Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2023 Jan 5;18(1):e0279564. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0279564

Relationship of mental health and burnout with empathy among medical students in Thailand: A multicenter cross-sectional study

Jarurin Pitanupong 1, Katti Sathaporn 1,*, Pichai Ittasakul 2, Nuntaporn Karawekpanyawong 3
Editor: Omnia Samir El Seifi4
PMCID: PMC9815634  PMID: 36602955

Abstract

Objectives

To explore mental health, burnout, and the factors associated with the level of empathy among Thai medical students.

Background

Empathy is an important component of a satisfactory physician-patient relationship. However, distress, including burnout and lack of personal well-being, are recognized to affect a lower level of empathy.

Material and methods

A cross-sectional study surveyed sixth-year medical students at three faculties of medicine in Thailand at the end of the 2020 academic year. The questionnaires utilized were: 1) Personal and demographic information questionnaire, 2) Thai Mental Health Indicator-15, 3) The Maslach Burnout Inventory-Thai version, and 4) The Toronto Empathy Questionnaire. All data were analyzed using descriptive statistics, and factors associated with empathy level were analyzed via the Chi-square test or Fisher’s exact test, logistic regression., and linear regression.

Results

There were 336 respondents with a response rate of 70.3%. The majority were female (61.9%). Most participants reported a below-average level of empathy (61%) with a median score (IQR) of 43 (39–40). Assessment of emotion comprehension in others and altruism had the highest median empathy subgroup scores, whereas behaviors engaging higher-order empathic responses had the lowest median empathy subgroup score. One-third of participants (32.1%) had poor mental health, and two-thirds (62.8%) reported a high level of emotional exhaustion even though most of them perceived having a high level of personal accomplishment (97%). The multivariate analysis indicated that mental health was statistically significantly associated with the level of empathy. The participants with higher levels of depersonalization had statistically lower scores of demonstrating appropriate sensitivity, altruism, and behaviors engaging higher-order empathic responding.

Conclusions

Most medical students had below-average empathy levels, and two-thirds of them had high emotional exhaustion levels, yet most of them reported having a high level of personal accomplishment and good mental health. There was an association between mental health and the level of empathy. Higher levels of depersonalization related to lower scores of demonstrating sensitivity, altruism, and behaviors responding. Therefore, medical educators should pay close attention to promoting good mental health among medical students.

Introduction

Empathy is the capacity to feel or understand what another person is experiencing from within their frame of reference [1]. It is the capacity to place oneself in another’s position [2] that facilitates the understanding of the emotions of another person [3, 4]. Nowadays, empathy has been defined as comprising three components: emotional contagion, emotional disconnection, and cognitive empathy [5]. Moreover, empathy is an emotional experience between an observer and a subject in which the observer, based on visual and auditory cues, identifies and transiently experiences the subject’s emotional state [6]. To be perceived as empathic, the observer must convey this understanding to the subject. During the initial phase of the process, the observer must not only identify but also understand the basis of the subject’s feelings [7].

The importance of empathy should be emphasized throughout medical school because a successful treatment depends in no small measure on effective patient-physician interaction. The physician who understands the patient at a personal level stands a better chance of experiencing and conveying empathy as well as treating the patient effectively than the physician who does not have this level of understanding [8]. Even though empathy is a major component of a satisfactory physician-patient relationship, prior studies suggested that the physician’s empathy level might decline throughout clinical training [9]. Four main themes influencing the development and expression of empathy have been identified: subject of medical education curriculum, student’s character, patient’s profile (“easy” or “difficult” patient), and surrounding conditions [10].

The medical education course taken by the medical student, be it hands-on experience, role models, science and theory, [10] or clinical training can impact empathy negatively [11]. Since the decline in empathy occurs throughout medical school, it can be hypothesized that the training curriculum itself contributes to a decline in compassion among medical students as they go through medical school. Concerning the impact of compassion, student character, insecurities, professional distance [10, 12], mood, maturity, and personal level of empathy are also related factors [10]. Besides, surrounding conditions: time pressure, stress, work environment, and job dissatisfaction are important influences [10]. In Thailand, a prior study found that about one-fifth of medical students had ever thought about dropping out. The most prominent causes of dropout thoughts were studying too hard or disliking the learning environment [13]. Therefore, personal characteristics, learning styles [14], perception of being abused or mistreated [12, 15, 16], and a hidden curriculum of cynicism may contribute to these problems [1719]. Distress, which includes burnout [20, 21], depression, anxiety, and lack of personal well-being, has also been recognized to exert an important influence on practice habits [22, 23], performance, or lower levels of empathy among medical students [24, 25].

The Division of Medical Education, Ministry of Education, Thailand proposes the core competencies of medical graduates. According to them, empathy is one constituent of professional habits, communication, and interpersonal skills [26, 27]. However, limited research data concerning this issue exist. To our knowledge, only one study conducted in Thailand in 2012 has explored empathy among medical students. It illustrated that female and preclinical-level students had higher empathy scores than their male and clinical-level counterparts. Notwithstanding, that study did not identify any factors that correlate with empathy level [28]. Therefore, our study aimed to determine whether levels of empathy among medical students are associated with personal distress or burnout and to explore whether a degree of personal well-being or mental health is associated with levels of empathy.

Materials and methods

Respondents and procedure

After approval from the corresponding ethics committees of the Faculty of Medicine, Prince of Songkla University (REC: 63-515-3-1), Mahidol University (REC: MURA 2021/52), and Chiang Mai University (REC: 024/2564), this cross-sectional study was conducted on sixth-year medical students attending these three medical schools. The study population comprised all sixth-year medical students of the above-mentioned institutions; they were surveyed at the end of the 2020 academic year. There was a total of 478 sixth-year medical students: 154 from Prince of Songkla University, 174 from Mahidol University, and 150 from Chiang Mai University. To be included, one had to meet the criteria of being a medical student aged more than 20 years and completing all of the questionnaires. Meanwhile, those who were foreign students, declined to participate, or decided to withdraw from the study were excluded. The size of the study was estimated using 57.1% proportion of below-average empathy level from the previous study [25] with 6% margin of error. The sample size required at least 261 for this study.

The data were collected in the given classroom following relevant guidelines by using a paper-based or online process. One participant had the right to choose only one way to answer the questionnaire. Before entering the study, all participants were asked whether responded on paper or online questionnaire to prevent double responses from completing this study. Concerning the paper-based method, a research assistant handed them an information sheet, which delineated the rationale for the study and the allotted time to complete the survey. They had at least 10–15 minutes to consider whether to collaborate in the study or not. If they wished to participate, the research assistant handed them the questionnaires. Adhering to the policy of strict confidentiality, the signatures of the participants were not required, and all the participants retained the right to withdraw from the research at any time. In regards to the online process, all parts of the questionnaire were transformed from their paper form to an online questionnaire using Google Forms. The participants joined the study by either clicking the provided link or scanning a QR code through social media advertisements. Once again, in line with the policy of strict confidentiality, the signatures of the participants were not required, and the participants retained the right to withdraw from the research at any time without giving any reason. Besides, the data were stored in a secure place, and only the researcher could access the information via a password.

Questionnaire

  1. Personal and demographic information questionnaire consisting of questions related to age, gender, religion, hometown, income, cumulative GPA, medical school, history of substance use, physical or psychiatric illness, and specialty preference.

  2. Thai Mental Health Indicator-15 (TMHI-15) consisted of 15 questions. The score of each question ranged from 1 to 4; 1 (never); 2 (rarely); 3 (sometimes); and 4 (always), and the total score was between 15 and 60. The interpretation of the total score was as follows: less than 43 (poor mental health), 44–50 (fair mental health), and 51–60 (good mental health). This tool had a Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of 0.7 [29].

  3. Thai version of the Maslach Burnout Inventory (MBI) questionnaire [30, 31] consisted of 22 items divided into three dimensions: emotional exhaustion (feelings of being emotionally overextended and exhausted by one’s work), depersonalization (unsympathetic and impersonal responses toward the recipients of one’s care or service), and personal accomplishment (feelings of competence and achievement in one’s work with people) [30]. Emotional exhaustion (EE) subscale ranged from 0 (never) to 6 (every day), with Cronbach’s alpha coefficient = 0.9. Depersonalization (DP) subscale ranged from 0 (never) to 6 (every day), with Cronbach’s alpha coefficient = 0.7. Personal accomplishment (PA) subscale ranged from 0 (every day) to 6 (never), with Cronbach’s alpha coefficient = 0.7. For the emotional exhaustion and depersonalization subscales, higher mean scores corresponded to higher degrees of burnout (emotional exhaustion score: 0–16 = low, 17–26 = moderate, >26 = high; depersonalization score: 0–6 = low, 7–12 = moderate, > 12 = high). Lower mean scores of personal accomplishment correspond to higher degrees of burnout (personal accomplishment score: > 38 = low, 32–38 = moderate, 0–31 = high). The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of each domain in the Thai version of MBI is between 0.65–0.92 [3032].

  4. The Toronto Empathy Questionnaire (TEQ) consisted of 16 questions and employed a 5-point rating scale for each question. Item responses were scored according to the following scale for positively worded items; 0 (never); 1 (rarely); 2 (sometimes); 3 (often); and 4 (always). The same scale was applied to reverse score negatively worded items. The scores of all 16 questions were summed, and they ranged from 0 to 64. Higher scores indicated higher levels of self-reported empathy, while total scores below 45 were indicative of below-average empathy levels. The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for this tool was 0.85. Besides, empathy was divided into six subgroups; perception of an emotional state in another that stimulates the same emotion in oneself; assessment of emotion comprehension in others; assessment of emotional states in others by indexing the frequency of behaviors demonstrating appropriate sensitivity; sympathetic physiological arousal; altruism; behaviors engaging higher-order empathic responding, such as pro-social helping behavior [33].

Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics such as proportions, means, standard deviation (SD), median and interquartile range (IQR) were calculated. The Chi-square test (or Fisher’s exact test), logistic regression, and linear regression analyses were used to identify associations between demographic characteristics, mental health, and burnout with the level of empathy. The analyses were conducted using R version 3.4.1 (R Foundation for Statistical Computing). Statistical significance was defined as a p-value of less than 0.05.

Results

Demographic characteristics

The sixth-year medical students who completed the questionnaire were 336 of the 478 total students that were approached; the response rate was 70.3%. Of them, 154 (45.8%) studied at Prince of Songkla University, 93 (27.7%) studied at Chiang Mai University, and 89 (26.5%) studied at Mahidol University. Demographic characteristics were shown in Table 1. Most of them were female (61.9%) and Buddhist (83.9%). Overall, their mean age was 23.5 ±1.5 years, the accumulative GPA was 3.3 ± 0.4, and the income level was 10,000 baht per month. No statistically significant difference in demographic data (gender, religion, GPA) was observed between the students according to the medical school they attended. Additionally, there was no statistically significant difference in demographic data (gender, religion) between participants and non-participants.

Table 1. Demographic characteristics, mental health, burnout, and empathy level (N = 336).

Variables Number (%)
Gender
 Male 128 (38.1)
 Female 208 (61.9)
Religion
 Buddhism 282 (83.9)
 Others (Islam, Christ, others) 39 (11.6)
 No answer 15 (4.5)
Physical illness
 No 269 (80.1)
 Yes 67 (19.9)
Psychiatric illness
 No 302 (89.9)
 Yes 33 (9.8)
 No answer 1 (0.3)
Alcohol drinking
 No 229 (68.2)
 Yes 107 (31.8)
Substance use
 No 328 (97.6)
 Yes 8 (2.4)
Specialty preference
 General / not specified 68 (20.2)
 Major 184 (54.8)
 Minor 84 (25.0)
Mental health
 Poor 108 (32.1)
 Fair 158 (47.0)
 Good 70 (20.8)
Burnout
Emotional exhaustion
 Low 50 (14.9)
 Moderate 74 (22.0)
 High 211 (62.8)
 No answer 1 (0.3)
Depersonalization
 Low 85 (25.4)
 Moderate 83 (24.7)
 High 167 (49.7)
 No answer 1 (0.3)
Personal accomplishment
 Low -
 Moderate 9 (2.7)
 High 326 (97.0)
 No answer 1 (0.3)
Empathy level
Below (<45) 205 (61.0)
Above (>45) 131 (39.0)

Mental health

According to the Thai Mental Health Indicator-15 (TMHI-15), 158 (47%) participants had fair mental health. Meanwhile, 108 (32.1%) respondent had poor mental health (Table 1).

Burnout

The Maslach Burnout Inventory (MBI)-Thai version findings indicated that 211 (62.8%) participants had high emotional exhaustion, and another 167 (49.7%) had high depersonalization scores. Only 85 (25.4%) participants had low depersonalization scores. The median MBI score (IQR) for emotional exhaustion was 32 (21–41), and the median score (IQR) for depersonalization was 12 (6–19). However, no one reported a low level of personal accomplishment; almost all of the respondents (97%) perceived a high level of personal accomplishment (Table 1 and Fig 1). The median score (IQR) for personal accomplishment was 13 (9–18).

Fig 1. Burnout rate according to The Maslach Burnout Inventory (MBI)-Thai version.

Fig 1

Empathy

The Toronto Empathy Questionnaire results revealed that 205 (61%) participants reported a below-average empathy level (Table 1). The total median TEQ score (IQR) was 43 (39–48). According to the six TEQ subgroups, the result showed that the assessment of emotion comprehension in others and altruism had the highest median TEQ subgroup scores (IQR) [3 (2–3) and 3 (2.6–3.3), respectively], whereas behaviors engaging higher-order empathic responding exhibited the lowest median TEQ subgroup score (IQR) [2 (2–3)] (Table 2). No statistically significant difference in the level of empathy between students from different medical schools was detected (p = 0.355).

Table 2. Subgroups of empathy according to the Toronto Empathy Questionnaire.

Domain of empathy Median (IQR)
Perception of an emotional state in others 2.5 (2–3)
Assessment of emotion comprehension in others 3 (2–3)
Demonstrating appropriate sensitivity 2.7 (2.4–3)
Sympathetic physiological arousal 2.8 (2.3–3)
Altruism 3 (2.6–3.3)
Behaviors engaging higher-order empathic responding 2 (2–3)

The association of demographic characteristics, mental health, burnout, and level of empathy

To identify factors associated with the level of empathy, demographic characteristics, specialty preference, mental health, and burnout were included in the bivariate analysis. Variables with p-values of less than 0.2 from the bivariate analysis were included in the initial model of the multivariate analysis (Table 3). The results showed no relationship between burnout and the level of empathy. There was only mental health remained statistically significantly associated with the level of empathy (Table 4).

Table 3. Bivariate analysis.

Variables Level of empathy Number (%) P-value
<45 ≥45
(N = 205) (N = 131)
Gender 0.213
 Male 84 (41.0) 44 (33.6)
 Female 121 (59.0) 87 (66.4)
Religion 1
 Buddhism 170 (87.6) 112 (88.2)
 Others (Islam, Christ, others) 24 (12.4) 15 (11.8)
Physical illness 0.506
 No 167 (81.5) 102 (77.9)
 Yes 38 (18.5) 29 (22.1)
Psychiatric illness 1
 No 184 (90.2) 118 (90.1)
 Yes 20 (9.8) 13 (9.9)
Alcohol drinking 0.77
 No 138 (67.3) 91 (69.5)
 Yes 67 (32.7) 40 (30.5)
Substance use 1a
 No 200 (97.6) 128 (97.7)
 Yes 5 (2.4) 3 (2.3)
Specialty preference 0.047
 General/not specified 49 (23.9) 19 (14.5)
 Major 112 (54.6) 72 (55.0)
 Minor 44 (21.5) 40 (30.5)
Mental health <0.001
 Poor 90 (43.9) 18 (13.7)
 Fair 97 (47.3) 61 (46.6)
 Good 18 (8.8) 52 (39.7)
Burnout
Emotional exhaustion 0.007
 Low 25 (12.3) 25 (19.1)
 Moderate 37 (18.1) 37 (28.2)
 High 142 (69.6) 69 (52.7)
Depersonalization <0.001
 Low 40 (19.6) 45 (34.4)
 Moderate 44 (21.6) 39 (29.8)
 High 120 (58.8) 47 (35.9)
Personal accomplishment 0.096a
 Low - -
 Moderate 8 (3.9) 1 (0.8)
 High 196 (96.1) 130 (99.2)

a Fisher’s exact test

Table 4. The association between mental health, burnout and empathy level (N = 335).

Factors Crude OR (95%CI) Adjusted OR (95%CI) P-value
LR-test
Mental health <0.001
Poor Reference Reference
Fair 0.32 (0.17,0.58) 0.35 (0.19,0.65)
Good 0.07 (0.03,0.14) 0.07 (0.03,0.16)
Burnout
Emotional exhaustion
 Low Reference Reference 0.522
 Moderate 1 (0.49,2.05) 0.79 (0.34,1.8)
 High 2.06 (1.1,3.84) 0.62 (0.27,1.44)
Depersonalization 0.088
 Low Reference Reference
 Moderate 1.23 (0.69,2.33) 0.99 (0.49,1.98)
 High 2.87 (1.67,4.95) 1.92 (0.94,3.93)
Personal accomplishment 0.081
 High Reference Reference
 Moderate 5.31 (0.66,42.93) 5.54 (0.59,52.45)

However, three dimensions of burnout (EE, DP, PA) were evaluated to determine if they had an impact on any subgroups of empathy. Data were analyzed using linear regression for each of only two dimensions of burnout (EE and DP) because the majority of participants (97%) reported high PA. It was discovered that participants with higher levels of DP had statistically lower scores of demonstrating appropriate sensitivity, altruism, and behaviors engaging higher-order empathic responding (p<0.001, p<0.003, and p<0.014, respectively).

Discussion

This study indicated that most sixth-year medical students (61%) understudy had below-average empathy levels, indicated by a median TEQ score (IQR) of 43 (39–48). Regarding empathy subgroups, the assessment of emotion comprehension in others and altruism showed the highest median TEQ subgroup scores, whereas behaviors engaging in higher-order empathic responding exhibited the lowest median TEQ subgroup score. Also, the majority of the respondents reported fair to good mental health; only one-third of them (32.1%) had poor mental health. Moreover, no one perceived a low level of personal accomplishment. Interestingly, most sixth-year medical students perceived high levels of personal accomplishment (97%) even though they reported high levels of emotional exhaustion (62.8%). Besides, the participants with higher levels of depersonalization had statistically lower scores of demonstrating appropriate sensitivity, altruism, and behaviors engaging higher-order empathic responding. There was an association between the level of empathy and mental health. Comparing the level of empathy discovered in our study with those reported by prior research, it was similar to the one found by a study from Pakistan [34]. However, our level of empathy was lower than those found in Japanese [35], Turkey [36], and Austrian [37] studies. This variance might be due to differences in study instruments.

In terms of empathy subgroups, it is important to keep in mind that empathy encompasses cognitive and affective or emotional dimensions. The cognitive dimension refers to ‘the ability to understand the patient’s inner experience and perspective, and the capability to communicate this understanding’ [38], whereas the affective dimension refers to ‘the ability to imagine the patient’s emotions and perspectives [39]. In this study, the assessment of emotion comprehension in others and altruism showed the highest empathy subgroup scores, whereas behaviors engaging in higher-order empathic responding exhibited the lowest empathy subgroup score. This might imply that most sixth-year medical students were able to understand the patient’s inner experience and imagine their emotions or perspectives. However, they might lack the ability to express empathy toward others. It is, however, agreed that effective interaction or good communication skills on the part of physicians should enable them to convey their actual feelings or experiences to patients. Physicians who are poor communicators and inept at expressing their feelings properly might be misunderstood by patients and the people around them [40]. Moreover, as has been well-established, when patients perceive that the physician understands their conditions and apprehensions, they feel more comfortable with and more willing to confide in the physician. Finally, empathic expression is beneficial to physicians; it reflects that the physician can attune to the psychosocial needs of his/her patient [41].

A prior study identified four main themes that influence the development and expression of empathy: 1) subject or course of study: hands-on experience, role models, science, and theory; 2) students: insecurities and lack of routine, increasing professionalism, previous work experiences, professional distance, mood, maturity, and personal level of empathy; 3) patients: “easy” and “difficult” patients including their state of health; and 4) surrounding conditions: time pressure/stress, work environment, and job dissatisfaction. It suggested that the development and use of empathy could be promoted by increasing hands-on experiences, possibilities to experience the patient’s point of view, and offering patient contact early in the curriculum. Besides, medical students need support in reflecting on their actions, behavior, and experiences with patients. Instructors need time and opportunities to reflect on their communication with and treatment of patients, on their teaching behavior, and their function as role models regard to treating patients empathically and preventing stress [10]. In light of this, it is noteworthy that the current changes implemented in some medical school curriculums in Thailand seem to go in the right direction by integrating patient contact early on in the curriculum and focusing more on teaching adequate communication and interaction behaviors [42]. However, empathy and concern for other’s minds may be psychologically distinct and empathy may be limited by our moral lives [43]. Therefore, medical educators should pay close attention to it.

Concerning mental health and burnout, this study determined that lower levels of empathy among medical students were not associated with burnout and that a higher degree of personal well-being or good mental health was also associated with higher levels of empathy. An explanation for this could be the possibility that the majority of our medical students perceived they were in fair to good mental health and perceived a high level of personal accomplishment (97%) even though they reported high levels of emotional exhaustion (62.8%). This could be attributed to the fact that mental health includes having good self-esteem, satisfaction with life, security, confidence in emotional control, being empathetic and happy when helping others, and acknowledging or accepting problems that are difficult to solve [30].

In addition, a prior study on perceived empathy among medical students identified associating factors and sorted them into four clusters: personal experiences, connections, and beliefs; negative feelings and attitudes toward patients; mentoring and clinical experiences that promote professional growth; and school and work experiences that undermine the development of empathy. Hence, it could be said that medical students’ experiences that promote personal and professional growth might be the most important factors affecting empathy in medical education. On the other hand, negative feelings and attitudes toward patients, as well as negative school and work experiences, might have a detrimental impact on empathy at all stages of education [42]. Therefore, practical experiences should be made less stressful and strive to promote good mental health among medical students [10].

This study had a few strengths and limitations worth mentioning. To our knowledge, this is the first study that explored mental health and burnout as potential associating factors with the level of empathy among Thai medical students from multiple faculties of medicine. However, this study had some limitations. It was a cross-sectional survey and utilized self-administered questionnaires; therefore, some misunderstandings regarding the intended meaning of questions might have occurred. Nevertheless, to minimize this, questionnaires with good reliability were utilized (good Cronbach’s alpha coefficient values). Another drawback was that our data was quantitative, and the sample size was restricted to only medical students who graduated from three medical schools. Additionally, there might have a potential bias due to the greater number of female respondents and a large number of Buddhist respondents. Hence, this dataset may not represent fairly the situation of all Thai medical students in the whole country.

Henceforward, studies are recommended to include all medical students attending all the faculties of medicine in Thailand. In other words, a more comprehensive multi-center study should be conducted. Moreover, other studies should employ more qualitative methods and survey the medical student longitudinally.

Conclusion

Most medical students had below-average empathy levels, and two-thirds of them had high emotional exhaustion levels, yet most of them reported having a high level of personal accomplishment and good mental health. There was an association between mental health and the level of empathy. Higher levels of depersonalization related to lower scores of demonstrating sensitivity, altruism, and behaviors engaging empathic responding. Therefore, medical educators should pay close attention to promoting good mental health among medical students.

Supporting information

S1 Questionnaire. English version questionnaire.

(DOC)

S2 Questionnaire. Thai version questionnaire.

(DOC)

S1 Data. English version minimal dataset.

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

The authors appreciative the invaluable contribution of Ms. Kruewan Jongborwanwiwat and Mrs. Nisan Werachattawan related to the data analysis. In addition, we would like to show our gratitude to all the medical students who collaborated in this survey. Moreover, we genuinely appreciate the Faculty of Medicine, Prince of Songkla University, Thailand, and the Office of International Affairs of the Faculty of Medicine for proofreading the English.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

The authors received no specific funding for this work.

References

  • 1.Mehrabian A, Epstein N. A measure of emotional empathy. J Pers. 1972;40: 525–543. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1972.tb00078.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Hogan R. Development of an empathy scale. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1969;33: 307–316. doi: 10.1037/h0027580 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Cohen D, Strayer J. Empathy in conduct-disordered and comparison youth. Dev Psychol. 1996;32: 988–998. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Davis MH. Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1983;44: 113–126. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Carre A, Stefaniak N, D’Ambrosio F, Bensalah L, Besche-Richard C. The Basic Empathy Scale in adults (BES-A): factor structure of a revised form. Psychol Assess. 2013;25: 679–691. doi: 10.1037/a0032297 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Buie DH. Empathy: its nature and limitations. J Am Psychoanal Assoc. 1981;29: 281–307. doi: 10.1177/000306518102900201 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Elam CL. Use of "emotional intelligence" as one measure of medical school applicants’ noncognitive characteristics. Acad Med. 2000;75: 445–446. doi: 10.1097/00001888-200005000-00011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hirsch EM. The role of empathy in medicine: a medical student’s perspective. Virtual Mentor. 2007;9: 423–427. doi: 10.1001/virtualmentor.2007.9.6.medu1-0706 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Sathaporn K, Pitanupong J. The relationship between mental health with the level of empathy among medical students in Southern Thailand: a university-based cross-sectional study. Siriraj Med J. 2021;73: 832–840. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Pohontsch NJ, Stark A, Ehrhardt M, Kotter T, Scherer M. Influences on students’ empathy in medical education: an exploratory interview study with medical students in their third and last year. BMC Med Educ. 2018;18: 231. doi: 10.1186/s12909-018-1335-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Chen D, Lew R, Hershman W, Orlander J. A cross-sectional measurement of medical student empathy. J Gen Intern Med. 2007;22: 1434–1438. doi: 10.1007/s11606-007-0298-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Pitanupong J, Sathaporn K. The prevalence and factors associated with mistreatment perception among Thai medical students in a southern medical school. Siriraj Med J. 2019;71: 310–317. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Pitanupong J, Sangkool J, Wiwattanaworaset P, Pongthanawisut S, Teetharathul T, Jiraphan A. Dropout thought among medical students at Faculty of Medicine Prince of Songkla University. TMJ. 2020;20: 175–184. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.McManus IC, Keeling A, Paice E. Stress, burnout and doctors’ attitudes to work are determined by personality and learning style: a twelve-year longitudinal study of UK medical graduates. BMC Med. 2004;2: 29. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-2-29 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Silver HK, Glicken AD. Medical student abuse. Incidence, severity, and significance. JAMA. 1990;263: 527–532. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Elnicki DM, Curry RH, Fagan M, Friedman E, Jacobson E, Loftus T, et al. Medical students’ perspectives on and responses to abuse during the internal medicine clerkship. Teach Learn Med. 2002;14: 92–97. doi: 10.1207/S15328015TLM1402_05 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Hafferty FW, Franks R. The hidden curriculum, ethics teaching, and the structure of medical education. Acad Med. 1994;69: 861–871. doi: 10.1097/00001888-199411000-00001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Hafferty FW. Beyond curriculum reform: confronting medicine’s hidden curriculum. Acad Med. 1998;73: 403–407. doi: 10.1097/00001888-199804000-00013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Thomas MR, Dyrbye LN, Huntington JL, Lawson KL, Novotny PJ, Sloan JA, et al. How do distress and well-being relate to medical student empathy? A multicenter study. J Gen Intern Med. 2007;22: 177–183. doi: 10.1007/s11606-006-0039-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Pitanupong J, Jatchavala C. A study on the comparison of burnout syndrome, among medical doctors in the restive areas and non-restive areas of the South Thailand Insurgency. J Health Sci Med Res. 2018;36: 277–289. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Chatlaong T, Pitanupong J, Wiwattanaworaset P. Sleep quality and burnout syndrome among residents in training at the Faculty of Medicine, Prince of Songkla University. Siriraj Med J. 2020;72: 307–314. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Firth-Cozens J, Greenhalgh J. Doctors’ perceptions of the links between stress and lowered clinical care. Soc Sci Med. 1997;44: 1017–1022. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(96)00227-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Bellini LM, Baime M, Shea JA. Variation of mood and empathy during internship. JAMA. 2002;287: 3143–3146. doi: 10.1001/jama.287.23.3143 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Kotter T, Wagner J, Bruheim L, Voltmer E. Perceived medical school stress of undergraduate medical students predicts academic performance: an observational study. BMC Med Educ. 2017;17: 256. doi: 10.1186/s12909-017-1091-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Sathaporn K, Pitanupong J. Factors associated with the improvement of the empathy levels among clinical-year medical students in Southern Thailand: a university-based cross-sectional study. BMC Psychology.2022;10:128. doi: 10.1186/s40359-022-00842-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Vitayanont A, Pitanupong J. Processes of communication. Songkla Med J. 2011;29: 195–201. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Pitanupong J, Vitayanon A. Knowledge, attitude and insight regarding communication skills in medical students. Songkla Med J. 2011;29: 109–116. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Jumroonrojana K, Zartrungpak S. Development of the Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy-Student Version (Thai Version). J Psychiatr Assoc Thailand. 2012;57: 213–224. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Mongkol A, Huttapanom W. Thai Happiness Indicators (THI-15). 2011 [cited 4 October 2020]. In: Department of Mental Health [Internet]. Nonthaburi: Department of Mental Health, Ministry of Public Health. Available from: http://www.dmh.go.th/test/qtest/
  • 30.Summawart S. Burnout among the staff nurses in Ramathibodi Hospital. Thesis, Bangkok: Mahidol University. 1989. Available from: http://mulinet11.li.mahidol.ac.th/e-thesis/scan/18486.pdf
  • 31.Maslach C, Jackson SE. The measurement of experienced burnout. J Occup Behav. 1981;2: 99–113. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Maslach C, Schaufeli WB, Leiter MP. Job burnout. Annu Rev Psychol. 2001;52: 397–422. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.397 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Spreng RN, McKinnon MC, Mar RA, Levine B. The Toronto Empathy Questionnaire: scale development and initial validation of a factor-analytic solution to multiple empathy measures. J Pers Assess. 2009;91: 62–71. doi: 10.1080/00223890802484381 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Tariq N, Tayyab A, Jaffery T. Differences in empathy levels of medical students based on gender, year of medical school and career choice. J Coll Physicians Surg Pak. 2018;28: 310–313. doi: 10.29271/jcpsp.2018.04.310 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Kataoka HU, Koide N, Hojat M, Gonnella JS. Measurement and correlates of empathy among female Japanese physicians. BMC Med Educ. 2012;12: 48. doi: 10.1186/1472-6920-12-48 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Akgun O, Akdeniz M, Kavukcu E, Avci HH. Medical students’ empathy level differences by medical year, gender, and specialty interest in Akdeniz University. J Med Educ Curric Dev. 2020;7: 2382120520940658. doi: 10.1177/2382120520940658 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Hegazi I, Wilson I. Maintaining empathy in medical school: it is possible. Med Teach. 2013;35: 1002–1008. doi: 10.3109/0142159X.2013.802296 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Hojat M, Gonnella JS, Mangione S, Nasca TJ, Magee M. Physician empathy in medical education and practice: experience with the Jefferson scale of physician empathy. Seminars in Integrative Medicine. 2003;1: 25–41. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Beckman HB, Frankel RM. Training practitioners to communicate effectively in cancer care: it is the relationship that counts. Patient Educ Couns. 2003;50: 85–89. doi: 10.1016/s0738-3991(03)00086-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Larson EB, Yao X. Clinical empathy as emotional labor in the patient physician relationship. JAMA. 2005;293: 1100–1106. doi: 10.1001/jama.293.9.1100 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Anfossi M, Numico G. Empathy in the doctor-patient relationship. J Clin Oncol. 2004;22: 2259–2260. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2004.99.302 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Winseman J, Malik A, Morison J, Balkoski V. Students’ views on factors affecting empathy in medical education. Acad Psychiatry. 2009;33: 484–491. doi: 10.1176/appi.ap.33.6.484 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Bloom P. Empathy and its discontents. Trends Cogn Sci. 2017;21:24–31. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.11.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Vanessa Carels

16 Feb 2022

PONE-D-21-15809Relationship of mental health and burnout with empathy among medical students in Thailand: A multicenter cross-sectional study

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Sathaporn,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

The manuscript has been evaluated by two reviewers, and their comments are available below.

The reviewers have raised a number of major concerns, and have requested additional information on methodological aspects of the study and analyses. Could you please carefully revise the manuscript to address all comments raised?

Please submit your revised manuscript by Apr 01 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Vanessa Carels

Staff Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please provide additional details regarding participant consent. In the ethics statement in the Methods and online submission information, please ensure that you have specified (1) whether consent was informed and (2) what type you obtained (for instance, written or verbal, and if verbal, how it was documented and witnessed). If your study included minors, state whether you obtained consent from parents or guardians. If the need for consent was waived by the ethics committee, please include this information.

If you are reporting a retrospective study of medical records or archived samples, please ensure that you have discussed whether all data were fully anonymized before you accessed them and/or whether the IRB or ethics committee waived the requirement for informed consent. If patients provided informed written consent to have data from their medical records used in research, please include this information

3. Your abstract cannot contain citations. Please only include citations in the body text of the manuscript, and ensure that they remain in ascending numerical order on first mention.

4. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Dear Authors,

I am delighted to see a study on mental health and burnout from Thailand. Appropriate, well-validated (within a North American context) questionnaires were utilized to assess whether there is an association with empathy, broadly speaking, burnout, mental health, as well as what I perceive to be 'control' variables, such as substance use within your demographics questionnaire. In North American studies, what we do find is generally a decrease in empathy (per scale used for that study), and generally a negative relationship between burnout and empathy. However, given that your study focuses on Thai medical students, these constructs may not hold the same sort of validity, or may need to be interpreted differently. Indications for me was 1) high personal achievement despite burnout, which is fairly surprising given the North American literature, and 2) the conflation of all three aspects of empathy to be equally important, when they may be best served as broken apart into cognitive v emotional empathy. There is a great deal of sociological/philosophical literature and debate on the utilization of emotional empathy (see Paul Bloom's Against Empathy) and whether it is even needed within the clinical encounter.

I recommend to the authors to do their analysis but breaking both burnout and empathy constructs apart into their three dimensions to better understand the reasons for the differences seen. Merely stating that they are cultural differences without pulling in evidence suggestive of such (in Thai, non medical student samples) is far too speculative.

Reviewer #2: Thank you for submitting this manuscript reporting results of your study of three Thai medical school students' self-reported mental health, burnout and empathy. The following comments are meant to help the authors refine their work. I feel these edits will be easy for the authors to make.

Abstract

At the conclusion of your results and in the conclusions, you suggest that the statistical relationship between the mental health scale with empathy is causal. The analysis indicates there is a positive association between the two measures, but it does not mean that better mental health improves empathy levels. The wording needs to be revised to reflect that.

Introduction

Line 96-98: I am not entirely convinced this sentence belongs here. It read as something that should be part of the conclusions of your manuscript and not the background. It may be worth discussing amongst yourselves or the editor to decide.

Materials and Methods

With regard to the administration of the questionnaire, I was curious if there was a plan of who would get the paper questionnaire and who would get the electronic version. The reason I ask is that what if you administer it to a class on paper and the student forgets they did that and completes the electronic version as well. I know it is unlikely, but it is possible. A better explanation of how these two administrations were done and why is needed.

For the section on questionnaires, TMHI-15 and MBI need to have a sentence similar to the TEQ that defines item response options and scores. Along those same lines, it would be helpful to have how many questions on MBI and TEQ are included in their subscores. You indicated scores for MBI, but that does not tell us how many questions nor the scale.

I also had a question about MBI. Is there an expected correlation between the three subscales? It seems that emotional exhaustion and depersonalization would have a positive correlation and the inverse with personal accomplishment. It may be worth noting what should be expected and if your study found similar results.

Results section read nicely.

Discussion

Some additional limitations that should be addressed include potential bias due to greater number of female respondents and large number of Buddhist respondents. Also, have you considered non-response bias for those students not completing the survey?

Conclusion

Line 301 should be modified per my comment about the abstract.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes: Gary L Beck Dallaghan

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2023 Jan 5;18(1):e0279564. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0279564.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


26 Jul 2022

Dear, editor and reviver team

Thank you for your kindly review, comments, and suggestion.

Author's Responses to Questions of reviewer

Reviewer #1: Dear Authors,

Q: I am delighted to see a study on mental health and burnout from Thailand. Appropriate, well-validated (within a North American context) questionnaires were utilized to assess whether there is an association with empathy, broadly speaking, burnout, mental health, as well as what I perceive to be 'control' variables, such as substance use within your demographics questionnaire. In North American studies, what we do find is generally a decrease in empathy (per scale used for that study), and generally a negative relationship between burnout and empathy. However, given that your study focuses on Thai medical students, these constructs may not hold the same sort of validity, or may need to be interpreted differently. Indications for me was 1) high personal achievement despite burnout, which is fairly surprising given the North American literature, and 2) the conflation of all three aspects of empathy to be equally important, when they may be best served as broken apart into cognitive v emotional empathy. There is a great deal of sociological/philosophical literature and debate on the utilization of emotional empathy (see Paul Bloom's Against Empathy) and whether it is even needed within the clinical encounter.

A: Thanks for your kindly and useful recommendation.

We remove the sentence or wording that may be too speculative or not relate to the evidence of this research as the following

This variance might be due to differences in study instruments, population ethnicity, and culture.

We add discussion related to Paul Bloom's Against Empathy [reference 42] as following

However, empathy and concern other’s mind may be psychologically distinct and empathy may be limited by our moral lives [42].

Q: I recommend to the authors to do their analysis but breaking both burnout and empathy constructs apart into their three dimensions to better understand the reasons for the differences seen. Merely stating that they are cultural differences without pulling in evidence suggestive of such (in Thai, non-medical student samples) is far too speculative.

A: We try to do more analysis by breaking both empathy constructs apart and burnout into three dimensions as below.

However, three dimensions of burnout (EE, DP, PA) were evaluated to determine if they had an impact on any subgroups of empathy. Data was analyzed using a linear regression for each of only two dimensions of burnout (EE and DP) because of the majority of participants (97%) reported high PA. It was discovered that participants with higher levels of DP had statistically lower scores of demonstrating appropriate sensitivity, altruism, and behaviors engaging higher-order empathic responding (p<0.001, p<0.003, and p<0.014, respectively).

Reviewer #2: Thank you for submitting this manuscript reporting results of your study of three Thai medical school students' self-reported mental health, burnout and empathy. The following comments are meant to help the authors refine their work. I feel these edits will be easy for the authors to make.

Thanks for your kindness review

Abstract

Q: At the conclusion of your results and in the conclusions, you suggest that the statistical relationship between the mental health scale with empathy is causal. The analysis indicates there is a positive association between the two measures, but it does not mean that better mental health improves empathy levels. The wording needs to be revised to reflect that.

A: We correct the conclusion according your suggestion as

Most medical students had below-average empathy levels, and two-thirds of them had high emotional exhaustion levels, yet most of them reported having a high level of personal accomplishment and good mental health. Better mental health resulted a protective factor that improves the level of empathy There was an association between mental health and the level of empathy. Higher levels of depersonalization related to lower scores of demonstrating sensitivity, altruism, and behaviors responding.

Introduction

Q: Line 96-98: I am not entirely convinced this sentence belongs here. It read as something that should be part of the conclusions of your manuscript and not the background. It may be worth discussing amongst yourselves or the editor to decide.

A: We remove all these sentence as suggest.

The findings of this study provide useful information for efforts to enhance empathy, promote well-being, and reduce distress among medical students as well as establish educational programs in the medical curriculum geared at boosting medical professionalism.

Materials and Methods

Q: With regard to the administration of the questionnaire, I was curious if there was a plan of who would get the paper questionnaire and who would get the electronic version. The reason I ask is that what if you administer it to a class on paper and the student forgets they did that and completes the electronic version as well. I know it is unlikely, but it is possible. A better explanation of how these two administrations were done and why is needed.

A: We add the more explanation of these issue as

“The data were collected in the given classroom following relevant guidelines by using a paper-based or online process. One participant had the right to choose only one way to answer the questionnaire. Before entering the study, all participants were asked whether responded on paper or online questionnaire to prevent double response of completing this study.”

Q: For the section on questionnaires, TMHI-15 and MBI need to have a sentence similar to the TEQ that defines item response options and scores. Along those same lines, it would be helpful to have how many questions on MBI and TEQ are included in their subscores. You indicated scores for MBI, but that does not tell us how many questions nor the scale.

I also had a question about MBI. Is there an expected correlation between the three subscales? It seems that emotional exhaustion and depersonalization would have a positive correlation and the inverse with personal accomplishment. It may be worth noting what should be expected and if your study found similar results.

A: We add the more explanation of the range of each sub-scores in TMHI, MBI Tool as

2) Thai Mental Health Indicator-15 (TMHI-15) consisted of 15 questions. The score of each question ranged from 1 to 4; 1 (never); 2 (rarely); 3 (sometimes); and 4 (always),

3) Thai version of the Maslach Burnout Inventory (MBI) questionnaire [29,30] consisted of 22 items divided into three dimensions: emotional exhaustion (feelings of being emotionally overextended and exhausted by one’s work), depersonalization (unsympathetic and impersonal responses toward the recipients of one’s care or service), and personal accomplishment (feelings of competence and achievement in one’s work with people) [30]. Emotional exhaustion (EE) subscale ranged from 0 (never) to 6 (every day), Cronbach’s alpha coefficient=0.9. Depersonalization (DP) subscale ranged from 0 (never) to 6 (every day), Cronbach’s alpha coefficient=0.7. Personal accomplishment (PA) subscale ranged from 0 (every day) to 6 (never), Cronbach’s alpha coefficient=0.7.

Results

Q: section read nicely.

A: We feel good, thank you so much ^__^

Discussion

Q: Some additional limitations that should be addressed include potential bias due to greater number of female respondents and large number of Buddhist respondents. Also, have you considered non-response bias for those students not completing the survey?

A: We add the limitation according to suggestion as

Additionally, there might have potential bias due to greater number of female respondents and large number of Buddhist respondents.

Q: Also, have you considered non-response bias for those students not completing the survey?

A: We considered non-response bias for those students who not completing the survey, then we add more explanation in difference of demographic characteristic as

Additionally, there was no statistically significant difference in demographic data (gender, religion) between participants and non-participants.

Conclusion

Q: Line 301 should be modified per my comment about the abstract.

A: We correct the conclusion according the suggestion as

Better mental health was found to be a significant protective factor for improving the level of empathy. There was the association between mental health and the level of empathy. Higher levels of depersonalization related to lower scores of demonstrating sensitivity, altruism, and behaviors engaging empathic responding.

Finally

We have sent our “Revised manuscript” for re-checked and re-proofreading the English from the Office of International Affairs of the Faculty of Medicine.

Sincerely thanks for every nice, kind comments and suggestion, they are very useful to improve our manuscript.

Best regards

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Omnia Samir El Seifi

18 Oct 2022

PONE-D-21-15809R1Relationship of mental health and burnout with empathy among medical students in Thailand: A multicenter cross-sectional studyPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Sathaporn,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

==============================

ACADEMIC EDITOR:please respond to the comments to make your research suitable for publicationGrammar revision is requested

==============================

Please submit your revised manuscript by Dec 02 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Omnia Samir El Seifi, Professor

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #3: (No Response)

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #3: Partly

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #3: No

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #3: No

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #3: Major revision is needed

One of the objectives is to determine the association between burnout and empathy: So, it is not true to adjust for burnout in logistic regression. Burnout must be included in the model beside mental health.

It is not true to make two models (one for personal and one for depersonalization): you should make one logistic regression model that includes mental health, depersonalization and personal exhaustion. I mean the relation between burnout and empathy should be clearly reported in the logistic regression.

In your models (table 3 and 4), I noticed that you report one p value; it should be two p values: one for each category as compared with the reference group. Do this for the new model.

The sequence of table is not correct: begin with all the descriptive analysis then move to bivariate and then to multivariate: so, table one should include only descriptive statistics. Put the association just before the last table which is the multiple logistic regression.

If there are new findings, discuss them and include them in the conclusion.

Have you checked for multicollinearity in multiple logistic regression?

Your conclusion does not reflect the findings. Please rewrite it.

After conclusion, write your recommendation according to your findings.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #3: Yes: SAMI ABDO RADMAN AL-DUBAI

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2023 Jan 5;18(1):e0279564. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0279564.r004

Author response to Decision Letter 1


9 Nov 2022

Response to review Comments

Title: Relationship of mental health and burnout with empathy among medical students in Thailand: A multicenter cross-sectional study.

Reviewer #3: Major revision is needed

C: One of the objectives is to determine the association between burnout and empathy: So, it is not true to adjust for burnout in logistic regression. Burnout must be included in the model beside mental health.

It is not true to make two models (one for personal and one for depersonalization): you should make one logistic regression model that includes mental health, depersonalization and personal exhaustion. I mean the relation between burnout and empathy should be clearly reported in the logistic regression.

In your models (table 3 and 4), I noticed that you report one p value; it should be two p values: one for each category as compared with the reference group. Do this for the new model.

The sequence of table is not correct: begin with all the descriptive analysis then move to bivariate and then to multivariate: so, table one should include only descriptive statistics. Put the association just before the last table which is the multiple logistic regression.

A: We have modified the manuscript and tables as suggested (as table 1 show only descriptive data, and table 2,3 show bivariate and then multivariate, respectively)

C: If there are new findings, discuss them and include them in the conclusion.

A: The finding is not different from the original or previous results.

C: Have you checked for multicollinearity in multiple logistic regression?

A: Yes, we check it. Thanks.

C: If your conclusion does not reflect the findings. Please rewrite it.

A: The finding is the same as the prior results, then the conclusion is the same.

C: After the conclusion, write your recommendation according to your findings.

A: The finding is the same as the prior results. However, we concern about your valuable suggestion, then we add our recommendation according to our findings in conclusion part “Therefore, medical educators should pay close attention to promoting good mental health among medical students.”

Finally, we are grateful for your good wishes and valuable suggestion. This is helpful so much and encourages us to make this manuscript to get better. We wish to have more best wishes from you again. Thanks.

Attachment

Submitted filename: response to reviewer V3.docx

Decision Letter 2

Omnia Samir El Seifi

12 Dec 2022

Relationship of mental health and burnout with empathy among medical students in Thailand: A multicenter cross-sectional study

PONE-D-21-15809R2

Dear Dr. Sathaporn,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Omnia Samir El Seifi, Professor

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Omnia Samir El Seifi

16 Dec 2022

PONE-D-21-15809R2

Relationship of mental health and burnout with empathy among medical students in Thailand: A multicenter cross-sectional study

Dear Dr. Sathaporn:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Professor Omnia Samir El Seifi

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Questionnaire. English version questionnaire.

    (DOC)

    S2 Questionnaire. Thai version questionnaire.

    (DOC)

    S1 Data. English version minimal dataset.

    (XLSX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to reviewers.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: response to reviewer V3.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES