Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2024 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2022 Dec 19;78:102665. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2022.102665

Neural ensembles in navigation: from single cells to population codes

Emily A Aery Jones 1,*, Lisa M Giocomo 1,*
PMCID: PMC9845194  NIHMSID: NIHMS1853119  PMID: 36542882

Abstract

The brain can represent behaviorally relevant information through the firing of individual neurons as well as the coordinated firing of ensembles of neurons. Neurons in the hippocampus and associated cortical regions participate in a variety of types of ensembles to support navigation. These ensemble types include single cell codes, population codes, time-compressed sequences, behavioral sequences, and engrams. We present the physiological basis and behavioral relevance of ensemble firing. We discuss how these traditional definitions of ensembles can constrain or expand potential analyses due to the underlying assumptions and abstractions made. We highlight how coding can change at the ensemble level while underlying single cell codes remain intact. Finally, we present how ensemble definitions could be broadened to better understand the full complexity of the brain.

Introduction

Ensembles are collections of one or more neurons whose activity co-varies or explains variance in a decoded variable, such as spatial position. Ensembles are proposed to be the default state of the brain, formed by co-activity which exists before content is mapped onto them[13]. Through changing membership and temporal order, ensembles can represent more information than individual neurons alone, and exhibit a potentially infinite number of combinations of neural activity. These ensembles are involved collectively in neural computations, as most neurons cannot cause the discharge of a downstream neuron alone[4]. Furthermore, ensembles can encode variables beyond what can be encoded by single neurons.

Ensemble coding is observed in many brain systems[5], but is perhaps most thoroughly studied and at the widest variety of scales in the navigation system. Neural ensembles in brain regions involved in navigation can be generally viewed from six perspectives. These perspectives are largely differentiated by the analysis approach used, as any given neuron can participate in more than one ensemble type. First, single neurons can be studied by training a decoding model (Figure 1a), which learns from training data how to receive spike trains as input and predict a spatial variable as output[6]. Each neuron is said to encode or represent the spatial variable decoded by the model. Many CA1 neurons, for instance, are place cells, as their activity increases when the animal is inside the location decoded by the model, the neuron’s place field[7]. Second, population codes can be deduced by collectively analyzing all recorded neurons (Figure 1b), commonly followed by dimensionality reduction[5]. Within the dimensionally reduced subspace, the geometry of activity reveals the structure of the population code. The axes of this subspace reflect the greatest co-variation and can be mapped onto latent variables, which can reflect position but also abstract variables like choice. The population activity occupies one point in the subspace at each moment in time; that point represents the value of latent variables encoded by the population. Third, cell assemblies are commonly defined by identifying patterns of co-activity within a brief time window. Fourth, time-compressed sequences of multiple neurons can fire sequentially on the scale of 100 ms bounded by the power or phase of local field potentials (LFP), extracellular currents that reflect the summation of activity from neurons (Figure 1c). These sequences can represent a path through space faster than the speed of the animal. They are most commonly observed during running, bounded by a single theta cycle (theta sequences)[8], and during rest, bounded by a sharp-wave ripple (replays)[9]. A third LFP oscillation, slow gamma, chunks theta sequences and replays into shorter trajectories[10,11].

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Six views of neural ensembles in navigation. Each neuron can participate in multiple ensembles and in multiple ensemble types, even simultaneously. Triangles represent neurons and boxes represent environments. (a) A single neuron (top) can represent a variable decoded from its firing field, for instance a place field within an environment (bottom). (b) Activity from all simultaneously recorded neurons can be combined into a population, which can for instance be dimensionally reduced from the N dimensions of N neurons to a 3-dimensional subspace. (c) Neurons which are repeatedly co-active can be grouped into assemblies. (d) Sequential activity can be organized by LFPs, for instance a replay (top) during a sharp-wave ripple (bottom). (e) Neurons can also be ordered by their peak activity over a single trial. (f) Neurons which express immediate early genes (top) during exposure to a context (bottom) are classified as engrams.

Fifth, behavioral sequences are usually defined by sequential firing by all recorded neurons with a discrete firing field over the span of a single trial of a behavioral task[12] (Figure 1d). Finally, engrams can be defined as neurons which express immediate early genes (such as Fos) during the temporal window of several minutes of exposure to an environmental context[13,14] (Figure 1e). Collectively, these six views of ensembles are most commonly described in the CA1 region of the hippocampus, but have also been observed throughout related cortical regions.

Here, we compare neural ensemble codes across these six views. We first discuss how the brain’s physiology and experimenters’ analysis methods define the boundaries of which neurons are included in ensembles. We then detail how the ensemble lens through which we interpret our findings constrains or expands potential hypotheses due to the assumptions and abstractions made at each ensemble scale. Next, we explain how different views of ensembles are behaviorally relevant and yet can lead to differing conclusions. Overall, we propose that our definitions of ensembles are fundamentally circumscribed by analysis choices and therefore can only provide multiple perspectives on neural activity rather than identifying a true underlying biological grouping. Finally, we discuss how future research can broaden the definition of an ensemble to better understand how the brain supports navigation.

What determines ensemble boundaries?

Physiology

Neural ensembles can arise from physiological properties that spatially and temporally join or segregate neurons into groups. For instance, excitability, which is in part regulated externally by inhibitory inputs and internally by biophysical membrane properties, plays a key role in the regulation of ensemble membership. Inhibitory inputs limit how many CA1 neurons in the hippocampus can be induced to represent a place field[15] and constrain the spatial size of each place field[16]. A rise in CA1 membrane potential predicts the formation of a place field[17]. Inhibitory neurons also pace the oscillations that organize time-compressed sequences[18]. Expression of genes which increase[19] or decrease[20] excitability of cells temporally joins or segregates cells from engrams, respectively. In parallel, inhibitory inputs to these excitatory ensemble neurons modulate the maintenance[21], size[22,23], and memory recall of the represented context[24] of an engram. Thus, excitability can constrain ensemble size across multiple ensembles views and could be a rapidly adaptable substrate for changing ensemble membership.

The boundaries of ensemble membership may also be defined by the temporal window during which its outputs could be integrated by a downstream reader, as defined by membrane time constants and plasticity windows. The membrane time constant of many excitatory cells is 1–100ms[25], approximately the length of a sharp-wave ripple or single theta cycle which encapsulates a time-compressed sequence[26]. The time window over which inputs can induce long-term potentiation in many excitatory cells is 1–30ms[27], matching the length of a single slow gamma oscillation, which chunks theta sequences and replays into shorter trajectories[10,11]. Cell assemblies defined on this timescale have recently been shown to drive activity in downstream reader neurons within the prefrontal cortex-amygdala reciprocal circuit[28]. Finally, CA1 pyramidal cells can potentiate inputs on the scale of seconds[29], matching the length of behavioral sequences. How ensembles over longer time scales could be integrated is unclear, though downstream readers may themselves combine as an ensemble to integrate inputs from several upstream sub-ensembles.

Which neurons are active in any given ensemble could be regulated by excitability, and the temporal window over which neural activity could be collectively received could be regulated by integration by downstream readers. While classically defined ensemble types are circumscribed by these physiological properties, how we determine which neurons participate in a given ensemble is often restricted by the analysis methods selected.

Analysis methods

Most navigational studies analyze neural activity through one of six views, which are restricted by the experimental conditions and analysis methods used. First, single neuron activity can be observed using an adjacent electrode or a calcium indicator, thus neurons are only included in analysis if they meet spike sorting or voltage thresholds or region of interest (ROI) selection criteria. Whether a cell is defined to represent a particular variable, such as a spatial field, can depend on experimenter-defined tuning thresholds or comparison to a shuffled distribution. Simultaneously recorded single neurons which pass at least the activity detection thresholds are then analyzed as part of the larger ensemble sizes.

Second, all recorded neurons can be analyzed as a population. The types of variables that can be extracted depend on what analysis method was used, such as linear combinations or nonlinear dimensionality reductions. However, it is unclear which types of methods are physiologically relevant, or how a downstream region could perform such a transformation on an entire upstream population to decode a variable. Moreover, even if only a subset of a population is decoded downstream, we cannot determine whether all active cells belong to a primary, initiating ensemble or whether some cells are firing in feedback. For instance, decoding spatial variables from a subset of the ensemble can be as informative as decoding from the entire population[30]. It is unclear whether the remainder of the population is informative or redundant behaviorally.

Third, cell assemblies are bounded by the time window selected by the researcher, most commonly 10–50ms to align with the timescale of long-term potentiation and a single gamma cycle[31], but can also be bound by longer windows to align with a single theta cycle[32] or sharp-wave ripple[33]. Thus, the membership of a cell assembly relies heavily on the experimental hypothesis. To identify cell assemblies, the pairwise correlations of binned, normalized spike trains undergo principal component analysis, then each component above a set threshold is labeled an assembly. These then undergo independent component analysis to extract vectors of weights, which reflect how much each cell’s firing contributes to an assembly. Cells with weights above a certain threshold are labeled members of that assembly[34]. Thresholds for assembly detection and membership can be set by an experimenter-defined value or null hypothesis distribution. Neither of these thresholds has yet been linked to physiology. Thus, it remains incompletely understood what timescale, assembly resolution, or assembly size is most relevant to downstream readers or behavior.

Fourth, time-compressed sequences are usually bounded by the length of a single theta cycle or sharp-wave ripple[8,9] (but see also [35]). Such sequences are often only examined if they coincide with LFP or multi-unit activity (MUA) of a sufficient amplitude and if the decoded spatial locations of the neurons progress in a linear trajectory over time[36]. However, LFP or MUA amplitude and the correlation coefficient of decoded space over time exist along a continuum, thus similar sequences just below these two thresholds may be used by the brain, but discarded in analyses[37]. As discussed later, discarded spiking can be physiologically relevant[38], and such restrictive definitions can change the results of analyses[39]. In addition, sequential firing is identified by ordering neurons by their activity peak, only considering a single firing field from each neuron. This approach can introduce complexities when dealing with the multiple firing fields found in navigational brain regions such as CA1[40] and entorhinal cortex[41].

Fifth, behavioral sequences are captured over the span of a single trial of a behavioral task, which often spans a few seconds[12], though they have also recently been observed to span up to 100 seconds and occur independent of trials[42]. Their membership is less restrictive than time-compressed sequences, as they can include all cells active during a trial. However, like time-compressed sequences, they are restricted to neurons that, when sorted by peak firing field, progress linearly over time, discarding additional firing fields.

Finally, engrams are bounded by the length of exposure to an environmental context, thus they are defined by their association with that context[13,14]. A neuron must express immediate early genes to be included in an engram, and so functional engram analysis requires tagging of these neurons. Engram experiments thus discard neurons that don’t express these genes at a high enough level to elicit sufficient opsin expression, which can vary based on viral expression magnitude. It is also unclear how a downstream neuron would integrate engram codes, which are captured on the scale of minutes. Finally, it is unclear which subset of cells are required for the engram, as memory strength is unrelated to engram size[43].

In sum, systems neuroscience research is both expanded and constrained by examining neural spiking through the lens of classical definitions of ensembles. This syntax arises from underlying physiology but is defined by how we design experiments and analyses.

Comparing ensemble views

The ensemble definition adopted in a single experiment can restrict the questions that can be asked by abstracting away relevant variables. At the same time, each ensemble view can also expand the types of questions that can be asked by analysis methods that reveal new dimensions of representations. In many cases, analyzing data across multiple views reveals novel insights not found at a single view, as many ensemble views are complementary.

Forest or the trees: population codes vs single neurons

Single neuron spike trains can be examined independently to decode the variables each neuron represents or collectively to decode the variables the population represents (e.g. the spatial position of the animal). Collections of neural spike trains can be analyzed through linear combinations such as population decoding, where all neurons are used to train a model to decode a variable, and similarity analyses, where population vectors are compared to reveal variables distinguished by the whole population. Both analyses can decode variables not detectably coded by individual neurons, such as environmental context or the order, valence, or identity of cues[30,4448]. Linear combinations can reveal phenomena which are represented by individual neurons, but below detection thresholds, as their combination increases statistical power. Alternatively, collections of spike trains can be analyzed through linear or nonlinear dimensionality reduction, where the N-dimensional spike trains of N neurons can be constrained to a lower-dimensional subspace, and the dynamics of the population through that subspace can represent variables[49]. Nonlinear methods can reveal phenomena which aren’t represented by individual neurons, but only by the nonlinear dynamics of the population. For instance, Shahbaba et al used a neural network approach to reduce CA1 spike trains to two-dimensions and found the geometry in this subspace could distinguish the order of stimuli[44]. Nieh et al used a manifold inference approach on CA1 activity and found that the resulting manifold was a conjoined cognitive map of physical and abstract variable spaces[50,51] (Figure 2a). The manifold was so robust that it could be used to decode physical and abstract variables as well as the raw spike trains, even when a manifold from a different mouse was used. Population ensembles can thus be analyzed through linear and nonlinear methods to decode below thresholds or abstract variables.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Different ensemble views reveal different aspects of the neural code. Unless specified, heatmaps range from red (high) to blue (low) activity or firing rate. (a) CA1 pyramidal cells jointly encoded place and accumulated evidence as mice ran down a linear track accumulating evidence to decide whether to turn left or right (top). Example place fields on the two trials (bottom). Reproduced with permission from [50] (b) Grid cells in the entorhinal cortex changed their firing rate, orientation, and spatial scale when a rewarded location was introduced (left vs right). Reproduced with permission from [52]. (c) (Left) A single neuron recorded as a mouse navigated a virtual linear track remaps between two representations on a trial-by-trial basis. (Right) Dimensionally reducing all neurons to the first three principal components revealed two circular manifolds, each representing one progression along the circular linear track. The population activity progressed along the manifolds and jumped between them during remap events. Adapted with permission from [53]. (d) Mice were trained on a collection of virtual linear tracks whose appearance morphed along a continuum from Track A to Track B. Mice were divided into two groups: those who mostly saw Tracks A and B (rare morph) and those who saw an even sample along the continuum (frequent morph). When exposed to all morphed tracks, place cells did not appear to remap differently between the two groups (left). Representational similarity analyses revealed a more abrupt remap in the rare morph group (center). The remapping dynamics of the two groups (discrete vs continuous) was made clear by examining the two principal eigenvectors of the similarity matrix (left). Adapted with permission from [55]. (e) A neural network model received velocity inputs (v) of a simulated animal exploring a 2D arena and outputted the animal’s location through place fields (right). The recurrently connected hidden units had firing fields that were similar to actual grid cells (bottom). Adapted with permission from [57]. (f) The activity of 149 grid cells reduced into three dimensions revealed a twisted torus geometry, which had been predicted by continuous attractor models[60]. A trajectory through space (left) matched a trajectory along the torus (middle), and activity from a single grid cell (top right) mapped to a single region on the torus (bottom right). Reproduced with permission from [58]. (g) Engram cells are place cells. Engram cells were active exclusively in one context, but were spatially unstable. Place cells were active in multiple contexts, but were spatially stable within a single context. Reproduced with permission from [61].

Analyses at the single neuron and population scale can answer different questions. For example, Butler, Hardcastle, and Giocomo observed changes in single entorhinal neuron tuning of multiple spatial properties only of neurons whose firing fields were near a rewarded location[52] (Figure 2b). Population analyses would have abstracted away the multiple spatial properties and firing field locations, and thus missed this finding. In contrast, Low et al and Sheintuch et al used single neuron and population analyses to find that medial entorhinal cortex and hippocampus, respectively, contained multiple maps of a single linear track, which switched between trials[53,54]. This was initially revealed by raster plots of individual neuron spike trains, but was best captured and understood when population analyses revealed that the whole population of neurons was alternating between two stable attractors (Figure 2c). Finally, Plitt et al showed that three dimensionality reduction methods found a CA1 remapping dynamic that could not be fully captured from individual neuron fields[55] (Figure 2d). Thus, single neuron analyses capture a wider variety of input dimensions, while population analyses produce a wider variety of output dimensions.

Population analyses are also used in artificial neural networks, a modeling architecture based on the brain, which lead to insights into how the brain might perform computations. For instance, optimizing a recurrent neural network to estimate head direction from angular velocity inputs creates a network layer whose weights match the firing patterns of Compass and Shifter neurons, two neural types in the fly head direction circuit[56]. Training a biologically plausible recurrent neural network to estimate position based on velocity inputs creates a network layer whose weights match firing patterns of grid cells[57] (Figure 2e). Thus, neural networks can recapitulate the structure and function of real neurons. These models have also led to testable hypotheses, such as grid-structured firing being generated by neurons organized into a continuous attractor network, which was recently demonstrated to be biologically accurate in entorhinal grid cells[58] (Figure 2f).

Population analyses can reveal structure in how neurons interact with each other, over time[59], and with underlying, abstract variables coded by the whole population. Single neuron coding models, by contrast, consider heterogeneous coding and properties beyond spike trains, such as tuning specificity. Both analyses are complementary, and experiments which study both scales can reveal the true complexity underlying spatial variable representation.

Place or context: sequences vs engrams

The intermediate ensemble sizes - sequences and engrams - are almost never examined together due to the different experimental conditions that define them. The CA1 place cell code and the CA1 engram code have long been studied in parallel as memory storage mechanisms in the hippocampus, with place cells encoding a cognitive map of space and engrams encoding a contextual index. Two recent studies compared these codes by examining spatial representations in cells expressing Fos at high (engram cells) or low levels. The first, conducted in novel environments where Fos expression is higher, found that high Fos-expressing cells are in fact place cells, but ones with sparse firing, low spatial stability, and activity specifically in one context[61] (Figure 2g), thus encode context more faithfully than space. The second, conducted in familiar environments where Fos expression is lower, found that high Fos-expressing cells had more stable and accurate spatial coding, and Fos was required to stabilize this code[62]. Thus, contextual indexing and spatial coding are different representation-oriented views of the same ensembles.

Sequences contain several additional dimensions of variables due to their temporal organization. Sequences encode temporal relationships between variables[63], and integrating over this temporal order can represent additional variables, such as movement direction[64]. In behavioral sequences, the order of peak firing of neurons can change between trials to distinguish variables not encoded by individual neurons, such as the trial type[44,65], animal’s choice[12,66,67], and distance traveled[68]. The oscillations that organize time-compressed sequences can both chunk sequences into sub-ensembles readable by a downstream neuron[69] and coordinate this reading by downstream brain regions[70]. These oscillations also provide a second layer of representation, as adding the theta phase at which a neuron fires to a decoding model improves accuracy[71].

Engrams, on the other hand, have been extensively causally linked to memory. Myriad studies have manipulated them to suppress or enhance recall, or even to induce an artificial association[19]. Similar manipulations have been made of place cells[15,72,73] and recall has been suppressed by disrupting replays[26]. However, just one study thus far has successfully enhanced recall by manipulating replays[74], and no studies yet have altered recall through manipulating behavioral sequences.

Engrams and sequences contain the same neurons yet encode different but complementary variables. Sequences reveal how the neural code adapts and is structured to represent temporal order, while engrams reveal how the neurons are recruited by gene expression changes and represent context. Future studies should examine the temporal activity patterns of engram cells, which could provide insight into how this minutes-long timescale code could be processed by the brain on the milliseconds-long timescale of dendritic integration and synaptic plasticity.

Are ensemble codes relevant to behavior?

To examine whether classically defined ensembles might form a behaviorally relevant syntax, we must test whether these ensembles affect navigation behavior. Multiple definitions of ensembles can be related to behavior to predict spatial paths and learning. Theta sequences[75] and replays[7679] (but see also [80]) can predict subsequent trajectories to goals. During correct trials as compared to error trials, behavioral sequences have steeper slopes[81] and replays have more co-active cell pairs[82]. As learning progresses, the strength of cell assemblies in the hippocampus[83] and prefrontal cortex[33] increases, either through more cells participating or greater participation of existing member cells. Humans with more flexible cell assembly membership have greater recall[84]. Mice with more distinct representations of different conditions, through context-specific place cells[85] or remodeled assemblies[32], learn faster. Rats with more place cells representing the goal location learn to find this goal faster[86]. Finally, Alzheimer’s model mice with fewer sharp-wave ripples learn spatial goal approach and goal avoidance slower[87]. Therefore, the content, structure, and incidence of some ensembles correlates with goal-directed navigation performance.

Beyond correlations, optogenetic manipulations provide compelling, causal evidence that ensemble codes support spatial navigation behaviors (Table 1). Driving just a few dozen CA1 place cells linked to reward at an encoded location can induce behavior that indicates the animal has associated that location with reward. Targeting place cells whose fields were inside the rewarded zone on a linear track, Robinson et al activated these neurons in a different location, inducing reward consumption at the new location[73]. Inducing an ensemble of neurons to form a place field at a future reward location improves learning of that reward location[15]. Closed-loop stimulation of the medial forebrain bundle upon spiking of a target place cell during wake or sleep induces place preference for the location that cell encodes[72]. In sum, place cell representations of rewarded locations are causally related to acquiring a goal.

Table 1.

Manipulating CA1 ensembles alters navigation behavior.

Manipulation Target Condition Effect Reference
Drive Place cells with fields in the reward zone at end of track At an earlier location Reward consumed at earlier location [73]
Drive Place cells with fields at start of track At a later location Overshoot goal without consumption [73]
Induce place coding Pyramidal cells At future goal location Faster acquisition when new goal introduced [15]
Stimulate medial forebrain bundle Place cells with fields at target location When these place cells fire, during sleep or wake Conditioned place preference [72]
Drive Engram In context not associated with footshock Ectopic recall [19]
Inhibit Engram In context associated with footshock or during sleep Impaired recall [19,89]
Interrupt Sharp-wave ripples At goal location or during sleep Slower learning [26,88]
Interrupt Sharp-wave ripples When replay encodes goal location during sleep Impaired recall [90]
Lengthen Sharp-wave ripples During inter-trial interval Faster learning [74]

Manipulating ensembles on the scale of CA1 replays and engrams also alters spatial memory. Driving activity in engram cells induces freezing outside of the associated fear context, while inhibiting these cells suppresses freezing inside the associated context[19]. Similarly, suppressing sharp-wave ripples specifically at a goal location impairs learning[88]. Closed-loop manipulations have shown that disrupting replays during sharp-wave ripples[26] or reactivation of engrams during sleep[89] impairs learning. However, most studies have disrupted all sharp-wave ripples, including those replaying content from other environments. To address this, Gridchyn et al used an online decoder to disrupt only replays of a specific environment to show that it only impairs recall of that environment[90]. Finally, artificially extending sharp-wave ripples also extends their replay trajectories and improves spatial working memory[74]. Thus, disrupting engrams and replays, during recall or consolidation, disrupts spatial memory, while enhancing engrams and replays enhances spatial memory.

Collections of neurons can be manipulated to alter navigation behavior. Although we restrict our targeting and interpretation of manipulations to specific ensemble definitions, these likely affect neurons outside the target ensemble and could be viewed from different ensemble perspectives, perhaps to different conclusions. Future work should try to manipulate population structure or demonstrate a correlation between changes in population structure and navigation.

Different conclusions from different views

Conclusions are often drawn from just viewing neural activity through the lens of one or two ensemble definitions. However, coding can change - due to altered environments or disease - through one ensemble lens but not another. For instance, when barriers are introduced or goal locations are changed, different ensembles emerge without individual neurons remapping - changing their tuning to reflect the environment change. Replays adapt to represent paths around changing barriers without place cells remapping[78], and novel goal locations are overrepresented in replays despite not being overrepresented by the place cell population’s mean firing rate[91]. In a recent study, Mau et al identified that cell assemblies reduced their co-activity over a single reversal learning session in the same environment, while place fields of the cells in the assemblies did not remap[32]. Animals can acquire new goals without remapping of single neurons, while remapping of assemblies and replays emerges early in learning[32,91], suggesting these ensembles may be the substrate for spatial learning. In contrast, when a larger change like a different context or different task is introduced, representations can be stable at the population level while dynamic at the single neuron level, allowing a coherent map to be preserved while individual neurons remap to convey context[30,92,93]. Similarly, population representations of a context can be stable over weeks while individual neuron representations drift[94]. This suggests that when single neurons do remap, they may do so in a coordinated manner, such that the underlying dimensionally reduced manifold retains the same shape but is rotated or translated in the subspace, as has been previously seen[53]. In sum, small environmental changes can alter replays and assemblies without changes to individual neurons, while larger changes can remap individual neurons while keeping population codes intact. Studies that examine only one of these views may not capture the full complexity of remapping.

Coding changes can be also observed through one ensemble view but not another in disease models. Many disease models with impaired spatial memory have normal place coding at the single neuron level, but impaired ensemble coding. First, Suh et al found that schizophrenia model mice have normal place fields, but no sequential relationship between spike timing and decoded spatial position in replays[95]. Disorganized replays have since also been found in schizophrenia patients[96]. Second, mice hemizygous for Scn2a, a gene involved in several neurodevelopmental disorders, have normal place fields but truncated replays with fewer place cells recruited[97]. Third, Poll et al identified sub-ensembles within the contextual engram classified by the conditions in which they were active. While control and Alzheimer’s model mice had similarly sized engrams, Alzheimer’s model mice had a subensemble which encoded novelty. When this sub-ensemble was suppressed, it prevented the novelty signal from interfering with the engram, rescuing recall[98]. Finally, Viana da Silva et al found that mice expressing amyloid precursor protein in CA3 had normal place fields yet disordered theta sequences[99]. Thus, disorganized time-compressed sequences and aberrant novelty ensembles are present in disease models lacking spatial tuning impairments. Examining place coding across a broad range of ensemble definitions has revealed that coordination of neurons may be a target for therapeutics and a biomarker for spatial memory rescue.

Beyond classic ensemble definitions

Looking beyond experimenter-defined ensemble classifications, new methods of detecting relationships between spike trains have uncovered novel ensemble structures. Repeated subsetting approaches have identified that neurons in CA1 sequentially fire in small, stable ensembles, which later combine into behavioral and replay sequences[100,101]. This organization could explain why theta sequences and replays are chunked into smaller trajectories, each within a single slow gamma oscillation[10,11]. Two models which approximate neural activity using spatiotemporal motifs have been used to order neurons into spiking sequences, which extracted behavioral sequences spanning a linear track run without the model having access to the decoded location[102,103]. This could identify new forms of sequential firing which don’t map cleanly onto a sequential variable such as time or location. Finally, our understanding of replays has been extended by new classification algorithms that identified that most replays don’t meet traditional definitions of replay, yet represent clear spatial trajectories[39,104]. This not only revealed that these previously discarded replay events might contain useful, decodable information, but also that an earlier finding about replay content depended on which subset of replays was considered[39]. Thus, restricting analyses to only ensembles that meet the stricter, classic definitions can change findings and ignore information the brain is integrating. However, classically defined ensembles are easier to target and manipulate, thus whether these new ensembles alter behavior is unknown. In sum, unsupervised methods reveal many activity structures previously unknown, without making assumptions about how the brain works, but their biological relevance is still unstudied.

Conclusion

Neural ensembles arise from intrinsic neural dynamics and can reveal structure, variables, and remapping independent of individual neuron representations. Studies thus far have restricted themselves to viewing ensembles through just one or two definitions, which constrains which hypotheses can be tested and can miss changes that are visible through one view but not others. Notable recent exceptions have uncovered incredibly rich coding by examining single neurons, behavioral sequences, population decoding, and dimensionally reduced subspaces in the same dataset[44,50]. No one view of ensembles can explain computation. Each ensemble definition provides a unique perspective of the collective underlying neural activity. Broadening our definition of ensemble membership, looking across scales of temporal, neuronal, and representational restrictions on definitions, while remaining couched in physiological plausibility, and subsequent testing for behavioral relevance will help the field better understand the brain from a less experimenter-biased perspective.

Future research directions

Future research could further expand our definition of ensembles. Identifying ensembles in a variety of animal models performing naturalistic navigation strategies, such as seed caching in birds and hunting in bats, will reveal what aspects of ensemble codes are unique to goal-oriented navigation in rodents and which are ecologically common (Figure 3a). Technological advances will continue to increase the number of simultaneous neurons which can be recorded, which will clarify the size of ensembles and reveal new, sparser codes. Recordings across multiple brain regions will enable identification of how neural codes in sensory cortex translate to associational areas and then to motor cortex, or how ensembles may span multiple regions. Ensemble membership should also be expanded to include oft excluded cell types which can nonetheless encode spatial variables - such as inhibitory neurons[105,106] and astrocytes[107,108], as well as electrical activity besides action potentials, such as dendritic activity[109] and subthreshold activity (Figure 3b).

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Future research directions. Future research should: (a) examine ensembles during more behaviors, in more animal models (such as seed caching in chickadees), across more brain regions, and with more simultaneously recorded neurons, (b) expand ensemble membership beyond excitatory cell spiking, such as to astrocytic activity, inhibitory neuron spikes, or non-spiking activity, (c) identify sequences and clusters with the fewest assumptions, while considering pre-existing connectivity, multiple variables, and multiple dimensions of variables, and (d) selectively silence or drive ensembles to examine the effects in downstream brain regions and on behavior. Chickadee reprinted with permission from [113].

Future research should also test the breadth of ensemble representations and their biological relevance. Using unsupervised approaches can identify ensembles without experimenter-imposed structure (Figure 3c). Ensemble analyses should consider how pre-existing dynamics constrain potential single neuron representations, rather than assuming that all neurons are equally likely to represent all variables[1,2]. Models that decode variables from ensembles should consider that a single neuron can encode multiple variables[110,111] and that multiple dimensions of a variable, such as peak firing rate and specificity, may be behaviorally relevant. Finally, the biological relevance and plausibility of ensembles should be tested by artificially altering their activity (Figure 3d). Future tools may be able to artificially induce sequential ensembles[74] or alter the formation of ensembles independent of individual neuron tuning. The biological plausibility of the resolution of decoded signal should be considered. For instance, population decoding can discriminate visual contrasts at 100x the resolution of what the mouse behaviorally indicates it can distinguish[112]. To test biological relevance, future work should continue to assess how behavior is casually altered by ensembles and should begin to study how codes are integrated by downstream readers.

Highlights.

  • Neural ensembles support navigation

  • Multi-neuron ensembles can represent variables independent of the single neuron code

  • Different views of ensembles can represent different variables

  • Broader definitions of ensembles reveals the full richness of coding

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Dr. Marielena Sosa, Dr. Alexander Gonzalez, the members of Dr. Giocomo’s lab, and the members of the Simon’s Global Brain 542987SPI collaboration for their feedback on the ideas presented here. This work was supported by the Stanford School of Medicine (EAAJ), the A.P. Giannini Foundation (EAAJ), the National Institute of Mental Health (MH106475 and MS118284 to LMG), the Simons Foundation Collaboration on the Global Brain 542987SPI (LMG), the James S McDonnell Foundation (LMG), and the Vallee Foundation (LMG).

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

No conflicts of interest

References

  • 1.Buzsáki G: The Brain from Inside Out. Oxford University Press; 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.McKenzie S, Huszár R, English DF, Kim K, Christensen F, Yoon E, Buzsáki G: Preexisting hippocampal network dynamics constrain optogenetically induced place fields. Neuron 2021, 109:1040–1054.e7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Dragoi G, Tonegawa S: Selection of preconfigured cell assemblies for representation of novel spatial experiences. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2014, 369:20120522. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Spruston N, Stuart G, Häusser M: Principles of dendritic integration. In Dendrites.. Oxford University Press; 2016:351–398. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Vyas S, Golub MD, Sussillo D, Shenoy KV: Computation Through Neural Population Dynamics. Annu Rev Neurosci 2020, 43:249–275. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Paninski L, Pillow J, Lewi J: Statistical models for neural encoding, decoding, and optimal stimulus design. In Progress in Brain Research. Edited by Cisek P, Drew T, Kalaska JF. Elsevier; 2007:493–507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.O’Keefe J, Dostrovsky J: The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Res 1971, 34:171–175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Foster DJ, Wilson MA: Hippocampal theta sequences. Hippocampus 2007, 17:1093–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Louie K, Wilson MA: Temporally structured replay of awake hippocampal ensemble activity during rapid eye movement sleep. Neuron 2001, 29:145–156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Pfeiffer BE, Foster DJ: Autoassociative dynamics in the generation of sequences of hippocampal place cells. Science 2015, 349:180–183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Zheng C, Bieri KW, Hsiao Y-T, Colgin LL: Spatial Sequence Coding Differs during Slow and Fast Gamma Rhythms in the Hippocampus. Neuron 2016, 89:398–408. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Harvey CD, Coen P, Tank DW: Choice-specific sequences in parietal cortex during a virtual-navigation decision task. Nature 2012, 484:62–68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Liu X, Ramirez S, Pang PT, Puryear CB, Govindarajan A, Deisseroth K, Tonegawa S: Optogenetic stimulation of a hippocampal engram activates fear memory recall. Nature 2012, 484:381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Ramirez S, Liu X, Lin P-A, Suh J, Pignatelli M, Redondo RL, Ryan TJ, Tonegawa S: Creating a False Memory in the Hippocampus. Science 2013, 341:387–391. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15**.Rolotti SV, Ahmed MS, Szoboszlay M, Geiller T, Negrean A, Blockus H, Gonzalez KC, Sparks FT, Canales ASS, Tuttman AL, et al. : Local feedback inhibition tightly controls rapid formation of hippocampal place fields. Neuron 2022, 110:783–794.e6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; ● This paper demonstrated that inhibitory neurons limit how many CA1 neurons can be induced to represent a place field. By clamping inhibition, the authors were able to induce a large ensemble of neurons to have a place field. When they induced this field at a future goal location, mice acquired this goal faster when it was introduced.
  • 16.Grienberger C, Milstein AD, Bittner KC, Romani S, Magee JC: Inhibitory suppression of heterogeneously tuned excitation enhances spatial coding in CA1 place cells. Nat Neurosci 2017, 20:417–426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Bittner KC, Grienberger C, Vaidya SP, Milstein AD, Macklin JJ, Suh J, Tonegawa S, Magee JC: Conjunctive input processing drives feature selectivity in hippocampal CA1 neurons. Nat Neurosci 2015, 18:1133–1142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Klausberger T, Somogyi P: Neuronal diversity and temporal dynamics: the unity of hippocampal circuit operations. Science 2008, 321:53–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Josselyn SA, Tonegawa S: Memory engrams: Recalling the past and imagining the future. Science 2020, 367:eaaw4325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Shen Y, Zhou M, Cai D, Filho DA, Fernandes G, Cai Y, de Sousa AF, Tian M, Kim N, Lee J, et al. : CCR5 closes the temporal window for memory linking. Nature 2022, 606:146–152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Guo N, Soden ME, Herber C, Kim MT, Besnard A, Lin P, Ma X, Cepko CL, Zweifel LS, Sahay A: Dentate granule cell recruitment of feedforward inhibition governs engram maintenance and remote memory generalization. Nat Med 2018, 24:438–449. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Stefanelli T, Bertollini C, Lüscher C, Muller D, Mendez P: Hippocampal Somatostatin Interneurons Control the Size of Neuronal Memory Ensembles. Neuron 2016, 89:1074–1085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Morrison DJ, Rashid AJ, Yiu AP, Yan C, Frankland PW, Josselyn SA: Parvalbumin interneurons constrain the size of the lateral amygdala engram. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2016, 135:91–99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Szőnyi A, Sos KE, Nyilas R, Schlingloff D, Domonkos A, Takács VT, Pósfai B, Hegedüs P, Priestley JB, Gundlach AL, et al. : Brainstem nucleus incertus controls contextual memory formation. Science 2019, 364:eaaw0445. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Tripathy SJ, Savitskaya J, Burton SD, Urban NN, Gerkin RC: NeuroElectro: a window to the world’s neuron electrophysiology data. Front Neuroinform 2014, 8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Sosa M, Gillespie AK, Frank LM: Neural Activity Patterns Underlying Spatial Coding in the Hippocampus. In Current Topics in Behavioral Neuroscience.. Springer; Berlin Heidelberg; 2018:43–100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Dan Y, Poo M-M: Spike Timing-Dependent Plasticity: From Synapse to Perception. Physiol Rev 2006, 86:1033–1048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Boucly CJ, Pompili MN, Todorova R, Leroux EM, Wiener S, Zugaro M: Flexible communication between cell assemblies and “reader” neurons. bioRxiv 2022, doi: 10.1101/2022.09.06.506754. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Bittner KC, Milstein AD, Grienberger C, Romani S, Magee JC: Behavioral time scale synaptic plasticity underlies CA1 place fields. Science 2017, 357:1033–1036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30**.Levy ERJ, Park EH, Redman WT, Fenton AA: A neuronal code for space in hippocampal coactivity dynamics independent of place fields. bioRxiv 2021, doi: 10.1101/2021.07.26.453856. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; ● This paper took a unique approach by hypothesizing that the neural code for context is in the co-activity of a population, while the neural code for place is in the activity of a population. The authors recorded CA1 place cells using calcium imaging while exploring two environments over several days. They found that these two population codes are independent from one another, and that a small subset of neurons which are highly anticoactive are responsible for distinguishing contexts. The two contexts could be distinguished by using IsoMap to project the neural co-activity to a 2D geometry.
  • 31.van de Ven GM, Trouche S, McNamara CG, Allen K, Dupret D: Hippocampal Offline Reactivation Consolidates Recently Formed Cell Assembly Patterns during Sharp Wave-Ripples. Neuron 2016, 92:968–974. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32*.Mau W, Morales-Rodriguez D, Dong Z, Pennington ZT, Francisco T, Baxter MG, Shuman T, Cai DJ: Ensemble remodeling supports memory-updating. bioRxiv 2022, doi: 10.1101/2022.06.02.494530. [DOI] [Google Scholar]; ● This paper examined the dynamics of cell assemblies over learning. Cell assemblies were stable across sessions, but a subset of them weakened their co-activity over the course of a single reversal learning session, the extent of which predicted performance. Weakened co-activity was due to low co-activity neurons ceasing participation in an assembly, suggesting that pruning of low co-activity neurons, similar to pruning of low activity neurons[114], could be the basis for remapping.
  • 33.Peyrache A, Khamassi M, Benchenane K, Wiener SI, Battaglia FP: Replay of rule-learning related neural patterns in the prefrontal cortex during sleep. Nat Neurosci 2009, 12:919–926. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Lopes-dos-Santos V, Ribeiro S, Tort ABL: Detecting cell assemblies in large neuronal populations. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 2013, 220:149–166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Davidson TJ, Kloosterman F, Wilson MA: Hippocampal Replay of Extended Experience. Neuron 2009, 63:497–507. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Tingley D, Peyrache A: On the methods for reactivation and replay analysis. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2020, 35:20190231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Liu AA, Henin S, Abbaspoor S, Bragin A, Buffalo EA, Farrell JS, Foster DJ, Frank LM, Gedankien T, Gotman J, et al. : A consensus statement on detection of hippocampal sharp wave ripples and differentiation from other fast oscillations. Nat Commun 2022, 13:6000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Yu JY, Frank LM: Prefrontal cortical activity predicts the occurrence of nonlocal hippocampal representations during spatial navigation. PLoS Biol 2021, 19:e3001393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39*.Krause EL, Drugowitsch J: A large majority of awake hippocampal sharp-wave ripples feature spatial trajectories with momentum. Neuron 2022, 110:722–733.e8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; ● This paper used a statistical model to classify replays rather than only considering replays which progress at constant velocity. The authors mapped replays onto one of five dynamics models, including two spatially coherent models: diffusion, with progressing location constant velocity, and momentum, with progressing velocity. As in [104], spatially coherent models fit nearly all replays. Specifically, the momentum model fit most replays and matched awake run trajectories. The authors also found that once the definition of replays was extended using their model, a previous finding that replays at a home location predict future run trajectories no longer held true.
  • 40.Harland B, Contreras M, Souder M, Fellous J-M: Dorsal CA1 hippocampal place cells form a multi-scale representation of megaspace. Curr Biol 2021, 31:2178–2190.e6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Hafting T, Fyhn M, Molden S, Moser M-B, Moser EI: Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex. Nature 2005, 436:801–806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Cogno SG, Obenhaus HA, Jacobsen RI, Donato F, Moser M-B, Moser EI: Minute-scale oscillatory sequences in medial entorhinal cortex. bioRxiv 2022, doi: 10.1101/2022.05.02.490273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Rao-Ruiz P, Yu J, Kushner SA, Josselyn SA: Neuronal competition: microcircuit mechanisms define the sparsity of the engram. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2019, 54:163–170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44**.Shahbaba B, Li L, Agostinelli F, Saraf M, Cooper KW, Haghverdian, Elias GA, Baldi P, Fortin NJ: Hippocampal ensembles represent sequential relationships among an extended sequence of nonspatial events. Nat Commun 2022, 13:787. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; ● This paper examined CA1 neuron activity while rats performed a nonspatial sequence memory task. CA1 neurons represented time from port entry, firing in a behavioral sequence which distinguished odor identity. This activity is also organized into time-compressed sequences during theta cycles. The authors then trained an autoencoder using spike trains as input, and the resulting two-dimensional subspace in the innermost layer could distinguish the order of stimuli.
  • 45.Fetterhoff D, Sobolev A, Leibold C: Graded remapping of hippocampal ensembles under sensory conflicts. Cell Rep 2021, 36:109661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Rubin A, Geva N, Sheintuch L, Ziv Y: Hippocampal ensemble dynamics timestamp events in long-term memory. eLife 2015, 4:e12247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Nagelhus A, Andersson S, Cogno SG, Moser EI, Moser M-B: Object-centered population coding in CA1 of the hippocampus. bioRxiv 2022, doi: 10.1101/2022.07.07.499197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.McKenzie S, Frank AJ, Kinsky NR, Porter B, Rivière PD, Eichenbaum H: Hippocampal Representation of Related and Opposing Memories Develop within Distinct, Hierarchically Organized Neural Schemas. Neuron 2014, 83:202–215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Dubreuil A, Valente A, Beiran M, Mastrogiuseppe F, Ostojic S: The role of population structure in computations through neural dynamics. Nat Neurosci 2022, 25:783–794. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50**.Nieh EH, Schottdorf M, Freeman NW, Low RJ, Lewallen S, Koay SA, Pinto L, Gauthier JL, Brody CD, Tank DW: Geometry of abstract learned knowledge in the hippocampus. Nature 2021, 595:80–84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; ● In this trailblazing study, mice were trained to accumulate evidence by counting left and right visual cues as they ran down a linear track, then selected the side with the most cues. Individual neurons jointly encoded position and accumulated evidence and organized into behavioral sequences that encoded choice. Doublets which consistently fired in a particular order predicted the animal’s choice. Manifold inference revealed that population activity could be constrained to a four to six dimensional space, suggesting it encoded that many variables. This low-dimensional geometry could be used to decode physical and abstract variables as well as the raw spike trains.
  • 51.Rogers J: A conjoined cognitive map. Nat Rev Neurosci 2021, 22:518–519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Butler WN, Hardcastle K, Giocomo LM: Remembered reward locations restructure entorhinal spatial maps. Science 2019, 363:1447–1452. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53**.Low IIC, Williams AH, Campbell MG, Linderman SW, Giocomo LM: Dynamic and reversible remapping of network representations in an unchanging environment. Neuron 2021, 109:2967–2980.e11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; ● Similar to findings in CA1[54], the authors observed rapid, complete, coordinated switches between multiple discrete maps of the same environment by neurons in the entorhinal cortex. Despite this remap, spatial information could be decoded from a model trained on either map, suggesting conservation of positional information. By dimensionally reducing all neurons to the first three principal components, the authors found two circular manifolds, one for each map. Population activity progressed along the manifolds as the animal moved down the linear track and jumped between them during remap events.
  • 54*.Sheintuch L, Geva N, Baumer H, Rechavi Y, Rubin A, Ziv Y: Multiple Maps of the Same Spatial Context Can Stably Coexist in the Mouse Hippocampus. Curr Biol 2020, 30:1467–1476.e6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; ● The authors uncovered a novel phenomenon: simultaneous remapping of CA1 excitatory cells between brief visits to the same environment. By tracking the same cells over time using calcium imaging, the authors found that these multiple maps of the same space are stable over weeks. They proposed that the multiple maps arise from high variability in the initial conditions of the CA1 network during initial context exposure, and that remaps are transitions between the two basins (attractor states) of an energy landscape.
  • 55*.Plitt MH, Giocomo LM: Experience-dependent contextual codes in the hippocampus. Nat Neurosci 2021, 24:705–714. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; ● In this paper, the dynamics of remapping between two environments was revealed to be predicted by the optimal estimate based on prior experience. Mice ran on a collection of virtual linear tracks whose appearance morphed between two extremes. Mice were divided into two groups: those who mostly saw the extremes (rare morph) and those who saw an even sample along the continuum (frequent morph). Mice were then exposed to all track morphs. By dimensionally reducing the neural activity space using principal component analysis, the authors showed that the rare morph group discretely remapped between two maps, while the frequent morph group remapped along a continuum.
  • 56.Cueva CJ, Wang PY, Chin M, Wei X-X: Emergence of functional and structural properties of the head direction system by optimization of recurrent neural networks. arXiv 2020, doi: 10.48550/arXiv.1912.10189. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Sorscher B, Mel GC, Ocko SA, Giocomo L, Ganguli S: A unified theory for the computational and mechanistic origins of grid cells. bioRxiv 2020, doi: 10.1101/2020.12.29.424583. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58**.Gardner RJ, Hermansen E, Pachitariu M, Burak Y, Baas NA, Dunn BA, Moser M-B, Moser EI: Toroidal topology of population activity in grid cells. Nature 2022, 602:123–128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; ● By simultaneously recording from hundreds of entorhinal grid cells, this groundbreaking study demonstrated that activity from grid cells in a single grid module lies along a twisted torus-shaped manifold, as had been predicted by continuous attractor models[60] for years prior. Activity from individual cells occupied a single area on the torus. Positions on the torus mapped to positions in the animal’s environment and were stable across environments and sleep. This mapping was created by reducing the activity of all the cells in a grid module into six dimensions using principal component analysis and then three dimensions using uniform manifold approximation and projection.
  • 59.Williams AH, Linderman SW: Statistical neuroscience in the single trial limit. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2021, 70:193–205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Burak Y, Fiete IR: Accurate Path Integration in Continuous Attractor Network Models of Grid Cells. PLoS Comput Biol 2009, 5:e1000291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Tanaka KZ, He H, Tomar A, Niisato K, Huang AJY, McHugh TJ: The hippocampal engram maps experience but not place. Science 2018, 361:392–397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Pettit NL, Yap E-L, Greenberg ME, Harvey CD: Fos ensembles encode and shape stable spatial maps in the hippocampus. Nature 2022, doi: 10.1038/s41586-022-05113-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Buzsáki G, Tingley D: Space and Time: The Hippocampus as a Sequence Generator. Trends Cogn Sci 2018, 22:853–869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Zutshi I, Leutgeb JK, Leutgeb S: Theta sequences of grid cell populations can provide a movement-direction signal. Curr Opin Behav Sci 2017, 17:147–154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Pastalkova E, Itskov V, Amarasingham A, Buzsáki G: Internally Generated Cell Assembly Sequences in the Rat Hippocampus. Science 2008, 321:1322–1327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Crowe DA, Averbeck BB, Chafee MV: Rapid Sequences of Population Activity Patterns Dynamically Encode Task-Critical Spatial Information in Parietal Cortex. J Neurosci 2010, 30:11640–11653. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Fujisawa S, Amarasingham A, Harrison MT, Buzsáki G: Behavior-dependent short-term assembly dynamics in the medial prefrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci 2008, 11:823–833. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Villette V, Malvache A, Tressard T, Dupuy N, Cossart R: Internally Recurring Hippocampal Sequences as a Population Template of Spatiotemporal Information. Neuron 2015, 88:357–366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Harris KD, Csicsvari J, Hirase H, Dragoi G, Buzsáki G: Organization of cell assemblies in the hippocampus. Nature 2003, 424:552–556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Jadhav SP, Rothschild G, Roumis DK, Frank LM: Coordinated Excitation and Inhibition of Prefrontal Ensembles during Awake Hippocampal Sharp-Wave Ripple Events. Neuron 2016, 90:113–127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Jensen O, Lisman JE: Position Reconstruction From an Ensemble of Hippocampal Place Cells: Contribution of Theta Phase Coding. J Neurophysiol 2000, 83:2602–2609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.de Lavilléon G, Lacroix MM, Rondi-Reig L, Benchenane K: Explicit memory creation during sleep demonstrates a causal role of place cells in navigation. Nat Neurosci 2015, 18:493–495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73**.Robinson NTM, Descamps LAL, Russell LE, Buchholz MO, Bicknell BA, Antonov GK, Lau JYN, Nutbrown R, Schmidt-Hieber C, Häusser M: Targeted Activation of Hippocampal Place Cells Drives Memory-Guided Spatial Behavior. Cell 2020, 183:1586–1599.e10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; ● This was the first causal demonstration that CA1 place cell activity is sufficient to alter behavior. The authors used holographic optogenetic stimulation and calcium imaging during a virtual linear track. The authors targeted place cells active at the reward location and stimulated them at a different location, inducing reward consumption at this ectopic location. They also targeted place cells active at the start of the linear track and stimulated them near the end, inducing mice to run past the goal location without consuming the reward.
  • 74.Fernández-Ruiz A, Oliva A, Fermino de Oliveira E, Rocha-Almeida F, Tingley D, Buzsáki G: Long-duration hippocampal sharp wave ripples improve memory. Science 2019, 364:1082–1086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Wikenheiser AM, Redish AD: Hippocampal theta sequences reflect current goals. Nature Neuroscience 2015, 18:289–294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Pfeiffer BE, Foster DJ: Hippocampal place-cell sequences depict future paths to remembered goals. Nature 2013, 497:74–79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Xu H, Baracskay P, O’Neill J, Csicsvari J: Assembly Responses of Hippocampal CA1 Place Cells Predict Learned Behavior in Goal-Directed Spatial Tasks on the Radial Eight-Arm Maze. Neuron 2019, 101:119–132.e4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Widloski J, Foster DJ: Flexible rerouting of hippocampal replay sequences around changing barriers in the absence of global place field remapping. Neuron 2022, 110:1547–1558.e8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Ólafsdóttir HF, Carpenter F, Barry C: Task Demands Predict a Dynamic Switch in the Content of Awake Hippocampal Replay. Neuron 2017, 0:925–935.e6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Gillespie AK, Astudillo Maya DA, Denovellis EL, Liu DF, Kastner DB, Coulter ME, Roumis DK, Eden UT, Frank LM: Hippocampal replay reflects specific past experiences rather than a plan for subsequent choice. Neuron 2021, 109:3149–3163.e6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Zheng C, Hwaun E, Loza CA, Colgin LL: Hippocampal place cell sequences differ during correct and error trials in a spatial memory task. Nat Commun 2021, 12:3373. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Singer AC, Carr MF, Karlsson MP, Frank LM: Hippocampal SWR activity predicts correct decisions during the initial learning of an alternation task. Neuron 2013, 77:1163–73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Fernández-Ruiz A, Oliva A, Soula M, Rocha-Almeida F, Nagy GA, Martin-Vazquez G, Buzsáki G: Gamma rhythm communication between entorhinal cortex and dentate gyrus neuronal assemblies. Science 2021, 372:eabf3119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Umbach G, Tan R, Jacobs J, Pfeiffer BE, Lega B: Flexibility of functional neuronal assemblies supports human memory. Nat Commun 2022, 13:6162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Zemla R, Moore JJ, Basu J: Behaviorally emergent hippocampal place maps remain stable during memory recall. bioRxiv 2021, doi: 10.1101/2021.07.08.451449. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Dupret D, O’Neill J, Pleydell-Bouverie B, Csicsvari J: The reorganization and reactivation of hippocampal maps predict spatial memory performance. Nat Neurosci 2010, 13:995–1002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Jones EA, Gillespie AK, Yoon SY, Frank LM, Huang Y: Early Hippocampal Sharp-Wave Ripple Deficits Predict Later Learning and Memory Impairments in an Alzheimer’s Disease Mouse Model. Cell Rep 2019, 29:2123–2133.e4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Jarzebowski P, Tang CS, Paulsen O, Hay YA: Impaired spatial learning and suppression of sharp wave ripples by cholinergic activation at the goal location. eLife 2021, 10:e65998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Clawson BC, Pickup EJ, Ensing A, Geneseo L, Shaver J, Gonzalez-Amoretti J, Zhao M, York AK, Kuhn FR, Swift K, et al. : Causal role for sleep-dependent reactivation of learning-activated sensory ensembles for fear memory consolidation. Nat Commun 2021, 12:1200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90*.Gridchyn I, Schoenenberger P, Neill JO, Csicsvari J: Assembly-Specific Disruption of Hippocampal Replay Leads to Selective Memory Deficit Article Assembly-Specific Disruption of Hippocampal Replay Leads to Selective Memory Deficit. Neuron 2020, 106:291–300.e6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; ● This paper is the first to disrupt sharp-wave ripples based on their content. The authors decoded replayed position during recordings and disrupted only replays that did not have a high probability of representing a trajectory through the control environment. Following disruption during sleep, recall on a goal-directed task was impaired only in the disrupted environment. That environment’s place map destabilized until the goal was relearned. This showed that sharp-wave ripples consolidate memories based on replay content, rather than broadly stabilizing the network independent of their content.
  • 91.Pfeiffer BE: Spatial learning drives rapid goal representation in hippocampal ripples without place field accumulation or goal-oriented theta sequences. J Neurosci 2022, 42:3975–3988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Ziv Y, Burns LD, Cocker ED, Hamel EO, Ghosh KK, Kitch LJ, Gamal AE, Schnitzer MJ: Long-term dynamics of CA1 hippocampal place codes. Nat Neurosci 2013, 16:264–266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Hallock HL, Griffin AL: Dynamic coding of dorsal hippocampal neurons between tasks that differ in structure and memory demand. Hippocampus 2013, 23:169–186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Keinath AT, Mosser C-A, Brandon MP: The representation of context in mouse hippocampus is preserved despite neural drift. Nat Commun 2022, 13:2415. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Suh J, Foster DJ, Davoudi H, Wilson MA, Tonegawa S: Impaired hippocampal ripple-associated replay in a mouse model of schizophrenia. Neuron 2013, 80:484–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Nour MM, Liu Y, Arumuham A, Kurth-Nelson Z, Dolan RJ: Impaired neural replay of inferred relationships in schizophrenia. Cell 2021, 184:4315–4328.e17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Middleton SJ, Kneller EM, Chen S, Ogiwara I, Montal M, Yamakawa K, McHugh TJ: Altered hippocampal replay is associated with memory impairment in mice heterozygous for the Scn2a gene. Nat Neurosci 2018, 21:996–1003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98**.Poll S, Mittag M, Musacchio F, Justus LC, Giovannetti EA, Steffen J, Wagner J, Zohren L, Schoch S, Schmidt B, et al. : Memory trace interference impairs recall in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. Nat Neurosci 2020, 23:952–958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; ● This study examined engram cells in Alzheimer’s model and control mice during contextual fear conditioning. In line with engram strength being unrelated to size, Alzheimer’s model and control mice had similarly sized engrams despite Alzheimer’s model mice having impaired recall. By tracking when CA1 cells expressed immediate early genes, the authors found that a subset of engram cells were active in all novel contexts. This novelty ensemble was active in Alzheimer’s model mice during recall trials, impairing performance. Reducing activity in this ensemble rescued recall in Alzheimer’s model mice.
  • 99.Viana da Silva S, Haberl MG, Gaur K, Patel R, Narayan G, Ledakis M, Fu ML, Koo EH, Leutgeb JK, Leutgeb S: Localized APP pathology in the hippocampus is sufficient to result in progressive disorganization of the timing of neuronal firing patterns. bioRxiv 2022, doi: 10.1101/2022.10.24.513188. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Liu K, Sibille J, Dragoi G: Generative Predictive Codes by Multiplexed Hippocampal Neuronal Tuplets. Neuron 2018, 99:1329–1341.e6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Malvache A, Reichinnek S, Villette V, Haimerl C, Cossart R: Awake hippocampal reactivations project onto orthogonal neuronal assemblies. Science 2016, 353:1280–1283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Williams AH, Degleris A, Wang Y, Linderman SW: Point process models for sequence detection in high-dimensional neural spike trains. arXiv 2020, doi: 10.48550/arXiv.2010.04875. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Mackevicius EL, Bahle AH, Williams AH, Gu S, Denisenko NI, Goldman MS, Fee MS: Unsupervised discovery of temporal sequences in high-dimensional datasets, with applications to neuroscience. eLife 2019, 8:e38471. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104*.Denovellis EL, Gillespie AK, Coulter ME, Sosa M, Chung JE, Eden UT, Frank LM: Hippocampal replay of experience at real-world speeds. eLife 2021, 10:e64505. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; ● This is the first paper to classify replays using multiple movement dynamics rather than restricting the definition to sequences that fit a linear trajectory at a constant velocity. The authors mapped replays onto stationary, continuous, or fragmented movement dynamics, with some probability of transitioning between dynamics within a single replay. This method classified nearly all replays as having spatially coherent (stationary or continuous) content for at least part of the event; most of these events would be discarded with prior methods. See also [39].
  • 105.Cummings KA, Clem RL: Prefrontal somatostatin interneurons encode fear memory. Nat Neurosci 2020, 23:61–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Kropff E, Carmichael JE, Moser M-B, Moser EI: Speed cells in the medial entorhinal cortex. Nature 2015, 523:419–424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Doron A, Rubin A, Benmelech-Chovav A, Benaim N, Carmi T, Kreisel T, Ziv Y, Goshen I: Hippocampal Astrocytes Encode Reward Location. bioRxiv 2021, doi: 10.1101/2021.07.07.451434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108*.Curreli S, Bonato J, Romanzi S, Panzeri S, Fellin T: Complementary encoding of spatial information in hippocampal astrocytes. PLoS Biol 2022, 20:e3001530. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; ● This paper found that calcium signals in CA1 astrocytes encode spatial information complementary to CA1 pyramidal cells. Interestingly, the soma and processes of a single astrocyte could represent different locations. Decoding position from place cells was improved when astrocytic calcium events were added to the model.
  • 109.Takahashi S, Sakurai Y: Coding of spatial information by soma and dendrite of pyramidal cells in the hippocampal CA1 of behaving rats. Eur J Neurosci 2007, 26:2033–2045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Hardcastle K, Maheswaranathan N, Ganguli S, Giocomo LM: A Multiplexed, Heterogeneous, and Adaptive Code for Navigation in Medial Entorhinal Cortex. Neuron 2017, 94:375–387.e7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Nayebi A, Attinger A, Campbell MG, Hardcastle K, Low IIC, Mallory CS, Mel GC, Sorscher B, Williams AH, Ganguli S, et al. : Explaining heterogeneity in medial entorhinal cortex with task-driven neural networks. bioRxiv 2021, doi: 10.1101/2021.10.30.466617. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Stringer C, Michaelos M, Tsyboulski D, Lindo SE, Pachitariu M: High-precision coding in visual cortex. Cell 2021, 184:2767–2778.e15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Applegate MC, Aronov D: Flexible use of memory by food-caching birds. eLife 2022, 11:e70600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Grosmark AD, Buzsáki G: Diversity in neural firing dynamics supports both rigid and learned hippocampal sequences. Science 2016, 351:1440–1443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES