Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2025 Jan 1.
Published in final edited form as: Behav Med. 2022 Jul 14;50(1):26–36. doi: 10.1080/08964289.2022.2092441

Adding Insult to Injury: Everyday Discrimination Moderates Stressor-Related Negative Affect

Amandeep Kaur a, Susan T Charles a, Kristine M Molina a, David M Almeida b
PMCID: PMC9839892  NIHMSID: NIHMS1852665  PMID: 35834177

Abstract

Studies examining the effects of discrimination on emotional well-being have often overlooked: (a) differential effects of both everyday and lifetime discrimination, and (b) how both types of discrimination may exacerbate stressor-related affect – even when daily stressors are unrelated to discrimination. The current study examined the effects of daily stressors not attributed to discrimination (i.e., non-discrimination-related daily stressors) on daily negative and positive affect in the presence of either form of discrimination (everyday and lifetime). Participants who completed the second wave of the Survey of Midlife Development in the US (MIDUS-II) and the National Study of Daily Experiences (NSDE-II) answered questionnaires about everyday and lifetime discrimination. Later, they completed daily phone interviews across eight consecutive days, asking about the non-discrimination-related daily stressors and the positive and negative affect they had experienced that day. Multilevel model analyses revealed that everyday discrimination was associated with decreased daily positive affect, and lifetime discrimination was associated with increased daily negative affect. Moreover, higher frequency of everyday discrimination exacerbated the within-person effects of non-discriminatory daily stressors on negative affect. Results underscore the importance of considering both independent and synergistic effects of discrimination on daily emotional well-being.

Keywords: lifetime discrimination, everyday discrimination, daily stressor, daily affect

Introduction

Discrimination, broadly defined, is the differential treatment of members of socially defined groups across a variety of experiences.1 Patterns of discrimination reflect social hierarchies where those in lower social positions (e.g., racial/ethnic minorities, sexual minorities, aging populations, women) report more discrimination than those in privileged social positions.25 Discrimination exerts negative consequences for all affected groups, including Whites and people of color.6,7 A considerable amount of research has documented discrimination’s adverse effects on subjective and objective indices of health (for a review, see Lewis et al.).8

Paralleling the stress literature, discrimination often has been classified into two broad categories. The stress literature distinguishes between more seemingly minor and frequent stressors, referred to as daily hassles (such as a deadline at work or an argument with a friend) and stressors that are less frequent and more severe, referred to as major stressful life events (such as a home foreclosure or the death of a family member). Similarly, researchers have distinguished between acts of everyday discrimination (such as being forced to wait longer for services) as types of daily hassles, and major lifetime discrimination (such as being denied a job or being fired) to major life event stressors.1,8,9 Everyday discrimination refers to devaluation through chronic, daily exposure to socially patterned interpersonal unfair treatment (e.g., disrespect, insults).1013 Lifetime discrimination, in contrast, refers to specific albeit infrequent experiences of maltreatment that are the result of systematic exclusion of individuals from accessing resources including institutions of education (e.g., being denied higher education) employment (e.g., being unfairly fired), and legal systems (e.g., being threatened/harassed by law enforcement).12,14 Although less common, lifetime discrimination still occurs with some frequency; a national study found that 31% of adults reported at least one experience of discrimination during their lifetime, with unfair firing from work being the most common.3

Both everyday and lifetime discrimination are related to lower emotional well-being.11,15 Brondolo and colleagues use a social-cognitive framework to explain this process, positing that experiences of discrimination often prompt changes in schemas and threat-related appraisals.16 Over time, these cognitions can decrease moods, thereby increasing vulnerability to depressive episodes. Changes to cognitions may also shape how individuals attend to and respond to social interactions. For instance, viewing the world as unjust and uncontrollable can lead to hypervigilance, whereby individuals constantly scan their environment and prepare for the potential of harm.16 Social exclusion and devaluation also elicits a range of negative emotions, including sadness, hopelessness, and anger, but these reactions may be even stronger in people who have altered perceptions of the world as a result of their maltreatment.17 Everyday discrimination has been related to increased vigilance,18 and to perceptions of greater social exclusion/harassment and social tensions even after adjusting for personality traits.19 These thoughts and feelings are readily recalled even during interactions not attributed to discrimination.16

Associations between both lifetime and everyday discrimination and lower well-being, including greater psychological distress and more depressive symptoms,2,2022 and greater anger/hostility19,23 are well documented. However, everyday discrimination often has a more consistent and robust relationship with poor mental health when both forms of discrimination are examined together.2,2426 Less research has examined the differential effects of the two types of discrimination on positive affect. Different forms of discrimination (e.g., overt vs. subtle) have been associated with diminished positive affect,2730 but the effects are often less robust than those with negative affect and sometimes even not significant (see Douglass et al.).30

Discrimination may also exacerbate stressor-related affect even when daily stressors are not explicitly attributed to discrimination. The stress sensitization/kindling framework suggests that over time, constant exposure to stress, such as discrimination, makes a person more sensitive, or reactive, to subsequent stressors.3133 Consistent with this framework, discrimination is associated with chronic activation of neurobiological systems (e.g., hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and anterior cingulate cortex). These brain regions are critical for processing socioemotional information and particularly for perceiving social exclusion. Prolonged activation of these regions lowers the threshold of a stress response (for a review, see Brondolo et al.,16 Berger & Sarnyai,34 Berger et al.).35 For this reason, enduring greater lifetime and everyday discrimination may make encountering additional daily stressors, even when not attributed to discrimination, more upsetting.36

Studies have demonstrated that prior experiences of discrimination can worsen the effects of stress on emotional well-being. For example, among African American mothers, experiences of racial discrimination exacerbated the effects of stressor pileup (i.e., negative life events, including legal issues, illness/injuries, marital problems, as well as financial and job stressors) on psychological distress,37 and decreased relationship well-being (i.e., marital satisfaction, stability, warmth, and hostility) on psychological functioning (i.e., distress and anxiety symptoms).38 Further, in a racially diverse community sample, greater lifetime ethnic discrimination was associated with increased daily reports of anger in the presence of routine social interactions.19

The current study addresses gaps in the existing literature by: (a) examining how two types of discrimination (i.e., lifetime and everyday discrimination) are each uniquely associated with daily negative and positive affect; and (b) whether each type of discrimination exacerbates the association between daily non-discrimination-related stressors and affect. To our knowledge, no other study has examined how experiences of lifetime and everyday discrimination, together in one model, relate to daily affective states. We hypothesize that both types of discrimination are uniquely associated with increased daily negative affect and decreased positive affect. We further hypothesize that discrimination moderates the association between non-discrimination-related daily stressors and affect, consistent with models of stress sensitization. We expect, however, that everyday discrimination will have a more consistent and robust association with affective states than lifetime discrimination, consistent with prior literature.2426

Methods

Sample and procedures

Data were obtained from the second survey of Midlife Development in the United States Study (MIDUS-II; N = 4,963), a national longitudinal survey study assessing psychosocial processes and health across adulthood.39 The MIDUS included a subsample from Milwaukee County, Wisconsin (N = 592) to increase representation of African Americans.40 A subset of participants (N = 2,022) also completed the National Study of Daily Experiences (NSDE-II), a substudy of MIDUS-II which consists of daily telephone interviews across eight consecutive days where people are asked about the events of their day (e.g., stressful experiences, daily emotions).41 The analytic sample included those who participated in both MIDUS-II and NSDE-II. In addition, given the small number of Latinx respondents (only 60 people), we excluded them from the analyses, leaving only non-Hispanic White and non-Hispanic Black participants. A full description of the study is available at http://www.midus.wisc.edu/. Secondary data analysis for the present study was deemed exempt from the Institutional Review Board.

Comparing respondents who participated in the NSDE-II (N = 2,022; daily diaries = 16,176) and those who only participated in the MIDUS-II (N = 3,533) survey, significant sociodemographic differences between the two groups were observed for race/ethnicity, sex, marital status, age, education, work status, and neuroticism (all ps < .05). There were no differences in household income and chronic illness or in levels of lifetime and everyday discrimination.

Measures

Measures described below are from both the MIDUS-II (person-level variables) survey and the daily diary study of the NSDE-II (day-level variables).

Person-level variables

Lifetime discrimination.

The 11-item subscale of the Perceived Discrimination Scale12 assesses acute, major discriminatory events occurring over the life course across various life domains. Participants are asked how many times in their lives they have encountered discrimination for each of the 11 events. Sample items include: being discouraged by a teacher or advisor from seeking higher education; were not hired for a job; prevented from renting or buying a home in the neighborhood you wanted; and were hassled by the police. Across the 11 events, the average number of encounters was 5.33 (SD = 37.12), with a range between 0 (no incidents) to 1009 encounters. Given that results are highly skewed (e.g., one person reported 1009 instances when summed across the 11 items), we instead summed across the number of items that were endorsed at least once regardless of number of times, for a possible score ranging from 0 to 11. This scoring method for this scale has been validated and used in previous studies examining lifetime discrimination.12,14,42 Higher scores indicated greater counts of lifetime discrimination (α = .88).

Everyday discrimination.

The nine-item everyday discrimination subscale of the Perceived Discrimination Scale12 assesses how frequently on a day-to-day basis respondents experience instances of unfair treatment. Sample items include: being treated with less courtesy than other people; received poorer service than other people at restaurants or stores; people act if they are afraid of you; and you are called names or insulted. Response options range from 1 (often) to 4 (never). Responses were reverse coded and summed, with higher scores reflecting greater frequency of everyday discrimination (α = .92). A follow-up item asks participants about the main reason for these experiences (e.g., race, ethnicity, gender, age, sexual orientation).

Day-level variables

Daily stressors.

The Daily Inventory of Stressful Events43 was used to assess non-discrimination-related daily stressors during the daily diary phone interview over the course of eight days. Participants were asked whether they experienced any of the six stressors on a given day. Sample items include: “Did you have an argument or disagreement with anyone since (this time/we spoke) yesterday?” and “since (this time/we spoke) yesterday, did anything happen at work or school (other than what you already mentioned) that most people would consider stressful?” Participants answered yes or no for each stressor. One item asked participants whether they experienced any race, sex, or age discrimination in the last 24 hours. Fewer than one percent of the stressors were related to discrimination. This item was removed as our focus is on daily stressors not attributed to discrimination. Following procedures used in previous studies,44 a dichotomous variable was created to categorize participants as having had experienced any of the stressors (1) or not (0) on each of the eight days.

Negative and positive affect.

Negative and positive affect were assessed using scales developed for the NSDE study.45,46 During the daily diary phone interview, participants indicated on a 5-item Likert scale ranging from 0 (none of the time) to 4 (all of the time) how much of the time they experienced each of 27 emotions (14 negative and 13 positive) during the last 24-hour period. Sample negative emotion items included: nervous; so sad nothing could cheer you up; hopeless; lonely; ashamed; and upset. The within-person and between-person reliability for these items was .77 and .97, respectively.47 Sample positive items included: in good spirits; cheerful; calm and peaceful; full of life; like you belong; and confident. Items were averaged across negative and positive affect for each of the eight days. The within-person and between-person reliability for these items was .86 and .99, respectively.47 Given the moderate correlations between positive and negative affect, we adjust for the other affective outcome in each model (e.g., in the model where negative affect is the outcome, we adjust for positive affect).

Covariates

The present study adjusted for several sociodemographic variables known to be associated with affect, stressor occurrence, or discrimination, including age (years), sex (male/female), marital status (married; separated/divorced/widowed; never married), race/ethnicity (non-Hispanic White; non-Hispanic Black/African American), education (ranging from no school/some grade school to doctorate/professional degree), and current work status (employed/unemployed). The household income, including wages, pension, social security, and other governmental assistance, was converted into a Z-score. Because prior research documents positive associations between negative affect and both chronic illness48,49 and neuroticism,50,51 we also adjusted for these covariates. Participants indicated whether they had a chronic illness (yes/no). Neuroticism was measured using a scale developed for the MIDUS survey where respondents were asked how much the following adjectives described them on a scale of 1 (not at all) to 4 (a lot): moody, nervous, worrying, and calm (reverse-coded; α = .74).52 Items were summed, where higher scores reflect greater levels of neuroticism. Mean substitution was used for the following continuous covariates that were missing data: household income (n = 113), and neuroticism (n = 80). Mean substitution procedure performs similar to other methods for handling missing data (e.g., imputations) when less than 10% of data are missing on a variable.53 For the race/ethnicity variable, an additional category was created for the missing data (n = 132), which resulted in three categories (i.e., non-Hispanic White; non-Hispanic Black/African American; refused to state). This variable was used in the main analyses. We used the original variables (i.e., non-Hispanic White; non-Hispanic Black/African American) for the supplementary analyses examining interactions with race/ethnicity.

Analytic strategy

Intraclass correlations (ICC) were calculated to determine the extent to which daily negative and daily positive affect were clustered differentially within and between people. Slightly more than half (55%) of the variance in daily negative affect was between people (and 45% of the variance in daily negative affect was within people). Similarly, 76% of the variance in daily positive affect was between people (and 24% of the variance in daily positive affect was within people), revealing sufficient variance at the daily level for multi-level modeling.

To examine how the occurrence of a stressor is related to daily affect, and how this may be exacerbated by higher levels of discrimination, we calculated multi-level models (MLM) for continuous outcomes using Proc Mixed in SAS.54 An annotated equation for the final model is provided in the supplemental material. We used separate models to examine negative and positive affect. All continuous predictors were mean centered to reduce multicollinearity. All models adjusted for covariates, as described in the Measures section.

To test for the extent to which discrimination (lifetime and everyday) shaped the direction or strength of the association between occurrence of daily stressors and each affective outcome (i.e., moderation effect), we included interaction terms between each discrimination measure and the non-discrimination-related daily stress variable (i.e., occurrence of daily stressors × lifetime discrimination; occurrence of daily stressors × everyday discrimination).

Results

Descriptive statistics

Table 1 includes characteristics of sociodemographic and main study variables for the participants who comprise the analytic sample (N = 2,022; daily diaries = 16,176). Participants were primarily non-Hispanic-White (82%, n = 1,167). Participants, on average, experienced at least one stressor on 40% of the daily diary days. On average, participants reported experiencing positive affect on nearly all days and negative affect on over half (56%) of the daily diary days. More than half (57%) of the participants reported experiencing at least some everyday discrimination, and among the 71% of those who specified the reason for this unfair treatment, they mentioned such aspects as race, ethnicity, gender, age, religion, height/weight, appearance, disability, sexual orientation, and other. Among those reporting everyday discrimination, 37% reported one main reason whereas 34% reported two or more reasons. For participants who reported only one reason for unfair treatment, gender (23%) was most often endorsed. Everyday and lifetime discrimination were moderately correlated with one another (r = .46), and higher levels of each were related to greater negative affect and lower positive affect (see Table 2 for correlations among key study variables).

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics for covariates and key study variables (N = 2,022).

Total Sample Missing Data

N % %

Sex 0.0%
Female 1,157 57.2%
Male 865 42.8%
Race 6.5%
Non-Hispanic White 1,667 82.4%
Non-Hispanic Black 223 11.0%
Education 0.2%
Some high school or less 128 6.3%
High School graduate/GED 501 24.8%
Some college 472 23.3%
College graduate 538 26.6%
Graduate school and beyond 379 18.7%
Marital Status 0.1%
Married 1,387 68.6%
Widowed/Separated or Divorced 454 22.5%
Never Married 179 8.9%
Work Status 0.3%
Employed 1,012 49.7%
Unemployed 1,004 50.0%
Chronic Illness 3.3%
Yes 1,540 76.2%
No 415 20.5%

M (Range) SD

Age, years 56.25 (33–84) 12.20 0.0%
Household Income $67,434.63 (0–300K) $57,119.04 5.6%
Neuroticism 2.05 (1–4) 0.63 4.0%
Average Number of Stressors 0.53 (0–4) 0.47 0.0%
Daily Stressor* 38.8% 0.00 7.9%
Lifetime Discrimination 1.07 (0–11) 1.77 6.0%
Everyday Discrimination 12.92 (9–34) 4.70 4.3%
Negative Affect .19 (0–3.5) 0.32 7.9%
Positive Affect 2.74 (0–4) 0.79 7.9%

Note.

*

For daily stressor (i.e., daily stress exposure across 8 days), proportion and standard error are reported. Missing data for daily stressor, negative affect, and positive affect are on individual days across all participants. There is no missingness for average number of stressors as there were no participants missing data on daily stressor across all days.

Table 2.

Correlations among key study variables.

1 2 3 4 5

1. Everyday Discrimination --
2. Lifetime Discrimination .46 --
3. Daily Stressor (at least one stressor across eight days) .08 .08 --
4. Positive Affect −.19 −.09 −.21 --
5. Negative Affect .18 .15 .35 −.49 --

Note. Pearson product-moment correlation coefficients are reported for correlations between continuous variables. Point-Biserial correlation coefficients are reported for correlations between continuous and dichotomous variables. The reference group for daily stressor variables is 0 (i.e., did not endorse any stressor across the 8 days).

All coefficients are significant at p <.001.

Multilevel models: predicting daily negative and positive affect

Negative affect

Table 3 reports the results from MLM with negative affect as the outcome. The model with just the main effects revealed that only lifetime discrimination (γ = .008, SE = .003, 95% CI [.002, .014], p < .01), but not everyday discrimination was associated with greater daily negative affect. In addition, non-discrimination-related daily stressor occurrence (γ = .137, SE = .005, 95% CI [.127, .147], p < .001) was also associated with greater daily negative affect.

Table 3.

Parameter estimates for negative affect and interactions between occurrence of daily stressors and discrimination types (N = 1878)

95% C.I.
Variables Estimate (SE) LB UB

Fixed Effects
Intercept .133 (.027)*** .080 .186
Main Effects Everyday Discrimination −.003 (.001)* −.005 .000
Lifetime Discrimination .007 (.003)* .000 .014
Daily Stressor (At least one stressor on each of eight days) .137 (.005)*** .127 .146
Interactions Daily Stressor* Everyday Discrimination .006 (.001)*** .004 .008
Daily Stressor* Lifetime Discrimination .003 (.003) −.003 .009
Covariates Average Number of Stressors .112 (.011)*** .090 .134
Sex (Male) −.004 (.010) −.230 .015
Race (Non-Hispanic White) −.011 (.018) −.047 .025
Race (Non-Hispanic Black) .049 (.023)* .004 .094
Age .000 (.000) −.001 −.001
Education −.009 (.002)*** −.013 −.005
Household Income −.006 (.005) −.016 .004
Chronic Illness (Yes) −.012 (.012) −.034 .011
Work Status (Employed) −.026 (.010)* −.460 −.005
Married −.26 (.017) −.060 .007
Separated/Divorced/Widowed −.002 (.019) −.038 .035
Neuroticism .047 (.008)*** .032 .063
Positive Affect −.181 (.004)*** −.188 −.173
Random Effects σ z

Intercept .028 (.001)*** 20.73
Daily Stressor (At least one stressor on each of eight days) .007 (.001)*** 10.06
Residual .037 (.001)*** 71.26

Note. All estimates are standardized. SE = Standard Error. C.I. = Confidence Interval. LB = Lower Bound. UB = Upper Bound.

*

p < .05

**

p < .01

***

p < .001

In the model testing whether discrimination moderated the effects of daily stressors on negative affect, we found a significant result only for the interaction between daily stressor and everyday discrimination (γ = .006, SE = .001, 95% CI [.004, .009], p < .001). To unpack this interaction, we plotted the simple slopes at low (everyday discrimination = 9, or no discrimination), moderate (everyday discrimination greater than 9 but less than 16), and high (everyday discrimination greater than 16) values of the moderator. All simple slopes were significantly different than 0 (p <.001) (see Figure 1 for interaction plot).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Daily negative affect as a function of everyday discrimination and occurrence of daily stressors.

Positive affect

Table 4 reports results from MLM with positive affect as the outcome. The model with only main effects revealed a significant negative association with everyday discrimination (γ = −.012, SE = .003, 95% CI [−.018, −.005], p < .01), but not lifetime discrimination. Daily stressor occurrence (γ = −.034, SE = .008, 95% CI [−.051, −.018], p < .001) was also negatively associated with daily positive affect. Interactions between occurrence of a daily stressor and discrimination (of either type) were not significant.

Table 4.

Parameter estimates for positive affect and interactions between occurrence of daily stressors and discrimination types (N = 1878)

95% C.I.
Variables Estimate (SE) LB UB

Fixed Effects
Intercept 2.930 (.080)*** 2.773 3.088
Main Effects Everyday Discrimination −.013 (.004)*** −.020 −.006
Lifetime Discrimination .019 (.010)* .001 .038
Daily Stressor (At least one stressor on each of eight days) .034 (.008)*** −.051 −.018
Interactions Daily Stressor* Everyday Discrimination .003 (.002) −.001 .007
Daily Stressor* Lifetime Discrimination −.006 (.005) −.016 .004
Covariates Average Number of Stressors −.161 (.033)*** −.225 −.097
Sex (Male) −.089 (.029)** −.145 −.033
Race (Non-Hispanic White) −.045 (.055) −.154 .064
Race (Non-Hispanic Black) −.016 (.069) −.151 .119
Age .005 (.001)*** .002 .008
Education −.018 (.006)** −.030 −.007
Household Income .012 (.016) −.019 .042
Chronic Illness (Yes) −.120 (.035)*** −.188 −.053
Work Status (Employed) −.013 (.031) −.048 .074
Married .078 (.051) −.023 .178
Separated/Divorced/Widowed .048 (.056) −.062 .157
Neuroticism .259 (.023)*** −.305 −.214
Negative Affect −.689 (.015)*** −.718 −.659
Random Effects σ z

Intercept .320 (.011)*** 27.84
Daily Stressor (At least one stressor on each of eight days) .010 (.002)*** 5.01
Residual .126 (.002)*** 72.27

Note. All estimates are standardized. SE = Standard Error. C.I. = Confidence Interval. LB = Lower Bound. UB = Upper Bound.

*

p < .05

**

p < .01

***

p < .001

Supplementary analyses

We examined whether lifetime discrimination was significantly associated with daily positive affect in our model with just the main effects if we removed the effects of everyday discrimination. However, lifetime discrimination was still not significantly related to daily positive affect (p > .68).

Similarly, our aforementioned models found that occurrence of daily stressors by lifetime discrimination interaction was not significantly associated with daily negative affect, so we examined whether this interaction was significant if we removed the occurrence of daily stressors by everyday discrimination interaction. Doing so yielded a significant daily stressor by lifetime discrimination interaction (γ = .010, SE = .003, 95% CI [.004, .015], p < .01) in the model with daily negative affect as the outcome.

Moreover, given that prior research finds the effects of discrimination has differential impacts by race/ethnicity,55,56 we tested whether race/ethnicity moderated the effects found in our main analyses. We conducted four 3-way interactions: one for each type of discrimination (occurrence of daily stressors × everyday discrimination × race/ethnicity; occurrence of daily stressors × lifetime discrimination × race/ethnicity) with each affective outcome (daily negative; positive affect). However, none of these interactions were significant (ps > .25).

Discussion

The present study examined how discrimination was related to positive and negative affect on a given day and how discrimination exacerbated these within-person daily stress processes among a sample of U.S. adults. In zero-order correlations, both types of discrimination were significantly associated with greater negative affect and lower positive affect. In the full models where both types of discrimination were entered together, results only partially supported our hypotheses. Lifetime discrimination was associated with higher daily negative affect but had no unique association with positive affect. Everyday discrimination was associated with lower positive affect. Everyday discrimination also moderated stressor-related affect.

Consistent with our second hypothesis, we found a synergistic effect for everyday discrimination and daily stress on negative affect. Specifically, everyday discrimination strengthened the association between occurrence of a daily stressor and negative affect. The social cognitive framework of racism offers support for our findings that greater frequency of everyday discrimination exacerbates the effects of non-discrimination based daily stressors on negative affect. This framework posits that experiences of discrimination evoke feeling of worthlessness and hopelessness, a diminished sense of purpose, and less self-acceptance. Our finding is consistent with this model and the stress-sensitivity hypothesis, whereby constant experiences of discrimination in daily life may sensitize people to react more strongly to stressors, even when they are not explicitly related to discrimination.

In addition, this finding is similar to prior empirical studies that demonstrate that discrimination moderates the association between life stressors (e.g., financial, legal, and marital functioning) and psychological well-being.37,38 Our findings are also in line with stress sensitization theory.32 This may be especially the case in the context of everyday discrimination, given its chronic and ambiguous nature. Prior research suggests that more chronic and ambiguous forms of discrimination have greater cognitive costs that can drain regulatory processes and increase risk for experiencing negative affect.16,57 Thus, in the context of routine experiences of unfair treatment and daily stressors, individuals may not have sufficient cognitive resources to cope, thereby resulting in increased daily negative emotions.

Lifetime discrimination did not moderate stressor-related negative affect. Our study did not test underlying mechanisms to explain this pattern of results, but perhaps lifetime discrimination exerts its effect on daily life through its overlap with everyday discrimination. For example, lifetime discrimination is defined by denial of resources and opportunities (e.g., loans, education, promotions), and lower socioeconomic status and financial worries are often related to greater distress.36 Lifetime discrimination may result in daily slights (e.g., being treated rudely by others due to a lack money or education), captured by the daily discrimination measure. Moreover, lifetime discrimination did moderate stressor-related negative affect when we removed the effects of everyday discrimination, which strengthens the hypothesis that the effects of lifetime discrimination may exert themselves through these everyday injustices.

We did not find a main effect of lifetime discrimination on positive affect, even when we removed the main effect of everyday discrimination. Using a system blame perspective, perhaps individuals are more likely to attribute lifetime discrimination to institutional structures and barriers, which can protect from internalizing and ascribing experiences of lifetime discrimination to the self.58

Our findings must be interpreted considering some limitations. First, all measures were self-reported, and thus are susceptible to recall and social desirability bias. Future studies should assess daily discrimination in “real time” using ecological momentary assessment methods (EMA) and include objective measures of discrimination and daily stressors to minimize reporting bias and increase ecological validity.59,60 Second, we did not examine the source of daily stressors in the study, and individuals may be more sensitive to the effects of discrimination when they encounter specific types of daily stressors. For example, discrimination exacerbated the effects of lower relationship well-being on psychological functioning, but not other stressors (e.g., stressful life events such as financial challenges) among rural African American women38 (see also, Wardecker et al.).61 Further, the daily stressors in our study may be indirectly related to discrimination. For instance, prior research documents that the effects of discrimination experiences can spill over to other household members62,63 (e.g., parental experiences of workplace discrimination can spill over to parent-child relationships).64 Structural racism can also expose racialized groups to harms, such as neighborhood violence and harsh parenting (as reviewed in Brondolo et al.).65 Although not seemingly racial, gendered, aged, or other, daily stressors could be the result of experiencing unfair treatment in institutional and interpersonal contexts. For instance, someone stating that they were in a disagreement/argument could be referring to a disagreement with a spouse about taking out the trash, but they could also be referring to an argument at a store/restaurant due to unfair treatment. Daily stressors assessed in the study include those related to actual or potential arguments with others, stressful events in the workplace or home, discrimination, stressors within social networks, and other, unspecified stressors. Participants reported that very few (less than one percent) of daily stressors were due to race, sex, or age discrimination. However, 89% of all stressors reported occurred across the following domains: interpersonal, work, home, and social networks. These stressors may have arisen as a consequence of racism.

Finally, although data are drawn from a larger national sample, the analytic sample was not nationally representative, and participants were predominately White. Most of the African American participants were from Milwaukee County, Wisconsin, a relatively impoverished and segregated region.66 Experiences of African Americans residing in Milwaukee County may not be representative of African Americans residing in less segregated US regions (e.g., Laredo, Texas)67,68 or to other racial/ethnic groups. For instance, we excluded Latinx individuals given their low representation in the sample (i.e., about three percent of the sample). In addition, even though our supplementary analyses suggest that racial/ethnic groups appear similarly impacted by discrimination, it could be that insufficient power prevented us from detecting differences in effects across racial/ethnic groups. Thus, future studies could examine this with more racially/ethnically heterogeneous samples from a range of different communities.

Conclusions

The current study reveals that lifetime and everyday discrimination are differentially associated with emotional well-being, and also that everyday discrimination exacerbates negative affect on stressor days. Based on our findings, population-level solutions that increase awareness about and redress discrimination, and interventions that help individuals cope with this form of psychosocial stressor may prove particularly effective at reducing emotional stress-reactivity and optimizing stress resilience. For example, Kwate69 found that a public health intervention that exposed community residents to racism countermarketing outdoor advertisements was associated with decreased psychological distress 3-months post intervention. At the individual level, interventions shown to reduce emotional stress-reactivity and promote positive emotions (e.g., mindfulness meditation training) may offer viable solutions in the context of everyday discrimination and daily stressors.70,71 Further work is warranted to understand how, for whom, and under which conditions discrimination impacts health. Elucidating these processes is essential for advancing our understanding of the stress process and for developing targeted interventions across contexts in which discrimination and stressors occur.

Supplementary Material

Supplemental Materials

References

  • 1.Bailey ZD, Williams DR. Stress, Health, and Disparities. Neurobiology of Brain Disorders. Published online 2015:765–779. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Ayalon L, Gum AM. The relationships between major lifetime discrimination, everyday discrimination, and mental health in three racial and ethnic groups of older adults. Aging & mental health. 2011;15(5):587–594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Luo Y, Xu J, Granberg E, Wentworth WM. A longitudinal study of social status, perceived discrimination, and physical and emotional health among older adults. Research on Aging. 2012;34(3):275–301. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Beatty Moody DL, Waldstein SR, Leibel DK, et al. Race and other sociodemographic categories are differentially linked to multiple dimensions of interpersonal-level discrimination: Implications for intersectional, health research. PloS one. 2021;16(5):e0251174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Ayhan CHB, Bilgin H, Uluman OT, Sukut O, Yilmaz S, Buzlu S. A systematic review of the discrimination against sexual and gender minority in health care settings. International Journal of Health Services. 2020;50(1):44–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Versey HS, Curtin N. The differential impact of discrimination on health among Black and White women. Social Science Research. 2016;57:99–115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Brondolo E, Hausmann LR, Jhalani J, et al. Dimensions of perceived racism and self-reported health: examination of racial/ethnic differences and potential mediators. Annals of behavioral medicine. 2011;42(1):14–28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Lewis TT, Cogburn CD, Williams DR. Self-Reported Experiences of Discrimination and Health: Scientific Advances, Ongoing Controversies, and Emerging Issues. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2015;11(1):407–440. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112728 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Williams DR, Neighbors HW, Jackson JS. Racial/ethnic discrimination and health: Findings from community studies. American Journal of Public Health. 2003;93(2):200–208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Essed P Understanding Everyday Racism: An Interdisciplinary Theory. Vol 2. Sage; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Jones CP. Levels of racism: a theoretic framework and a gardener’s tale. American journal of public health. 2000;90(8):1212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Williams DR, Yu Y, Jackson JS, Anderson NB. Racial differences in physical and mental health: Socio-economic status, stress and discrimination. Journal of health psychology. 1997;2(3):335–351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Bourabain D, Verhaeghe PP. The conceptualization of everyday racism in research on the mental and physical health of ethnic and racial groups: a systematic review. Journal of Racial and Ethnic Health Disparities. 2021;8(3):648–660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Kessler RC, Mickelson KD, Williams DR. The prevalence, distribution, and mental health correlates of perceived discrimination in the United States. Journal of health and social behavior. Published online 1999:208–230. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Williams DR, Lawrence JA, Davis BA, Vu C. Understanding how discrimination can affect health. Health Serv Res. 2019;54(S2):1374–1388. doi: 10.1111/1475-6773.13222 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Brondolo E, Blair IV, Kaur A. Biopsychosocial Mechanisms Linking Discrimination to Health: A Focus on Social Cognition. Vol 1. (Major B, Dovidio JF, Link BG, eds.). Oxford University Press; 2017. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190243470.013.8 [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Leary MR. Reactions to discrimination, stigmatization, ostracism, and other forms of interpersonal rejection: a multimotive model. Psychological Review. 2009;116(2):365–383. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Pichardo CM, Molina KM, Rosas CE, Uriostegui M, Sanchez-Johnsen L. Racial discrimination and depressive symptoms among Latina/o college students: the role of Racism-Related vigilance and sleep. Race and Social Problems. 2021;13(2):86–101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Broudy R, Brondolo E, Coakley V, et al. Perceived ethnic discrimination in relation to daily moods and negative social interactions. Journal of behavioral medicine. 2007;30(1):31–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Yoon E, Coburn C, Spence SA. Perceived discrimination and mental health among older African Americans: The role of psychological well-being. Aging & mental health. 2019;23(4):461–469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Banks KH, Kohn-Wood LP, Spencer M. An examination of the African American experience of everyday discrimination and symptoms of psychological distress. Community mental health journal. 2006;42(6):555–570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Ong AD, Fuller-Rowell T, Burrow AL. Racial discrimination and the stress process. Journal of personality and social psychology. 2009;96(6):1259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Brondolo E, Kelly KP, Coakley V, et al. The perceived ethnic discrimination questionnaire: development and preliminary validation of a community version 1. Journal of applied social psychology. 2005;35(2):335–365. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Lee H, Turney K. Investigating the relationship between perceived discrimination, social status, and mental health. Society and mental health. 2012;2(1):1–20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Bennett IM, Culhane JF, Webb DA, et al. Perceived discrimination and depressive symptoms, smoking, and recent alcohol use in pregnancy. Birth. 2010;37(2):90–97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Pascoe EA, Richman LS. Perceived Discrimination and Health: A Meta-Analytic Review. Psychol Bull. 2009;135(4):531–554. doi: 10.1037/a0016059 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Noh S, Kaspar V, Wickrama KA. Overt and subtle racial discrimination and mental health: Preliminary findings for Korean immigrants. American journal of public health. 2007;97(7):1269–1274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Ong AD, Burrow AL. Affective reactivity to daily racial discrimination as a prospective predictor of depressive symptoms in African American graduate and postgraduate students. Development and psychopathology. 2018;30(5):1649–1659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Ong AD, Edwards LM. Positive affect and adjustment to perceived racism. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 2008;27(2):105–126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Douglass RP, Conlin SE, Duffy RD, Allan BA. Examining moderators of discrimination and subjective well-being among LGB individuals. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2017;64(1):1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Harkness KL, Hayden EP, Lopez-Duran NL. Stress sensitivity and stress sensitization in psychopathology: an introduction to the special section. Journal of abnormal psychology. 2015;124(1):1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Monroe SM, Harkness KL. Life stress, the” kindling” hypothesis, and the recurrence of depression: considerations from a life stress perspective. Psychological review. 2005;112(2):417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Post RM. Transduction of psychosocial stress into the neurobiology of recurrent affective disorder. The American journal of psychiatry. Published online 1992. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Berger M, Sarnyai Z. “More than skin deep”: stress neurobiology and mental health consequences of racial discrimination. Stress. 2015;18(1):1–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Berger M, Juster RP, Sarnyai Z. Mental health consequences of stress and trauma: allostatic load markers for practice and policy with a focus on Indigenous health. Australasian Psychiatry. 2015;23(6):644–649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Williams DR, Mohammed SA. Discrimination and racial disparities in health: evidence and needed research. Journal of behavioral medicine. 2009;32(1):20–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Murry VM, Brown PA, Brody GH, Cutrona CE, Simons RL. Racial Discrimination as a Moderator of the Links Among Stress, Maternal Psychological Functioning, and Family Relationships. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2001;63(4):915–926. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2001.00915.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Murry VM, Harrell AW, Brody GH, et al. Long-Term Effects of Stressors on Relationship Well-Being and Parenting Among Rural African American Women. Fam Relat. 2008;57(2):117–127. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3729.2008.00488.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Ryff C, Almeida DM, Ayanian J, et al. Midlife in the United States (MIDUS 2), 2004–2006: Version 7. Published online 2007. doi: 10.3886/ICPSR04652.V7 [DOI]
  • 40.Ryff C, Almeida D, Ayanian J, et al. Midlife in the United States (MIDUS 2): Milwaukee African American Sample, 2005–2006: Version 5. Published online 2008. doi: 10.3886/ICPSR22840.V5 [DOI]
  • 41.Ryff CD, Almeida DM. Midlife in the United States (MIDUS 2): Daily Stress Project, 2004–2009: Version 2. Published online 2009. doi: 10.3886/ICPSR26841.V2 [DOI]
  • 42.Brown TT, Partanen J, Chuong L, Villaverde V, Chantal Griffin A, Mendelson A. Discrimination hurts: The effect of discrimination on the development of chronic pain. Soc Sci Med. 2018;204:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.03.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Almeida DM, Wethington E, Kessler RC. The daily inventory of stressful events: An interview-based approach for measuring daily stressors. Assessment. 2002;9(1):41–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Almeida DM, Neupert SD, Banks SR, Serido J. Do daily stress processes account for socioeconomic health disparities? The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2005;60(Special_Issue_2):S34–S39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Kessler RC, Andrews G, Colpe LJ, et al. Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in non-specific psychological distress. Psychological medicine. 2002;32(6):959–976. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Mroczek DK, Kolarz CM. The effect of age on positive and negative affect: a developmental perspective on happiness. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1998;75(5):1333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Scott SB, Sliwinski MJ, Zawadzki M, et al. A coordinated analysis of variance in affect in daily life. Assessment. 2020;27(8):1683–1698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Kunzmann U, Schilling O, Wrosch C, et al. Negative emotions and chronic physical illness: A lifespan developmental perspective. Health Psychology. 2019;38(11):949. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Charles ST, Almeida DM. Daily reports of symptoms and negative affect: Not all symptoms are the same. Psychology and Health. 2006;21(1):1–17. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Rafienia P, Azadfallah P, Fathi-Ashtiani A, Rasoulzadeh-Tabatabaiei K. The role of extraversion, neuroticism and positive and negative mood in emotional information processing. Personality and Individual Differences. 2008;44(2):392–402. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Charles ST, Reynolds CA, Gatz M. Age-related differences and change in positive and negative affect over 23 years. Journal of personality and social psychology. 2001;80(1):136. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Zimprich D, Allemand M, Lachman ME. Factorial structure and age-related psychometrics of the MIDUS personality adjective items across the life span. Psychological assessment. 2012;24(1):173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Parent MC. Handling item-level missing data: Simpler is just as good. The Counseling Psychologist. 2013;41(4):568–600. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Littell RC, Milliken GA, Stroup WW, Wolfinger RD. SAS system for mixed models. Cary, NC: SAS Institute. Published online 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Barnes LL, de Leon CFM, Lewis TT, Bienias JL, Wilson RS, Evans DA. Perceived Discrimination and Mortality in a Population-Based Study of Older Adults. Am J Public Health. 2008;98(7):1241–1247. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2007.114397 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Lewis TT, Barnes LL, Bienias JL, Lackland DT, Evans DA, Mendes de Leon CF. Perceived discrimination and blood pressure in older African American and white adults. Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biomedical Sciences and Medical Sciences. 2009;64(9):1002–1008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Murphy MC, Richeson JA, Shelton JN, Rheinschmidt ML, Bergsieker HB. Cognitive costs of contemporary prejudice. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. 2013;16(5):560–571. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Crocker J, Major B. Social stigma and self-esteem: The self-protective properties of stigma. Psychological review. 1989;96(4):608. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Lau JT, Thomas J, Liu JL. Mobile phone and interactive computer interviewing to measure HIV-related risk behaviours: the impacts of data collection methods on research results. Aids. 2000;14(9):1277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Stone AA, Shiffman S. Capturing momentary, self-report data: A proposal for reporting guidelines. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2002;24(3):236–243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Wardecker BM, Surachman A, Matsick JL, Almeida DM. Daily Stressor Exposure and Daily Well-Being Among Sexual Minority and Heterosexual Adults in the United States: Results from the National Study of Daily Experiences (NSDE). Annals of Behavioral Medicine. Published online September 18, 2021:kaab062. doi: 10.1093/abm/kaab062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Cooper AN, Tao C, Totenhagen CJ, Randall AK, Holley SR. Daily stress spillover and crossover: Moderating effects of difficulties in emotion regulation in same-sex couples. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2020;37(4):1245–1267. doi: 10.1177/0265407519891777 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Molina KM, Little TV, Rosal MC. Everyday Discrimination, Family Context, and Psychological Distress Among Latino Adults in the United States. Journal of Community Psychology. 2016;44(2):145–165. doi: 10.1002/jcop.21747 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Huynh VW, Rahal D, Mercado E, et al. Discrimination and health: A dyadic approach. J Health Psychol Published online June 20, 2019:135910531985717. doi: 10.1177/1359105319857171 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Brondolo E, Byer K, Gianaros PJ, et al. Stress and health disparities: contexts, mechanisms, and interventions among racial/ethnic minority and low socioeconomic status populations. American Psychological Association (APA) Working Group Report. Published online 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Loyd JM, Bonds A. Where do Black lives matter? Race, stigma, and place in Milwaukee, Wisconsin. The Sociological Review. 2018;66(4):898–918. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Williams DR, Collins C. Racial Residential Segregation: A Fundamental Cause of Racial Disparities in Health. Public Health Reports. 2001;116:13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Logan JR, Stults B. The Persistence of Segregation in the Metropolis: New Findings from the 2020 Census. Diversity and Disparities Project Published online 2021:26. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Kwate NOA. “Racism Still Exists”: A Public Health Intervention Using Racism “Countermarketing” Outdoor Advertising in a Black Neighborhood. J Urban Health. 2014;91(5):851–872. doi: 10.1007/s11524-014-9873-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Lindsay EK, Chin B, Greco CM, et al. How mindfulness training promotes positive emotions: Dismantling acceptance skills training in two randomized controlled trials. Journal of personality and social psychology. 2018;115(6):944. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Shallcross AJ, Spruill TM. The protective role of mindfulness in the relationship between perceived discrimination and depression. Mindfulness. 2018;9(4):1100–1109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplemental Materials

RESOURCES