Abstract
Almost all brain cells contain cilia, antennae-like microtubule-based organelles. Yet, the significance of cilia, once considered vestigial organelles, in the higher-order brain functions is unknown. Cilia act as a hub that senses and transduces environmental sensory stimuli to generate an appropriate cellular response. Similarly, the striatum, a brain structure enriched in cilia, functions as a hub that receives and integrates various types of environmental information to drive appropriate motor response. To understand cilia’s role in the striatum functions, we used loxP/Cre technology to ablate cilia from the dorsal striatum of male mice and monitored the behavioral consequences. Our results revealed an essential role for striatal cilia in the acquisition and brief storage of information, including learning new motor skills, but not in long-term consolidation of information or maintaining habitual/learned motor skills. A fundamental aspect of all disrupted functions was the “time perception/judgment deficit.” Furthermore, the observed behavioral deficits form a cluster pertaining to clinical manifestations overlapping across psychiatric disorders that involve the striatum functions and are known to exhibit timing deficits. Thus, striatal cilia may act as a calibrator of the timing functions of the basal ganglia-cortical circuit by maintaining proper timing perception. Our findings suggest that dysfunctional cilia may contribute to the pathophysiology of neuro-psychiatric disorders, as related to deficits in timing perception.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s12035-022-03095-9.
Keywords: Cilia, Striatum, Timing, Behaviors
Introduction
Primary cilia, antennae-like microtubule-based organelles emanating from the cell surface, function as a signaling hub that senses environmental sensory stimuli and transduces them to generate appropriate cellular responses [1–4]. As an essential center for non-synaptic neuronal communications, cilia signaling is achieved through specific receptors and downstream pathways’ components such as the Sonic Hedgehog (Shh) signaling pathway, G-protein-coupled receptors (GPCRs), and ion channels [5–12].
Cilia’s dynamic structure, reflected by the vibrant length, morphology, and protein composition, allows cilia to quickly respond to environmental stimuli such as light, mechanical stimuli, pH, and chemical signals (signaling molecules, neurotransmitters, and nutrients) [13–19]. Although most ciliopathies are associated with cognitive impairments, cilia have only been scarcely investigated for their roles in higher-order cognitive functions [20–25]. We recently showed dysregulations of genes associated with cilia’s structural and functional components in four psychiatric disorders: schizophrenia, autism, bipolar disorder, and major depressive disorder [26]. Furthermore, many dysregulated cilia genes overlapped across these psychiatric disorders, indicating that common cilia signaling pathways’ dysfunctions may underlie some pathophysiological aspects of these psychiatric disorders.
Like cilia, though at larger spatial and organizational scales, the striatum, which comprises the primary input station to the basal ganglia, functions as a hub receiving and integrating various environmental information, including contextual, motor, and reward [27–31]. Striatal neurons process the information and project to the output structures of the basal ganglia, substantia nigra pars reticulata (SNr)/medial globus pallidus (GPm) (for review [32–35]). The striatum is enriched in cilia [36, 37]. Furthermore, a number of cilia-associated GPCRs are expressed in the striatum (e.g., dopamine receptors, serotonin receptor 6 (5-HT6), melanin-concentrating hormone receptor 1 (MCHR1), and the orphan receptors GPR88) [5–10, 38–56].
As a central part of cortico-basal ganglia-thalamic-cortico circuits, the striatum controls various executive functions, including motor movements, planning, decision-making, working memory, and attention [57–60]. Dysfunctions of cortico-basal ganglia-thalamic circuits are involved in several neurological and psychiatric (neuro-psychiatric) disorders such as attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), Huntington’s disease (HD), Parkinson’s disease (PD), schizophrenia (SCZ), autism spectrum disorder (ASD), Tourette syndrome (TS), and obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD) [61–76]. The overlap in clinical features of these disorders suggests common signaling pathways that may underlie some pathophysiological aspects of these neuro-psychiatric disorders. Hence, it is tempting to ask whether cilia in the striatum mediate some of its functions and whether cilia are involved in psychiatric disorders associated with striatum dysfunctions.
According to our recent study, the striatum was the only brain structure that shared rhythmic cilia genes with every other brain region studied [18]. The spatiotemporal expressions of circadian cilia genes in the basal ganglia-cortical neurons follow the same sequential order of this circuitry in controlling movement, though on different time scales. Therefore, this study aims to examine the behavioral consequences of cilia ablation in the striatum and explore whether abnormal striatal cilia are a unifying pathophysiological factor in striatum-associated neuropsychiatric disorders. For this purpose, we used the loxP/Cre conditional deletion system to selectively delete the intraflagellar transport (IFT88) gene, an essential factor for primary cilia formation, from the dorsal striatum. We then monitored the behavioral phenotypes resulting from striatal cilia ablation, focusing on neuropsychiatric phenotypes/manifestations of the striatum functional domains.
Material and Methods
Animals
The Ift88fl mice possess loxP sites flanking exons 4–6 of the intraflagellar transport 88 (Ift88) gene (Jackson Laboratories, #022,409). All experimental procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of California, Irvine, and were performed in compliance with national and institutional guidelines for the care and use of laboratory animals. The experimental design is illustrated in Fig. 1a. Only male mice were used in this study.
Fig. 1.
Selective cilia deletion in the striatum and confirmation of mice’s normal gross growth and well-being. a Schematic view of experimental design and behavior assays performed and their sequence. Diagram was created with the BioRender.com webpage. b Schematic showing bilateral viral injection into the dorsal striatum. c and d Verification of cilia removal in ciliated neurons of the striatum using immunostaining of ADCY3. Scale bar = 10 μm. c Representative images of ADCY3 immunostaining showing the intact cilia in the control mice and the conditional ablation of cilia in the dorsal striatum neurons of Ift88fl mice (counterstained with DAPI, blue); d Quantification of the ciliated cells in the rostral-dorsal striatum (n = 8 control, 6 IFT88-KO). Unpaired t-test (t = 17.26, P < 0.0001) ****P < 0.0001. Data are presented as means ± S.E.M. Scale bar = 10 μm. e–g ADCY3 immunostaining in the caudal striatum. e Representative images of ADCY3 immunostaining in the caudal striatum showing that the selective removal of cilia from the dorsal rostral striatum does not affect f the number of ciliated cells (t = 0.30, P > 0.05) or g the cilia length (t = 0.30, P > 0.05, n = 4) in the caudal striatum. Scale bar = 10 μm. h–j ADCY3 immunostaining in the ventral striatum (nucleus accumbens). h Representative images of ADCY3 immunostaining in the ventral striatum showing that the selective removal of cilia from the rostral striatum does not affect i the number of ciliated neurons (t = 0.52, P > 0.05) or j the cilia length (t = 0.08, P > 0.05, n = 4) in the ventral striatum. Scale bar = 10 μm. k Effect of cilia removal on body weight to confirm normal gross growth (n = 8 control, 6 IFT88-KO). Unpaired t-test (t = 0.2463, P = 0.8096) revealed no significant difference in body weight. ns, not significant. Data are presented as means ± S.E.M. l Verification of well-being (n = 8 control, 6 IFT88-KO). Unpaired t-test (t = 0.2060, P = 0.8403) showed normal response to nociceptive stimulus. ns, not significant. Data are presented as means ± S.E.M
Genotyping
The genotyping protocol was provided by Jackson Laboratory (JAX). DNA was extracted from mice using the following protocol. One millimeter of the tip of the tail was cut and digested in lysis buffer along with proteinase K overnight followed by isopropanol to precipitate the DNA, and ethanol to wash the pellet, and finally dissolved in TE buffer. DNA concentration and purity were checked. The DNA was used for the following PCR reaction. The following primers were used Forward 5′-GACCACCTTTTTAGCCTCCTG, Reverse 5′-AGGGAAGGGACTTAGGAATGA. Amplification was performed using KAPA2GFast HotStart PCR kit (Roche Cat. No 07960930001). Touchdown cycling was performed and ran on a 1% agarose gel with the homozygous at ~ 410 bp, heterozygous at 365 bp and ~ 410 bp, and wildtype at 365 bp.
Stereotaxic Surgery
Eight-week-old male Ift88fl mice were subjected to stereotaxic surgery along with wild-type littermates. Mice were anesthetized with 2% isoflurane and mounted on the stereotaxic frame. The skin between the eyes and ears was shaved, and an incision was made to uncover the skull and reveal bregma. A small hole was drilled bilaterally, and all mice received 0.5 μl of the adenovirus expressing Cre: AAV.CamKII.HI.GFP-Cre.WPRE.SV40 (titer 2.4 × 10^13 G.C./ml, serotype AAV9), which was bilaterally injected into the dorsal segment of the rostral striatum, at the stereotaxic coordinates anteroposterior at 1.3 mm, mediolateral at ± 1.3 mm, and dorsoventral at 3.2 mm (Fig. 1b). AAV.CamKII.HI.GFP-Cre.WPRE.SV40 was a gift from James M. Wilson (Addgene viral prep # 105,551-AAV9 http://n2t.net/addgene:105551; RRID:Addgene_105551). The skin was sutured with silk non-absorbable sutures, and mice were allowed a week to recover before behavioral experiments. After the last behavioral experiment at approximately 16 weeks old, mice were perfused, and AAV infection was analyzed.
Behavioral Experiments
Two weeks after the surgery, mice were tested in a battery of behavioral paradigms in the following order (Fig. 1a): locomotion and stereotypy/open field, rotarod, grooming, social interaction and novelty, spontaneous T-maze alternation, novel object/location recognition, prepulse inhibition, forced swim, hot plate, and contextual fear conditioning. The sequence of specific assays spaced by 3–6 days inter-assay interval was adapted from previously published reports [77–79].
Locomotor Activity Test and Open Field
The locomotor activity and open field assays were carried out as we previously described [78]. The experiment is divided into two phases of a total of 90 min experiment, which is divided into 30 min acclimation and 60 min locomotor activity test [78]. Animals were placed in a 40 × 40 cm locomotion chamber (Med Associates, Inc.), and their activity was logged every 5 min, over the assay duration using Activity Monitor 5 software (Med Associates, Inc.).
Open field assay was carried out in the first 10 min after placing the mice into the chambers, and the distance traveled and activity time in the central and peripheral zones were recorded and analyzed using Activity Monitor 5 software.
Rotarod Test
In order to evaluate motor coordination, mice were subjected to the rotarod assay. Animals were placed on an elevated rotating rod divided into 5 lanes with trip plates below each lane to stop the timer when a subject falls. Each trial is 420 s with a starting speed at 4 RPM that continues to incrementally speed up to 60 RPM. Animals were subjected to 3 trials with 15 min between each trial. Data were analyzed based on latency and speed to fall over each trial.
T-maze Spontaneous Alternation
Mice were placed in the entrance at the base of the T-maze. Mice were acclimated at the base of the T-maze for 30 sec. After acclimation, the doors were opened, and animals were free to explore either the left or right arm of the maze. After a choice had been made, the door was closed, allowing the animal to explore the sidearm chosen for 30 sec. Mice were then returned to the maze base to start the subsequent trial. Eight total trials were carried out with 7 possible alternations, and the alternation percentage was calculated as 100x (number of alternations/7). The time taken to make the alternation decision was recorded as well.
Self-Grooming Behavioral Assay
Mice were placed in an empty cage for 20 min. They were first allowed to acclimate to the environment for 10 min and then were monitored for grooming activity for the last 10 min. Time spent grooming over the 10 min was recorded. Videos were recorded, and grooming was manually scored for grooming activities such as licking, face swiping, scratching, or nibbling. Each video was scored by three persons blinded to the animal genotype, and the average of the scores was recorded.
Social Interaction and Social Novelty
The 3-chamber box used for these assays is a rectangular plexiglass box consisting of a left, middle, and right chamber with removable doors, which separate the chambers. Empty mesh wire cups were placed in the middle of both the left and right chambers. In the social interaction assay, mice were allotted 5 min to explore the middle chamber. After 5 min, a control mouse of the same gender, age, and strain as the experimental mouse was placed inside one of the cups in either the right or left chamber. The doors were then removed, allowing the experimental mice to explore all three chambers for 10 min. The total time experimental mice spent interacting with both the empty cup and the control mouse cup was recorded.
Immediately following the social interaction assay, the social novelty assay began. The experimental mouse was returned to the middle chamber, and a new control mouse was placed underneath the empty cup. Doors were removed, and the experimental mouse was allotted 10 min to explore all three chambers. The total time experimental mice spent interacting with the mouse from the social interaction assay and the novel mouse was recorded. ANY-maze software (Stoelting, Wood Dale, IL, USA) was used to record and analyze these interactions.
Novel Object Recognition
Novel object recognition (NOR) consists of a training and testing phase. All mice were handled for 1–2 min a day for 3 days prior to the training phase. Mice were then allowed to habituate to the experimental apparatus, a rectangular box for 3 consecutive days without the presence of objects. During the training phase, mice were exposed to two identical objects in the apparatus and allowed to explore for 10 min. After 24 h, mice were subjected to the testing phase, where they were allotted 5 min to explore the apparatus with the familiar object and a novel object. The total time each subject mouse spent interacting with both the familiar and novel object was recorded individually. ANY-maze software (Stoelting, Wood Dale, IL, USA) was used to document and analyze these interactions.
Novel Location Recognition
Novel location recognition (NLR) consists of the training and testing phase. All mice were handled for 1–2 min a day for 3 days prior to the training phase. Mice were then allowed to habituate to the experimental apparatus, a rectangular box for 3 consecutive days without the presence of objects. During the training phase, mice were exposed to two identical objects in the apparatus and allowed to explore for 10 min. After 24 h, mice were subjected to the testing phase, where one location of the object is moved. Mice were allotted 5 min to explore the apparatus with the familiar location and a novel location. The total time each subject mouse spent interacting with both the familiar and novel location was recorded individually. ANY-maze software (Stoelting, Wood Dale, IL, USA) was used to document and analyze these interactions.
Forced Swim
Mice were placed in a cylinder containing water at 25 °C for 6 min. Mice were recorded, and following the assay, the last 4 min were scored and analyzed. The immobility time or time the mice spent floating was recorded. ANY-maze software was used to record and analyze immobility time (Stoelting Co.).
Prepulse Inhibition
Mice were habituated to the startle chambers for 5 min with 65 dB of background noise. The PPI sessions consisted of 5 trials, either no stimulus (65 dB), 3 prepulse (20-ms prepulse at 68 dB, 71 dB, or 77 dB, a 100-ms interstimulus interval, followed by a 40-ms duration startle stimulus at 120 dB. The amount of prepulse inhibition was calculated as a percentage score for each acoustic prepulse intensity: % PPI = 100 - ([(startle response for prepulse + pulse trials) / (startle response for pulse − alone trials)] X 100).
Hot Plate
Mice were habituated to the hot plate with no heat to establish a baseline. Following the habituation, mice were subjected to a 52 °C hot plate, and were monitored for an initial response of either a rear paw lift, paw shake, or paw lick in response to the heat or once the cutoff time is reached. Once the initial response was seen, the time was recorded, and animals were returned to their home cage.
Contextual Fear Conditioning
This assay consists of a training and a testing session 24 h following the training session. On day 1, mice were placed in the conditioning chamber for 3 min, received a 2-s 0.7 mA foot shock at 2.5 min, and were placed back into their home cage on day 2, animals were returned to the same chamber for 5 min without shock. Freezing behavior was measured pre- and post-shock sessions and was scored as freezing (1) or not (0) within a 5-s interval and calculated as 100x (the number of intervals of freezing / total intervals).
Immunohistochemistry
Ninety minutes after the fear conditioning assay, mice were anesthetized with isoflurane and transcardially perfused with saline and 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA). Brains were harvested, kept in PFA overnight, and switched to 30% sucrose. Brains were coronally sectioned at 20 μm using a microtome. Using the Allen Brain Atlas, 3–4 sections were selected from specific regions of interest. Sections were blocked with goat serum in PBS with 0.3% Triton X-100 for 1 h. Next, brain sections were incubated in blocking buffer with the primary antibody, either cFos, 1:500 (Abcam cFos ab190289 Rb pAb to cFos Lot#: GR339395) or ADCY3, 1:500 (LSBIO-C204505 ADCY3 Lot#: 193037). Following the primary antibody incubation, sections were washed with PBS and then incubated with the secondary antibody, 1:500 (Invitrogen AlexaFluor546 goat anti-rabbit ref: A101035 Lot: 2,273,730), and DAPI, 1:10,000 (Thermo Scientific Ref: 62,248 Lot: WF3296471). Sections were washed with PBS and mounted on slides. Images were carried out using the Leica SP8 confocal microscope with a 63 × objective lens (UCI optical biology core facility) for visualizing cilia, and Keyence BZ-9000 microscope with a 10x objective lens for visualizing cFos positive neurons.
Image Analysis
cFos positive neurons and ADCY3 were counted bilaterally, and the mean of the three sections per 4–6 brains was calculated. All image analysis and cell counts were performed in Fiji (ImageJ). Automatic particle quantification and analysis methods were used for counting cFos labeled neurons. Briefly, images were opened in Fiji, and color channels were split into the appropriate color. Color images were then converted to grayscale. All regions were defined with specific dimensions across all images for cohesiveness. Once in grayscale, a threshold was set to highlight fluorescent particles and create a binary image. The "analyze particle" function was used to select the size of particles and set the circularity of the particles. Cilia length was measured using the line measurement tools in Fiji, and the length unit was converted from pixels to micron.
Statistical Analysis
GraphPad Prism (GraphPad Software, Inc.) was employed to perform statistical analysis. Data were presented as means ± S.E.M. Results were analyzed using student t-test or ANOVA followed by the appropriate post hoc comparisons, and P < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.
Results
Selective Deletion of Cilia in the Striatum
To examine the physiological role of primary cilia in the striatum, we used the conditional deletion system and disrupted intraflagellar transport (IFT) machinery by deleting the Ift88 gene. Stereotactic infusion of IFT88fl/fl mice with AAV.CamKII.HI.GFP-Cre into the striatum (Bregma level 1.3 mm, ± 1.3 mm, 3.2 mm) resulted in mice with primary cilia deficiency exclusively in the dorsal part of the rostral striatum (Fig. 1b).
Cilia deletion from the dorsal-rostral striatum did not affect the number of striatal neurons, indicating that the dorsal-rostral striatum of IFT88-KO mice is intact, and the neurons were viable. Next, we performed a coexpression analysis of primary cilia with GFP-Cre via immunohistochemistry, using an antibody against ADCY3, a well-known marker of primary cilia. While the numbers of neurons expressing primary cilia and the cilia length were comparable in caudal striatum and nucleus accumbens between the control and IFT88-KO mice, these numbers were markedly reduced in the dorsal-rostral striatum of IFT88-KO mice (Fig. 1c–j). Furthermore, comparable body weights and normal response to nociceptive stimulus (hot plate) confirmed that the IFT88-KO mice have normal gross growth and well-being (Fig. 1k, l).
Primary Cilia in the Striatum Are Required to Maintain Normal Motor Coordination But Not the Spontaneous Motor Activity
Although the striatum is identified as a brain site for motor control timing, it is unknown whether striatum primary cilia play a role in regulating motor functions. To address this question, spontaneous motor activity and motor coordination were monitored in IFT88-KO mice. After the habituation period, IFT88-KO mice displayed similar locomotor activity to the control mice, measured by distance traveled (Fig. 2a, b). However, the locomotor activity in the first 10 min after placing mice into the open box was significantly lower in the IFT88-KO than in the control mice. In addition, IFT88-KO mice showed a reduced latency to fall and a lower rotation speed at which they fall in the rotarod test (Fig. 2c, d), indicating an impairment of motor coordination in the IFT88-KO mice. This may also be interpreted as impairment of procedural learning as trial 1 shows IFT88-KO mice just as coordinated as the control mice.
Fig. 2.
Primary cilia ablation in the striatum affects motor and sensorimotor-related behaviors. a Distance traveled in the last 60 min of the locomotor assay (n = 8 control, 6 IFT88-KO). Two-way ANOVA (F(11, 144) = 0.4143, P > 0.05) followed by Bonferroni’s post hoc test showed that IFT88-KO mice displayed similar locomotor activity to the control mice; ns, not significant. b Total locomotor activity in the locomotor assay. Unpaired t-test (t = 0.9608, P = 0.3556), ns, not significant. Data are presented as means ± S.E.M. c and d Motor skill learning on the accelerated rotarod. c Speed to fall in rotarod assay (n = 8). Two-way ANOVA (cilia removal factor: F(1,42) = 9.399, P = 0.0038, trial factor F(2,42) = 1.927, P > 0.05) followed by Bonferroni post hoc test: IFT88-KO vs control, **P < 0.001. Data are presented as means ± S.E.M; d Latency to fall in rotarod assay (n = 8). Two-way ANOVA (cilia removal factor: F(1,42) = 9.246, P = 0.0041, trial factor: F(2,42) = 1.93, P > 0.05) followed by Bonferroni post hoc test: IFT88-KO vs control, ***P < 0.001. Data are presented as means ± S.E.M. e Repetitive behavior in grooming behavior assays (n = 8 control, 6 IFT88-KO), unpaired t-test (t = 4.357, P = 0.0009), IFT88-KO vs control. Data are presented as means ± S.E.M. f and g Performance of mice in PPI assay. f Startle reactivity in prepulse inhibition assay (n = 8), unpaired t-test (t = 1.424, P = 0.1763), ns, not significant. g Prepulse inhibition in PPI assay (n = 8), two-way ANOVA (cilia removal factor: F(1,56) = 41.69, P < 0.0001, prepulse intensity factor: F(3,56) = 19.55, P < 0.0001), control vs IFT88-KO, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001, ****P < 0.0001. Data are presented as means ± S.E.M
Ablation of Primary Cilia in the Striatum Increases Repetitive Behavior and Impairs Sensorimotor Gating
The IFT88-KO mice showed a higher level of compulsive grooming than the control mice (Fig. 2e), indicating an enhanced compulsive, repetitive behavior in these animals. Grooming was measured as the time mice spent face swiping, paw licking, rubbing of the head and ears, and cleaning the entire body. IFT88-KO mice also exhibited reduced values of prepulse inhibition of the startle reaction at 71 dB and 77 dB prepulses, as well as the average prepulse, although the startle reactions were comparable to those in the control mice (Fig. 2f, g). The average PPI value was also lower in IFT88-KO mice than the control ones. These results indicate that ablation of cilia in the striatum causes a deficit in sensorimotor gating without affecting the startle reaction.
Primary Cilia in the Striatum Are Not Involved in Anxiety, Sociability, and Depressive-Like Behaviors
To test whether cilia ablation affects anxiety-like behavior, we used open field assay, which is based on the assumption that mice placed in a new environment tend to avoid open space and, therefore, spend more time in the peripheral than central arenas. Cilia ablation did not affect anxiety-like behavior, revealed by the longer times spent in the peripheral than central arenas in IFT88-KO mice, which were comparable to those in the control mice (Fig. 3a). Although the total distance and the distance traveled in the peripheral zone were significantly lower in the IFT88-KO mice than the control mice (Fig. 3b, c), this does not indicate an altered anxiety-related behavior; rather, it reflects a slower response to the new environment. The immobility time in the forced swim test, which measures helplessness behavior, was similar in the control and IFT88-KO mice (Fig. 3d). Furthermore, IFT88-KO mice showed normal social behavior, revealed by spending significantly more time with a stranger mouse than an empty cup (Fig. 3e, f). These results indicate that cilia ablation in the striatum does not affect anxiety, sociability, and depressive-like behavior.
Fig. 3.
Primary cilia removal in the dorsal striatum does not affect anxiety, sociability, and depressive-like behaviors. a Time spent in central vs time in peripheral zones in open field assay (n = 8 control, 6 IFT88-KO), two-way ANOVA (cilia removal factor: F(1,24) = 0.00, P > 0.999, zone factor: F(1,24) = 538.1, P < 0.0001) followed by Bonferroni post hoc test. ****P < 0.0001. Data are presented as means ± S.E.M. b Total distance traveled in open field assay, unpaired t-test (t = 3.598, P = 0.0037), IFT88-KO vs control, **P < 0.01. Data are presented as means ± S.E.M. c Distance traveled in central vs time in peripheral zones in open field assay (n = 8 control, 6 IFT88-KO), two-way ANOVA (cilia removal factor: F(1,24) = 21.1, P = 0.0001, zone factor: F(1,24) = 68.31, P < 0.0001), followed by Bonferroni post hoc test, ****P < 0.0001. e Social interaction (n = 8), two-way ANOVA (cup factor: F(1,28) = 26.25, P < 0.0001, cilia removal factor: F(1,28) = 2.28, P = 0.1420); Empty cup vs stranger mouse, **P < 0.0001. Data are presented as means ± S.E.M. f Social interaction discrimination index, unpaired t-test (t = 0.2422, P = 0.8121), control vs IFT88-KO, ns, not significant. Data are presented as means ± S.E.M
Primary Cilia in the Striatum Are Required for Spatial Working Memory But Not Other Memory Types
We examined the effects of cilia ablation in the striatum on different types of memories, including spatial working memory, social recognition memory, object recognition memory, spatial memory (location recognition), and contextual memory. Spatial working memory was tested in the T-maze paradigm, which is based on the mice’ tendency to repeatedly alternate between the right and left arms in order to optimize their navigation of their environment [80]. The short-term social memory (social recognition) was tested using the three-chamber assay, which is based on the instinctive tendency of mice to investigate and spend more time with unfamiliar social subjects (strange mouse) than familiar ones [81]. Long-term memory formation involves encoding, short-term memory consolidation (storage), and long-term memory reconsolidation and retrieval [82] and is usually tested in mice 24 h after conditioning [71].
The spatial working memory was significantly impaired in the IFT88-KO mice, revealed by the higher incorrect choices in these mice than in the control mice (Fig. 4a). Associated with working memory impairment in IFT88-KO, the latency for decision-making was significantly longer in these mice compared with than the control mice (Fig. 4b). Interestingly, while the ablation of primary cilia in the striatum did not affect social behavior, it caused an impairment in social recognition memory (Fig. 4c, d), as reflected by the similar time IFT88-KO mice spent with the old and new stranger mice.
Fig. 4.
Primary cilia in the striatum are required for spatial working memory but not other memories. a Spatial working memory alteration percentage in T-maze (n = 8 control, 6 IFT88-KO), unpaired t-test (t = 7.679, P < 0.0001), control vs IFT88-KO, ****P < 0.0001, b Decision latency in T-maze test (n = 8 control, 6 IFT88-KO), unpaired t-test (t = 5.295, P = 0.0002), control vs IFT88-KO, ***P < 0.001, c social novelty recognition (n = 8), two-way ANOVA (cilia removal factor: F(1,28) = 10.90, P = 0.0026, novel mouse factor: (F(1,28), P = 0.9612), followed by Bonferroni post hoc test: old mouse vs new mouse, **P < 001, ns, not significant. Data are presented as means ± S.E.M. d Discrimination index in social novelty recognition (n = 8), unpaired t-test (t = 3.604, P = 0.0029), control vs IFT88-KO, **P < 0.01. e Novel object recognition (n = 8), two-way ANOVA (object factor: F(1,28) = 21.24, P < 0.0001, cilia removal factor: F(1,28) = 0.02662, P = 0.8716): old object vs new object: *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01. Data are presented as means ± S.E.M. f Discrimination index in novel object recognition (n = 8), unpaired t-test (t = 0.90, P = 0.38), control vs IFT88-KO, ns, not significant. Data are presented as means ± S.E.M. g Novel location recognition (n = 8), two-way ANOVA (object factor: F(1,28) = 20.30, P < 0.0001, cilia removal factor: F(1,28) = 0.2.46, P = 0.8716): *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01. h Discrimination index in novel location recognition (n = 8), unpaired t-test (t = 0.46, P = 0.65), control vs IFT88-KO, ns, not significant. i Fear conditioning (n = 8), two-way ANOVA (P > 0.05) followed by Bonferroni post hoc test: control vs. IFT88-KO. ns, not significant. Data are presented as means ± S.E.M
Ablation of primary cilia in the striatum did not affect the object recognition memory, as evidenced by the more time mice spent with the novel object than the old object (Fig. 4e, f). Similarly, in the novel location recognition assay, IFT88-KO spent more time with the novel location than the old location (Fig. 4g, h), indicating a normal spatial memory. In addition, the contextual memory, measured using the fear conditioning test, was intact in the IFT88-KO mice, as revealed by the similar freezing time on the test day compared with the control mice (Fig. 4i).
Neural Activity Is Changed in Striatal Input and Output Nuclei Pathways
The expression of the immediate-early gene cFos was used as a molecular marker of neural activity. We examined cFos immunoreactivity (number of cFos-positive cells) in structures that are parts of striatal circuits and those known to project to or receive projections from the striatum (Fig. 5a, b). First, the rostral dorsal striatum, but not the caudal striatum of IFT88-KO mice, exhibited a significant decrease of cFos immunoreactivity (Fig. 5c, d). Within the basal ganglia circuit, there was a trend for cFos immunoreactivity reductions in the output regions (SNr and the GPm), but not in the nuclei of the indirect pathway structures (lateral globus pallidus and subthalamic nucleus) (Fig. 5c, d). The main input regions to the striatum include the dopaminergic neurons of the substantia nigra pars compact (SNc) and the glutamatergic neurons of the cortices. While IFT88-KO mice exhibited significant decreases in cFos immunoreactivity in several cortices, including the prefrontal cortex, primary motor area, secondary motor area, and somatosensory area, there was a trend for cFos decrease in the SNc, albeit not significant (Fig. 5c, d).
Fig. 5.
Effects of cilia removal in the dorsal striatum on cFos expression in the striatum, its input and output structures. a Schematic view of neuronal circuits in mice brain. Amy, amygdala; CTX, cortex; GPL, lateral globus pallidus; GPm, medial globus pallidus; Hipp, hippocampus; Hyp, hypothalamus; SNc, substantia nigra pars compacta; SNr, substantia nigra pars reticulata; STN, subthalamic nucleus; STR, striatum; THA, thalamus; VTN, ventral tegmental area. Diagram was created with the BioRender.com webpage. b cFos immunostaining in four levels of the mouse brain. c Representative images of cFos immunostaining in the striatum and its input and output structures: striatum (STR), substantia nigra pars compacta (SNc), substantia nigra pars reticulata (SNr), lateral globus pallidus (GPl), medial globus pallidus (GPm), subthalamic nucleus (STN), prefrontal cortex (PFC), primary motor cortex (PMC), secondary motor cortex (SMC), primary somatosensory area (PSSA). Scale bar = 10 μm. d Quantification of the cFos-positive cells in the control and IFT88-KO mice. Two-way ANOVA, control vs IFT88-KO (F(1,88) = 94.28, P < 0.0001), *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001, ****P < 0.0001, ns, not significant. Data are presented as means ± S.E.M; n = 3 sections of 5 mice per group
Discussion
In this study, we examined the role of cilia in the striatum using a selective conditional deletion system. Our data provide the first evidence for the essential role of striatal primary cilia in specific functions of the striatum, namely, sensorimotor and executive functions.
Methodological Considerations
To examine whether striatal cilia mediate some of the striatum functional domains, we used loxP/Cre technology to selectively delete IFT88, an essential protein for cilia genesis, from the dorsal striatum. The deletion of IFT88 resulted in cilia ablation in the dorsal striatum, as evidenced by the profound decrease in ADCY3-immunostaining. We monitored the behavioral phenotypes resulting from striatal cilia ablation, focusing on neuropsychiatric phenotypes related to three striatum functional domains: the sensorimotor, associative, and limbic domains. The sensorimotor domain is essential for habitual behaviors that are automatically evoked, whereas the associative (cognitive) domain is responsible for driving consciously formed actions and goal-directed behaviors. The dorsal striatum mediates these two functional domains, whereas the limbic domain, associated with motivational and affective behaviors, is sited in the ventral striatum (nucleus accumbens in rodents) [83–88].
Cilia in the Dorsal Striatum Are Necessary for Sensorimotor Learning and Execution of Goal-Directed Behaviors, But Not Affective Behaviors
Cilia ablation from the dorsal striatum did not affect spontaneous motor in mice, evidenced by the normal locomotor activity in the IFT88-KO mice. However, motor activity was lower in IFT88-KO directly after placing them into the open field apparatus, indicating that their reactions to a new environment are diminished. Interestingly, the performance of cilia-ablated mice in the first trial of the rotarod assay was comparable to that of the control mice, indicating that cilia-ablated mice can walk normally on the rotarod. In contrast to the control group, however, in the second and third trials, cilia-ablated mice’ performance not only did not improve but also worsened. The proper performance in rotarod assay reflects motor coordination and learning, which are dependent on striatal function in processing and integrating new sensory information and coordinating the time sequence of motor response [89–92]. Our findings indicate an essential role for striatal cilia in acquiring new motor skills learning, but not in maintaining habitual or already learned motor skills. While we cannot make a concrete conclusion on whether this function of cilia is mediated through processing and integrating new sensory information or by coordinating the time sequence of motor response, we speculate that cilia might be involved in both mechanisms.
In support of this assumption, we found that cilia ablation in the striatum impaired sensorimotor gating, as revealed by the reduced PPI levels [93]. Sensorimotor gating, a largely striatum-thalamus-dependent phenomenon, represents the ability of the brain to respond to a sensory stimulus by suppressing a motor response. Through this mechanism, the brain filters irrelevant sensory information input, prior processing and transmitting to motor output systems. The PPI deficits in cilia-ablated mice further support an essential role for cilia in the sensorimotor integration process and motor executing actions.
The role of cilia in sensorimotor functions is further evidenced by our finding that cilia-ablated mice exhibited excessive repetitive behavior, revealed by increased self-grooming. Repetitive behaviors result from excessive automatized actions or exaggerations of habitual behaviors, for which the dorsal striatum plays an essential role [94–96]. Our results, thus, indicate that these actions of the dorsolateral striatum are mediated through its primary cilia. One important question posed by our findings is whether the failure of acquiring new motor skills in cilia-ablated mice is causally correlated with their enhanced repetitive behavior. Robust evidence, lending a premise to this posit, is that repetitive behaviors arise from the inflexibility in switching and transitioning between habitual and novel motor behavioral patterns [97–99]. Accordingly, cilia may provide an adaptation mechanism that adjusts the transition of habitual behaviors to repetitive behaviors.
We tested the effects of cilia ablation in the striatum on the three types of memory: working memory, short-term memory, and long-term memory. Our results reveal that striatal cilia ablation impairs spatial working memory and short-term social memory and delays decision and task execution in the working memory test but does not affect any long-term memory types.
Working memory encodes and stores information in its temporal context and permits its further manipulation to guide decision-making and behavior execution [100, 101]. The dorsal striatum is involved in the initial storage of temporal information in working memory, and dysfunctions of the striatum or dopaminergic projections to the striatum are known to impair the execution of working memory [102–104]. The striatum also plays an essential role in social recognition, in which different sensory cues are acquired for coding social information [105–107]. Taken together, our results indicate that cilia in the striatum are essential for initial acquisition and brief storage of information but not for its long-term consolidation or retrieval. Our results also suggest that cilia are key components of brain networks for action selection and timing of the decision process. Previous studies have shown that cilia removal from the hippocampus and cortex resulted in deficits in fear conditioning memory [23, 108]. However, our finding of the lack of an impact of cilia loss from the rostral striatum on the contextual fear memory does not seem to contradict with these previous results, since the hippocampus and cortex rather than the striatum are known to be involved in contextual fear memory formation.
Dorsal Striatal Cilia Ablation Does Not Affect Limbic Functional Domains
Cilia ablation in the dorsal striatum did not impair motivational and affective functions, as revealed by the normal sociability, anxiety, nociception, and depressive-like behaviors in the cilia-ablated mice. These results are interesting, though not surprising, given that the dorsal striatum has a minor role in these functions compared with the ventral striatum (nucleus accumbens in rodents), which is the main site for limbic functional domains [109–111].
Striatal Cilia Ablation Alters Neuronal Activities in Striatum Input Structures
Associated with the behavioral phenotype induced by striatal cilia ablation, cFos expression, as a marker for neural activity, was altered in the dorsal striatum itself and in the cortical structures that project to the dorsal striatum, including nuclei of sensorimotor and associative cortical-basal ganglia-thalamic-cortical circuits. The striatum receives projections from the cortex, thalamus, and SNc [28, 31, 112], and its neurons project to the outputs of the basal ganglia SNr/GPm via the direct and indirect pathways via GPl and the subthalamic nucleus (STN) (for review [32–35]). The primary motor cortex is responsible for generating the signals that control movement execution, whereas the secondary motor areas control motor planning. The thalamus, a main output target of the basal ganglia, is under the inhibitory (GABAergic) tone of the SNr and GPm, and in its turn projects, using glutamate transmitter, back to the cortex [113–116]. On the other hand, the dopaminergic neurons of the SNc project to the striatum, stimulating the direct pathway through D1 receptors and inhibiting the indirect pathway via D2 receptors. Surprisingly, none of the basal ganglia structures that receive projections from the striatum showed altered cFos expression. This finding is interesting and, together with the finding of decreased cFos in the cortical regions, indicates that cilia removal from the striatum decreases the activity of presynaptic neurons projecting to the striatum more than neurons that receive projections from the cilia-ablated neurons. It is noteworthy, however, that cFos expression was analyzed in mice' brains 90 min after the fear conditioning retrieval test. Given that cilia-ablated mice showed normal performance in this assay, the profound changes in cFos expressions in several brain regions suggest that striatal cilia ablation caused alteration in the constitutive cFos levels [117, 118].
Striatal Cilia as a Modulator of Timing Perception
An interesting observation made of the reconciliation of our results is that the disrupted functions in striatum cilia-ablated mice share a common fundamental aspect: “impaired time perception,” i.e., losing the ability of quick and timely adjustment of behavior in response to changing environmental cues and, thus, failing to maintain appropriate, goal-directed motor response [119]. Time perception is embedded in the processing and integration of environmental sensory information and in motor and executive actions. Timing judgment allows for the timely selection of appropriate responses to the environment sensory. In this sense, prepulse inhibition of startle reaction, repetitive motor, and motor coordination all involve a sequence of motor actions, for which execution requires precise timing and coordination, usually in the millisecond timescale. Therefore, the exclusive impairments of these functions in cilia-ablated mice may indicate a disruption of time judgment and adjustment, which may impede the successful execution of these motor activities [120–125]. Time perception is also critical for cognitive processes. Successful performance of working memory, attention, decision-making, and executive function requires accurate and precise timing judgment, usually within a millisecond to minute timescale [126–133]. In the context of working memory, the encoding, maintenance, and synchronization of stimulus attributes are presented in specific temporal sequences [134, 135]. Memory formation begins when environmental stimuli first elicit a timing mechanism, in which information about elapsed time is stored in working memory or short term. When the stimulus ends or another event happens, the value of that duration is stored in long-term memory [100, 136]. The impairments of working memory and the delay in the decision and executive function in cilia-ablated mice further support the speculation of interval timing disruption in these mice. This speculation is in line with the known role of the basal ganglia circuit in performing central clock functions in the brain [137, 138]. Particularly, the SNc and striatum are the two basal ganglia regions necessary for interval timing and are parts of cortical-basal-ganglia circuits that form neuronal clocks [139–146]. These circuits coordinate the estimation and reproduction of time, wherein dopamine modulates the clock speed and timing judgment functions [147–149]. For example, increased dopamine levels result in a faster internal clock process, whereas decreased dopamine slows down the clock speed [147–149]. Furthermore, dysfunctions of the striatum have been shown to cause impairments of interval-timing judgments, particularly as related to the spatial working memory [104, 150–152]. Interestingly, we recently found that most cilia transcripts in the primate basal ganglia-cortical circuit are circadian, and they peak in a sequential pattern similar to the sequential order of activation of structures of this circuit during movement coordination albeit on completely different time scales. Taken together, our findings, in line with robust evidence from the literature, suggest a critical role for striatal cilia in the brain’s central clock function.
Implications of Cilia Dysfunctions in Neuropsychiatric Disorders
Intriguingly, the distinctive behavioral phenotype induced by striatal cilia ablation in mice appears to pertain to clinical manifestations of specific neurological and psychiatric disorders related to both the striatum functions and timing deficits. We base our view on the following notions:
The behavioral deficits in cilia-ablated mice represent a cluster that extensively overlaps across specific disorders, including SCZ, PD, HD, ASD, OCD, ADHD, and TS, with some of these deficits more significant in particular disorders than in others. Motor coordination deficits, for example, are the main features of PD and HD but are also observed in TS, ASD, OCD, SCZ, and ADHD [153–162]. On the other hand, repetitive behavior is a common feature of ASD, TS, OCD, and SCZ but is also found in ADHD and PD [163–174]. While sensorimotor gating deficit is a characteristic feature of SCZ, it is also observed in ASD, HD, PD, ADHD, OCD, and TS [175–187]. Furthermore, social recognition memory is impaired in ASD, SCZ, and OCD [188–193]. Lastly, decision execution and working memory deficits are common features in SCZ, ASD, ADHD, PD, HD, and OCD [194–204].
Dysfunctions of the striatum, as a target of dopaminergic pathways and an essential part of the cortico-basal ganglia-thalamic circuits, underlie fully or partially the pathophysiology of these neuro-psychiatric disorders [61–76].
A common feature of striatum-related disorders and the seven discussed disorders is a profound decrease in patients’ ability to accurately calculate the timing of the initiation and termination of voluntary actions [205–220]. While time perception deficits have been extensively studied in the PD, SCZ, ADHD, and HD individuals, timing judgment deficits in ASD, OCD, and TS have recently begun to receive attention [205–220].
It is also important to note that it is possible that the disruption of cilia may alter the morphology and function of other parts of the neuron, and thus, the manipulation might cause some indirect effects on neuron morphology which may alter connectivity or excitability. Based on our findings and the discussion above, we propose a model in which cilia in the striatum act as a calibrator of the timing function of the basal ganglia-cortical circuit by maintaining proper timing perception. According to this model, abnormal cilia functions might be a unifying factor contributing to the pathophysiology of neurological and psychiatric disorders, particularly as related to the deficits in timing judgment.
In conclusion, our findings shed light on the roles that striatal cilia may play in timing-dependent functions. These findings enhance our understanding of brain function in the context of the crucial roles played by this previously unappreciated organelle and may open new avenues for therapeutic intervention through cilia-targeted therapies.
Supplementary Information
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank Dr. Olivier Civelli for the helpful discussions.
Author Contribution
AA designed the experiments and wrote the manuscript; WA, SA, MB, and JC conducted the experiments, and RM and HN contributed to data analysis.
Funding
The work of AA was supported in part by R01-HL147311-02S1, and WA was supported in part by 1F31MH126565-01A1.
Data Availability
Data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.
Declarations
Ethics Approval
All experimental procedures were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of California, Irvine.
All experiments were performed in compliance with national and institutional guidelines for the care and use of laboratory animals.
Consent to Participate
Not applicable.
Consent for Publication
Not applicable.
Competing Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Research Resource Identifiers (RRID)
AAV.CamKII.HI.GFP-Cre.WPRE.SV40 was a gift from James M. Wilson (Addgene viral prep # 105551-AAV9 http://n2t.net/addgene:105551; RRID:Addgene_105551).
AlexaFluor546 goat anti-rabbit: A101035 Lot: 2273730, Invitrogen, USA.
Anti-c-Fos antibody: ab190289 Rb pAb to c-Fos Lot#: GR339395, Abcam, USA.
Anti-ADCY3 antibody: LSBIO-C204505 ADCY3 Lot#:193037, LSBIO, USA.
DAPI, 1:10,000, Thermo Scientific Ref: 62248 Lot: WF3296471.
Footnotes
Significance
• Our study sheds light on the roles that striatal cilia may play in timing-dependent functions.
• Our findings reveal that cilia in the striatum, the input structure to the basal ganglia, act as a calibrator of the basal ganglia-cortical circuit “clock” function by maintaining proper timing perception.
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Fuchs JL, Schwark HD. Neuronal primary cilia: a review. Cell Biol Int. 2004;28(2):111–118. doi: 10.1016/j.cellbi.2003.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Baudoin JP, et al. Tangentially migrating neurons assemble a primary cilium that promotes their reorientation to the cortical plate. Neuron. 2012;76(6):1108–1122. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Gerdes JM, Davis EE, Katsanis N. The vertebrate primary cilium in development, homeostasis, and disease. Cell. 2009;137(1):32–45. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2009.03.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Abou Alaiwi WA, Lo ST, Nauli SM. Primary cilia: highly sophisticated biological sensors. Sensors. 2009;9(9):7003–7020. doi: 10.3390/s90907003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Brailov I, et al. Localization of 5-HT(6) receptors at the plasma membrane of neuronal cilia in the rat brain. Brain Res. 2000;872(1–2):271–275. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(00)02519-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Berbari NF, Lewis JS, Bishop GA, Askwith CC, Mykytyn K. Bardet-Biedl syndrome proteins are required for the localization of G protein-coupled receptors to primary cilia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105(11):4242–4246. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0711027105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Domire JS, et al. Dopamine receptor 1 localizes to neuronal cilia in a dynamic process that requires the Bardet-Biedl syndrome proteins. Cell Mol Life Sci : CMLS. 2011;68(17):2951–2960. doi: 10.1007/s00018-010-0603-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Handel M, et al. Selective targeting of somatostatin receptor 3 to neuronal cilia. Neuroscience. 1999;89(3):909–926. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(98)00354-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Omori Y, et al. Identification of G protein-coupled receptors (GPCRs) in primary cilia and their possible involvement in body weight control. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(6):e0128422. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0128422. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Varela L, Horvath TL. Neuronal cilia: another player in the melanocortin system. Trends Mol Med. 2018;24(4):333–334. doi: 10.1016/j.molmed.2018.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Louvi A, Grove EA. Cilia in the CNS: the quiet organelle claims center stage. Neuron. 2011;69(6):1046–1060. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Trulioff A, Ermakov A, Malashichev Y. Primary cilia as a possible link between left-right asymmetry and neurodevelopmental diseases. Genes. 2017;8(2):48. doi: 10.3390/genes8020048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Nauli SM, et al. Non-motile primary cilia as fluid shear stress mechanosensors. Methods Enzymol. 2013;525:1–20. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-397944-5.00001-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Atkinson KF, Sherpa RT, Nauli SM. The role of the primary cilium in sensing extracellular pH. Cells. 2019;8(7):704. doi: 10.3390/cells8070704. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Christensen ST, Voss JW, Teilmann SC, Lambert IH. High expression of the taurine transporter TauT in primary cilia of NIH3T3 fibroblasts. Cell Biol Int. 2005;29(5):347–351. doi: 10.1016/j.cellbi.2005.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Bargmann CI (2006) Chemosensation in C. elegans. WormBook 1–29 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 17.Alhassen W, et al. Regulation of brain primary cilia length by MCH signaling: evidence from pharmacological, genetic, optogenetic, and chemogenic manipulations. Mol Neurobiol. 2022;59(1):245–265. doi: 10.1007/s12035-021-02511-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Baldi P, et al. Large-scale analysis reveals spatiotemporal circadian patterns of cilia transcriptomes in the primate brain. J Neurosci Res. 2021;99(10):2610–2624. doi: 10.1002/jnr.24919. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Chen S, et al. Dynamic changes of brain cilia transcriptomes across the human lifespan. Int J Mol Sci. 2021;22(19):10387. doi: 10.3390/ijms221910387. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Hartwig C, et al. Neurodevelopmental disease mechanisms, primary cilia, and endosomes converge on the BLOC-1 and BORC complexes. Dev Neurobiol. 2018;78(3):311–330. doi: 10.1002/dneu.22542. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Valente EM, Rosti RO, Gibbs E, Gleeson JG. Primary cilia in neurodevelopmental disorders. Nat Rev Neurol. 2014;10(1):27–36. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2013.247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Reiter JF, Leroux MR. Genes and molecular pathways underpinning ciliopathies. Nat Rev Mol Cell Biol. 2017;18(9):533–547. doi: 10.1038/nrm.2017.60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Berbari NF, et al. Hippocampal and cortical primary cilia are required for aversive memory in mice. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(9):e106576. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0106576. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Bowie E and Goetz SC (2020) TTBK2 and primary cilia are essential for the connectivity and survival of cerebellar Purkinje neurons. eLife 9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 25.Ramos C, et al. Neuron-specific cilia loss differentially alters locomotor responses to amphetamine in mice. J Neurosci Res. 2021;99(3):827–842. doi: 10.1002/jnr.24755. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Alhassen W, et al. Patterns of cilia gene dysregulations in major psychiatric disorders. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2021;109:110255. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2021.110255. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Reig R, Silberberg G. Multisensory integration in the mouse striatum. Neuron. 2014;83(5):1200–1212. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.07.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Pan WX, Mao T, Dudman JT. Inputs to the dorsal striatum of the mouse reflect the parallel circuit architecture of the forebrain. Front Neuroanat. 2010;4:147. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2010.00147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Voorn P, Vanderschuren LJ, Groenewegen HJ, Robbins TW, Pennartz CM. Putting a spin on the dorsal-ventral divide of the striatum. Trends Neurosci. 2004;27(8):468–474. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Gerfen CR. The neostriatal mosaic: multiple levels of compartmental organization in the basal ganglia. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1992;15:285–320. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.15.030192.001441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Gerfen CR. The neostriatal mosaic: compartmentalization of corticostriatal input and striatonigral output systems. Nature. 1984;311(5985):461–464. doi: 10.1038/311461a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Morita M, Hikida T. [Distinct roles of the direct and indirect pathways in the basal ganglia circuit mechanism] Nihon shinkei seishin yakurigaku zasshi = Japanese J Psychopharmacol. 2015;35(5-6):107–111. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Ikemoto S, Yang C, Tan A. Basal ganglia circuit loops, dopamine and motivation: a review and enquiry. Behav Brain Res. 2015;290:17–31. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.04.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Kreitzer AC, Malenka RC. Striatal plasticity and basal ganglia circuit function. Neuron. 2008;60(4):543–554. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Foley P, Riederer P. The motor circuit of the human basal ganglia reconsidered. J Neural Transm Suppl. 2000;58:97–110. doi: 10.1007/978-3-7091-6284-2_8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Trujillo CM, Yanes CM, Marrero A, Monzon M. Neuronal cilia in the embryonic thalamus and striatum of Gallotia galloti (Reptilia, Lacertidae) J Hirnforsch. 1986;27(6):691–694. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Milhaud M, Pappas GD. Cilia formation in the adult cat brain after pargyline treatment. J Cell Biol. 1968;37(3):599–609. doi: 10.1083/jcb.37.3.599. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Hilgendorf KI, Johnson CT, Jackson PK. The primary cilium as a cellular receiver: organizing ciliary GPCR signaling. Curr Opin Cell Biol. 2016;39:84–92. doi: 10.1016/j.ceb.2016.02.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Lancaster MA, Gleeson JG. The primary cilium as a cellular signaling center: lessons from disease. Curr Opin Genet Dev. 2009;19(3):220–229. doi: 10.1016/j.gde.2009.04.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Christensen ST, Morthorst SK, Mogensen JB, Pedersen LB. Primary cilia and coordination of receptor tyrosine kinase (RTK) and transforming growth factor beta (TGF-beta) signaling. Cold Spring Harbor Perspect Biol. 2017;9(6):a028167. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a028167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Valencia-Gattas M, Conner GE, Fregien NL. Gefitinib, an EGFR tyrosine kinase inhibitor, prevents smoke-mediated ciliated airway epithelial cell loss and promotes their recovery. PLoS ONE. 2016;11(8):e0160216. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0160216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Evans MJ, et al. Fibroblast growth factor-2 during postnatal development of the tracheal basement membrane zone. Am J Physiol Lung Cell Mol Physiol. 2002;283(6):L263–1270. doi: 10.1152/ajplung.00180.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Nauli SM, Pala R, Kleene SJ. Calcium channels in primary cilia. Curr Opin Nephrol Hypertens. 2016;25(5):452–458. doi: 10.1097/MNH.0000000000000251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Nachury MV. The molecular machines that traffic signaling receptors into and out of cilia. Curr Opin Cell Biol. 2018;51:124–131. doi: 10.1016/j.ceb.2018.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Liu B, Chen S, Cheng D, Jing W, Helms JA. Primary cilia integrate hedgehog and Wnt signaling during tooth development. J Dent Res. 2014;93(5):475–482. doi: 10.1177/0022034514528211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.May-Simera HL, Kelley MW. Cilia, Wnt signaling, and the cytoskeleton. Cilia. 2012;1(1):7. doi: 10.1186/2046-2530-1-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Hirst WD, et al. Differences in the central nervous system distribution and pharmacology of the mouse 5-hydroxytryptamine-6 receptor compared with rat and human receptors investigated by radioligand binding, site-directed mutagenesis, and molecular modeling. Mol Pharmacol. 2003;64(6):1295–1308. doi: 10.1124/mol.64.6.1295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Graybiel AM. Neurotransmitters and neuromodulators in the basal ganglia. Trends Neurosci. 1990;13(7):244–254. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(90)90104-I. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Mizushima K, et al. A novel G-protein-coupled receptor gene expressed in striatum. Genomics. 2000;69(3):314–321. doi: 10.1006/geno.2000.6340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Kumar JS, et al. Radiosynthesis and in vivo evaluation of neuropeptide Y5 receptor (NPY5R) PET Tracers. ACS Chem Neurosci. 2016;7(5):540–545. doi: 10.1021/acschemneuro.5b00315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Gerfen CR, et al. D1 and D2 dopamine receptor-regulated gene expression of striatonigral and striatopallidal neurons. Science. 1990;250(4986):1429–1432. doi: 10.1126/science.2147780. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Kobayashi Y, et al. Properties of primary cilia in melanin-concentrating hormone receptor 1-bearing hippocampal neurons in vivo and in vitro. Neurochem Int. 2021;142:104902. doi: 10.1016/j.neuint.2020.104902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Ehrlich AT, et al. Mapping GPR88-Venus illuminates a novel role for GPR88 in sensory processing. Brain Struct Funct. 2018;223(3):1275–1296. doi: 10.1007/s00429-017-1547-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Miyoshi K, et al. Lack of dopaminergic inputs elongates the primary cilia of striatal neurons. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(5):e97918. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0097918. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Brodsky M, et al. 5-HT6 receptor blockade regulates primary cilia morphology in striatal neurons. Brain Res. 2017;1660:10–19. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2017.01.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Jasso KR, et al. An N-terminal fusion allele to study melanin concentrating hormone receptor 1. Genesis. 2021;59(7–8):e23438. doi: 10.1002/dvg.23438. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Middleton FA, Strick PL. Basal ganglia output and cognition: evidence from anatomical, behavioral, and clinical studies. Brain Cogn. 2000;42(2):183–200. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1999.1099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Chudasama Y, Robbins TW. Functions of frontostriatal systems in cognition: comparative neuropsychopharmacological studies in rats, monkeys and humans. Biol Psychol. 2006;73(1):19–38. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Hikosaka O, Ghazizadeh A, Griggs W, Amita H. Parallel basal ganglia circuits for decision making. J Neural Transm. 2018;125(3):515–529. doi: 10.1007/s00702-017-1691-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.McDonald RJ, Hong NS. How does a specific learning and memory system in the mammalian brain gain control of behavior? Hippocampus. 2013;23(11):1084–1102. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Haynes WIA, et al. Altered anatomical connections of associative and limbic cortico-basal-ganglia circuits in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Eur Psychiatry: J Assoc Eur Psychiatrists. 2018;51:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2018.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Maia TV, Cooney RE, Peterson BS. The neural bases of obsessive-compulsive disorder in children and adults. Dev Psychopathol. 2008;20(4):1251–1283. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000606. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Caligiore D, Mannella F, Arbib MA, Baldassarre G. Dysfunctions of the basal ganglia-cerebellar-thalamo-cortical system produce motor tics in Tourette syndrome. PLoS Comput Biol. 2017;13(3):e1005395. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005395. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Ramkiran S, Heidemeyer L, Gaebler A, Shah NJ, Neuner I. Alterations in basal ganglia-cerebello-thalamo-cortical connectivity and whole brain functional network topology in Tourette’s syndrome. NeuroImage Clin. 2019;24:101998. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Vicente AM, Martins GJ, Costa RM. Cortico-basal ganglia circuits underlying dysfunctional control of motor behaviors in neuropsychiatric disorders. Curr Opin Genet Dev. 2020;65:151–159. doi: 10.1016/j.gde.2020.05.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Mills KL, et al. Altered cortico-striatal-thalamic connectivity in relation to spatial working memory capacity in children with ADHD. Front Psych. 2012;3:2. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2012.00002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Kwak Y, et al. Altered resting state cortico-striatal connectivity in mild to moderate stage Parkinson’s disease. Front Syst Neurosci. 2010;4:143. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Perez-Costas E, Melendez-Ferro M, Roberts RC. Basal ganglia pathology in schizophrenia: dopamine connections and anomalies. J Neurochem. 2010;113(2):287–302. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2010.06604.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Bogerts B, Meertz E, Schonfeldt-Bausch R. Basal ganglia and limbic system pathology in schizophrenia. A morphometric study of brain volume and shrinkage. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1985;42(8):784–791. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1985.01790310046006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Avram M, Brandl F, Bauml J, Sorg C. Cortico-thalamic hypo- and hyperconnectivity extend consistently to basal ganglia in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology : Off Publ Am Coll Neuropsychopharmacol. 2018;43(11):2239–2248. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0059-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Fuccillo MV. Striatal circuits as a common node for autism pathophysiology. Front Neurosci. 2016;10:27. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Schuetze M, et al. Morphological alterations in the thalamus, striatum, and pallidum in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology : Off Publ Am Coll Neuropsychopharmacol. 2016;41(11):2627–2637. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Chiken S, Takada M, Nambu A. Altered dynamic information flow through the cortico-basal ganglia pathways mediates Parkinson’s disease symptoms. Cereb Cortex. 2021;31(12):5363–5380. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Martinu K, Monchi O. Cortico-basal ganglia and cortico-cerebellar circuits in Parkinson’s disease: pathophysiology or compensation? Behav Neurosci. 2013;127(2):222–236. doi: 10.1037/a0031226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Braak H, Del Tredici K. Cortico-basal ganglia-cortical circuitry in Parkinson’s disease reconsidered. Exp Neurol. 2008;212(1):226–229. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2008.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Blumenstock S, Dudanova I. Cortical and striatal circuits in Huntington’s disease. Front Neurosci. 2020;14:82. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.00082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Paylor R, Spencer CM, Yuva-Paylor LA, Pieke-Dahl S. The use of behavioral test batteries, II: effect of test interval. Physiol Behav. 2006;87(1):95–102. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Alachkar A, et al. Prenatal one-carbon metabolism dysregulation programs schizophrenia-like deficits. Mol Psychiatry. 2018;23(2):282–294. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Alhassen S, et al. Intergenerational trauma transmission is associated with brain metabotranscriptome remodeling and mitochondrial dysfunction. Commun Biol. 2021;4(1):783. doi: 10.1038/s42003-021-02255-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Deacon RM, Rawlins JN. T-maze alternation in the rodent. Nat Protoc. 2006;1(1):7–12. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Kaidanovich-Beilin O, Lipina T, Vukobradovic I, Roder J, Woodgett JR (2011) Assessment of social interaction behaviors. J Vis Exp (48):2473 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 82.McGaugh JL. Memory–a century of consolidation. Science. 2000;287(5451):248–251. doi: 10.1126/science.287.5451.248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Eryilmaz H, et al. Neural determinants of human goal-directed vs. habitual action control and their relation to trait motivation. Sci Rep. 2017;7(1):6002. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-06284-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Balleine BW, O'Doherty JP. Human and rodent homologies in action control: corticostriatal determinants of goal-directed and habitual action. Neuropsychopharmacology : Off Publ Am Coll Neuropsychopharmacol. 2010;35(1):48–69. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Thorn CA, Atallah H, Howe M, Graybiel AM. Differential dynamics of activity changes in dorsolateral and dorsomedial striatal loops during learning. Neuron. 2010;66(5):781–795. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.04.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Dezfouli A, Balleine BW. Actions, action sequences and habits: evidence that goal-directed and habitual action control are hierarchically organized. PLoS Comput Biol. 2013;9(12):e1003364. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003364. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Gremel CM, Costa RM. Orbitofrontal and striatal circuits dynamically encode the shift between goal-directed and habitual actions. Nat Commun. 2013;4:2264. doi: 10.1038/ncomms3264. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Floresco SB. The nucleus accumbens: an interface between cognition, emotion, and action. Annu Rev Psychol. 2015;66:25–52. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Shiotsuki H, et al. A rotarod test for evaluation of motor skill learning. J Neurosci Methods. 2010;189(2):180–185. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.03.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Deacon RM. Measuring motor coordination in mice. J Vis Exp : JoVE. 2013;75:e2609. doi: 10.3791/2609. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Graybiel AM, Aosaki T, Flaherty AW, Kimura M. The basal ganglia and adaptive motor control. Science. 1994;265(5180):1826–1831. doi: 10.1126/science.8091209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Geddes CE, Li H, Jin X. Optogenetic editing reveals the hierarchical organization of learned action sequences. Cell. 2018;174(1):32–43 e15. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2018.06.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Geyer MA, Braff DL. Startle habituation and sensorimotor gating in schizophrenia and related animal models. Schizophr Bull. 1987;13(4):643–668. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.4.643. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Fobbs WC, et al. Continuous representations of speed by striatal medium spiny neurons. J Neurosci: Off J Soc Neurosc. 2020;40(8):1679–1688. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1407-19.2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Yttri EA, Dudman JT. Opponent and bidirectional control of movement velocity in the basal ganglia. Nature. 2016;533(7603):402–406. doi: 10.1038/nature17639. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Rueda-Orozco PE, Robbe D. The striatum multiplexes contextual and kinematic information to constrain motor habits execution. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18(3):453–460. doi: 10.1038/nn.3924. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Lipton DM, Gonzales BJ, Citri A. Dorsal striatal circuits for habits, compulsions and addictions. Front Syst Neurosci. 2019;13:28. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2019.00028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Banca P, et al. Imbalance in habitual versus goal directed neural systems during symptom provocation in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain : J Neurol. 2015;138(Pt 3):798–811. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu379. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Smith KS, Graybiel AM. A dual operator view of habitual behavior reflecting cortical and striatal dynamics. Neuron. 2013;79(2):361–374. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.05.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Gu BM, van Rijn H, Meck WH. Oscillatory multiplexing of neural population codes for interval timing and working memory. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;48:160–185. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Brodziak A, Brewczynski A, Bajor G. Clinical significance of knowledge about the structure, function, and impairments of working memory. Med Sci Monitor : Int Med J Exp Clin Res. 2013;19:327–338. doi: 10.12659/MSM.883900. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Nyberg L, Eriksson J. Working memory: maintenance, updating, and the realization of intentions. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol. 2015;8(2):a021816. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a021816. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Cools R, Gibbs SE, Miyakawa A, Jagust W, D'Esposito M. Working memory capacity predicts dopamine synthesis capacity in the human striatum. J neurosci: Off J Soc Neurosci. 2008;28(5):1208–1212. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4475-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Akhlaghpour H, Wiskerke J, Choi JY, Taliaferro JP, Au J, Witten IB (2016) Dissociated sequential activity and stimulus encoding in the dorsomedial striatum during spatial working memory. eLife 5:e19507 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 105.Haeger A, Lee H, Fell J, Axmacher N. Selective processing of buildings and faces during working memory: the role of the ventral striatum. Eur J Neurosci. 2015;41(4):505–513. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.van den Bos R. The dorsal striatum and ventral striatum play different roles in the programming of social behaviour: a tribute to Lex Cools. Behav Pharmacol. 2015;26(1–2):6–17. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0000000000000110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Goodin P, Lamp G, Hughes ME, Rossell SL, Ciorciari J. Decreased response to positive facial affect in a depressed cohort in the dorsal striatum during a working memory task-a preliminary fMRI study. Front Psych. 2019;10:60. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Rhee S, Kirschen GW, Gu Y, Ge S. Depletion of primary cilia from mature dentate granule cells impairs hippocampus-dependent contextual memory. Sci Rep. 2016;6:34370. doi: 10.1038/srep34370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Joel D, Weiner I. The connections of the dopaminergic system with the striatum in rats and primates: an analysis with respect to the functional and compartmental organization of the striatum. Neuroscience. 2000;96(3):451–474. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(99)00575-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Groenewegen HJ, Trimble M. The ventral striatum as an interface between the limbic and motor systems. CNS Spectr. 2007;12(12):887–892. doi: 10.1017/S1092852900015650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Lopes da Silva FH, Arnolds DE, Neijt HC. A functional link between the limbic cortex and ventral striatum: physiology of the subiculum accumbens pathway. Exp Brain Res. 1984;55(2):205–214. doi: 10.1007/BF00237271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Kelley AE, Domesick VB, Nauta WJ. The amygdalostriatal projection in the rat–an anatomical study by anterograde and retrograde tracing methods. Neuroscience. 1982;7(3):615–630. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(82)90067-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Haber SN, Calzavara R. The cortico-basal ganglia integrative network: the role of the thalamus. Brain Res Bull. 2009;78(2–3):69–74. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2008.09.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Baladron J, Hamker FH. A spiking neural network based on the basal ganglia functional anatomy. Neural Netw : off J Int Neural Netw Soc. 2015;67:1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2015.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Garcia-Munoz M, Carrillo-Reid L, Arbuthnott GW. Functional anatomy: dynamic states in basal ganglia circuits. Front Neuroanat. 2010;4:144. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2010.00144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Herrero MT, Barcia C, Navarro JM. Functional anatomy of thalamus and basal ganglia. Child's Nerv Syst: ChNS : Off J Int Soc Pediatr Neurosurg. 2002;18(8):386–404. doi: 10.1007/s00381-002-0604-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Ishihara H, Yoshida K, Nemoto K, Tsuneoka K, Shikita M. Constitutive overexpression of the c-fos gene in radiation-induced granulocytic leukemia in mice. Radiat Res. 1993;135(3):394–399. doi: 10.2307/3578880. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Reiss M, Radin AI, Weisberg TF. Constitutive expression of the c-fos protooncogene in murine keratinocytes: potentiation of the mitogenic response to insulin-like growth factor 1. Can Res. 1990;50(20):6641–6648. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Allman MJ, Teki S, Griffiths TD, Meck WH. Properties of the internal clock: first- and second-order principles of subjective time. Annu Rev Psychol. 2014;65:743–771. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Mathias B, Tillmann B, Palmer C. Sensory, cognitive, and sensorimotor learning effects in recognition memory for music. J Cogn Neurosci. 2016;28(8):1111–1126. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Hidalgo-Balbuena AE, Luma AY, Pimentel-Farfan AK, Pena-Rangel T, Rueda-Orozco PE. Sensory representations in the striatum provide a temporal reference for learning and executing motor habits. Nat Commun. 2019;10(1):4074. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-12075-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Howley SA, Prasad SE, Pender NP, Murphy KC. Relationship between reaction time, fine motor control, and visual-spatial perception on vigilance and visual-motor tasks in 22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome. Res Dev Disabil. 2012;33(5):1495–1502. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2012.03.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Terry P, Doumas M, Desai RI, Wing AM. Dissociations between motor timing, motor coordination, and time perception after the administration of alcohol or caffeine. Psychopharmacology. 2009;202(4):719–729. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1352-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Schubotz RI, Friederici AD, von Cramon DY. Time perception and motor timing: a common cortical and subcortical basis revealed by fMRI. Neuroimage. 2000;11(1):1–12. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1999.0514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Treisman M, Faulkner A, Naish PL. On the relation between time perception and the timing of motor action: evidence for a temporal oscillator controlling the timing of movement. Q J Exp Psych. A, Human Exp Psychol. 1992;45(2):235–263. doi: 10.1080/14640749208401326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Gu H, Schulz KP, Fan J, Yang Y. Temporal dynamics of functional brain states underlie cognitive performance. Cereb Cortex. 2021;31(4):2125–2138. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhaa350. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Barrouillet P, Bernardin S, Portrat S, Vergauwe E, Camos V. Time and cognitive load in working memory. J Exp Psychol. Lear, Mem, Cogn. 2007;33(3):570–585. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Zhang G, Li Y, Zhang J. Tracking the dynamic functional network interactions during goal-directed auditory tasks by brain state clustering. Front Neurosci. 2019;13:1220. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.01220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Huberle E, Brugger P. Altered time judgements highlight common mechanisms of time and space perception. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2018;35(8):458–470. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2018.1549027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Fontes R, et al. Time perception mechanisms at central nervous system. Neurol Int. 2016;8(1):5939. doi: 10.4081/ni.2016.5939. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Pouthas V, Perbal S. Time perception depends on accurate clock mechanisms as well as unimpaired attention and memory processes. Acta Neurobiol Exp. 2004;64(3):367–385. doi: 10.55782/ane-2004-1520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Gallistel CR, Gibbon J. Time, rate, and conditioning. Psychol Rev. 2000;107(2):289–344. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.107.2.289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Meck WH, Doyere V, Gruart A. Interval timing and time-based decision making. Front Integr Neurosci. 2012;6:13. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Bonnefond M, Jensen O. Alpha oscillations serve to protect working memory maintenance against anticipated distracters. Current biology : CB. 2012;22(20):1969–1974. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.08.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Brody CD, Hernandez A, Zainos A, Romo R. Timing and neural encoding of somatosensory parametric working memory in macaque prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2003;13(11):1196–1207. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Fortin C, Breton R. Temporal interval production and processing in working memory. Percept Psychophys. 1995;57(2):203–215. doi: 10.3758/BF03206507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Macar F, Coull J, Vidal F. The supplementary motor area in motor and perceptual time processing: fMRI studies. Cogn Process. 2006;7(2):89–94. doi: 10.1007/s10339-005-0025-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Wiener M, Turkeltaub P, Coslett HB. The image of time: a voxel-wise meta-analysis. Neuroimage. 2010;49(2):1728–1740. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Magalhaes F, et al. Neurochemical changes in basal ganglia affect time perception in parkinsonians. J Biomed Sci. 2018;25(1):26. doi: 10.1186/s12929-018-0428-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Talakoub O, et al. Time-course of coherence in the human basal ganglia during voluntary movements. Sci Rep. 2016;6:34930. doi: 10.1038/srep34930. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Yin HH. Action, time and the basal ganglia. Philos Trans R Soc Lond Ser B, Biol Sci. 2014;369(1637):20120473. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0473. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Gershman SJ, Moustafa AA, Ludvig EA. Time representation in reinforcement learning models of the basal ganglia. Front Comput Neurosci. 2014;7:194. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2013.00194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143.Li X, Luo F, Shi L, Woodward DJ, Chang J. Ensemble neural activity of the frontal cortical basal ganglia system predicts reaction time task performance in rats. Neurosci Res. 2011;71(2):149–160. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2011.06.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Jin DZ, Fujii N, Graybiel AM. Neural representation of time in cortico-basal ganglia circuits. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106(45):19156–19161. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0909881106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145.Lo CC, Wang XJ. Cortico-basal ganglia circuit mechanism for a decision threshold in reaction time tasks. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9(7):956–963. doi: 10.1038/nn1722. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146.Vakil E, Kahan S, Huberman M, Osimani A. Motor and non-motor sequence learning in patients with basal ganglia lesions: the case of serial reaction time (SRT) Neuropsychologia. 2000;38(1):1–10. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00058-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147.Bussi IL, Levin G, Golombek DA, Agostino PV. Involvement of dopamine signaling in the circadian modulation of interval timing. Eur J Neurosci. 2014;40(1):2299–2310. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148.Jones CR, Malone TJ, Dirnberger G, Edwards M, Jahanshahi M. Basal ganglia, dopamine and temporal processing: performance on three timing tasks on and off medication in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Cogn. 2008;68(1):30–41. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.02.121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Yang YK, et al. Association between cognitive performance and striatal dopamine binding is higher in timing and motor tasks in patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2004;131(3):209–216. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2003.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150.Coslett HB, Wiener M, Chatterjee A. Dissociable neural systems for timing: evidence from subjects with basal ganglia lesions. PLoS ONE. 2010;5(4):e10324. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151.Mello GB, Soares S, Paton JJ. A scalable population code for time in the striatum. Current biology : CB. 2015;25(9):1113–1122. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.02.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152.Gouvea TS, Monteiro T, Motiwala A, Soares S, Machens C, Paton JJ (2015) Striatal dynamics explain duration judgments. eLife 4:e11386 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 153.Asai Y, Nomura T, Abe K, Matsuo Y, Sato S. Classification of dynamics of a model of motor coordination and comparison with Parkinson’s disease data. Biosystems. 2003;71(1–2):11–21. doi: 10.1016/S0303-2647(03)00105-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 154.Verschueren SM, Swinnen SP, Dom R, De Weerdt W. Interlimb coordination in patients with Parkinson’s disease: motor learning deficits and the importance of augmented information feedback. Exp Brain Res. 1997;113(3):497–508. doi: 10.1007/PL00005602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 155.Yaguez L, Canavan AG, Lange HW, Homberg V. Motor learning by imagery is differentially affected in Parkinson’s and Huntington’s diseases. Behav Brain Res. 1999;102(1–2):115–127. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00005-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 156.Heindel WC, Butters N, Salmon DP. Impaired learning of a motor skill in patients with Huntington’s disease. Behav Neurosci. 1988;102(1):141–147. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.102.1.141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 157.Bloch MH, et al. Poor fine-motor and visuospatial skills predict persistence of pediatric-onset obsessive-compulsive disorder into adulthood. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52(9):974–983. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02366.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 158.Bedard MJ, Joyal CC, Godbout L, Chantal S. Executive functions and the obsessive-compulsive disorder: on the importance of subclinical symptoms and other concomitant factors. Arch Clin Neuropsychol : Off J Natl Acad Neuropsychol. 2009;24(6):585–598. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acp052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 159.Fenollar-Cortes J, Gallego-Martinez A, Fuentes LJ. The role of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity in the fine motor coordination in children with ADHD. Res Dev Disabil. 2017;69:77–84. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2017.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 160.Fliers EA, et al. Genome-wide association study of motor coordination problems in ADHD identifies genes for brain and muscle function. World J Biol Psychiatry : Off J World Fed Soc Biol Psychiatry. 2012;13(3):211–222. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2011.560279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 161.Schiffman J, et al. Childhood motor coordination and adult schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2009;166(9):1041–1047. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.08091400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 162.Martino D, et al. Motor timing in Tourette syndrome: the effect of movement lateralization and bimanual coordination. Front Neurol. 2019;10:385. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.00385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 163.Eapen V, et al. Social communication deficits and restricted repetitive behavior symptoms in Tourette syndrome. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2019;15:2151–2160. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S210227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 164.Leckman JF, King RA, Bloch MH. Clinical features of Tourette syndrome and tic disorders. J Obs-compuls Relat Disord. 2014;3(4):372–379. doi: 10.1016/j.jocrd.2014.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 165.South M, Ozonoff S, McMahon WM. Repetitive behavior profiles in Asperger syndrome and high-functioning autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2005;35(2):145–158. doi: 10.1007/s10803-004-1992-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 166.Bodfish JW, Symons FJ, Parker DE, Lewis MH. Varieties of repetitive behavior in autism: comparisons to mental retardation. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000;30(3):237–243. doi: 10.1023/A:1005596502855. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 167.Morrens M, Hulstijn W, Lewi PJ, De Hert M, Sabbe BG. Stereotypy in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2006;84(2–3):397–404. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.01.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 168.Tracy JI, et al. Repetitive behaviors in schizophrenia: a single disturbance or discrete symptoms? Schizophr Res. 1996;20(1–2):221–229. doi: 10.1016/0920-9964(95)00104-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 169.Selles RR, et al. Initial psychometrics, outcomes, and correlates of the Repetitive Body Focused Behavior Scale: examination in a sample of youth with anxiety and/or obsessive-compulsive disorder. Compr Psychiatry. 2018;81:10–17. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2017.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 170.McDougle CJ, et al. A case-controlled study of repetitive thoughts and behavior in adults with autistic disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 1995;152(5):772–777. doi: 10.1176/ajp.152.5.772. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 171.Kurlan R. Disabling repetitive behaviors in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord : Off J Mov Disord Soc. 2004;19(4):433–437. doi: 10.1002/mds.10625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 172.O'Boyle DJ, Freeman JS, Cody FW. The accuracy and precision of timing of self-paced, repetitive movements in subjects with Parkinson’s disease. Brain : J Neurol. 1996;119(Pt 1):51–70. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.1.51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 173.Pastor MA, Jahanshahi M, Artieda J, Obeso JA. Performance of repetitive wrist movements in Parkinson’s disease. Brain : a J Neurol. 1992;115(Pt 3):875–891. doi: 10.1093/brain/115.3.875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 174.Koyama T, Tachimori H, Osada H, Kurita H. Cognitive and symptom profiles in high-functioning pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2006;36(3):373–380. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0075-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 175.Cheng CH, Chan PS, Hsu SC, Liu CY. Meta-analysis of sensorimotor gating in patients with autism spectrum disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2018;262:413–419. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.09.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 176.Oranje B, Lahuis B, van Engeland H, Jan van der Gaag R, Kemner C. Sensory and sensorimotor gating in children with multiple complex developmental disorders (MCDD) and autism. Psychiatry Res. 2013;206(2–3):287–292. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.10.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 177.San-Martin R, et al. Meta-analysis of sensorimotor gating deficits in patients with schizophrenia evaluated by prepulse inhibition test. Schizophr Bull. 2020;46(6):1482–1497. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbaa059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 178.Hazlett EA, Buchsbaum MS. Sensorimotor gating deficits and hypofrontality in schizophrenia. Front Biosci. 2001;6:D1069–1072. doi: 10.2741/Hazlett. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 179.Braff DL, Geyer MA. Sensorimotor gating and schizophrenia. Human and animal model studies. Arch Gen psychiatry. 1990;47(2):181–188. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1990.01810140081011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 180.Pittenger C, et al. OCD is associated with an altered association between sensorimotor gating and cortical and subcortical 5-HT1b receptor binding. J Affect Disord. 2016;196:87–96. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.02.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 181.Schleyken S, et al. Deep brain stimulation and sensorimotor gating in tourette syndrome and obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Psychiatr Res. 2020;129:272–280. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2020.07.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 182.Ahmari SE, Risbrough VB, Geyer MA, Simpson HB. Impaired sensorimotor gating in unmedicated adults with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology : Off Publ Am Coll Neuropsychopharmacol. 2012;37(5):1216–1223. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 183.Buse J, Beste C, Herrmann E, Roessner V. Neural correlates of altered sensorimotor gating in boys with Tourette syndrome: a combined EMG/fMRI study. World J Biol Psychiatry : Off J World Feder Soc Biol Psychiatry. 2016;17(3):187–197. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2015.1112033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 184.Zoetmulder M, Biernat HB, Nikolic M, Korbo L, Jennum PJ. Sensorimotor gating deficits in multiple system atrophy: comparison with Parkinson’s disease and idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2014;20(3):297–302. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2013.11.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 185.Holstein DH, et al. Sensory and sensorimotor gating in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Psychiatry Res. 2013;205(1–2):117–126. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.08.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 186.Valls-Sole J, Munoz JE, Valldeoriola F. Abnormalities of prepulse inhibition do not depend on blink reflex excitability: a study in Parkinson’s disease and Huntington’s disease. Clin Neurophysiol : Off J Int Feder Clin Neurophysiol. 2004;115(7):1527–1536. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.02.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 187.Swerdlow NR, et al. Impaired prepulse inhibition of acoustic and tactile startle response in patients with Huntington’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1995;58(2):192–200. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.58.2.192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 188.Tao K, et al. Disrupted social memory ensembles in the ventral hippocampus underlie social amnesia in autism-associated Shank3 mutant mice. Mol Psychiatry. 2022;9:7. doi: 10.1038/s41380-021-01430-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 189.Henderson HA, et al. Self-referenced memory, social cognition, and symptom presentation in autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009;50(7):853–861. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.02059.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 190.Harvey PO, Lepage M. Neural correlates of recognition memory of social information in people with schizophrenia. J Psychiatry Neurosci : JPN. 2014;39(2):97–109. doi: 10.1503/jpn.130007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 191.Takahashi H, et al. Spatial working memory deficit correlates with disorganization symptoms and social functioning in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2005;59(4):453–460. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2005.01398.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 192.Mavrogiorgou P, et al. Social cognition and metacognition in obsessive-compulsive disorder: an explorative pilot study. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2016;266(3):209–216. doi: 10.1007/s00406-016-0669-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 193.Sowerby P, Seal S, Tripp G. Working memory deficits in ADHD: the contribution of age, learning/language difficulties, and task parameters. J Atten Disord. 2011;15(6):461–472. doi: 10.1177/1087054710370674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 194.Demeter G, et al. Intact short-term memory and impaired executive functions in obsessive compulsive disorder. Ideggyogyaszati szemle. 2013;66(1–2):35–41. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 195.Dittrich WH, Johansen T. Cognitive deficits of executive functions and decision-making in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Scand J Psychol. 2013;54(5):393–400. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 196.Dutschke LL, et al. Gesture impairments in schizophrenia are linked to increased movement and prolonged motor planning and execution. Schizophr Res. 2018;200:42–49. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 197.Carnahan H, Aguilar O, Malla A, Norman R. An investigation into movement planning and execution deficits in individuals with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 1997;23(3):213–221. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(96)00106-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 198.Shafer RL, Lewis MH, Newell KM, Bodfish JW. Atypical neural processing during the execution of complex sensorimotor behavior in autism. Behav Brain Res. 2021;409:113337. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2021.113337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 199.Stoit AM, van Schie HT, Slaats-Willemse DI, Buitelaar JK. Grasping motor impairments in autism: not action planning but movement execution is deficient. J Autism Dev Disord. 2013;43(12):2793–2806. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1825-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 200.Chey J, Lee J, Kim YS, Kwon SM, Shin YM. Spatial working memory span, delayed response and executive function in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2002;110(3):259–271. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(02)00105-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 201.van der Wee NJ, et al. Spatial working memory deficits in obsessive compulsive disorder are associated with excessive engagement of the medial frontal cortex. Neuroimage. 2003;20(4):2271–2280. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 202.Steele SD, Minshew NJ, Luna B, Sweeney JA. Spatial working memory deficits in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37(4):605–612. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0202-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 203.Possin KL, Filoteo JV, Song DD, Salmon DP. Spatial and object working memory deficits in Parkinson’s disease are due to impairment in different underlying processes. Neuropsychology. 2008;22(5):585–595. doi: 10.1037/a0012613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 204.Possin KL, et al. Egocentric and allocentric visuospatial working memory in premotor Huntington’s disease: a double dissociation with caudate and hippocampal volumes. Neuropsychologia. 2017;101:57–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.04.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 205.Avanzino L, et al. Time processing and motor control in movement disorders. Front Hum Neurosci. 2016;10:631. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00631. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 206.Boyd LA, et al. Motor sequence chunking is impaired by basal ganglia stroke. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2009;92(1):35–44. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2009.02.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 207.Nagasaki H, Nakamura R, Taniguchi R. Disturbances of rhythm formation in patients with Parkinson’s disease: part II. a forced oscillation model. Perceptual Mot Skills. 1978;46(1):79–87. doi: 10.2466/pms.1978.46.1.79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 208.Lemoine L, et al. The specific role of the striatum in interval timing: the Huntington’s disease model. NeuroImage Clinical. 2021;32:102865. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2021.102865. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 209.Churchyard AJ, et al. Gait dysfunction in Huntington’s disease: parkinsonism and a disorder of timing. Implications for movement rehabilitation. Adv Neurol. 2001;87:375–385. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 210.Freeman JS, et al. Abnormalities of motor timing in Huntington’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 1996;2(2):81–93. doi: 10.1016/1353-8020(96)00009-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 211.Snowden AW, Buhusi CV. Neural correlates of interval timing deficits in schizophrenia. Front Hum Neurosci. 2019;13:9. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 212.Ward RD, Kellendonk C, Kandel ER, Balsam PD. Timing as a window on cognition in schizophrenia. Neuropharmacology. 2012;62(3):1175–1181. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.04.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 213.Carroll CA, O'Donnell BF, Shekhar A, Hetrick WP. Timing dysfunctions in schizophrenia span from millisecond to several-second durations. Brain Cogn. 2009;70(2):181–190. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 214.Shapiro Z, Huang-Pollock C. A diffusion-model analysis of timing deficits among children with ADHD. Neuropsychology. 2019;33(6):883–892. doi: 10.1037/neu0000562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 215.Slater JL, Tate MC. Timing deficits in ADHD: insights from the neuroscience of musical rhythm. Front Comput Neurosci. 2018;12:51. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2018.00051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 216.Noreika V, Falter CM, Rubia K. Timing deficits in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): evidence from neurocognitive and neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychologia. 2013;51(2):235–266. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.09.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 217.Falter CM, Noreika V, Wearden JH, Bailey AJ. More consistent, yet less sensitive: interval timing in autism spectrum disorders. Q J Exp Psychol. 2012;65(11):2093–2107. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.690770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 218.D'Cruz AM, et al. Lateralized response timing deficits in autism. Biol Psychiat. 2009;66(4):393–397. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.01.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 219.Allman MJ, Meck WH. Pathophysiological distortions in time perception and timed performance. Brain : a J Neurol. 2012;135(Pt 3):656–677. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 220.Vicario CM, et al. Time processing in children with Tourette’s syndrome. Brain Cogn. 2010;73(1):28–34. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.