Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2023 Jan 18;52(4):1819–1840. doi: 10.1007/s10508-022-02519-w

Effects of a 7-Day Pornography Abstinence Period on Withdrawal-Related Symptoms in Regular Pornography Users: A Randomized Controlled Study

David P Fernandez 1,, Daria J Kuss 1, Lucy V Justice 1, Elaine F Fernandez 2, Mark D Griffiths 1
PMCID: PMC9847461  PMID: 36652136

Abstract

Little is known about whether withdrawal-like symptoms manifest when regular pornography users attempt to abstain from pornography. The present study used a randomized controlled design to examine whether (1) negative abstinence effects that may be potentially reflective of withdrawal-related symptoms manifest when a non-clinical sample of regular pornography users attempt to abstain from pornography for a 7-day period and (2) these negative abstinence effects would only manifest (or manifest more strongly) for those with higher levels of problematic pornography use (PPU). A total of 176 undergraduate students (64.2% female) who were regular pornography users (defined as having used pornography ≥ three times a week in the past 4 weeks) were randomly assigned to an abstinence group (instructed to attempt abstinence from pornography for 7 days, n = 86) or a control group (free to watch pornography as usual, n = 90). Participants completed measures of craving, positive and negative affect, and withdrawal symptoms at baseline and each night of the 7-day period. Contrary to the confirmatory hypotheses, there were no significant main effects of group (abstinence vs. control) or group × PPU interaction effects on any of the outcome measures, controlling for baseline scores. These findings indicate that no evidence of withdrawal-related symptoms was found for abstaining participants, and this was not dependent on level of PPU. However, exploratory analyses showed a significant three-way interaction (group × PPU × past 4-week frequency of pornography use [FPU]) on craving, where an abstinence effect on craving was found at high levels of PPU only once past 4-week FPU reached the threshold of daily use. While these exploratory findings should be interpreted with caution, they suggest that abstinence effects could potentially manifest when there is a combination of high PPU and high FPU—a hypothesis that warrants investigation in future prospective abstinence studies.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s10508-022-02519-w.

Keywords: Pornography, Abstinence, Addiction, Withdrawal, Craving, Affect

Introduction

Pornography use is a common activity in the developed world, with nationally representative studies showing that 76% of men and 41% of women in Australia reported using pornography within the past year (Rissel et al., 2017), and that 47% of men and 16% of women in the USA reported using pornography at a monthly or greater frequency (Grubbs et al., 2019a).

Problematic Pornography Use

Given the increasing prevalence of pornography use, the potential negative psychological effects of pornography use (particularly in relation to its problematic or addiction potential) have been the subject of growing empirical attention in recent years (de Alarcón et al., 2019; Grubbs & Kraus, 2021; Grubbs et al., 2020). Research to date has generally indicated that while the majority of individuals who use pornography report doing so in a non-problematic way (Bőthe et al., 2020b), a subset of users report addiction-like symptoms such as impaired control over their pornography use (Bőthe et al., 2018; Kor et al., 2014) and self-identify as being ‘addicted’ to pornography (Grubbs et al., 2018).

Despite these self-reports, researchers still disagree about whether habitual pornography users can develop genuine addictions to pornography and manifest addiction-related symptomatology akin to substance addictions. Some have argued that pornography is inherently addictive due to it being a particularly novel and rewarding stimulus (Hilton Jr., 2013) and that symptoms of dysregulated pornography use fit within an addiction framework, sharing similar neurobiological mechanisms with substance addictions and other behavioral addictions (Gola et al., 2017; Love et al., 2015), while others hold the view that pornography addiction is not a valid clinical entity and can instead be explained by non-pathological learning (Ley et al., 2014). One model (Grubbs et al., 2019b) has posited that while some users may experience genuine dysregulation in their pornography use, other users may inaccurately pathologize their use even in the absence of genuine dysregulation due to moral incongruence (i.e., using pornography while morally disapproving of it)—suggesting that self-diagnoses of “pornography addiction” should be regarded with caution.

The lack of consensus concerning how best to conceptualize dysregulated use of pornography has also been in large part due to ongoing debates in the field about how to conceptualize out-of-control sexual behaviors more broadly (Fernandez & Griffiths, 2021). There have been numerous theoretical conceptualizations of out-of-control sexual behaviors over the years, including sexual addiction (Carnes, 1983), sexual impulsivity (Barth & Kinder, 1987), compulsive sexual behavior (Coleman, 1991), and hypersexual disorder (Kafka, 2010). As a result of the varying theoretical frameworks, the term problematic pornography use (PPU) has typically been used in the literature likely because it is the most theoretically neutral term that encompasses any conceptualization of the phenomenon and includes problematic use at the subclinical end of the spectrum (Fernandez & Griffiths, 2021).

Most recently (in 2019), the World Health Organization (WHO) included the diagnosis of compulsive sexual behavior disorder (CSBD) as an impulse control disorder in the eleventh revision of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11; WHO, 2019), under which PPU may be subsumed. According to the ICD-11, CSBD is characterized by “a persistent pattern of failure to control intense, repetitive sexual impulses or urges, resulting in repetitive sexual behavior… over an extended period (e.g., six months or more) and causes marked distress or significant impairment in personal, family, social, educational, occupational or other important areas of functioning” (World Health Organization, 2019, p. 1). The ICD-11 also states that distress entirely related to moral judgments and disapproval about sexual impulses, urges or behaviors is not sufficient to meet the distress criterion. Overall, a conservative approach was taken for the ICD-11 in classifying CSBD as an impulse control disorder instead of an addictive disorder because there is (to date) insufficient evidence to determine whether the processes involved in the development and maintenance of the disorder are equivalent to other recognized forms of addiction (Kraus et al., 2018).

While the current ICD-11 CSBD diagnosis is a good starting point for unified assessment of the disorder, more research on its precise phenomenology and neurobiological underpinnings is needed to determine whether it should be re-classified as an addictive disorder in future iterations of diagnostic manuals (Griffiths, 2022; Kraus et al., 2016, 2018; Sassover & Weinstein, 2022). Determining whether CSBD (and by extension, PPU) is best conceptualized as an addictive disorder is important to ensure that appropriate pharmacological and psychological treatments are delivered depending on the specific neurobiological mechanisms and clinical features involved (Bőthe et al., 2022; Briken & Turner, 2022; Kingston, 2015; Kor et al., 2013; Lew-Starowicz & Coleman, 2022; Potenza et al., 2017). While debated (Castro-Calvo et al., 2022; Rumpf & Montag, 2022), some (Sassover & Weinstein, 2022) have proposed that to properly assess the phenomenology of CSBD, a comprehensive examination of all six components of Griffiths’ (2005) components model of addiction (i.e., salience, mood modification, conflict, withdrawal, tolerance and relapse) is needed. The present study focuses on the potential manifestation of the withdrawal component specifically in relation to PPU.

The Assessment of Withdrawal in Relation to Problematic Pornography Use

Withdrawal can be defined as the unpleasant affective states and/or physical reactions that occur when a substance or behavior is abruptly ceased or reduced (Griffiths, 2005). Withdrawal syndromes for most addictive substances are well-established, common symptoms of which include symptoms related to emotional distress such as depressed mood, anxiety or insomnia (Shmulewitz et al., 2015), while similar withdrawal-like symptoms such as depressed mood, irritability and/or anger, and anxiety have been retrospectively self-reported in clinical samples across problematic behaviors such as gambling (e.g., Blaszczynski et al., 2008) and sexual behaviors (e.g., Wines, 1997).

In the present study, it is posited that investigating the potential manifestation of withdrawal-related symptoms in relation to PPU is important for a few reasons. First, some limited empirical research has indicated that self-perceptions of withdrawal-like symptoms are not uncommon among individuals who have attempted to abstain from pornography. A cross-sectional survey of Polish students who were current pornography users (N = 4260) found that among those who had at least one past pornography abstinence attempt (n = 2169), 72.2% recalled experiencing at least one withdrawal-like symptom upon cessation (Dwulit & Rzymski, 2019). The most commonly endorsed symptoms were erotic dreams (53.5%), irritability (26.4%) and attention disturbance (26.0%). Moreover, a recent qualitative study that analyzed abstinence journals of male members of an online pornography abstinence forum found that some members reported heightened negative affective states during abstinence, which some interpreted to possibly be withdrawal symptoms (Fernandez et al., 2021). The most commonly self-perceived withdrawal-like symptoms included depression, mood swings, anxiety, ‘brain fog,’ fatigue, headache, insomnia, restlessness, loneliness, frustration, irritability, stress and decreased motivation. These reports suggest that the possible existence of a pornography withdrawal syndrome warrants further investigation.

Second, the presence or absence of withdrawal-related symptoms in relation to PPU will inform appropriate classification of the disorder. It is worth noting that withdrawal is not considered a main component of addiction in all definitions of addiction (see, for example, Sussman & Sussman, 2011) and some scholars in the field have argued against including withdrawal in the conceptualization of behavioral addictions (e.g., Starcevic, 2016).1 There has also been a lack of expert consensus about the clinical relevance of withdrawal for specific behavioral addictions such as gaming disorder (Castro-Calvo et al., 2021), and withdrawal has notably not been included as a diagnostic criterion for either gaming disorder or CSBD in the ICD-11. Nonetheless, withdrawal remains an important part of key existing nosological and theoretical conceptualizations of behavioral addictions in the field. For example, withdrawal is listed as a diagnostic criterion in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5), for the first officially recognized behavioral addiction, gambling disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Furthermore, in Griffiths’ (2005) aforementioned components model of addiction, withdrawal is one of six components that need to be present before a behavior can be diagnosed as an addiction. In sum, while withdrawal may not be necessary component of addiction in all models, the presence of withdrawal symptoms would provide additional support for potentially characterizing PPU as an addictive disorder depending on how addiction is operationalized.

Third, withdrawal is already an important component of existing theoretical conceptualizations of PPU within the empirical literature. A recent systematic review of PPU psychometric instruments found that items assessing withdrawal were included in nine of 22 instruments reviewed (Fernandez & Griffiths, 2021). One of these instruments, the Problematic Pornography Consumption Scale (PPCS; Bőthe et al., 2018, 2021b), was constructed based on Griffiths’ (2005) framework and has become a widely used measure in PPU research. Recent studies on the PPCS using network analysis have found withdrawal to be one of the central symptoms of PPU in an online sample of Hungarian men (Bőthe et al., 2020a) and the most crucial symptom of PPU among three separate samples (i.e., two community samples of Hungarian and Chinese men, and a subclinical sample of Chinese men—Chen et al., 2021). This suggests that withdrawal is a key component in the theoretical conceptualization of PPU and merits greater research attention.

Finally, it is worth acknowledging that negative psychological effects of abstinence may not necessarily be interpreted within an addiction framework as withdrawal symptoms. For example, heightened negative affect during abstinence could instead reflect normal, non-pathological reactions to sexual deprivation (Castro-Calvo et al., 2022) or a disruption of routine particularly if abstinence is involuntary or not intrinsically motivated (King et al., 2016; Szabo, 1995). Alternatively, anxiety-related effects during abstinence could be interpreted within an obsessive–compulsive framework as a consequence of being prevented from engaging in the compulsive behavior to relieve anxiety-evoking obsessive thoughts (Coleman, 1990). However, irrespective of the specific theoretical interpretation of negative abstinence effects, understanding the types of adverse reactions typically experienced by pornography users during abstinence would still be useful from a clinical standpoint as these symptoms can then become potential treatment targets in PPU interventions (Bőthe et al., 2020a).

Methodological Limitations of Problematic Pornography Use Withdrawal Studies

To date, studies investigating withdrawal in PPU have methodological limitations that make them less than ideal for determining whether actual withdrawal symptoms manifest during periods of abstinence. Retrospective reports of negative abstinence effects during prior abstinence attempts as reported in cross-sectional surveys (e.g., Dwulit & Rzymski, 2019) are subject to recall bias (Hughes, 2007b). PPU self-report instruments that have included withdrawal items (e.g., “I became agitated when I was unable to watch porn” in the PPCS; Bőthe et al., 2018) are inherently limited if respondents typically have unrestricted access to pornography and/or never try to abstain from pornography because withdrawal symptoms, by definition, only arise under abstinence conditions (Fernandez et al., 2020; Kaptsis et al., 2016b).

Therefore, in order to properly investigate the potential manifestation of withdrawal, prospective studies of abstinence effects, preferably using intensive longitudinal methods such as ecological momentary assessment or daily diaries, are needed (Fernandez et al., 2020). Apart from observing naturally occurring abstinence situations (e.g., intrinsic quit attempts), abstinence from pornography can be experimentally manipulated to examine its effects. To date, three studies have experimentally manipulated pornography abstinence, but these have focused on its effects on relationship commitment (Lambert et al., 2012), delay discounting (Negash et al., 2016) and perceived compulsivity (Fernandez et al., 2017) rather than withdrawal-related symptoms.

The Present Study

To address the aforementioned gaps in the literature, the present study used a randomized controlled trial (RCT) design to examine effects of a 7-day experimentally manipulated pornography abstinence period on withdrawal-related symptoms among regular pornography users. A 7-day abstinence period was chosen on the basis that the onset and peak of most substance withdrawal syndromes typically occur within the first 7 days of abstinence (Budney et al., 2003; Hughes et al., 1994; McGregor et al., 2005; Shmulewitz et al., 2015). Therefore, it can be reasonably extrapolated that withdrawal symptoms of potential behavioral addictions (if any exist) are also likely to begin manifesting within the first 7 days of abstinence. Participants were randomly assigned to one of two conditions: (1) an abstinence group, where they were given instructions to try their best to abstain from pornography for 7 days, or (2) a control group, where they were told that they are free to use pornography as usual. Outcome measures were assessed at baseline the night before the start of the experimental period (retrospectively about the past 7 days) and each night of the experimental period using end-of-day online surveys.

Three potential manifestations of withdrawal-related symptoms were examined in the present study. First, craving for pornography was examined in light of findings from a recent systematic review that craving was the most common abstinence effect across multiple behaviors including gambling, gaming, mobile phone use and social media use (Fernandez et al., 2020). Therefore, it would be logical to examine whether craving is also an abstinence effect for pornography use. Second, positive and negative affect was examined because affective disturbances are common self-perceived withdrawal-like symptoms reported by pornography users (Dwulit & Rzymski, 2019; Fernandez et al., 2021). Increased negative affect and/or decreased positive affect may be a possible manifestation of withdrawal, in line with previous prospective studies of abstinence from substances such as nicotine (e.g., Hughes, 2007a; Klemperer et al., 2021) and behaviors such as exercise (Fernandez et al., 2020). Third, withdrawal symptoms as assessed by an adaptation of the Wisconsin Smoking Withdrawal Scale (Welsch et al., 1999) for pornography use were examined because retrospective survey and qualitative research has shown that the more commonly self-reported withdrawal-like symptoms by pornography users (i.e., depressed mood, irritability/frustration, anxiety and difficulty in concentrating—Dwulit & Rzymski, 2019; Fernandez et al., 2021) overlap to a considerable extent with symptoms of cigarette-smoking withdrawal (Hughes, 2007a).

A non-clinical sample of regular pornography users was used instead of a clinical sample (e.g., treatment-seeking individuals) because withdrawal-like symptoms have been shown to manifest during abstinence from other behaviors (i.e., exercise, social media use, mobile phone use and gaming) even for regular users who have no apparent indication of problematic use (Fernandez et al., 2020). Several authors (e.g., Fernandez et al., 2020; Kaptsis et al., 2016a) have also recommended against using clinical samples in abstinence studies until any possible negative abstinence effects are first understood in less complex populations. Nonetheless, within a non-clinical sample of regular users, it would be expected that those with higher self-reported levels of PPU would experience more pronounced negative abstinence effects, if any.

Taken together, the present study sought to answer the following two broad research questions: (1) Do negative abstinence effects (potentially indicative of withdrawal-related symptoms) manifest when regular pornography users try to abstain from pornography for a 7-day period? and (2) Do these negative abstinence effects only manifest (or manifest more strongly) for those with higher levels of PPU? If a pornography withdrawal syndrome were assumed to exist, the following hypotheses would be expected to be supported:

H1

There are significant main effects of group (abstinence vs. control) on craving, positive affect, negative affect and withdrawal symptoms, controlling for baseline scores. More specifically, compared to the control group, the abstinence group are expected to score significantly higher on craving, negative affect and withdrawal symptoms, and significantly lower on positive affect.

H2

There is a significant group × PPU interaction on craving, positive affect, negative affect and withdrawal symptoms, controlling for baseline scores. More specifically, it is predicted that abstinence effects either (a) only manifest for those with higher PPU and not for those with lower PPU, or (b) manifest more strongly for those with higher PPU compared to those with lower PPU.

Method

Participants

Participants were psychology undergraduate students at a university in Malaysia. An a priori power analysis using the pwr.f2.test function in R determined that to detect an effect size of f2 = 0.06 (Cohen’s d = 0.5), assuming alpha = 0.05, power = 0.8 and numerator degrees of freedom = 2, a sample size of 164 participants was required. Cohen’s d = 0.5 was set as the smallest effect size of interest (Lakens, 2022) for the present study because it has been specified in the literature to be the smallest difference individuals are able to detect in health-related quality of life outcomes (Norman et al., 2003).

Eligibility criteria for participation included: (1) being at least 18 years old and (2) being a regular pornography user, operationally defined here as having watched pornography at least three times a week in the 4 weeks leading up to the study start date. While this definition of “regular” pornography use is arbitrary, it was reasoned that this minimum frequency was needed for a 7-day abstinence period to represent a significant enough deprivation of participants’ weekly pornography use habits to be able to induce potential withdrawal symptoms. A custom definition of pornography adapted and modified from definitions found in the past literature (Grubbs et al., 2019a; McKee et al., 2020) was provided for participants as follows: “any sexually explicit films, video clips or pictures which intend to sexually arouse the viewer; this may be seen on the internet, in a magazine, in a book, or on television.” While males report higher rates of pornography use (e.g., Hald, 2006; Kvalem et al., 2014; Regnerus et al., 2016) and PPU (e.g., Grubbs et al., 2019a; Kor et al., 2014) than females, there is no evidence to suggest that withdrawal symptoms, if any exist, would manifest only for male pornography users. Previous cross-sectional research found that both males and females recalled experiencing withdrawal-like symptoms during a past cessation attempt (Dwulit & Rzymski, 2019). Therefore, there was no gender restriction for participating in the present study.

A total of 184 students signed up for the study and were assessed for eligibility through their responses to questions on the baseline survey. Eight participants were excluded at this stage because they did not meet the requirement of being a regular pornography user. A total of 176 participants were randomized, resulting in 86 participants allocated to the abstinence group and 90 participants allocated to the control group. No participants failed to complete any daily survey post-randomization (144 completed all seven surveys, 23 completed six surveys, six completed five surveys, two completed four surveys, and one completed three surveys). Daily survey completion rate across the 7 days ranged from 92.2% to 100.0%. An intention-to-treat strategy (Gupta, 2011) to the analysis was applied. Therefore, all randomized participants were retained for data analysis. The final sample comprised 176 participants (64.2% female, 34.7% male, 1.1% agender, Mage = 21.38 years, SD = 1.50). Figure 1 depicts a Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) flow chart detailing the flow of participants through each stage of the study.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

CONSORT flow diagram

Baseline characteristics of the sample are presented in Table S1 in Supplementary Material. At baseline, 48.9% of participants reported desire to reduce but not completely quit their pornography use, 40.9% of participants reported no desire to reduce nor quit their pornography use, and 10.2% reported desire to completely quit their pornography use. In line with CONSORT best practice recommendations for RCTs (Altman et al., 2001), significance tests of baseline differences between the groups were not conducted as due to the randomization procedure, any potential observed baseline differences would be attributable to chance (de Boer et al., 2015; Harvey, 2018).

It is important to highlight that Malaysia, where the present study was conducted, is a sexually conservative country with restrictive laws regulating pornography use (e.g., access to pornographic websites is blocked by the government) and with religious and cultural values that may influence perception of pornography use as immoral or a sin (Goh et al., 2019; Tan et al., 2022). Despite this conservatism, empirical research has shown that pornography use is not uncommon among university students in Malaysia (Ali et al., 2021). Of note, it appears that most participants in the present sample had relatively liberal attitudes toward pornography use as indicated by the low rate of agreement in the baseline survey with the statement “I believe that pornography use is morally wrong” (i.e., 67.6% disagreed at least to some extent, 18.2% neither agreed nor disagreed, and only 14.2% agreed at least to some extent). Nonetheless, due to the conservative sociocultural context, the possibility of additional self-selection bias and social desirability bias (potentially leading to underreporting of pornography use and sexual behaviors) among participants needs to be acknowledged.

Procedure

The study was advertised to prospective participants as “The pornography abstinence experiment: An 8-day daily diary study of regular pornography users.” Participants were informed that the purpose of the study would be to track the daily experiences of regular pornography users who will be randomly assigned either to an “abstinence” group or a “no abstinence” group.2 Three separate 8-day sessions of the study were conducted over the course of 2 months in 2021 (one in February and two in March). All three sessions began on a Monday and ended on the following Monday (see Fig. 2). Participants were required to complete a daily survey each night during the 8-day period (including the baseline survey on the first Monday). At 8.00 pm each night of the 8-day period, online Qualtrics survey links were emailed to participants. Participants were asked to complete the surveys at the end of their day, as close as possible before they went to bed. The survey links expired at 5.00 am the next morning to ensure that participants did not complete the surveys after waking.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Timeline of the 8-day study period

After completing the baseline survey, participants were randomly allocated to the abstinence and control groups by the Qualtrics randomizer function, which was set to assign a roughly equal number of participants to each condition.3 Participants in the abstinence group received instructions to try their best to abstain from pornography for the next 7 days, starting from 5.00 am the next morning (Tuesday) to 5.00 am the following Tuesday morning (exactly 7 days). Participants were told that if they did watch pornography during this period, to report it honestly during the daily surveys, and that it would not have an effect on any compensation they were entitled to receive. They were also told that they were allowed to masturbate without pornography or engage in any other non-pornography-related sexual activity during this period. The control group were told that they were free to watch pornography as usual for the next 7 days (see Appendix A in Supplementary Material for full instructions provided to the abstinence and control groups).

As compensation, participants received course credit if they completed at least six of the seven surveys during the experimental period, and in addition received a gift voucher worth MYR 30.00 (approximately 7.00 USD) if they completed all seven surveys. Informed consent was obtained from all participants, and all study procedures were approved by the research team’s university ethics committees.

Measures

All measures used in the present study are provided in their full form in Appendix B in Supplementary Material and are summarized below.

Daily Measures

Daily frequency and/or duration of sexual behavior During each daily survey, five items asked about the frequency and/or duration of sexual behaviors participants engaged in during two separate time windows: (1) after completing last night’s survey and before going to bed and (2) since waking up that day. This was to ensure that any sexual behavior engaged in after each nightly survey was also accounted for. For all five items, daily totals were computed by summing the frequency/duration reported about both time frames that made up one full day: (1) since waking up that day and (2) after completing that night’s survey and before going to bed (reported on the following day’s survey). These five items are summarized as follows:

Daily frequency of pornography use (FPU) Two free response items asked participants to report how many times they had (1) watched pornography without masturbating and (2) watched pornography while masturbating, respectively. Daily FPU was computed by summing the frequencies reported on both items.

Daily duration of pornography use A third free response item asked participants to report (in minutes) how much time in total they had spent watching pornography.

Daily frequency of masturbation without pornography A fourth free response item asked participants to report how many times they had masturbated without watching pornography.

Daily frequency of alternative sexual activity A fifth free response item asked participants to report how many times they had engaged in any sexual activity other than pornography use or masturbation (e.g., oral sex, intercourse).

Daily abstinence effort Daily abstinence effort was assessed using a single item adapted from a previous pornography abstinence study (Fernandez et al., 2017) that asked participants to rate the extent to which they were trying their best to abstain from pornography use since waking up that day, on a 10-point scale (0 = “I did not try at all” to 10 = “I tried my best”).

Daily craving Daily craving was assessed using a modified four-item version of the five-item Penn Alcohol Craving Scale (PACS; Flannery et al., 1999), adapted for pornography use.4 Four PACS items were modified to refer to pornography use instead of alcohol and asked participants to rate their craving for pornography since waking up that day in terms of frequency of thoughts about pornography, intensity of craving at its strongest point, difficulty in resisting pornography and average craving since waking. A fifth item from the original PACS assessing the duration of thoughts of alcohol was excluded on the basis that it would be challenging for participants to distinguish craving frequency from craving duration when reporting on a 24-h period (Hallgren et al., 2018). Responses were indicated on a seven-point scale, with response options differing according to each item. The scale demonstrated excellent internal consistency across all time points (α = 0.90–0.97).

Daily positive and negative affect Daily positive and negative affect was assessed using the 10-item International Positive and Negative Affect Schedule-Short Form (I-PANAS-SF; Thompson, 2007). The I-PANAS-SF comprises two five-item subscales, positive affect and negative affect, which are each composed of a list of adjectives describing positive (e.g., “inspired”) and negative (e.g., “upset”) affective states, respectively. Participants rated the extent to which they felt these affective states since waking up that day on a five-point scale (1 = “very slightly or not at all” to 5 = “extremely”). Internal consistency was very good across all time points for both positive affect (α = 0.80–0.88) and negative affect (α = 0.80–0.86) subscales.

Daily withdrawal symptoms Daily withdrawal symptoms were assessed using a modified 14-item version of the 28-item Wisconsin Smoking Withdrawal Scale (WSWS; Welsch et al., 1999), adapted for pornography use. The 28 original WSWS items are divided into seven subscales (anger, anxiety, concentration, craving, hunger, sadness and sleep). Only items from four of the subscales (anger, anxiety, concentration and sadness) were retained as these mapped onto the more commonly self-reported categories of withdrawal-like symptoms in previous pornography studies (i.e., irritability/frustration, anxiety, difficulty in concentrating and depressed mood; Dwulit & Rzymski, 2019; Fernandez et al., 2021). The modified scale comprised 14 items which were composed of statements describing specific symptoms (e.g., “It has been difficult to think clearly”). Participants rated their agreement with these statements on a five-point scale (0 = “strongly disagree” to 4 = “strongly agree”) based on how they felt in general since waking up that day. A composite score was computed based on the mean score of all the items. The scale demonstrated excellent internal consistency across all time points (α = 0.91–0.93).

Baseline Measures

Demographic information Participants reported demographic information at baseline, including gender, age, sexual orientation, relationship status and nationality.

Past 4-week frequency of pornography use (FPU) One item assessed participants’ FPU over the previous 4 weeks on a seven-point scale (0 = “less than 3 times a week on average” to 6 = “more than once a day on average”).

Past 4-week average duration of pornography use per session One item assessed participants’ average duration of pornography use per session over the previous 4 weeks on a 10-point scale (1 = “5 min or less” to 10 = “3 h or more”).

Past 4-week frequency of masturbation without pornography One item assessed participants’ frequency of masturbation without pornography over the past 4 weeks on a six-point scale (1 = “never” to 6 = “about 7 or more times a week on average”).

Past 4-week percentage of pornography use accompanied by masturbation One item asked participants to estimate what percentage (from 0 to 100) of all their pornography watching sessions in the past 4 weeks was accompanied by masturbation.

Past 7-day frequency of pornography use (FPU) Two free response items asked how many times over the past 7 days participants had (1) watched pornography without masturbating and (2) watched pornography while masturbating, respectively. Past 7-day FPU was computed by summing the frequencies reported on both items.

Past 7-day duration of pornography use Past 7-day duration of pornography use was assessed by one free response item that asked participants to report how much time in total (in minutes) they watched pornography over the past 7 days.

Baseline abstinence effort (past 7 days) Baseline abstinence effort was assessed using the same single item used to assess daily abstinence effort, but asked participants to rate the extent to which they were trying their best to abstain from pornography use over the past 7 days.

Baseline craving (past 7 days) Baseline craving was assessed using the same four PACS items used to assess daily craving, but asked participants to rate their craving for pornography during the previous 7 days. The scale demonstrated very good internal consistency (α = 0.81).

Baseline positive and negative affect (past 7 days) Baseline positive and negative affect was assessed using the 10-item I-PANAS-SF, but asked participants to rate their positive and negative affect during the past 7 days. Internal consistency was fair for both positive affect (α = 0.77) and negative affect (α = 0.77) subscales.

Baseline withdrawal symptoms (past 7 days) Baseline withdrawal symptoms were assessed using the same modified 14-item WSWS used to assess daily withdrawal symptoms, but asked participants to rate their withdrawal symptoms during the past 7 days. The scale demonstrated very good internal consistency (α = 0.89).

Problematic pornography use (PPU) PPU was assessed using the 18-item Problematic Pornography Consumption Scale (PPCS; Bőthe et al., 2018) (α = 0.92). PPCS assesses past 6-month PPU covering six factors: salience, tolerance, mood modification, withdrawal, relapse and conflict (Griffiths, 2005) on a seven-point scale (1 = “never” to 7 = “all the time”). Higher scores indicate greater problematic pornography consumption.

Intrinsic desire to quit or reduce pornography use One item assessed whether or not participants intrinsically desired to quit or reduce their pornography use at the time of the baseline survey. Participants indicated their agreement with one of the following three response options: “I want to reduce my pornography use, but I don’t want to completely quit/stop using pornography,” “I want to completely quit/stop using pornography” or “I have no desire to reduce or completely quit/stop using pornography.”

Moral disapproval of pornography Moral disapproval of pornography was assessed using the single item “I believe that pornography use is morally wrong” used in previous studies (Grubbs et al., 2019a) rated on a seven-point scale (1 = “strongly disagree” to 7 = “strongly agree”).

Validity of Outcome Measures

Since the outcome measures used in the present study (four-item PACS adapted for pornography use, I-PANAS-SF and 14-item WSWS adapted for pornography use) were not developed for the purpose of assessing pornography withdrawal-related symptoms, their structural and construct validity were briefly examined. First, confirmatory factor analyses were conducted on each baseline outcome measure to examine their factor structure in the present sample. Second, associations of the outcome measures with each other and with pornography use variables at baseline were examined to assess their convergent and discriminant validity. Results of these analyses are reported in detail in Appendix C and Tables S2-S5 in Supplementary Material. Overall, the outcome measures appeared to demonstrate adequate structural and construct validity for the purposes of the present study.

Attention Check Measures

To screen for inattentive responding and to encourage attentive responding, attention check items (i.e., “Please select [specific answer] to this question”; Shamon & Berning, 2020) were embedded in the surveys (one in the baseline survey and two in each daily survey). If participants selected the wrong answer on these items, they were alerted with a pop-up message encouraging them to remain attentive throughout the rest of the survey.

Data Analysis

Prior to conducting any substantive analyses, the dataset was screened for careless responding based on several criteria typically used in online survey research. First, examination of responses to the attention check items showed that all participants passed more than half of all attention checks, with 95% of the sample having an attention check pass rate of ≥ 88%. Second, an examination of response time per item indicated that there were no participants who completed any of the surveys too quickly (i.e., quicker than two seconds per item—Huang et al., 2012). Third, long-string analysis indicated that two participants appeared to engage in invariant responding on one of the scales in one of the daily surveys (i.e., providing a string of consistent responses on more than half of the scale—Curran, 2016). However, these two participants had a 100% attention check pass rate across all the surveys. Therefore, when considered together, none of the participants were flagged for careless responding.

All analyses were performed within the R environment (R Core Team, 2021). Due to the nested nature of the data (daily measures [level 1] nested within participants [level 2]) multilevel modeling (MLM) was utilized to examine group differences. Multilevel models model all available data from all participants regardless of missing observations and as such are robust to missing data. MLM is consistent with the intent-to-treat approach as all participants are retained in the analysis (listwise deletion is not required).

Models with continuous data (e.g., craving) were fit using the lmer() function in the lme4 package (Bates et al., 2015) with restricted maximum likelihood (REML) estimation. The p-values were estimated with the lmertest package using Satterthwaite’s degrees of freedom method (Kuznetsova et al., 2017). These models were checked to verify that they met assumptions of linearity, homoscedasticity and normality using a residuals versus fitted values plot, a scale location plot and a normal probability plot, respectively. The plots indicated that these assumptions were met for all models, except for the negative affect model, which indicated deviation from normality. Negative affect was log-transformed to reduce skewness and approximated a normal distribution after transformation. Models with count data (e.g., FPU) were fit using the glmer() function in the lme4 package using a Poisson distribution, while the model with ordinal data (i.e., abstinence effort) was fit using the clmm() function from the ordinal package (Christensen, 2019).

For all models, the maximal random effects structure (Barr et al., 2013) was used, with a random intercept for participant and a random slope for time (i.e., Time | Participant), which allowed each participant to have their own intercept and slope across time (days). Where models failed to converge while using the default optimizer in lme4, models were refit with all available optimizers (Bates et al., 2015). If models converged and produced similar results while using the other optimizers, the non-convergence warnings were regarded as false positives and an alternative optimizer was used instead. If models still failed to converge while using the other optimizers, the random effects structure was simplified to 1 | Participant (by removing the random slope term) to facilitate model convergence.5

For all confirmatory and exploratory analyses,6 all predictors and interactions were first entered into an initial “full” model. Using a backward elimination procedure, reduced models were specified by removing nonsignificant predictors. At each step, the more parsimonious model was compared to the previous model using the anova() function, which computes χ2 statistics representing the differences in deviance between the two models with corresponding p-values based on likelihood ratio test comparisons (Bates et al., 2015). This process was repeated until the best-fitting model was identified for each separate outcome measure. The baseline score of each outcome measure,7 frequency of masturbation without pornography and frequency of alternative sexual activity were controlled for as covariates in all confirmatory and exploratory models. Given the relatively broad definition of regular pornography use in the present study (≥ three times a week), past 4-week FPU was also included as a covariate in all confirmatory models. Results of key best-fitting models are presented in Tables 1 and 2 in Results section, while all model fit and comparison statistics are presented in Tables S8-9, S11-12 and S15-16 in supplementary material.

Table 1.

Multilevel model results for all outcome variables in confirmatory analyses

Outcome variable Fixed effect Estimate (SE) df t p 95% CI Semi-partial R2
Craving Intercept 0.48 (0.97) 169.57 0.49 0.626 − 1.43–2.38
Group − 0.34 (0.50) 165.38 − 0.68 0.498 − 1.32–0.64 0.003
PPU 0.09 (0.02) 164.16 4.45  < 0.001 0.05–0.13 0.108
Baseline craving 0.08 (0.10) 162.89 0.85 0.394 − 0.11–0.27 0.004
Past 4-week FPU 0.28 (0.16) 171.74 1.67 0.096 − 0.05–0.60 0.016
Frequency of masturbation without pornography 0.46 (0.30) 937.60 1.53 0.127 − 0.13–1.04 0.002
Frequency of alternative sexual activity − 0.33 (0.34) 1008.16 − 0.97 0.334 − 0.99–0.34 0.001
Time − 0.06 (0.07) 169.49 − 0.86 0.392 − 0.21–0.08 0.004
Positive affect Intercept 4.92 (0.96) 177.57 5.10  < 0.001 3.03–6.81
Group − 0.34 (0.40) 170.18 − 0.85 0.396 − 1.13–0.45 0.004
Baseline positive affect 0.59 (0.05) 172.79 10.83  < 0.001 0.48–0.69 0.405
Past 4-week FPU − 0.03 (0.12) 174.38 − 0.21 0.832 − 0.27–0.22 0.000
Frequency of masturbation without pornography − 0.23 (0.21) 1042.37 − 1.13 0.260 − 0.64–0.17 0.001
Frequency of alternative sexual activity 0.20 (0.23) 979.97 0.86 0.389 − 0.25–0.64 0.001
Time 0.07 (0.05) 168.00 1.37 0.172 − 0.03–0.17 0.011
Negative affecta Intercept 1.49 (0.08) 172.69 18.19  < 0.001 1.33–1.65
Group 0.00 (0.04) 165.79 0.12 0.906 − 0.07–0.08 0.000
Baseline negative affect 0.05 (0.00) 166.29 11.56  < 0.001 0.04–0.06 0.445
Past 4-week FPU 0.01 (0.01) 168.72 0.80 0.425 − 0.01–0.03 0.004
Frequency of masturbation without pornography − 0.00 (0.02) 1062.73 − 0.19 0.850 − 0.04–0.03 0.000
Frequency of alternative sexual activity 0.02 (0.02) 975.31 0.94 0.347 − 0.02–0.06 0.001
Time − 0.01 (0.00) 167.64 − 2.68 0.008 − 0.02 to − 0.00 0.041
Withdrawal symptoms Intercept 0.35 (0.15) 167.19 2.18 0.031 0.03–0.66
Group 0.10 (0.07) 164.02 1.42 0.157 − 0.04–0.24 0.012
PPU 0.01 (0.00) 165.11 2.61 0.010 0.00–0.01 0.040
Baseline withdrawal symptoms 0.58 (0.05) 169.21 11.40  < 0.001 0.48–0.68 0.434
Past 4-week FPU − 0.02 (0.02) 168.35 − 1.03 0.305 − 0.07–0.02 0.006
Frequency of masturbation without pornography 0.02 (0.04) 1023.51 0.56 0.576 − 0.05–0.10 0.000
Frequency of alternative sexual activity − 0.04 (0.04) 990.12 − 0.86 0.389 − 0.12–0.05 0.001
Time − 0.03 (0.01) 172.70 − 3.28 0.001 − 0.05 to − 0.01 0.061

aLog-transformed. All models were random slope models with Time | Participant random effects. The best-fitting models are presented. CI confidence interval, FPU frequency of pornography use, PPU problematic pornography use, SE standard error

Table 2.

Multilevel model results for all outcome variables in exploratory analyses with past 4-week FPU as moderator

Outcome variable Fixed effect Estimate (SE) df t p 95% CI Semi-partial R2
Craving Intercept 5.41 (2.84) 160.01 1.90 0.059 − 0.17–10.98
Group − 7.36 (3.61) 160.97 − 2.04 0.043 − 14.45 to − 0.27 0.025
PPU − 0.04 (0.05) 160.81 − 0.78 0.437 − 0.13–0.06 0.017
Past 4-week FPU − 0.93 (0.79) 158.53 − 1.17 0.244 − 2.48–0.63 0.002
Gender 0.44 (0.57) 160.82 0.77 0.445 − 0.68–1.56 0.004
Baseline craving 0.07 (0.09) 158.10 0.70 0.488 − 0.12–0.25 0.003
Frequency of masturbation without pornography 0.37 (0.30) 905.33 1.26 0.209 − 0.21–0.96 0.002
Frequency of alternative sexual activity − 0.37 (0.34) 997.84 − 1.09 0.277 − 1.03–0.30 0.001
Time − 0.05 (0.07) 170.18 − 0.68 0.496 − 0.19–0.09 0.003
Group × PPU 0.19 (0.06) 161.66 2.93 0.004 0.06–0.32 0.050
Group × past 4-week FPU 1.22 (1.01) 160.69 1.21 0.228 − 0.76–3.19 0.009
PPU × past 4-week FPU 0.03 (0.01) 159.08 2.46 0.015 0.01–0.05 0.013
Group × PPU × past 4-week FPU − 0.04 (0.02) 160.43 − 2.30 0.023 − 0.07 to − 0.01 0.032
Positive affect Intercept 4.65 (1.15) 171.94 4.03 < .0001 2.39–6.91
Group − 0.39 (0.41) 167.43 − 0.95 0.342 − 1.18–0.41 0.005
Gender 0.13 (0.49) 167.14 0.26 0.795 − 0.86–1.08 0.000
Baseline positive affect 0.59 (0.05) 170.30 10.84 < 0.001 0.49–0.70 0.408
Frequency of masturbation without pornography − 0.24 (0.21) 1032.06 − 1.17 0.244 − 0.65–0.16 0.001
Frequency of alternative sexual activity 0.19 (0.23) 969.39 0.86 0.389 − 0.25–0.64 0.001
Past 4-week FPU 0.01 (0.14) 169.09 0.10 0.924 − 0.27–0.29 0.000
Time 0.07 (0.05) 167.82 1.28 0.201 − 0.04–0.17 0.010
Negative affecta Intercept 1.29 (0.10) 163.05 12.50 < 0.001 1.09–1.50
Group 0.01 (0.04) 160.87 0.34 0.736 − 0.06–0.09 0.001
PPU 0.00 (0.00) 161.39 2.09 0.039 0.00–0.00 0.026
Past 4-week FPU 0.01 (0.01) 162.45 1.00 0.318 − 0.01–0.04 0.006
Gender 0.10 (0.05) 161.44 2.19 0.030 0.01–0.20 0.029
Baseline negative affect 0.05 (0.00) 162.00 10.76 < 0.001 0.04–0.06 0.417
Frequency of masturbation without pornography − 0.00 (0.02) 1046.72 − 0.09 0.930 − 0.04–0.04 0.000
Frequency of alternative sexual activity 0.02 (0.02) 964.45 0.86 0.392 − 0.02–0.06 0.001
Time − 0.01 (0.00) 167.42 − 2.61 0.010 − 0.02 to − 0.00 0.039
Withdrawal symptoms Intercept 0.19 (0.18) 162.50 1.04 0.301 − 0.17–0.55
Group 0.10 (0.07) 161.48 1.34 0.181 − 0.04–0.24 0.011
PPU 0.01 (0.00) 162.22 2.75 0.007 0.00–0.01 0.045
Past 4-week FPU − 0.00 (0.03) 163.66 − 0.15 0.885 − 0.05–0.05 0.000
Gender 0.15 (0.09) 161.93 1.75 0.082 − 0.02–0.32 0.019
Baseline withdrawal symptoms 0.57 (0.05) 166.33 10.81 < 0.001 0.46–0.67 0.413
Frequency of masturbation without pornography 0.02 (0.04) 1014.50 0.64 0.521 − 0.05–0.10 0.000
Frequency of alternative sexual activity − 0.04 (0.04) 980.29 − 0.94 0.347 − 0.12–0.04 0.001
Time − 0.03 (0.01) 166.64 − 3.18 0.002 − 0.05 to − 0.01 0.057

aLog-transformed. One participant in the abstinence group and one participant in the control group who identified as “agender” were excluded from these analyses, resulting in N = 174. All models are random slope models with Time | Participant random effects. The best-fitting models are presented. CI confidence interval, FPU frequency of pornography use, PPU problematic pornography use, SE standard error

Effect sizes of individual fixed effects were determined by computing the semi-partial R2 statistic (Jaeger et al., 2017) through the r2beta function in the r2glmm package (Jaeger, 2017) using the Kenward–Roger approach. The semi-partial R2 statistic represents the unique proportion of variance explained by each individual predictor in the model, above and beyond the variance explained by all other predictors in the model. Wherever a final model included a significant interaction, post hoc analyses were conducted to clarify the nature of the interaction using the emmeans package (Lenth, 2021). The emmeans function was used to compute estimated marginal means and conduct pairwise comparisons of interest, while the emmip function was used to create interaction plots using estimated marginal means.

Results

Manipulation Check Analyses

To determine whether participants in the abstinence group adhered to instructions to try their best to abstain from pornography during the experimental period, group differences in abstinence effort, FPU and duration of pornography use during the experimental period were examined (see Table S6 in Supplementary Material). The abstinence group reported significantly greater daily abstinence effort (M = 4.72, SD = 3.77) compared to the control group (M = 0.96, SD = 2.39), controlling for past 7-day abstinence effort (odds ratio = 0.01, z =  −  9.37, p < 0.001). The abstinence group also reported significantly lower daily FPU (M = 0.27, SD = 0.80) compared to the control group (M = 0.93, SD = 1.04), controlling for past 7-day FPU (incidence rate ratio [IRR] = 4.95, z = 9.38, p < 0.001). The abstinence group also reported significantly lower daily duration (in minutes) of pornography use (M = 4.25, SD = 15.30) compared to the control group (M = 16.91, SD = 23.05), controlling for past 7-day duration of pornography use (b = 12.70, t[171.76] = 6.97, p < 0.001). Taken together, these group differences indicate that the abstinence instructions were effective at inducing attempts to abstain from pornography in the abstinence group during the experimental period.

While the majority of participants in the abstinence group (n = 47/86; 54.65%) did not report any pornography use at all during the experimental period, a considerable proportion of participants (n = 39/86; 45.35%) reported using pornography at least once during this period. More specifically, seven participants (8.14%) reported using pornography once, eleven (12.79%) reported using pornography twice, nine reported using pornography three times (10.47%) and twelve (13.95%) reported using pornography four or more times. For these participants, pornography use during the experimental period might be indicative of two possibilities: (1) a lack of adherence to instructions to try their best to abstain from pornography and/or (2) a failure to abstain from pornography despite attempting to do so (i.e., a “lapse”). These participants were not excluded from the analyses for two reasons. First, the intention-to-treat principle states that all randomized participants should be included in analyses according to their originally assigned condition, regardless of protocol adherence in order to maintain comparability between groups obtained through randomization and minimize the risk of bias (Gupta, 2011). Second, the purpose of the abstinence instructions was to induce attempts at abstinence (which is within participants’ control) and not necessarily the achievement of successful nonuse (which may not entirely be within all participants’ control, given the difficulty that may come with trying to regulate habitual pornography use for 7 days). Lapses are common in real-world pornography abstinence attempts (Fernandez et al., 2021), and therefore, emphasizing attempted abstinence over successful nonuse may arguably have greater ecological validity. Furthermore, nicotine abstinence research has demonstrated that excluding participants who relapse from analyses may be counterproductive because these participants are most likely to experience the greatest amount of withdrawal in the first place (see Hughes, 2007b; Piasecki, et al., 2003a, 2003b; Shiffman et al., 2004).

Confirmatory Analyses

Means and standard deviations for all study variables are presented in Table S7 in Supplementary Material.

Results of the MLM analyses (see Table 1) showed no statistically significant main effects of group on craving (b = − 0.34, t[165.38] = − 0.68, p = 0.498, semi-partial R2 = 0.003), positive affect (b = − 0.34, t[170.18] = − 0.85, p = 0.396, semi-partial R2 = 0.004), negative affect (b = 0.00, t[165.79] = 0.12, p = 0.906, semi-partial R2 = 0.000) or withdrawal symptoms (b = 0.10, t[164.02] = 1.42, p = 0.157, semi-partial R2 = 0.012), controlling for baseline scores, past 4-week FPU, frequency of masturbation without pornography and frequency of alternative sexual activity. This means that contrary to H1, there were no significant differences between the abstinence and control groups on any of the outcome measures. The addition of a group × PPU interaction term did not improve model fit (Tables S8-S9 in Supplementary Material) for the craving model (χ2 = 1.35, p = 0.245), positive affect model (χ2 = 1.57, p = 0.210), negative affect model (χ2 = 0.59, p = 0.443) or withdrawal symptoms model (χ2 = 1.50, p = 0.220). This means that contrary to H2, there was no significant interaction between group and PPU on any of the outcome measures. In sum, neither of the confirmatory hypotheses were supported. 

Although not hypothesized a priori, MLM analyses showed a statistically significant main effect of time on both negative affect (b = − 0.01, t[167.64] = − 2.68, p = 0.008, semi-partial R2 = 0.041) and withdrawal symptoms (b = − 0.03, t[172.70] = − 3.28, p = 0.001, semi-partial R2 = 0.061), indicating a decrease in negative affect and withdrawal scores in both groups over the experimental period.

Exploratory Analyses

In considering the lack of support for the confirmatory hypotheses, it was noted that in terms of past 4-week FPU, the large majority of the sample reported using pornography three times a week or four times a week (61.4%), with fewer reporting five times a week or six times a week (19.3%) and once a day or more than once a day (19.3%). This appeared to be contributed to by the large proportion of female participants (64.2%) in the sample, most of whom reported past 4-week FPU of three times a week or four times a week (77.0%), with fewer reporting five times a week or six times a week (13.3%) and once a day or more than once a day (9.7%). This was in contrast to male participants, who had a roughly equal proportion of participants reporting past 4-week FPU of three times a week or four times a week (32.8%), five times a week or six times a week (31.1%) and once a day or more than once a day (36.1%). Examination of baseline characteristics of the sample by gender showed statistically significant gender differences, most notably male participants having significantly higher PPU (F[1,172] = 9.027, p = 0.003), craving (F[1,172] = 7.168, p = 0.008) and past 4-week FPU (F[1,172] = 51.768, p < 0.001) than female participants (see Table S10 in Supplementary Material for a complete summary of baseline characteristics by gender).

On the basis that abstinence effects might manifest only at higher levels of past 4-week FPU, exploratory MLM analyses were run on each outcome measure to account for potential moderating effects of past 4-week FPU. All variables in the confirmatory MLM models were retained, but with past 4-week FPU now specified as a moderator (i.e., three-way interaction [group × PPU × past 4-week FPU] or two-way interaction [group × past 4-week FPU]; see Tables S11 and S12 in Supplementary Material).8 Given that there were important gender differences in baseline characteristics, gender was controlled for as a covariate in these models. MLM results showed that past 4-week FPU played a significant moderating role in the craving model but not the positive affect, negative affect and withdrawal symptoms models (see Table 2).

In the craving model, there was a significant main effect of group (b = − 7.36, t[160.97] = − 2.04, p = 0.043, semi-partial R2 = 0.025), a significant two-way group × PPU interaction (b = 0.19, t[161.66] = 2.93, p = 0.004, semi-partial R2 = 0.050) and a significant two-way PPU × past 4-week FPU use interaction (b = 0.03, t[159.08] = 2.46, p = 0.015, semi-partial R2 = 0.013). However, the main effect and two-way interactions were qualified by a significant three-way group × PPU × past 4-week FPU interaction (b = − 0.04, t[160.43] = − 2.30, p = 0.023, semi-partial R2 = 0.032). To probe the pattern of this three-way interaction, contrasts between abstinence and control groups (see Table S13 in Supplementary Material) were examined at combinations of either high (+ 1 SD) or low (-1 SD) values on the PPCS and six values corresponding to all six response anchors on the past 4-week FPU item (1 = “3 times a week,” 2 = “4 times a week,” 3 = “5 times a week,” 4 = “6 times a week,” 5 = “once a day” and 6 = “more than once a day”). The plot of this interaction is presented in Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Effect of group on craving by PPU level (± 1 SD) at all six levels of past 4-week FPU

At low levels of PPU, there were no significant differences between abstinence and control groups on craving at all levels of past 4-week FPU. However, at high levels of PPU, there were differential effects of group on craving depending on level of past 4-week FPU. More specifically, the control group had significantly higher craving than the abstinence group over the experimental period when PPU level was high and past 4-week FPU was “3 times a week” (b = − 3.38, t[163] = − 3.04, p = 0.016; abstinence group estimated marginal mean [EMM] = 6.19, SE = 0.77 vs. control group EMM = 9.57, SE = 0.90). On the contrary, the abstinence group had significantly higher craving than the control group over the experimental period when PPU level was high and past 4-week FPU was “once a day” (b = 2.65, t[160] = 2.65, p = 0.036; abstinence group EMM = 11.34, SE = 0.79 vs. control group EMM = 8.69, SE = 0.77) or when PPU level was high and past 4-week FPU was “more than once a day” (b = 4.16, t[161] = 3.17, p = 0.016; abstinence group EMM = 12.62, SE = 1.04 vs. control group EMM = 8.47, SE = 0.97). There were no significant differences between abstinence and control groups at high levels of PPU and when past 4-week FPU was “4 times a week,” “5 times a week” or “6 times a week.”

Finally, at the request of a reviewer, a supplementary set of exploratory MLM analyses was run on each outcome measure to rule out the possibility that abstinence effects manifested only at specific time points (e.g., in the middle or toward the end) of the 7-day period. To achieve this, all variables in the previous exploratory MLM models were retained, but with time now specified as a moderator (i.e., three-way interaction [group × PPU × time] or two-way interaction [group × time]; see Tables S14-S16 in Supplementary Material). In all models, both linear and quadratic time terms were specified, to allow for potential nonlinear trends. MLM results indicated no significant group × PPU × (linear/quadratic) time nor group × (linear/quadratic) time interactions in any of the models, suggesting that there were no significant abstinence effects on any of the outcome variables at a specific time point of the 7-day period. Overall, it is important to emphasize that all exploratory analyses in the present study should be interpreted with caution given the limited statistical power in the present study’s sample to detect three-way interaction effects if any exist.

Discussion

The present study used a randomized controlled design to examine whether (1) negative abstinence effects potentially reflective of withdrawal-related symptoms manifest when regular pornography users (defined in the present study as having used pornography ≥ three times a week in the past four weeks) try to abstain from pornography for a 7-day period and (2) these negative abstinence effects only manifest (or manifest more strongly) for those with higher levels of PPU.

Confirmatory Analyses

The results showed that both confirmatory hypotheses (H1 and H2) were not supported. Contrary to the first hypothesis, there were no significant main effects of group (abstinence vs. control) on craving, negative affect, positive affect or withdrawal symptoms during the experimental period, controlling for baseline scores. This indicates that when assessed prospectively in comparison with a non-abstaining control group, no evidence of withdrawal-related symptoms was found for the abstaining participants in this sample. This finding is in contrast to previous cross-sectional research relying on retrospective self-report that found 72.2% of pornography users who had at least one past pornography abstinence attempt recalled experiencing at least one withdrawal-like symptom upon cessation (Dwulit & Rzymski, 2019).

The second hypothesis that negative abstinence effects during the experimental period would only manifest (or manifest more strongly) for those with higher levels of PPU was also not supported, as there were no significant group × PPU interaction effects on all outcome measures. Nonetheless, it is important to interpret this finding in light of the fact that because this was a non-clinical sample of regular pornography users, the range of scores on the PPCS was relatively low [M = 54.72, SD = 18.60, with only 15.3% of the sample having PPCS scores that met or exceeded the suggested clinical cutoff of ≥ 76 in Bőthe et al. (2018)]. This finding does not rule out the possibility that in a sample with higher levels of PPU, moderating effects of PPU might be observed.

An unexpected finding was that both negative affect and withdrawal symptoms decreased over the experimental period for both abstinence and control groups. This decrease appeared to be indicative of the “downward drift” phenomenon (see Gilbert et al., 2019, p. 538), which is the tendency for participants to report progressively fewer negative affect-related symptoms when tested repeatedly, regardless of assignment to treatment or control. This phenomenon has been observed in studies of abstinence from cigarette smoking (Gilbert et al., 2002) and gaming (Evans et al., 2018) and also non-abstinence-related studies (e.g., Sharpe & Gilbert, 1998). While multiple explanations have been proposed for the phenomenon (Arrindell, 2001; Sharpe & Gilbert, 1998), one plausible explanation is that repeated testing (in the present study, the daily surveys) acts as an intervention that facilitates self-monitoring of negative affect and possibly coping mechanisms to deal with the negative affect. This finding further underscores the importance of including a non-abstaining control group in future prospective abstinence studies, as decreases in negative affective symptoms over time by abstaining participants could easily be misattributed to abstinence.

Exploratory Analyses

Importantly, exploratory analyses provided partial support for the possibility that the null findings observed in the confirmatory analyses could have been the result of not accounting for the moderating effects of a third variable (i.e., past 4-week FPU). The majority of the sample (61.4%) reported past 4-week FPU on the lower end of the spectrum (i.e., three or four times a week), in large part contributed to by the large proportion of female participants in the sample (64.2%) most of whom also reported past 4-week FPU of three or four times a week (77.0%). Consistent with past research (Chen et al., 2018; Grubbs et al., 2019c; Weinstein et al., 2015), female participants in this sample had lower baseline rates of FPU, PPU and craving than male participants. Therefore, the possibility that abstinence effects might only manifest at higher levels of FPU was examined in exploratory models with past 4-week FPU added as a moderator and gender controlled for as a covariate.

A significant three-way group × PPU × past 4-week FPU interaction on craving was found, but past 4-week FPU did not play a significant moderating role in the negative affect, positive affect and withdrawal symptoms models. At high levels of PPU (+ 1 SD), the control group had higher craving scores than the abstinence group over the experimental period when past 4-week FPU was “three times a week,” but inversely the abstinence group had higher craving scores than the control group over the experimental period when past 4-week FPU was “once a day” or “more than once a day.” Notably, this indicates that there was an abstinence effect on craving when PPU was high, but only once past 4-week FPU reached the threshold of daily use. It is noteworthy that this effect was found even though high PPU at + 1 SD (i.e., M = 72.82) in this sample did not reach the clinical cutoff of ≥ 76 specified in Bőthe et al. (2018). This finding is similar to a previous cross-sectional survey of undergraduates in Canada that found an increase in addiction-related symptomatology once FPU reached the threshold of daily use (Harper & Hodgins, 2016). It is uncertain as to why the abstinence group reported significantly lower craving than the control group at high levels of PPU and past 4-week FPU of ‘three times a week,’ but one speculative explanation could be that abstaining participants below a specific threshold of FPU (i.e., three times a week) due to lower habit strength (Sirriani & Vishwanath, 2016) had enough self-regulation such that they were able to successfully implement strategies to regulate their experience of craving (e.g., prevent its occurrence or reduce its intensity whenever it did emerge) to help them achieve the abstinence goal.

There are two possible interpretations of the abstinence effect on craving under high PPU and high FPU conditions that are not necessarily mutually exclusive. First, if negative abstinence effects are interpreted within an addiction framework as withdrawal-related symptoms, then craving could potentially be a withdrawal symptom in PPU. High FPU on its own (without high PPU) may be indicative of preoccupation with pornography that may not be associated with negative life consequences (Bőthe et al., 2020a, 2020b), but the fact that craving manifested for those with daily or more FPU only when PPU was high reinforces the idea that abstinence-induced craving could be reflective of a dependency on pornography and potentially a withdrawal symptom (if a pornography withdrawal syndrome exists). This interpretation would be consistent with a finding from a recent systematic review that craving was the most common abstinence effect across multiple potential behavioral addictions (Fernandez et al., 2020). Second, craving could also have been a manifestation of sexual desire and/or arousal during the experimental period. Because higher baseline rates of FPU could have to some extent been reflective of higher sexual drive (Leonhardt et al., 2021), the fact that craving only manifested for those with high PPU when FPU was daily or more supports the sexual desire explanation. If participants’ primary sexual outlet was masturbating to pornography, then urges to use pornography could be a natural manifestation of sexual desire and/or arousal throughout the deprivation period (Castro-Calvo et al., 2022). The fact that an abstinence effect was found for craving and not the other outcome variables (i.e., positive affect, negative affect and withdrawal symptoms) further lends support to this non-pathological interpretation, because affect-related disturbances would arguably be expected to also be present if an actual withdrawal syndrome exists. At the same time, the lack of abstinence effects on these other outcome variables needs to be interpreted with caution because given the limited statistical power in the sample to detect three-way interaction effects, similar but smaller effects may not have been detected for these variables. Overall, all findings in these exploratory analyses should be interpreted with caution because of their post hoc exploratory nature and limited statistical power to detect effects of interest. While these exploratory findings are noteworthy, they should be regarded as hypothesis-generating for future studies.

Implications

In sum, the findings of the present study have two main implications for understanding the potential manifestation of withdrawal-related symptoms during pornography abstinence. First, confirmatory analyses showed that there was no evidence of negative abstinence effects (i.e., withdrawal-related symptoms) among a sample of pornography users who were using pornography at least three times a week, and this was not dependent on level of PPU (but with the caveat that the sample had relatively low levels of PPU). These null findings are important to emphasize as they provide preliminary evidence that the average regular pornography user who uses pornography somewhat regularly (i.e., a few times a week) generally does not experience withdrawal-like symptoms while trying to abstain from pornography for a 7-day period.

Second, exploratory findings raise the possibility that negative abstinence effects might manifest only when both baseline FPU and PPU are high—a hypothesis that needs to be tested in future prospective studies. Exploratory analyses found that craving was an abstinence effect when PPU was high, but only once past 4-week FPU was at least daily or more. This tentatively suggests that craving could potentially be a PPU withdrawal symptom if a pornography withdrawal syndrome exists, but future adequately powered studies need to verify this finding a priori and rule out alternative theoretical explanations (e.g., craving being solely a manifestation of sexual arousal). While craving was the only significant abstinence effect under these conditions, it cannot be ruled out that adequately powered studies might find other abstinence effects.

Overall, it is crucial that the findings of the present study be considered in terms of its specific sample characteristics (i.e., non-clinical, majority female sample of undergraduates from a sexually conservative country, most of whom were using pornography 3–4 times a week [61.4%], had PPCS scores below the clinical cutoff of 76 [84.7%] and had no intrinsic desire to quit their pornography use [89.8%]). These findings may not generalize to clinical samples, non-clinical samples with higher FPU or PPU, predominantly male samples, samples from more sexually liberal countries or samples composed solely of pornography users intrinsically motivated to quit their pornography use. Future studies using similar prospective designs and diverse samples with varying gender ratios and levels of FPU, PPU and intrinsic motivation to abstain from pornography use are needed to replicate and extend these findings.

It is also important to consider when interpreting the present study’s findings that the study period (February–March 2021) was during the third wave of the COVID-19 pandemic in Malaysia, when movement control and social distancing measures were enforced throughout the country (Rampal & Liew, 2021; Zamri et al., 2021). This could have potentially influenced the present study’s findings in two ways. First, participants’ self-reported affective states during the study period could potentially have been impacted by elevated psychological distress levels brought about by the pandemic (Marzo et al., 2021; Necho et al., 2021). Second, participants’ sexual lives could have also been affected by the pandemic. Frequency of partnered sex among participants may have decreased due to reduced access to potential sexual partners (Herbenick et al., 2022). While some longitudinal data have shown that as a general trend, FPU and PPU did not increase over time during the pandemic, this might not necessarily have been the case for all pornography users (Grubbs et al., 2022; Koós et al., 2022). For example, there has been evidence of retrospective self-reports, particularly among male participants, of increased FPU and frequency of masturbation during the pandemic when compared to pre-pandemic frequencies (Gleason et al., 2021; Sallie et al., 2021). It is plausible that some participants in the present study may have developed an increased reliance on masturbating to pornography as a sexual outlet in the absence of opportunities for partnered sex during stay-at-home orders, which could have made abstaining from pornography more challenging than usual. In sum, it is important to take into account that the present study’s findings may not necessarily generalize to a post-pandemic setting.

Limitations and Directions for Future Research

The present study has several limitations that need to be highlighted. First, because the study’s aim was to investigate abstinence effects irrespective of gender, there were no gender restrictions in the inclusion criteria, resulting in a sample that was close to two-thirds (64.2%) female. Compared to females, males tend to use pornography more for sexual pleasure (Bőthe et al., 2021a; Grubbs et al., 2019c) and have a stronger sex drive in general (Baumeister et al., 2001). As such, males may be more reliant on pornography as a sexual outlet than females. Therefore, it may be reasonable to speculate that males could have greater difficulty in abstaining from pornography than females even if they have similar rates of baseline FPU. However, because females had lower past 4-week FPU than males in the present sample, exploratory analyses with gender as moderator were not run because they would have been difficult to disentangle from exploratory analyses with past 4-week FPU as moderator. Future studies could consider restricting inclusion criteria to just male participants to examine whether abstinence effects manifest in a male-only sample or run adequately powered studies with both genders included (but with narrower inclusion criteria for baseline FPU [e.g., ≥ six times a week]) to investigate potential varying abstinence effects by gender, if any. Differing contexts of pornography use across genders (e.g., women in relationships are more likely than men to use pornography primarily or only with their partner; Carroll et al., 2017) could also be accounted for as a potential moderator of effects in future studies.

Second, nearly half (45.4%) of participants in the abstinence group reported using pornography at least once during the experimental period, which could have led to a mitigation in the frequency and/or intensity of any withdrawal-related symptoms experienced for these participants. While excluding participants who lapse from analyses may introduce bias because these participants are likely to experience the greatest amount of withdrawal in the first place (Hughes, 2007b; Piasecki et al., 2003a, 2003b; Shiffman et al., 2004), it is important to keep in mind that withdrawal-related symptom scores of the abstinence group as a whole may have been influenced by the considerable proportion of participants who did lapse in the present study.

Third, because the focus of the present study was to examine effects of abstinence from pornography specifically, participants in the abstinence group were allowed to masturbate without pornography or engage in non-pornography-related sexual activity. An advantage of allowing non-pornography-related sexual outlets is that potential manifestations of withdrawal-like symptoms can be more clearly attributed to a dependency on pornography use instead of dependency on masturbation or sexual activity (e.g., if withdrawal-like symptoms persist even after masturbating without pornography). However, a drawback of this is that it may not resemble many real-world abstinence attempts, given that many individuals with PPU may decide to abstain from pornography and masturbation or abstain from sexual activity altogether as a short-term recovery strategy (Fernandez et al., 2021). Future studies could consider modifying abstinence instructions so that they also include masturbation or sexual activity altogether.

Fourth, while the experimental design used in the present study contributed to rigorous internal validity due to randomization and use of a control group, experimental designs are inherently limited in the extent to which the studies’ results are generalizable to the wider population (because of the use of a relatively small, homogenous sample) and applicable to real-world situations (because of the emphasis on controlled conditions that prioritize internal validity over ecological validity). Importantly, abstinence in the present study may not be representative of real-world abstinence attempts because participants knew that abstinence would only last for a temporary 7-day period, they could lapse without consequence, and they could terminate their participation at any moment. Non-experimental longitudinal studies of intrinsic abstinence attempts are needed to supplement data provided by studies that experimentally manipulate abstinence.

Fifth, because it is plausible that a pornography withdrawal syndrome, akin to the withdrawal syndromes of some substances (cf. Hughes et al., 1994), might only begin or worsen after a significant period of abstinence (e.g., one or two weeks), the fairly brief duration of the present study (i.e., 7 days) meant that any potential withdrawal symptoms that might have emerged beyond an initial 7-day period could not be captured. Future studies could consider increasing the duration of the abstinence period to investigate this possibility.

Sixth, the outcome measures used to assess potential pornography withdrawal-related symptoms in the present study were a limitation because they were modified from existing scales constructed to assess alcohol craving (Flannery et al., 1999), smoking withdrawal symptoms (Welsch et al., 1999) and general affect (Thompson, 2007). Measures of pornography-specific withdrawal-related symptoms need to be developed and validated for use in future abstinence studies.

Seventh, despite having a significantly shorter recall period compared to a single end-of-week assessment, end-of-day surveys still rely on retrospection and as such remain susceptible to some recall bias (Newman & Stone, 2019). Research comparing aggregated momentary affect ratings throughout the day to retrospective end-of-day affect ratings demonstrates that end-of-day ratings of negative affect tend to be slightly biased toward peak and recent affect (Neubauer et al., 2020). Future studies can use ecological momentary assessment (EMA) instead of daily surveys for greater sensitivity to fluctuations of affect throughout the day.

Eighth, because the baseline measures for all outcome variables had a bigger recall period (past 7 days) compared to the daily measures (past day), changes in daily scores relative to baseline could not be examined. Incorporating a pre-intervention period where baseline data are also collected using the same daily surveys would allow for standardization of measures and examination of changes from baseline.

Finally, the present study did not account for the type or genre of pornography that participants typically used. It is possible that specific types of pornography (e.g., non-mainstream content; Hald et al., 2018) or greater variability of pornography content consumed (Lewczuk et al., 2021) may be associated with increased difficulty in abstaining from pornography use and can be accounted for in future abstinence studies.

Conclusion

Current understanding of the potential manifestation of withdrawal-related symptoms during abstinence from pornography is still in its infancy. The present study is the first to prospectively examine the manifestation of withdrawal-related symptoms in abstaining participants in comparison to a non-abstaining control group, in contrast to previous research that has relied on retrospective self-report of perceived abstinence effects. The present study contributes preliminary data that represent a first step toward understanding whether and under what conditions withdrawal-related symptoms may (or may not) manifest when regular pornography users attempt to abstain from pornography use for a 7-day period. More prospective data across diverse samples are needed to verify and expand upon the findings of the present study.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Funding

This research was supported by a HELP University IRGS Grant (Grant No. 21-01-002) awarded to the fourth author.

Declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Footnotes

1

Primary reasons cited include: (1) withdrawal not being necessary for the diagnosis of addiction more broadly (as the cessation of some substances does not produce a withdrawal syndrome—Starcevic, 2016) and (2) methodological weaknesses in the assessment of withdrawal in behavioral addictions (e.g., relying on individuals’ subjective complaints of feeling irritable instead of psychophysiological measurement) has meant that the evidence base supporting the existence of behavioral addiction withdrawal syndromes has been argued to be weak (Kardefelt-Winther et al., 2017; Pies, 2009; van Rooij & Prause, 2014).

2

Prospective participants were informed about the nature of the random assignment from the outset because a willingness to attempt abstinence from pornography if assigned to the abstinence group was necessary for participation. While this may have led to demand characteristics, it was deemed unethical to withhold this information from prospective participants.

3

Because the control group had been randomly allocated one or two participants more than the abstinence group in all three surveys corresponding to the three study sessions (i.e., February session [67 vs. 65], first March session [16 vs. 15], second March session [7 vs. 6]), this added up to a slightly higher total number of participants in the control group than the abstinence group (90 vs. 86).

4

The PACS has also been adapted as a measure of gaming withdrawal symptomatology in previous gaming abstinence studies (Evans et al., 2018; Katpsis et al., 2016a).

5

Specific models that had non-convergence issues and how these were addressed are detailed in Tables S6, S11 and S15.

6

Confirmatory analyses refer to analyses that tested the present study’s a priori hypotheses, while exploratory analyses refer to analyses that did not test a priori hypotheses but were conducted in a post-hoc, exploratory capacity.

7

Because the baseline score of each outcome measure represented a retrospective estimation of how participants felt in general over the past seven days, whether daily scores increased or decreased relative to baseline was not examined. The tendency for individuals to overestimate the intensity of positive and negative affect when recalling past affect is a robust finding in the literature (Ben-Zeev et al., 2009; Kardum & Daskijević, 2001; Thomas & Diener, 1990). Therefore, the baseline scores would be expected to be inflated relative to the daily scores and as such were only controlled for as covariates.

8

All exploratory models in the present study followed the same backward elimination procedure as the confirmatory models, where more complex models were compared to reduced models until the best-fitting model was found for each outcome measure.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  1. Ali AAZ, Muhammad NA, Jamil TR, Ahmad S, Abd Aziz NA. Internet pornography exposures amongst young people in Malaysia: A cross-sectional study looking into the role of gender and perceived realism versus the actual sexual activities. Addictive Behaviors Reports. 2021;14:100350. doi: 10.1016/j.abrep.2021.100350. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Altman DG, Schulz KF, Moher D, Egger M, Davidoff F, Elbourne D, Gøtzsche PC, Lang T. The revised CONSORT statement for reporting randomized trials: Explanation and elaboration. Annals of Internal Medicine. 2001;134(8):663–694. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-134-8-200104170-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing
  4. Arrindell WA. Changes in waiting-list patients over time: data on some commonly-used measures. Beware! Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2001;39(10):1227–1247. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00104-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barr DJ, Levy R, Scheepers C, Tily HJ. Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language. 2013;68(3):255–278. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Barth RJ, Kinder BN. The mislabeling of sexual impulsivity. Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy. 1987;13(1):15–23. doi: 10.1080/00926238708403875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software. 2015;67(1):1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  8. Baumeister RF, Catanese KR, Vohs KD. Is there a gender difference in strength of sex drive? Theoretical views, conceptual distinctions, and a review of relevant evidence. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2001;5(3):242–273. doi: 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0503_5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Ben-Zeev D, Young MA, Madsen JW. Retrospective recall of affect in clinically depressed individuals and controls. Cognition and Emotion. 2009;23(5):1021–1040. doi: 10.1080/02699930802607937. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  10. Blaszczynski A, Walker M, Sharpe L, Nower LIA. Withdrawal and tolerance phenomenon in problem gambling. International Gambling Studies. 2008;8(2):179–192. doi: 10.1080/14459790802140007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bőthe B, Koós M, Demetrovics Z. Contradicting classification, nomenclature, and diagnostic criteria of Compulsive Sexual Behavior Disorder (CSBD) and future directions. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. 2022;11(2):204–209. doi: 10.1556/2006.2022.00030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bőthe B, Lonza A, Štulhofer A, Demetrovics Z. Symptoms of problematic pornography use in a sample of treatment considering and treatment non-considering men: A network approach. Journal of Sexual Medicine. 2020;17(10):2016–2028. doi: 10.1016/j.jsxm.2020.05.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bőthe B, Tóth-Király I, Bella N, Potenza MN, Demetrovics Z, Orosz G. Why do people watch pornography? The motivational basis of pornography use. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2021;35(2):172. doi: 10.1037/adb0000603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bőthe B, Tóth-Király I, Demetrovics Z, Orosz G. The short version of the Problematic Pornography Consumption Scale-6: A reliable and valid measure in general and treatment-seeking populations. Journal of Sex Research. 2021;58(3):342–352. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2020.1716205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bőthe B, Tóth-Király I, Potenza MN, Orosz G, Demetrovics Z. High-frequency pornography use may not always be problematic. Journal of Sexual Medicine. 2020;17(4):793–811. doi: 10.1016/j.jsxm.2020.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bőthe B, Tóth-Király I, Zsila Á, Griffiths MD, Demetrovics Z, Orosz G. The development of the Problematic Pornography Consumption Scale. Journal of Sex Research. 2018;55(3):395–406. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2017.1291798. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Briken P, Turner D. What does “sexual” mean in compulsive sexual behavior disorder? Journal of Behavioral Additions. 2022;11(2):222–225. doi: 10.1556/2006.2022.00026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Budney, A. J., Moore, B. A., Vandrey, R. G., & Hughes, J. R. (2003). The time course and significance of cannabis withdrawal. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112(3), 393–402. 10.1037/0021-843X.112.3.393 [DOI] [PubMed]
  19. Carnes PJ. Out of the shadows: Understanding sexual addiction. CompCare; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  20. Carroll, J. S., Busby, D. M., Willoughby, B. J., & Brown, C. C. (2017). The porn gap: Differences in men's and women's pornography patterns in couple relationships. Journal of Couple & Relationship Therapy, 16(2), 146–163. 10.1080/15332691.2016.1238796.
  21. Castro-Calvo J, Flayelle M, Perales JC, Brand M, Potenza MN, Billieux J. Compulsive sexual behavior disorder should not be classified by solely relying on component/symptomatic features. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. 2022;11(2):210–215. doi: 10.1556/2006.2022.00029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Castro-Calvo J, King DL, Stein DJ, Brand M, Carmi L, Chamberlain SR, Billieux J. Expert appraisal of criteria for assessing gaming disorder: An international Delphi study. Addiction. 2021;116(9):2463–2475. doi: 10.1111/add.15411. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Chen L, Ding C, Jiang X, Potenza MN. Frequency and duration of use, craving and negative emotions in problematic online sexual activities. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity. 2018;25(4):396–414. doi: 10.1080/10720162.2018.1547234. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  24. Chen L, Luo X, Bőthe B, Jiang X, Demetrovics Z, Potenza MN. Properties of the Problematic Pornography Consumption Scale-18 in community and subclinical samples in China and Hungary. Addictive Behaviors. 2021;112:106591. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Christensen, R. H. B. (2019). ordinal—Regression models for ordinal data. R package version 2019.12–10. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=ordinal
  26. Coleman E. The obsessive-compulsive model for describing compulsive sexual behavior. American Journal of Preventive Psychiatry and Neurology. 1990;2(3):9–14. [Google Scholar]
  27. Coleman E. Compulsive sexual behavior: New concepts and treatments. Journal of Psychology & Human Sexuality. 1991;4(2):37–52. doi: 10.1300/J056v04n02_04. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. Curran PG. Methods for the detection of carelessly invalid responses in survey data. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2016;66:4–19. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2015.07.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. de Alarcón R, de la Iglesia JI, Casado NM, Montejo AL. Online porn addiction: What we know and what we don’t—A systematic review. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2019;8(1):91. doi: 10.3390/jcm8010091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. de Boer, M. R., Waterlander, W. E., Kuijper, L. D., Steenhuis, I. H., & Twisk, J. W. (2015). Testing for baseline differences in randomized controlled trials: An unhealthy research behavior that is hard to eradicate. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity,12(1). 10.1186/s12966-015-0162-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  31. Dwulit AD, Rzymski P. Prevalence, patterns and self-perceived effects of pornography consumption in Polish university students: A cross-sectional study. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2019;16(10):1861. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16101861. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Evans C, King DL, Delfabbro PH. Effect of brief gaming abstinence on withdrawal in adolescent at-risk daily gamers: A randomized controlled study. Computers in Human Behavior. 2018;88:70–77. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.06.024. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  33. Fernandez DP, Griffiths MD. Psychometric instruments for problematic pornography use: A systematic review. Evaluation and the Health Professions. 2021;44(2):111–141. doi: 10.1177/0163278719861688. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Fernandez DP, Kuss DJ, Griffiths MD. Short-term abstinence effects across potential behavioral addictions: A systematic review. Clinical Psychology Review. 2020;76:101828. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2020.101828. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Fernandez DP, Kuss DJ, Griffiths MD. The pornography “rebooting” experience: A qualitative analysis of abstinence journals on an online pornography abstinence forum. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2021;50(2):711–728. doi: 10.1007/s10508-020-01858-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Fernandez DP, Tee EY, Fernandez EF. Do Cyber Pornography Use Inventory-9 scores reflect actual compulsivity in internet pornography use? Exploring the role of abstinence effort. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity. 2017;24(3):156–179. doi: 10.1080/10720162.2017.1344166. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  37. Flannery BA, Volpicelli JR, Pettinati H. Psychometric properties of the Penn Alcohol Craving Scale. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 1999;23(8):1289–1295. doi: 10.1097/00000374-199908000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Gilbert DG, McClernon FJ, Rabinovich NE, Plath LC, Masson CL, Anderson AE, Sly KF. Mood disturbance fails to resolve across 31 days of cigarette abstinence in women. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2002;70(1):142–152. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.70.1.142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Gilbert DG, Rabinovich NE, Gilbert-Matuskowitz EA, Klein KP, Pergadia ML. Smoking abstinence symptoms across 67 days compared with randomized controls—Moderation by nicotine replacement therapy, bupropion, and negative-affect traits. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2019;27(6):536–551. doi: 10.1037/pha0000278. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Gleason N, Banik S, Braverman J, Coleman E. The impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on sexual behaviors: Findings from a national survey in the United States. Journal of Sexual Medicine. 2021;18(11):1851–1862. doi: 10.1016/j.jsxm.2021.08.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Goh JN, Lewis GL, Leong PP. Sexy media dissidents: State regulation, dissidence, pornography and gay men in Malaysia. Sexuality & Culture. 2019;23(1):192–208. doi: 10.1007/s12119-018-9550-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  42. Gola M, Wordecha M, Sescousse G, Lew-Starowicz M, Kossowski B, Wypych M, Makeig S, Potenza MN, Marchewka A. Can pornography be addictive? An fMRI study of men seeking treatment for problematic pornography use. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2017;42(10):2021–2031. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Griffiths M. A ‘components’ model of addiction within a biopsychosocial framework. Journal of Substance Use. 2005;10(4):191–197. doi: 10.1080/14659890500114359. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Griffiths MD. Disorders due to addictive behaviors: Further issues, debates, and controversies. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. 2022;11(2):180–185. doi: 10.1556/2006.2022.00025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Grubbs JB, Grant JT, Engelman J. Self-identification as a pornography addict: Examining the roles of pornography use, religiousness, and moral incongruence. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity. 2018;25(4):269–292. doi: 10.1080/10720162.2019.1565848. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  46. Grubbs JB, Hoagland KC, Lee BN, Grant JT, Davison P, Reid RC, Kraus SW. Sexual addiction 25 years on: A systematic and methodological review of empirical literature and an agenda for future research. Clinical Psychology Review. 2020;82:101925. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2020.101925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Grubbs JB, Kraus SW. Pornography use and psychological science: A call for consideration. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2021;30(1):68–75. doi: 10.1177/0963721420979594. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  48. Grubbs JB, Kraus SW, Perry SL. Self-reported addiction to pornography in a nationally representative sample: The roles of use habits, religiousness, and moral incongruence. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. 2019;8(1):88–93. doi: 10.1037/adb0000114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Grubbs JB, Perry SL, Grant Weinandy JT, Kraus SW. Porndemic? A longitudinal study of pornography use before and during the COVID-19 pandemic in a nationally representative sample of Americans. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2022;51(1):123–137. doi: 10.1007/s10508-021-02077-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Grubbs JB, Perry SL, Wilt JA, Reid RC. Pornography problems due to moral incongruence: An integrative model with a systematic review and meta-analysis. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2019;48(2):397–415. doi: 10.1007/s10508-018-1248-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Grubbs JB, Wright PJ, Braden AL, Wilt JA, Kraus SW. Internet pornography use and sexual motivation: A systematic review and integration. Annals of the International Communication Association. 2019;43(2):117–155. doi: 10.1080/23808985.2019.1584045. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  52. Gupta SK. Intention-to-treat concept: A review. Perspectives in Clinical Research. 2011;2(3):109–112. doi: 10.4103/2229-3485.83221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Hald GM. Gender differences in pornography consumption among young heterosexual Danish adults. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2006;35(5):577–585. doi: 10.1007/s10508-006-9064-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Hald GM, Stulhofer A, Lange T. Sexual arousal and sexually explicit media (SEM): Comparing patterns of sexual arousal to SEM and sexual self-evaluations and satisfaction across gender and sexual orientation. Sexual Medicine. 2018;6(1):30–38. doi: 10.1016/j.esxm.2017.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Hallgren KA, Delker BC, Simpson TL. Effects of initiating abstinence from alcohol on daily craving and negative affect: Results from a pharmacotherapy clinical trial. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2018;42(3):634–645. doi: 10.1111/acer.13591. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Harper C, Hodgins DC. Examining correlates of problematic internet pornography use among university students. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. 2016;5(2):179–191. doi: 10.1556/2006.5.2016.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Harvey LA. Statistical testing for baseline differences between randomised groups is not meaningful. Spinal Cord. 2018;56(10):919. doi: 10.1038/s41393-018-0203-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Herbenick D, Hensel DJ, Eastman-Mueller H, Beckmeyer J, Fu TC, Guerra-Reyes L, Rosenberg M. Sex and relationships pre-and early-COVID-19 pandemic: Findings from a probability sample of U.S. undergraduate students. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2022;51(1):183–195. doi: 10.1007/s10508-021-02265-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Hilton DL., Jr Pornography addiction—A supranormal stimulus considered in the context of neuroplasticity. Socioaffective Neuroscience & Psychology. 2013;3:20767. doi: 10.3402/snp.v3i0.20767. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Huang JL, Curran PG, Keeney J, Poposki EM, DeShon RP. Detecting and deterring insufficient effort responding to surveys. Journal of Business and Psychology. 2012;27(1):99–114. doi: 10.1007/s10869-011-9231-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  61. Hughes JR. Effects of abstinence from tobacco: Valid symptoms and time course. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. 2007;9(3):315–327. doi: 10.1080/14622200701188919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Hughes JR. Measurement of the effects of abstinence from tobacco: A qualitative review. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2007;21(2):127–137. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.21.2.127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Hughes, J. R., Higgins, S. T., & Bickel, W. K. (1994). Nicotine withdrawal versus other drug withdrawal syndromes: Similarities and dissimilarities. Addiction, 89(11), 1461–1470. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1994.tb03744.x [DOI] [PubMed]
  64. Jaeger, B. (2017). r2glmm: Computes R squared for mixed (multilevel) models. R package version 0.1.2. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=r2glmm
  65. Jaeger BC, Edwards LJ, Das K, Sen PK. An R2 statistic for fixed effects in the generalized linear mixed model. Journal of Applied Statistics. 2017;44(6):1086–1105. doi: 10.1080/02664763.2016.1193725. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  66. Kafka MP. Hypersexual disorder: A proposed diagnosis for DSM-V. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2010;39(2):377–400. doi: 10.1007/s10508-009-9574-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Kaptsis D, King DL, Delfabbro PH, Gradisar M. Trajectories of abstinence-induced Internet gaming withdrawal symptoms: A prospective pilot study. Addictive Behaviors Reports. 2016;4:24–30. doi: 10.1016/j.abrep.2016.06.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Kaptsis D, King DL, Delfabbro PH, Gradisar M. Withdrawal symptoms in internet gaming disorder: A systematic review. Clinical Psychology Review. 2016;43:58–66. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Kardefelt-Winther D, Heeren A, Schimmenti A, van Rooij A, Maurage P, Carras M, Edman J, Blaszynski A, Khazaal Y, Billieux J. How can we conceptualize behavioural addiction without pathologizing common behaviours? Addiction. 2017;112(10):1709–1715. doi: 10.1111/add.13763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Kardum I, Daskijević KT. Absolute and relative accuracy in the retrospective estimate of positive and negative mood. European Journal of Psychological Assessment. 2001;17(1):69–77. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.17.1.69. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  71. King DL, Kaptsis D, Delfabbro PH, Gradisar M. Craving for Internet games? Withdrawal symptoms from an 84-h abstinence from massively multiplayer online gaming. Computers in Human Behavior. 2016;62:488–494. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.04.020. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  72. Kingston DA. Debating the conceptualization of sex as an addictive disorder. Current Addiction Reports. 2015;2(3):195–201. doi: 10.1007/s40429-015-0059-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  73. Klemperer EM, Hughes JR, Peasley-Miklus CE, Callas PW, Cook JW, Streck JM, Morley NE. Possible new symptoms of tobacco withdrawal III: Reduced positive affect—A review and meta-analysis. Nicotine and Tobacco Research. 2021;23(2):259–266. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntaa044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Koós M, Demetrovics Z, Griffiths MD, Bőthe B. No significant changes in addictive and problematic behaviors during the COVID-19 pandemic and related lockdowns: a three-wave longitudinal study. Frontiers in Psychology. 2022;13:837315. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.837315. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Kor A, Fogel YA, Reid RC, Potenza MN. Should hypersexual disorder be classified as an addiction? Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity. 2013;20(1–2):27–47. doi: 10.1080/10720162.2013.768132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Kor A, Zilcha-Mano S, Fogel YA, Mikulincer M, Reid RC, Potenza MN. Psychometric development of the Problematic Pornography Use Scale. Addictive Behaviors. 2014;39(5):861–868. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.01.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Kraus SW, Krueger RB, Briken P, First MB, Stein DJ, Kaplan MS, Voon V, Abdo CHN, Grant JE, Atalla E, Reed GM. Compulsive sexual behaviour disorder in the ICD-11. World Psychiatry. 2018;17(1):109–110. doi: 10.1002/wps.20499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Kraus SW, Voon V, Potenza MN. Should compulsive sexual behavior be considered an addiction? Addiction. 2016;111(12):2097–2106. doi: 10.1111/add.13297. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Kuznetsova A, Brockhoff PB, Christensen RH. lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software. 2017;82(13):1–26. doi: 10.18637/jss.v082.i13. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  80. Kvalem IL, Træen B, Lewin B, Štulhofer A. Self-perceived effects of internet pornography use, genital appearance satisfaction, and sexual self-esteem among young Scandinavian adults. Cyberpsychology: Journal of Psychosocial Research on Cyberspace. 2014 doi: 10.5817/CP2014-4-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  81. Lakens D. Sample size justification. Collabra Psychology. 2022;8(1):33267. doi: 10.1525/collabra.33267. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  82. Lambert NM, Negash S, Stillman TF, Olmstead SB, Fincham FD. A love that doesn't last: Pornography consumption and weakened commitment to one's romantic partner. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 2012;31(4):410–438. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2012.31.4.410. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  83. Lenth, R. V. (2021). emmeans: Estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means. R package version 1.6.2-1. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=emmeans
  84. Leonhardt ND, Busby DM, Willoughby BJ. Do you feel in control? Sexual desire, sexual passion expression, and associations with perceived compulsivity to pornography and pornography use frequency. Sexuality Research and Social Policy. 2021;18(2):377–389. doi: 10.1007/s13178-020-00465-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  85. Lewczuk K, Lesniak J, Lew-Starowicz M, Gola M. Variability of pornographic content consumed and longest session of pornography use associated with treatment seeking and problematic sexual behavior symptoms. Sexual Medicine. 2021;9(1):100276. doi: 10.1016/j.esxm.2020.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Lew-Starowicz M, Coleman E. Mental and sexual health perspectives of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11) Compulsive Sexual Behavior Disorder. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. 2022;11(2):226–229. doi: 10.1556/2006.2022.00032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Ley D, Prause N, Finn P. The emperor has no clothes: A review of the ‘pornography addiction’ model. Current Sexual Health Reports. 2014;6(2):94–105. doi: 10.1007/s11930-014-0016-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  88. Love T, Laier C, Brand M, Hatch L, Hajela R. Neuroscience of internet pornography addiction: A review and update. Behavioral Sciences. 2015;5(3):388–433. doi: 10.3390/bs5030388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Marzo RR, Ismail Z, Htay MNN, Bahari R, Ismail R, Villanueva EQ, III, Su TT. Psychological distress during pandemic Covid-19 among adult general population: Result across 13 countries. Clinical Epidemiology and Global Health. 2021;10:100708. doi: 10.1016/j.cegh.2021.100708. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. McGregor, C., Srisurapanont, M., Jittiwutikarn, J., Laobhripatr, S., Wongtan, T., & White, J. M. (2005). The nature, time course and severity of methamphetamine withdrawal. Addiction, 100(9), 1320–1329. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2005.01160.x [DOI] [PubMed]
  91. McKee A, Byron P, Litsou K, Ingham R. An interdisciplinary definition of pornography: Results from a global Delphi panel [Technical Report] Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2020;49(3):1085–1091. doi: 10.1007/s10508-019-01554-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Necho M, Tsehay M, Birkie M, Biset G, Tadesse E. Prevalence of anxiety, depression, and psychological distress among the general population during the COVID-19 pandemic: A systematic review and meta-analysis. International Journal of Social Psychiatry. 2021;67(7):892–906. doi: 10.1177/00207640211003121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Negash S, Sheppard NVN, Lambert NM, Fincham FD. Trading later rewards for current pleasure: Pornography consumption and delay discounting. Journal of Sex Research. 2016;53(6):689–700. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2015.1025123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Neubauer AB, Scott SB, Sliwinski MJ, Smyth JM. How was your day? Convergence of aggregated momentary and retrospective end-of-day affect ratings across the adult life span. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2020;119(1):185–203. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Newman DB, Stone AA. Understanding daily life with ecological momentary assessment. In: Kardes F, Herr PM, Schwarz N, editors. Handbook of research methods in consumer psychology. Routledge; 2019. pp. 259–275. [Google Scholar]
  96. Norman GR, Sloan JA, Wyrwich KW. Interpretation of changes in health-related quality of life: The remarkable universality of half a standard deviation. Medical Care. 2003;41(5):582–592. doi: 10.1097/01.MLR.0000062554.74615.4C. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Piasecki TM, Jorenby DE, Smith SS, Fiore MC, Baker TB. Smoking withdrawal dynamics: I. Abstinence distress in lapsers and abstainers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2003;112(1):3–13. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.112.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Piasecki TM, Jorenby DE, Smith SS, Fiore MC, Baker TB. Smoking withdrawal dynamics: II. Improved tests of withdrawal-relapse relations. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2003;112(1):14–27. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.112.1.14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Pies R. Should DSM-V designate “internet addiction” a mental disorder? Psychiatry. 2009;6(2):31–37. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Potenza MN, Gola M, Voon V, Kor A, Kraus SW. Is excessive sexual behaviour an addictive disorder? The Lancet Psychiatry. 2017;4(9):663–664. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30316-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. R Core Team. (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. http://www.R-project.org
  102. Rampal L, Liew BS. Malaysia's third COVID-19 wave-a paradigm shift required. Medical Journal of Malaysia. 2021;76(1):1–4. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Regnerus M, Gordon D, Price J. Documenting pornography use in America: A comparative analysis of methodological approaches. Journal of Sex Research. 2016;53(7):873–881. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2015.1096886. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Rissel C, Richters J, De Visser RO, McKee A, Yeung A, Caruana T. A profile of pornography users in Australia: Findings from the second Australian Study of Health and Relationships. Journal of Sex Research. 2017;54(2):227–240. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2016.1191597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Rumpf H, Montag C. Where to put Compulsive Sexual Behavior Disorder (CSBD)? Phenomenology matters. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. 2022;11(2):230–233. doi: 10.1556/2006.2022.00039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Sallie SN, Ritou VJ, Bowden-Jones H, Voon V. Assessing online gaming and pornography consumption patterns during COVID-19 isolation using an online survey: Highlighting distinct avenues of problematic internet behavior. Addictive Behaviors. 2021;123:107044. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2021.107044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Sassover E, Weinstein A. Should compulsive sexual behavior (CSB) be considered as a behavioral addiction? A debate paper presenting the opposing view. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. 2022;11(2):166–179. doi: 10.1556/2006.2020.00055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Shamon H, Berning C. Attention check items and instructions in online surveys with incentivized and non-incentivized samples: Boon or bane for data quality? Survey Research Methods. 2020;14(1):55–77. doi: 10.18148/srm/2020.v14i1.7374. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  109. Sharpe JP, Gilbert DG. Effects of repeated administration of the Beck Depression Inventory and other measures of negative mood states. Personality and Individual Differences. 1998;24(4):457–463. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00193-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  110. Shiffman S, West RJ, Gilbert DG. Recommendation for the assessment of tobacco craving and withdrawal in smoking cessation trials. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. 2004;6(4):599–614. doi: 10.1080/14622200410001734067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Shmulewitz, D., Greene, E. R., & Hasin, D. (2015). Commonalities and differences across substance use disorders: Phenomenological and epidemiological aspects. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 39(10), 1878–1900. 10.1111/acer.12838 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  112. Sirianni JM, Vishwanath A. Problematic online pornography use: A media attendance perspective. Journal of Sex Research. 2016;53(1):21–34. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2014.980496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Starcevic V. Tolerance and withdrawal symptoms may not be helpful to enhance understanding of behavioural addictions. Addiction. 2016;111(7):1307–1308. doi: 10.1111/add.13381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Sussman S, Sussman AN. Considering the definition of addiction. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2011;8(10):4025–4038. doi: 10.3390/ijerph8104025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Szabo A. The impact of exercise deprivation on well-being of habitual exercisers. Australian Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport. 1995;27(3):68–77. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Tan SA, Goh YS, Mohd Zaharim N, Gan SW, Yap CC, Nainee S, Lee LK. Problematic internet pornography use and psychological distress among emerging adults in Malaysia: Gender as a moderator. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022;19(6):3682. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19063682. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Thomas DL, Diener E. Memory accuracy in the recall of emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1990;59(2):291–297. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.59.2.291. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  118. Thompson ER. Development and validation of an internationally reliable short-form of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2007;38(2):227–242. doi: 10.1177/0022022106297301. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  119. van Rooij A, Prause N. A critical review of “internet addiction” criteria with suggestions for the future. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. 2014;3(4):203–213. doi: 10.1556/jba.3.2014.4.1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Weinstein AM, Zolek R, Babkin A, Cohen K, Lejoyeux M. Factors predicting cybersex use and difficulties in forming intimate relationships among male and female users of cybersex. Frontiers in Psychiatry. 2015;6:54. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Welsch SK, Smith SS, Wetter DW, Jorenby DE, Fiore MC, Baker TB. Development and validation of the Wisconsin Smoking Withdrawal Scale. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 1999;7(4):354–361. doi: 10.1037//1064-1297.7.4.354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Wines D. Exploring the applicability of criteria for substance dependence to sexual addiction. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity. 1997;4(3):195–220. doi: 10.1080/10720169708404228. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  123. World Health Organization. (2019). ICD-11: International classification of disease (11th ed.). Retrieved April 22, 2022, from https://icd.who.int/browse11/l-m/en
  124. Zamri ASSM, Singh S, Ghazali SM, Herng LC, Dass SC, Aris T, Gill BS. Effectiveness of the movement control measures during the third wave of COVID-19 in Malaysia. Epidemiology and Health. 2021;43:e2021073. doi: 10.4178/epih.e2021073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials


Articles from Archives of Sexual Behavior are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES