Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2023 Jan 23;18(1):e0280781. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0280781

Making sense of the pandemic: Parent-child conversations in two cultural contexts

Pirko Tõugu 1,*, Tiia Tulviste 1, Lisa Schröder 2
Editor: Sergio A Useche3
PMCID: PMC9870112  PMID: 36689431

Abstract

The present study focused on parent-child conversations about COVID-19 related changes in children’s lives in Estonia and Germany with an aim to understand how children’s conceptual understanding of the disease and their emotional security is created and reflected in these interactions. Twenty-nine parent-child dyads from both cultural contexts provided self-recorded conversations. The conversations were analyzed for the type of explanations, emotional content, and valence. Estonian conversations were longer than those of German dyads. Explanatory talk appeared in both contexts but was general in nature. Conversations in both cultural contexts also included very few emotional references and tended to focus on both positive and negative aspects of the situation. The conversations show that parents tend to support children’s coping with stressful situations by helping them conceptually understand COVID-19 and paying little attention to children’s comprehension of feelings about the situation.

Introduction

Children accumulate knowledge and create an understanding about the world in everyday experiences that often include conversations with more knowledgeable persons [1]. Conversations with parents that include active involvement and explanations support knowledge building [24]. In addition, parent-child conversations are an important tool for socio-emotional development [see 5] and carry implications for children’s mental health [6]. Therefore, it is important to know how parents talk to their children about the COVID-19 pandemic and the related changes, how they help them create an understanding of the events and pave the way for better coping.

Cognitive and socio-emotional development support in parent-child conversations

Parent-child conversations serve several functions in child development. According to sociocultural theory [1], such conversations are an important source of socialization and a tool for knowledge building. Parents scaffold children’s budding knowledge and understanding in everyday conversations and socialize children towards culturally appropriate expression and management of emotions [e.g., 7, 8].

Young children’s everyday conversations with their parents are a mechanism for remembering [9]. At the same time, these conversations are rich in explanations that help children make sense of real-life events [1012]. Everyday explanations and parental guidance in exploring complicated things lead to improved knowledge on the matter [2]. Children also actively seek explanations from their parents [13]. Preschool children are more satisfied with and show better memory for explanations as compared to non-explanation responses to their questions and prefer explanations of higher quality over low-quality or scant explanations [14, 15].

Studies of children’s concepts show that kindergarten children’s understanding of natural phenomena (e.g., plant growth, getting a cold etc.) often reflects both naturalistic or scientific understanding and non-scientific (often animistic or antropomorphic) or intuitive theories [e.g., 1618]. Intuitive theories about getting sick (e.g., one gets sick due to cold weather) may co-exist alongside scientific theories (e.g., illness is caused by contact with germs) even in adults’ understanding and could be more prominent in some cultural contexts than in others [18]. Hernandez et al. [18] also showed that these cultural differences are apparent in parent-child conversations.

Emotion-related conversations are especially important for children’s socio-emotional development [e.g., 19, 20] and mental health [6]. Talking about negative emotions that children have experienced could nurture coping and understanding of the causes of emotions, as these conversations usually involve sophisticated discussions of emotions [21, 22]. Such conversations could carry most developmental potential during preschool age [23].

In stressful times, family conversations often hold the key for children’s coping. A study focusing on parental explanatory style after the 9/11 attacks has indicated that most parents report providing some fact-based explanations of the event to their pre-teen and teenage children, while others talk about emotions or provide assurance [24]. Regarding explanatory styles, Wilson et al. [24] showed that during stressful times the use of self-focused explanations (e.g., talk about parent’s own negative affect or distress) is aversely related to the psychological outcomes of children. Difficulties in family communication (e.g., rumination, discouragement to express one’s feelings) have been proposed to be related to negative outcomes for children (e.g., stress, trauma symptoms) during the COVID-19 pandemic [25, 26]. At the same time, ample research indicates that interpretative conversations about negative events often involve discussion and explanation of negative emotions [e.g., 27, 28]. Such discussions have positive implications for children’s mental health outcomes [6].

Children’s experience with and understanding of COVID-19

The COVID-19 pandemic has changed the every-day routines and activities for most families around the world. Besides the threat of the illness itself, families are faced with difficulties to cope with changes, restrictions, and insecurities about the future. The impact on families and children has been documented and changes in daily routines [2933] and mental health [34] reported. Socio-cultural factors, including positive family environment, has been reported to help children and teenagers cope with pandemic related changes [35]. To improve coping, UNICEF has issued guidelines about how to talk to children: these highlight engagement in communication and providing explanations in order to relieve distress in difficult circumstances (UNICEF, 2019). Psychologists and researchers have also called attention to the importance of the opportunities for everyday conversations and parental engagement in children’s meaning-making. They recommend parents to (a) provide children with adequate and relevant information, (b) listen for (and curtail sometimes harmful) magical thinking, and (c) participate in emotion-focused conversations [3638].

There are several studies that have tried to capture children’s view and experience of the pandemic. Idoiaga et al. [39] focused on children’s conceptualization of the Coronavirus disease. The authors asked 3-12-year-olds what they associate with the Coronavirus disease and the results indicate that kindergarten children mainly see it as an enemy or a “bad bug” that the doctors are fighting [39]. The study also identified several conflicting feelings that children may experience while being forced to stay at home: safety and happiness on the one hand and boredom, fear, anger, and even loneliness, on the other [39]. Tambling et al. [40] have focused on the topics of pandemic related conversations between parents and children as reported by parents. They found that most often parents reported talking to their children about personal and social hygiene; and about 10% of parents mention children’s emotions when probed about children’s stress management [40].

Several studies have tried to capture the understanding fostered by parents via collecting parental reports of the questions children pose and responses parents provide [4143]. These studies show that according to parents, younger children ask more about the changes in everyday life, while older children also want to know about the consequences of the disease [4143]. About one third of the questions are explanation seeking questions in the US context [42], but only one fifth can be classified as such in the Turkish context [43], suggesting that there could be cultural differences in the questions children ask or parents attend to. Interestingly, Turkish parents provide explanations to their children rather than leaving the questions unattended [43], while in about half of the cases parents in the US do not provide explanations [42]. The responses children receive are realistic but mostly very general [41, 42]. If parents provide more specific responses, then parents of younger children will reference germs in their responses, while parents of older children will be more likely to use illness analogies in their explanations [41]. The responses to children’s questions about the reasons for restrictions during the pandemic most often center on the safety and mitigation measures [4143] and sometimes also authority related explanations. Also, parents often report wanting to shield their children from COVID-19 related information [42, 43].

Prior studies focusing on children’s experience and understanding of the pandemic have used parents as informants [4042]. Parental reports are valuable, but they can only provide an approximation of the actual understanding and experience of the child. To our knowledge there is no comparative observational study investigating parent-child conversations about COVID-19, and no documentation of what the focus of such talk is, and how children’s understanding is cultivated by their parents in different cultural contexts. In addition, radical changes and disruptions in everyday routines that have taken place due to the pandemic may give rise to stress and a variety of negative emotions (fear, anxiety, etc.) in children. Parent-child discussion of negative events have been shown to include emotional content. It would be informative to see if parent-child conversations about the pandemic in different cultural contexts address emotions and whether the conversations reflect the recommendations given by experts.

Present study

The present study focuses on parent-child conversations about the pandemic-related changes in children’s life in Estonia and Germany. Despite the importance of parent-child conversations, we do not know how parents in different countries talk to their children about stressful situations, in this case the COVID-19 pandemic. Such information would provide an understanding of the universal characteristics regarding such conversations and the adaptive idiosyncrasies related to cultural styles of talking to children. In the present study, we focus on the explanations regarding the pandemic-related changes in parent-child conversations to understand how parents and children create a conceptual understanding of the pandemic. We also look for the emotional content in the talk with the hope to describe and compare how parents support children’s coping and socio-emotional development.

The study focuses on two different Western cultural contexts, Estonia and Germany. According to the theoretical perspectives of different developmental pathways based on the relative importance of two basic human needs–autonomy and relatedness–in socialization of children, Germany represents a prototypical Western autonomy-oriented context [44]. In autonomy-oriented contexts socialization of children focuses on their psychological experience, while in relatedness-oriented contexts children are coaxed to see themselves as part of a social system [44, 45]. Parenting in autonomy-oriented cultural context sensitizes children to their subjective mental states such as their wishes, preferences, thoughts, emotions, etc. In this context, children are expected to verbalize and express positive emotions. In relatedness-oriented contexts, children are sensitized to social obligations [46]. Studies show that Estonian mothers promote autonomy in socialization of children as much as German mothers. At the same time, they differ from German mothers by placing more value on relatedness [47, 48], thus representing a psychological relatedness context posed by Kağitçibaşi [45] or autonomy-oriented context where relatedness is more highly valued than in many other typical Western contexts.

Studies of parent-child conversations in these contexts highlight the cultural differences consistent with the cultural models: a pragmatic approach by Estonian parents is revealed in shorter conversations compared to prototypical autonomy-oriented European families, like German or Swedish families, during reminiscing [47, 49] and during mealtime [50]. At the same time, Estonian parents value education highly [51] and seem to be factually oriented in conversations as they ask more information-seeking questions than parents from other European countries [47, 50].

Both Estonian and German mothers and their 4-year-old children [49] and Estonian and Swedish mothers and teens [52] talk little about mental states (incl. emotions). Prior studies in Germany show that mothers refer to emotions when talking about a negative event with their children and provide emotion explanations [53]. However, emotion talk is still more infrequent than in an autonomous related context from Costa Rica.

Hypothesis

We expect parent-child conversations in both cultures to be rich in explanations and provide some emotion talk. In terms of explanations, general explanations are expected to dominate, but we anticipate both scientific and magical or animistic explanations to appear. Due to the pragmatic approach to conversation, we expect Estonian parent-child conversations to focus more on the knowledge about the disease, description of the changes in children’s life, and the explanations for the changes than the German conversations. Compared to Estonian dyads, German dyads are expected to talk more about emotions regarding the pandemic and provide more evaluations of the situation. In addition, we pose a research question regarding the possible negative stress-related emotions that could appear in these conversations: what is the valence of the conversations; do parents and children focus more on the positive or negative aspects of the situation.

Method

Sample

Twenty-nine parents from Estonia and 29 from Germany provided audio-recorded conversations with their children during the first or second wave of the pandemic in 2020 and 2021. All German conversations were collected during the first wave; 20 of the Estonian conversations were collected during the first and 9 during the second wave of the pandemic. Conversations collected during the first and second wave in Estonia were compared using a Poisson regression with a log link analysis and the results indicate that the wave predicted the length of the conversations (β = .37, χ2(1) = 37.11 p < .001); the conversations during the first wave were longer (M = 56.85 clauses) than conversations during the second wave (M = 39.22).

The demographic information about the participants is provided in Table 1. There are no statistically significant differences in the gender distribution or age of the child between the samples. The age of parents did not differ in the two contexts. A Chi-square analysis indicated that there was a significant relationship between cultural context and parental education (χ2 (2; 58) = 8.90, p < .05); German mothers were more likely to have a Master’s degree or above, while more Estonian mothers had a Bachelor’s degree. The situation with day-care varied throughout the data collection period: in Germany kindergartens were closed during the lock-down, except for children of front-line workers; the period of lock-down differed according to the geographical region. In Estonia, kindergartens were not officially closed, but children were strongly advised to stay at home during the pandemic. Attending kindergarten was related to the length of the conversation: Poisson regression with a log link analysis indicated that conversations with children attending the kindergarten were shorter that conversation with children staying at home (β = -.26, χ2(1) = 37.24, p < .001).

Table 1. Participants’ demographic information.

Estonia Germany
M (SD)/ No (frequency) Range M (SD)/ No (frequency) Range
Age of child (months) 50 (5.96) 36–59 48 (5.99) 37–58
Gender of child
    Boys (%) 15 (52%) 10 (48%)
Children attending kindergarten (%) 8 (28%) 17 (58%)
Parent’s age (years) 37 (4.27) 27–46 36 (3.90) 30–45
Parent’s education
    High-school diploma/ vocational training 4 (14%) 6 (21%)
    Bachelor’s degree 17 (59%) 6 (21%)
    Master’s degree 8 (27%) 17 (58%)

Procedure

Parents of preschool children were informed of the study as a call for participants was distributed via social media and university webpages. Participants were asked to provide three conversations with their children and fill in a questionnaire on background information, values, and COVID-19 related changes in the family life and well-being. The parents provided informed written consent to participate in the study, filled in the questionnaire online, and uploaded the conversations via an approved upload page. The present study focuses on one of the conversations where parents were instructed to discuss the changes in children’s life during the pandemic and the causes for them. The instructions stressed that they could record the conversation any time that was convenient for them, that they should talk with the child as they normally would, and that the conversation could be as long or short as is natural to them. The prompt for the conversation provided to parents was “Please talk to your child about the changes that have taken place in his/her life after the Coronavirus pandemic started. Also, discuss the reasons for the changes.” The procedure used was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the University of Tartu and University of Applied Sciences Magdeburg-Stendal.

Coding

The conversations were transcribed, and coders used the MAXQDA program (MAXQDA, 1989–2021) to assign codes to the transcribed conversations. Each main clause by the parent and child was coded using code categories presented in Table 2.

Table 2. Coding categories for parent-child conversations.

Categories and subcategories Definition Examples
Information Talk Talk about the coronavirus disease Parent: What does Coronavirus do?
Changes Talk Talk describing pandemic related changes Child: Now you have to wear a mask.
Explanatory Talk Clauses that either provided or asked for explanations. P: Why can’t you go to kindergarten?
C: Because if one has Corona, they will pass it to others, and other children get Corona.
Explanatory Talk: general Explanations that only referenced the disease in general; also, questions for explanations were given this code. P: Why can’t you go to kindergarten?
C: You can’t go to the kindergarten because there is the disease.
Explanatory Talk: scientific Explanations that refer to the mechanisms of disease transmission P: Because germs come out of our nose and mouth when we talk, and masks help us not to spread germs.
C: Because you could get infected.
Explanatory Talk: animistic Explanations that prescribe human characteristics and aspirations to the virus C: Corona looks for our mouth to come in.
Explanatory Talk: other These included pragmatic explanations, socially oriented explanations, and explanations focusing on rules. C: (I am not going to kindergarten), because you are at home.
C: We don’t go out, because otherwise we meet friends and give them hugs.
C: (I am not going to kindergarten), because it is not allowed.
Evaluative talk Clauses that provided personal evaluations of the situation
Positive Clauses providing positive evaluations. C: I think it’s nice to be inside home and play with Legos.
Negative Clauses providing negative evaluations. C: It is boring.
Undefined These included evaluations or questions for evaluations that could not be classified as positive or negative. P: What do you think of being at home?
Emotion Talk Talk that referred to emotions regarding the situation.
Positive Talk that referred to positive emotions regarding the situation. P: And this makes you happy?
Negative Talk that referred to negative emotions regarding the situation. P: Are you afraid of Corona?
Undefined This category included questions about emotions and feelings that could not be categorized as positive or negative. P: How do you feel in this situation?
Other Talk This category included utterances about life in general or the situation at hand. P: Do you like to go the playground?
C: Let’s play!

The coding system was exhaustive and exclusive; two coders coded 20% of the Estonian and German transcripts, intercoder reliability was κ = .80 for child utterances and κ = .84 for parent utterances.

Results

For the hypothesis testing count numbers of the different code categories were used and Poisson regression models with a log function were constructed to identify the effect of context. Maternal education was included as a covariate to control for the differences in the conversation that could be ascribed to maternal education. For the exploratory analyses, the ratio of negative and positive evaluations to all evaluations and the ratio of negative and positive emotions to all emotions by the dyad was calculated. Nonparametric tests were used to compare the ratio measures of the Estonian and German context.

Descriptive analyses

The average length of the coded conversations was 44 (SD = 28.54, range 6–165) main clauses, with parents uttering 29.27 (SD = 19.58) clauses on average and children contributing 14.66 (SD = 10.59) clauses. The largest part of the conversations was devoted to pandemic related changes in both contexts (Mclauses = 28.12, range 2–69). The means and standard deviations for all the coded categories in the two contexts are presented in Table 3.

Table 3. The means, standard deviations, and proportion of talk for all the coded categories by cultural context.

Category Estonian dyads German dyads
Mean number of clauses (SD) Proportion of the talk devoted to the category Mean number of clauses (SD) Proportion of the talk devoted to the category
Information talk 6.34 (7.43)*** .12 3.21 (4.62)*** .08
Changes talk 32.10 (18.52)*** .65 24.14 (14.91)*** .63
Explanatory talk (total) 5.21 (6.24) .12 4.55 (5.60) .12
General explanations 3.38 (4.79) 3.17 (4.01)
Scientific explanations .48 (.91) .86 (1.33)
Animistic explanations .07 (.25) .00 (.00)
Other explanations 1.28 (1.96) .52 (1.24)
Evaluative talk (total) 3.79 (5.99)*** .07 6.86 (9.09)*** .16
Negative .93 (2.22) 2.62 (3.85)
Positive 1.97 (2.69) 1.79 (3.24)
Emotion talk (total) 3.93 (9.20) .04 .69 (1.79) .02
Negative 1.69 (3.81) .51 (1.77)
Positive .59 (1.86) .14 (.44)
Total no of main clauses 51.38 (34.25)*** 1.0 39.45 (22.36)*** 1.0

***- statistically significant effect of context (p < .001) detected using Poisson regression with a log link.

Note: The Estonian sample includes an outlier; the results remained the same when analyses were run with and without the participant.

First, separate Poisson regressions with a log function were used to establish the effect of context (Estonia vs Germany) on the total number of main clauses, the number of clauses devoted to Information talk, Changes talk, Explanatory talk, and Evaluative talk. Context predicted the total number of main clauses (β = 3.68, χ2(1) = 45.58 p < .001), the number of clauses devoted to Information talk (β = .77, χ2(1) = 35.06, p < .001), and Changes talk (β = .29, χ2(1) = 31.54, p < .001). Estonian dyads allotted more main clauses to these topics than German dyads. Evaluative talk was also predicted by the context (β = -.59, χ2(1) = 24.90, p < .001) with German dyads devoting more utterances to evaluative talk than Estonian dyads. The amount of Explanatory talk did not differ between the contexts. Maternal education was a significant covariate for the total number of main clauses (β = .10, χ2(1) = 11.83, p < .001), Information talk (β = .32, χ2(1) = 12.13, p < .001), and Evaluative talk (β = .19, χ2(1) = 5.66, p < .05).

As emotion talk was very infrequent with 72% of Estonian and 76% of German dyads not mentioning emotions, a categorical variable was built for this measure (dyads mentioning emotions vs dyads not mentioning emotions). A Chi-square analysis was run to see if the context was related to the dyad’s use of emotions. The use of Emotion talk did not differ between the contexts.

For the exploratory analyses, ratios of positive and negative evaluations to total number of evaluations were calculated to investigate the valence of the talk. Separate Mann-Whitney U tests were run to investigate the differences between contexts in the negative and positive evaluation ratios. The ratio of negative evaluations did not differ between the contexts, but the ratio of positive evaluations did (U = 93.5, p = .007): Estonian dyads used more positive evaluations relative to all evaluations than German dyads.

Discussion

Parent-child conversations about the COVID-19 pandemic related changes in children’s life and the reasons for the changes were studied for their focus and valence in Estonia and Germany. Specifically, we were interested in the types of explanations regarding COVID-19 related changes and the naturally occurring emotion talk concerning the pandemic.

As expected, the conversations did contain explanations, including scientific ones, but contrary to our expectations, only a couple of animistic explanations appeared. Studies show that regarding falling ill, people often refer to intuitive theories (e.g., one gets a cold due to being cold) [18]. There are cultural differences in children’s biological explanations [54] and in cause of illness understanding [18] indicating that some cultural contexts are more prone to some intuitive theories. Earlier studies also indicate that young children make sense of natural phenomena using magical or animistic thinking [55, 56]. Prior studies of explanations regarding the Coronavirus show that parents report providing correct realistic responses to children’s questions in different cultural contexts [4143]. Haber et al. [41] also showed that parents of 3-5year-olds often reference germs.

In the present study, both parents and children provide correct, albeit general information regarding the disease and the pandemic-related changes. The present study included mostly highly educated parents and their children of two European contexts and maternal education was not a significant covariate for the use of explanations. It appears that all parents were rather well-informed of the causes of the pandemic and the transfer mechanisms of the disease; the conversations with their children seem to reflect that as they do not resort to animistic explanations or intuitive theories. It is also possible that the findings reflect the tendency of parents with strong autonomous orientation to support children’s cognitive development [44, 45]. In any case, it appears that regarding pandemic-related explanations, parents in these two Western contexts have managed to curtail the sometimes-harmful magical thinking that experts warn against [e.g., 36, 37].

Similar to the results of Haber et al. [41], most explanations in the conversations were general in nature. One possible reason is that parents consider scientific explanations to be beyond their children’s grasp. And prior research has shown that it is very difficult for adults to appropriate scientific explanations to children’s level of comprehension [57]. Also, general explanations could dominate among explanations because it was often the parent who asked for the explanation and the child that provided one. Few children in our sample were asking for the reasons behind specific changes. After children provided an explanation, mothers often did not elaborate further. May be to let the child decide on how much they want to focus on the matter or considering general explanations to be appropriate for children of that age. The following excerpts are typical examples:

C: Eh, I know what we are not allowed to do. Namely, we are not allowed to cuddle with each other.

P: Mhm. With nobody?

C: But only with our families.

P: Mhm. And do you know why?

C: Because otherwise, because we don’t know whether they have Corona.

P: Hm, do you know why you stayed home for so long?

C: Because of the virus.

P: Because of the virus, exactly. Do you remember what’s going on with the virus?

C: All people sick.

P: Exactly. A lot of people got sick. And because of that?

C: Because of that?

P: You weren’t allowed to go to the kindergarten, right?

C: Right.

P: And what else happened? Who wasn’t allowed to go to work in the beginning?

C: Mom.

(German parents and children)

Interestingly, a study by Menendez et al. [42] conducted in the US showed that parents of children aged 3–12 report providing mostly socially oriented explanations for the changes related to the pandemic. This contradicts the present study where we see that the majority or explanations discussed are either general (e.g., “Because of the disease.”) or scientific that provide information about how the disease spreads. Here, the reason for such difference could be the method of research: Menendez et al. [42] use parental reports where parents provided a selected sample of questions. The difference could also appear due to the different age groups studied: the sample of Menendez et al. [42] also included older children whose social circle is wider and who could therefore request more information and explanations regarding social restrictions. Finally, it would be interesting to see if observational studies with kindergarteners in the US would have the same focus.

In general, the Estonian and German conversations seemed rather similar, indicating that parents approached the task in a similar manner. At the same time, differences in volume appeared: Estonian conversations were longer as parents and children talked more about the Coronavirus itself and the related changes than German parents and children. While in other situations such as talking about personal experiences parent-child talk in Estonia has been shown to be brief compared to other European contexts [49, 50], the result is surprising. It could, perhaps, be ascribed to the pragmatic perspective on conversations in Estonia attributed to the autonomy-relatedness oriented context: it is possible that Estonian parents feel comfortable talking to children about actual rather than personal matters. Estonians have been found not to be very good in small talk, they speak when there is something important to say, and tolerate silence more than people from many other nations [58]. It also seemed that they would not try to shield the child from unpleasant topics; the conversations featured serious consequences, being ill, and even death, as illustrated in the following excerpt:

P: But why do we have to stay home?

C: Because there is Corona.

P: But why is it bad? It is a bad disease.

C: Yes, and then people die.

P: People die, yes. Did you have Corona too or?

C: Yes.

P: Who else had it?

C: You and daddy.

These findings resonate with prior studies conducted in different cultural contexts. Studies focusing on prototypical autonomy-oriented contexts have shown that parents would want to shield children from pandemic related information [42] or not focus on the unfortunate consequences of the pandemic with young children [41]. A study conducted in Turkey [43] that is a more relatedness-oriented context showed that parents and children focused more on the virus itself rather than life-style changes.

German parent-child dyads were expected to discuss the emotional effect of the pandemic more that Estonian dyads, but this was not the case. Instead, all participants talked very little about emotions. This is surprising, as from the theoretical viewpoint, it is developmentally a good time to discuss emotions with children in order to support their socio-emotional development [e.g., 20, 21]. From the practical viewpoint, discussing emotions at difficult times is important for better coping and mental health [6, 38]. The present study did not prompt for emotion talk and prior conversational studies focusing on other situations suggest that emotion talk is not very prominent [49, 53]. Also, Tambling et al. [40] showed that when parents were surveyed about managing children’s stress, only about ten percent of parents mentioned emotions. Yet, it is still surprising that even in stressful situations, when lives of families have been turned upside down, the talk about emotions with children is very sparse.

Although studies suggest that preschool age is an important period of children’s lives to discuss emotional reactions [23], it is possible that children of this age group are not as much emotionally or at least negatively affected by the pandemic as perhaps older children and parents. A closer look at the conversations reveals that a considerable proportion of emotion and evaluative talk is devoted to positive aspects of the situation. The positive aspect is illustrated in the following excerpt:

P: But did you miss your friends?

C: No. I just think that it is fun to be at home and fun to play at home.

(Estonian mother and child)

The negative aspects mentioned most often revolved around boredom and not fear or anxiety. An excerpt-example on boredom:

P: … But the things we are doing every day, don’t you like them too, are they bad too?

C: Boring.

P: Boring? You are bored? What is boring? Tell me.

(German parent and child)

It is also important to note that maternal education was a significant covariate for the length of the conversations, for talking about Corona virus in general, and for providing evaluations of the situation: children of more educated mothers participated in conversations that were longer, included more information about the disease, and discussed the pros and cons of the situation more often than children of less educated mothers in both contexts. Maternal education has been identified as a crucial factor influencing input in language development [59], therefore future studies of children’s experiences could also pay attention to differences linked to maternal education.

The present study focused on conversations between preschool children and their parents about the pandemic related changes and reasons for the changes. The study is not without limitations, the most significant being that the study was voluntary and included mostly highly educated parents. It is probable that parents or children who are really struggling with coping in the new reality opted out of the study. Salmon [38] has pointed out that the pandemic could be especially detrimental to more vulnerable families and further research should make an effort to include a wider range of parents and children. Future studies could also clarify if the findings generalize to more educationally diverse samples. In addition, the study had a rather small sample and focused on one conversation that parents provided: future studies should also try to capture naturally occurring talk and expand the sample. As parental conversations are related to children’s language development [59], it would be advisable to include a measure of child language development.

Nevertheless, the results show that parents support children’s conceptual understanding of the pandemic and hence cognitive development as they discuss the disease and the reasons for changes with their children. The conversations showcase factually correct (not animistic or magical), yet rather general notions of the disease and the pandemic related changes. Surprisingly, little attention is paid to the emotions and feelings about the stressful situation and hence socio-emotional development. The results carry practical implications: educated parents in European contexts have a good grasp on supporting cognitive understanding of current situations. At the same time, they could use a reminder to also focus on discussing emotions regarding the situation in order to support socio-emotional development and children’s coping in stressful situations.

Supporting information

S1 File. An anonymized dataset of parent-child conversations about COVID-19 in Estonia and Germany.

(SAV)

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by the Estonian Research Council grants (PSG296, PI Pirko Tõugu and PRG1761, PI Tiia Tulviste). The funder had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Vygotsky L. Mind in society: The development of higher psychologycal processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1978. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Crowley K, Callanan MA, Jipson JL, Galco J, Topping K, Shrager J. Shared scientific thinking in everyday parent-child activity. Science Education. 2001;85(6):712–32. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Hedrick AM, San Souci P, Haden CA, Ornstein PA. Mother-child joint conversational exchanges during events: Linkages to children’s memory reports over time. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2009;10(3):143–61. doi: 10.1080/15248370903155791 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Wellman HM. Reinvigorating explanations for the study of early cognitive development. Child Development Perspectives. 2011;5(1):33–8. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Fivush R, Haden CA, Reese E. Elaborating on elaborations: Role of maternal reminiscing style in cognitive and socioemotional development. Child Development. 2006;77(6):1568–88. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00960.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Koh JBK, Wang Q. Mother-child reminiscing about emotionally negative events and children’s long-term mental health. Frontiers in Psychology. 2021; 12(632799), doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.632799 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Schröder L, Keller H, Kärtner J, Kleis A, Abels M, Yovsi RD, et al. Early reminiscing in cultural context: Cultural models, maternal reminiscing style, and children’s memories. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2013;14(1):10–34. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Wang Q, Fivush R. Mother–child conversations of emotionally salient events: Exploring the functions of emotional reminiscing in European-American and Chinese families. Social Development. 2005;14(3):473–95. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Haden CA, Tõugu P. Socialization of early autobiographical memory. In: Gülgöz S, Sahin-Acar B, editors. Autobiographical Memory Development: Routledge; 2020. p. 22–36. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Aukrust V, Snow C. Narratives and explanations in Norwegian and American mealtime conversations. Language in Society. 1998;27(2):221–46. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Beals DE. Explanatory talk in low-income families’ mealtime conversations. Applied Psycholinguistics. 1993;14(4):489–513. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Hickling AK, Wellman HM. The emergence of children’s causal explanations and theories: Evidence from everyday conversation. Developmental psychology. 2001;37(5):668. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.37.5.668 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Callanan MA, Oakes LM. Preschoolers’ questions and parents’ explanations: Causal thinking in everyday activity. Cognitive Development. 1992;7(2):213–33. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Corriveau KH, Kurkul KE. “Why does rain fall?”: Children prefer to learn from an informant who uses noncircular explanations. Child development. 2014;85(5):1827–35. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12240 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Frazier BN, Gelman SA, Wellman HM. Young children prefer and remember satisfying explanations. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2016;17(5):718–36. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2015.1098649 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Christidou V, Hatzinikita V. Preschool children’s explanations of plant growth and rain formation: A comparative analysis. Research in Science Education. 2006;36(3):187–210. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Hannust T, Kikas E. Children’s knowledge of astronomy and its change in the course of learning. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2007;22(1):89–104. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Hernandez IG, Menendez D, Seitz V, Pinto-Pro I, Zeitler MH, Rosengren KS. Parent-Child conversations of germ and cold weather theories of the common cold in two cultures. PsyArXiv; 2020. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/7j8pb [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Aznar A, Tenenbaum HR. Spanish parents’ emotion talk and their children’s understanding of emotion. Frontiers in Psychology. 2013;4:670. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00670 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Laible D. Mother-child discourse in two contexts: links with child temperament, attachment security, and socioemotional competence. Developmental Psychology. 2004;40(6):979–92. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.979 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Laible D. Does it matter if preschool children and mothers discuss positive vs. negative events during reminiscing? Links with mother‐reported attachment, family emotional climate, and socioemotional development. Social Development. 2011;20(2):394–411. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Laible D, Panfile Murphy T, Augustine M. Constructing emotional and relational understanding: The role of mother–child reminiscing about negatively valenced events. Social Development. 2013;22(2):300–18. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Leyva D, Catalán Molina D, Suárez C, Tamis-Lemonda CS, Yoshikawa H. Mother-Child Reminiscing and first-graders’ emotion competence in a low-income and ethnically diverse sample. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2021:1–22. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Wilson AC, Lengua LJ, Meltzoff AN, Smith KA. Parenting and temperament prior to September 11, 2001, and parenting specific to 9/11 as predictors of children’s posttraumatic stress symptoms following 9/11. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2010;39(4):445–59. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2010.486317 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Achterberg M, Dobbelaar S, Boer OD, Crone EA. Perceived stress as mediator for longitudinal effects of the COVID-19 lockdown on wellbeing of parents and children. Scientific reports. 2021;11(1):1–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Whittle S, Bray K, Lin S, Schwartz O. Parenting and child and adolescent mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic. PsyArXiv; 2020. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/ag2r7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Ackil JK, Van Abbema DL, Bauer PJ. After the storm: Enduring differences in mother–child recollections of traumatic and nontraumatic events. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2003;84(4):286–309. doi: 10.1016/s0022-0965(03)00027-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Burch MM, Austin J, Bauer PJ. Understanding the emotional past:: Relations between parent and child contributions in emotionally negative and nonnegative events. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2004;89(4):276–97. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.07.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Brownell CA. Peer social skills in toddlers: Competences and constraints illustrated by same-age and mixed-age interaction. Child Development. 1990;61:838–48. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Israel O, Dorit A, Tom C-C, Galia MK, Laly M. Shared book reading at home and at school prior to and since the COVID-19 outbreak. Reading and Writing. 2022. 10.1007/s11145-022-10354-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Medrano M, Cadenas‐Sanchez C, Oses M, Arenaza L, Amasene M, Labayen I. Changes in lifestyle behaviours during the COVID‐19 confinement in Spanish children: A longitudinal analysis from the MUGI project. Pediatric Obesity. 2021;16(4):e12731. doi: 10.1111/ijpo.12731 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Philippe K, Chabanet C, Issanchou S, Monnery-Patris S. Child eating behaviors, parental feeding practices and food shopping motivations during the COVID-19 lockdown in France:(How) did they change? Appetite. 2021;161:105132. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2021.105132 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Stoecklin D, Gervais C, Kutsar D, Heite C. Lockdown and children’s well-being: experiences of children in Switzerland, Canada and Estonia. Childhood Vulnerability Journal. 2021:1–19. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Panda PK, Gupta J, Chowdhury SR, Kumar R, Meena AK, Madaan P, et al. Psychological and behavioral impact of lockdown and quarantine measures for COVID-19 pandemic on children, adolescents and caregivers: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Tropical Pediatrics. 2021;67(1). doi: 10.1093/tropej/fmaa122 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Kutsar D, Kurvet-Käosaar L. The impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on families: Young people’s experiences in Estonia. Frontiers in Sociology. 2021;6(163). doi: 10.3389/fsoc.2021.732984 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Dalton L, Rapa E, Stein A. Protecting the psychological health of children through effective communication about COVID-19. The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health. 2020;4(5):346–7. doi: 10.1016/S2352-4642(20)30097-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Provenzi L, Baroffio E, Ligabue S, Borgatti R. The little professor and the virus: Scaffolding children’s meaning making during the COVID-19 emergency. Frontiers in Psychiatry. 2020;11(817). doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00817 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Salmon K. The ecology of youth psychological wellbeing in the COVID-19 pandemic. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition. 2021;10(4):564–76. doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2021.11.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Idoiaga N, Berasategi N, Eiguren A, Picaza M. Exploring children’s social and emotional representations of the Covid-19 pandemic. Frontiers in Psychology. 2020;11(1952). doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01952 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Tambling R, Tomkunas A, Russell B, Horton A, Hutchison M. Thematic analysis of parent–child conversations about covid-19:“playing it safe”. Journal of Child and Family Studies. 2021;30(2):325–37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Haber AS, Kumar SC, Puttre H, Dashoush N, Corriveau KH. “Why can’t I see my friends and family?”: Children’s questions and parental explanations about Coronavirus. Mind, Brain, and Education. 2022;16(1):54–61. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Menendez D, Klapper RE, Golden MZ, Mandel AR, Nicholas KA, Schapfel MH, et al. “When will it be over?” US children’s questions and parents’ responses about the COVID-19 pandemic. PLOS ONE. 2021;16(8). doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0256692 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Ünlütabak B, Velioğlu I. Examining children’s questions and parents’ responses about COVID-19 pandemic in Turkey. Current Psychology. 2022. doi: 10.1007/s12144-022-03331-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Keller H, Lamm B, Abels M, Yovsi RD, Borke J, Jensen H, et al. Cultural models, socialization goals, and parenting ethnotheories: A multicultural analysis. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2006;37(2):155–72. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Kağitçibaşi Ç. Family, self, and human development across cultures: Theory and applications. 2nd ed. Mahwah, New Jersey: Psychology Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Kärtner J. The autonomous developmental pathway: The primacy of subjective mental states for human behavior and experience. Child Development. 2015;86(4):1298–309. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12377 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Tõugu P, Tulviste T, Schröder L, Keller H, De Geer B. Socialization of past event talk: Cultural differences in maternal elaborative reminiscing. Cognitive Development. 2011;26:142–54. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Tulviste T, Mizera L, De Geer B. Socialization values in stable and changing societies: A comparative study of Estonian, Swedish, and Russian Estonian mothers. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2012;43(3):480–97. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Tulviste T, Tõugu P, Keller H, Schröder L, De Geer B. Children’s and mothers’ contribution to joint reminiscing in different sociocultural contexts: who speaks and what is said. Infant and Child Development. 2016;25(1):43–63. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Tulviste T, Mizera L, De Geer B. Expressing communicative intents in Estonian, Finnish, and Swedish mother-adolescent interactions. Journal of Child Language. 2004;31:801–19. doi: 10.1017/s0305000904006488 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Tulviste T, Kikas E. Qualities to be developed in Estonian children at home and at school. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2010;31(4):315–21. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Tulviste T, De Geer B. Autonomy orientation in Estonian and Swedish family interactions. Pragmatics. 2009;19(2):1–13. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Carmiol AM, Schröder L. Emotion talk during mother–child reminiscing and book sharing and children’s socioemotional competence: Evidence from Costa Rica and Germany. Culture and Brain. 2019;7(2):126–47. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Legare C, Zhu L, Wellman H. Examining biological explanations in Chinese preschool children: A cross-cultural comparison. Journal of Cognition and Culture. 2013;13(1–2):67–93. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Ahi B. Thinking about digestive system in early childhood: A comparative study about biological knowledge. Cogent Education. 2017;4(1). doi: 10.1080/2331186X.2017.1278650 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Thulin S, Pramling N. Anthropomorphically speaking: On communication between teachers and children in early childhood biology education. International Journal of Early Years Education. 2009;17(2):137–50. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Vlach HA, Noll N. Talking to children about science is harder than we think: characteristics and metacognitive judgments of explanations provided to children and adults. Metacognition and Learning. 2016;11(3):317–38. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Mizera L, Tulviste T, Konstabel K, Lausa E. Silent and slow Estonians, emotional and fast Russians: A comparative study of communication stereotypes in two neighboring countries. Communication Quarterly. 2013;61(3):268–83. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Rowe M L., Snow CE. Analyzing input quality along three dimensions: Interactive, linguistic, and conceptual. Journal of Child Language. 2020;47(1):5–21. doi: 10.1017/S0305000919000655 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Avanti Dey

7 Sep 2022

PONE-D-22-19799Making Sense of the Pandemic: Parent-Child Conversations in Two Cultural ContextsPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Tõugu,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

 Can you please address the concerns raised by the two expert reviewers?

Please submit your revised manuscript by Oct 21 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Avanti Dey, PhD

Staff Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf.

2. Thank you for stating the following in the Acknowledgments Section of your manuscript: 

"This work was supported by the Estonian Research Council grant (PSG296)."

We note that you have provided funding information that is currently declared in your Funding Statement. However, funding information should not appear in the Acknowledgments section or other areas of your manuscript. We will only publish funding information present in the Funding Statement section of the online submission form. 

Please remove any funding-related text from the manuscript and let us know how you would like to update your Funding Statement. Currently, your Funding Statement reads as follows: 

"This work was supported by the Estonian Research Council grant (PSG296), (PI Pirko Tõugu). The funder had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript."

Please include your amended statements within your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

3. We note that you have indicated that data from this study are available upon request. PLOS only allows data to be available upon request if there are legal or ethical restrictions on sharing data publicly. For information on unacceptable data access restrictions, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions. 

In your revised cover letter, please address the following prompts:

a) If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially identifying or sensitive patient information) and who has imposed them (e.g., an ethics committee). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent.

b) If there are no restrictions, please upload the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate your study findings as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. Please see http://www.bmj.com/content/340/bmj.c181.long for guidelines on how to de-identify and prepare clinical data for publication. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories.

We will update your Data Availability statement on your behalf to reflect the information you provide.

4. Please include your full ethics statement in the ‘Methods’ section of your manuscript file. In your statement, please include the full name of the IRB or ethics committee who approved or waived your study, as well as whether or not you obtained informed written or verbal consent. If consent was waived for your study, please include this information in your statement as well. 

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: Partly

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: PONE-D-22-19799

I think this study is interesting and has merit in terms of shedding light on parent-child conversations and how these may vary across contexts that seem similar at first (two Western nations). At the same time I have some serious concerns about the framing of the paper. The study analyzed a single parent-child conversation surrounding the Coronavirus -- “the changes in children’s life during the pandemic and the causes for them”. The authors frame the study as examining a naturalistic conversation. I am not certain that this is really the case given the nature of the prompt for the parents (there is no way to really assess how regularly these types of conversations occur) nor the reliance on parents having to record the conversations, which is certainly subject to social desirability bias.

The literature review needs to be elaborated, both in terms of research on parent-child talk and interactions during Covid-19 (see some of the studies below), as well as the section on talk during stressful times. The authors need to strengthen their case for why to look particularly at conversations during this time as a way to examine the particular characteristics of these conversations. For example, are these conversations more or less likely to include emotion-related talk?

Please see some of the following published studies, which should be included in the research relating to parent-child talk surrounding Covid-19:

Tambling, R. R., Tomkunas, A. J., Russell, B. S., Horton, A. L., & Hutchison, M. (2021). Thematic analysis of parent–child conversations about covid-19:“playing it safe”. Journal of child and family studies, 30(2), 325-337.

Haber, A. S., Kumar, S. C., Puttre, H., Dashoush, N., & Corriveau, K. H. (2022). “Why Can't I See My Friends and Family?”: Children's Questions and Parental Explanations About Coronavirus. Mind, Brain, and Education, 16(1), 54-61.

Israel, O. M. E. P., Dorit, A., Tom, C. C., Galia, M. K., & Laly, M. Shared Book Reading at Home and at School Prior to and Since the COVID-19 Outbreak.

Aram, D., Asaf, M., Karabanov, G. M., Ziv, M., Sonnenschein, S., Stites, M., ... & López-Escribano, C. (2022). Beneficial Parenting According to the “Parenting Pentagon Model”: A Cross-Cultural Study During a Pandemic. In The Impact of COVID-19 on Early Childhood Education and Care (pp. 215-236). Springer, Cham.

Methods:

• More information about the children is needed – are they in some kind of preschool setting (and does this vary between Estonia & Germany)? What was the state of these settings during the time of data collection (lockdown, etc.).

• The authors note that data was collected during the first and second wave of the pandemic. How many conversations were collected during each wave? Was any assessment done to examine differences in conversations between the time points?

• Were the children evaluated for language development? This can significantly impact how parents talk with their children.

• What was the prompt given to parents in terms of the conversation?

Results:

• GLM for Poisson distribution were run – were these negative binomial regressions? If so, please specify. If not, please specify exactly which type of regressions were run.

• The authors assessed length by number of total clauses, but as parent-child talk is very variable by individual, was this made into some kind of ratio or presented in some other way that might account for this variability? All that this will tell us otherwise is that some parents speak more than others (plus, as mentioned above, there was no accounting for children’s language development).

Discussion:

• The authors note cultural differences relating to types of explanations (scientific, animistic, etc.). Have any of these been examined specifically in the Estonian/German contexts? If so, how do your results here support/contradict those? If no studies have been done in these contexts, then I think some additional exploration or thoughts of why these types of explanations might be common in these contexts is warranted here.

• I believe that the limitations of the study need to be expanded. This was a study conducted with a fairly small number of highly educated parents, examining a single conversation. This should be acknowledged as an important limitation. The need to further examine conversation of various types is necessary. Another significant limitation is the lack of any kind of information about the children’s language ability, which can certainly impact the nature of parent-child conversations. This must be acknowledged.

Editing:

• Be consistent in terminology – sometimes you write Coronavirus (one word) and sometimes Corona virus (two words).

• The article should be carefully edited for English language – commas, subject-verb agreement, etc.

Reviewer #2: This is a well written manuscript investigating an important topic namely how parents actually talk with their children about the COVID-19 pandemic. An additional strength is the cultural comparison. The Introduction sets up the study very well, and I particularly liked the writing style, which was engaging and remarkably jargon-free.

Comments:

1. It would have been useful to have more information on the differences and similarities between the two comparison cultures (Estonia, Germany) to set up hypotheses regarding cultural differences in reminiscing. The authors suggest that these cultures differ “slightly” in their focus on autonomy vs relatedness, and parent socialisation factors reflect these differences. How different is “slight” and how might this be manifested more generally in each culture?

2. Relatedly, as the authors acknowledge, both samples were quite small, and both were highly educated. How were the samples recruited? Were the samples sufficiently large and diverse to detect “true” cultural differences?

3. More information about the approach to coding would be appreciated. How does the MAXDA programme operate? What is the role of human coders vs the programme?

4. How were the particular conversational examples selected for the Discussion.

5. There are some presentation issues: the list format of the coding in the Method; the use of the words “of which” in Table 1 (it is unclear what this means).

The following manuscript that might be helpful in placing parents’ response to the pandemic in a broader context, and includes a focus on parent-child reminiscing, might be useful.

Salmon, K. (2021). The ecology of youth psychological wellbeing in the COVID-19 pandemic. Journal of Applied Research on Memory and Cognition,10,(4), 564-576 .

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2023 Jan 23;18(1):e0280781. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0280781.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


12 Oct 2022

Response letter to the reviewers

Thank you very much for reviewing the manuscript and pointing out important caveats in the form it was first submitted. We have tried to address all of your concerns and we hope that you find the manuscript much improved. I will respond and point out the amendments point by point below.

Concerns raised by the academic editor:

1. The style of the manuscript (titles, references etc.) was aligned with the requirements of PLOS ONE.

2. The Acknowledgement section was deleted. As it was, it only doubled the information already presented in the Funding Section. The Funding Section contains all necessary information and does not need to be updated.

3. The anonymized data set is uploaded.

4. The method section is updated with information about written consent and the ethics committees that oversighted the project.

Reviewer 1

The introduction and especially the part about Corona experience was updated and carefully reworked in order to point out the importance of the manuscript (observational comparative study) yet be respectful of its limitations (not naturally occurring talk, but self-selected recordings). We have also taken care to clarify why emotions are expected to be mentioned.

In the method section:

- More information is provided about the preschool setting during data collection.

- It is clarified how much data was collected during the first and the second wave.

- Unfortunately, children’s language abilities were not assessed in this study. We do now refer to the need to do so in future studies in the limitation section.

- The prompt provided to parents is now presented in the method section.

In the results section:

- Poisson regression with the log link were run to analyze the data, it is now also clarified in the manuscript.

- The mean number of clauses for children and parents are also now provided in the Descriptive analyses section. In addition, proportions of the type of talk relative to the amount of talk are presented in Table 2.

Discussion:

- Unfortunately, the types of explanations for illness have not been studied in these contexts before. At the same time, we have rewritten the introduction and discussion section to provide a better foundation for hypothesis and the explanation for the results.

- The limitation of the study were expanded; both the restrictions impinged by the sample and the fact that we do not have language assessment data are highlighted.

In general:

- Inconsistences in terminology were removed.

- The manuscript was carefully proofread.

Reviewer 2

1. We rewritten the instruction (and especially “Present study” section) where we now provide more information about the slightly different focus on autonomy and relatedness and the related differences in socialization.

2. More information is provided about recruitment and the small sample size and need for more diversity is acknowledged in the limitation section of the manuscript

3. The MAXQDA program was used to manually assign codes to segments of the transcript. No automatic coding was used. This is now also clarified in the manuscript.

4. Examples were selected from among other similar ones to display the particular coding category under discussion and to illustrate the results.

5. Presentation issues were fixed, and the suggested literature added to the introduction.

We thank you again for your input and hope you find the manuscript improved!

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Alice Coles-Aldridge

14 Nov 2022

PONE-D-22-19799R1Making Sense of the Pandemic: Parent-Child Conversations in Two Cultural ContextsPLOS ONE​

Dear Dr. Tõugu,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

The manuscript has been evaluated by two reviewers, and their comments are available below.

The reviewers have raised a number of concerns and clarifications required regarding the sample, the examples of coding categories, some of the statistical results, and the discussion. 

Please submit your revised manuscript by Dec 28 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Alice Coles-Aldridge

Editorial Office

PLOS ONE

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Partly

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: I appreciate the changes the authors have made, particularly the expanded explanations of the contexts between the two countries. I still have some questions/clarifications before I think the article can be published:

- Regarding the sample:

o Did you run any kind of t-tests regarding education? There seem to be many more German mothers with master’s degrees. Was this a significant difference? Since we know education can impact conversations, this has the potential to impact the results.

o Similarly, was any evaluation done on the conversations between those who were in kindergarten compared to those who were not? The ones who were might also have been getting information from kindergarten staff or hearing other explanations, which might have made a difference.

- It would be helpful if the examples in the table of the coding categories provided examples of child utterances as well as parents, or at least clearly distinguished which is which.

- The second table (descriptive statistics of the conversations) should be labeled Table 2.

o At the bottom of the table, you note that the * represents statistical significance at p<.001, but traditionally, a single star denotes significance at p<.05. If indeed significance was at p<.001, you should use three asterisks to denote this (or whatever alternative the journal format calls for).

- For the Mann-Whitney results, you also noted p<.005, but this is an unusual reporting. Did you mean .05 or .001? Or was p=.005? Please clarify.

- In the Discussion, you refer to Menendez et al., but do not note the context, which may be relevant. Please add this.

- There is still some editing that needs to be done (e.g., the first sentence of the abstract has grammatical mistakes).

Reviewer #2: This manuscript is much improved and I congratulate the authors on their comprehensive approach to the revisions.

I have a couple of outstanding comments.

1. The number of boys and girls differs between cultural samples. As reminiscing style is, at least at times, influenced by gender, it would be good to see greater consideration of this factor in the Discussion and Results. For example, do the results remain the same when child gender is a control variable?

2. There could be greater nuance in the discussion of cultural differences. For example, in the Introduction (p.6) there is a statement made about the difference between US and Turkish samples - yet the US (and possibly Turkey) is comprised of myriad subcultures and I"m wondering if it's possible to generalise in this way. Perhaps the authors could describe their samples more - they talk about the high education in both their German and Estonian samples in the Discussion, which is helpful.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2023 Jan 23;18(1):e0280781. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0280781.r004

Author response to Decision Letter 1


29 Nov 2022

Thank you very much for your careful attention to the manuscript and the points that you raised! We have addressed all your concerns and hope you find the manuscript improved! Here are the changes we made in respect to reviewers’ comments:

Reviewer 1:

• As suggested by reviewer 1 Analyses were run regarding the parental education of the sample. It turned out the German mothers are more likely to have a Master’s degree or above, while Estonian mothers are more likely to have a Bachelor’s degree. A note about possible effects of this educational difference is also added to the limitations section.

• Also, analyses were run in order to establish if attending kindergarten was related to the length of the conversations and the results are reported in the manuscript.

• It is now indicated whether the utterance is provided by the child or parent in the Table. It is not possible to provide examples by parent and child at all times, as not all categories were used by both e.g. children did not use Evaluative Undefined category or parents did not use Animistic Explanations.

• The error in the heading of the second table was fixed and it is Table 2 in the revised version of the manuscript. The reference in the text is also fixed.

• Asterix were added to the values to adhere to reporting conventions.

• The mistake is fixed and an exact p value for Mann-Whitney is reported in the manuscript.

• The context where the Menendez et al study was carried out is included in the Discussion.

• The manuscript was carefully proof-read.

Reviewer 2:

• The question of gender difference in parent child conversation is an interesting question in itself. But in the current study, the difference in the number of girls and boys in the two samples is not statistically significant. Therefore, there is no reason to include gender as a control variable, especially considering the small sample size.

• We have included the information about parent age in the manuscript in order to describe the sample more. The sample demographics are now also organized in a table to provide a better overview. We originally also asked about household size, but something got lost in the translation, and we unfortunately do not have that data to provide.

Thank you very much for your valuable input and for helping us improve the quality of the manuscript!

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to reviewers .docx

Decision Letter 2

Sergio A Useche

19 Dec 2022

PONE-D-22-19799R2Making Sense of the Pandemic: Parent-Child Conversations in Two Cultural ContextsPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Tõugu,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process. Your paper has been re-reviewed. Although the comments received from our referees are overall positive, one of them asks for a couple of minor revisions. Could you please address them to the best of your ability and resubmit? If the amendments are outstanding, I will proceed to make an editorial decision without the need for a further round of reviews.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Feb 02 2023 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Sergio A. Useche, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: Partly

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: I appreciate the authors addressing the previous round of comments. Regarding the significant differences in parents' education between contexts - I don't see how you can add this to the limitations but not control for it in the regression analyses. It would make sense that the type and content of conversation would relate to education. If there were significant differences between the context on this variable, then comparing the contexts without taking this variable into consideration is problematic.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2023 Jan 23;18(1):e0280781. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0280781.r006

Author response to Decision Letter 2


28 Dec 2022

We would also like to thank the Reviewer for their suggestion. We have changed the manuscript as follows:

- We have taken into account the suggestion by Reviewer 1 and run the regression analyses with maternal education as a covariate. The results are provided in the manuscript. We also added a paragraph to the Discussion to refer to the results of these analyses.

Thank you for having our work reviewed!

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 3

Sergio A Useche

8 Jan 2023

Making Sense of the Pandemic: Parent-Child Conversations in Two Cultural Contexts

PONE-D-22-19799R3

Dear Dr. Tõugu,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Sergio A. Useche, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Thanks for the amendments made. The paper can now be accepted in its present form.

Acceptance letter

Sergio A Useche

13 Jan 2023

PONE-D-22-19799R3

Making Sense of the Pandemic: Parent-Child Conversations in Two Cultural Contexts

Dear Dr. Tõugu:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Sergio A. Useche

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 File. An anonymized dataset of parent-child conversations about COVID-19 in Estonia and Germany.

    (SAV)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to reviewers .docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES