Skip to main content
Frontiers in Psychology logoLink to Frontiers in Psychology
. 2023 Jan 13;13:1065598. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1065598

Resting frontal alpha asymmetry as a predictor of executive and affective functioning in children with neurodevelopmental differences

Sarah R Edmunds 1,2,*, Jason Fogler 2,3,4, Yael Braverman 2, Rachel Gilbert 5, Susan Faja 2,3
PMCID: PMC9880425  PMID: 36710763

Abstract

The relative difference of resting EEG frontal alpha activation between left and right hemispheres (FAA; i.e., asymmetry) correlates with global approach and avoidance tendencies. FAA may relate to problems with executive and affective functioning in children with neurodevelopmental differences, including autism and ADHD. We (1) characterize relative left vs. right FAA in autistic, ADHD, and neurotypical children (NT) and (2) investigate whether FAA predicts “hot” executive function or emotion dysregulation. Participants were 97 7- to 11-year-old autistic, ADHD, and NT Children. Children with ADHD displayed greater left (relative to right) FAA compared to autistic and neurotypical children. Children with ADHD displayed greater challenges with “hot” EF on a gambling task than autistic children, whereas children with co-occurring autism and ADHD had greater parent-reported emotion dysregulation than NT and autism-only groups. Greater left FAA predicted worse hot EF for all children but was not significantly related to emotion dysregulation. Regardless of clinical diagnosis, relatively greater left FAA relates to hot EF. While hot EF deficits may be specific to ADHD rather than autism, both together confer additive risk for emotion dysregulation. Future research should explore the functional relation between FAA, reward processing, and affect for children with different EF-related neurodevelopmental differences.

Keywords: autism, executive function, emotion regulation, EEG, asymmetry, ADHD, neurodevelopment

Introduction

Identifying early childhood risk factors of later functional impairment can inform prevention intervention efforts during children’s early years, which represent a period of increased neuroplasticity (Wakschlag et al., 2019). Increasingly, the National Institute of Mental Health’s Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) perspective encourages a focus on dimensional risk that spans traditional diagnostic categories, can be measured at multiple levels, and explains functional impairment (Insel et al., 2010). For example, while one might characterize neurodevelopmental disorders as discrete diagnostic categories [i.e., attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD); autism spectrum disorder (ASD)], children with neurodevelopmental disorders may be more dimensionally characterized as having challenges with impulsivity, hyperactivity, social interaction, and/or emotion dysregulation.

Emotional dysregulation is a powerful predictor of mental-health-related functional impairment (e.g., Mazefsky et al., 2013; Graziano and Garcia, 2016). Emotional regulation is the ability to change or calm one’s emotional state and involves the dual development of both bottom-up approach/avoidance reactivity and top-down executive control (Zelazo and Carlson, 2012; Grabell et al., 2017; Wakschlag et al., 2018). Some emotion regulation ability may be inexorably linked to trait-based emotion reactivity, whereas other emotion strategies are cognitive in nature and are more malleable (Jahromi et al., 2012). Both ADHD and ASD are associated with increased emotion regulation challenges (Shaw et al., 2014; Conner et al., 2021; Day et al., 2022), but the mechanisms by which this is the case are unclear. In ADHD, reduced top-down executive control may contribute to increased emotion dysregulation (Shaw et al., 2014). In ASD, decreased emotion regulation may mechanistically related to ASD characteristics in part due to reduced brain activation during cognitive appraisal of emotive faces (Richey et al., 2015).

Frontal alpha EEG asymmetry is one long-studied physiological index of emotional reactivity within the RDoC’s Arousal and Regulatory Systems domain (Wheeler et al., 1993; Coan and Allen, 2004; Gatzke-Kopp et al., 2014). Brain activity in the alpha frequency band reflects individual differences in awareness and attention (Hanslmayr et al., 2011), and may play a role in both emotion reactivity and regulation (Wheeler et al., 1993). Frontal alpha asymmetry (FAA) is operationalized as the relative difference in frontal alpha activation between left and right hemispheres and is thought to reflect approach (left) and avoidance (right) impulses. FAA measured during a resting state is viewed as a temperament or “trait” measure of approach/avoidance emotional reactivity. Resting FAA predicts clinically significant individual differences in internalizing and externalizing symptomatology in children (Baving et al., 2002; Blackhart et al., 2006; Gatzke-Kopp et al., 2014).

However, FAA may more accurately index the top-down executive control aspect of emotion regulation than bottom-up emotion reactivity. The asymmetric inhibition model (Grimshaw and Carmel, 2014) explains the relation between FAA and the executive function-dependent aspects of emotion regulation. They cite EEG studies using source localization procedures to highlight that FAA may most directly reflect the activity of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dl-PFC). The dl-PFC is centrally involved in executive functioning, including behavioral inhibition, and is active during the cognitive appraisal of emotion. According to the model, the left dl-PFC acts to inhibit avoidance-based motivational urges as they distract from one’s intentional goals, while the right dl-PFC acts to inhibit approach-based motivation as they distract from one’s intentional goals. In this manner, relatively lower left (and therefore, higher right) FAA reflects poorer executive control of internalizing emotions like sadness and anxiety, while relatively lower right (and therefore, higher left) FAA reflects poorer executive control of externalizing, approach-oriented emotions like anger and joy. There is ample literature to reflect this left-externalizing vs. right-internalizing FAA pattern (see Grimshaw and Carmel, 2014 for a review).

FAA may contribute differentially to cascading clinically impairing challenges with affective regulation in children with different neurodevelopmental disorders. At least 50% of autistic children and 33% of children with ADHD present with co-occurrence of anxiety and depression, including 15–20% with a mood disorder by preschool or elementary school (Salazar et al., 2015; Schendel et al., 2016). Executive function (EF) deficits are highly linked to the behavioral regulation of emotion in both ASD and in all children (Demetriou et al., 2019). EF is a core deficit in ADHD (Seidman, 2006) and linked to emotion dysregulation in these children (Garcia et al., 2020). Autistic children also have less developed executive function (Demetriou et al., 2019), including delay of gratification (Faja and Dawson, 2013), a skill that involves the top-down inhibition of a prepotent response in the “hot” or emotionally valenced context of reward.

There is clear evidence of diagnostic group differences in resting FAA for ADHD, but findings are mixed for autism. Children with ADHD demonstrated greater relative left FAA during a behavioral inhibition task than did NT children (Ellis et al., 2017), which suggests that they have higher approach motivation or less inhibited avoidance motivation than NT children. Similarly, adults with ADHD have greater relative left FAA than NT adults (e.g., Keune et al., 2015). In autism, a study of 6- to 18-month-olds at higher likelihood for ASD by virtue of having an older sibling with ASD found that these infants had relatively greater right FAA than “low likelihood” infants at 6 months, but that this pattern faded by 12 months (Gabard-Durnam et al., 2015). Children aged 9–14 years with ASD and no intellectual impairment were found to have relatively greater left midfrontal activation than typically developing children (Sutton et al., 2005). A similar study of 8- to 15-year-old children with ASD and below average to superior verbal IQ found no diagnostic group differences in the number of children with relative left versus right FAA, measured dichotomously (Burnette et al., 2011). To summarize, group differences in FAA are complicated by measurement (i.e., dichotomous vs. continuous asymmetry; lateral vs. midfrontal) and cognitive ability. Inconsistent findings also potentially strengthen the hypothesis that FAA may better reflect transdiagnostic characteristics than diagnostic categories.

It appears that FAA predicts emotion dysregulation for children in general, but it is unclear whether diagnostic group (autism, ADHD) moderates the strength of this relationship. For all children, emerging evidence supports the relation between relatively greater resting right FAA and broad measurements of later internalizing symptoms (Diego et al., 2006; Smith and Bell, 2010) as well as relatively greater resting left FAA and later externalizing symptoms (Smith and Bell, 2010). There is mixed evidence as to whether the relation between FAA and emotion dysregulation differs for autistic children compared to neurotypical (NT) children or children with ADHD. In a study of autistic adolescents, Schiltz et al. (2018) found that individuals with relatively greater right FAA had greater anxiety symptoms, which is consistent with previous literature on FAA and anxiety in the general population. However, in the two studies of FAA in slightly younger children with ASD and NT development, the relation between asymmetry and internalizing symptoms in ASD is less clear. Specifically, for their ASD samples, both studies found that increased relative left FAA was correlated with greater general anxiety (Sutton et al., 2005) and higher OCD and anger-related symptoms (Burnette et al., 2011), which is opposite what would be expected. Finally, while left FAA may predict behavioral inhibition in ADHD (Ellis et al., 2017), there has been little research on whether FAA predicts EF in emotionally valenced contexts (i.e., “hot” EF) in autism.

The current study

Understanding diagnostic group and transdiagnostic differences in FAA may help the field better understand heterogeneity in autism as well as co-occurring mental-health challenges (Schiltz et al., 2018). If FAA either differs by diagnostic classification or predicts individual differences in executive functioning or emotion dysregulation, it would represent a risk factor that is easily measurable at an early age, may identify subgroups of children, and may predict differential responses to intervention. The goals of this study were to:

  1. Characterize relative left vs. right frontal alpha activation (FAA) in children with autism, ADHD, co-occurring autism and ADHD, and neurotypical development (NT). Specifically, we hypothesized that children with ADHD will show greater relative left FAA than other children. We did not make a directional hypothesis about FAA in autistic children because previous findings are mixed.

  2. Investigate the extent to which children’s FAA predicts “hot” EF during a behavioral task. Because hot EF tasks involve approach/avoidance decision-making within an emotionally valenced environment, we predicted that FAA will be related to hot EF for all children regardless of diagnosis.

  3. Relate children’s FAA to parent-report measures of their emotion dysregulation. We predicted that FAA will be related to emotion dysregulation, given prior work. We hypothesized that this relation will be stronger for children with ADHD than autism, given the more consistent evidence of a relation between FAA and mental-health challenges in ADHD compared to autism.

Materials and methods

Participants

Participants were 97 7- to 11-year-old children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD; n = 29), attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; n = 27), co-occurring autism and ADHD (n = 16), and neurotypical development (NT; n = 25). Participants were 8.30–8.93 years old on average, 74–93% male (n = 82), and 67–84% White (depending on group). Demographic characteristics are reported in Supplementary Table 1. Participants were recruited through a research registry, clinical referrals, community sources, and word of mouth. Exclusion criteria included colorblindness, inability to complete procedures in English, below-average cognitive ability, sensory or motor impairments that impeded ability to complete the test battery, medical disorders or medications that impact the central nervous system, history of seizures or use of seizure medication, and prolonged prenatal substance exposure. Other medication use (stimulant and non-stimulant) was non-exclusionary and did not differ by group (see Faja et al., 2021). The study was conducted at a hospital in New England and approved by its Human Subjects Division. All parents provided written consent and children provided written assent to participate.

This sample size allows for the detection of medium to large effect sizes with the ANCOVA (one covariate) and regression approaches planned for this study. Prior literature supports the expectation of medium to large effect sizes (e.g., Sutton et al., 2005; Ellis et al., 2017).

Procedure

Parents of participants completed a phone screening to establish eligibility. Diagnostic status and cognitive ability were assessed during the first visit, and findings were supervised and reviewed by a licensed psychologist. Over two additional visits, all participants completed a battery of executive function (EF) and social cognition tasks while parents completed questionnaires about children’s behavior and functioning. A subset of participants completed additional visits for an intervention study; only baseline data are considered here.

Measures

Participants in the present study completed clinical characterization, EEG resting data collection, and the Hungry Donkey gambling task (see below). Participants’ parents then completed a questionnaire about their children’s behavioral and emotional functioning.

Clinical characterization

All participants completed assessments of cognitive and adaptive functioning using the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Second edition (Vineland-2; Sparrow et al., 2005), and the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence-2 (WASI-2; Wechsler, 2011), which also assured verbal ability.

All children in the ASD group had an existing diagnosis of ASD. Diagnosis was confirmed according to DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) criteria based on expert clinical judgment, the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R; Rutter et al., 2003), and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, second edition (ADOS-2; Lord et al., 2012). ADHD symptoms were assessed as a continuous variable, using the ADHD subscale of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach and Rescorla, 2001). There is little agreement on how to quantify ADHD symptoms in autistic children; as such, a T-score of 65 was used as a cut-point to identify children within a borderline clinical range of ADHD for the ADHD group (Achenbach and Rescorla, 2001), in line with other work (Andersen et al., 2013; Cremone-Caira et al., 2021). Children with a confirmed autism diagnosis and T-scores ≥65 on the CBCL ADHD subscale were determined to have clinically significant ADHD symptoms and were included in the ASD + ADHD group.

Resting EEG data collection

Participants completed 2 minutes of alternating eyes open, eyes closed resting EEG data collection. During eyes open periods, children were asked to fixate on a central cross on a screen directly in front of them. Neural responses were continuously recorded via a Net Amps 400 (Electrical Geodesics, Inc.) using the 128-channel HydroCel sensor net 2.0 (HSN).

Resting EEG data preprocessing

EEG data preprocessing and power analyses were conducted using the Batch EEG Automated Processing Platform (BEAPP; Levin et al., 2018). Data were first bandpass filtered using a high pass (1 Hz) and low pass (100 Hz) filter and then downsampled from 500 to 250 Hz.

Next, data were artifact detected and corrected using the Harvard Automated Preprocessing Pipeline for EEG (HAPPE), a pipeline optimized for short recording EEG data collected from young children with neurodevelopmental disorders (Gabard-Durnam et al., 2018). HAPPE first applied linenoise removal to the data at 60 Hz, and next performed detection and removal of bad channels. Artifacts in the data, such as eyeblinks and eye or muscle movements, were automatically detected using first wavelet-enhanced independent component analysis (w-ICA) and then ICA with the multiple artifact rejection algorithm (MARA; Winkler et al., 2011, 2014). The following channels, in addition to the 10–20 electrodes, were used for ICA with MARA: 34, 28, 16, 1, 47, 51, 37, 60, 72, 30, 6, 117, 105, 116, 32, 98, 97, 85, 87, and 75. Electrodes were spread evenly across the scalp, and the number of electrodes was chosen relative to our recording length to maximize ICA performance and prevent overfitting of the algorithm (Särelä and Vigário, 2003). Following artifact removal, bad channels were interpolated and each channel was re-referenced to the average of all channels.

Data were then segmented into 2 s windows. Segments were inspected again for artifact and segments with an amplitude greater than 40 μV were not included in the final data analysis. A 40 μV cutoff reflects the smaller amplitude that results from the wavelet thresholding and ICA steps during artifact detection (Gabard-Durnam et al., 2018).

EEG power analysis

Using a fast Fourier transform with a 1 s Hanning Window, we computed the power spectrum for each segment for all electrodes: For each electrode, power across all segments was averaged for the alpha frequency band (8–13 Hz). Data within the alpha frequency band (8–13 Hz) from electrodes indexing left and right frontal activation (F3 and F4) were filtered and processed using BEAPP (Levin et al., 2018).

Left and right relative activation

The alpha frequency band (8–13 Hz) from electrodes indexing left and right frontal activation (F3 and F4) was extracted. F3 and F4 were selected to align with the convention in this literature, which allows for more direct comparison (Coan and Allen, 2004; Sutton et al., 2005; Reznik and Allen, 2018). Absolute power (mean power per hertz) was derived. Alpha power reflects the inverse of activation. Asymmetry in children’s FAA was calculated using residual values (Coan and Allen, 2004; Reznik and Allen, 2018). Residual values are more sensitive than traditional difference scores (Meyer et al., 2017). Left residual power values were residual values from a model of right power predicting left power (i.e., left power, controlling for right). Right residual power values were residual values from a model of right power, controlling for left. The inverse of each value was taken (i.e., the inverse of power is activation). Each variable (right and left residual) therefore represents unique frontal hemispheric activation. No significant difference in FAA by gender was found, p = 0.92.

Hungry donkey task: “Hot” executive function

The Hungry Donkey gambling task (Supplementary Figure 2) assesses children’s strategic long-term decision-making in the heightened emotional context of reward and is conceptualized here as a “hot” executive function (EF) measure (Crone and van der Molen, 2004). Children fed a cartoon “hungry” donkey by selecting freely from one of four doors across a total of five blocks, with 20 trials per block. Two doors were advantageous and resulted in a net gain (lesser vs. greater) of apples. Two doors were disadvantageous and resulted in a net loss (lesser vs. greater) of apples. This was therefore a reward contingency learning task that pitted immediate rewards against a net reward over time (on advantageous doors).

Contingency reward learning was operationalized as the ratio of advantageous to disadvantageous decisions made across the 20 trials of the final block (once children had a chance to learn door contingencies). For example, a score of −20 would indicate that a child made all disadvantageous decisions for the final block.

Parent questionnaire of emotion dysregulation

Participants’ caregivers completed the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach and Rescorla, 2001), from which the 18-item Emotion Dysregulation Index (EDI) was derived (Samson et al., 2014). The CBCL is a normed parent-report questionnaire that assesses behavioral and emotional functioning. The EDI is an 18-item scale derived from CBCL items across subscales that indexes transdiagnostic emotion dysregulation and has been validated with autistic children (Samson et al., 2014; Berkovits et al., 2017).

Results

Relative frontal alpha activation differs by diagnostic group

A one-way ANCOVA was conducted and found a trend toward a statistically significant difference between diagnostic groups on FAA, controlling for WASI-2 IQ, F(3, 96) = 2.45, p = 0.057, partial η2 = 0.07. Planned least significant difference (LSD)-corrected post-hoc pairwise comparisons of adjusted means revealed that children with ADHD displayed greater left (relative to right) FAA compared to both autistic, p = 0.020, and neurotypical children, p = 0.022, but not children with ASD + ADHD, p = 0.234 (Figure 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Group differences in left frontal alpha activation (relative to right). Children with ADHD displayed greater left (relative to right) FAA compared to both autistic, p = 0.020, and neurotypical children, p = 0.022, but not children with ASD + ADHD, p = 0.234. *p < 0.05. Error bars are +/− one standard error.

FAA predicts “hot” executive function

A one-way ANCOVA found a statistically significant difference between diagnostic groups on contingency reward learning during the Hungry Donkey task, controlling for WASI-2 IQ, F(3, 82) = 2.97, p = 0.037, partial η2 = 0.10. Post-hoc pairwise comparisons of adjusted means revealed that children with ADHD displayed greater challenges with “hot” EF than did autistic children, p = 0.009 (Figure 2). Other post-hoc comparisons were non-significant.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Group differences in reward contingency learning on the hungry donkey task. Children with ADHD displayed greater challenges with “hot” EF than did autistic children, p = 0.009. *p < 0.05. Error bars are +/− one standard error.

A multiple regression model controlling for IQ revealed that for all children, higher relative left FAA predicted worse reward contingency learning, β = 0.32, p = 0.002 (Figure 3). Moderation by group was non-significant, ps > 0.05.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Higher relative left (vs. right) FAA predicts less advantageous decision-making across diagnostic groups. For all children, higher relative left FAA predicted worse reward contingency learning, β = 0.32, p = 0.002.

FAA does not significantly predict emotion dysregulation

A one-way ANCOVA revealed a statistically significant difference between diagnostic groups on CBCL Emotion Dysregulation Index (EDI) score, F(3, 92) = 11.91, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.28. Of note, an additional 40 children who had caregiver-report data only were available for this analysis; results do not change when including these participants [F(3, 136) = 21.86, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.33; NT n = 33; ASD n = 46; ADHD n = 36; ASD + ADHD n = 26]. Post-hoc pairwise comparisons of adjusted means indicated that children with co-occurring autism and ADHD had greater caregiver-reported emotion dysregulation than NT children, p = <0.001, and children with autism alone p < 0.001 (Figure 4).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Diagnostic group differences in emotion dysregulation. Children with co-occurring autism and ADHD had greater caregiver-reported emotion dysregulation than NT children, p = <0.001, and children with autism alone p < 0.001. FAA was not significantly predictive of emotion dysregulation. *p < 0.05. Error bars are +/− one standard error. Difference between ASD + ADHD and ADHD groups trends toward significance (p = 0.06).

A multiple regression model controlling for IQ revealed that for the full sample, FAA was not significantly related to parent-reported emotion regulation, β = 0.15, p = 0.11. Moderation by diagnostic group was also non-significant, ps > 0.05.

Discussion

Children with ADHD displayed greater left (relative to right) frontal alpha activation (FAA) compared to both autistic and neurotypical children. While children with ADHD displayed greater challenges with “hot” EF (reward contingency learning) than did autistic children, children with co-occurring autism and ADHD were reported to have greater emotion regulation challenges than neurotypical (NT) children and children with autism alone. Higher relative left FAA predicted worse reward contingency learning for all children, but it was not significantly related to parent-reported emotion dysregulation. The strength of these relations did not differ by diagnostic group.

Our hypothesis that children with ADHD would display greater left (relative to right) FAA compared to both autistic and neurotypical children was supported and is consistent with the literature (e.g., Keune et al., 2015; Ellis et al., 2017). However, we did not find an omnibus effect or any differences for autistic individuals compared to other groups. There is additional evidence that neurodevelopmental group differences in FAA may be moderated by both participant characteristics and task context. For example, researchers found a gender difference such that 4- to 8-year-old males with ADHD had relatively greater resting left FAA than NT males, while females with ADHD had relatively greater right FAA than NT females (Baving et al., 1999). Compared to NT children, 7- to 14-year-old children with ADHD had relatively greater left FAA during failed trials of a go/no-go task, a measure of inhibition, but not during other trials, although FAA was not measured at rest (Ellis et al., 2017).

We found that children’s relative left FAA was related to their executive function in a “hot,” emotionally valenced context. We also found that reward contingency learning scores during the Hungry Donkey task were worse for children with ADHD compared to all other groups, suggesting that this type of hot EF may be an ADHD-specific deficit. However, the relation between greater relative left FAA and hot EF performance held across all children. In other words, relatively greater approach motivation (or a lack of adaptive inhibition to avoidance) relates to reward contingency learning, regardless of neurodevelopmental diagnosis. This suggests that children who are better at inhibiting the extent to which their emotional reactivity affects their behavior may make better long-term decisions “under pressure.” That this finding is not specific to—or stronger for—children with ADHD, but rather extends to all children, suggests one of two things: that the effect of EEG-indexed emotional reactivity on in-the-moment EF behaviors is transdiagnostic, or that its effect is stronger and more detectable in relation to behavioral tasks than to more global or parent-report measures. For example, He et al. (2010) found that infants’ relative left FAA (i.e., increased approach motivation) was associated with increased anger via a behavioral observation measure (rather than parent-report measure). This is one of the few studies in children to demonstrate a link between frontal asymmetry and behaviorally measured action urges in emotionally valenced situations.

We observed differing levels of emotion dysregulation by diagnostic group, but FAA appears to be unrelated to emotion dysregulation in our sample. In contrast, Richey et al. (2015) found that for adults with ASD, activation in the dl-PFC (the same area that is theorized to be indexed by EEG FAA) indexed via fMRI was reduced when viewing emotive faces, compared to neurotypical adults. Therefore, while we found evidence for a neural mechanism (i.e., FAA) by which approach motivation may be related to reward contingency learning, we do not find a neural mechanism for emotion dysregulation. We also do not find group differences in FAA levels or the relation between FAA and our affective variables of interest, suggesting that FAA-based neural mechanisms may be transdiagnostic.

Children with co-occurring autism and ADHD had the highest reported challenges with emotion regulation, pointing toward an additive effect of neurodevelopmental differences on outward signs of emotion regulation. This kind of additive effect has rarely been observed (England-Mason, 2020). That FAA does not significantly predict or underpin these diagnostic differences in emotion dysregulation may point toward diagnosis-specific functional pathways for emotion dysregulation. For example, individuals with ASD but relatively low symptom severity may have increased internalizing or externalizing symptomatology as a result of the effort needed to “compensate” for social communication challenges inherent to the disorder (Livingston et al., 2019). While for children with ADHD, emotional reactivity may be more directly linked with impulsivity-related EF deficits, and some researchers even posit an “irritable subtype” of ADHD, for which emotion regulation challenges are central (Karalunas et al., 2019). More research is needed to explore these possibilities.

Limitations and future directions

Limitations of this study include its cross-sectional design, lack of measurement of other potential covariates beyond IQ (e.g., social communication, baseline negative affect, cold EF), relatively small sample size, and reliance on caregiver report for emotion dysregulation and ADHD classification. While FAA, as a brain-based measure, is conceptualized as a more proximal influence on EF and emotion regulation than behavioral indexes of these variables, this study could draw stronger conclusions if children were studied longitudinally and at younger ages, when EF and emotion regulation first develop. To better understand the relation between FAA, reward processing, and affective functioning, future work should seek to understand whether the structure or strength of these processes fundamentally differs for children with different EF-related neurodevelopmental differences. For example, this study did not include a measure of EF outside of an emotionally valenced context (i.e., “cold” EF), or examine whether, as in adolescents with ADHD, negative affect moderates the degree to which FAA predicts diagnostic characteristics (Alperin et al., 2019). Future research should explore both “hot” and “cold” EF skills in autistic children and children with co-occurring ADHD.

This study does not include a measure of social communication, another important potential correlate of functional impairment in autism and ADHD. There have been mixed findings as to whether FAA relates to social impairment, and if so, whether the relation is mediated by emotion regulation challenges. For example, while Sutton et al. (2005) found that higher right FAA related to greater social communication challenges, Burnette et al. (2011) found that higher right FAA related to fewer restricted and repetitive behaviors. These findings are complicated by differences in the operationalization of FAA. More work is needed to understand how and if FAA may relate to social communication in children with neurodevelopmental disorders.

Conclusion

This study was one of the first to examine whether children’s relative left or right frontal alpha activation (FAA) was related to their executive function (EF) in a “hot,” emotionally valenced context. We found that for all children, greater relative left FAA, or more approach-oriented motivation, related to worse functioning during a hot EF task. While there has been much more research linking FAA to internalizing and externalizing symptoms, we did not find a relation between FAA and emotion dysregulation-based symptoms in our sample, for all children or for children with ADHD or ASD specifically. Instead of comparing children by diagnostic category, commonly observed variability in functional impairment in children with neurodevelopmental disorders may be better explained by using an RDoC approach to explore dimensional characteristics in approach/avoidance reactivity, executive function, and clinically impairing emotion dysregulation.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found at: The dataset analyzed for this study can be found in the National Database for Autism Research (NDAR reference number 2030).

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Boston Children’s Hospital IRB and University of Washington IRB. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

SE conceptualized the study, conducted the statistical analyses, and drafted the manuscript. JF and SF assisted in conceptualizing the study and reviewed intermediate and final drafts of the manuscript. YB processed the EEG data, drafted the EEG methods, and reviewed the final manuscript. RG collected behavioral and EEG data, conducted preliminary analyses, and reviewed the final manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by NICHD R00HD071966 to SF.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the staff and students who assisted with collecting and scoring these measures and who interacted with our participants. The authors specially thank the children and families who contributed their time to this study and joined in the effort to better understand the executive function of children on the autism spectrum and with ADHD. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1065598/full#supplementary-material

References

  1. Achenbach T. M., Rescorla L. A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms and profiles.
  2. Alperin B. R., Smith C. J., Gustafsson H. C., Figuracion M. T., Karalunas S. L., Health O., et al. (2019). The relationship between alpha asymmetry and ADHD depends on negative affect level and parenting practices. J. Psychiatr. Res. 116, 138–146. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2019.06.016, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5). 5th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. [Google Scholar]
  4. Andersen P. N., Hovik K. T., Skogli E. W., Egeland J., Øie M. (2013). Symptoms of ADHD in children with high-functioning autism are related to impaired verbal working memory and verbal delayed recall. PLoS One 8. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0064842, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Baving L., Laucht M., Schmidt M. H. (1999). Atypical frontal brain activation in ADHD: Preschool and elementary school boys and girls. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 38, 1363–1371. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199911000-00010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Baving L., Laucht M., Schmidt M. H. (2002). Frontal brain activation in anxious school children. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 43, 265–274. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Berkovits L., Eisenhower A., Blacher J. (2017). Emotion regulation in young children with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 47, 68–79. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2922-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Blackhart G. C., Minnix J. A., Kline J. P. (2006). Can EEG asymmetry patterns predict future development of anxiety and depression? A preliminary study. Biol. Psychol. 72, 46–50. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.06.010, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Burnette C. P., Henderson H. A., Inge A. P., Zahka N. E., Schwartz C. B., Mundy P. C. (2011). Anterior EEG asymmetry and the modifier model of autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 41, 1113–1124. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1138-0, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Coan J. A., Allen J. J. B. (2004). Frontal EEG asymmetry as a moderator and mediator of emotion. Biol. Psychol. 67, 7–50. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.03.002, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Conner C. M., Golt J., Shaffer R., Righi G., Siegel M., Mazefsky C. A. (2021). Emotion dysregulation is substantially elevated in autism compared to the general population: impact on psychiatric services. Autism Res. 14, 169–181. doi: 10.1002/aur.2450, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cremone-Caira A., Trier K., Sanchez V., Kohn B., Gilbert R., Faja S. (2021). Inhibition in developmental disorders: a comparison of inhibition profiles between children with autism spectrum disorder, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and comorbid symptom presentation. Autism 25, 227–243. doi: 10.1177/1362361320955107, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Crone E. A., van der Molen M. W. (2004). Developmental changes in real life decision making: performance on a gambling task previously shown to depend on the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Dev. Neuropsychol. 25, 251–279. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2503_2, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Day T. N., Mazefsky C. A., Wetherby A. M. (2022). Characterizing difficulties with emotion regulation in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 96:101992. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2022.101992 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Demetriou E. A., DeMayo M. M., Guastella A. J. (2019). Executive function in autism spectrum disorder: history, theoretical models, empirical findings, and potential as an endophenotype. Front. Psychol. 10:753. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00753, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Diego M. A., Field T., Jones N. A., Hernandez-Reif M. (2006). Withdrawn and intrusive maternal interaction style and infant frontal EEG asymmetry shifts in infants of depressed and non-depressed mothers. Infant Behav. Dev. 29, 220–229. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2005.12.002, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Ellis A. J., Kinzel C., Salgari G. C., Loo S. K. (2017). Frontal alpha asymmetry predicts inhibitory processing in youth with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychologia 102, 45–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.06.003, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. England-Mason G. (2020). Emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic feature in children with neurodevelopmental disorders. Curr. Dev. Disord. Rep. 7, 130–138. doi: 10.1007/s40474-020-00200-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  19. Faja S., Clarkson T., Gilbert R., Vaidyanathan A., Greco G., Rueda M. R., et al. (2021). A preliminary randomized, controlled trial of executive function training for children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism 26, 346–360. doi: 10.1177/13623613211014990 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Faja S., Dawson G. (2013). Reduced delay of gratification and effortful control among young children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism 19, 91–101. doi: 10.1177/1362361313512424, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Gabard-Durnam L., Mendez Leal A. S., Wilkinson C. L., Levin A. R. (2018). The Harvard Automated Processing Pipeline for Electroencephalography (HAPPE): Standardized processing software for developmental and high-artifact data. Front. Neurosci. 12, 1–24. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00097 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gabard-Durnam L., Tierney A. L., Vogel-Farley V., Tager-Flusberg H., Nelson C. A. (2015). Alpha asymmetry in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 45, 473–480. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1926-4, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Garcia A. M., Dick A. S., Graziano P. A. (2020). A multimodal assessment of emotion dysregulation in young children with and without ADHD. 1–33. PsyArXiv. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  24. Gatzke-Kopp L. M., Jetha M. K., Segalowitz S. J. (2014). The role of resting frontal EEG asymmetry in psychopathology: afferent or efferent filter? Dev. Psychobiol. 56, 73–85. doi: 10.1002/dev.21092 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Grabell A. S., Olson S. L., Tardif T., Thompson M. C., Gehring W. J. (2017). Comparing self-regulation-associated event related potentials in preschool children with and without high levels of disruptive behavior. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 45, 1119–1132. doi: 10.1007/s10802-016-0228-7, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Graziano P. A., Garcia A. (2016). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and children's emotion dysregulation: a meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 46, 106–123. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.04.011, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Grimshaw G. M., Carmel D. (2014). An asymmetric inhibition model of hemispheric differences in emotional processing. Front. Psychol. 5:489. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00489, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hanslmayr S., Gross J., Klimesch W., Shapiro K. L. (2011). The role of alpha oscillations in temporal attention. Brain Res. Rev. 67, 331–343. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2011.04.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. He J., Degnan K. A., McDermott J. M., Henderson H. A., Hane A. A., Xu Q., et al. (2010). Anger and approach motivation in infancy: relations to early childhood inhibitory control and behavior problems. Infancy 15, 246–269. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2009.00017.x, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Insel T., Cuthbert B., Garvey M., Heinssen R., Pine D. S., Quinn K., et al. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 167, 748–751. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Jahromi L. B., Meek S. E., Ober-reynolds S. (2012). Emotion regulation in the context of frustration in children with high functioning autism and their typical peers. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 53, 1250–1258. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02560.x, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Karalunas S. L., Gustafsson H. C., Fair D., Musser E. D., Nigg J. T. (2019). Do we need an irritable subtype of ADHD? Replication and extension of a promising temperament profile approach to ADHD subtyping. Psychol. Assess. 31, 236–247. doi: 10.1037/pas0000664, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Keune P. M., Wiedemann E., Schneidt A., Schönenberg M. (2015). Frontal brain asymmetry in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): extending the motivational dysfunction hypothesis. Clin. Neurophysiol. 126, 711–720. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2014.07.008, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Levin A. R., Méndez Leal A. S., Gabard-Durnam L. J., O'Leary H. M. (2018). BEAPP: the batch electroencephalography automated processing platform. Front. Neurosci. 12:513. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00513, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Livingston L. A., Colvert E., Social Relationships Study Team. Bolton P., Happé F. (2019). Good social skills despite poor theory of mind: exploring compensation in autism spectrum disorder. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 60, 102–110. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12886, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Lord C., Rutter M., DiLavore P. C., Risi S., Gotham K., Bishop S. (2012). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, 2nd Edn. Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services. [Google Scholar]
  37. Mazefsky C. A., Herrington J., Siegel M., Scarpa A., Maddox B. B., Scahill L., et al. (2013). The role of emotion regulation in autism spectrum disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 52, 679–688. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.05.006, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Meyer A., Lerner M. D., De Los Reyes A., Laird R. D., Hajcak G. (2017). Considering ERP difference scores as individual difference measures: issues with subtraction and alternative approaches. Psychophysiology 54, 114–122. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12664, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Reznik S. J., Allen J. (2018). Frontal asymmetry as a mediator and moderator of emotion: an updated review. Psychophysiology 55:e12965. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12965, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Richey J. A., Damiano C. R., Sabatino A., Rittenberg A., Petty C., Bizzell J., et al. (2015). Neural mechanisms of emotion regulation in autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 45, 3409–3423. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2359-z, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Rutter M., Le Couteur A., Lord C. (2003). ADI-R: Autism diagnostic interview-revised (ADI-R). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. [Google Scholar]
  42. Salazar F., Baird G., Chandler S., Tseng E., O'sullivan T., Howlin P., et al. (2015). Co-occurring psychiatric disorders in preschool and elementary school-aged children with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 45, 2283–2294. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2361-5, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Samson A. C., Phillips J. M., Parker K. J., Shah S., Gross J. J., Hardan A. Y. (2014). Emotion dysregulation and the core features of autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 44, 1766–1772. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-2022-5, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Särelä J., Vigário R. (2003). Overlearning in marginal distribution-based ICA: analysis and solutions. J. Mach. Learn. Res. 4, 1447–1469. [Google Scholar]
  45. Schendel D. E., Overgaard M., Christensen J., Hjort L., Jørgensen M., Vestergaard M., et al. (2016). Association of psychiatric and neurologic comorbidity with mortality among persons with autism spectrum disorder in a Danish population. JAMA Pediatr. 170, 243–250. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.3935, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Schiltz H. K., McVey A. J., Barrington A., Haendel A. D., Dolan B. K., Willar K. S., et al. (2018). Behavioral inhibition and activation as a modifier process in autism spectrum disorder: examination of self-reported BIS/BAS and alpha EEG asymmetry. Autism Res. 11, 1653–1666. doi: 10.1002/aur.2016, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Seidman L. J. (2006). Neuropsychological functioning in people with ADHD across the lifespan. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 26, 466–485. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.01.004, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Shaw P., Stringaris A., Nigg J. T., Leibenluft E. (2014). Emotion dysregulation in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Psychiatr. 171, 276–293. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13070966, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Smith C. L., Bell M. A. (2010). Stability in infant frontal asymmetry as a predictor of toddlerhood internalizing and externalizing behaviors. Dev. Psychobiol. 52, 158–167. doi: 10.1002/dev.20427, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Sparrow S. S., Cicchetti D. V., Balla D. A., Doll E. A. (2005). Vineland adaptive behavior scales: survey forms. 2nd Edn. Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  51. Sutton S. K., Burnette C. P., Mundy P. C., Meyer J., Vaughan A., Sanders C., et al. (2005). Resting cortical brain activity and social behavior in higher functioning children with autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 46, 211–222. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00341.x, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Wakschlag L. S., Perlman S. B., Blair R. J., Leibenluft E., Briggs-Gowan M. J., Pine D. S. (2018). The neurodevelopmental basis of early childhood disruptive behavior: irritable and callous phenotypes as exemplars. Am. J. Psychiatry 175, 114–130. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.17010045, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Wakschlag L. S., Roberts M. Y., Flynn R. M., Smith J. D., Krogh-Jespersen S., Kaat A. J., et al. (2019). Future directions for early childhood prevention of mental disorders: a road map to mental health, earlier. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 48, 539–554. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2018.1561296, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Wechsler D. (2011). WASI-II: Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. 2nd Edn. Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson. [Google Scholar]
  55. Wheeler R. E., Davidson R. J., Tomarken A. J. (1993). Frontal brain asymmetry and emotional reactivity: a biological substrate of affective style. Psychophysiology 30, 82–89. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb03207.x, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Winkler I., Brandl S., Horn F., Waldburger E., Allefeld C., Tangermann M. (2014). Robust artifactual independent component classification for BCI practitioners. J. Neural Eng. 11:035013. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/11/3/035013, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Winkler I., Haufe S., Tangermann M. (2011). Automatic classification of artifactual ICA-components for artifact removal in EEG signals. Behav. Brain Funct. 7:30. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-7-30, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Zelazo P. D., Carlson S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: development and plasticity. Child Dev. Perspect. 6, 354–360. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00246.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found at: The dataset analyzed for this study can be found in the National Database for Autism Research (NDAR reference number 2030).


Articles from Frontiers in Psychology are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES