Abstract
Locomotor movements cause visual images to be displaced across the eye, a retinal slip that is counteracted by stabilizing reflexes in many animals. In insects, optomotor turning causes the animal to turn in the direction of rotating visual stimuli, thereby reducing retinal slip and stabilizing trajectories through the world. This behavior has formed the basis for extensive dissections of motion vision. Here, we report that under certain stimulus conditions, two Drosophila species, including the widely studied D. melanogaster, can suppress and even reverse the optomotor turning response over several seconds. Such ‘anti-directional turning’ is most strongly evoked by long-lasting, high-contrast, slow-moving visual stimuli that are distinct from those that promote syn-directional optomotor turning. Anti-directional turning, like the syn-directional optomotor response, requires the local motion detecting neurons T4 and T5. A subset of lobula plate tangential cells, CH cells, show involvement in these responses. Imaging from a variety of direction-selective cells in the lobula plate shows no evidence of dynamics that match the behavior, suggesting that the observed inversion in turning direction emerges downstream of the lobula plate. Further, anti-directional turning declines with age and exposure to light. These results show that Drosophila optomotor turning behaviors contain rich, stimulus-dependent dynamics that are inconsistent with simple reflexive stabilization responses.
Intro
Visual navigation requires active mechanisms to stabilize trajectories through the world. Insects exhibit an optomotor turning response, a behavior in which they rotate their bodies in the direction of visual patterns that rotate about them 1–3. This behavior is analogous to optomotor turning responses in fish 4 and the optokinetic response in mammals 5. In insects, this response is thought to be a course-stabilization mechanism that minimizes retinal slip, allowing animals to maintain their trajectory in the face of external or unexpected rotational forces 2,6. For instance, if an insect attempts to walk in a straight line, it may slip and turn to the right. From the point of view of the insect, this turn is observed as optic flow rotating to the left. By responding to this leftward optic flow with a leftward turn, the insect can recover its original trajectory.
In fruit flies, the optomotor response relies on well-characterized circuitry 7. Photoreceptor signals are split into parallel ON and OFF pathways in the lamina and medulla 8–11, which are not direction-selective. These signals provide input to T4 and T5 cells, which compute direction-selective responses along four directions at every point in the fly visual field 12–16. The outputs of T4 and T5 cells are then summed across visual space by lobula plate tangential cells (LPTCs) 12,17–20. Different LPTCs provide distinct signals about the overall pattern of motion surrounding the fly, and have been linked to head and body movements 21–23.
Interestingly, there have been several reports of flies turning in the direction opposite to what is predicted by the optomotor turning response. In some cases, these counter-intuitive behaviors were observed using periodic stimuli with spatial wavelengths smaller than the receptive field of individual ommatidia, and thus can be accounted for by aliasing 3,24,25. Work in a tethered flight simulator showed that when a moving pattern is presented in front of the fly, the animal turned in the direction of the stimulus motion 26, as expected 27. However, if the moving pattern was presented behind the fly, it attempted to turn in the direction opposite to stimulus motion 26. In a different experimental preparation, rotational patterns were presented on a dome around freely-walking flies 28. Under these conditions, flies generally turned in the direction of motion of the stimulus, but these rotations were often punctuated by brief, large-magnitude saccades in the opposite direction. Similarly, experiments using flight simulators have reported spikes in the torque in the direction opposite the stimulus rotation 29.
Here we show that rotational stimuli can elicit strong, consistent anti-directional turning behavior in two drosophilid species, D. melanogaster and D. yakuba. We report that flies respond to high contrast, high luminance rotational motion stimuli by first turning in the direction of stimulus motion, and then reversing their trajectory after approximately one second, depending on the species. In Drosophila melanogaster, we characterize the dynamics of this behavior and the stimuli that drive it, showing that it is distinct from prior observations of anti-directional turning. The behavior depends critically on adaptation to back-to-front motion. We use the genetic tools available in Drosophila melanogaster to show that this behavior relies on the motion detecting neurons T4 and T5. Silencing HS and CH, two widefield neurons downstream of T4 and T5, resulted in small changes in this complex turning behavior. However, the visually evoked responses of these direction-selective neurons could not account for the anti-directional behavior. Thus, the observed reversal must be mediated by downstream circuitry. Overall, these results show that circuits in the fly generate behaviors that oppose the direction of wide-field visual motion, showing that Drosophila turning responses are more complex than a simple stabilizing reflex.
Results
Anti-directional turning responses to high contrast stimuli
Optomotor turning responses are central to gaze stabilization, so we sought to examine this response across different conditions. Many studies have investigated this behavior using stimuli with low contrast, low light intensity, or both 2,3,30–33, at a variety of different speeds. However, natural scenes can have relatively high contrast and luminance, conditions have been poorly explored in the laboratory. In this experiment, we presented flies with rotational stimuli using high contrast and relatively high luminance.
We tethered individual female D. melanogaster above a freely rotating ball to characterize the optomotor response 3,34 (Fig. 1a). As expected, low contrast, slow-moving sinusoidal gratings caused flies to turn in the same direction as the moving gratings via the classical optomotor turning response (Fig. 1b) 1,3,8,25,26,30,31,35–42. However, when we changed the stimulus to high contrast sinusoidal gratings (nominal 100% Weber contrast), flies turned in the stimulus direction for approximately 1 second, but then reversed course, and turned in the direction opposite to the stimulus motion for the duration of the stimulus presentation. Because this turning response is in the opposite direction of stimulus and the syn-directional optomotor turning response, we refer to it as anti-directional turning.
We swept a range of contrasts and compared the fly turning in the first 500 milliseconds to the turning after one second (Fig. 1c). As contrast increased, the flies turned faster during the first half second of stimulus presentation, reaching a plateau at around 0.5 contrast, consistent with previous results 3,35,36,43–45. Fly behavior after the first second of stimulation was more complex. As contrast increased from 0 to 0.25, flies turned in the same direction as the stimulus, with faster turning as the contrast increased. When the contrast was greater than 0.25, turning decreased, lowering to no net sustained turning at around 0.8 contrast. Above a contrast of 0.8, flies began to turn in the direction opposite the stimulus.
These initial experiments took place in the lab of author DAC. To confirm that these unexpected responses did not reflect some idiosyncrasy of one specific behavioral apparatus or environment, we repeated these experiments in a second lab, that of author TRC. Under similar conditions, using the same strain of Drosophila melanogaster, we reproduced the rapid deceleration after an initial, transient syn-directional response (Fig. 1d), with some individual flies exhibiting significant anti-directional turning (Supp. Fig. S1). This demonstrates that the key features of this behavioral response are stable across experimental systems and laboratories, though the magnitude of anti-directional turning behavior in D. melanogaster is sensitive to some unknown experimental parameter differences between the laboratories.
Individual strains of D. melanogaster, and other drosophilid species, display significant variation in their locomotor patterns during walking 46. Indeed, when we tested a Canton-S D. melanogaster strain, we observed milder but significant anti-directional turning at long timescales (Supp. Fig. S2b). We reasoned that a strong test of the generality of anti-directional turning would be to examine turning behavior in another species, and selected D. yakuba. Strikingly, D. yakuba also displayed anti-directional turning behavior under similar conditions (Fig. 1e). Thus, this behavior is not an idiosyncratic feature of a single laboratory strain.
Conditions for anti-directional turning behaviors
While anti-directional turning behaviors have been reported before, other groups have presented similar stimuli without observing anti-directional behavior 2,3,30–33. We wondered what aspects of our experimental setup could lead to these behavioral differences. In our experiments, anti-directional turning was strongly linked to display brightness (Supp. Fig. S2a). When the mean brightness of the screens was reduced from 100 cd/m2 to 1 cd/m2, we saw no anti-directional turning in 5 second trials (though average optomotor behavior did decrease over the course of the stimulus presentation). When we further reduced the mean brightness to 0.1 cd/m2, flies persisted in their optomotor behavior throughout the stimulus presentation. We note that in these low luminance experiments, low levels of ambient light in the nominally dark experimental rig could also reduce the effective contrast of the stimulus.
We tested a variety of other factors that might affect anti-directional turning. Anti-directional turning occurred when experiments were run both at hot temperatures and at room temperature (Supp. Fig. S2b). We also observed anti-directional behavior when flies were reared in the dark and on different media. We also tested several other experiment conditions (Supp. Fig. S2c). Flies responded with anti-directional turning to high contrast stimuli presented at both blue and green wavelengths. We glued fly heads to their thorax to ensure stimuli could not be affected by head movements 21,22, but found no difference between head-fixed and head-free flies. We did find a few factors that modulated anti-directional turning behavior. In particular, rearing D. melanogaster at 25°C instead of 20°C or testing flies that were two weeks old instead of 12–60 hours old both reduced overall turning behavior and eliminated anti-directional turning. In these cases, optomotor turning still decreased over the course of the 5 second, high contrast trials, but did not reverse. As details of rearing temperature and the age at which behavior tests are run often vary across labs, it is likely that these factors, as well as stimulus brightness, account for the differences between our observations and the previous literature.
Distinct spatiotemporal tuning of the anti-directional behavioral response
To further characterize the anti-directional response, we swept the spatial and temporal frequency of the sinusoidal grating stimulus. Using only Weber contrasts of 1, we compared the early response (first quarter second, Fig. 2a) to the late response (after one second, Fig. 2b). Drosophila melanogaster always turned in the optomotor direction during the early stimulus response. In this early response, flies turned most vigorously to stimuli with short spatial frequencies (~20° wavelength) and fast temporal frequencies (~8 Hz), in agreement with earlier studies 26,37,39. However, during the longer-timescale response to high-contrast stimuli, flies only turned in the optomotor direction at very high temporal frequencies (> ~16 Hz) and at very low temporal frequencies (<0.5 Hz). At intermediate temporal frequencies, flies showed a sustained anti-directional response. The maximal anti-directional response was achieved at 1 Hz and 45° wavelength, distinct from the conditions for peak classical turning responses. Interestingly, the stimuli that elicit the strongest anti-directional response appear similar to those that maximally activate T4 and T5 neurons when those neurons are measured in head-fixed flies 12,37,39,47–49.
Anti-directional turning results from adaptation effects
We were intrigued by the switch from syn-directional to anti-directional turning behavior. To investigate the dynamics of these changes, we presented a rotating sinusoidal stimulus at contrast 1 for five seconds, and then changed the contrast to 0.25 (Fig. 2c). After the switch to low contrast, the flies quickly reverted classical, syn-directional optomotor behavior, demonstrating that no long-term switch in directional turning occurs during high contrast stimulus presentation. This effect did not depend on the periodic nature of these stimuli: a rotating stimulus consisting of 5°-wide vertical bars with randomly-chosen, binary contrasts 38 yielded similar behavioral responses (Fig. 2d).
To further isolate the causes of this switch in behavior, we developed a stimulus to adapt the fly to different stimuli before presenting high-contrast rotational sinusoidal gratings to elicit the anti-directional turning response. This adapting stimulus consisted of five seconds of high contrast ‘translational’ stimuli, which was then followed by a rotational stimulus (Fig. 2e). The translational stimuli consisted of both left and right hemifields moving either front-to-back or back-to-front across the fly’s two eyes 39. These stimuli resulted in no net turning by the flies 39,42. Adapting the fly with front-to-back stimuli did not have a strong effect on the subsequent response to rotational stimuli. However, adapting with back-to-front stimuli generated responses that no longer showed an initial syn-directional turning response, but instead exhibited anti-directional turning immediately after the rotational stimulus began. This result indicates that the anti-directional turning results from slow-timescale changes that depend on strong back-to-front motion stimulation.
Anti-directional turning is elicited when stimuli are presented in front of the fly
A previous report of anti-directional turning behavior in flying tethered flies showed that flies turn in the opposite direction to stimuli that are presented behind their midline 26. To test whether our results were caused by this effect, we split our stimulus into three regions: 90 degrees in front of the fly, 45 degrees in front of the midline on either side of the fly, and 45 degrees behind the midline on either side of the fly (Fig. 3a). We found that flies displayed anti-directional turning when presented with stimuli only in the front region or only just in front of the midline (Fig. 3bc). They did not display anti-directional turning when moving stimuli were presented behind the midline (Fig. 3bc). This suggests a different mechanism from the behaviors that depend on posterior spatial location to elicit reverse-turning 26.
Anti-directional responses do not depend on saccades
Anti-directional saccades have been reported in walking and flying flies 28,29. In walking flies 28, flies largely turned in syn-directionally, but these turns were sometimes interrupted by brief, high-amplitude saccades in the opposite direction, against the stimulus direction. If such saccades were frequent or high amplitude, the net effect could shift the average turning we measured, creating apparent anti-directional turning. To investigate this possibility, we plotted the turning response on a per-trial basis (Fig. 3d). We then discarded information about the magnitude of the turns and considered only the direction of the turning at each point in time (Fig. 3e). Strikingly, in many trials, flies continued to turn opposite to the stimulus for several seconds, a behavior unlike brief saccades. We then calculated a turning index for each response timepoint (sampled at 60 Hz). This turning index represented the fraction of trials where the fly turned in the direction of the stimulus at each timepoint minus the fraction of trials where the fly turned in the opposite direction (Fig. 3f). Since this turning index does not include the magnitude of turning, it is strongly affected by sustained low-amplitude turns and discounts any brief high-amplitude saccades. When presented with high contrast stimuli, flies maintained a negative turning index, indicating that sustained turns, and not high velocity saccades, underlie this anti-directional turning behavior. As such, it appears distinct from the reports of anti-directional saccades.
Anti-directional turning requires elementary motion detectors
What neurons are involved in this anti-directional turning behavior? Previous work demonstrated that T4 and T5 are required for directional neural responses 18, as well as for optomotor turning 12,40,50, for walking speed regulation 39, and for responses to visual looming stimuli 51. We silenced the neurons T4 and T5 using shibirets 52 and measured responses to sinusoidal stimuli that switched from high to low contrast (Fig. 4a). Flies in which T4 and T5 had been silenced displayed only minimal responses to motion stimuli, with anti-directional turning suppressed along with classical syn-directional turning. Thus, we conclude that, like optomotor turning behaviors, this anti-directional behavior depends critically on signals from T4 and T5.
Anti-directional turning requires the CH lobula plate tangential cell
Since the switch from optomotor to anti-directional behavior seems to be dependent on the direction of motion adaptation (Fig. 2e), we reasoned that neurons involved in this behavior were likely to be downstream from T4 and T5. Relatively little is known about circuitry that connects the neurons T4 and T5 to optomotor turning behavior. However, Horizontal System (HS) cells are well-studied postsynaptic partners of T4 and T5 9,20. These lobula plate tangential cells integrate information from front-to-back and back-to-front selective T4 and T5 cells across the fly’s visual field 17. HS cells have been implicated in visually-evoked head turns 21 and body rotations in flight 22 and in maintenance of direction during walking 53. When we silenced HS neurons, we found small deficits in syn-directional turning behavior, consistent with prior results, but no deficits in anti-directional turning (Fig. 4b), indicating that HS cells synaptic output is not required specifically for anti-directional turning behavior.
Next, we turned to the CH lobula plate tangential cells. These cells are GABAergic and are both pre-synaptic and post-synaptic in the lobula plate 54. In blowflies, these neurons play an inhibitory role in an interconnected LPTC circuit that shapes behavior 55. When we silenced CH neurons, we found a small increase in syn-directional turning and a decrease in anti-directional turning (Fig. 4c). Overall, silencing this neuron type caused the flies to turn more in the direction of motion. This result suggests that CH activity contributes to the anti-directional turning response. However, since adapting to back-to-front translational stimuli significantly affected the dynamics of anti-directional turning, it seems likely that other neurons beyond HS and CH are involved, since these two neurons both respond selectively to front-to-back motion 20,56.
Early direction-selective cells do not adapt to the stimulus
The anti-directional turning response is preceded by an initial syn-directional response. This change in behavior must be the result of changes in neural activity, but this change could happen at any point along the neural pathway between photoreceptors and motor neurons. In order to constrain possible mechanisms for generating the anti-directional turning behaviors, we used calcium imaging to interrogate the activity of direction selective neurons during high and low contrast stimulation (Fig. 5a). However, as calcium imaging experiments using two photon microscopy require additional spectral filtering of the projector, we first confirmed that these spectral differences did not alter anti-directional turning responses. To do this, we re-measuring the anti-directional turning behavior using optical filtering matched to the conditions needed for imaging. Using this spectrally distinct illuminant, we observed both syn-directional and anti-directional turning behaviors, following the previously observed dynamics (Supp. Fig. S3).
As T4 and T5 neurons play a critical role in both the syn- and anti-directional turning responses, we first measured the calcium activity of these neurons as they responded to sine wave gratings at a range of contrasts in their preferred and null directions. The T4 and T5 neurons responded to sine wave gratings in their preferred direction by increasing their calcium activity for the full duration of the stimulus presentation, reaching a plateau after approximately 1 second (Fig. 5bc, middle). As we increased the contrast of the preferred direction stimuli, we found that both T4 and T5 cells had increased calcium activity throughout the contrast range (Fig. 5bc, right), consistent with prior measurements 12. Thus, the responses of T4 and T5 cells do not capture the transition from syn-directional to anti-directional turning behavior.
Next we examined two LPTCs downstream of T4 and T5 cells. Calcium activity in HS cells followed similar trends to T4 and T5. Calcium signals increased at the start of preferred direction stimuli presentation and stayed high until the end of the presentation (Fig. 5d, middle). Increasing contrast caused stronger calcium responses with a mild saturation effect at high contrast (Fig. 5d, right), consistent with prior voltage measurements 20. These results indicate that the changes in the time course of optomotor behavior at high contrast are not related to changes in HS activity. Finally, we measured calcium activity in CH cells. CH cells responded to visual stimuli more quickly than HS cells (Fig. 5e, middle), and showed decreased calcium signals in response to null direction stimuli (Fig. 5e, right). However, they also showed sustained responses to high contrast stimuli, as in T4, T5, and HS. These measurements suggest that the switch from syn- to anti-directional turning behavior is driven by cells downstream of or parallel to T4, T5, HS, and CH.
Adult plasticity in anti-directional turning behavior
In behaving flies, the strength of anti-directional turning was dependent both on rearing temperature, which alters the rate of growth, and on age (Supp. Fig. S2). This raises the possibility that syn- and anti-directional turning responses might be plastic during the early adult stages of development. To probe this possibility, we presented 1 Hz, high-contrast, rotating sinusoidal grating at various stages during early adulthood (Fig. 6). Strikingly, as flies aged from 0.5 to 4 days post eclosion (dpe), the initial syn-directional turning became less transient and more sustained, indicative of a weaker anti-directional turning drive. We then wondered whether this plasticity was intrinsically programmed, or dependent on visual input. To disambiguate these possibilities, we reared flies in darkness to 2 or 4 dpe and measured their turning responses (Fig. 6, gray). Dark-reared flies exhibited a stronger deceleration away from syn-directional turning, similar to that found in more juvenile flies, arguing that visual input may sculpt the balance of syn- and anti-directional turning. Finally, we examined whether optomotor response plasticity could be detected in D. yakuba. However, in this species, anti-directional responses were stable across the first four days of adulthood, arguing that the role of visual experience in shaping these responses is itself evolutionarily tuned in drosophilids (Supp. Fig. 4).
Discussion
In this study, we found we could elicit robust turning in the opposite direction of high contrast motion stimuli (Fig. 1). This behavior is qualitatively different from other turning behaviors reported in the literature (Figs. 2 and 3), but shares elements with the circuitry necessary for optomotor behavior (Fig. 4). However, the switch from syn-directional turning behavior to anti-directional turning behavior is not a reflection of changes in the activity of known direction-selective neuron types in the early visual system (Fig. 5). Moreover, this anti-directional turning behavior exhibits a degree of experience-dependent plasticity (Fig. 6).
Anti-directional turning is distinct from other against-stimuli behaviors
The anti-directional turning behavior we have characterized is distinct from previous reports of flies turning in the direction opposite to the stimulus motion. First, some opposite-direction turning behaviors can be explained by stimulus aliasing 3. Aliasing cannot explain our results because the stimulus that maximally activates anti-directional behavior has a spatial frequency of 1/60 cycles per degree, well below the Nyquist frequency of the fly eye (~1/10 cycles per degree) 3,24 and below reports of higher acuity vision in flies 57. Aliasing would also not explain the dependence on stimulus contrast.
Second, our observations also cannot be explained by stimuli to the rear of the fly driving it in the opposite direction 26, since we observe anti-directional turning even when stimuli are only presented in only the 90 degrees in front of the fly (Fig. 3).
Third, it is also distinct from previous reports of reverse body saccades 28 since it manifests in persistent turns in the opposite direction of the stimulus and can be measured even when the magnitude of the turns is discarded (Fig. 3).
Fourth, the behavior observed here also appears to be distinct from previously-observed stimulus-density dependent behavioral reversals 58. Those previously reported behaviors showed immediate reversals, but it took ~1 second for flies in our paradigm to switch between optomotor and anti-directional behaviors.
Anti-directional turning is unlikely to be due to adaptation to contrast alone
In mammalian retina, the direction preference of cells can switch because of upstream circuit adaptation 59,60. However, we do not believe the anti-directional turning we observe has similar causes. In the mammalian retina, direction switching occurs when non-direction-selective neurons adapt to high contrast stimuli, which distorts the downstream direction-selective computation. Since the adaptation in those experiments occurs in non-direction-selective neurons, it cannot be affected by the direction of the adapter stimulus. However, we see differences in turning behavior depending on whether we adapt with front-to-back or back-to-front stimuli (Fig. 2e). This observation rules out a mechanism based solely on contrast, since the contrast content of front-to-back and back-to-front stimuli are identical.
The fly’s visual system, however, adapts its gain to stimulus contrast 61,62. Importantly, the phenomenology of the anti-directional turning also argues that the contrast adaptation is incomplete or heterogeneous among neurons, since contrast 1 and contrast 0.25 stimuli result in such different behaviors. Contrast adaptation reported in the fly is also faster than the 1–2 seconds preceding the shift to anti-directional turning in these experiments.
Anti-directional turning behavior may require specific experimental and rearing conditions
Despite these previous reports of anti-directional turning under certain conditions, other labs have measured sustained optomotor turning in response to high contrast stimuli 2,31,32,36. Two major causes of this difference are likely display brightness and rearing conditions. Some experiments employ displays with mean luminances less than 5 cd/m2 31,33,36. Our screens, with a mean luminance of 100 cd/m2, are substantially brighter, but not especially bright when compared to natural scenes. In daytime natural scenes, foliage and the ground have average luminances of 200–500 cd/m2 and the sky has an average luminance of around 4000 cd/m2 63. We suspect that as researchers move to using displays that can more accurately depict natural scene luminances, anti-directional turning behaviors will be encountered more frequently.
Rearing conditions also significant influenced anti-directional turning behavior. Flies reared at 25°C showed less anti-directional behavior than those reared at 20°C. Temperature has known developmental effects on neural connectivity 64. We also found differences based on fly age and fly strain. Notably, all three of these parameters vary significantly across the field, with prior studies varying rearing temperatures from 18 to 20 to 25°C (see for instance 36,39,57,65,66), ages from 1 day to 10 days (see for instance 26,30,42), and strain between CantonS or OregonR (see for instance 8,33). Thus, these factors likely account for the fact that this phenomenon has not previously been reported, even as rotating sinusoids have been widely used in behavioral experiments.
Tuning of anti-directional turning matches tuning of direction selective neurons
The study of anti-directional turning behavior may yield clues about the temporal tuning of fly motion detectors. Optomotor behavior is tuned to visual stimuli in the range of 8–22 Hz 26,37,39,67, while anti-directional behavior is tuned to stimuli in the range of 0.5–4 Hz (Fig. 2). Intriguingly, this slower tuning matches the tuning of T4, T5, and HS neurons, as measured via calcium imaging or electrophysiology 12,20,39,68. Previous studies have suggested that the difference in tuning between behavior and imaging are due to octopamine that is released during behavior but not necessarily released during imaging 37,47,68. In this work, we demonstrate a motion-related behavior tuned to low frequencies, comparable to those in neural measurements, during behavior that requires T4 and T5 neurons. Overall, this suggests that T4 and T5 are required for behaviors with very different temporal tuning, which in turn suggests that the temporal tuning of behavior is not determined solely by T4 and T5 tuning, but by other, parallel pathways as well.
Anti-directional turning reveals circuits that turn the fly counter to visual motion
Experiments that show a decrease of turning over time to high contrast stimuli (e.g., Fig. 1d) could plausibly be explained by some kind of gain reduction or adaptation over time. However, the existence of turning in the direction opposite the stimulus motion in D. melanogaster and in D. yakuba requires a different explanation. These experiments reveal that over long timescales, a circuit that opposes the syn-directional optomotor turning response can dominate the behavioral response. Thus, this circuit is not simply scaling the magnitude of turning responses, but rather must be implementing an antagonistic, subtractive operation. Measurements of free walking behavior have shown that the time constant of the autocorrelation of fly turning is around 100 ms 69,70. Opposing syn- and anti-directional turning circuits could be used to balance and tune the strength of turning responses on short timescales, while the anti-directional turning is revealed on longer timescales. This sort of subtractive processing predominates in computing motion signals in the visual systems of insects 17 and mammals 71, and could also explain the existence of syn- and anti-directional turning behaviors.
In summary, we have presented evidence of a transition from syn-directional turning to no turning or to anti-directional turning when high contrast stimuli are presented to the fly. This persists across laboratory environments and across Drosophila species and shows plasticity with age. This behavior suggests that turning in response to rotational stimuli is not a simple reflex. Instead, the turning is likely driven by circuits with opposing influences on turning direction. These circuits appear to differentially adapt to the direction and contrast of the stimulus. This complexity makes the optomotor response a model for studying the interactions of circuits as they control the low-dimensional behaviors that change an animal’s orientation.
Methods
Data availability
Raw behavioral and imaging data, along with code to run the analyses and create the plots in this paper, are available on Dryad: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.stqjq2c77.
Fly strains
Strains used in these experiments are listed in the tables below:
Fly rearing (DAC lab)
Unless otherwise noted, flies were reared at 20 degrees Celsius in Panasonic MIR-154-PA incubators (Panasonic/PHC, Tokyo, Japan). The flies were circadian entrained on 12-hour light-dark cycles. Flies were raised on Archon Scientific glucose food (recipe D20102, Archon Scientific, Durham, NC). We used CO2 to anesthetize flies more than 12 hours before the behavioral experiments.
Flies were tested for behavior in rigs built in the labs of DAC and TRC. Behavior shown in Figs. 1d, 1e, 6c, 6d, S1, and S4 was acquired in the lab of TRC, while the rest was obtained in the lab of DAC.
Fly rearing (TRC lab)
Flies were reared at 25°C, on molasses-based food, and circadian entrained on 12-hour light-dark cycles. Flies were collected within three hours of eclosion using brief CO2 anesthetization. D. melanogaster and D. yakuba were raised under identical conditions. Dark-reared flies were put in a dark chamber within 3 hours of eclosion. Flies tested at 0.5 days post eclosion were collected during the first two hours of the light cycle and were exposed to light until they were tested.
Stimulus generation and behavioral turning assays (DAC lab)
Stimuli were presented using DLP Lightcrafter (Texas Instruments, Dallas, TX) projectors 34. Mirrors were used to bounce the projected light onto three screens made of back-projection material, surrounding the fly. The screens covered the front 270 degrees around the fly, and ~45 degrees in elevation above and below the fly. The projectors were set to monochrome mode (green unless otherwise noted), updating at 180 Hz. Stimulus video was generated through a custom MATLAB (Mathworks, Natick, MA) application using PsychToolbox 73. Stimuli were mapped onto a virtual cylinder around the fly and the MATLAB application generated a viewpoint-corrected video signal.
Behavioral experiments were performed 12–60 hours after staging. For behavioral experiments, we selected female flies, and co-housed them with males after staging. Flies were cold-anesthetized and fixed to needles using UV-cured epoxy (Norland optical adhesive #63, Norland Products, Cranbury, NJ). Flies were then placed above air-suspended polypropylene balls. These balls were 6 mm in diameter and weighed ~120 mg. The balls were painted with two layers of marker coatings- a base silver layer and a red top layer. The motion of balls was detected by either a Parallax mouse sensor board (Parallax, Rocklin, CA) with an MCS-12086 sensor (Unity Opto Technology, Taipei, Taiwan), or a custom board with an ADNS 2080 sensor (Avago Technologies / Broadcom Inc, San Jose, TX). The data from these sensors were transferred to a custom MATLAB application via an Arduino Uno board.
Stimulus generation and behavioral turning assays (TRC lab)
Stimuli were presented using a DLP Lightcrafter (Texas Instruments, Dallas, TX) projector. Three coherent optic fibers were used to direct the projected light onto three screens made of back-projection material, surrounding the fly 8,38. The screens covered the front 270 degrees around the fly, and ~45 degrees in elevation above and below the fly. The projectors were set to monochrome mode, updating at 120 Hz. Stimulus video was generated through Flystim (https://github.com/ClandininLab/flystim), a custom Python application developed in the Clandinin Lab 74. Stimuli were mapped onto a virtual cylinder around the fly and Flystim generated a viewpoint-corrected video signal.
Behavioral experiments were performed 12–48 hours after eclosion, as described in the figures. Flies were cold-anesthetized and fixed to needles using UV-cured adhesive (Bondic, Niagara Falls, NY). Flies were then placed above air-suspended balls made with LAST-A-FOAM FR-4615 polyurethane foam (General Plastics, Tacoma, WA). These balls were 9 mm in diameter and weighed ~91.7 mg. The motion of balls was detected by a Flea3 FL3-U3–13Y3M camera (Teledyne Flir, Wilsonville, OR) and Fictrac software 75.
Imaging procedures
Two photon imaging (Fig. 5) was performed as previously described 76. Briefly, two-photon images were acquired with a Scientifica microscope at between 6 and 13 Hz using a 930 nm femtosecond laser (SpectraPhysics, Santa Clara, USA) using ScanImage 77. Visual stimuli were presented on three screens occupying 270° of azimuthal angle about the fly using projectors 34. Optical filters on the projector and emission filters prevented the visual stimulus light from leaking into the two-photon images.
Regions of interest (ROIs) were extracted from image timeseries using a watershed algorithm. Responsive ROIs were included in the analyses. For T4 and T5 neurons, each ROI was identified as a T4-dominant or T5-dominant ROI by its response to light vs. dark edges, following prior procedures 78. For all neuron types, responses were averaged over ROIs and over trials of each stimulus type to obtain a measurement for each fly; these fly measurements acted as the independent measurements to compute means and standard error bars for the figure.
Statistical tests
Throughout the paper, each fly was considered an independent sample for statistical purposes. Means and standard errors were computed over flies. For imaging experiments, regions of interest from a specific neuron type were first averaged within each fly, creating a value for each fly’s response. These values were used to calculate means and standard errors over the tested flies. In the silencing experiments, a 2-sample Student t-test was used to test for significant differences between the experimental genotype and parental controls.
Supplementary Material
Table 1:
Name | Genotype | Source | Stock # |
---|---|---|---|
| |||
Wildtype | +; +; + (IsoD1) | 72 | N/A |
T4T5-Gal4 | +; +; R42F06-Gal4 (IsoD1 background) | BDSC | BDSC 41253 |
HS-Gal4 | +; +; R27B03-Gal4 (IsoD1 bg) | 31 | BDSC 49211 |
CH-Gal4 | w; +; R35A10-Gal4 (Janelia bg) | BDSC | BDSC 49897 |
UAS-Shibirets | +; +; UAS-Shibirets (IsoD1 bg) | 42 | N/A |
Empty Gal4 | w; +; pBDPGAL4.1Uw (Janelia bg) | BDSC | BDSC 68384 |
GCaMP6f | w; UAS-GCaMP6f; + | BDSC | BDSC 42747 |
jGCaMP7b | w; +; UAS-jGCaMP7b | BDSC | BDSC 79029 |
mtdTomato | w; +; UAS-mtdTomato | BDSC | BDSC 30124 |
Table 2:
Experimental | Gal4 Control | UAS Control | Background Control |
---|---|---|---|
| |||
T4T5-Gal4 x UAS-Shibirets: +; +; R42F06-Gal4/UAS-Shibirets | T4T5-Gal4 x IsoD1: +;+;R42F06-Gal4/+ | IsoD1 x UAS-Shibirets: +; +; +/UAS-Shibirets | IsoD1: +; +; + |
HS-Gal4 x UAS-Shibirets: +; +; R27B03-Gal4/UAS-Shibirets | HS-Gal4 x IsoD1: +; +; R27B03-Gal4/+ | IsoD1 x UAS-Shibirets: +; +; +/UAS-Shibirets | IsoD1: +; +; + |
CH-Gal4 x UAS-Shibirets: w/+; +; R35A10-Gal4/UAS-Shibirets | CH-Gal4 x IsoD1: w/+; +; R35A10-Gal4/+ | Empty Gal4 x UAS-Shibirets: +/w; +; pBDPGAL4.1Uw /UAS-Shibirets | Empty Gal4 X IsoD1: +/w; +; +/ pBDPGAL4.1Uw |
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by NIH R01EY026555 (DAC), R01EY022638 (TRC), and by a Chan-Zuckerberg Investigator Award (TRC). MC was supported by an NDSEG Fellowship; RT was supported by the Takenaka Foundation; MSC was supported by an NSF GRFP; NCBM was supported by a CAPES fellowship; JS was supported by a Ford Foundation Fellowship. We also gratefully acknowledge Irving E. Wang, who performed initial experiments examining optomotor responses in many drosophilid species in the lab of TRC.
Citations
- 1.Hassenstein B., and Reichardt W. (1956). Systemtheoretische Analyse der Zeit-, Reihenfolgen-und Vorzeichenauswertung bei der Bewegungsperzeption des Rüsselkäfers Chlorophanus. Zeits. Naturforsch. 11, 513–524. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Götz K., and Wenking H. (1973). Visual control of locomotion in the walking fruitfly Drosophila. J. Comp. Physiol. A 85, 235–266. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Buchner E. (1976). Elementary movement detectors in an insect visual system. Biol. Cybern. 24, 85–101. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Clark D. (1981). Visual responses in developing zebrafish. Brachydanio rerio. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Koerner F., and Schiller P.H. (1972). The optokinetic response under open and closed loop conditions in the monkey. Exp. Brain Res. 14, 318–330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Götz K.G. (1975). The optomotor equilibrium of theDrosophila navigation system. Journal of comparative physiology 99, 187–210. 10.1007/BF00613835. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Yang H.H., and Clandinin T.R. (2018). Elementary motion detection in Drosophila: algorithms and mechanisms. Ann. Rev. Vis. Sci. 4, 143–163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Clark D.A., Bursztyn L., Horowitz M.A., Schnitzer M.J., and Clandinin T.R. (2011). Defining the computational structure of the motion detector in Drosophila. Neuron 70, 1165–1177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Joesch M., Schnell B., Raghu S., Reiff D., and Borst A. (2010). ON and OFF pathways in Drosophila motion vision. Nature 468, 300–304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Strother J.A., Nern A., and Reiser M.B. (2014). Direct observation of ON and OFF pathways in the Drosophila visual system. Curr. Biol. 24, 976–983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Behnia R., Clark D.A., Carter A.G., Clandinin T.R., and Desplan C. (2014). Processing properties of ON and OFF pathways for Drosophila motion detection. Nature 512, 427–430. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Maisak M.S., Haag J., Ammer G., Serbe E., Meier M., Leonhardt A., Schilling T., Bahl A., Rubin G.M., Nern A., et al. (2013). A directional tuning map of Drosophila elementary motion detectors. Nature 500, 212–216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Takemura S. y., Bharioke A., Lu Z., Nern A., Vitaladevuni S., Rivlin P.K., Katz W.T., Olbris D.J., Plaza S.M., Winston P., et al. (2013). A visual motion detection circuit suggested by Drosophila connectomics. Nature 500, 175–181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Shinomiya K., Huang G., Lu Z., Parag T., Xu C.S., Aniceto R., Ansari N., Cheatham N., Lauchie S., Neace E., et al. (2019). Comparisons between the ON-and OFF-edge motion pathways in the Drosophila brain. eLife 8, e40025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Bausenwein B., Dittrich A., and Fischbach K.-F. (1992). The optic lobe of Drosophila melanogaster. Cell Tissue Res. 267, 17–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Henning M., Ramos-Traslosheros G., Gür B., and Silies M. (2022). Populations of local direction–selective cells encode global motion patterns generated by self-motion. Science advances 8, eabi7112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Mauss A.S., Pankova K., Arenz A., Nern A., Rubin G.M., and Borst A. (2015). Neural circuit to integrate opposing motions in the visual field. Cell 162, 351–362. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Schnell B., Raghu S.V., Nern A., and Borst A. (2012). Columnar cells necessary for motion responses of wide-field visual interneurons in Drosophila. J. Comp. Physiol. A 198, 389–395. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Barnhart E.L., Wang I.E., Wei H., Desplan C., and Clandinin T.R. (2018). Sequential nonlinear filtering of local motion cues by global motion circuits. Neuron 100, 229–243. e223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Joesch M., Plett J., Borst A., and Reiff D. (2008). Response properties of motion-sensitive visual interneurons in the lobula plate of Drosophila melanogaster. Curr. Biol. 18, 368–374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Kim A.J., Fenk L.M., Lyu C., and Maimon G. (2017). Quantitative predictions orchestrate visual signaling in Drosophila. Cell 168, 280–294. e212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Haikala V., Joesch M., Borst A., and Mauss A.S. (2013). Optogenetic control of fly optomotor responses. J. Neurosci. 33, 13927–13934. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Krapp H.G., and Hengstenberg R. (1996). Estimation of self-motion by optic flow processing in single visual interneurons. Nature 384, 463–466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Götz K.G. (1970). Fractionation of Drosophila Populations According to Optomotor Traits. Journal of Experimental Biology 52, 419–436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Götz K. (1964). Optomotorische untersuchung des visuellen systems einiger augenmutanten der fruchtfliege Drosophila. Biol. Cybern. 2, 77–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Tammero L., Frye M., and Dickinson M. (2004). Spatial organization of visuomotor reflexes in Drosophila. J. Exp. Biol. 207, 113–122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Goetz K.G. (1968). Flight control in Drosophila by visual perception of motion. Biol. Cybern. 4, 199–208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Williamson W.R., Peek M.Y., Breads P., Coop B., and Card G.M. (2018). Tools for Rapid High-Resolution Behavioral Phenotyping of Automatically Isolated Drosophila. Cell Rep. 25, 1636–1649. e1635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Wolf R., and Heisenberg M. (1990). Visual control of straight flight in Drosophila melanogaster. Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology 167, 269–283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Bahl A., Ammer G., Schilling T., and Borst A. (2013). Object tracking in motion-blind flies. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 730–738. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Seelig J., Chiappe M., Lott G., Dutta A., Osborne J., Reiser M., and Jayaraman V. (2010). Two-photon calcium imaging from head-fixed Drosophila during optomotor walking behavior. Nat. Methods. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Bosch D.S., van Swinderen B., and Millard S.S. (2015). Dscam2 affects visual perception in Drosophila melanogaster. Frontiers in behavioral neuroscience 9, 149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Rister J., Pauls D., Schnell B., Ting C., Lee C., Sinakevitch I., Morante J., Strausfeld N., Ito K., and Heisenberg M. (2007). Dissection of the peripheral motion channel in the visual system of Drosophila melanogaster. Neuron 56, 155–170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Creamer M.S., Mano O., Tanaka R., and Clark D.A. (2019). A flexible geometry for panoramic visual and optogenetic stimulation during behavior and physiology. J. Neurosci. Methods 323, 48–55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Bahl A., Serbe E., Meier M., Ammer G., and Borst A. (2015). Neural mechanisms for Drosophila contrast vision. Neuron 88, 1240–1252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Strother J.A., Wu S.-T., Wong A.M., Nern A., Rogers E.M., Le J.Q., Rubin G.M., and Reiser M.B. (2017). The emergence of directional selectivity in the visual motion pathway of Drosophila. Neuron 94, 168–182. e110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Strother J.A., Wu S.-T., Rogers E.M., Eliason J.L., Wong A.M., Nern A., and Reiser M.B. (2018). Behavioral state modulates the ON visual motion pathway of Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 115, E102–E111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Clark D.A., Fitzgerald J.E., Ales J.M., Gohl D.M., Silies M., Norcia A.M., and Clandinin T.R. (2014). Flies and humans share a motion estimation strategy that exploits natural scene statistics. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 296–303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Creamer M.S., Mano O., and Clark D.A. (2018). Visual Control of Walking Speed in Drosophila. Neuron 100, 1460–1473. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Salazar-Gatzimas E., Chen J., Creamer M.S., Mano O., Mandel H.B., Matulis C.A., Pottackal J., and Clark D.A. (2016). Direct measurement of correlation responses in Drosophila elementary motion detectors reveals fast timescale tuning. Neuron 92, 227–239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Leonhardt A., Ammer G., Meier M., Serbe E., Bahl A., and Borst A. (2016). Asymmetry of Drosophila ON and OFF motion detectors enhances real-world velocity estimation. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 706–715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Silies M., Gohl D.M., Fisher Y.E., Freifeld L., Clark D.A., and Clandinin T.R. (2013). Modular use of peripheral input channels tunes motion-detecting circuitry. Neuron 79, 111–127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Heisenberg M., and Buchner E. (1977). The role of retinula cell types in visual behavior ofDrosophila melanogaster. Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology 117, 127–162. [Google Scholar]
- 44.McCann G.D., and MacGinitie G. (1965). Optomotor response studies of insect vision. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences 163, 369–401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Duistermars B., Chow D., Condro M., and Frye M. (2007). The spatial, temporal and contrast properties of expansion and rotation flight optomotor responses in Drosophila. J. Exp. Biol. 210, 3218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.York R.A., Brezovec L.E., Coughlan J., Herbst S., Krieger A., Lee S.-Y., Pratt B., Smart A.D., Song E., and Suvorov A. (2022). The evolutionary trajectory of drosophilid walking. Curr. Biol. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Arenz A., Drews M.S., Richter F.G., Ammer G., and Borst A. (2017). The temporal tuning of the Drosophila motion detectors is determined by the dynamics of their input elements. Curr. Biol. 27, 929–944. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Leong J.C.S., Esch J.J., Poole B., Ganguli S., and Clandinin T.R. (2016). Direction selectivity in Drosophila emerges from preferred-direction enhancement and null-direction suppression. J. Neurosci. 36, 8078–8092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Wienecke C.F., Leong J.C., and Clandinin T.R. (2018). Linear Summation Underlies Direction Selectivity in Drosophila. Neuron. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Salazar-Gatzimas E., Agrochao M., Fitzgerald J.E., and Clark D.A. (2018). The Neuronal Basis of an Illusory Motion Percept Is Explained by Decorrelation of Parallel Motion Pathways. Curr. Biol. 28, 3748–3762. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Schilling T., and Borst A. (2015). Local motion detectors are required for the computation of expansion flow-fields. Biology open, bio. 012690. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Kitamoto T. (2001). Conditional modification of behavior in Drosophila by targeted expression of a temperature-sensitive shibire allele in defined neurons. J. Neurobiol. 47, 81–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Fujiwara T., Brotas M., and Chiappe M.E. (2022). Walking strides direct rapid and flexible recruitment of visual circuits for course control in Drosophila. Neuron. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Wei H., Kyung H.Y., Kim P.J., and Desplan C. (2020). The diversity of lobula plate tangential cells (LPTCs) in the Drosophila motion vision system. J. Comp. Physiol. A 206, 139–148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Borst A., and Weber F. (2011). Neural Action Fields for Optic Flow Based Navigation: A Simulation Study of the Fly Lobula Plate Network. PLOS ONE 6, e16303. 10.1371/journal.pone.0016303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Eckert H., and Dvorak D.R. (1983). The centrifugal horizontal cells in the lobula plate of the blowfly, Phaenicia sericata. J. Insect Physiol. 29, 547–560. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Juusola M., Dau A., Song Z., Solanki N., Rien D., Jaciuch D., Dongre S.A., Blanchard F., de Polavieja G.G., and Hardie R.C. (2017). Microsaccadic sampling of moving image information provides Drosophila hyperacute vision. Elife 6, e26117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Katsov A., and Clandinin T. (2008). Motion processing streams in Drosophila are behaviorally specialized. Neuron 59, 322–335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Rivlin-Etzion M., Wei W., and Feller M.B. (2012). Visual stimulation reverses the directional preference of direction-selective retinal ganglion cells. Neuron 76, 518–525. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Vlasits A.L., Bos R., Morrie R.D., Fortuny C., Flannery J.G., Feller M.B., and Rivlin-Etzion M. (2014). Visual stimulation switches the polarity of excitatory input to starburst amacrine cells. Neuron 83, 1172–1184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Matulis C.A., Chen J., Gonzalez-Suarez A., Behnia R., and Clark D.A. (2020). Heterogeneous temporal contrast adaptation in Drosophila direction-selective circuits. Curr. Biol. 30, 222–236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Drews M.S., Leonhardt A., Pirogova N., Richter F.G., Schuetzenberger A., Braun L., Serbe E., and Borst A. (2020). Dynamic Signal Compression for Robust Motion Vision in Flies. Curr. Biol. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Frazor R.A., and Geisler W.S. (2006). Local luminance and contrast in natural images. Vision Res. 46, 1585–1598. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Kiral F.R., Dutta S.B., Linneweber G.A., Hilgert S., Poppa C., Duch C., von Kleist M., Hassan B.A., and Hiesinger P.R. (2021). Brain connectivity inversely scales with developmental temperature in Drosophila. Cell Rep. 37, 110145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Mongeau J.-M., and Frye M.A. (2017). Drosophila spatiotemporally integrates visual signals to control saccades. Curr. Biol. 27, 2901–2914. e2902. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Ketkar M.D., Sporar K., Gür B., Ramos-Traslosheros G., Seifert M., and Silies M. (2020). Luminance information is required for the accurate estimation of contrast in rapidly changing visual contexts. Curr. Biol. 30, 657–669. e654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Tuthill J.C., Nern A., Holtz S.L., Rubin G.M., and Reiser M.B. (2013). Contributions of the 12 neuron classes in the fly lamina to motion vision. Neuron 79, 128–140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Chiappe M.E., Seelig J.D., Reiser M.B., and Jayaraman V. (2010). Walking modulates speed sensitivity in Drosophila motion vision. Curr. Biol. 20, 1470–1475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.DeAngelis B.D., Zavatone-Veth J.A., and Clark D.A. (2019). The manifold structure of limb coordination in walking Drosophila. eLife 8, e46409. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Katsov A.Y., Freifeld L., Horowitz M.A., Kuehn S., and Clandinin T.R. (2017). Dynamic structure of locomotor behavior in walking fruit flies. eLife 6, e26410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Rust N.C., Mante V., Simoncelli E.P., and Movshon J.A. (2006). How MT cells analyze the motion of visual patterns. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 1421–1431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Gohl D.M., Silies M.A., Gao X.J., Bhalerao S., Luongo F.J., Lin C.C., Potter C.J., and Clandinin T.R. (2011). A versatile in vivo system for directed dissection of gene expression patterns. Nat. Methods 8, 231–237. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Kleiner M., Brainard D., Pelli D., Ingling A., Murray R., and Broussard C. (2007). What’s new in Psychtoolbox-3. Perception 36, 1. [Google Scholar]
- 74.Turner M.H., Krieger A., Pang M.M., and Clandinin T.R. (2022). Visual and motor signatures of locomotion dynamically shape a population code for visual features in Drosophila. bioRxiv. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Moore R.J., Taylor G.J., Paulk A.C., Pearson T., van Swinderen B., and Srinivasan M.V. (2014). FicTrac: a visual method for tracking spherical motion and generating fictive animal paths. J. Neurosci. Methods 225, 106–119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Tanaka R., and Clark D.A. (2022). Neural mechanisms to exploit positional geometry for collision avoidance. Curr. Biol. 32, 2357–2374. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Pologruto T.A., Sabatini B.L., and Svoboda K. (2003). ScanImage: flexible software for operating laser scanning microscopes. Biomed. Eng. Online 2, 13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Agrochao M., Tanaka R., Salazar-Gatzimas E., and Clark D.A. (2020). Mechanism for analogous illusory motion perception in flies and humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 117, 23044–23053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Raw behavioral and imaging data, along with code to run the analyses and create the plots in this paper, are available on Dryad: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.stqjq2c77.