Abstract
The increase in online media use and mental health problems have prompted investigations into their association, although most literature is focussed on deleterious effects. We assessed the aetiology of media use and mental health associations (M age = 22.14, SD = 0.85) using twin (n = 4000 pairs) and polygenic score methods (n = 6000 unrelated individuals) in the Twins Early Development Study. Beyond the traditionally explored negative uses of online media (online victimisation and problematic internet use), we investigate general media uses such as posting online and watching videos and distinguish both positive (pro-social behaviour) and negative (anxiety, depression, peer and behaviour problems) mental health measures. Negative media use correlated with poor mental health (r = 0.11–0.32), but general media use correlated with prosocial behaviour (r = 0.20) and fewer behavioural problems (r = − 0.24). Twin analyses showed that both general and negative media use were moderately heritable (ranging from 20 to 49%) and their associations with mental health were primarily due to genetic influences (44–88%). Genetic sensitivity analysis combining polygenic scores with heritability estimates also suggest genetic confounding. Results indicate research on the mental health impact of media use should adopt genetically informed designs to strengthen causal inference.
Subject terms: Behavioural genetics, Development
Introduction
Previous research on the negative impact of screen time focussed on time spent watching television. The dominant interpretation was that time spent watching television was not only negatively impacting obesity, sleep and skills1–3, but was also leading to poor educational achievement4, increased aggression5,6 and pervasive attention problems7,8. Correlations between television viewing and negative outcomes were often viewed as reflecting the causal impact of television on developmental outcomes9.
In 1990, the first genetically informed analysis of individual differences in time spent watching television challenged the notion that television is a pure ‘environmental’ factor10. Both parent–offspring and sibling adoption designs yielded evidence for substantial genetic influence on individual differences in television viewing, suggesting that young people are not passive recipients of their television environment but actively select, modify and interpret their experiences in line with their genetics11. The authors concluded that if television viewing shows substantial genetic influence, then it is possible that associations between behavioural outcomes and television viewing are in part, explained by genetic variation.
Decades later, the anxiety surrounding time spent on television has been replaced by concerns about time spent online12. Considerable research effort has been dedicated to understanding problematic mobile phone use and the extent to which excessive use and impulse control problems are sufficient to warrant a label of ‘addiction’12,13. It remains less understood to what extent mobile phone use shares the behavioural and neurobiological components typically associated with more established addictions. In addition, terminologies used to describe excessive phone use vary and include, but are not limited to ‘mobile phone addiction’, ‘problematic use’, ‘excessive use’, and ‘overuse of mobile phones’, and although ‘addictive’ like behavior associated with mobile phone use is a recognized problem, it is not recognized as a category in diagnostic manuals. Recent calls for better integration of the uniqueness of mobile phone use and the heterogeneity in its manifestation can be addressed by integrating individual differences into the framework, which is the goal of the present study.
Media use is one of the most influential environments during emerging adulthood14,15, a developmental stage in the late teens and early twenties marked by greater autonomy and identity exploration14. Online media use has become ubiquitous in developed countries with young adults spending more time on mobile phones than any other age group16. Most of the time spent on mobile phones does not involve making phone calls. Mobile phones serve many other functions such as sending messages, watching and recording videos, posting and sharing media content17. Crucially, online media serves as a constant source of connection to social media platforms, which are changing the landscape of social interactions and prompting new research avenues exploring the impact of online social connections on offline relations.
In contrast to television viewing at home, emerging adults have a great deal more autonomy about when they engage online, what they do online and how long they do it for. From a behavioural genetic perspective, this increased autonomy could be reflected in decreased estimates of shared environmental influence and increased estimates of genetic influence, as young people are able to tailor their media use to their individual interests, which we know to be genetically influenced18. We previously demonstrated that inherited DNA differences account for up to 40% of individual differences in time spent on educational media, entertaining media, gaming and the social network Facebook19. These results were based on a sample of over 2000 16-year-old twin pairs participating in the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS), a longitudinal birth cohort study of twin pairs born in England and Wales20. Online media use in these previous analyses was limited to home computer use before the first 4G network became available in the UK, which led to an exponential increase in use of mobile phones and other portable devices21. An updated exploration of genetic influence on mobile phone use is needed to reflect the profound changes in the digital environment of emerging adults.
The increase in young people’s online media use coincides with a substantial increase in self-reported mental health problems including greater demand for counselling services, more admissions for self-harm and more frequent referrals to mental health services22–24. Despite these reported increases, little is known about the role of social media use in these negative life outcomes25,26. Better understanding of the aetiology of online media use during early adulthood is needed given this life period has been acknowledged as a particular sensitive period for the emergence of mental health problems and a prodromal stage for which mental health interventions may be more effective. In addition, there has been an increased demand for more nuanced investigation of associations between media use and mental health, moving past a focus solely on the deleterious effects of online media use, to a more balanced view incorporating potential positive social media influences and individual differences27. It is timely to systematically investigate the relationship between diverse measures of online media use and mental health in emerging adulthood.
Genetically informed designs are needed to quantify associations between media use and mental health given decades of genetic research demonstrating that measures of the environment are partly heritable. As such, it is unclear whether shared genetic influences partly confound the relationships between media use and mental health. This could happen, for example, if heritable traits such as impulsiveness influence social media use and confer risk to mental health problems (e.g., ADHD). This research answers the call for novel approaches to address the role of individual differences in interactions with digital technologies28.
To date, a limited number of genetically informed studies have examined the relationship between media use and mental health. These studies focussed primarily on the negative side of online media use, including online bullying, victimisation and problematic internet use and were largely based on small twin samples29–33 or candidate gene studies34.
The present study investigates the links between explicitly negative and general media use and mental health in young people using multiple genetically informed approaches, with complementary strengths. In a large sample of British twins, we applied twin analyses and polygenic scoring methods to (1) estimate overall genetic influence on online media use and measures of mental health and wellbeing and (2) to assess possible genetic origins of the association between media and mental health. In addition, we employed a novel genetic sensitivity analysis to examine genetic confounding in the media use and mental health association with increasing polygenic score accuracy (to that of SNP and twin-based heritability estimates).These complementary analyses rely on different assumptions, enabling triangulation of evidence on the genetic origins of media use and its association with mental health.
Materials and methods
Sample
Analyses were based on participants in the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS), a longitudinal birth cohort study of twins born in England and Wales between 1994 and 1996. TEDS participants with available phenotypic data on measures of general media use (n = 3930 twin pairs), online victimisation (n = 3931 twin pairs) and problematic media use (n = 3934 twin pairs), were included in the present study. Sample sizes for all media and mental health measures by zygosity including the number of monozygotic (MZ), dizygotic same sex (DZss) and dizygotic opposite sex (DSoss) twin pairs can be found in Supplementary Tables S1 and S2. Despite some attrition, the remaining cohort, as well as the genotyped subsample have been shown to reasonably represent the UK population for that birth cohort (i.e., 93% white in National averages from UK cohorts of parents with children born in late 1990’s early 2000’s compared to 92.9% white in TEDS)20. DNA was obtained and genotyped for one member of a twin pair, yielding a genotyped sample of unrelated individuals with media and mental health measures for the DNA based analyses of 6415. Both the twin sample (92.9%) and the genotyped subsample (99.9%) were predominately white European, therefore findings cannot readily generalise to other populations. Ethical approval for this study was received from King’s College London Ethics Committee and all methods were carried out in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations. All participants provided written informed consent.
Phenotypic measures
Phenotypic measures were collected when the twins had reached emerging adulthood (M age = 22.14, SD = 0.85)20. Data were collected using either a smartphone application, via the web, or paper and pencil questionnaires.
Problematic media use
A large literature demonstrates robust associations between problematic internet use and negative life outcomes including sleep disturbances, depression and poor social adjustment yet psychometrically validated screening instruments are lacking. We assessed problematic media use using an adapted version of the Problematic and Risky Internet Use Screening Scale35 (PRIUSS). This 18-item scale assessed problematic internet use that resulted in social or emotional impairment, including questions such as ‘How often have you lost motivation to do other things that need to get done because of the internet’ and ‘How often have you lost sleep due to nighttime internet use’. Based on analyses of pilot data, the scale was reduced to a 6-item measure with responses given on a 5-point scale (0 = “never”, 1 = “rarely”, 2 = “sometimes/quite often”, 3 = “often”, 4 = “very often/always”, 5 = “always”,). The 6 items included: ‘Have you felt irritated when the internet is not working?’, ‘Have you experienced feelings of withdrawal from not using the internet?’, ‘Have you prioritised internet use over important, everyday activities?’, ‘Have you lost motivation to do other things that need to get done because of the internet?’, ‘Have you lost sleep due to night time internet use?’, ‘Do you feel you have used the internet excessively?’. The PRIUSS shows excellent reliability and good validity, but was specifically developed to identify problems associated with Internet use. To better capture mobile phone use specifically, we amended the original questions, changing ‘internet’ to ‘on your mobile phone’. The ammended scale demonstrated high internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.92) and good test–retest reliability (r = 0.70).
Online victimisation
Online media use in young adulthood coincides with a neurodevelopmental period of less self-regulation and heightened social and emotional vulnerability, as many mental health problems have their onset during this period. Increased online media use may expose some young people to negative consequences such as being the perpetrator or victim of bullying and represents an important aspect of online environments needing further genetically sensitive research attention. For this reason, we assessed online victimisation using the Multidimensional Peer Victimization Scale–Revised36 (MPVS-revised)which assessed the occurrence of online aggression and whether the individual was the target or perpetrator. Online victimisation frequently co-occurs with face-to-face bullying victimisation, but with the increased anonymity of the perpetrator and its inescapable nature, due to not being confined to particular locations or times (i.e., at school), may represent a potent threat to young people’s mental health. The MPVS-revised is a reliable measure with good psychometric properties and has been validated in a UK sample like the present study. For the present analyses, only items about online victimisation were included. Items assessing online victimisation included, for example, “How often has someone sent you a nasty text (excluding family or partner)?” and “How often has someone written something spiteful about you in a chat room (excluding family or partner)?”. Online victimisation was measured in four items, with responses given on a 3-point scale (0 = “not at all”, 1 = “once”, and 2 = “more than once”). These measures showed good reliability (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.89) and test–retest reliability (r = 0.79).
General media use
Current research trends tend to view media use as primarily problematic and negative for young people. A small but growing body of literature37 suggests that, for some people, online media use can be beneficial. For example, online media use might facilitate individualised learning, creating social networks, accessing online support, and maintaining relationships. We created a ‘general media use’ measure by adapting the Media and Technology Usage and Attitudes Scale (MTUAS)38. The MTUAS assessed media usage across 11 scales representing smartphone usage, general social media usage, Internet searching, e-mailing, media sharing, text messaging, video gaming, online friendships, Facebook friendships, phone calling, and watching television. An additional four subscales assessed attitudes toward media use: positive attitudes, negative attitudes, technological anxiety/dependence, and attitudes toward task-switching. Based on analysis of extensive pilot data, we reduced the MTUAS to 11 items capturing three media use scales including social media use, video game use and general phone use. These scales showed good reliability (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.8 and above) and test–retest reliability (r = 0.73 and above). We also created a composite of these three scales as a measure of general media use. The composite included 6 items assessed on a 6-point scale (1 = “Never”, 2 = “Several times a year”, 3 = “Several times a month”, 4 = “Several times a week”, 5 = “Several times a day”, 6 = “Several times an hour”). The items included in our reduced MTUAS scale are provided in Supplementary Table S3.
Mental health
Parent and self-report measures of mental health were included in the TEDS emerging adulthood assessment. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ), a 25-item assessment of positive and negative behavioural attributes, was used to assess five broad domains including emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, peer relationship problems, and prosocial behaviour39. The Cronbach’s alphas for each of the subdomains suggest good reliability (emotional symptoms = 0.67, conduct problems = 0.64, hyperactivity = 0.71, peer problems = 0.61, prosocial behaviour = 0.70), with good test–retest reliability (ranging from 0.70 to 80). In addition, the first four domains (excluding prosocial behaviour) were summed to create a total behavioural problems score. We considered the prosocial behaviour subdomain of the SDQ as an indicator of positive mental health, which is in line with previous research indicating prosociality correlates with markers of wellbeing, life satisfaction and happiness.
The Moods and Feelings Questionnaire (MFQ) is a screening tool used to assess DSM-III-R criteria for depression in children and young people40. The MFQ includes a series of descriptive phrases assessing how the individual has been feeling or acting recently, for example: ‘I felt I was no good anymore’; ‘I felt lonely’; ‘I hated myself’. A total score taking the mean of the 11 MFQ items was derived for the purposes of these analyses. The MFQ showed good reliability (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.91) and test–retest reliability (r = 0.75).
Sample sizes for the media and mental health measures are detailed in Supplementary Online Table S1.
Genome-wide polygenic scores
Nine genome-wide polygenic scores (GPS) indexing psychiatric disorders and associated traits (i.e., education), were created in our independent sample of unrelated individuals based on publicly available genome-wide association (GWA) summary statistics from the Psychiatric Genetics Consortium: Schizophrenia41, Bipolar Disorder42, Major Depressive Disorder43, Autism Spectrum Disorder44, Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder45, Obsessive–Compulsive Disorder46, Anorexia Nervosa47, Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder48; and the Social Science Genetic Association Consortium: Years of Education49 (Supplementary Table S4). Construction of the GPS is described in detail in the Supplementary Online Methods.
Statistical analyses
Phenotypic analyses
To examine the overall associations between media use and mental health, we computed correlations between each of our media use and mental health measures. The p-values for the correlations were adjusted for multiple comparisons using the false discovery rate (FDR) method50. Raw values for the phenotypic correlations between all media and mental health measures, separately for girls and boys, are reported in Supplementary Fig. S1a,b. Means and standard deviations for the media use measures are reported for the whole sample, males and females separately, and for all five sex and zygosity groups: monozygotic (MZ) males, dizygotic (DZ) males, MZ females, DZ females, and DZ opposite-sex twin pairs (Table S5). In order to maintain independence of data, one twin per pair was randomly selected, and analyses of variance (ANOVA) was performed to test the significance of these group differences. The ANOVA results indicated that sex and zygosity together account for less than 1% of the variance across measures, therefore to increase power all subsequent analyses were performed on the full sample, combining males and females and including opposite-sex pairs. All variables were age and sex regressed. In the analysis of twin data this age and sex regression is necessary because members of a twin pair are identical in age and MZ twins are also identical for sex, and this would otherwise inflate twin estimates of shared environment51. For the subsequent twin analyses, all measures were corrected for skewness by mapping them on to a standard normal distribution using the rank-based van der Waerden's transformation (vdW)52,53, analyses were compared with and without vDW and results were broadly consistent.
Genetic analyses
Twin analyses
Univariate twin analyses
To assess the relative genetic and environmental contributions to variance in general media use, online victimisation and problematic media use, we used univariate twin analyses. Heritability (A), refers to the genetic contribution to phenotypic variance and is narrowly defined as the proportion of individual differences in a population that can be attributed to additive effects of inherited DNA differences between individuals. Doubling the difference between MZ and DZ correlations on a trait provides a rough estimate of ‘A’. Environmental influences in twin analyses are split into those non-inherited influences that are shared (C) and unique (E) to twins growing up in the same home. C contributes to twin similarity and can be calculated by subtracting ‘A’ from the MZ twin correlations. The E component is calculated by deducting the A and C components from unity. In addition to capturing environmental experiences unique to the individual, E includes measurement error.
The ACE estimates and their confidence intervals can be calculated more precisely using structural equation modelling (SEM) with the OpenMX software package54.
Bivariate twin analyses
To investigate genetic and environmental covariance between online media use and mental health outcomes, we used bivariate twin analyses. Bivariate twin analyses are an extension of univariate twin analyses that rely on cross-twin cross-trait correlations to decompose phenotypic covariance between multiple traits into genetic and environmental components of covariance. We estimated the proportion of the phenotypic correlation between online media use and mental health measures that is explained by genetic (rphA), shared environmental (rphC), and nonshared environmental (rphE) influences. Detailed description of univariate and multivariate twin analyses in the TEDS sample have been published55.
Polygenic score analyses
To investigate the contribution of measured genetic liability to psychopathology and related traits to (1) media use, and (2) the association between media use and mental health, we applied multiple polygenic score analyses, which have been shown to increase predictive power when compared to single polygenic score approaches56. We conducted multiple regression analyses to investigate the proportion of variance in our media use measures that can be accounted for using polygenic scores (GPS) as well as the relative contribution of each of the 9 GPS to trait prediction. Beta coefficients were inspected to investigate the sign (positive or negative) of the effect—whether higher polygenic loading is associated with increased or decreased risk of general media use, online victimisation and problematic media use.
Genetic sensitivity analysis
Current polygenic scores explain much less of the heritable variance in psychiatric traits than is expected from twin studies, in part due to their reliance on additive effects of common (present in more than 1% of the population) Single Nucleotide Polymorphisms (SNPs) and dependence on the sample size of the discovery GWAS for which their effect estimates are derived. As such, polygenic scores are likely to under-estimate genetic confounding. To address this, we employed a novel genetic sensitivity analysis called Gsens. Gsens involves estimating genetic confounding from a Structural Equation Model based on a matrix of observed correlations between polygenic scores, exposure and outcomes. Critically, this correlation matrix can be modified under scenarios in which polygenic scores explain greater heritability in the outcomes (by using estimates of SNP and twin-based heritability). This enables estimation of genetic confounding based on more predictive (latent) polygenic scores. Further methodological details are provided in. In line with recommendations, the sensitivity analysis was restricted to those mental health outcomes for which we had corresponding polygenic scores, namely emotional symptoms (for which we used a polygenic score for anxiety), depression (for which we used the depression polygenic score) and hyperactivity (for which we used the ADHD polygenic score). The SNP and twin heritability estimates used for the Gsens analyses are reported in Supplementary Table S7.
All statistical analyses were conducted in Linux environment using R version 3.5.057.
Results
Phenotypic associations between media use and mental health
Figure 1 shows the phenotypic correlations between general and explicitly negative media use and each of our mental health measures. Negative media use (online victimisation, problematic media use) was associated with mental health problems, including depressive symptoms, behavioural and peer problems, anxiety symptoms, conduct behaviour and hyperactivity. Problematic media use was also associated with less prosocial behaviour. By contrast, general media use was modestly associated with fewer symptoms of mental health problems, with the most robust evidence emerging for fewer peer problems. General media use was associated with more prosocial behaviour. Results were similar for girls and boys (see Fig. S1a,b).
Twin analyses
Genetic influences on media use
Results from our univariate twin analyses are illustrated in Table 1. Heritability estimates were 43% for problematic media use, 26% for online victimisation and 49% for general media use. We did not find evidence for shared environmental influences for any of the three measures of media use.
Table 1.
Media use | A | C | E | Intraclass correlations | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
MZ | DZ | ||||
Problematic media | 0.43 | 0.00 | 0.57 | 0.46 | 0.17 |
(0.40–0.45) | (0.00–0.00) | (0.54–0.60) | (0.43–0.49) | (0.14–0.20) | |
Online victimisation | 0.26 | 0.00 | 0.74 | 0.26 | 0.15 |
(0.23–0.30) | (0.00–0.05) | (0.70–0.77) | (0.23–0.29) | (0.12–0.18) | |
General media | 0.49 | 0.00 | 0.51 | 0.49 | 0.23 |
(0.42–0.51) | (0.00–0.00) | (0.49–0.54) | (0.46–0.51) | (0.21–0.26) |
A = additive genetic factors; C = shared (common) environmental factors that make siblings growing up in the same home more similar; E = unique/nonshared environmental factors that do not correlate between siblings, including measurement error. Intraclass correlations are reported for monozygotic (MZ problematic media n = 2890, online victimisation n = 2888 and general media n = 2886) and dizygotic (DZ problematic media n = 4879, online victimisation n = 4878 and general media n = 4858) twin pairs, including DZ opposite sex pairs. The 95% confidence intervals are presented under each estimate in brackets.
Genetic influences on the associations between media use and mental health
We computed bivariate twin analyses to estimate the proportion of the correlations between online media use and mental health that was driven by genetic and environmental factors (Figs. 2, 3, 4).
For those media use measures that demonstrated robust (r ≤ 0.20) phenotypic associations with mental health measures, a large part of their association was accounted for by additive genetic factors (ranging from 38% [online victimisation and conduct problems] to 88% [general media use and prosocial behaviour]). Shared environmental factors did not contribute to the association between media use and mental health for problematic media use, online victimisation or general media use (Figs. 2, 3, 4). The bivariate twin estimates and confidence intervals are detailed in Supplementary Table S6.
Genome-wide polygenic score results
Genome-wide polygenic score (GPS) analyses also indicated genetic influence on both general and explicitly negative media use, although effect sizes were small, explaining less than 1% of the variance. For problematic media use, of the 9 tested GPS, higher schizophrenia GPS (r = 0.057, p < .001), higher ASD GPS (r = 0.038, p = .01) and lower education GPS (r = − 0.055, p < .01) associations survived multiple testing correction. Online victimisation was associated with four GPS, including higher MDD GPS (r = 0.036, p = .01), higher PTSD GPS (r = 0.049, p < .001), higher ADHD GPS (r = 0.065, p < .001) and lower education GPS (r = − 0.084, p < .001). Finally, for general media use, three significant GPS associations emerged including lower schizophrenia GPS (r = − 0.043, p < .001), lower ASD GPS (r = − 0.040, p < .001), and higher education GPS (r = 0.047, p < .001). Together, the 9 GPS yielded significant (p < .001) multiple correlations with problematic media use (R = 0.079), online victimisation (R = 0.099) and general media use (R = 0.066). Correlations between each of the 9 GPS and the media use measures are presented in Supplementary Fig. S2.
Sensitivity analyses results
Results from our Gsens analysis using polygenic scores to test for genetic confounding on the association between media use and mental health are provided in Figs. S3–S18 of the Supplementary Online Material. For consistency with our twin analyses, Gsens was performed on media use and mental health measures where phenotypic correlations were ≥ 0.20).
Effect sizes of our media use and mental health associations were reduced by 0.6–1.8% using the observed depression, anxiety and ADHD polygenic scores, indicating little genetic confounding (Figs. S3, S6, S9, S12, S15).
We then tested for genetic confounding under a SNP- and twin- based scenario where polygenic scores explain the upper bound SNP- and twin- based heritability of mental health measures, respectively (Figs. S4, S7, S10, S13, S16). The results show that under a SNP heritability scenario, the association between problematic media use and mental health is attenuated by 23–100%. A similar pattern of results was observed for online victimization where under the SNP based scenario effect sizes for the association with mental health were decreased by 41–81%.
For both problematic media use and online victimization, under a twin-based scenario where polygenic scores explain twin heritability, the media use and mental health association was completely confounded by genetics (Figs. S5, S8, S11, S14, S17).
For simplicity, we only depict Gsens results for the association between online victimisation and depression below (Fig. 5) and refer the reader to supplementary for the remainder of the Gsens results, including full model output (Supplementary Fig. S18).
Discussion
Extant genetic research on the impact of online media use on mental health has predominantly focused on the deleterious effects of excessive media use. In a sample of nearly 8000 emerging adults, we demonstrate that general media use correlates phenotypically with fewer peer problems (r = − 0.15) and more prosocial behaviour (r = 0.19), a measure of positive mental health. Capitalising on approximately 4000 twin pairs we have demonstrated that genetic influences (1) affect both the general and explicitly negative sides of online media use, and (2) partly underlie the associations between online media use and mental health. Indeed, bivariate twin analyses on all robust phenotypic correlations indicate that 38 to 62% of the correlation between negative media use and negative mental health and 80 to 88% of the correlation between general media use and positive mental health is driven by genetic influences. Genome-wide polygenic score (GPS) analyses also indicate genetic confounding in the association between media use and mental health, explaining up to 100% of the association with increasing polygenic score accuracy.
Finding genetic influences on associations between mental health and online media use in emerging adulthood confirms and extends previous analyses using the same sample where we demonstrated genetic influence on home computer use when the twins were aged 1619. Importantly, here we found strong evidence that the associations between media use and mental health problems are specific to negative uses of the media including experiences of online victimisation and problematic internet use. These results represent an important first step in better characterising media use and mental health associations and highlight the need for further genetically informed analyses. Inclusion of longitudinal designs and causal inference models such as Mendelian Randomization will be essential to better understanding the temporal causality of these associations while more adequately accounting for confounding59. For example, our finding of genetic contributions to media use and mental health associations does not preclude reciprocal causal influences between media use and mental health.
Genetic differences contribute to how young people create their online environments and support the widely demonstrated finding that many so-called “environments” are themselves under substantial genetic influence60,61. Shared environmental influences, which include those factors that make siblings growing up in the same home more similar, were zero for the media use measures, which may in part reflect the fact that these young people have now left their family home and therefore parental monitoring on media use is less prominent. Polygenic score analyses indicated that young people with genetic liability to underlying mental health problems may be particularly vulnerable to using online media use in a negative way, which may in turn further impact their wellbeing at a time when they have less familial support. Finding no shared environmental effects in young adulthood is consistent with decades of behavioural genetic research which demonstrates that genetics, and not the shared environment, contributes to similarities between family members post-childhood. This interpretation can explain why for example, parents and their children can show high correlations in their media use, because parents and their offspring share on average 50% of their genetic material.
The absence of shared environmental influence also reflects the consistent behavioural genetic finding that when individuals are increasingly free to select their environments, we see greater genetic influences while shared environmental influences decrease developmentally11. Because young adults have greater choice and autonomy over their media use compared to teenagers, genetic influences manifest more strongly while the influence of the shared family environment decreases. We observe this trend when we compare the current results to our previous analyses on genetic influence on media use on the home computer when the twins were aged 16. Shared environmental influences were significant (8–20%) for home computer use in their teens compared to now when they are young adults freer to create, modify and interpret their online experiences.
Co-occurring rises in online media use and self-reported mental health problems in early adulthood have garnered considerable research attention27. The complexity of these relationships within the rapidly evolving media landscape requires novel approaches if we are to better understand individual differences and their impact. Greater nuance is needed within these investigations, including a more balanced view of the potential positive impacts of media use, which we sought to incorporate in the present study by including a general media use measure. More interdisciplinary approaches are needed to better understand the nature of media use—and as we demonstrate here, with sensitivity to genetic contributions.
Importantly these results indicate the cooccurrence of media use and mental health problems can be largely accounted for by common genetic vulnerabilities. This suggests that the inherited characteristics that make individuals more vulnerable to mental health problems are also increasing their vulnerability to online victimisation and for example, loss of sleep due to night-time internet use.
This was suggested not only by the bivariate twin results, but also by the genetic sensitivity analysis, which estimated genetic confounding under scenarios in which polygenic scores captured increasing heritability in the outcomes. Under a scenario in which polygenic scores captured SNP heritability in mental health outcomes, the associations between media use and mental health were attenuated by at least 20%. However, when accounting for greater genetic variance using twin heritability estimates, the associations between media use and mental health were attenuated to null. Future studies could employ these methods to explore, for example, how other mental health symptoms relate to media use, even with the current limited predictive power of psychiatric polygenic scores.
Although media use is ubiquitous in early adulthood, experiences and uses differ and are not always negative. For example, there is a growing body of literature showing that for marginalized communities, social media may in some circumstances serve as a platform to share resources and curate self-affirming content. These aspects of media use are understudied in general, and especially in genetically informed designs where most samples are obtained from white European samples. This is a key limitation in the present study as the TEDS twin (93%) and genotyped sample (99%) is predominately white. Until studies better represent global diversity, our understanding of media use will be inadequate27 and work will continue to perpetuate disparities in health research with knowledge benefits largely withheld from racialised communities62,63.
Our results must be understood within the context of the known limitations of the twin and polygenic score designs64. Discrepancy between twin and polygenic score-based estimates of genetic influence are expected given unlike twin studies which capture all forms of genetic variation including gene–gene and gene-environment interactions, polygenic scores are limited to the additive effects of common SNPs. Our genome-wide polygenic scores are not subject to the same assumptions as the twin method. However, our GPS analyses are limited to the power of current genome-wide association studies (GWAS), the size of their discovery samples and the biological validity of current diagnostic systems65. Triangulation of our results using these complementary genetic methods with different assumptions provides further evidence for the need for genetically informed designs to better understand the reciprocal causal relationships between those phenotypes.
As TEDS is a population representative sample, severe experiences of behavioural and mental health problems may be less represented, although behavioural genetic evidence indicates that common disorders represent the extreme end of a behavioural continuum and therefore our population sample can still provide useful insights to the aetiology of these associations66.
Our findings should also be considered in the context of the measures used. All the media use and mental health measures were self-reported, and therefore rater bias may have influenced our results67. Indeed, previous studies have found that the associations between media use and mental health are stronger for self-reported outcomes compared to outcomes reported by other informants26,68,69. This is a concern with all self-report measures and efforts are needed to include multiple sources of information from different raters (self, parent, teacher) and in ancestrally diverse samples. In addition, for the purposes of the present study and to capture variability in mental health, we considered prosocial behaviour as a marker of positive mental health. While there is a literature supporting this interpretation, we acknowledge that the relationship is likely more complex, with some evidence for a ‘goldilocks’ effect’ whereby too much or too little prosocial behaviour is associated with poor mental health.
We highlight and acknowledge the shortcomings in our measure of general media use, which was derived from the residuals of a regression of the general media use composite on the available problematic media use and online victimisation scales. Our general media use measure correlated highly with the composite derived from the validated Media and Technology Usage and Attitudes Scale phenotypically and genetically. We chose this approach to steer future research toward addressing the clear gap in diverse media experiences beyond explicitly negative use, however we strongly encourage new development of questionnaires designed to interrogate ways in which young people find media use positive. Large, genotyped cohort samples of representative young adults have great potential for investigating the role of positive and general media use in mental health. Although we were limited in the media use measures that are currently available in TEDS, we hope that this research can spur discussion in other cohorts to collect broader measures70,71. Critically, scales are needed that go beyond measuring amount of time spent on social media devices to query the psychological processes, motivations, and socialising goals that help to sustain these behaviours. Distinctions between active, passive, social and non-social media use are an important nuance in how we define future scales as well as the need to integrate knowledge from focus groups and in-depth discussion with young people about their ever-changing experiences online . Despite this limitation, our results indicating an association between general media use and positive mental health outcomes is consistent with a recent study where routine social media use as measured by an adapted general media use scale was associated with social well-being, positive mental health, and self-rated health.
Recent valid criticisms that many of the scales used to assess problematic media use have been raised. These include concerns that (1) in lumping diverse measures of media use together researchers may be grouping constructs that originate from very heterogenous personal characteristics with distinct etiological mechanisms (2) we are pathologizing normal behavior (3) in the eagerness to measure online behavior less emphasis has been placed on validating the psychometric properties of the scales used. These limitations can be uniquely addressed using genetics. Diverse measures of media use can be tested for their underlying genetic structure and in doing so can isolate constructs of similar etiologies. Twin and DNA based methods can be used to compare the heritability of diverse measures of media use with the expectation that those scales more reliably tapping into true variation in online behavior also display the highest contribution by additive genetic variation. Combining longitudinal approaches with genetics can help provide nuance to item selection as variation in the problematic aspects of media use is likely more complex than the simplistic ‘more is worse’ approach currently adopted. Despite the replication crisis in science, it is noteworthy that genetic and genomic results replicate reliably. For this reason, despite the limitations of current scales, it is not the time to throw the baby out with the bathwater, but instead to capitalize on genomics as a tool to inform our understanding of these complex online environments and to construct better, more reliable tools for advancing knowledge of this ubiquitous aspect of young adulthood.
In our increasingly digital word, young adults are disproportionately using digital devices and increasingly reporting mental health problems. Our findings show that observed associations between media use and mental health are largely accounted for by genetic influences. As such, future research efforts to understand the mental health impact of media use should adopt genetically informed designs to strengthen causal inference.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
We gratefully acknowledge the ongoing contribution of the participants in the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS) and their families. TEDS is supported by a program Grant to RP from the UK Medical Research Council (MR/M021475/1 and previously G0901245), with additional support from the US National Institutes of Health (AG046938). The research leading to these results has also received funding from the European Research Council under the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007-2013)/Grant Agreement 602768 and ERC Grant Agreement 295366. ZA is supported by a Marie Skłodowska-Curie Fellowship from the European Union (894675). JRB is supported by a Sir Henry Wellcome Postdoctoral Fellowship (Grant 215917/Z/19/Z). KR is supported by a Sir Henry Wellcome Postdoctoral Fellowship (Grant 213514/Z/18/Z). This research was funded in whole, or in part by the Wellcome Trust. For the purpose of open access, the author has applied a CC BY public copyright licence to any Author Accepted Manuscript version arising from this submission.
Author contributions
All authors contributed to the study concept and design, interpretation of the data and critically reviewed the paper. Z.A. analysed the data and wrote the paper.
Data availability
Please refer to the Twins Early Development Study data access policy available here: https://www.teds.ac.uk/researchers/teds-data-access-policy.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
The original online version of this Article was revised: The original version of this Article contained an error in Reference 68, which was incorrectly given as: ‘Baldwin, J., Rijsdijk, F., Schoeler, T., Arsenault, L., Ayorech, Z. & Pingault, J. Cyber-victimisation and mental health in young people: A co-twin control study. PNAS’. The correct reference is: ‘Baldwin, J. R., Ayorech, Z., Rijsdijk, F. V., Schoeler, T., & Pingault, J. B. Cyber-victimisation and mental health in young people: a co-twin control study. Psychol. Med., 51(15), 2620-2630, Doi:10.1017/S0033291720001178 (2021).’
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Change history
2/21/2023
A Correction to this paper has been published: 10.1038/s41598-023-30112-1
Contributor Information
Ziada Ayorech, Email: ziada.ayorech@psykologi.uio.no.
Robert Plomin, Email: robert.plomin@kcl.ac.uk.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-022-25374-0.
References
- 1.Dietz, W. H. The obesity epidemic in young children: reduce television viewing and promote playing. (2001). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 2.Lin L-Y, Cherng R-J, Chen Y-J, Chen Y-J, Yang H-M. Effects of television exposure on developmental skills among young children. Infant Behav. Dev. 2015;38:20–26. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2014.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Van den Bulck J. Television viewing, computer game playing, and Internet use and self-reported time to bed and time out of bed in secondary-school children. Sleep. 2004;27:101–104. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.1.101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Hancox RJ, Milne BJ, Poulton R. Association of television viewing during childhood with poor educational achievement. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2005;159:614–618. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.159.7.614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Coyne SM. Effects of viewing relational aggression on television on aggressive behavior in adolescents: A three-year longitudinal study. Dev. Psychol. 2016;52:284. doi: 10.1037/dev0000068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Pagani LS, Fitzpatrick C, Barnett TA, Dubow E. Prospective associations between early childhood television exposure and academic, psychosocial, and physical well-being by middle childhood. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2010;164:425–431. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2010.50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Johnson JG, Cohen P, Kasen S, Brook JS. Extensive television viewing and the development of attention and learning difficulties during adolescence. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2007;161:480–486. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.161.5.480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Landhuis CE, Poulton R, Welch D, Hancox RJ. Does childhood television viewing lead to attention problems in adolescence? Results from a prospective longitudinal study. Pediatrics. 2007;120:532–537. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-0978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Hancox RJ, Milne BJ, Poulton R. Association between child and adolescent television viewing and adult health: A longitudinal birth cohort study. Lancet. 2004;364:257–262. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(04)16675-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Plomin R, Corley R, DeFries JC, Fulker DW. Individual differences in television viewing in early childhood: Nature as well as nurture. Psychol. Sci. 1990;1:371–377. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00244.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Knopik V, Neiderheiser J, DeFries J, Plomin R. Behavioral Genetics. Worth; 2017. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Thomée S, Härenstam A, Hagberg M. Mobile phone use and stress, sleep disturbances, and symptoms of depression among young adults—A prospective cohort study. BMC Public Health. 2011;11:66. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-11-66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Montag C, Wegmann E, Sariyska R, Demetrovics Z, Brand M. How to overcome taxonomical problems in the study of Internet use disorders and what to do with “smartphone addiction”? J. Behav. Addict. 2021;9:908–914. doi: 10.1556/2006.8.2019.59. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Arnett JJ. Emerging adulthood: Understanding the new way of coming of age. Emerg. Adults Am. Coming Age 21st Century. 2006;22:3–19. doi: 10.1037/11381-001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Coyne SM, Padilla-Walker LM, Howard E. Emerging in a digital world a decade review of media use, effects, and gratifications in emerging adulthood. Emerg. Adulthood. 2013;1:125–137. doi: 10.1177/2167696813479782. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Ofcom. Do you personally use a smartphone?*—By age. Available at: https://www.statista.com/statistics/300402/smartphone-usage-in-the-uk-by-age/. Accessed 14 May 2019.
- 17.Palfrey, J. G. & Gasser, U. Born Digital: Understanding the First Generation of Digital Natives. (ReadHowYouWant. com, 2011).
- 18.Polderman TJ, et al. Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. Nat. Genet. 2015 doi: 10.1038/ng.3285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Ayorech Z, von Stumm S, Haworth CM, Davis OS, Plomin R. Personalized media: A genetically informative investigation of individual differences in online media use. PLoS ONE. 2017;12:e0168895. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0168895. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Rimfeld K, et al. Twins early development study: A genetically sensitive investigation into behavioral and cognitive development from infancy to emerging adulthood. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 2019;22:508–513. doi: 10.1017/thg.2019.56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Internet Access Quarterly Update: Q4 2013. Office for National Statistics (2014). Available at: https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/householdcharacteristics/homeinternetandsocialmediausage/bulletins/internetaccessquarterlyupdate/2014-02-19. Accessed 19 May 2014.
- 22.McManus, S., Bebbington, P., Jenkins, R. & Brugha, T. Mental health and wellbeing in England: Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey 2014. A survey carried out for NHS Digital by NatCen Social Research and the Department of Health Sciences, University of Leicester. (2016).
- 23.Pitchforth J, et al. Mental health and well-being trends among children and young people in the UK, 1995–2014: Analysis of repeated cross-sectional national health surveys. Psychol. Med. 2019;49:1275–1285. doi: 10.1017/S0033291718001757. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Andreasson, U. & Birkjær, M. In the Shadow of Happiness. (Nordic Council of Ministers, 2018).
- 25.Orben A, Dienlin T, Przybylski AK. Social media’s enduring effect on adolescent life satisfaction. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2019;116:10226–10228. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1902058116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Orben A, Przybylski AK. The association between adolescent well-being and digital technology use. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2019;3:173. doi: 10.1038/s41562-018-0506-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Prinstein MJ, Nesi J, Telzer EH. Commentary: An updated agenda for the study of digital media use and adolescent development–future directions following Odgers & Jensen (2020) J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2020;61:349–352. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Orben A. Teenagers, screens and social media: A narrative review of reviews and key studies. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2020;55:407–414. doi: 10.1007/s00127-019-01825-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Deryakulu D, Ursavaş ÖF. Genetic and environmental influences on problematic Internet use: A twin study. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2014;39:331–338. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.07.038. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Li M, Chen J, Li N, Li X. A twin study of problematic internet use: Its heritability and genetic association with effortful control. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 2014;17:279–287. doi: 10.1017/thg.2014.32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Long EC, et al. The genetic and environmental contributions to internet use and associations with psychopathology: A twin study. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 2015;19:1–9. doi: 10.1017/thg.2015.91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Hahn E, Reuter M, Spinath FM, Montag C. Internet addiction and its facets: The role of genetics and the relation to self-directedness. Addict. Behav. 2017;65:137–146. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.10.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Vink JM, van Beijsterveldt TCEM, Huppertz C, Bartels M, Boomsma DI. Heritability of compulsive Internet use in adolescents. Addict. Biol. 2015 doi: 10.1111/adb.12218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Montag, C. & Reuter, M. Molecular genetics, personality, and internet addiction revisited. In Internet Addiction 141–160 (Springer, 2017).
- 35.Jelenchick LA, et al. The Problematic and Risky Internet Use Screening Scale (PRIUSS) for adolescents and young adults: Scale development and refinement. Comput. Human Behav. 2014;35:171–178. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.01.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Betts LR, Houston JE, Steer OL. Development of the multidimensional peer victimization scale–revised (MPVS-R) and the multidimensional peer bullying scale (MPVS-RB) J. Genet. Psychol. 2015;176:93–109. doi: 10.1080/00221325.2015.1007915. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Yau JC, Reich SM. Are the qualities of adolescents’ offline friendships present in digital interactions? Adolesc. Res. Rev. 2018;3:339–355. doi: 10.1007/s40894-017-0059-y. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Rosen LD, Whaling K, Carrier LM, Cheever NA, Rokkum J. The media and technology usage and attitudes scale: An empirical investigation. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2013;29:2501–2511. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.06.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Goodman R. The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 1997;38:581–586. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Sharp C, Goodyer IM, Croudace TJ. The Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (SMFQ): A unidimensional item response theory and categorical data factor analysis of self-report ratings from a community sample of 7-through 11-year-old children. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2006;34:365–377. doi: 10.1007/s10802-006-9027-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Pardiñas AF, et al. Common schizophrenia alleles are enriched in mutation-intolerant genes and in regions under strong background selection. Nat. Genet. 2018;50:381–389. doi: 10.1038/s41588-018-0059-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Sklar P, et al. Large-scale genome-wide association analysis of bipolar disorder identifies a new susceptibility locus near ODZ4. Nat. Genet. 2011;43:977. doi: 10.1038/ng.943. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Wray NR, et al. Genome-wide association analyses identify 44 risk variants and refine the genetic architecture of major depression. Nat. Genet. 2018;50:668. doi: 10.1038/s41588-018-0090-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Grove J, et al. Identification of common genetic risk variants for autism spectrum disorder. Nat. Genet. 2019;51:431–444. doi: 10.1038/s41588-019-0344-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Demontis D, et al. Discovery of the first genome-wide significant risk loci for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nat. Genet. 2019;51:63–75. doi: 10.1038/s41588-018-0269-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Arnold PD, et al. Revealing the complex genetic architecture of obsessive-compulsive disorder using meta-analysis. Mol. Psychiatry. 2018;23:1181. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Duncan L, et al. Significant locus and metabolic genetic correlations revealed in genome-wide association study of anorexia nervosa. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2017;174:850–858. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.16121402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Duncan LE, et al. Largest GWAS of PTSD (N = 20 070) yields genetic overlap with schizophrenia and sex differences in heritability. Mol. Psychiatry. 2018;23:666–673. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Lee JJ, et al. Gene discovery and polygenic prediction from a genome-wide association study of educational attainment in 1.1 million individuals. Nat. Genet. 2018;50:1112–1121. doi: 10.1038/s41588-018-0147-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B. 1995;57:289–300. [Google Scholar]
- 51.McGue M, Bouchard TJ., Jr Adjustment of twin data for the effects of age and sex. Behav. Genet. 1984;14:325–343. doi: 10.1007/BF01080045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Lehmann, Erich Leo, and H. J. D. Nonparametrics: Statistical Methods Based on Ranks. (Holden-Day, 1975).
- 53.Van der Waerden BL. On the sources of my book Moderne Algebra. Hist. Math. 1975;2:31–40. doi: 10.1016/0315-0860(75)90034-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Boker S, et al. OpenMx: An open source extended structural equation modeling framework. Psychometrika. 2011;76:306–317. doi: 10.1007/s11336-010-9200-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Smith-Woolley E, Ayorech Z, Dale PS, von Stumm S, Plomin R. The genetics of university success. Sci. Rep. 2018;8:14579. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-32621-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Schoeler T, et al. Multi-polygenic score approach to identifying individual vulnerabilities associated with the risk of exposure to bullying. JAMA Psychiat. 2019;76:730–738. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.0310. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Team, R. C. R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Internet]. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2014. (2015).
- 58.Angold A, Costello EJ. Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (MFQ) Developmental Epidemiology Program, Duke University; 1987. [Google Scholar]
- 59.Pingault J-B, et al. Genetic sensitivity analysis: Adjusting for genetic confounding in epidemiological associations. PLoS Genet. 2021;17:e1009590. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1009590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Plomin R, Bergeman CS. The nature of nurture: Genetic influence on “environmental” measures. Behav. Brain Sci. 1991;14:373–386. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00070278. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Plomin R, Reiss D, Hetherington EM, Howe GW. Nature and nurture: Genetic contributions to measures of the family environment. Dev. Psychol. 1994;30:32. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.30.1.32. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Sirugo G, Williams SM, Tishkoff SA. The missing diversity in human genetic studies. Cell. 2019;177:26–31. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2019.02.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Peterson RE, et al. Genome-wide association studies in ancestrally diverse populations: Opportunities, methods, pitfalls, and recommendations. Cell. 2019;179:589–603. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2019.08.051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Rijsdijk FV, Sham PC. Analytic approaches to twin data using structural equation models. Brief. Bioinform. 2002;3:119–133. doi: 10.1093/bib/3.2.119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Wray NR, et al. Research review: Polygenic methods and their application to psychiatric traits. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2014;55:1068–1087. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Plomin R, Haworth CM, Davis OS. Common disorders are quantitative traits. Nat. Rev. Genet. 2009;10:872–878. doi: 10.1038/nrg2670. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Podsakoff PM, MacKenzie SB, Lee J-Y, Podsakoff NP. Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 2003;88:879. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Baldwin JR, Ayorech Z, Rijsdijk FV, Schoeler T, Pingault JB. Cyber-victimisation and mental health in young people: a co-twin control study. Psychol. Med., 2021;51(15):2620–2630. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720001178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Schoeler T, Duncan L, Cecil CM, Ploubidis GB, Pingault J-B. Quasi-experimental evidence on short-and long-term consequences of bullying victimization: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2018;144:1229. doi: 10.1037/bul0000171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Billieux J, Schimmenti A, Khazaal Y, Maurage P, Heeren A. Are we overpathologizing everyday life? A tenable blueprint for behavioral addiction research. J. Behav. Addict. 2015;4:119–123. doi: 10.1556/2006.4.2015.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Montag C, et al. Is it meaningful to distinguish between generalized and specific Internet addiction? Evidence from a cross-cultural study from Germany, Sweden, Taiwan and China. Asia Pac. Psychiatry. 2015;7:20–26. doi: 10.1111/appy.12122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Please refer to the Twins Early Development Study data access policy available here: https://www.teds.ac.uk/researchers/teds-data-access-policy.