungal diversity to be associated with improved response time for BLC, related to the neurological domain of attention and psychomotor speed. As such, a reduction in response time corresponds with improved attention, making this association compatible with the observed reduction in hyperactivity/inattention problems of the SDQ [47].
In contrast, indoor microbial quantity was found to be associated with an increased likelihood of a child having hyperactivity problems and Total Difficulties, in addition to an observed increase in the response time for DMS. Though different from the neurological domain of attention and psychomotor speed, the visual recognition memory, which in turn corresponds strongly with visual working memory [48–50], is innately attention-driven and thus an increase in response time is compatible with decreased attention [51]. Our results suggests that, similar to immune-related health outcomes, early-life exposure to a higher fungal diversity is associated with improved cognitive outcomes, whereas exposure to higher microbial quantities is associated with decreased cognitive function [52–54]. In our study, we mainly see an association between fungal but not bacterial diversity and behavioral and cognitive outcomes, whereas both fungal and bacterial loads are associated with worse outcomes. The positive correlations between the bacterial and fungal load measures, and the negative correlations between richness/diversity and the load measures in our study might have contributed to these observations and complicate disentangling effects of the individual microbial measurements.
Our results support the hypothesis that early-life microbial exposure is associated with the development of behavioral problems and cognitive functioning. The hypothesized mechanism for this relationship is through the close connection between cognitive and immune development [16–18]. Consequently, indoor microbiota might influence our behavioral and cognitive functioning through modulation of the immune system in early life [55]. Indeed, exposure to environmental microbiota, in particular exposure to inhalable microbial agents as present in settled dust, is known to be involved in the development of our immune system [13, 14]. Previous research has, for instance, established the link between diversity and specific taxa compositions of the indoor microbial environment and the development of asthma [6–10]. Besides being explicitly involved in regulating our immune system, environmental microbial diversity might play a role in shaping the human microbiome, such as the gut microbiota, which can utilize our immune system and produce neurotransmitters influencing brain regulation [13, 56, 57]. The human gut microbiota are similarly connected to our cognitive development through immunomodulation, resulting in an analogous beneficial influence of microbial diversity on cognitive performance, including improved response times and visuospatial working memory [58–61]. However, it is important to note that without detailed immunological information can only speculate that a well-developed immune system that follows from a highly diverse microbial environment and the environmental contribution to the gut microbiota are driving the associations.
We acknowledge some study limitations. First, cognitive testing in young children can be slightly more difficult to interpret; however, our results were robust to sensitivity analyses accounting for potential age-related problems, including trouble understanding the task or having a potential lack of motivation [43]. Moreover, all cognitive tests were performed in a standardized manner with trained researchers, limiting inter-observer bias and improving reliable data collection. Second, we only used the parent-reported SDQ to assess child behavior. This has, however, proven to be a reliable and validated questionnaire demonstrated to be associated with academic performance in later life [39–41, 62]. In addition, we cannot exclude the possibility of residual confounding. It is possible that certain unaccounted lifestyle characteristics or environmental exposures could modify the indoor microbial exposure, and influence the behavioral and cognitive outcomes. We did, however, perform multiple analyses adjusting for a variety of potential confounders and found only limited changes to the observed associations, supporting the robustness of our findings. Next, we did not investigate the taxa abundances or the microbial profiles that might help explain the underlying processes; however, the observed associations regarding microbial diversity and load are relevant for public health and consistent with studies focusing on immunology. While we acknowledge that the microbial and cognitive assessments were done not at the exact same time, the time difference between those assessments was in all limited to a very large degree and analysis adjusting for this time difference did not change our results (data not shown). While an ideal home microbial exposure assessment would have included multiple sampling points throughout early childhood, we implemented long-term (four to nine weeks) integrated sampling of settling, airborne dust for qualitative and quantitative measurements of indoor microbiota, which avoids issues related to the known short-term variability of indoor air microbial exposures and which is considered to be more representative of indoor microbial exposure over time [63, 64]. Nevertheless, we cannot exclude the possibility that the indoor microbial environment changes over time and that the measured microbial exposure is not fully reflective of early-life indoor microbial exposure. Apart from these restrictions, we provide novel evidence that the indoor microbiota might influence behavioral and cognitive function in young children aged 4–6 years in a large number of households (n=172), whilst adjusting for potential confounders and performing sensitivity analyses. We used a comprehensive quantitative and qualitative microbial approach combined with complementary detailed behavioral and cognitive outcomes. Given that our study did not collect sufficient immunological information, we cannot further speculate on the predominant mechanisms. More research is needed to investigate the potential mechanism by which the indoor air-associated microbiota are associated with childhood cognitive function.
Conclusion
Our findings provide evidence that the early-life microbial environment may play a role in behavior and cognitive function. More specifically, our results suggest that high indoor fungal diversity may be beneficial to behavioral outcomes, and particularly to hyperactive behavior, as well as improved attention and psychomotor speed. In contrast, exposure to high airborne microbial loads could potentially result in worse behavioral and cognitive outcomes. These findings are comparable to the known relationship between early-life exposure to a highly diverse environment and reduced allergies, supporting immunomodulation as a potential mechanism that connects microbiota and cognition. Nevertheless, we need further research to better understand the potential mechanisms involved in the associations observed in this study.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
Not applicable.
Authors’ contributions
YD was involved in writing the original draft. YD, LL, MP, LR, HS, KW, and MV were involved in the investigation of the study. YD performed the formal analysis. YD, MT, and LC were responsible for the methodology and conceptualization, and TN and LC were responsible for the supervision. All of the authors were involved in writing-review and editing of the manuscript. The author(s) read and approved the final manuscript.
Funding
ENVIRONAGE birth cohort was supported by the EU research council (ERC-2012-StG310898), Research Foundation Flanders (FWO) (G073315N/G048420N), and Methusalem. Indoor dust measurements were funded by FWO (grant number 151809N). Lidia Casas is a recipient of a post-doctoral fellowship a of the Research Foundation Flanders (FWO), grant number 12I1517N. The work at the Finnish Institute for Health and Welfare was supported by the Academy of Finland grant 296817. The authors acknowledge funding from the Special Research Fund (BOF) from Hasselt University granted to LR (BOF20DOC15).
Availability of data and materials
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
Declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
The study protocol was approved by the ethical committee of Hasselt University and complied with the Helsinki Declaration. Parents gave written informed consent and children verbal permissio. (reference number B9115201836553).
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Yinthe Dockx, Email: yinthe.dockx@uhasselt.be.
Martin Täubel, Email: martin.taubel@thl.fi.
Janneke Hogervorst, Email: janneke.hogervorst@uhasselt.be.
Leen Luyten, Email: leen.luyten@uhasselt.be.
Martien Peusens, Email: martien.peusens@uhasselt.be.
Leen Rasking, Email: leen.rasking@uhasselt.be.
Hanne Sleurs, Email: hanne.sleurs@uhasselt.be.
Katrien Witters, Email: katrien.witters@uhasselt.be.
Michelle Plusquin, Email: michelle.plusquin@uhasselt.be.
Maria Valkonen, Email: maria.valkonen@thl.fi.
Tim S. Nawrot, Email: tim.nawrot@uhasselt.be
Lidia Casas, Email: lidia.Casas@uantwerpen.be.
References
- 1.Barker DJ. Developmental origins of adult health and disease. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2004;58:114–115. doi: 10.1136/jech.58.2.114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Barker D, Thornburg K. Placental programming of chronic diseases, cancer and lifespan: a review. Placenta. 2013;34(10):841–845. doi: 10.1016/j.placenta.2013.07.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Rice D, Barone S., Jr Critical periods of vulnerability for the developing nervous system: evidence from humans and animal models. Environ Health Perspect. 2000;108(Suppl 3):511–533. doi: 10.1289/ehp.00108s3511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.World Health Organization . Principles for evaluating health risks in children associated with exposure to chemicals. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Klepeis NE, Nelson WC, Ott WR, Robinson JP, Tsang AM, Switzer P, Behar JV, Hern SC, Engelmann WH. The National Human Activity Pattern Survey (NHAPS): a resource for assessing exposure to environmental pollutants. J Expo Anal Environ Epidemiol. 2001;11:231–252. doi: 10.1038/sj.jea.7500165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Bonamichi-Santos R, Aun MV, Agondi RC, Kalil J, Giavina-Bianchi P. Microbiome and asthma: what have experimental models already taught us? J Immunol Res. 2015;2015:614758. doi: 10.1155/2015/614758. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Ege MJ, Mayer M, Normand A-C, Genuneit J, Cookson WO, Braun-Fahrländer C, Heederik D, Piarroux R, von Mutius E. Exposure to environmental microorganisms and childhood asthma. N Engl J Med. 2011;364:701–709. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1007302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Karvonen AM, Kirjavainen PV, Täubel M, Jayaprakash B, Adams RI, Sordillo JE, Gold DR, Hyvärinen A, Remes S, von Mutius E, Pekkanen J. Indoor bacterial microbiota and development of asthma by 10.5 years of age. J Allergy Clin Immunol. 2019;144:1402–1410. doi: 10.1016/j.jaci.2019.07.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.von Mutius E. The microbial environment and its influence on asthma prevention in early life. J Allergy Clin Immunol. 2016;137:680–689. doi: 10.1016/j.jaci.2015.12.1301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kirjavainen PV, Karvonen AM, Adams RI, Täubel M, Roponen M, Tuoresmäki P, Loss G, Jayaprakash B, Depner M, Ege MJ, et al. Farm-like indoor microbiota in non-farm homes protects children from asthma development. Nat Med. 2019;25:1089–1095. doi: 10.1038/s41591-019-0469-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Belkaid Y, Hand TW. Role of the microbiota in immunity and inflammation. Cell. 2014;157:121–141. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.03.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Fung TC. The microbiota-immune axis as a central mediator of gut-brain communication. Neurobiol Dis. 2020;136:104714. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2019.104714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Rook GA. Regulation of the immune system by biodiversity from the natural environment: an ecosystem service essential to health. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110:18360–18367. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1313731110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.von Hertzen L, Hanski I, Haahtela T. Natural immunity. Biodiversity loss and inflammatory diseases are two global megatrends that might be related. EMBO Rep. 2011;12:1089–1093. doi: 10.1038/embor.2011.195. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Dantzer R. Neuroimmune interactions: from the brain to the immune system and vice versa. Physiol Rev. 2018;98:477–504. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00039.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Biederman J, Milberger S, Faraone SV, Guite J, Warburton R. Associations between childhood asthma and ADHD: issues of psychiatric comorbidity and familiality. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1994;33:842–848. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199407000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Brawley A, Silverman B, Kearney S, Guanzon D, Owens M, Bennett H, Schneider A. Allergic rhinitis in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Ann Allergy Asthma Immunol. 2004;92:663–667. doi: 10.1016/S1081-1206(10)61434-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Wang L-J, Yu Y-H, Fu M-L, Yeh W-T, Hsu J-L, Yang Y-H, Chen WJ, Chiang B-L, Pan W-H. Attention deficit–hyperactivity disorder is associated with allergic symptoms and low levels of hemoglobin and serotonin. Sci Rep. 2018;8:1–7. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-28702-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Kyriklaki A, Margetaki K, Kampouri M, Koutra K, Bitsios P, Chalkiadaki G, Dermitzaki E, Venihaki M, Sarri K, Anousaki D. Association between high levels of inflammatory markers and cognitive outcomes at 4 years of age: the Rhea mother-child cohort study, Crete, Greece. Cytokine. 2019;117:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.cyto.2019.01.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Lee SE, West KP, Cole RN, Schulze KJ, Wu LS-F, Yager JD, Groopman J, Christian P. General intelligence is associated with subclinical inflammation in Nepalese children: a population-based plasma proteomics study. Brain Behav Immun. 2016;56:253–263. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2016.03.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Jonker I, Klein HC, Duivis HE, Yolken RH, Rosmalen JG, Schoevers RA. Association between exposure to HSV1 and cognitive functioning in a general population of adolescents. The TRAILS study. PLoS One. 2014;9:e101549. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0101549. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Casas L, Tiesler C, Thiering E, Brüske I, Koletzko S, Bauer CP, Wichmann HE, von Berg A, Berdel D, Krämer U, et al. Indoor factors and behavioural problems in children: the GINIplus and LISAplus birth cohort studies. Int J Hyg Environ Health. 2013;216:146–154. doi: 10.1016/j.ijheh.2012.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Casas L, Torrent M, Zock JP, Doekes G, Forns J, Guxens M, Täubel M, Heinrich J, Sunyer J. Early life exposures to home dampness, pet ownership and farm animal contact and neuropsychological development in 4 year old children: a prospective birth cohort study. Int J Hyg Environ Health. 2013;216:690–697. doi: 10.1016/j.ijheh.2012.12.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Casas L, Karvonen AM, Kirjavainen PV, Täubel M, Hyytiäinen H, Jayaprakash B, Lehmann I, Standl M, Pekkanen J, Heinrich J. Early life home microbiome and hyperactivity/inattention in school-age children. Sci Rep. 2019;9:17355. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-53527-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Jedrychowski W, Maugeri U, Perera F, Stigter L, Jankowski J, Butscher M, Mroz E, Flak E, Skarupa A, Sowa A. Cognitive function of 6-year old children exposed to mold-contaminated homes in early postnatal period. Prospective birth cohort study in Poland. Physiol Behav. 2011;104:989–995. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.06.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Janssen BG, Madhloum N, Gyselaers W, Bijnens E, Clemente DB, Cox B, Hogervorst J, Luyten L, Martens DS, Peusens M, et al. Cohort profile: the ENVIRonmental influence ON early AGEing (ENVIRONAGE): a birth cohort study. Int J Epidemiol. 2017;46:1386–1387m. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyx033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Dockx Y, Bijnens EM, Luyten L, Peusens M, Provost E, Rasking L, Sleurs H, Hogervorst J, Plusquin M, Casas L, Nawrot TS. Early life exposure to residential green space impacts cognitive functioning in children aged 4 to 6 years. Environ Int. 2022;161:107094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 28.Madhloum N, Luyten LJ, Provost EB, De Boever P, Dockx Y, Sleurs H, Plusquin M, Roodt JO, Vrijens K, Nawrot TS. Establishing reference values for macro- and microvascular measurements in 4-to-5 year-old children of the ENVIRONAGE prospective birth cohort. Sci Rep. 2020;10:5107. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-61987-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Hyvarinen A, Roponen M, Tiittanen P, Laitinen S, Nevalainen A, Pekkanen J. Dust sampling methods for endotoxin - an essential, but underestimated issue. Indoor Air. 2006;16:20–27. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0668.2005.00392.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Dockx Y, Täubel M, Bijnens EM, Witters K, Valkonen M, Jayaprakash B, Hogervorst J, Nawrot TS, Casas L. Residential green space can shape the indoor microbial environment. Environ Res. 2021;201:111543. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2021.111543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Caporaso JG, Lauber CL, Walters WA, Berg-Lyons D, Lozupone CA, Turnbaugh PJ, Fierer N, Knight R. Global patterns of 16S rRNA diversity at a depth of millions of sequences per sample. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(Suppl 1):4516–4522. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1000080107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Smith DP, Peay KG. Sequence depth, not PCR replication, improves ecological inference from next generation DNA sequencing. PLoS One. 2014;9:e90234. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0090234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Quast C, Pruesse E, Yilmaz P, Gerken J, Schweer T, Yarza P, Peplies J, Glockner FO. The SILVA ribosomal RNA gene database project: improved data processing and web-based tools. Nucleic Acids Res. 2013;41:D590–D596. doi: 10.1093/nar/gks1219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Nilsson RH, Larsson K-H, Taylor AFS, Bengtsson-Palme J, Jeppesen TS, Schigel D, Kennedy P, Picard K, Glöckner FO, Tedersoo L, et al. The UNITE database for molecular identification of fungi: handling dark taxa and parallel taxonomic classifications. Nucleic Acids Res. 2018;47:D259–D264. doi: 10.1093/nar/gky1022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Davis NM, Proctor DM, Holmes SP, Relman DA, Callahan BJ. Simple statistical identification and removal of contaminant sequences in marker-gene and metagenomics data. Microbiome. 2018;6:226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 36.Caporaso JG, Kuczynski J, Stombaugh J, Bittinger K, Bushman FD, Costello EK, Fierer N, Peña AG, Goodrich JK, Gordon JI, et al. QIIME allows analysis of high-throughput community sequencing data. Nat Methods. 2010;7:335–336. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.f.303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Haugland RA, Varma M, Wymer LJ, Vesper SJ. Quantitative PCR analysis of selected Aspergillus, Penicillium and Paecilomyces species. Syst Appl Microbiol. 2004;27:198–210. doi: 10.1078/072320204322881826. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Goodman R. Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40:1337–1345. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200111000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Goodman R, Ford T, Simmons H, Gatward R, Meltzer H. Using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) to screen for child psychiatric disorders in a community sample. Br J Psychiatry. 2000;177:534–539. doi: 10.1192/bjp.177.6.534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Goodman R, Renfrew D, Mullick M. Predicting type of psychiatric disorder from Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) scores in child mental health clinics in London and Dhaka. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;9:129–134. doi: 10.1007/s007870050008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Van Leeuwen K, Meerschaert T, Bosmans G, De Medts L, Braet C. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire in a community sample of young children in Flanders. Eur J Psychol Assess. 2006;22:189–197. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.22.3.189. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Meltzer H, Gatward R, Goodman R, Ford T. Mental health of children and adolescents in Great Britain (Reprinted from 2000) Int Rev Psychiatry (Abingdon, England) 2003;15:185–187. doi: 10.1080/0954026021000046155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Luciana M. Practitioner review: computerized assessment of neuropsychological function in children: clinical and research applications of the Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Battery (CANTAB) J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2003;44:649–663. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Janssen S, Dumont G, Fierens F, Mensink C. Spatial interpolation of air pollution measurements using CORINE land cover data. Atmos Environ. 2008;42:4884–4903. doi: 10.1016/j.atmosenv.2008.02.043. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.R Development Core Team . R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2019. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Tischer C, Weikl F, Probst AJ, Standl M, Heinrich J, Pritsch K. Urban dust microbiome: impact on later atopy and wheezing. Environ Health Perspect. 2016;124:1919–1923. doi: 10.1289/EHP158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Hall CL, Guo B, Valentine AZ, Groom MJ, Daley D, Sayal K, Hollis C. The validity of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) for children with ADHD symptoms. PLoS One. 2019;14:e0218518. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0218518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Mitsven SG, Cantrell LM, Luck SJ, Oakes LM. Visual short-term memory guides infants’ visual attention. Cognition. 2018;177:189–197. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.04.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Kibbe MM. Varieties of visual working memory representation in infancy and beyond. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2015;24:433–439. doi: 10.1177/0963721415605831. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Oakes L, Baumgartner H, Barrett F, Messenger I, Luck S. Developmental changes in visual short-term memory in infancy: evidence from eye-tracking. Front Psychol. 2013;4:697. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00697. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Dadvand P, Tischer C, Estarlich M, Llop S, Dalmau-Bueno A, López-Vicente M, Valentín A, de Keijzer C, Fernández-Somoano A, Lertxundi N, et al. Lifelong residential exposure to green space and attention: a population-based prospective study. Environ Health Perspect. 2017;125:097016. doi: 10.1289/EHP694. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Heederik D, von Mutius E. Does diversity of environmental microbial exposure matter for the occurrence of allergy and asthma? J Allergy Clin Immunol. 2012;130:44–50. doi: 10.1016/j.jaci.2012.01.067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Kanchongkittiphon W, Mendell MJ, Gaffin JM, Wang G, Phipatanakul W. Indoor environmental exposures and exacerbation of asthma: an update to the 2000 review by the Institute of Medicine. Environ Health Perspect. 2015;123:6–20. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1307922. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Zhang Z, Reponen T, Hershey GKK. Fungal exposure and asthma: IgE and non-IgE-mediated mechanisms. Curr Allergy Asthma Rep. 2016;16:86. doi: 10.1007/s11882-016-0667-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Chida Y, Hamer M, Steptoe A. A bidirectional relationship between psychosocial factors and atopic disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychosom Med. 2008;70:102–116. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31815c1b71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Galland L. The gut microbiome and the brain. J Med Food. 2014;17:1261–1272. doi: 10.1089/jmf.2014.7000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Strandwitz P. Neurotransmitter modulation by the gut microbiota. Brain Res. 2018;1693:128–133. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2018.03.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Liddicoat C, Sydnor H, Cando-Dumancela C, Dresken R, Liu J, Gellie NJC, Mills JG, Young JM, Weyrich LS, Hutchinson MR, et al. Naturally-diverse airborne environmental microbial exposures modulate the gut microbiome and may provide anxiolytic benefits in mice. Sci Total Environ. 2020;701:134684. doi: 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2019.134684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Konya T, Koster B, Maughan H, Escobar M, Azad MB, Guttman DS, Sears MR, Becker AB, Brook JR, Takaro TK, et al. Associations between bacterial communities of house dust and infant gut. Environ Res. 2014;131:25–30. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2014.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Fujimura KE, Demoor T, Rauch M, Faruqi AA, Jang S, Johnson CC, Boushey HA, Zoratti E, Ownby D, Lukacs NW, Lynch SV. House dust exposure mediates gut microbiome Lactobacillus enrichment and airway immune defense against allergens and virus infection. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014;111:805. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1310750111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Canipe LG, Sioda M, Cheatham CL. Diversity of the gut-microbiome related to cognitive behavioral outcomes in healthy older adults. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2021;96:104464. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2021.104464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Keilow M, Sievertsen HH, Niclasen J, Obel C. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire and standardized academic tests: reliability across respondent type and age. PLoS One. 2019;14:e0220193. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0220193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Adams RI, Tian Y, Taylor JW, Bruns TD, Hyvärinen A, Täubel M. Passive dust collectors for assessing airborne microbial material. Microbiome. 2015;3:46. doi: 10.1186/s40168-015-0112-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Clark N, Ammann H, Brunekreef B, Eggleston P, Fisk W, Fullilove R, Guernsey J, Nevalainen A, Von Essen S. Damp indoor spaces and health. Washington, DC: Institute of Medicine of the National Academies; 2004. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.