Abstract
The brain’s remarkable ability to learn and execute various motor behaviours harnesses the capacity of neural populations to generate a variety of activity patterns. Here we explore systematic changes in preparatory activity in motor cortex that accompany motor learning. We trained rhesus monkeys to learn an arm-reaching task1 in a curl force field that elicited new muscle forces for some, but not all, movement directions2,3. We found that in a neural subspace predictive of hand forces, changes in preparatory activity tracked the learned behavioural modifications and reassociated4 existing activity patterns with updated movements. Along a neural population dimension orthogonal to the force-predictive subspace, we discovered that preparatory activity shifted uniformly for all movement directions, including those unaltered by learning. During a washout period when the curl field was removed, preparatory activity gradually reverted in the force-predictive subspace, but the uniform shift persisted. These persistent preparatory activity patterns may retain a motor memory of the learned field5,6 and support accelerated relearning of the same curl field. When a set of distinct curl fields was learned in sequence, we observed a corresponding set of field-specific uniform shifts which separated the associated motor memories in the neural state space7–9. The precise geometry of these uniform shifts in preparatory activity could serve to index motor memories, facilitating the acquisition, retention and retrieval of a broad motor repertoire.
Motor learning encompasses a wide range of phenomena, from low-level calibration of movement parameters to high-level cognitive decisions in action selection10. Motor adaptation is a form of motor learning by which motor commands are modified to achieve desired movements in a new environment. Decades of studies have explored the behavioural principles of motor adaptation, describing the process of error-driven movement calibration, the generalization of learned skills across contexts, memory retention and savings, and interference between multiple skills2,3,7,8,10–13. However, the neural mechanisms that support these diverse motor learning phenomena remain poorly understood.
One emerging approach to understanding these neural computations is through the study of neural population dynamics, which has provided insight into complex activity patterns that defy understanding at the level of individual neurons14–16. Recently, this framework has begun to elucidate the neural foundation of motor learning at the population level17–20. Preparatory neural activity that precedes movement serves to initialize the pattern-generating neural population dynamics that control movement. To support motor learning, preparatory neural states and the subsequent neural population dynamics must adapt to modify outgoing motor commands. Consequently, we expect that some changes in preparatory activity that accompany motor learning would be tethered to changes in motor output18,20. Moreover, we propose that additional changes in neural preparatory activity, not directly coupled to movement output, might also emerge during learning. Such changes might facilitate learning and retention by organizing the population dynamics that implement new motor behaviours. We sought to test these hypotheses using a curl force field motor learning task.
Motor adaptation in a curl field task
We trained two rhesus monkeys (U and V) to perform an instructed-delay reaching task that elicited adaptation to counteract a curl force field (Fig. 1a, Extended Data Fig. 1a). Before learning, monkeys made straight centre-out reaches towards each of 12 targets by controlling a haptic device. In the learning block, monkeys reached towards a single trained target while the device applied a curl force field that was perpendicular to movement direction and proportional to hand speed. Late in learning, the curl field remained active for reaches to the trained target, which were interleaved with reaches to all 12 targets with an error clamp rendered by the device. The error clamp constrained movements to a straight line towards the target, hence clamping error feedback to zero to assess the feed-forward learning of the curl field. Finally, in the washout block, the curl field and error clamp were removed to probe the after-effects of learning and de-adaptation.
Monkeys displayed gradual behavioural learning and washout, performing straighter reaches with reduced lateral deviation (Fig. 1b, c). The error clamp revealed a bell-shaped spatial pattern of generalization where the strength of learning-induced force changes fell off with increasing angular distance from the trained target (Fig. 1d), consistent with human behavioural studies2,3,11.
A neural subspace tracks generalization
We recorded neural activity in dorsal premotor (PMd) and primary motor (M1) cortices using Neuropixels probes, Utah arrays and V-probe linear arrays. Single-neuron activity during learning and washout was heterogeneous and complex, consistent with previous reports5,19. To search for structured changes in preparatory neural population activity accompanying learning14,15,21, we applied targeted dimensionality reduction22 (TDR) on before-learning trials, which identified a neural subspace in which preparatory states were predictive of initial hand forces in the upcoming movement. In this force-predictive subspace, before-learning neural states were radially organized by reach directions18,23 (Fig. 2a); during learning, preparatory states of the trained target rotated towards the preparatory state of the adjacent target opposite to the curl field direction (Fig. 2a, top-right inset). This rotatory progression probably reflected the preparation of initial compensatory forces to counter the curl field. Preparatory states in this subspace predicted the observed hand forces with high accuracy (Fig. 2a, bottom inset, Extended Data Fig. 2a, b).
Following learning, preparatory states for nearby, untrained targets also rotated towards the adjacent preparatory states (Fig. 2b). These rotatory neural state shifts followed a similar spatial profile as behavioural generalization (Fig. 2c, Extended Data Fig. 2c), which was bell-shaped around the trained target with spatial asymmetry (Extended Data Fig. 1c) and thereby constituted a neural correlate of motor learning generalization, as predicted by previous work24. These neural changes may reflect the state of an adapting internal model that maps between desired movements and neural commands. In this framework, generalization may result from a neural population code in which spatial basis functions are shared by reaches to nearby targets25. Adapting to a curl field at the trained target modifies this shared basis, thereby influencing untrained reaches in a spatially localized manner.
The learning-induced changes in preparatory states within this force-predictive subspace were closely coupled to changes in movement output. These changes are similar to the re-aiming strategy reported in visuomotor rotation (VMR) learning tasks, in which motor preparatory activity rotates in the opposite direction of the rotated visual feedback18,26,27. They are also consistent with a more general ‘reassociation’ strategy observed during short-term brain-computer interface (BCI) learning4. Within this neural subspace, the motor system may repurpose existing activity patterns, reflecting a common strategy across different motor learning contexts4,18,26–30.
A uniform shift of neural population activity
We next applied principal component analysis (PCA) to preparatory activity to probe for additional changes during learning. The first two principal components largely overlapped with the force-predictive subspace (Extended Data Fig. 3a); however, along the third principal component, preparatory states shifted uniformly during learning for all targets, including those far from the trained target (Fig. 2d). To determine a neural axis that captures this uniform shift in the full-dimensional neural space, we defined the uniform-shift axis as the vector connecting the centroid of before-learning states to the centroid of after-learning states (subtracting a consistently small component within the force-predictive TDR subspace; Extended Data Fig. 3a). Along this uniform-shift axis, preparatory states of the trained target shifted gradually during learning (Fig. 2d, Extended Data Fig. 2d). We performed a variety of control analyses that demonstrated that the uniform shift could not be attributed to learning-unrelated changes in behaviour, including speed, muscle activation, stiffness, posture and error-clamp movements (Extended Data Figs. 1d, 3–5), or to changes in neural tuning, including preferred direction and background drift31 (Supplementary Table 2, Supplementary Note 2). The uniform shift therefore may facilitate learning itself rather than mirror behavioural changes.
This uniform shift reflected the emergence of new preparatory activity patterns that were not used before learning. These new patterns were identified using a neural repertoire metric4 (Fig. 2e, Extended Data Fig. 2e), which revealed that these changes were specific to learning, compared to control sessions without curl fields or with random pulse perturbation forces that simulated the magnitude of the curl field but did not admit learning (Fig. 2f, g). Moreover, we did not observe a uniform shift or repertoire change during VMR learning (Extended Data Fig. 2f, g) where reassociation was observed, consistent with previous studies18,20.
Uniform shifts index motor memories
Next, we tested whether distinct uniform shifts might accompany learning multiple curl fields. We trained monkeys to learn different curl fields sequentially within the same session or over multiple sessions. To compare learning over multiple sessions, we tracked a stable neural population with highly similar cross-session waveforms (Extended Data Fig. 6a, b). We then identified the uniform-shift axis for each learned field and computed the dot product between each pair of axes. For two opposite curl fields applied to the same target, the uniform-shift axes were nearly antiparallel, such that preparatory neural states shifted in opposite directions with respect to the before-learning states (Fig. 3a, Extended Data Fig. 6c). For curl fields trained at different reach directions (up, right or down), the uniform-shift axes were nearly orthogonal (Fig. 3b, c, Extended Data Fig. 6c). Curl-field identity was reliably decoded above chance on the basis of the shifted post-learning preparatory states (Fig. 3d). These geometric relationships suggest that uniform shifts index specific curl fields, mapping motor memories to distinct, precisely arranged locations in neural state space.
Uniform-shift geometry and interference
This contrast between orthogonal uniform shifts at well-spaced trained targets and opposing shifts at the same target suggests a connection between uniform-shift geometry and interference. Interference is a slowing of learning that can occur when adapting to opposing fields simultaneously3,7,8,10. When two fields interfere, the associated uniform shifts might be oriented so that trial-by-trial changes partially oppose each other. To test this, we designed an interference experiment which interleaved reaches to two targets separated by 30° (within the effect of spatial generalization, Fig. 1d) with opposite curl fields applied (Fig. 4a). Hand deviation errors decreased over hundreds of trials (Fig. 4b, Extended Data Fig. 7a); however, more trials were required to learn either field (more than 400 trials) versus when learning each field individually (fewer than 200 trials; Fig. 5g), indicating that simultaneous learning was slowed by partial interference. In a subsequent sequential-learning block, both fields were trained separately and hand deviation errors further decreased (Fig. 4b, Extended Data Fig. 7a).
Within a force-predictive TDR subspace, preparatory states for the two trained targets gradually rotated in opposite directions during simultaneous learning, and this progression continued during sequential learning, mirroring behavioural performance (Fig. 4d, Extended Data Fig. 7c). At nearby targets, we observed behavioural and neural generalization from learning both fields (Fig. 4c, Extended Data Fig. 7b). We then identified uniform-shift axes for the two fields using preparatory states during sequential learning, which were oriented 127° (monkey U) and 128° (monkey V) apart. This angle is intermediate between orthogonal uniform-shift axes for fields applied at targets 90° apart (Fig. 3b), and antiparallel axes for opposing fields applied at the same target (Fig. 3a). During simultaneous learning, shifts of preparatory states along these axes partially opposed each other, suggesting a neural mechanism of interference (Fig. 4e, Extended Data Fig. 7d).
We conducted a second interference experiment with opposite fields applied at the same reach target on randomly interleaved trials (Extended Data Fig. 8a). As expected, monkeys were unable to learn the two fields simultaneously, indicating complete interference7, but subsequently learned both fields sequentially (Extended Data Fig. 8b). As the uniform-shift axes associated with opposing fields at a single target were antiparallel (Fig. 3a), we predicted little net progress along this shared dimension during simultaneous learning. Indeed, preparatory neural states for the two fields remained unseparated within the force-predictive subspace and along the shared uniform-shift axis (Extended Data Fig. 8c, e). Subsequently, preparatory states shifted along the antiparallel uniform-shift axes during sequential learning (Extended Data Fig. 8e). Collectively, these findings indicate a correspondence between interference and the geometry of uniform-shift axes during motor preparation.
Additionally, in both interference experiments, we observed a residual neural shift that accompanied interference during simultaneous learning, orthogonal to the field-specific uniform shifts during sequential learning (Fig. 4f, Extended Data Figs. 7d, 8e). Notably, the residual interference shift occurred even when no net learning was observed. We speculate that this residual shift probably relates to an attempt to index neural activity patterns specific to the interference context, which might facilitate strategies tailored to adapting to an unpredictable environment (for example, impedance control to stabilize the limb32,33).
Uniform shift may retain a motor memory
Finally, we examined whether learning-induced shifts in preparatory activity persisted after de-adaptation as a motor memory5,24,34. Over hundreds of washout trials without the field, monkeys gradually reverted to their before-learning reaching behaviour (Fig. 1b, c, Extended Data Fig. 4a). Washout preparatory states in the force-predictive subspace correspondingly rotated back towards the before-learning states (Fig. 5a, c, Extended Data Fig. 9a). By contrast, along the uniform-shift axis, washout states remained separated from before-learning states (Fig. 5b, d, Extended Data Fig. 9b). Furthermore, preparatory states shifted uniformly again during washout along a second, nearly orthogonal dimension (Fig. 5e, Extended Data Fig. 9e). Before-learning, late-learning, and late-washout conditions could be reliably decoded from single-trial preparatory states (Fig. 5f). Collectively, these results underscore that washout is not simply the reverse of learning and suggest that the persistent uniform shift of preparatory activity potentially retains a motor memory of the learned field.
Furthermore, we performed a relearning experiment in which monkeys were exposed to the same field again after washout within the same session10. Monkeys relearned the curl field faster than the initial learning, a hallmark of motor memory retention (Fig. 5g, Extended Data Fig. 9c). Neural trajectories during relearning approached the late-learning neural trajectory faster than during initial learning (Fig. 5h, Extended Data Fig. 9d). Moreover, preparatory states after relearning were indistinguishable from the initial learning states within each session (Fig. 5i). We also observed that uniform shifts for the same field in two sessions 18 days apart were close to parallel (Extended Data Fig. 9f). These results support the hypothesis that the uniform shift indexes and stores a field-specific motor memory.
We also assessed the relationship between distances neural states progressed along the uniform-shift learning axis and behavioural learning rates. Within a session, uniform-shift distances were significantly smaller during relearning than during initial learning (Fig. 5j). Across five sessions with a consistent neural population, uniform-shift distances were strongly correlated with behavioural learning rates (Fig. 5k), suggesting that if preparatory states begin further along a given uniform-shift axis, learning will proceed faster.
Uniform shift is specific to motor preparation
The uniform shift emerged during motor preparation and our results were largely insensitive to the preparatory time window analysed (not shown) owing to relatively stationary neural activity during the preparatory period (Extended Data Fig. 10a). By contrast, we did not find repertoire changes in baseline activity (Extended Data Fig. 10b) or uniform shifts in peri-movement activity. Shifts of peri-movement states during learning were local and matched the profile of behavioural generalization (Extended Data Fig. 10c–f). Peri-movement states reverted to before-learning patterns after washout (Extended Data Fig. 10g, h), mirroring the de-adapted movement. Correspondingly, late-washout neural trajectories were more similar to before-learning neural trajectories during the peri-movement period than the preparatory period (Extended Data Fig. 9g, h). Taken together, the uniform shift was a learning-related feature of neural population activity specific to motor preparation.
Discussion
Through the lens of curl field learning, we identified structured changes in cortical preparatory activity that reflected distinct components of motor learning. We found reassociation-like changes in preparatory activity in a movement-predictive neural subspace closely coupled to changes in movement parameters, similar to those reported in VMR and short-term BCI learning4,18,26,27,29,30 (see Supplementary Discussion). Notably, we discovered a shift of preparatory states along an orthogonal neural dimension that occurred uniformly for all reach targets, including those with unaltered movement. In a series of learning experiments with multiple curl fields, we established that these uniform shifts were arranged in neural state space with a precise geometry that appeared to index distinct motor memories and reduce interference. Following washout, the uniform shift persisted even as reaching behaviour de-adapted. This persistent uniform shift correlated with faster relearning and may serve to retain a short-term memory of recent learning.
Cortical preparatory states provide the initial condition of the dynamical system whose evolution generates activity patterns that drive movement14,21,23,35–37. Uniform shifts that separate these initial states may serve to isolate learning-induced modifications to the subsequent neural dynamics, thus separating motor memories to facilitate adaptive behavioural improvements in a specific context7,8. Conversely, when the motor system is unable to engage separate indices, such as when opposing curl fields are randomly interleaved3,13, opposing modifications to neural dynamics adjust the neural trajectory originating from the shared preparatory state, resulting in interference. Recent behavioural studies demonstrate that when certain contextual cues or movement components are added to differentiate movements, opposing fields can be learned without interference7,8. Our results suggest that the motor system leverages the shifts of preparatory states along orthogonal neural dimensions to index distinct motor memories, consistent with the central role of movement preparation reported in these studies.
Extended Data
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We thank the members of the Shenoy laboratory at Stanford University for comments and discussions on the methods and results; T. Fisher for help with experiments; M. Risch, M. Wechsler and R. Reeder for expert veterinary care; B. Davis for administrative support; and W. L. Gore for donating Preclude artificial dura used as part of the chronic electrode array implantation procedure. X.S. was supported by a Stanford Interdisciplinary Graduate Fellowship, a Stanford Bio-X Honorary Fellowship and Stanford Department of Biology Funding. D.J.O. was supported by a US National Science Foundation graduate research fellowship and a Stanford Graduate Fellowship. M.D.G. was supported by an NIH K99/R00 award NIMH-K99MH121533. E.M.T. was supported by an NIH NRSA grant 1F31NS089376-01, a Stanford Graduate Fellowship and an NSF IGERT grant 0734683. S.V. was supported by an NIH F31 Ruth L. Kirschstein National Research Service Award 1F31NS103409-01, an NSF Graduate Research Fellowship and a Ric Weiland Stanford Graduate Fellowship. K.V.S. was supported by the following awards: NIH Director’s Pioneer Award 8DP1HD075623, Defense Advanced Research Projects Agency (DARPA) Biological Technology Office (BTO) ‘NeuroFAST’ award W911NF-14-2-0013, the Simons Foundation Collaboration on the Global Brain awards 543045, the Office of Naval Research N000141812158 and the Howard Hughes Medical Institute.
Competing interests
K.V.S. is a consultant to Neuralink Corp. and CTRL-Labs Inc., in the Reality Labs Division of Meta (formerly FaceBook). He is on the Scientific Advisory Boards of Inscopix Inc., Mind X Inc. and Heal Inc. These entities did not support or influence this work. The authors declare no other competing interests.
Footnotes
Online content
Any methods, additional references, Nature Research reporting summaries, source data, extended data, supplementary information, acknowledgements, peer review information; details of author contributions and competing interests; and statements of data and code availability are available at https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021-04329-x.
Code availability
The code for the repertoire change analysis is available on github (https://github.com/mattgolub/bci_learning). The code for the TDR and uniform-shift analyses is available on github (https://github.com/xlsun79/TDRandUniformShiftAnalyses). All other code is available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
Supplementary information The online version contains supplementary material available at https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021-04329-x.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this paper.
Data availability
The data that support the findings of the current study are available from the corresponding authors upon reasonable request. Source data are provided with this paper.
References
- 1.Shadmehr R & Mussa-Ivaldi FA Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task. J. Neurosci 14, 3208–3224 (1994). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Shadmehr R Generalization as a behavioral window to the neural mechanisms of learning internal models. Hum. Mov. Sci 23, 543–568 (2004). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Howard IS & Franklin DW Neural tuning functions underlie both generalization and interference. PLoS ONE 10, e0131268 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Golub MD et al. Learning by neural reassociation. Nat. Neurosci 21, 607–616 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Li CS, Padoa-Schioppa C & Bizzi E Neuronal correlates of motor performance and motor learning in the primary motor cortex of monkeys adapting to an external force field. Neuron 30, 593–607 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Xiao J Premotor neuronal plasticity in monkeys adapting to a new dynamic environment. Eur. J. Neurosci 22, 3266–3280 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Sheahan HR, Franklin DW & Wolpert DM Motor planning, not execution, separates motor memories. Neuron 92, 773–779 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Sheahan HR, Ingram JN, Žalalytė GM & Wolpert DM Imagery of movements immediately following performance allows learning of motor skills that interfere. Sci. Rep 8, 14330 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Schween R, Langsdorf L, Taylor JA & Hegele M How different effectors and action effects modulate the formation of separate motor memories. Sci. Rep 9, 17040 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Krakauer JW, Hadjiosif AM, Xu J, Wong AL & Haith AM Motor learning. Compr. Physiol 9, 613–663 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Thoroughman KA & Shadmehr R Learning of action through adaptive combination of motor primitives. Nature 407, 742–747 (2000). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Herzfeld DJ, Vaswani PA, Marko MK & Shadmehr R A memory of errors in sensorimotor learning. Science 345, 1349–1353 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Howard IS, Wolpert DM & Franklin DW The effect of contextual cues on the encoding of motor memories. J. Neurophysiol 109, 2632–2644 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Shenoy KV, Sahani M & Churchland MM Cortical control of arm movements: a dynamical systems perspective. Annu. Rev. Neurosci 36, 337–359 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Vyas S, Golub MD, Sussillo D & Shenoy KV Computation through neural population dynamics. Annu. Rev. Neurosci 43, 249–275 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Sauerbrei BA et al. Cortical pattern generation during dexterous movement is input-driven. Nature 577, 386–391 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Stavisky SD, Kao JC, Ryu SI & Shenoy KV Trial-by-trial motor cortical correlates of a rapidly adapting visuomotor internal model. J. Neurosci 37, 1721–1732 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Vyas S et al. Neural population dynamics underlying motor learning transfer. Neuron 97, 11771186.e3 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Perich MG, Gallego JA & Miller LE A neural population mechanism for rapid learning. Neuron 100, 964–976.e7 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Vyas S, O’Shea DJ, Ryu SI & Shenoy KV Causal role of motor preparation during error-driven learning. Neuron 106, 329–339.e4 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Churchland MM et al. Neural population dynamics during reaching. Nature 487, 51–56 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Mante V, Sussillo D, Shenoy KV & Newsome WT Context-dependent computation by recurrent dynamics in prefrontal cortex. Nature 503, 78–84 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Even-Chen N, Sheffer B, Vyas S, Ryu SI & Shenoy KV Structure and variability of delay activity in premotor cortex. PLoS Comput. Biol 15, e1006808 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Arce F, Novick I, Mandelblat-Cerf Y & Vaadia E Neuronal correlates of memory formation in motor cortex after adaptation to force field. J. Neurosci 30, 9189–9198 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Donchin O, Francis JT & Shadmehr R Quantifying generalization from trial-by-trial behavior of adaptive systems that learn with basis functions: theory and experiments in human motor control. J. Neurosci 23, 9032–9045 (2003). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Jarosiewicz B et al. Functional network reorganization during learning in a brain-computer interface paradigm. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 19486–19491 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Chase SM, Kass RE & Schwartz AB Behavioral and neural correlates of visuomotor adaptation observed through a brain–computer interface in primary motor cortex. J. Neurophysiol 108, 624–644 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Oby ER et al. New neural activity patterns emerge with long-term learning. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 116, 15210–15215 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Sakellaridi S et al. Intrinsic variable learning for brain–machine interface control by human anterior intraparietal cortex. Neuron 102, 694–705.e3 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Hwang EJ, Bailey PM & Andersen RA Volitional control of neural activity relies on the natural motor repertoire. Curr. Biol 23, 353–361 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Rokni U, Richardson AG, Bizzi E & Seung HS Motor learning with unstable neural representations. Neuron 54, 653–666 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Franklin DW, Osu R, Burdet E, Kawato M & Milner TE Adaptation to stable and unstable dynamics achieved by combined impedance control and inverse dynamics model. J. Neurophysiol 90, 3270–3282 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Heald JB, Franklin DW & Wolpert DM Increasing muscle co-contraction speeds up internal model acquisition during dynamic motor learning. Sci. Rep 8, 16355 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Richardson AG, Borghi T & Bizzi E Activity of the same motor cortex neurons during repeated experience with perturbed movement dynamics. J. Neurophysiol 107, 3144–3154 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Churchland MM, Cunningham JP, Kaufman MT, Ryu SI & Shenoy KV Cortical preparatory activity: representation of movement or first cog in a dynamical machine? Neuron 68, 387–400 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Russo AA et al. Motor cortex embeds muscle-like commands in an untangled population response. Neuron 97, 953–966.e8 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Ames KC, Ryu SI & Shenoy KV Neural dynamics of reaching following incorrect or absent motor preparation. Neuron 81, 438–451 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of the current study are available from the corresponding authors upon reasonable request. Source data are provided with this paper.