Abstract
Neurorehabilitation relies on core principles of neuroplasticity to activate and engage latent neural connections, promote detour circuits, and reverse impairments. Clinical interventions incorporating these principles have been shown to promote recovery and demote compensation. However, many clinicians struggle to find interventions centered on these principles in our nascent, rapidly growing body of literature. Not to mention the immense pressure from regulatory bodies and organizational balance sheets that further discourage time-intensive recovery-promoting interventions, incentivizing clinicians to prioritize practical constraints over sound clinical decision making. Modern neurorehabilitation practices that result from these pressures favor strategies that encourage compensation over those that promote recovery. To narrow the gap between the busy clinician and the cutting-edge motor recovery literature, we distilled 5 features found in early-phase clinical intervention studies—ones that value the more enduring biological recovery processes over the more immediate compensatory remedies. Filtering emerging literature through this lens and routinely integrating promising research into daily practice can break down practical barriers for effective clinical translation and ultimately promote durable long-term outcomes. This perspective is meant to serve a new generation of mechanistically minded and caring clinicians, students, activists, and research trainees, who are poised to not only advance rehabilitation science, but also erect evidence-based policy changes to accelerate recovery-based stroke care.
Keywords: stroke recovery, arm and hand rehabilitation, clinical education, neuroplasticity
Introduction
The central tenet of neurorehabilitation is to promote better function through gradual biological and psychological recovery, returning impaired effectors and neural synapses to their premorbid levels of motor control and connectivity.1–3 However, the unreal demands of our health care regulatory bodies and balance sheets implicitly incentivize a speedier “compensatory” approach, one that focuses on the repurposing of seemingly unaffected effectors and neural synapses to achieve a minimum level of function.4–9,* Although the lure of rapid and obvious functional gains through a compensatory strategy may outweigh the gradual and subtle changes achieved through a recovery process, succumbing to a compensatory approach early in the rehabilitation progression may adversely hinder recovery later and moreover result in irreversible and negative functional outcomes (eg, learned nonuse, paretic limb contractures).10,11 Even though a compensatory approach is certainly essential when recovery is less likely and may serve as an intermediate step toward recovery in the early stages of rehabilitation,12 it should neither be the gold standard nor the first line of defense, especially in the acute periods after stroke.
Clinical research provides guidance on how neurorehabilitation could harness the biological and psychological processes of recovery. Systematic reviews, clinical practice guidelines, and large-scale multisite phase III randomized controlled trials (RCTs) enable well-powered statistical inferences, readily digestible even for the novice clinician. These larger-scale intervention studies are nonetheless difficult to come by, demanding years of toil from conceptualization to publication. Moreover, many of these multisite trials have yielded null results, proving no better than standard care.13,† Given the slow and often disappointing results from phase III RCTs, where should the clinician look for insights to continuously improve their neurorehabilitation practice?
Answer: early-phase clinical intervention studies. Robust clinical insights are not only present in systematic reviews, clinical practice guidelines, and large-scale multisite RCTs, but also found in early-phase clinical studies preceding large-scale RCTs.14,15,‡ Even though this literature may be relatively underpowered, early-phase clinical research that survives rigorous peer review gives rise to some of the most novel, insightful, and impactful ideas, holding the most promise to improve the lives of our patients today.
The dialogue on these frontiers between researchers and clinicians is nonetheless fraught with confusion. One primary source of confusion originates linguistically, with early-phase clinical studies riddled with (necessary) lingo, embedded in a scientific writing format demanding experience to efficiently parse.16,17 To encourage more effective dialogue, we distilled 5 features found in rigorous, early-phase, recovery-promoting neurorehabilitation intervention studies to provide busy clinicians with roadmaps to navigate the lingo and efficiently extract simple insights from dense articles (Table 1). A keen eye for these 5 features may not only sharpen the busy clinicians’ ability to efficiently uncover findings worthy of translation, but also inspire the researchers’ ability to design better interventions for motor recovery.
Table 1.
Number | Feature | Guiding questions |
---|---|---|
1 | Introduction: is grounded in basic science research | a. Does the gap in the literature extend from both basic and clinical research? b. Is the gap motivated by a diverse range of research ideas and techniques? c. What is the hypothesized mechanism for recovery? |
2 | Methods: rely on experience-dependent plasticity | a. Is there a close correspondence between the parameters chosen and those found in comparable larger-scale clinical trials working with this population? b. Is there a close correspondence between the parameters chosen and the hypothesized mechanism(s) proposed in the Introduction? c. Is there a close correspondence between the parameters chosen and those employed in your clinical experience working with this population? |
3 | Results: include impairment and participant-level measures | a. Were impairment-level and participant-level measures included? b. Were clinically meaningful improvements observed? c. Were the participants engaged, satisfied, and motivated? |
4 | Discussion: draws on converging evidence | a. Does your interpretation of the results align with those of the authors? b. Are you persuaded by the convergent or nonconvergent evidence put forth? c. Would you translate elements of this intervention into your clinical practice today? |
5 | The study, as a whole: considers the person’s perspective, perceptions, and readiness for rehabilitation | a. Was the study designed to be enriching and engaging? b. Was the intervention tailored for each participant in a meaningful way? c. Can insights gleaned from patient-reported outcome measures be used to augment this intervention? d. Does the intervention accelerate motor recovery and enhance a person’s well-being? e. Does the study describe socioeconomic, lifestyle, family support, occupational, compliance, and secondary medical factors that might facilitate or interfere with recovery? |
Feature 1: the Introduction Is Grounded in Basic Science Research
Promising clinical research finds its roots in basic science.18–20 With insights generated from behavioral neuroscience with animal models21–26 to cognitive neuroscience with human participants,21–23 basic science researchers can guide the direction of clinical research. Although the clinical application of these studies may not be immediately apparent, the history of science, in particular the fields of motor control and motor learning, have provided countless examples of ideas that have been successfully translated from the bench to the bedside.24–29 For instance, the recovery-promoting effects of rehabilitation in an enriched environment were first characterized in animal models,30,31 which subsequently springboarded the design of human intervention studies.32
Whether an early-phase clinical intervention is sufficiently motivated by basic science research can be assessed by reading the Introduction and supportive citations. Researchers often use the Introduction to identify the core “knowns” and gaps in the literature, providing a rationale behind why these gaps need to be filled. A mechanism33,§ is often put forth to motivate why an intervention could be effective. While reading these introductory sections, ask yourself whether these gaps naturally extend from both basic and clinical research and, moreover, whether these gaps are motivated by a confluence of research ideas and techniques (eg, systems neuroscience in animals, cognitive neuroscience in humans). Then, ask yourself whether the hypothesized mechanism logically extends previous research. Studies well-positioned at the intersection of basic and clinical research domains are likely those that assert well-reasoned hypotheses relevant to neurorehabilitation.
Feature 2: the Methods Rely on Experience-Dependent Plasticity
It is indisputable that repetition, timing, and challenge are vital ingredients to improve brain reorganization and functional outcomes.34 Repetition instantiates skill within the neural circuitry by increasing the number of synapses,35 altering synaptic weights,36–38 and pruning unnecessary synapses.39 A well-timed rehabilitation during sensitive periods, typically within the first 3 months poststroke, accelerates recovery.18,40,41 The amount of challenge, titrated by the amount of problem-solving required for task success, has also been shown to promote engagement as well as enhance subsequent motor recovery and motor learning.42–47 A study does not need to include all these elements of experience-dependent plasticity to make it impactful, but it should be philosophically grounded in these ideas.
Whether an early-phase clinical intervention is highly dosed, properly timed, and sufficiently challenging should be evident in the Methods. Ask yourself whether the therapeutic parameters outlined in this section are clearly detailed and show a close correspondence with the hypothesized mechanisms proposed in the Introduction. Furthermore, ask yourself whether the therapeutic parameters chosen are consistent with related research involving a similar demographic cohort (eg, stroke chronicity)48–51,‖ and whether these parameters jive with those proven efficacious in your own clinical practice.
Feature 3: the Results Include Impairment and Participant-Level Measures
Early-phase clinical studies often underscore improvements on activity-level measures (eg, dressing, eating). However, improvements on impairment-level measures (eg, individual joint control, coordination, strength, dexterity, movement quality, visuospatial reasoning, short-term memory, attention) should not be neglected because changes in these measures closely reflect the trajectory of cognitive and motor recovery (especially in the acute stage after stroke). For instance, improvements on the Fugl-Meyer Motor Assessment and Scandinavian Stroke Scale subscores, 2 motor impairment–level measures,52 are intimately linked to the time frame of neural repair.53,54 Importantly, participant-level measures should not be forgotten; these measures gauge an intervention’s impact on the individual—a critical ingredient for longer-lasting therapeutic and neurophysiological changes (see Feature 5).55–57
Whether a study includes a comprehensive battery of outcome measures should be evident in the Results. While reading this section, ask yourself whether the main findings encompassed impairment-level measures and, if so, whether any clinically meaningful improvements were observed.57 Furthermore, ask yourself whether the main findings included participant-level outcomes (eg, quality-of-life questionnaires, poststudy satisfaction surveys). If so, did participants find the intervention engaging and meaningful?
Feature 4: the Discussion Draws on Converging Evidence
Well-established clinical knowledge is supported by convergent lines of evidence. Different methods (eg, quantitative, qualitative), models (eg, animal, human), and levels of evidence (eg, case studies, systematic reviews, meta-analyses) should coalesce to support the study’s conclusions. For instance, constraint-induced movement therapy (CIMT), an intervention that involves functionally oriented task practice of the paretic upper extremity along with restraint of the less-impaired upper extremity, has efficaciously minimized many deleterious effects of learned nonuse in humans58,59 and in deafferented monkeys.43 In addition, the signature CIMT protocol or a modified version has been applied during the acute,49 subacute,60 and chronic58 periods after stroke. Evidence in favor of CIMT ranges from a very early case study61 to phase II49 and phase III RCTs,58 altogether forming an enriching but nonetheless controversial scientific discourse.20,62–66,¶ Although it is not a perfect form of therapy, the convergence of evidence to date suggests that some but not all of the components of CIMT are ripe for clinical translation.64,67
Whether a study’s findings are supported by converging lines of research should be evident in the Discussion, a place where the authors put forth their interpretation of the results. Ask yourself whether your interpretation jives with that of the authors? If there was a discrepancy, was it resolved with reference to supporting evidence from a diverse range of research domains (eg, brain imaging, animal work, observational studies) and multiple levels of verification (eg, case studies, systematic reviews)? Ultimately, a well-written Discussion should help you situate the study’s findings into a broader scientific context and with strong converging evidence (or a strong rationale for nonconverging evidence) may inspire you to translate these insights into your clinical practice.
Feature 5: the Study, as a Whole, Considers the Person’s Perspective, Perceptions, and Readiness for Rehabilitation
An intervention centered on principles of experience-dependent plasticity may promote recovery; however, its effectiveness may rely on the participant’s level of engagement and motivation for self-practice.68,69 Meaningful and individually tailored interventions, via goal setting sessions and simulations of real-life scenarios,70 may promote longer-lasting therapeutic improvements and sustained neurophysiological changes.50 Moreover, interventions that account for the person’s preferences, lifestyle, occupation, family support system, socioeconomic milieu, and personal well-being may further augment motor performance and learning.50,51,71–74 Although interventions that are meaningful and engaging have achieved similar,71 if not better, transfer of motor ability from the clinic to real-life settings,75 how these interventions can be used to reverse motor impairments beyond the outcomes achieved by conventional therapy remains to be seen.76,#
Although recovery in neurorehabilitation is complex and multifaceted, this process may be perfected through genuine collaboration between the person and the clinician-scientist.77,78 This collaborative spirit may be overtly hardwired into the study’s design, like for instance, with each participant receiving an individually tailored intervention plan in the participant’s home setting. This collaborative spirit may also be subtle, for instance, with the inclusion of satisfaction surveys that periodically solicit the participant’s opinion, level of interest, effort, and engagement in the intervention. Whether an intervention is designed to be collaborative may permeate through various sections of the article: As you read the Introduction, ask yourself whether the study’s intervention was designed to engage the participant in an enriching environment (Feature 1). As you read the Methods, ask yourself whether the study is designed to challenge the participant in a meaningful and individually tailored manner (Features 2 and 5)? When you read the Results, ask yourself whether important insights can be gleaned from patient-reported outcome measures or person-specific information (eg, socioeconomic, lifestyle, family support, occupational, compliance, secondary medical factors) that might facilitate or interfere with the intervention’s effectiveness for future participants (Feature 3). Finally, as you read the Discussion, ask yourself whether the converging evidence indicates that the intervention can be applied to not only accelerate motor recovery, but also enhance a person’s well-being (Feature 4).79,80
Conclusion
To narrow the gap between the busy clinician’s working knowledge and the cutting-edge motor recovery literature, we have distilled 5 features of early-phase neurorehabilitation research centered on recovery-based interventions. We submit these aspirational features to a new generation of mechanistically minded, caring clinicians who strive to advance their clinical practice as soon as the most promising evidence becomes available.
Funding
The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Footnotes
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Whereas motor recovery and compensation are introduced as distinct categories, they can be better conceptualized as a continuum.
In spite of the null results (eg, the experimental intervention not being superior to the control intervention), both interventions may have elicited similar beneficial effects on motor performance.13
Although early-phase clinical studies span a wide spectrum, they are broadly classified into 3 phases.14,15 Phase I clinical studies are designed to evaluate whether an intervention is safe. These studies are often inspired by animal work and case studies. Phase II clinical studies are designed to determine whether an intervention is effective. Phase III clinical studies are designed to confirm an intervention’s effectiveness, monitor side effects, and compare it with standard or similar treatments.
Mechanism of action is the means by which an intervention’s active ingredients (eg, the amount of weight support provided to the upper extremity) modifies a treatment target (eg, ability to individuate finger movements without eliciting an upper-extremity synergy pattern).33
The therapeutic parameters chosen often depend on a proposed mechanism of action, which varies widely with stroke chronicity.48 Dromerick et al,49 Winstein et al,50 and Ward et al,51 are 3 larger-scale studies that detail parameters chosen to maximize recovery in individuals in the acute, subacute, and chronic periods after stroke, respectively.49–51
The nature of improvements following CIMT vary widely as a function of stroke chronicity and specific outcome measure. CIMT applied during the chronic phase induced functional improvements mediated by compensatory strategies, with no real changes in impairment level measures, signifying recovery and restitution.62 There is also uncertainty about the mechanism of action,63 the persistence of therapeutic effects,58–60 and adherence to the signature CIMT protocol given its intense and “constrained” nature.64–66
Recently, and contrary to predictions, a small-scale randomized trial—comparing a novel neuroanimation experience with conventional therapy in the subacute stroke setting—found that the engaging, fun, and intensive intervention did not improve motor impairments beyond intensive conventional therapy.76
References
- 1.Kitago T, Krakauer JW. Motor learning principles for neurorehabilitation. Handb Clin Neurol. 2013;110:93–103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Bernhardt J, Hayward KS, Kwakkel G, et al. Agreed definitions and a shared vision for new standards in stroke recovery research: the Stroke Recovery and Rehabilitation Roundtable Taskforce. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2017;31:793–799. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Krakauer JW, Carmichael ST. Broken Movement: The Neurobiology of Motor Recovery After Stroke. MIT Press; 2017. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Cirstea MC, Levin MF. Compensatory strategies for reaching in stroke. Brain. 2000;123(pt 5):940–953. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.5.940 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Dobkin BH. Clinical practice: rehabilitation after stroke. N Engl J Med. 2005;352:1677–1684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Kalra L. The influence of stroke unit rehabilitation on functional recovery from stroke. Stroke. 1994;25:821–825. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Allred RP, Jones TA. Experience—a double edged sword for restorative neural plasticity after brain damage. Future Neurol. 2008;3:189–198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Hsu JE, Jones TA. Contralesional neural plasticity and functional changes in the less-affected forelimb after large and small cortical infarcts in rats. Exp Neurol. 2006;201:479–494. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Allred RP, Jones TA. Maladaptive effects of learning with the less-affected forelimb after focal cortical infarcts in rats. Exp Neurol. 2008;210:172–181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Taub E, Uswatte G, Mark VW, Morris DMM. The learned nonuse phenomenon: implications for rehabilitation. Eura Medicophys. 2006;42:241–256. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Fisher B, Woll S. Considerations in the restoration of motor control. In: Montgomery J, ed. Physical Therapy for Traumatic Brain Injury. Churchill Livingstone; 1995:55–78. [Google Scholar]
- 12.van Kordelaar J, van Wegen EEH, Nijland RHM, et al. Assessing longitudinal change in coordination of the paretic upper limb using on-site 3-dimensional kinematic measurements. Phys Ther. 2012;92:142–151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Stinear CM, Lang CE, Zeiler S, Byblow WD. Advances and challenges in stroke rehabilitation. Lancet Neurol. 2020;19:348–360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Dobkin BH. Progressive staging of pilot studies to improve phase III trials for motor interventions. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2009;23:197–206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.NIH Clinical Trials and You. The basics. Published May 14, 2015. Accessed September 11, 2020. https://www.nih.gov/health-information/nih-clinical-research-trials-you/basics [Google Scholar]
- 16.Hubbard KE, Dunbar SD. Perceptions of scientific research literature and strategies for reading papers depend on academic career stage. PLoS One. 2017;12:e0189753. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Hachinski V, Donnan GA, Gorelick PB, et al. Stroke: working toward a prioritized world agenda. Int J Stroke. 2010;5:238–256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Zeiler SR, Hubbard R, Gibson EM, et al. Paradoxical motor recovery from a first stroke after induction of a second stroke: reopening a postischemic sensitive period. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2016;30:794–800. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Nudo RJ, Jenkins WM, Merzenich MM, Prejean T, Grenda R. Neurophysiological correlates of hand preference in primary motor cortex of adult squirrel monkeys. J Neurosci. 1992;12:2918–2947. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Pons T, Garraghty P, Ommaya A, Kaas J, Taub E, Mishkin M. Massive cortical reorganization after sensory deafferentation in adult macaques. Science. 1991;252:1857–1860. doi: 10.1126/science.1843843 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Krakauer JW, Pine ZM, Ghilardi MF, Ghez C. Learning of visuomotor transformations for vectorial planning of reaching trajectories. J Neurosci. 2000;20:8916–8924. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Taylor JA, Ivry RB. Implicit and explicit processes in motor learning. In: Prinz W, Beisert M, Herwig A, eds. Action Science. MIT Press; 2013:63–87. [Google Scholar]
- 23.O’Shea J, Revol P, Cousijn H, et al. Induced sensorimotor cortex plasticity remediates chronic treatment-resistant visual neglect. Elife. 2017;6:e26602. doi: 10.7554/eLife.26602 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Ganguly K, Poo MM. Activity-dependent neural plasticity from bench to bedside. Neuron. 2013;80:729–741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Roemmich RT, Bastian AJ. Closing the loop: from motor neuroscience to neurorehabilitation. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2018;41:415–429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Reisman DS, Wityk R, Silver K, Bastian AJ. Locomotor adaptation on a split-belt treadmill can improve walking symmetry post-stroke. Brain. 2007;130(pt 7):1861–1872. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Taub E, Uswatte G, Elbert T. New treatments in neurorehabilitation founded on basic research. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:228–236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Corbett D, Carmichael ST, Murphy TH, et al. Enhancing the alignment of the preclinical and clinical stroke recovery research pipeline: consensus-based core recommendations from the Stroke Recovery and Rehabilitation Roundtable translational working group. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2017;31:699–707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Winstein C, Lewthwaite R, Blanton SR, Wolf LB, Wishart L. Infusing motor learning research into neurorehabilitation practice: a historical perspective with case exemplar from the accelerated skill acquisition program. J Neurol Phys Ther. 2014;38:190–200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Biernaskie J, Corbett D. Enriched rehabilitative training promotes improved forelimb motor function and enhanced dendritic growth after focal ischemic injury. J Neurosci. 2001;21:5272–5280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Nithianantharajah J, Hannan AJ. Enriched environments, experience-dependent plasticity and disorders of the nervous system. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7:697–709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Rosbergen ICM, Grimley RS, Hayward KS, et al. Embedding an enriched environment in an acute stroke unit increases activity in people with stroke: a controlled before-after pilot study. Clin Rehabil. 2017;31:1516–1528. doi: 10.1177/0269215517705181 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Whyte J, Dijkers MP, Hart T, et al. Development of a theory-driven rehabilitation treatment taxonomy: conceptual issues. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2014;95(1, suppl):S24–S32.e2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Kleim JA, Jones TA. Principles of experience-dependent neural plasticity: implications for rehabilitation after brain damage. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2008;51:S225–S239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Kleim JA, Hogg TM, VandenBerg PM, Cooper NR, Bruneau R, Remple M. Cortical synaptogenesis and motor map reorganization occur during late, but not early, phase of motor skill learning. J Neurosci. 2004;24:628–633. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Xu T, Yu X, Perlik AJ, et al. Rapid formation and selective stabilization of synapses for enduring motor memories. Nature. 2009;462:915–919. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Rioult-Pedotti MS, Friedman D, Donoghue JP. Learning-induced LTP in neocortex. Science. 2000;290:533–536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Harms KJ, Rioult-Pedotti MS, Carter DR, Dunaevsky A. Transient spine expansion and learning-induced plasticity in layer 1 primary motor cortex. J Neurosci. 2008;28:5686–5690. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Sanes JN, Donoghue JP. Plasticity and primary motor cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000;23:393–415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Krakauer JW, Carmichael ST, Corbett D, Wittenberg GF. Getting neurorehabilitation right: what can be learned from animal models? Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2012;26:923–931. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Zeiler SR, Krakauer JW. The interaction between training and plasticity in the poststroke brain. Curr Opin Neurol. 2013;26:609–616. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Guadagnoli MA, Lee TD. Challenge point: a framework for conceptualizing the effects of various practice conditions in motor learning. J Mot Behav. 2004;36:212–224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Onla-or S, Winstein CJ. Determining the optimal challenge point for motor skill learning in adults with moderately severe Parkinson’s disease. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2008;22:385–395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Pollock CL, Boyd LA, Hunt MA, Garland SJ. Use of the challenge point framework to guide motor learning of stepping reactions for improved balance control in people with stroke: a case series. Phys Ther. 2014;94:562–570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Lotay R, Mace M, Rinne P, Burdet E, Bentley P. Optimizing self-exercise scheduling in motor stroke using Challenge Point Framework theory. IEEE Int Conf Rehabil Robot. 2019;2019:435–440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Kleim JA, Barbay S, Nudo RJ. Functional reorganization of the rat motor cortex following motor skill learning. J Neurophysiol. 1998;80:3321–3325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Ellis MD, Sukal-Moulton T, Dewald JPA. Progressive shoulder abduction loading is a crucial element of arm rehabilitation in chronic stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2009;23:862–869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Xu J, Branscheidt M, Schambra H, et al. Rethinking interhemispheric imbalance as a target for stroke neurorehabilitation. Ann Neurol. 2019;85:502–513. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Dromerick AW, Lang CE, Birkenmeier RL, et al. Very Early Constraint-Induced Movement during Stroke Rehabilitation (VECTORS): a single-center RCT. Neurology. 2009;73:195–201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Winstein CJ, Wolf SL, Dromerick AW, et al. Effect of a task-oriented rehabilitation program on upper extremity recovery following motor stroke: the ICARE randomized clinical trial. JAMA. 2016;315:571–581. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Ward NS, Brander F, Kelly K. Intensive upper limb neurorehabilitation in chronic stroke: outcomes from the Queen Square programme. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2019;90:498–506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Kwakkel G, Van Wegen EEH, Burridge JH, et al. Standardized measurement of quality of upper limb movement after stroke: consensus-based core recommendations from the Second Stroke Recovery and Rehabilitation Roundtable. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2019;33:951–958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Duncan PW, Goldstein LB, Matchar D, Divine GW, Feussner J. Measurement of motor recovery after stroke: outcome assessment and sample size requirements. Stroke. 1992;23:1084–1089. doi: 10.1161/01.str.23.8.1084 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Nakayama H, Jørgensen HS, Raaschou HO, Olsen TS. Recovery of upper extremity function in stroke patients: the Copenhagen Stroke Study. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1994;75:394–398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Forkan R, Pumper B, Smyth N, Wirkkala H, Ciol MA, Shumway-Cook A. Exercise adherence following physical therapy intervention in older adults with impaired balance. Phys Ther. 2006;86:401–410. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Grönstedt H, Frändin K, Bergland A, et al. Effects of individually tailored physical and daily activities in nursing home residents on activities of daily living, physical performance and physical activity level: a randomized controlled trial. Gerontology. 2013;59:220–229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Morley JE, Philpot CD, Gill D, Berg-Weger M. Meaningful activities in the nursing home. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2014;15:79–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Wolf SL, Winstein CJ, Miller JP, et al. Effect of constraint-induced movement therapy on upper extremity function 3 to 9 months after stroke: the EXCITE randomized clinical trial. JAMA. 2006;296:2095–2104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Taub E, Morris DM. Constraint-induced movement therapy to enhance recovery after stroke. Curr Atheroscler Rep. 2001;3:279–286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Page SJ, Sisto S, Johnston MV, Levine P. Modified constraint-induced therapy after subacute stroke: a preliminary study. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2002;16:290–295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Ostendorf CG, Wolf SL. Effect of forced use of the upper extremity of a hemiplegic patient on changes in function: a single-case design. Phys Ther. 1981;61:1022–1028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Kitago T, Liang J, Huang VS, et al. Improvement after constraint-induced movement therapy: recovery of normal motor control or task-specific compensation? Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2013;27:99–109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Gauthier LV, Taub E, Perkins C, Ortmann M, Mark VW, Uswatte G. Remodeling the brain. Stroke. 2008;39:1520–1525. doi: 10.1161/strokeaha.107.502229 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Pedlow K, Lennon S, Wilson C. Application of constraint-induced movement therapy in clinical practice: an online survey. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2014;95:276–282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Page SJ, Levine P, Sisto S, Bond Q, Johnston MV. Stroke patients’ and therapists’ opinions of constraint-induced movement therapy. Clin Rehabil. 2002;16:55–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Wolf SL. Revisiting constraint-induced movement therapy: are we too smitten with the mitten? Is all nonuse “learned?” and other quandaries. Phys Ther. 2007;87:1212–1223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Winstein C, Wolf SL, Schweighofer N. Task-oriented training to promote upper extremity recovery. In: Stein J, Harvey R, Winstein C, Zorowit R, Wittenberg G, eds. Stroke Recovery and Rehabilitation. 2nd ed. Demos Medical; 2014;597–636. [Google Scholar]
- 68.Winstein C, Varghese R. Been there, done that, so what’s next for arm and hand rehabilitation in stroke? NeuroRehabilitation. 2018;43:3–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Wang C, Winstein C, D’Argenio DZ, Schweighofer N. The efficiency, efficacy, and retention of task practice in chronic stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2020;34:881–890. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Barker RN, Gill TJ, Brauer SG. Factors contributing to upper limb recovery after stroke: a survey of stroke survivors in Queensland Australia. Disabil Rehabil. 2007;29:981–989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.van Vliet PM, Wulf G. Extrinsic feedback for motor learning after stroke: what is the evidence? Disabil Rehabil. 2006;28:831–840. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Wulf G, Lewthwaite R. Optimizing performance through intrinsic motivation and attention for learning: the OPTIMAL theory of motor learning. Psychon Bull Rev. 2016;23:1382–1414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Kapral MK, Wang H, Mamdani M, Tu JV. Effect of socioeconomic status on treatment and mortality after stroke. Stroke. 2002;33:268–273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Cox AM, McKevitt C, Rudd AG, Wolfe CDA. Socioeconomic status and stroke. Lancet Neurol. 2006;5:181–188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Lewthwaite R, Winstein CJ, Lane CJ, et al. Accelerating stroke recovery: body structures and functions, activities, participation, and quality of life outcomes from a large rehabilitation trial. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2018;32:150–165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Krakauer JW, Kitago T, Goldsmith J, et al. Comparing a novel neuroanimation experience to conventional therapy for high-dose, intensive upper-limb training in subacute stroke: the SMARTS2 randomized trial. bioRxiv. Published online August 7, 2020. doi: 10.1101/2020.08.04.20152538 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Committee on Quality of Health Care in America, Institute of Medicine. Crossing the Quality Chasm: A New Health System for the 21st Century. National Academies Press; 2001. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Winstein C. Thoughts about the negative results of clinical trials in rehabilitation medicine. Kinesiol Rev (Champaign). 2018;7:58–63. [Google Scholar]
- 79.Mead N, Bower P. Patient-centredness: a conceptual framework and review of the empirical literature. Soc Sci Med. 2000;51:1087–1110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Rosenbaum L. The whole ball game—overcoming the blind spots in health care reform. N Engl J Med. 2013;368:959–962. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]