Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2023 Jan 11;18(1):e0272952. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0272952

Gender differences in motor and non-motor symptoms in individuals with mild-moderate Parkinson’s disease

Amit Abraham 1,2, Allison A Bay 3, Liang Ni 3, Nicole Schindler 4, Eeshani Singh 4, Ella Leeth 3, Ariyana Bozorg 3,5, Ariel R Hart 3, Madeleine E Hackney 3,5,6,7,8,*
Editor: Karsten Witt9
PMCID: PMC9833587  PMID: 36630320

Abstract

Background

Parkinson’s disease (PD) affects both men and women with documented gender differences across functional domains, with findings varying among reports. Knowledge regarding gender differences in PD for different geographic locations is important for further understanding of the disease and for developing personalized gender-specific PD assessment tools and therapies.

Objective

This study aimed to examine gender differences in PD-related motor, motor-cognitive, cognitive, and psychosocial function in people with PD from the southern United States (US).

Methods

199 (127 men and 72 women; M age: 69.08±8.94) individuals with mild-moderate idiopathic PD (Hoehn &Yahr (H&Y) Median = 2, stages I-III) from a large metro area in the southeastern US were included in this retrospective, cross-sectional study. Motor, motor-cognitive, cognitive, and psychosocial data were obtained using standardized and validated clinical tests. Univariate analyses were performed, adjusting for age and housing type.

Results

After adjustment for age, housing, PD duration and fall rate, men exhibited statistically significantly greater motor (Movement Disorders Society (MDS)-Unified Parkinson Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS)-II) and non-motor (MDS-UPDRS-I) impact of PD, and more severe motor signs (MDS-UPDRS-III). Men exhibited worse PD-specific health-related quality of life related to mobility, activities of daily living, emotional well-being, cognitive impairment, communication, and more depressive symptoms. Men performed worse on a subtraction working memory task. Women had slower fast gait speed.

Conclusions

In the southeastern United States, men may experience worse PD-related quality of life and more depression than women. Many non-motor and motor variables that are not PD specific show no differences between genders in this cohort. These findings can contribute to the development of gender-sensitive assessment and rehabilitation policies and protocols for people with PD.

Introduction

Parkinson’s disease (PD), the second most common neurodegenerative disease, results in motor and non-motor (i.e., cognitive, sensory, etc.) impairments that deteriorate quality of life (QoL) and wellbeing [1]. PD burdens men and women affected by the disease as well as families, health care systems, and societies [2]. Estimated costs associated with PD are as high as $34 billion per year [3]. PD affects about 1% of individuals over 60 years of age [4], and more than 10 million men and women worldwide, including 1,238,000 Americans by the year 2030 [5].

Differences between men and women in PD epidemiology, clinical presentation, and response to pharmacological therapy have been reported [68]. These differences may be explained by gender-specific age-related physical and cognitive changes, role expectations, societal attitudes [9], gender differences in life expectancy [10] and age at PD onset [7] (women are older at onset). Additional factors, such as social support [11] and living environment [12], also impact functional and psychological status and quality of life (QoL) in individuals with PD.

Indeed, there may be some pathophysiological basis for differences in several motor, cognitive and psychosocial factors between men and women with PD. Gender seems to influence the expression of several polymorphisms in PD. Genetic factors might differentially influence the manifestations of PD in men and women. It is important to consider estrogen and reproductive factors also. Female and male reproductive hormones have important influence that can significantly alter individual thermoregulatory responses during the lifespan [13]. Vascular responsiveness exhibits age- and sex-based differences in healthy subjects and trauma patients and estrogen appears to be protective [14]. Higher estrogen and progesterone levels may be related inversely to upper gastrointestinal cancers, which may help explain lower incidence rates of such cancer in women compared with men [15].

There may be a positive effect of estrogens on the dopaminergic system. Gonadal hormones and sex chromosomes might modulate PD risk by influencing epigenetic mechanisms. Preclinical evidence suggests potential neuroprotective effects of estrogens against dopaminergic damage through anti-inflammatory, anti-oxidative, and anti-apoptotic mechanisms in addition to possible inhibitory effects on the formation and stabilization of α-synuclein fibrils—a pathological feature of PD [16]. Women with presumed higher cumulative lifetime levels of both endogenous and exogenous estrogen had a significantly reduced risk of PD relative to those with lower lifetime estrogen exposure [17]. Further, neuroimaging findings support structural and functional signatures in women with PD, that are characterized by a more preserved presynaptic system and higher striatal dopaminergic levels at disease onset compared with men [16].

In addition, sociocultural and demographic factors may affect men and women differently in the southeastern United States. Older adults in the Southeastern United States, compared to the rest of the country have considerable health and health-related quality of life (QoL) challenges [18]. The region has high numbers of individuals with other debilitating chronic diseases besides PD, such as diabetes mellitus, cardiovascular disease, obesity and cancer. For example, diabetes has a great influence on life expectancy for many older adults who live in the south, and persons with diabetes born in the south were more likely to have developed chronic conditions or disabilities and spent more of their life with the chronic conditions compared to other regions in the United States [19]. In patients with other comorbid conditions (e.g., HIV), those in the South were on average more vulnerable to falls than were individuals from other regions [20]. As regards potential environmental risk factors for PD, women tend to have a lower exposure to occupational toxins and a lower incidence of head trauma than men, reflecting differences in behavioral and social factors [16]. Examining the status of people with PD from this region is important and whether gender modifies this relationship deserves more research.

The role of gender in PD, motor, and motor-cognitive outcomes is not entirely clear to date, with conflicting findings across studies [21]. For example, while some studies found activities of daily living (ADL), cognition and communication-related aspects of health related QoL (HRQoL) were rated lesser in men [22, 23], other studies reported worse disability, QoL and HRQoL in women [24, 25]. Further, studies that directly compare men and women on performance of frequently used assessments of functional mobility and motor-cognitive function are rare, although these data and comparisons would be of great interest to the rehabilitation field. Empirical data regarding the influence of gender on PD management and care is scarce [26], especially regarding how gender impacts response to non-invasive and non-pharmacological therapies, which should influence the development and implementation of personalized rehabilitative protocols for individuals with PD [6, 26]. Therefore, further exploration of the impact of gender on PD-related motor and non-motor symptoms as well as performance on standard clinical measures of mobility, motor-cognitive and cognitive function is warranted and could promote the development of PD patient-tailored, gender-sensitive rehabilitation assessment and therapies. The following example illustrates this issue’s importance. A study of 1,463 participants (914 men with a mean age of 64.5±10.37 and 549 women with a mean age of 65.7±10.97) found at diagnosis of PD, women were more likely to be less educated, more anxious, and faced greater instrumental activities of daily living (IADL) disability [25]. Yet, current physical and rehabilitative therapies for PD do not incorporate this important information into clinical decision-making, rendering gender-sensitive treatment protocols effectively unavailable to date [26].

Gaining additional knowledge regarding the role of gender on PD, and particularly within the Southeastern United States region is important for: 1) further understanding the impact of PD on both men and women from a comparatively more vulnerable population of older adults (Southern U.S. older adults) to better interpret findings; 2) gaining a window into future research about disease mechanisms and treatment strategies; and 3) promoting gender-specific PD rehabilitation and therapies. Addressing the importance and need for additional research into “detailed and specific cognitive and functional assessment by gender,” [27] this paper will examine gender differences in cognitive and functional variables of rehabilitative interest. The terms “sex” and “gender” are not equivalent but are frequently used interchangeably in the literature [6], with “gender” including both biological (i.e., sex) as well as social-environmental, cultural, and personal aspects and implications of being a woman or a man (i.e., gender) [6, 28, 29]. Because all variables considered in this study are behavioral, which encompasses social components, and given that “gender” is viewed to be more relevant for describing how biological and social components affect health outcomes [29], this paper uses the term, gender, which is in line with PD literature [6, 9, 28, 3032]. This study sought to examine and compare performance in a sample of men and women with diagnosed mild-moderate PD (Hoehn & Yahr stages I-III) residing in the Southeast of the United States in motor, motor-cognitive, cognitive, psychosocial and PD-specific function.

Methods

The study was approved by the Emory Institutional Review Board and the VA Review committee (Protocols IRB060613, IRB055977, and IRB047231). All participants gave written informed consent prior to participation in study activities.

Participants

Data from 199 (127 men and 72 women; M age: 69.08±8.94) individuals with idiopathic PD (H&Y Median = 2, stages I-III) were included in this retrospective, cross-sectional study. To be included in the study, all participants had to have a diagnosis of idiopathic “definite PD” that was determined by a board certified Movement Disorders trained neurologist [33], which meant that at time of diagnosis they reported unilateral onset of symptoms, they exhibited three of the four cardinal signs of PD (i.e., rigidity, tremor, bradykinesia, and postural instability) and exhibited clear symptomatic benefit from antiparkinsonian medications [34]. Inclusion criteria were ability to walk ten or more feet with or without an assistive device and having no other diagnosed neurological disorders. Exclusion criteria were major psychiatric illness, history of stroke, or traumatic brain injury, alcohol abuse and/or use of antipsychotics, severe cardiac disease, and any other significant co-morbid disease that would impair ability to participate. Participant demographics are detailed in Table 1.

Table 1. Participants’ demographics and clinical characteristics.

Total (n = 199) Men (n = 127) Women (n = 72) Difference (P value)
Age (y) 69.08±8.94 68.17±9.44 70.68±7.79 0.057
BMI 26.40±4.83 25.05±5.50 27.16±4.25 0.006**
Race 0.109
    Black 30 (15.2) 14 (11.1) 16 (22.2)
    White 155 (78.3) 103 (81.7) 52 (72.2)
    Other 13 (6.6) 9 (7.1) 4 (5.6)
Hoehn & Yahra 2 (1) 2 (0.5) 2 (1) 0.977
MoCA (/30) 25.18±3.9 24.91±4.2 25.67±3.4 0.223
Education (years) 16.36±2.29 16.44±2.26 16.22±2.34 0.530
Number of Falls in the Past Year 7.76±37.73 6.9±33.36 9.26±44.58 0.672
Number of Comorbidities 3.42±1.81 3.24±1.79 3.72±1.82 0.074
Composite Physical Function (/24) 19±5.08 19.35±4.99 18.39±5.2 0.201
Physical Activity Scale for the Elderly (PASE) 103.34±70.2 104.17±69.4 101.95±72.2 0.843
Number of Medication 5.9±4.13 6.15±4.26 5.51±3.93 0.336
Years with PD (y) 6.64±4.58 6.34±4.49 7.16±4.73 0.235
Housing 0.005**
    Assisted/senior living 21 (10.7) 7 (5.6) 14 (19.4)
    Self/independently 176 (89.3) 118 (94.4) 58 (80.6)
Transportation 0.089
    Family 40 (20.3) 30 (24) 10 (13.9)
    Public 6 (3) 2 (1.6) 4 (5.6)
    Self 150 (76.1) 93 (74.4) 57 (79.2)
    Service 1 (0.5) 0 (0) 1 (1.4)
Leaving House Frequency 0.909
    0–2 per week 16 (8.2) 11 (8.8) 5 (7)
    3–4 per week 55 (28.1) 35 (28) 20 (28.2)
    Daily 125 (63.8) 79 (63.2) 46 (64.8)
Freezerb 0.902
    No 50 (62.5) 33 (61.1) 17 (65.4)
    Yes 30 (37.5) 21 (38.9) 9 (34.6)
Use of Assistive Device 0.859
    No 118 (69.8) 73 (68.9) 45 (71.4)
    Yes 51 (30.2) 33 (31.1) 18 (28.6)

Values are presented as Mean ± SD for continuous variables, and n (%) for categorical variables.

BMI = Body Mass Index.

P values were calculated with t-test/ANOVA for continuous variables and Chi-square test for categorical variables.

a Values are median (Interquartile range). P value obtained from Mann-Whitney U test.

b Freezer = experiencing freezing of gait.

*p < 0.05

**p < 0.01

Data collection & outcome measures

Data were collected using surveys, which were self-completed at home on paper, online, in person at the assessment site or over the phone with research assistant help. Research assistants verified that participants understood the intent of questionnaires. Participants were asked to verify that they self-completed the surveys. Motor and cognitive data were collected on-site. Standardized, valid, and reliable clinical measures were used for assessing motor, cognitive, and psychosocial function.

Socio-demographics and disease severity & stage

Socio-demographic factors (age, race, education, number of falls in the past year, number of comorbidities, number of medications, years with PD, housing, transportation, frequency of leaving the house, freezer status (i.e., freezing of gait: yes or no), and use of assistive device) were collected via a health questionnaire. In addition, The Composite Physical Function (CPF) questionnaire, which characterizes participants’ ability to complete activities of daily living (ADLs) (/24; higher = better), was completed [35]. Participants’ self-reported frequency and duration of physical activity was assessed using the Physical Activity Scale for the Elderly (PASE) questionnaire, which has scores that range from 0―400, with higher scores indicating greater physical activity [36]. The MoCA, which assesses global cognition through 8 cognitive domains [37], was administered. A higher score represents better global cognition.

Measures of disease severity included the MDS-UPDRS parts I-IV, on which higher scores indicate greater disease severity [38]. Part I assesses the non-motor impact of PD on patients’ experiences of daily living, including apathy, anxiety, sleep, hallucinations, fatigue etc. Part II assesses the motor impact of PD on the patient’s experience of daily living. Items covered include drooling, eating, handwriting, and rolling over in a bed. Part III assesses the motor signs of PD. This section is administered by a trained examiner wherein the examiner observes the participant performing several motor tasks (i.e., rising from a chair, toe tapping, etc.) The examiner also observes tremor, muscle rigidity, posture, and gait. Part IV is delivered in interview format and is a self-reported indication of medication-related motor fluctuations (MRMF): dyskinesias, time spent in the off state, functional impact and complexity of fluctuation, and off-state dystonia. Current Hoehn & Yahr (HY; Stages 1–5) staging at the time of the assessment was also measured by the MDS-UPDRS [38].

PD quality of life was assessed using the self-report measure, the Parkinson’s Disease Questionnaire 39 (PDQ-39). The PDQ-39 measures health-related QoL over the past month for PD patients in 8 domains: mobility, activities of daily living (ADLs), emotional well-being, stigma, social support, cognition, communication, and bodily discomfort, plus a summary index (PDQ-39 SI) which indicates the global impact of PD on health status, which is internally reliable and valid [3941]. Lower scores indicate better quality of life.

Motor

Fullerton Advanced Balance (FAB; /40) was used to measure static and dynamic balance and is valid for use in PD [42]. Lower FAB scores indicate difficulty with higher level static and dynamic balance tasks. Gait speeds (preferred, backward, fast) were obtained by recording the time (in seconds) for the participant to walk 6 meters at their self-selected (‘normal’) gait speed, as fast as possible (‘fast’) forward gait speed, and self-selected backward (‘backward’) gait speed, and are reported in meters per second (m/s). Number of steps required to complete each task was also recorded. Backward walking is more likely to be impaired earlier in the disease than forward walking and is more sensitive to change over time [43]. In the Timed Up and Go (TUG) test, individuals are instructed to stand up from a chair, as quickly and as safely as possible, walk three meters, cross a line marked on the floor, turn around, walk back, and sit down. The TUG-Cognitive version, which involves a cognitive dual task, is described below in Motor Cognitive variables.

Motor cognitive

In the TUG-Cognitive assessment, participants complete TUG task while also counting backward by threes from a randomly selected number between 20 and 100, paying equal attention to walking and counting [44]. The TUG percent time change is calculated as the time (sec) for TUG-Cognitive minus the time needed to complete simple TUG, divided by simple TUG and multiplied by 100.

The Body Position Spatial Task (BPST) incorporates spatial memory and navigational skills while maintaining posture. The examiner demonstrates (verbally and visually) a pattern of side, forward, and turning (in place) steps, which the examinee repeats. The patterns increase in number of steps and turns with each additional level. Scores obtained are the number of correctly remembered sequences (Correct Trials) and length of the longest sequence remembered correctly (Span) [45].

Cognition

The Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System™ (D-KEFS™) Color Word Interference Test (CWIT) [46] measures executive function over four conditions: color naming, word reading, inhibition, and inhibition/switching [47] Scaled scores have been age-adjusted by normative performance by age group per the Delis Kaplan Executive Function System manual. Corsi Blocks assesses short-term and working memory using nonverbal analog and consists of a board containing nine cubes at fixed, pseudorandom positions. The blocks are labeled with numbers only visible to the experimenter. The experimenter taps several blocks, after which the participant attempts to tap this block sequence in the reverse order as that presented. The block sequences gradually increase in length, and the scores obtained are the number of correctly remembered sequences (Correct Trials) and the length of the longest sequence that was remembered correctly (Span) [48]. The Tower of London (ToL) assesses organization and planning ability, an aspect of executive function [49]. The administrator presents a card depicting a specific arrangement and the participants move five rings of varying sizes on three pegs to match the arrangement. The number of moves and the time it takes to complete the task are recorded.

Psychosocial

The Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) is a self-report of the behavioral manifestation of depression [50] over the previous two weeks. Higher scores indicate more depressive symptoms.

Data analysis

Secondary analyses were performed on baseline data from longitudinal studies conducted in our lab between 2011–2019 [5154]. Data normality was tested using the Shapiro test and skewness and kurtosis, using Q-Q plots (not presented). Sample sizes for each variable are indicated in the tables. Missing data were observed and recorded. Some data were not available for lack of compliance (patient refused) and for operator error (approximately 1–3% of data points). Descriptive analyses describe demographic variables. Differences between groups were determined with independent t-tests for continuous variables and Chi-square test for categorical variables. Multiple linear regression analyses compared the mean of outcome variables between men and women with adjustments for age (Model 1), age and housing, (Model 2) and age, housing, PD duration and number of falls in the past year (Model 3). Age, PD duration and number of falls were chosen to be covariates because of their relevance to rehabilitation outcomes that could have been influenced by gender. For example, in the general older adult population, injurious falls occur more often in older women than men [55]. We also examined BMI as a covariate as well but it did not alter the findings beyond Models 1, 2 and 3. The P value significance level was set at < 0.05.

Some data were not available for some variables because of assessments not having been administered for these participants due to scheduling or other barriers, participant refusal (rare), data not having been adequately or accurately captured (rare), because the assessment was not administered to a particular cohort, or because some measures, e.g., the MDS-UPDRS were more widely and popularly used later in the course of the study years, whereas the UPDRS was used earlier in the study.

Results

All data were normally distributed. Demographic and clinical characteristics for each gender group are presented in Table 1. The gender distribution within the current sample (63.81% men and 36.19% women) is similar to previous studies [9, 25, 30]. Participants were 20 percent non-white, had mild-moderate PD (stages I-III) and the equivalent of a bachelor’s degree in education attainment. Differences were noted between men and women on housing status, and this was therefore used in the model. Men were less likely to live in senior housing than women (p < 0.01).

Mean gender differences for disease severity & symptoms, motor, cognitive, motor-cognitive, and psychosocial measures are presented in Tables 2 and 3 along with adjusted models evaluating differences between groups.

Table 2. Differences between women and men on disease severity, symptoms and psychosocial function.

Women Men Un-adj P Model 1 (Age-adjusted) Model 2 (Age & Housing Adjusted) Model 3 (Full Model)
N M ± SD N M ± SD Beta (95% CI) β^ Adj P Beta (95% CI) β^ Adj P Beta (95% CI) β^ Adj P
Disease Severity         
MDS-UPDRS
    Part I 52 10.71±7.27 90 13.44±7.64 0.039* 2.55 (-0.1, 5.2) 0.16 0.059 2.98 (0.3, 5.7) 0.19 0.031* 3.13 (0.5, 5.7) 0.20 0.019*
    Part II 51 10.63±7.07 89 16.88±8.87 <0.001** 6.54 (3.6, 9.5) 0.36 <0.001** 6.75 (3.7, 9.8) 0.37 <0.00** 6.79 (3.9, 9.6) 0.37 <0.01**
    Part III 72 31.83±12.16 127 34.1±12.14 0.207 3.18 (-0.3, 6.7) 0.13 0.075 3.74 (0.2, 7.3) 0.15 0.041* 4.05 (0.6, 7.5) 0.16 0.022*
    Part IV 51 3.1±3.45 89 4.2±3.8 0.089 0.83 (-0.5, 2.1) 0.11 0.206 0.84 (-0.5, 2.2) 0.11 0.212 0.96 (-0.3, 2.2) 0.12 0.133
Psychosocial         
PDQ-39
    Mobility 68 20.71±21.56 122 25.16±22.13 0.182 4.37 (-2.3, 11) 0.1 0.198 6.7 (0, 13.4) 0.15 0.05 7.89 (1.5, 14.3) 0.17 0.016*
    ADL 69 17.66±16.08 124 27.72±20.47 <0.001** 9.2 (3.5, 14.9) 0.23 0.002** 9.47 (3.6, 15.3) 0.23 0.002** 10.48 (4.8, 16.1) 0.26 <0.001 **
    Emotional Well Being 69 18±20.27 123 23.07±18.98 0.084 4.11 (-1.6, 9.8) 0.1 0.159 5.82 (0, 11.6) 0.14 0.049* 6.4 (0.6, 12.2) 0.16 0.03*
    Stigma 69 15.88±19.23 123 14.68±18.95 0.677 -2.48 (-8.1, 3.1) -0.06 0.381 -1.6 (-7.3, 4.1) -0.04 0.58 -0.27 (-5.9, 5.3) -0.01 0.925
    Social Support 68 15.84±19.85 123 17.17±26.99 0.697 0.1 (-7.3, 7.5) 0 0.98 0.9 (-6.6, 8.4) 0.02 0.812 1.28 (-6.3, 8.8) 0.02 0.739
    Cognitive Impairment 69 23.34±17.51 124 28.73±20.58 0.068 5.2 (-0.7, 11.1) 0.13 0.083 5.16 (-0.9, 11.2) 0.13 0.094 6.23 (0.2, 12.3) 0.15 0.044*
    Communication 69 18.63±20.27 124 26.41±20.71 0.013* 7.68 (1.5, 13.9) 0.18 0.015* 7.81 (1.5, 14.1) 0.18 0.015* 9.45 (3.4, 15.5) 0.22 0.002**
    Bodily Discomfort 69 33.33±23.44 124 30.31±20.81 0.356 -3.73 (-10.2, 2.8) -0.08 0.258 -3.06 (-9.7, 3.6) -0.07 0.367 -2.19 (-8.9, 4.5) -0.05 0.521
    Summary Index 68 20.62±14.41 124 24.05±14.4 0.116 2.73 (-1.5, 7) 0.09 0.209 3.63 (-0.7, 8) 0.12 0.099 4.64 (0.4, 8.9) 0.15 0.032*
    Beck Depression Inventory-II (/63) 62 10.53±7.86 110 13.74±8.51 0.016* 2.71 (0.1, 5.3) 0.16 0.041* 3.4 (0.8, 6) 0.19 0.011* 3.51 (0.9, 6.1) 0.20 0.009**

Performance on measures of disease severity and symptoms and psychosocial function is presented.

^β: Standardized beta. Model 1 is age-adjusted, Model 2 is age and housing type adjusted and Model 3 is age, housing type, PD duration and number of falls in the past year adjusted; a PDQ-39 Question (Q) Scoring: 5-point Likert Scale (0–4); Mobility (MOB) = 100*((sum([Q1-Q10]))/(4*10)); ADL = 100*((sum([Q11-Q16]))/(4*6)); Emotional Wellbeing (EWB) = 100*((sum([Q17-Q22]))/(4*6)); Stigma (STIG) = 100*((sum([Q23-Q26]))/(4*4)); Social Support (SS) = 100*((sum([Q27-Q29]))/(4*3)); Cognitive Impairment (CI) = 100*((sum([Q30-Q33]))/(4*4)); Communication (COMM) = 100*((sum([Q34-Q36]))/(4*3])); Bodily Discomfort (BD) = 100*((sum([Q37-Q39]))/(4*3])); Summary Index = (sum([MOB], [ADL], [EWB], [STIG], [SS], [CI], [COMM], [BD]))/8. Lower scores are better for the PDQ-39 and the MDS-UPDRS.

*p < 0.05.

**p < 0.01.

Table 3. Differences between women and men on motor, cognitive, and motor-cognitive function.

Women Men Un-adj P Model 1 (Age-adjusted) Model 2 (Age & Housing Adjusted) Model 3 (Full Model)
N M ± SD N M ± SD Beta (95% CI) β^ Adj P Beta (95% CI) β^ Adj P Beta (95% CI) β^ Adj P
Motor        
    Fullerton Balance (/40) 50 25.32±8.8 89 27.87±7.45 0.072 1.74 (-0.7, 4.2) 0.1 0.169 1.17 (-1.3, 3.7) 0.07 0.355 0.72 (-1.7, 3.2) 0.04 0.564
    Preferred Gait Speed (m/s) 64 1.01±0.28 106 1.01±0.24 0.997 -0.02 (-0.1, 0.1) -0.03 0.697 -0.04 (-0.1, 0) -0.08 0.275 -0.05 (-0.1, 0) -0.09 0.234
    Backward Gait Speed (m/s) 63 0.58±0.27 106 0.66±0.3 0.078 0.06 (0, 0.1) 0.1 0.171 0.03 (-0.1, 0.1) 0.06 0.44 0.03 (-0.1, 0.1) 0.04 0.558
    Fast Gait Speed (m/s) 64 1.31±0.36 106 1.45±0.38 0.019* 0.12 (0, 0.2) 0.15 0.045* 0.08 (0, 0.2) 0.1 0.167 0.08 (0, 0.2) 0.1 0.202
        Motor-Cognitive & Cognitive
    Timed Up and Go-Cognitive (s) 70 18.29±20.09 120 14.65±8.41 0.153 -2.67 (-6.8, 1.4) -0.09 0.2 -0.72 (-4.7, 3.3) -0.02 0.722 -0.29 (-4.3, 3.7) -0.01 0.886
    TUG Pct. Time Change (100*C.-S./S.) (%) 70 44.45±43.45 122 32.3±38.95 0.048* -11.04 (-23.1, 1) -0.13 0.072 -9.34 (-21.7, 3) -0.11 0.138 -8.96 (-21.5, 3.6) -0.11 0.161
    Serial 3 Correct Subtractions 72 6.43±3.65 124 8.83±4.03 <0.001** 2.18 (1, 3.3) 0.26 <0.001** 2.1 (0.9, 3.3) 0.25 <0.001** 1.93 (0.8, 3.1) 0.23 0.001**
    Corsi Span 72 4.21±1.11 125 4.52±1.42 0.09 0.27 (-0.1, 0.7) 0.1 0.16 0.21 (-0.2, 0.6) 0.08 0.292 0.19 (-0.2, 0.6) 0.07 0.357
    Corsi Trials 72 5.61±1.84 125 6.02±2.32 0.171 0.32 (-0.3, 0.9) 0.07 0.302 0.18 (-0.4, 0.8) 0.04 0.58 0.14 (-0.5, 0.8) 0.03 0.672
        BPST Span 68 3.57±0.87 124 3.83±1.04 0.085 0.19 (-0.1, 0.5) 0.09 0.204 0.09 (-0.2, 0.4) 0.05 0.525 0.07 (-0.2, 0.4) 0.03 0.637
        BPST Trials 68 4.21±1.58 124 4.48±1.67 0.263 0.12 (-0.4, 0.6) 0.03 0.624 -0.02 (-0.5, 0.4) -0.01 0.916 -0.09 (-0.6, 0.4) -0.03 0.715
    CWIT-Inhibition Scaled Score 44 10.09±3.44 70 8.91±3.76 0.095 -1.12 (-2.5, 0.3) -0.15 0.117 -1.15 (-2.6, 0.3) -0.15 0.117 -1.35 (-2.8, 0.1) -0.18 0.061
    CWIT-Inhibition/ Switching Scaled Score 44 9.59±3.7 67 8.88±3.39 0.3 -0.9 (-2.3, 0.5) -0.12 0.197 -1.05 (-2.4, 0.3) -0.15 0.137 -1.09 (-2.5, 0.3) -0.15 0.117
    CWIT-Contrast Scaled Score Inhibition vs. Color Naming 44 9.98±2.84 70 9.51±3.1 0.424 -0.55 (-1.7, 0.6) -0.09 0.347 -0.79 (-2, 0.4) -0.13 0.178 -0.91 (-2.1, 0.3) -0.15 0.125
    CWIT-Contrast Scaled Score Inhibition/ Switching vs. Inhibition 44 9.2±2.78 67 9.72±2.98 0.366 0.4 (-0.7, 1.5) 0.07 0.485 0.49 (-0.7, 1.6) 0.08 0.405 0.53 (-0.6, 1.7) 0.09 0.37
    Tower of London Total Achievement Score 50 15.22± 4.22 90 15.58±5.37 0.685 0.03 (-1.7, 1.8) 0 0.971 -0.02 (-1.8, 1.7) 0 0.982 -0.16 (-1.9, 1.6) -0.02 0.852
    Tower of London Time Ratio Scaled 50 8.46±3.93 90 8.62±4.24 0.824 -0.03 (-1.5, 1.4) 0 0.972 -0.01 (-1.5, 1.5) 0 0.993 0 (-1.5, 1.5) 0 0.995
    Tower of London Rule Violations 50 2.82±5.31 90 3.27±4.51 0.6 0.8 (-0.9, 2.5) 0.08 0.353 1.04 (-0.7, 2.8) 0.1 0.231 1.01 (-0.7, 2.7) 0.1 0.25

Performance on measures of motor, cognitive, and motor-cognitive function is presented.

^β: Standardized beta. Model 1 is age-adjusted, Model 2 is age and housing type adjusted and Model 3 is age, housing type, PD duration and number of falls in the past year adjusted TUG = Timed Up & Go; BPST = Body Position Spatial Task; CWIT = Color Word Interference Test; ToL = Tower of London; Lower scores are better for the PDQ-39 and the MDS-UPDRS.

*p < 0.05.

**p < 0.01

Men exhibited worse disease severity and symptoms, including greater non-motor impact on experiences of daily living (MDS-UPDRS-I; p < 0.05 in Models 2 & 3), greater motor impact of PD on experiences of daily living (MDS-UPDRS-II; p < 0.001 in Models 1, 2 & 3), and greater motor signs of PD (MDS-UPDRS-III; p < 0.05 in Models 2 & 3). (Table 2)

On mobility measures, women exhibited slower gait speed than men (p < .05; Model 1). No gender differences were detected in self-selected gait speed (p > 0.05; adjusted β range: -0.03 ― -0.09), backward gait speed (p > 0.05; adjusted β range: 0.04–0.1), and functional balance (as measured by FAB; p > 0.5; adjusted β range: 0.04–0.1). (Table 3)

For the cognitive and motor-cognitive measures, women performed worse on serial 3 subtractions (p < 0.001; Models 1, 2 & 3). (Table 3)

For the psychosocial and QoL measures, men exhibited worse scores for the PDQ-39 subscales of mobility (p < 0.05; Model 2), ADL (p < 0.001; Models 1, 2 & 3), emotional well-being (p < 0.05; Models 2 & 3), communication (p < 0.05 for Models 1 & 2; p < 0.01 for Model 3), cognitive impairment (p < 0.05; Model 3) and Summary Index Score (p < 0.05; Model 3). Men also exhibited more depressive symptoms (p < 0.05 for Models 1 & 2; p < 0.01 for Model 3; Table 2).

Discussion

The current study aimed to examine and compare motor, motor-cognitive, cognitive and psychosocial function between men and women with diagnosed mild-moderate PD. Novel findings of this study demonstrate for the first time that in these patients residing in the Southeast of the United States (US), the impact of gender is most evident in PD-specific measures of motor symptoms, QoL and activities of daily living (ADL). Men experienced overall greater impact of the disease than women as reflected by the PDQ-39, a measure of HRQoL, motor signs and motor and non-motor experiences of daily living (e.g., speech, swallowing, hygiene, dressing, handwriting, mobility skills and cognition, apathy, sleep, pain, and depression). For non-PD measures of function, men experienced more depression, while women had slower fast gait speed and gave fewer correct answers during serial 3s subtractions. The most striking differences were noted in PD-specific measures of motor and psychosocial function, while the differences between genders in non-PD specific measures were quite minor in this sample.

Gender differences in PD-related non-motor symptoms

Men in this sample exhibited greater impact of PD on non-motor experiences of daily living, per the MDS-UPDRS (parts I and II). Reports that used other instruments showed that women had more severe non-motor symptoms than men per the Non Motor Symptoms Scale (NMSS) [9], including sleep/fatigue, mood/apathy, constipation, restless legs, and pain, and sexual dysfunction [56]. Two other studies reported that women had more severe symptoms of fatigue [9, 57, 58], light-headedness, fainting, lack of interest in surroundings, and lack of motivation, feelings of nervousness and sadness [9, 57]. Worse QoL could have been impacted by worse disease severity, greater motor and non-motor impact of PD on daily activities, and more depression, all experienced by men in our study. Worse PD related QoL in men aligns with other studies that also used PDQ-39: One study with 210 people with PD (61.4% men; M age: 69.1±10.8) from Australia found lower PD related QoL for men in the ADL, communication and cognition domains [22]. Other studies, however, reported worse PD related QoL in women, with some studies using the SF-12 questionnaire [23, 24, 30], and others using a validated translated version of the PDQ-39 [56]. Age differences may have played a minor role: in a study that analyzed 12 PD cohorts, the mean age ranged from 59.56 to 70.53 (males) and 59.51 to 70.14 (females) [21]. Cultural and social factors, such as perceiving “quality of life” could also serve as covariates explaining this opposing trend in non-motor symptoms. For example, the PDQ-39 components of social support and bodily discomfort are influenced by cultural and social norms, which may resonate more with men than with women in this sample. These potential explanations are in line with previous studies which highlighted the controversial role of gender in QoL in people with PD [56].

It is possible that the PD-specific measures are more attuned to issues that most affect men from this region, as opposed to the women. The disease is widely known to be slightly more prevalent in men, and while the measures were intended to be gender neutral, whether this holds true for different regions, with varying overall health status, social expectations and education levels, remains to be determined.

Gender differences in PD-related motor symptoms

Finding worse scores on PD-related motor experiences of daily living (per the UPDRS-II) and PD motor signs (per the UPDRS-III) in men aligns with previous studies suggesting men have greater (i.e., worse) ADL [21, 59, 60] and motor [21, 22, 32, 61] symptoms severity. The lack of gender difference in self-reported indication of medication-related motor fluctuations, including dyskinesias (as measured by UPDRS-IV) does not align, however, with a previous report that found that females presented more dyskinesia than males [21]. In several studies, no differences between genders in scores on the UPDRS–Motor Examination (or the Movement Disorders Society revision (UPDRS-III), were shown [32, 56, 57, 59, 6264]. Other work demonstrated women scored worse on the UPDRS-III [31], had more postural problems [32], and had worse ADL impairments [30]. Such inconsistency between research findings might be explained by between-studies differences in measures (e.g., the use of modified UPDRS-III and specific sub-scale scores (e.g., instability) or the use of P15-item Penn Parkinson’s Disease Daily Activities Questionnaire for assessing cognition-related instrumental functional abilities [65], sample size [21, 30], participants’ mean age [21, 31], ethnicity distribution [32], and geographical and cultural elements [31, 57]. On medication-related motor fluctuations (as measured by MDS-UPDRS-IV), another study also observed no gender difference [32].

Another possibility is that the men in the Southeastern region of the United States may be physically sicker than the women, which is true of the general population of older adults, given that women generally outlive men by 5 or more years [66]. This overall greater health burden may be reflected in higher symptom scores seen in these southern men.

Gender differences in non-PD specific functional mobility

No gender differences were found in any of the non-PD specific functional mobility measures (besides gait speed). Slower fast gait speed in women, does not align with another study with 78 participants with PD that did not detect a gender difference in fast gait speed [67], possibly because of the use of normalized gait speeds (versus the use of non-normalized speeds in the current study) to calculate gender differences [67] and because these participants had longer disease duration (e.g., disease duration of 8.50±4.88 years versus 6.64±4.58 years in the current study). Fast gait speed demands maximal performance which may rely more of the ability of fast twitch muscles to generate power. Women tend to lose power in their lower limbs earlier than do men with the aging process [68]. The lack of gender difference in self-selected gait speed aligns with another study [67] while not aligning with other studies that detected slower gait speed in women over a longer distance (10-meters) [69]. Other studies assessing self-selected gait speed in people with PD could not be adequately compared to the current study due to significant differences in gait task characteristics and other methodological issues [70]. In sum, gender may affect gait speed, self-selected or fast, with women possibly walking slower during self-selected, due to average height differences, if data are not normalized and slower during fast gait, possibly because of the greater power involved.

The lack of gender differences (within adjusted models) in TUG-Cognitive and percent time change agree with a study that demonstrated no gender difference in gait speed interference (i.e., the relative change in dual-task walking speed in relation to walking speed only) [70].

Gender differences in cognitive function

The only gender differences in cognitive performance noted was worse performance of women on serial 3s subtraction, a test of mental status and working memory. A search revealed no other literature that also examined this test between genders in PD. No gender differences were detected for global cognition and executive function (measured by MoCA, CWIT, Tower of London), confirming some work [56, 57, 71, 72] while contradicting other work, including a study with 490 people with PD (M age: 67.9±9.3; 62.4% men) [32] and a study that analyzed longitudinal data from 12 PD cohorts [21], which found MoCA scores to be significantly lower in men. The latter study did not find a significant differences between genders in The Mini-Mental State Examination [21], a screening tool for cognitive impairment, considered to be less challenging than the MOCA and which was not assessed in the current study. The lack of gender differences in visuo-spatial function per the Corsi blocks, and the whole body spatial cognition test, BPST in the current study, agrees with previous reports that measured visuospatial ability in people with PD using mental rotation of objects [73], problem-solving tasks [74], and Constructional apraxia [75]. Other reports have suggested women had worse visuospatial function, including as measured by the Corsi block test in a study with 306 people with PD [75] and by the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Chinese Revision graphic arrangement test in a study with 311 Chinese people with PD (M age: 60.85±11.35, 55.31% males) [72]. Further, a study with 31 people with PD (51.61% females, M age: 60/62.8) found that women with PD performed worse (i.e., had greater veering) while walking straight with eyes closed, a task used to measure spatial navigation [76]. This measure of veering shares some similarity with the BPST [40] used here, as both involve physically spatially navigating. Interestingly the Cognitive Impairment subdomain of the subjective PDQ-39, a measure of HRQoL, indicated men reported their HRQoL was more impacted by cognitive impairment than women, which adds to the PD-specific measures that were differentiated by gender.

Gender differences in non-PD specific non-motor signs

A novel finding derived from this inquiry was that this sample of men reported greater depression (BDI-II; Models 1, 2 & 3) than women. Depression and anxiety were found by numerous studies to be more prevalent among women [7, 9, 25, 30, 56, 57, 71, 77, 78]. A study that analyzed baseline data from a multi-center study conducted by the National Institute of Health with 1121 (62.5%) males and 615 females (M age not reported) did not find a gender difference on the BDI-II measures, although the cohort was overall less depressed for both genders (BDI-II scores: women: 7.1±5.7; men: 6.8±5.5) [60] compared to the current study. Another study [9] used item 12 on the NMSS for assessing depression in 950 people with PD (M age: 56.43±10.78; 62.63% men) and did not detect gender differences. Other studies who found women were more depressed used different measures, e.g., the Brief Symptom Inventory-18 [25] and the Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale [56], which could explain discrepancies in trends. Again, the unique status of southern United States men, who are comparatively sicker than other regions, may lead to the greater depression scores in men.

In sum, this study appears to support the research that suggests a potential protective impact of estrogens, higher dopaminergic levels, and more intact presynaptic system in women, which others have suggested [16]. The impact of gender is especially important and pronounced in the PD-specific measures in this cohort.

Disentangling the literature with conflicting findings

Discrepancies between other studies’ findings and ours could be explained by between-studies differences in participants’ age, disease duration, race and ethnicity, and differences in cultural-related aspects. This study had 78.3% white participants, while other studies were mostly white [32] or included patients with more severe disease [71], whereas the current study H&Y range was by and large stages I-III, with a very few participants in stage IV. Further, the use of differing measures of different constructs could also affect results and their interpretations.

Findings at odds with the current study findings could originate from differences in scales used to measure symptoms, participants’ age, sample size, percentage of participants taking anti-parkinsonian medications and spectrum of PD stages, disease duration [56], and social-cultural factors differing between cultures (e.g., social interactions, community support, and self-conceptualizations of QoL). Caution should be taken when considering gender findings within current literature, given: first, substantial differences among studies in operational definitions, constructs, and outcome measures, especially those used for assessing motor functioning [9, 32], QoL [56], and cognition [27, 56], and even PD-related measures. Second, participants’ demographics, such as age [25, 56], ethnicity, and geography seem to impact gender differences in PD and thus should be thoroughly investigated and compared in future studies. Third, higher incidence of non-respondents among women, specifically relevant for epidemiological studies, has been suggested to affect research findings [79], and must be considered in the planning and design of future studies.

Limitations

This study has several limitations. More men than women participated in this study. Although like other studies [16, 80], the potential reasons for lesser participation include underdiagnosis of women, and women potentially having demographic/social situations that lead to their inability to participate in a study, e.g., no caregiver to support them through the study. Importantly, women were recruited fairly in this study from Movement Disorders clinics, from support groups, and from local organizations that support people with PD. The lack of long-term follow-up prevents us from gaining knowledge regarding progression of the disease related to gender. Further, the current sample derives from a specific geographic area (the southeastern US) and the findings cannot be generalized to the worldwide PD population or perhaps all regions of the US. The study lacks biomarker or neurophysiological measures, which would strengthen the rigor of the work. The neuropsychological battery lacks measures of language, and memory, which are crucial to examine for gender differences in a future study. The current study includes a single psychosocial measure of mood (BDI-II) while other neuropsychiatric symptoms (not considered in the current study) have been reported with gender differences in the related literature. In some variables, there were missing data, which may have influenced results in currently unknown ways. Future studies should also recruit from more non-white cohorts to be representative of all peoples. This study’s categorization of Black, White and Other was an imperfect solution to ethnic/racial characterization of these participants. We included this information because there may be factors related to race that if better understood could lead to better outcomes for people of all races. All limitations should be addressed in future studies.

Conclusion

A strength of this study is the wide battery of clinically relevant measures of motor and non-motor functions in PD that are used by many clinicians, giving the study more clinical relevance for neurorehabilitation approaches. In summary, this study adds to knowledge on the influence of gender on PD by suggesting that for individuals with mild-moderate PD, at least among this sample residing the Southeastern US, men are more impacted by burden of the disease on both QoL and self-reported motor and non-motor experiences of daily living as measured by PD-specific instruments. The current study also did not find statistically significant gender differences in functional balance (per the challenging FAB) and self-preferred and backward gait measures, as well as on several tests of executive function and visuospatial cognition. While this study does not provide the definitive story regarding functional measures and the genders, these effects were relatively robust. Except for the BDI-II, the differences between genders are most strongly noted in the PD-specific measures (MDS-UPDRS and PDQ-39). This trend was not expected, and the root causes of these differences are not fully understood. Possibly greater impact of PD exists for the men in this sample, as well as a more complex effect of gender and its various facets (e.g., physiological, psychological, and social) on the disease [26]. Another possibility is that because of the testing structure and the way test items are phrased or presented in PD-specific assessments, they are more likely to elicit differences between the genders. The current study provides behavioral knowledge which includes both self-reported as well as rater-reported measures that could promote a better understanding of the impact of PD on both genders. Further, this knowledge could promote the development of personalized gender-sensitive PD management policies and gender-sensitive assessment and rehabilitative protocols to be used in both research and clinical settings, including the development of gender-sensitive measures for assessing PD status, progression, and effectiveness of therapeutic interventions in this population. Implementing such knowledge in future research could assist overcoming some barriers toward PD optimal care for both men and women. The findings of this study should be further used, along with other literature, toward developing gender-targeted rehabilitation policies and guidelines. Such a process holds potential for fine tuning PD pharmaceutical and non-pharmaceutical therapies.

Supporting information

S1 Data

(CSV)

Acknowledgments

We thank the volunteers and participants for their time and effort devoted to this study. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by the United States Department of Veterans Affairs (Award: 5IK2RX000870-06) awarded to Madeleine Hackney, the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences of the National Institutes of Health (Award: UL1TR002378), the Emory University Udall Center (Award: P50NS071669-04) and the Parkinson Foundation (Award: A1-2016). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Muslimovic D, Post B, Speelman JD, Schmand B, de Haan RJ. Determinants of disability and quality of life in mild to moderate Parkinson disease. Neurology. 2008;70(23):2241–2247. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000313835.33830.80 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Dowding CH, Shenton CL, Salek SS. A review of the health-related quality of life and economic impact of Parkinson’s disease. Drugs Aging. 2006;23(9):693–721. doi: 10.2165/00002512-200623090-00001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Noyes K, Liu H, Li Y, Holloway R, Dick AW. Economic burden associated with Parkinson’s disease on elderly Medicare beneficiaries. Mov Disord. 2006;21(3):362–372. doi: 10.1002/mds.20727 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Nussbaum RL, Ellis CE. Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease. N Engl J Med. 2003;348(14):1356–1364. doi: 10.1056/NEJM2003ra020003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Marras C, Beck JC, Bower JH, et al. Prevalence of Parkinson’s disease across North America. NPJ Parkinsons Dis. 2018;4:21. doi: 10.1038/s41531-018-0058-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Picillo M, Nicoletti A, Fetoni V, Garavaglia B, Barone P, Pellecchia MT. The relevance of gender in Parkinson’s disease: a review. J Neurol. 2017;264(8):1583–1607. doi: 10.1007/s00415-016-8384-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Haaxma CA, Bloem BR, Borm GF, et al. Gender differences in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2007;78(8):819–824. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2006.103788 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Kompoliti K, Comella CL, Jaglin JA, Leurgans S, Raman R, Goetz CG. Menstrual-related changes in motoric function in women with Parkinson’s disease. Neurology. 2000;55(10):1572–1575. doi: 10.1212/wnl.55.10.1572 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Martinez-Martin P, Falup Pecurariu C, Odin P, et al. Gender-related differences in the burden of non-motor symptoms in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol. 2012;259(8):1639–1647. doi: 10.1007/s00415-011-6392-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Swerdlow RH, Parker WD, Currie LJ, et al. Gender ratio differences between Parkinson’s disease patients and their affected relatives. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2001;7(2):129–133. doi: 10.1016/s1353-8020(00)00029-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Subramanian I, Hinkle JT, Chaudhuri KR, Mari Z, Fernandez H, Pontone GM. Mind the gap: Inequalities in mental health care and lack of social support in Parkinson disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2021;93:97–102. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2021.11.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Liao YC, Wu YR, Tsao LI, Lin HR. The experiences of Taiwanese older individuals at different stages of Parkinson disease. J Neurosci Nurs. 2013;45(6):370–377. doi: 10.1097/JNN.0b013e3182a3cd5c [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Charkoudian N, Stachenfeld NS. Reproductive hormone influences on thermoregulation in women. Compr Physiol. 2014;Apr;4(2):793–804. doi: 10.1002/cphy.c130029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Li T, Xiao X, Zhang J, et al. Age and sex differences in vascular responsiveness in healthy and trauma patients: contribution of estrogen receptor-mediated Rho kinase and PKC pathways. Am J Physiol Heart Circ Physiol. 2014;Apr 15;306(8):H1105–15. doi: 10.1152/ajpheart.00645.2013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Freedman ND, Lacey JV Jr, Hollenbeck AR, Leitzmann MF, Schatzkin A, Abnet CC. The association of menstrual and reproductive factors with upper gastrointestinal tract cancers in the NIH-AARP cohort. Cancer. 2010;Mar 15;116(6):1572–81. doi: 10.1002/cncr.24880 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Meoni S, Macerollo A, Moro E. Sex differences in movement disorders. Nat Rev Neurol. 2020;Feb;16(2):84–96. doi: 10.1038/s41582-019-0294-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Gatto NM, Deapen D, Stoyanoff S, et al. Lifetime exposure to estrogens and Parkinson’s disease in California teachers. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2014;Nov;20(11):1149–56. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2014.08.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Kachan D, Tannenbaum SL, Olano HA, LeBlanc WG, McClure LA, Lee DJ. Geographical variation in health-related quality of life among older US adults, 1997–2010. Prev Chronic Dis. 2014;Jul 3;11:E110. doi: 10.5888/pcd11.140023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Zang E, Lynch SM, West J. Regional differences in the impact of diabetes on population health in the USA. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2021;75(1):56–61. doi: 10.1136/jech-2020-214267 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Abdo M, Wu X, Sharma A, et al. Regional Differences in Risk of Recurrent Falls Among Older U.S. Women and Men with HIV in the HIV Infection, Aging, and Immune Function Long-Term Observational Study. AIDS Res Hum Retroviruses. 2022;Feb 2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Iwaki H, Blauwendraat C, Leonard HL, et al. Differences in the Presentation and Progression of Parkinson’s Disease by Sex. Mov Disord. 2021;Jan;36(1):106–117. doi: 10.1002/mds.28312 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Lubomski M, Louise Rushworth R, Lee W, Bertram KL, Williams DR. Sex differences in Parkinson’s disease. J Clin Neurosci. 2014;21(9):1503–1506. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2013.12.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Moore O, Kreitler S, Ehrenfeld M, Giladi N. Quality of life and gender identity in Parkinson’s disease. J Neural Transm (Vienna). 2005;112(11):1511–1522. doi: 10.1007/s00702-005-0285-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Shulman LM. Gender differences in Parkinson’s disease. Gend Med. 2007;4(1):8–18. doi: 10.1016/s1550-8579(07)80003-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Abraham DS, Gruber-Baldini AL, Magder LS, et al. Sex differences in Parkinson’s disease presentation and progression. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2019;69:48–54. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2019.10.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Gottgens I, van Halteren AD, de Vries NM, et al. The Impact of Sex and Gender on the Multidisciplinary Management of Care for Persons With Parkinson’s Disease. Front Neurol. 2020;11:576121. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.576121 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Miller IN, Cronin-Golomb A. Gender differences in Parkinson’s disease: clinical characteristics and cognition. Mov Disord. 2010;25(16):2695–2703. doi: 10.1002/mds.23388 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Georgiev D, Hamberg K, Hariz M, Forsgren L, Hariz GM. Gender differences in Parkinson’s disease: A clinical perspective. Acta Neurol Scand. 2017;136(6):570–584. doi: 10.1111/ane.12796 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Institute of Medicine Board on Health Sciences Policy, Committee on Understanding the Biology of Sex and Gender Differences. In: Wizemann TM PM-L, ed. Exploring the biological contributions to human health: does sex matter? Institute of Medicine, Washington, DC: 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Baba Y, Putzke JD, Whaley NR, Wszolek ZK, Uitti RJ. Gender and the Parkinson’s disease phenotype. J Neurol. 2005;252(10):1201–1205. doi: 10.1007/s00415-005-0835-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Picillo M, Erro R, Amboni M, et al. Gender differences in non-motor symptoms in early Parkinson’s disease: a 2-years follow-up study on previously untreated patients. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2014;20(8):850–854. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2014.04.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Szewczyk-Krolikowski K, Tomlinson P, Nithi K, et al. The influence of age and gender on motor and non-motor features of early Parkinson’s disease: initial findings from the Oxford Parkinson Disease Center (OPDC) discovery cohort. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2014;20(1):99–105. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2013.09.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Racette BA, Rundle M, Parsian A, Perlmutter JS. Evaluation of a screening questionnaire for genetic studies of Parkinson’s disease. Am J Med Genet. 1999;88(5):539–543. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Kempster PA, Williams DR, Selikhova M, Holton J, Revesz T, Lees AJ. Patterns of levodopa response in Parkinson’s disease: a clinico-pathological study. Brain. 2007;130(Pt 8):2123–2128. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm142 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Dong X, Chang ES, Simon MA. Physical function assessment in a community-dwelling population of U.S. Chinese older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2014;69 Suppl 2:S31–38. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glu205 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Washburn RA, Smith KW, Jette AM, Janney CA. The Physical Activity Scale for the Elderly (PASE): development and evaluation. J Clin Epidemiol. 1993;46(2):153–162. doi: 10.1016/0895-4356(93)90053-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bedirian V, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53(4):695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Goetz CG, Tilley BC, Shaftman SR, et al. Movement Disorder Society-sponsored revision of the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS): scale presentation and clinimetric testing results. Mov Disord. 2008;23(15):2129–2170. doi: 10.1002/mds.22340 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Hagell P, Nilsson MH. The 39-Item Parkinson’s Disease Questionnaire (PDQ-39): Is it a Unidimensional Construct? Ther Adv Neurol Disord. 2009;2(4):205–214. doi: 10.1177/1756285609103726 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Hagell P, Nygren C. The 39 item Parkinson’s disease questionnaire (PDQ-39) revisited: implications for evidence based medicine. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2007;78(11):1191–1198. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2006.111161 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Herman T, Giladi N, Gruendlinger L, Hausdorff JM. Six weeks of intensive treadmill training improves gait and quality of life in patients with Parkinson’s disease: a pilot study. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2007;88(9):1154–1158. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2007.05.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Schlenstedt C, Brombacher S, Hartwigsen G, Weisser B, Moller B, Deuschl G. Comparing the Fullerton Advanced Balance Scale with the Mini-BESTest and Berg Balance Scale to assess postural control in patients with Parkinson disease. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2015;96(2):218–225. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2014.09.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Hackney ME, Earhart GM. Backward walking in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2009;24(2):218–223. doi: 10.1002/mds.22330 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Hofheinz M, Schusterschitz C. Dual task interference in estimating the risk of falls and measuring change: a comparative, psychometric study of four measurements. Clin Rehabil. 2010;24(9):831–842. doi: 10.1177/0269215510367993 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Battisto J, Echt KV, Wolf SL, Weiss P, Hackney ME. The Body Position Spatial Task, a Test of Whole-Body Spatial Cognition: Comparison Between Adults With and Without Parkinson Disease. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2018;32(11):961–975. doi: 10.1177/1545968318804419 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Delis DC, Kaplan E, Kramer JH. Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System: Technical Manual. In: Company HA, ed 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Long EC, Hill J, Luna B, Verhulst B, Clark DB. Disruptive behavior disorders and indicators of disinhibition in adolescents: The BRIEF-SR, anti-saccade task, and D-KEFS color-word interference test. J Adolesc. 2015;44:182–190. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2015.07.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Kessels RP, van den Berg E, Ruis C, Brands AM. The backward span of the Corsi Block-Tapping Task and its association with the WAIS-III Digit Span. Assessment. 2008;15(4):426–434. doi: 10.1177/1073191108315611 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Rainville C, Lepage E, Gauthier S, Kergoat MJ, Belleville S. Executive function deficits in persons with mild cognitive impairment: a study with a Tower of London task. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2012;34(3):306–324. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2011.639298 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1961;4:561–571. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Lally H, Hart AR, Bay AA, Kim C, Wolf SL, Hackney ME. Association Between Motor Subtype and Visuospatial and Executive Function in Mild-Moderate Parkinson Disease. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2020;101(9):1580–1589. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2020.05.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Prime M, McKay JL, Bay AA, et al. Differentiating Parkinson Disease Subtypes Using Clinical Balance Measures. J Neurol Phys Ther. 2020;44(1):34–41. doi: 10.1097/NPT.0000000000000297 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Drucker JH, Sathian K, Crosson B, et al. Internally Guided Lower Limb Movement Recruits Compensatory Cerebellar Activity in People With Parkinson’s Disease. Front Neurol. 2019;10:537. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.00537 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Bay AA, Hart AR, Michael Caudle W, Corcos DM, Hackney ME. The association between Parkinson’s disease symptom side-of-onset and performance on the MDS-UPDRS scale part IV: Motor complications. J Neurol Sci. 2019;396:262–265. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2018.12.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Stevens JA, Sogolow ED. Gender differences for non-fatal unintentional fall related injuries among older adults. Inj Prev. 2005;11:115–119. doi: 10.1136/ip.2004.005835 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Kovacs M, Makkos A, Aschermann Z, et al. Impact of Sex on the Nonmotor Symptoms and the Health-Related Quality of Life in Parkinson’s Disease. Parkinsons Dis. 2016;2016:7951840. doi: 10.1155/2016/7951840 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Solla P, Cannas A, Ibba FC, et al. Gender differences in motor and non-motor symptoms among Sardinian patients with Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Sci. 2012;323(1–2):33–39. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2012.07.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Beiske AG, Loge JH, Hjermstad MJ, Svensson E. Fatigue in Parkinson’s disease: prevalence and associated factors. Mov Disord. 2010;25(14):2456–2460. doi: 10.1002/mds.23372 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Jankovic J, Kapadia AS. Functional decline in Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol. 2001;58(10):1611–1615. doi: 10.1001/archneur.58.10.1611 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Augustine EF, Perez A, Dhall R, et al. Sex Differences in Clinical Features of Early, Treated Parkinson’s Disease. PLoS One. 2015;10(7):e0133002. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0133002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Lyons KE, Hubble JP, Troster AI, Pahwa R, Koller WC. Gender differences in Parkinson’s disease. Clin Neuropharmacol. 1998;21(2):118–121. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Hely MA, Morris JG, Reid WG, et al. Age at onset: the major determinant of outcome in Parkinson’s disease. Acta Neurol Scand. 1995;92(6):455–463. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.1995.tb00480.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Diamond SG, Markham CH, Hoehn MM, McDowell FH, Muenter MD. An examination of male-female differences in progression and mortality of Parkinson’s disease. Neurology. 1990;40(5):763–766. doi: 10.1212/wnl.40.5.763 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Louis ED, Tang MX, Cote L, Alfaro B, Mejia H, Marder K. Progression of parkinsonian signs in Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol. 1999;56(3):334–337. doi: 10.1001/archneur.56.3.334 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Brennan L, Siderowf A, Rubright JD, et al. The Penn Parkinson’s daily activities Questionnaire-15: psychometric properties of a brief assessment of cognitive instrumental activities of daily living in Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2016;25:21–26. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2016.02.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Pinkhasov RM, Shteynshlyuger A, Hakimian P, Lindsay GK, Samadi DB, Shabsigh R. Are men shortchanged on health? Perspective on life expectancy, morbidity, and mortality in men and women in the United States. Int J Clin Pract. 2010;Mar;64(4):465–74. doi: 10.1111/j.1742-1241.2009.02289.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Nemanich ST, Duncan RP, Dibble LE, et al. Predictors of gait speeds and the relationship of gait speeds to falls in men and women with Parkinson disease. Parkinsons Dis. 2013;2013:141720. doi: 10.1155/2013/141720 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Yanagawa N., Shimomitsu T., Kawanishi M. et al. Sex difference in age-related changes in knee extensor strength and power production during a 10-times-repeated sit-to-stand task in Japanese elderly. J Physiol Anthropol. 2015;34,40. doi: 10.1186/s40101-015-0072-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Paker N, Bugdayci D, Goksenoglu G, Demircioglu DT, Kesiktas N, Ince N. Gait speed and related factors in Parkinson’s disease. J Phys Ther Sci. 2015;27(12):3675–3679. doi: 10.1589/jpts.27.3675 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Rochester L, Nieuwboer A, Baker K, et al. Walking speed during single and dual tasks in Parkinson’s disease: which characteristics are important? Mov Disord. 2008;23(16):2312–2318. doi: 10.1002/mds.22219 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Riedel O, Klotsche J, Spottke A, et al. Cognitive impairment in 873 patients with idiopathic Parkinson’s disease. Results from the German Study on Epidemiology of Parkinson’s Disease with Dementia (GEPAD). J Neurol. 2008;255(2):255–264. doi: 10.1007/s00415-008-0720-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Gao L, Nie K, Tang H, et al. Sex differences in cognition among Chinese people with Parkinson’s disease. J Clin Neurosci. 2015;22(3):488–492. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2014.08.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Amick MM, Schendan HE, Ganis G, Cronin-Golomb A. Frontostriatal circuits are necessary for visuomotor transformation: mental rotation in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(3):339–349. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.06.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Cronin-Golomb A, Braun AE. Visuospatial dysfunction and problem solving in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychology. 1997;11(1):44–52. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.11.1.44 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Pasotti Chiara ZR , Sinforiani Elena, Minafra Brigida, Bertaina Ilaria, Pacchetti Claudio Cognitive function in Parkinson’s disease: The influence of gender. Basal Ganglia. 2013;3(2):131–135. [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Davidsdottir S, Wagenaar R, Young D, Cronin-Golomb A. Impact of optic flow perception and egocentric coordinates on veering in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2008;131(Pt 11):2882–2893. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn237 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Fernandez HH, Lapane KL, Ott BR, Friedman JH. Gender differences in the frequency and treatment of behavior problems in Parkinson’s disease. SAGE Study Group. Systematic Assessment and Geriatric drug use via Epidemiology. Mov Disord. 2000;15(3):490–496. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Leentjens AF, Dujardin K, Marsh L, Martinez-Martin P, Richard IH, Starkstein SE. Symptomatology and markers of anxiety disorders in Parkinson’s disease: a cross-sectional study. Mov Disord. 2011;26(3):484–492. doi: 10.1002/mds.23528 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Baldereschi M, Di Carlo A, Rocca WA, et al. Parkinson’s disease and parkinsonism in a longitudinal study: two-fold higher incidence in men. ILSA Working Group. Italian Longitudinal Study on Aging. Neurology. 2000;55(9):1358–1363. doi: 10.1212/wnl.55.9.1358 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Global, regional, and national burden of neurological disorders, 1990–2016: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet Neurol. 2019;18(5):459–480. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(18)30499-X [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Karsten Witt

4 Mar 2022

PONE-D-21-40790Gender Differences in Motor and Non-Motor Symptoms in Individuals with Mild-Moderate Parkinson’s DiseasePLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Hackney,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Apr 18 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Karsten Witt

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please amend your current ethics statement to address the following concerns:

a) Did participants provide their written or verbal informed consent to participate in this study?

b) If consent was verbal, please explain i) why written consent was not obtained, ii) how you documented participant consent, and iii) whether the ethics committees/IRB approved this consent procedure.

3. We note that the grant information you provided in the ‘Funding Information’ and ‘Financial Disclosure’ sections do not match.

When you resubmit, please ensure that you provide the correct grant numbers for the awards you received for your study in the ‘Funding Information’ section.

4. We note that you have indicated that data from this study are available upon request. PLOS only allows data to be available upon request if there are legal or ethical restrictions on sharing data publicly. For more information on unacceptable data access restrictions, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions.

In your revised cover letter, please address the following prompts:

a) If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially sensitive information, data are owned by a third-party organization, etc.) and who has imposed them (e.g., an ethics committee). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent.

b) If there are no restrictions, please upload the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate your study findings as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories.

We will update your Data Availability statement on your behalf to reflect the information you provide.

5. Your ethics statement should only appear in the Methods section of your manuscript. If your ethics statement is written in any section besides the Methods, please move it to the Methods section and delete it from any other section. Please ensure that your ethics statement is included in your manuscript, as the ethics statement entered into the online submission form will not be published alongside your manuscript.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: Partly

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The authors present a cross-sectional retrospective study of gender differences in motor and non motor functions in PD patients living in the Southeast of the USA.

The novelty and relevance of their findings need to be emphasized in the discussion, with particular regard to the variables of rehabilitative interest. The paper by Iwaki et al (Mov Disord 2021, 36-106-117), that analysed gender differences in the largest sample of PD patients so far, should be discussed.

Some information on the physiopathological basis of gender differences in PD would be also advisable.

Minor comments:

Introduction, page 1: Ref 14 seems to support two opposite statements, please clarify.

In the methods, please clarify which are the motor-cognitive variables used.

Reviewer #2: In this study, authors investigated clinical gender differences in a cohort of idiopathic Parkinson's disease (PD) from the southern United States. Clinical motor, cognitive and psychosocial gender differences emerged mainly favoring women as compared to men. The sample size is quite large and the clinical battery is wide including many PD-specific measures that authors described adequately in the methods. Many clinical gender differences here reported in part confirm existing literature and otherwise suggest the need of investigating sociocultural factors depending on geographic areas as important determinants of gender differences in PD. Although no biomarkers are investigated, the topic is relevant, clinically applicable, and supports personalized medicine.

The study is well conducted, thus there are only few comments with the intention to improve the paper.

1. In the introduction (page 4, lines 89-94), the authors report many factors which can explain clinical gender differences. Among them, estrogens and other reproductive factors should be mentioned with adequate references because of their important influences on dopaminergic systems and related motor and non-motor symptoms.

2. Throughout all the manuscript, the authors refer to “mild-moderate PD”. However, it is not clear how they defined it since in the methods (Participants section) they reported the usual clinical criteria for PD. Despite it is not explicit, we supposed the mild-moderate definition is because of the H&Y stage. Please clarify this point.

3. The psychosocial measures here only include depression scale, other neuropsychiatric symptoms (not considered here) have been reported with gender differences in the related literature. This is a missing point that needs to be addressed in the discussion.

4. The reason to choice the number of falls in the past year as a covariate in the analyses is not clear.

5. According to the first comment, when authors discuss the motor gender differences that benefit women (totally aligned with the literature), they should also discuss the possible underlying mechanisms, such as the estrogens-induced neuroprotection.

6. In the discussion, the authors mainly compared their results to the previous evidence providing pointless details. Whereas they lack a discussion of their results in terms of biological differences, social differences, etc., a benefit from avoiding detailed comparisons with other secondary aspects and favoring a deep explanation and critical discussion is mandatory.

7. The relevance of sociocultural factors depending on geographic areas for PD gender differences is a crucial point that deserves a better discussion. The authors merely report this point as a possible explanation of findings not totally aligned with previous reports in comparable cohorts from other countries. Please provide a detailed explanation on how sociocultural and geographic factors might influence gender differen

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2023 Jan 11;18(1):e0272952. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0272952.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


25 May 2022

Dear Dr. Witt,

We are grateful to have the opportunity to revise this manuscript for consideration of publication in PLOS One. We have carefully considered all the reviewers’ helpful critiques and have revised the manuscript accordingly. We strongly believe the manuscript is much improved as a result. We look forward to hearing from you soon.

Sincerely,

Madeleine Hackney, Amit Abraham and co-authors.

PONE-D-21-40790

Gender Differences in Motor and Non-Motor Symptoms in Individuals with Mild-Moderate Parkinson’s Disease

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Hackney,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Apr 18 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Karsten Witt

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

RESPONSE: We have ensured that our manuscript meets PLOS ONE’s style requirements.

2. Please amend your current ethics statement to address the following concerns:

a) Did participants provide their written or verbal informed consent to participate in this study?

RESPONSE: We have clarified that all participants gave written informed consent (Methods/first paragraph).

b) If consent was verbal, please explain i) why written consent was not obtained, ii) how you documented participant consent, and iii) whether the ethics committees/IRB approved this consent procedure.

RESPONSE: Consent was written and this point has been clarified in Methods.

3. We note that the grant information you provided in the ‘Funding Information’ and ‘Financial Disclosure’ sections do not match.

RESPONSE: We have clarified and corroborated the information provided in Funding Information and Financial Disclosure.

When you resubmit, please ensure that you provide the correct grant numbers for the awards you received for your study in the ‘Funding Information’ section.

RESPONSE: We have ensured that the correct grant numbers are provided.

4. We note that you have indicated that data from this study are available upon request. PLOS only allows data to be available upon request if there are legal or ethical restrictions on sharing data publicly. For more information on unacceptable data access restrictions, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions.

In your revised cover letter, please address the following prompts:

a) If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially sensitive information, data are owned by a third-party organization, etc.) and who has imposed them (e.g., an ethics committee). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent.

b) If there are no restrictions, please upload the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate your study findings as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories.

We will update your Data Availability statement on your behalf to reflect the information you provide.

RESPONSE: Thank you, we have uploaded the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate our study findings in a Supplemental Section. Therefore, all data are in the manuscript and supporting files

5. Your ethics statement should only appear in the Methods section of your manuscript. If your ethics statement is written in any section besides the Methods, please move it to the Methods section and delete it from any other section. Please ensure that your ethics statement is included in your manuscript, as the ethics statement entered into the online submission form will not be published alongside your manuscript.

RESPONSE: We have deleted the duplicative ethics statements.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: Partly

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The authors present a cross-sectional retrospective study of gender differences in motor and non motor functions in PD patients living in the Southeast of the USA.

The novelty and relevance of their findings need to be emphasized in the discussion, with particular regard to the variables of rehabilitative interest.

RESPONSE: Thank you. We have emphasized the novelty and relevance of the findings in the Discussion, (1st paragraph). The new text reads: “Novel findings of this study demonstrate for the first time that for patients from a large metro area in the southeastern US, the impact of gender is most evident in PD-specific measures of motor symptoms, QOL and activities of daily living. … Overall, the most striking differences were noted in PD-specific measures of motor and psychosocial function, while the differences between genders in non-PD specific measures were minor in this sample.”

The paper by Iwaki et al (Mov Disord 2021, 36-106-117), that analysed gender differences in the largest sample of PD patients so far, should be discussed.

RESPONSE: Thank you. We have now included and addressed the paper by Iwaki et al. We referred to this paper and its findings in the Introduction (3rd paragraph) and Discussion (2nd, 3rd, 6th paragraphs).

Some information on the physiopathological basis of gender differences in PD would be also advisable.

RESPONSE: We agree with the reviewer that presenting information on the physiopathological basis of gender differences in important. Please find a new section in the Introduction (3rd and 4th paragraphs) that discusses these factors in depth. Thank you for the suggestion!

Minor comments:

Introduction, page 1: Ref 14 seems to support two opposite statements, please clarify.

RESPONSE: Thank you. We have corrected the sentence. The new text reads: “some studies found certain aspects (activities of daily living (ADL), cognition and communication) of PD-related QOL to be rated lesser in men,13,14….”

In the methods, please clarify which are the motor-cognitive variables used.

RESPONSE: We have clarified the Motor Cognitive variables in Methods with a separate section.

Reviewer #2: In this study, authors investigated clinical gender differences in a cohort of idiopathic Parkinson's disease (PD) from the southern United States. Clinical motor, cognitive and psychosocial gender differences emerged mainly favoring women as compared to men. The sample size is quite large and the clinical battery is wide including many PD-specific measures that authors described adequately in the methods. Many clinical gender differences here reported in part confirm existing literature and otherwise suggest the need of investigating sociocultural factors depending on geographic areas as important determinants of gender differences in PD. Although no biomarkers are investigated, the topic is relevant, clinically applicable, and supports personalized medicine.

RESPONSE: Thank you.

The study is well conducted, thus there are only few comments with the intention to improve the paper.

RESPONSE: Thank you.

1. In the introduction (page 4, lines 89-94), the authors report many factors which can explain clinical gender differences. Among them, estrogens and other reproductive factors should be mentioned with adequate references because of their important influences on dopaminergic systems and related motor and non-motor symptoms.

RESPONSE: We agree and have included information (see response to Reviewer 1 above) about the pathophysiological differences between genders, related to estrogens and other reproductive factors. (See Introduction (3rd and 4th paragraphs). We touch upon the important influence these factors have on dopaminergic systems and related motor and non-motor symptoms.

2. Throughout all the manuscript, the authors refer to “mild-moderate PD”. However, it is not clear how they defined it since in the methods (Participants section) they reported the usual clinical criteria for PD. Despite it is not explicit, we supposed the mild-moderate definition is because of the H&Y stage. Please clarify this point.

RESPONSE: The reviewer is correct, Mild-moderate PD refers to stages I-III in Parkinson’s disease, and our participants were by and large representative of these stages. We have clarified this point in several locations where “mild-moderate” is iterated.

3. The psychosocial measures here only include depression scale, other neuropsychiatric symptoms (not considered here) have been reported with gender differences in the related literature. This is a missing point that needs to be addressed in the discussion.

RESPONSE: We agree with the reviewer. We have added a referral to this point in the Limitations section. Added text reads: “The current study includes a single psychosocial measure of mood (BDI-II) while other neuropsychiatric symptoms (not considered in the current study) have been reported with gender differences in the related literature.”

4. The reason to choice the number of falls in the past year as a covariate in the analyses is not clear.

RESPONSE: Thank you. We have added text for our reasons for including the falls in the past year as a covariate in the analyses in the Analysis section of Methods. The reason (in addition to that already given) is “For example, falls occur more often in older women than older men in the general older adult population.”

5. According to the first comment, when authors discuss the motor gender differences that benefit women (totally aligned with the literature), they should also discuss the possible underlying mechanisms, such as the estrogens-induced neuroprotection.

RESPONSE: We agree and have added two substantials paragraph of text devoted to the issue of gender differences in motor performance and symptoms that benefit women and the underlying mechanisms (e.g., estrogen-induced neuroprotection) in the Introduction (See response to Reviewer 1 above) and mention it again in Discussion..

6. In the discussion, the authors mainly compared their results to the previous evidence providing pointless details. Whereas they lack a discussion of their results in terms of biological differences, social differences, etc., a benefit from avoiding detailed comparisons with other secondary aspects and favoring a deep explanation and critical discussion is mandatory.

RESPONSE: We appreciate the critique and have endeavored to discuss more deeply the biological and social differences that likely contributed to the observed results. We have endeavored to reduce text that contains trivial details. We discuss sociocultural factors that may have affected our cohort in the Introduction (5th paragraph) and allude to it several times in the DISCUSSION.

7. The relevance of sociocultural factors depending on geographic areas for PD gender differences is a crucial point that deserves a better discussion. The authors merely report this point as a possible explanation of findings not totally aligned with previous reports in comparable cohorts from other countries. Please provide a detailed explanation on how sociocultural and geographic factors might influence gender differen

RESPONSE: Thank you. We agree with this important point and have included a paragraph addressing the sociocultural and geographic factors that might have influenced gender differences in this sample. The new text is found in Introduction (5th paragraph) to motivate the research and this is alluded to throughout in Discussion.

THANK YOU FOR THE HELPFUL CRITIQUES.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Plos One R1 Abraham et al Response.docx

Decision Letter 1

Karsten Witt

29 Jul 2022

Gender Differences in Motor and Non-Motor Symptoms in Individuals with Mild-Moderate Parkinson’s Disease

PONE-D-21-40790R1

Dear Dr. Hackney,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Karsten Witt

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: All my comments have been addressed. I have no further comments for the authors.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Maria Teresa Pellecchia

Reviewer #2: No

**********

Acceptance letter

Karsten Witt

7 Sep 2022

PONE-D-21-40790R1

Gender Differences in Motor and Non-Motor Symptoms in Individuals with Mild-Moderate Parkinson’s Disease

Dear Dr. Hackney:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Karsten Witt

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE


Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES