Abstract
Visual and somatosensory spatial attention both induce parietal alpha (8–14 Hz) oscillations whose topographical distribution depends on the direction of spatial attentional focus. In the auditory domain, contrasts of parietal alpha power for leftward and rightward attention reveal qualitatively similar lateralization; however, it is not clear whether alpha lateralization changes monotonically with the direction of auditory attention as it does for visual spatial attention. In addition, most previous studies of alpha oscillation did not consider individual differences in alpha frequency, but simply analyzed power in a fixed spectral band. Here, we recorded electroencephalography in human subjects when they directed attention to one of five azimuthal locations. After a cue indicating the direction of an upcoming target sequence of spoken syllables (yet before the target began), alpha power changed in a task-specific manner. Individual peak alpha frequencies differed consistently between central electrodes and parieto-occipital electrodes, suggesting multiple neural generators of task-related alpha. Parieto-occipital alpha increased over the hemisphere ipsilateral to attentional focus compared to the contralateral hemisphere, and changed systematically as the direction of attention shifted from far left to far right. These results showing that parietal alpha lateralization changes smoothly with the direction of auditory attention as in visual spatial attention provide further support to the growing evidence that the frontoparietal attention network is supramodal.
Keywords: Electroencephalography, Selective attention, Human behaviour, Parietal cortex
1. Introduction
Visual spatial attention engages a well-studied frontoparietal network (e.g., Capotosto et al., 2009; Corbetta, 1998; He et al., 2007; Shulman et al., 2010). This network involves distinct regions in lateral frontal cortex that are separated by areas biased towards processing auditory inputs (Michalka et al., 2016; Noyce et al., 2017). The visual attention network also includes a series of retinotopic maps that start near primary visual sensory cortex and ascend along intraparietal sulcus (IPS; e.g., see Sereno et al., 2001; Swisher et al., 2007). Activity in these retinotopic maps, which represent contralateral space, is modulated by visual spatial attention; indeed, spatial attention alone can lead to activation in these areas, even in the absence of visual stimulation (Saygin and Sereno, 2008; Silver et al., 2005).
Many have argued that the frontoparietal network is supramodal, involved not just in visual spatial attention, but also in somatosensory and auditory spatial attention. Recent fMRI evidence supports this view. Specifically, auditory tasks involving spatial attention and spatial working memory engage the same lateral frontal cortex regions active during visual tasks (Michalka et al., 2016; Noyce et al., 2017). Auditory spatial tasks, but not non-spatial tasks, engage IPS (e.g., Alain et al., 2001; Arnott et al., 2004), although this activation seems to be restricted to later, higher-order maps without engaging the earlier IPS maps nearer to visual cortex (Michalka et al., 2016).
Visual spatial representations in IPS are asymmetric. Both anatomical (De Schotten et al., 2011) and fMRI (Jansen et al., 2004; Shulman et al., 2010) studies show that the right hemisphere often is more active during visual spatial attention. A similar hemispheric asymmetry has also been reported during auditory spatial attention processing (Teshiba et al., 2013). Specifically, while the left IPS processes auditory information from only the opposite (right) hemifield, the right IPS is active during spatial attention to either hemifield (Huang et al., 2014).
Neuroelectric imaging studies (using electro- and magnetoencephalography—EEG and MEG) reveal a strong signature of the direction of visual spatial attention, attributed to activity in the retinotopic IPS regions. When attention is directed to one side of space, there is typically an increase in neural oscillation power in the alpha range (7–14 Hz) from ipsilateral parietal cortex, and a decrease in alpha power from contralateral parietal cortex (Kelly et al., 2006; Thut, 2006; Worden et al., 2000; Wöstmann et al., 2016). This lateralization of parietal alpha power varies smoothly as the direction of visual spatial attention shifts, providing a readout of the direction of visual attentional focus (Foster et al., 2016; Rihs et al., 2007; Samaha et al., 2016; Worden et al., 2000). Given that parietal lobes primarily encode information about events that are in contralateral exocentric space, parietal alpha lateralization is thought to reflect suppression of information (Foxe and Snyder, 2011; Klimesch, 2012; Klimesch et al., 2007; Romei et al., 2010). Specifically, in the parietal lobe ipsilateral to the direction of attention, alpha increases to suppress objects that are to be ignored, while in the parietal lobe contralateral to the direction of attention, alpha decreases to allow processing of an attended object (Ikkai et al., 2016).
Alpha is also thought to function as a gating mechanism during auditory spatial attention in both early sensory (auditory) cortex and in the frontoparietal attentional control network (Strauβ et al., 2014). Previous MEG studies report that alpha power in auditory cortex is modulated during auditory spatial attention tasks (Frey et al., 2014; Müller and Weisz, 2012; Weisz et al., 2014), as is alpha connectivity between auditory cortex and IPS (Weisz et al., 2014).
A few studies have contrasted parietal alpha lateralization when auditory spatial attention is directed to the left versus to the right, and found a pattern that is qualitatively similar to that seen in visual spatial attention (Klatt et al., 2018; Tune et al., 2018). Yet, there is little known about whether parietal alpha lateralization varies monotonically as attentional focus shifts, as it does in vision (Rihs et al., 2007; Samaha et al., 2015; van Gerven and Jensen, 2009; Worden et al., 2000).
The peak frequency of alpha power varies across participants, which may reflect differences in cognitive function. While resting alpha peak frequency is a heritable trait (Bodenmann et al., 2009), how the alpha peak frequency changes is state dependent (Haegens et al., 2014; Mierau et al., 2017). Increases in alpha peak frequency are linked to cognitive load (Haegens et al., 2014; Hülsdünker et al., 2016) and working memory performance (Klimesch, 1999). Within subjects, variation in alpha peak frequency is thought to reflect the engagement of distinct neural networks, where different neural networks give rise to alpha with different peak frequencies (Basar, 2012; Lopes da Silva et al., 1970). However, despite its functional relevance, differences in alpha peak frequency across the scalp have not been characterized during auditory spatial attention.
In this study, investigated whether parietal alpha lateralization varies monotonically as attentional focus shifts. We postulated that auditory spatial attention elicits parietal alpha lateralization similar to that seen in visual spatial attention. Specifically, we hypothesized that when auditory spatial attention focus shifts from left to right, parietal alpha power decreases in left IPS and increases in right IPS; however, we also expected asymmetry in the absolute power, which we thought would generally be greater in right IPS than in left. We also tested whether, within subject, the alpha peak frequencies differed in different scalp regions during an auditory spatial attention task, which would imply the existence of distinct alpha generating networks.
To study the effects of spatial auditory attention on alpha activity, we designed an auditory attention task in which listeners were cued at the start of each trial as to which of five spatial locations (varying in lateral position) would contain a target sequence. We measured EEG while listeners were actively engaged in the auditory spatial attention task. We investigated how the alpha peak frequency varied across the scalp, and how each subject’s individualized parietal alpha frequency power distribution was modulated by the direction of attention. One challenge in studying alpha is that there may be significant differences across subjects in the alpha peak frequency, as well as multiple generators of alpha, which also might vary in their peak frequency as well as their topography on the scalp (Haegens et al., 2014). To enhance the sensitivity of our analysis, we therefore determined individual estimates of parietal alpha frequency and analyzed how a narrow, 2 Hz wide band of power centered on this peak was modulated by the direction of attention (as opposed to analyzing the average power over the range of observed alpha, e.g., 7–14 Hz).
2. Materials and methods
2.1. Participants
Thirty subjects (14 females, 18–30 years of age) participated in this study. All subjects had normal hearing (hearing thresholds better than 20 dB at pure tone frequencies between 250 Hz and 8 kHz). All gave informed consent as approved by the Boston University Institutional Review Board. Two subjects were excluded from the study due to an inability to perform the task (percentage of correct responses equaled chance level). Subjects were asked to fill out an Edinburgh handedness inventory questionnaire (Oldfield, 1971) to determine their handedness preference. Fourteen out of the 28 remaining subjects were right-handed while the rest were left-handed.
2.2. Paradigm
Participants performed a spatial attention task in which they had to identify a target sequence of three spoken syllables from one direction while ignoring a distractor sequence of three similar syllables from another direction (Fig. 1). At the start of each trial, a visual fixation dot appeared on the screen. Two seconds later, an auditory cue was played from one of five possible locations to indicate the spatial location of the upcoming target sequence. The target sequence and distractor sequence onsets were separated by 200 ms, allowing the neural responses elicited by the onsets of syllables in each stream to be temporally resolved. Within each stream, the syllable onsets were separated by 500 ms. To make sure that listeners engaged spatial attention (rather than being able to rely on temporal expectations), in half of the trials, the target stream began before the distractor, while in the other trials the distractor began first. The first (target or distractor) sequence began to play 2 s after the auditory cue.
A circle appeared around the fixation dot 3.4 s after the sound cue (after both target and distractor sequences finished playing), indicating the period during which subjects could enter their responses on a number keypad. The syllables /ba/, /da/ or /ga/ could be entered by pressing keys 1, 2 or 3 on the keypad, respectively. The keys were marked with the syllables to remind the subjects of the mapping from syllable to key. After entering their response, the fixation dot and response circle changed color to indicate whether the subject correctly reported the three target syllables (blue circle), or misidentified one or more syllables (red circle). Subjects were paid a small cash bonus of $0.01 per correct response to ensure they remained attentive throughout the experiment and were motivated to perform well.
Participants performed 12 statistically identical blocks, each made up of 40 trials (for a total of 480 trials per subject). The order of the trials within each block was random, with the constraint that each of the five target locations was presented an equal number of times (each 8 times per block). Thus, over the course of the 12 blocks, each subject performed 96 trials with the same target location.
The syllables /ba/, /da/, & /ga/, spoken by the same female talker, were used both for both the auditory cue and to make up the target and distractor streams. The duration of each syllable was 369 ms. The auditory cue was a single presentation of the syllable /ba/ with the spatial attributes of the upcoming target. The three-syllable target and distractor sequences consisted of random sequences of the syllables, chosen with replacement, and chosen independently for the target and distractor on each trial. All syllables were presented over headphones at a sound level of 70 dB SPL.
We varied the interaural time difference (ITD) of the stimuli to manipulate their perceived lateral position. Target sequences had ITDs of −600, −250, 0, 250, or 600 μs (roughly corresponding angular locations of −60°, −25°, 0°, 25°, 60°; Wightman and Kistler, 1992; Smith and Price, 2014). On each trial, the distractor stream ITD was chosen to have an ITD that differed from the target ITD by one of 8 increments (−600, −450, −300, −150, 150, 300, 450, 600 μs), subject to the constraint that the absolute value of the resulting ITD value never equaled or exceeded the ethological range (max ITD magnitude of 700 μs; Feddersen et al., 1957; Kuhn, 1977). For example, if the target ITD was far to the right (target ITD: +600 μs), the distractor ITD was chosen from the set 0, 150, 300, or 450 μs (there were no possible ITDs farther to the right); if the target ITD was to the mid-left (target ITD: 250 μs, as in Fig. 1), then the distractor ITD was set to either −550 or −400 μs (to the left of the target) or −100, 50, 200, 350 or 500 μs (to the right of the target). This restriction was imposed to ensure that none of the trials was too easy, with very large separations between the target and the distractor.
2.3. Behavioral analysis
We calculated the percentage of correctly recalled syllables for each one of the three syllables in the target stream. For each of the syllables, we separately analyzed data from each of the 5 possible target locations, broken down based on whether the target or the distractor stream was temporally leading. Data were collapsed across the different distractor locations.
2.4. EEG analysis
2.4.1. EEG data acquisition and preprocessing
EEG data were recorded with 64-channel Biosemi ActiveTwo system in an Eckel sound-treated booth while participants performed the tasks. Two additional reference electrodes were placed on the mastoids. The stimulus timing was controlled by Matlab (Mathworks, Natick, MA) using the Psychtoolbox 3 extension (Brainard, 1997). EEG analyses included plotting scalp topographies using the EEGlab toolbox (Delorme and Makeig, 2004) and performing other functions in the Fieldtrip toolbox (Oostenveld et al., 2011).
EEG data from the correct trials were referenced against the average of the mastoid channels and down-sampled to 256 Hz. EEG data was then epoched from the sound cue onset to the end of the presentation period. Each epoch was baseline corrected by subtracting the mean from the baseline period (the 100 ms prior to the auditory cue). After baseline correction, trials with a maximum absolute value over 80 μV were rejected to remove artifacts (Delorme et al., 2007). Two subjects with excessive artifacts were removed from further EEG analysis (less than 60% of trials remaining in at least one condition after artifact rejection). For the remaining 26 subjects, there were at least 92 trials remaining for each condition after artifact rejection. To equate the number of trials, 92 trials were randomly sampled for each condition for each subject for all subsequent analyses.
2.4.2. Analysis of peak alpha power in central and parieto-occipital electrodes
To identify the peak alpha frequency for each condition, we applied a fast Fourier transform on 1 s data segment (1–2 s). For each epoch, the power spectrum was calculated over the 1 s long period before the stimulus onset, thereby avoiding inclusion of any strong evoked activity. Data segments were zero-padded to 10 s. Then the power spectra of the data segments were calculated using a fast Fourier transform. For each subject and condition, the power spectra were averaged across trials (92 trials per condition) to estimate the spectrum for each EEG channel.
We were interested in whether peak alpha frequency varied systematically across the scalp. To assess this, we grouped electrodes in central and parieto-occipital groups based on their locations on the scalp (see Fig. 3A). The peak alpha frequency was found by determining the local maxima of the power spectra within the 7–14 Hz band. Peak detection was performed using the Matlab function “findpeaks” to detect local maxima with a minimum peak width of 0.6 Hz. If there were multiple peaks within this alpha range (e.g., Fig. 3D), the peak with the maximum height was selected. This analysis was done for each trial on each electrode. The across-trial average peak frequencies were then averaged across electrodes within each electrode group.
For visualization, the across-trial average power spectra were averaged across electrodes within each electrode group (Fig. 3B and D). If there was no clear alpha peak in the average power spectra of the electrode group in at least one of the five conditions for either central or parieto-occipital groups, the subject was excluded from further alpha analysis. One left-handed subject was excluded for this reason.
2.4.3. Analysis of individualized parieto-occipital alpha frequency power
We wished to analyze how individual parieto-occipital alpha power changed with the spatial focus of auditory attention. The magnitude spectra calculated from the FFT analysis was used to identify individualized alpha peaks (described in 2.4.2). For all subjects with identifiable peak alpha frequencies, we defined the individual parieto-occipital alpha frequency (IPAF) in the parieto-occipital electrode group. The IPAF was calculated by averaging the EEG power spectra across all trials in all conditions and across all parieto-occipital electrodes, then finding the peak frequency. We used windows centered around individual alpha peak frequency instead of using a fixed, predefined alpha frequency range to avoid biasing the results for any subjects whose alpha peak frequency happened to fall close to one of the edges of a fixed range (e.g., Fig. 3D) (Haegens et al., 2014).
We were interested in the time course of alpha power over the whole trial with a frequency band centered at the IPAF. Once the IPAF was determined for each subject, we filtered all of their EEG data across the whole scalp with a 2 Hz wide bandpass filter (finite impulse response filter, FIR; zero-phase lag; Hamming window; order 256) centered on the IPAF (IPAF ± 1 Hz). We used a relatively low order for the filter to achieve better temporal resolution than the FFT analysis would have achieved. We applied a Hilbert transform to the bandpass filtered data to extract the individualized alpha energy envelope, and took the magnitude of the transformed data. For each electrode and target location, we calculated the time course of the IPAF power for each trial and then averaged across trials to estimate the individualized induced alpha power time course (Snyder and Large, 2005). We baseline corrected the average IPAF power against 1 s before the cue onset. The mean power averaged over the baseline period was subtracted from the IPAF power at each electrode. The resulting data was then divided by the standard deviation of the baseline period. We then calculated spatial z-scores of IAPF power for each electrode on the scalp by subtracting the mean IPAF power averaged across all electrodes and normalizing against the global field power (Murray et al., 2008; Skrandies, 1990). For each target location, we calculated the time course of the average IPAF power spatial z-scores.
To determine whether the direction of attention significantly altered the topographic distribution of alpha power, we contrasted the two extreme conditions: when subjects attended the leftmost target (target ITD: 600 μs) and when they attended the rightmost target (target ITD: +600 μs). Using the FieldTrip toolbox with Matlab, we performed a group-level analysis of GFP normalized IPAF power. For each subject, we computed the average of IPAF power over the whole trial for each electrode (0–3.4 s) for the leftmost and the rightmost conditions, resulting in two scalp topography plots for each subject. We then performed a spatial clustering analysis with FieldTrip to find clusters of electrodes across which the GFP normalized IPAF power differed significantly in these two topography plots. Multiple comparisons were controlled by undertaking a Monte Carlo permutation test with 1000 random iterations using Fieldtrip. The cluster-based control has a type I error level of α = 0.05. The resulting clusters were then used to define the set of electrodes to combine for further analysis.
IPAF power time courses of all electrodes within each statistically significant cluster were averaged to produce one IPAF power time course for each cluster. Temporal clustering analysis was performed on the cluster-based time courses across the five target locations to test whether IPAF power changed significantly with direction of attention. Each time course was divided into 200 ms long time bins. A linear regression model was applied for each time bin (independent variable of target direction, dependent variable of magnitude of normalized IPAF power). An ANOVA was applied to the linear regression model and Bonferroni correction was performed to control for multiple comparisons. This analysis identified time windows exhibiting significant variation in IPAF power for different directions of attention.
3. Results
3.1. Behavior
Overall, participants were accurate in reporting the target sequence. All subjects were able to perform significantly above chance level (33%). Averaged accuracy across conditions ranges from 52.78% to 88.33% across subjects, with a mean of 70.88% and standard deviation of 10.60%.
We conducted an ANOVA to examine how the percentage of correct responses varied across conditions. Given the sample size (N = 28), we checked the normality of the sample distributions using the Lilliefors normality test and found that all distributions passed (P > 0.05) before performing parametric statistical tests. The multi-way ANOVA had main factors of target location (five ITDs), leading stream (target or distractor), and syllable temporal position (first, second, or third). There was no main effect of target location [F(4,839) = 1.14, P = 0.34, η2 = 0.0046] or of leading stream [F(1,839) = 2.41, P = 0.12, η2 = 0.0025]. Furthermore, there was no significant interaction between target location and either of the other factors [target location × leading stream: F(4,839) = 1.22, P = 0.30, η2 = 0.005; target location × syllable position: F(8,839) = 0.22, P = 0.99, η2 = 0.018], indicating that task performance did not vary with target location. However, there was a significant main effect of syllable position [F(2,839) = 11.1, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.023] and a significant interaction between syllable position and leading stream [F(2,839) = 63.9, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.13].
Fig. 2 shows the percentage of correctly recalling each syllable in the target leading (left panel) and distractor leading (right panel) conditions (collapsing across target location). The data show that when the target leads, performance decreases from the first target syllable to the last target syllable, while this pattern reverses when the target lags.
To test the significance of these observations, we did post-hoc tests. We corrected for multiple comparisons by calculating post-hoc test statistics using the Benjamini-Hochberg FDR correction (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995). Our post hoc tests (paired t-tests) showed that the percentage of correct response decreases systematically with syllable number when the target stream leads (syllables 1 vs. 2, t(27) = 9.83, P < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.86; syllables 1 vs. 3, t(27) = 9.49, P < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.79; syllables 2 vs. 3, t(27) = 3.78, P < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.71), but performance improves from syllable to syllable when the distractor stream leads; (syllables 1 vs. 2, t(27) = 4.94, P < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.93; syllables 1 vs. 3, t(27) = 6.99, P < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.32; syllables 2 vs. 3, t(27) = 3.80, P < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.72).
We tested whether handedness influenced behavioral performance using a two-sample t-test at the group level. We found no significant difference between left-handed and right-handed groups’ performance [t(14) = 0.53, P = 0.60, Cohen’s d = 0.20].
3.2. Peak alpha power in central and parieto-occipital electrodes
For each subject and target location, we computed the frequency that had the greatest average power within the alpha range separately for central electrodes and parieto-occipital electrodes. These results, shown in Fig. 3, suggest that alpha peak frequency is higher in parieto-occipital electrodes than in central electrodes.
We explored the statistical significance of these observations by conducting a multi-way repeated-measure ANOVA on peak alpha frequency with main factors of electrode group (central and parieto-occipital) and target location (five ITD values). There was no significant main effect of target location on alpha peak frequency [F(4,249) = 1.63, P = 0.12, η2 = 0.0013)] and no significant interaction between target location and electrode group [F(4,249) = 0.80, P = 0.53, η2 = 0.0006]. However, there were significant main effects of both subject identity [F(24,249) = 220, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.92] and electrode group [F(1,249) = 59.27, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.010], as well as significant interactions of subject identity with both electrode group [F(24,249) = 6.61, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.028] and target location [F(96,249) = 1.51, P = 0.022, η2 = 0.025]. These results suggest that while peak alpha frequency varies across subjects, there is a consistent difference in the peak alpha frequency in central and parieto-occipital electrodes. Post-hoc t-test reveals that the alpha peak frequency is generally higher in the parieto-occipital region than in the central region [t(24) = 2.99, P = 0.006; , Cohen’s d = 0.60 Fig. 3B and C].
We also conducted a group level two-sample t-test to examine whether alpha peak frequency varied with handedness. We did not find any significant difference in peak alpha frequency between the left-handed and the right-handed groups [t(23) = 1.37, P = 0.18, Cohen’s d = 0.55].
3.3. Individualized parieto-occipital alpha frequency power
We explored how the topographic distribution of individual parieto-occipital alpha frequency power changed with the spatial focus of auditory attention. Fig. 4A shows a scalp topography plot of the difference, at each electrode, between the average IPAF power when attending far left (target ITD: 600 μs) and far right (target ITD: 600 μs). Significant electrodes from spatial clustering results are overlaid on the topography plot. A positive cluster was found on the left parieto-occipital region (P = 0.007) and a negative cluster was found on the right hemisphere (P = 0.009). This result is consistent consistent with the previous literature, which reports that alpha oscillation power decreases in the hemisphere contralateral to an attended location and increases in the hemisphere ipsilateral to an attended location (Banerjee et al., 2011; Kelly et al., 2006; Klimesch, 2012; Worden et al., 2000).
To investigate the influence of handedness on alpha lateralization, we calculated the degree of alpha lateralization for each subject in each attention condition. Specifically, for each target direction, we subtracted the IPAF power averaged across the right hemisphere cluster (which is generally negative for attention to the left and positive for attention to the right) from the IPAF power averaged across the left hemisphere cluster (which is generally positive for attention to the left and negative for attention to the right). ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of target location on lateralization (F(4,129) = 7.33, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.19) but no significant effect of handedness (F(2,129) = 2.68, P = 0.10, η2 = 0.018) and no significant interaction between handedness and attention focus (F(4,129) = 0.08, P = 0.99, η2 = 0.0022).
Within each cluster, the average was taken to render the time courses of IPAF power in each attention focus condition (Fig. 4B and C). The IPAF power time courses reveal that alpha power increases systematically as the focus of spatial attention shifts from left to the right in the left parieto-occipital cluster, and decreases systematically in the right parieto-occipital cluster.
To test for the significance of these effects, we performed a linear regression analysis for each 200 ms long time bin. Bonferroni corrected results revealed that alpha power varies significantly with direction of attention in a number of time periods. In the left parieto-occipital cluster, alpha power changed significantly with target direction immediately after the cue (0.2–0.8 s) and again from right before the target/distractor sound began until it finished played (1.6–3.2 s; Fig. 4B). The opposite trend is seen throughout the trial in the right parieto-occipital cluster; however, this variation was only statistically significant immediately after the cue appeared (0–0.8 s; Fig. 4C).
Given that both left and right parieto-occipital clusters show significant differences in alpha power during the preparatory period, we undertook a final analysis to quantify how alpha power varied across the scalp with direction of attentional focus. To this end, for each electrode we averaged alpha power in the preparation period (0.5 – 2 s, after the onset of the cue for where to attend, but before the onset of the target and distractor stimuli) for each target location.
Fig. 5A shows these average alpha power values across the scalp. As attentional focus shifts from left to right, there is a clear change in the power of alpha in parieto-occipital electrodes: alpha decreases systematically in the left parieto-occipital electrodes and increases systematically in the right parieto-occipital electrodes. To visualize these changes, we took the average alpha power over the left and right parieto-occipital clusters identified above (see Fig. 4A), and plotted the mean activity as a function of target ITD (Fig. 5B). These results show that in left parieto-occipital electrodes, alpha power is significantly greater than baseline when attention is directed ipsilaterally (far left of the left panel of Fig. 5B); decreases as attention shifts to contralateral, right exocentric space (moving from left to right in the panel); and is significantly below baseline (reduced alpha) when attention is focused on the right. Consistent with past work on parieto-occipital responses during attention, our results are not symmetric (Haegens et al., 2011; Ikkai et al., 2016). In the right parieto-occipital cluster, alpha power is greater than baseline when attention is directed ipsilaterally (far right of the right panel of Fig. 5B); decreases as attention shifts to contralateral, left exocentric space (moving from right to left in the panel); but never falls significantly below baseline, even when attention is focused on the far left.
We analyzed the relationship between performance and alpha lateralization. Response accuracy was averaged across the syllables and then across the five attention locations for each subject. Parietal alpha lateralization for each location was calculated by subtracting the average alpha power in the right hemisphere cluster from that in the left hemisphere cluster. The absolute amplitude of alpha lateralization was then averaged across locations for each subject. We found no correlation between alpha lateralization and behavioral performance in either the target-leading condition (Pearson Correlation: r = −0.11, P = 0.61) or the distractor-leading condition (Pearson Correlation: r = 0.093, P = 0.65).
4. Discussion
We tested subjects engaged in a challenging auditory spatial attention task and observed how alpha oscillation power changed during task performance. Importantly, the task was designed to require sustained auditory spatial attention: two competing streams, each a sequence of three syllables selected from the same set of tokens, spoken by the same talker, and overlapping in their time of presentation, were presented with two different ITDs. Subjects were asked to report the target sequence from the direction indicated by an auditory cue at the start of each trial, while ignoring the distractor (from an unknown direction). In order to force listeners to rely on spatial cues, which of the streams began first was random from trial to trial, and counter balanced.
Behaviorally, the pattern of behavioral results depended on the exact stimulus configuration. Specifically, when the target began before the distractor, listeners were very good at reporting the first target syllable; however, they were worse at reporting the second, and even worse at reporting the third. Given that the distractor began playing before the second syllable, this decrease in performance with syllable number is not very surprising, and likely reflects a combination of both energetic masking (e.g., see Arbogast et al., 2002; Brungart, 2001) and more central, cognitive interference (e.g., see Shinn-Cunningham, 2008). The opposite pattern occurred when the distractor began first: performance was worst for the first syllable, better for the middle syllable, and best for the final syllable. Again, this pattern makes sense. The sudden onset of the distractor at the start of the presentation undoubtedly grabs attention involuntarily (see, for example, Buschman and Miller, 2007; Conway et al., 2001; Elhilali et al., 2009). The first target syllable begins only 200 ms after the distractor; this brief delay is close to the limit for how quickly listeners can shift attention away from the salient distractor onset, which impacts the ability to report the first target syllable. Over time, spatial attention to the correct stream builds up, leading to better focus as the presentation continues, consistent with some previous studies of auditory attention (Best et al., 2008; Dai et al., 2018). Moreover, the distractor sequence ends before the third target syllable begins. Together, these effects lead to an improvement in performance from syllable to syllable on trials where the distractor begins first. It is worth noting that listener performance varied greatly; however, we elected not to exclude any subjects based on their behavioral performance. It is possible that low-performing subjects did not strongly engage spatial attention compared to high-performing subjects. However, if “bad” subjects did not engage spatial attention, it would have lead us to underestimate the alpha lateralization pattern that arises during auditory spatial attention. Still, we found significant and monotonically changing alpha lateralization with auditory spatial attention. In addition, most listeners were able to perform the task well, showing that they relied on auditory spatial cues to perform the task.
4.1. There are significant subject differences in alpha peak frequency
We observed that alpha peak frequency varied across individuals, consistent with previous reports (Basar, 2012; Bodenmann et al., 2009; Klimesch, 1999). For instance, we found that during the task, the peak alpha frequency in individual listeners’ parieto-occipital electrodes ranged from 9.32 to 11.19 Hz (standard deviation of 0.55 Hz). Using a fixed alpha range (typically 7–14 Hz) might skew estimates of alpha power for subjects whose peak frequency is close to one of the edges of a predefined range. For instance, for a subject with a peak frequency of 9 Hz, a fixed alpha range of 7–14 Hz would extend from −2 Hz to 5 Hz relative to the peak frequency. This observation argues for the importance of analyzing alpha based on individual alpha peak frequencies (Haegens et al., 2014). We, therefore, estimated the alpha peak frequency for each subject and used this to estimate alpha power in all subsequent analysis.
4.2. Alpha peak frequencies differ in central and parieto-occipital electrodes, consistent with multiple alpha sources
We separately analyzed the dominant frequency of alpha power in central and parieto-occipital electrodes. We found clear peaks in central and parieto-occipital sensors. Moreover, we found consistent differences in the frequency of the dominant alpha peak in central versus parieto-occipital electrodes. Specifically, the peak frequency of alpha in the central electrodes is significantly lower than in the parieto-occipital electrodes: 19 out of 25 subjects showed peak frequencies of central alpha that were lower than for parieto-occipital alpha (see Fig. 3). This difference in alpha peak frequency likely reflects multiple generators of alpha activity during auditory tasks, leading to different scalp topographies (one stronger over central electrodes and one stronger over parieto-occipital electrodes).
A number of previous studies have reported changes in alpha oscillation power during challenging auditory tasks. However, different studies attribute different roles to these oscillations and what they signify. For instance, previous studies have reported that during auditory spatial attention, alpha activity tends to lateralize, increasing ipsilateral to the direction of attention and decreasing contralateral to the direction of attention in both temporal (e.g., Hartmann et al., 2012) and posterior brain regions (e.g., Banerjee et al., 2011; Wöstmann et al., 2016). Increases in alpha power have been associated with increases in cognitive load in temporal as well as posterior portions of the brain (Van Dijk et al., 2010; Wilsch and Obleser, 2016; Wöstmann et al., 2017), and alpha power increases with listening effort (Lim et al., 2015; Wostmann et al., 2015). Prestimulus alpha has been shown to reflect decision processes (Wöstmann et al., 2019). The current study is consistent with the various reports of alpha power reflecting a range of different functions during auditory task performance; our results suggest that during our auditory spatial attention task there may be multiple generators of alpha, which may come from different neural regions and thus reflect different cognitive processes (see also Weisz et al., 2014). Indeed, the point that multiple alpha generators likely contribute during different auditory tasks has been put forth in a recent review paper (Strauβ et al., 2014).
Previous studies have shown that in addition to affecting alpha power, task engagement and even task load can influence peak alpha frequency (e.g., Basar, 2012; Haegens et al., 2014). In the current task, even though we expected to see (and saw) changes in parietal alpha power with the direction of attention (see the discussion below), we did not find any changes in alpha peak frequency when we varied target location. This result makes sense, given that task performance (and thus task difficulty) was similar for different target locations. Our results are consistent with the idea that, regardless of the specific direction of attention (target location), the same brain networks are engaged in performing the same basic cognitive functions during the task, which leads to the same frequencies of alpha oscillations across all conditions.
The alpha power we observe in central electrodes central alpha range oscillation could be a mu rhythm, related to motor planning (Llanos et al., 2013; Sabate et al., 2012). The frequency range of mu rhythms (7.5–12.5Hz) overlaps with alpha. In our task, the task-related modulation of central alpha led to greater alpha power in right-hemisphere electrodes, but not in left-hemisphere electrodes, and did not vary significantly with the direction of attention (consider Figs. 4A and 5A). This pattern is consistent with a right-handed motor response during the task, which leads to an increase of mu oscillations over right motor cortex [related to the suppression of movement of the left hand (Pfurtscheller et al., 2006, 2000; Wolpaw et al., 2002)]. While we tested both right- and left-handed subjects, even left-handed subjects used the numeric keypad (on the right side of a keyboard) with their right hands to enter their responses, consistent with the observed results. Alternatively, the more frontal alpha source could be from auditory sensory cortex, which has been reported to generate alpha power that fluctuates during auditory task performance (Frey et al., 2014).
Our study methods limit our ability to localize the generators of observed neural activity (EEG measures with a small number of sensors and without any individual models of anatomical structure); thus, we cannot, from the current results, make strong claims of whether multiple neural generators exist or where the different neural generators of alpha oscillations lie. Future work is needed to tease apart how different neural generators behave during tasks like that used here. To address these questions, neuroimaging techniques with better spatial resolution such as MEG should be employed to allow source localization of the underlying neural generators (Haegens et al., 2014).
4.3. Parieto-occipital alpha power topography reflects the lateral position of auditory spatial attentional focus
Our results show that parietal alpha lateralization varies monotonically as the focus of auditory spatial attention shifts. We found that when auditory attention is covertly oriented to a particular spatial location, alpha power in parieto-occipital electrodes lateralizes, increasing in the electrodes ipsilateral to the direction of attention and decreasing in the contralateral electrodes. These results are consistent with previous studies of both visual and auditory spatial attention (Frey et al., 2014; Sauseng et al., 2005; Strauβ et al., 2014; Thut, 2006; Wöstmann et al., 2016). Our study extends these previous findings by contrasting the topographic distribution of alpha power as a function of the lateralization of attention, testing five different target directions ranging from far left to far right: the farther lateralized the focus of attention focus, the greater the lateralization of parietal alpha power. While previous studies have shown that it is possible to decode the focus of visual attention from the distribution of alpha power (Foster et al., 2016; O’Sullivan et al., 2015; Rihs et al., 2007; Samaha et al., 2017), the topographic distribution of alpha power has, to our knowledge, not been shown previously to change systematically with the direction of auditory spatial attention. Previous studies of auditory spatial attention have generally only considered how alpha is distributed during attention to one location on the left versus attention to a symmetric location on the right, or even for dichotic sounds (a sound presented only to the left ear and a different sound presented only to the right ear) (e.g., Wöstmann et al., 2016).
The pattern of alpha lateralization that we report is consistent with the theory that alpha reflects a suppressive or inhibitory mechanism (Jensen and Mazaheri, 2010; Klimesch, 2012; Klimesch et al., 2007; Strauβ et al., 2014). Specifically, parietal cortex has a contralateral bias, primarily encoding information from contralateral exocentric space (Kaiser et al., 2000; Schonwiesner et al., 2006; Teshiba et al., 2013). Given that spatial auditory processing engages retinotopically organized parietal maps of contralateral space (Huang et al., 2014), increases in alpha power ipsilateral to the direction of attention are consistent with suppressing interfering information about events in the opposite direction. Attention to a particular retinotopic location is likely to cause an alpha-linked suppression of information in subnetworks of the brain representing other retinotopic locations. Our observation of a gradation of parietal alpha power lateralization that reflects the exact attentional focus is consistent with the theory that local alpha power modulation “reflects changes in the excitability of populations of neurons whose receptive fields match the locus of attention” (Ikkai et al., 2016; Klimesch, 2012).
Visual attention studies show that the topography of parietal alpha varies not only with left-right lateral angle, but also with elevation. Although perception of auditory elevation is substantially less precise than perception of auditory lateral angle (which is already much less precise than visual perceptionof angle), it would be interesting to explore whether changes in the elevation of auditory spatial attention (e.g., using free-field speakers to provide rich, realistic auditory elevation cues) also affect the distribution of parietal alpha power.
4.4. Is the frontoparietal network truly a supramodal spatial attention network?
As discussed above, we find that just like in both vision (e.g., Kelly et al., 2006; Worden et al., 2000) and touch (e.g., S. Haegens et al., 2011), spatial attention directed to an auditory target causes a shift in parietal alpha power (with relatively greater power in parieto-occipital electrodes ipsilateral to the direction of attention; Banerjee et al., 2011; Wöstmann et al., 2016). Given the difficulty in localizing the sources of the observed EEG results, however, this alone provides relatively weak support for the idea that the spatial attention network is shared between vision and audition.
Previous neuroelectric studies have directly compared parietal alpha lateralization (contrasting two locations only, one on the left and one on the right) for visual and auditory spatial attention, and found similar lateralization pattern but clear differences in topography (Banerjee et al., 2011; Frey et al., 2014). Indeed, in MEG, which provides better resolution of deeper brain structures than does EEG (Frey et al., 2014), auditory spatial attention was shown to modulate the lateralization of alpha in auditory sensory cortex, but not visual spatial attention. Yet, although there are differences in alpha topography for visual and auditory spatial attention tasks, alpha lateralization in parieto-occipital regions is similar across modalities. Thus, while there are multiple generators of alpha during auditory tasks, the alpha associated with suppression in parietal cortex may well reflect the same cognitive mechanism during visual and auditory spatial attention (Banerjee et al., 2011).
In addition to previous work contrasting leftward and rightward auditory attention, our results support the idea that vision and audition engage the same brain networks during spatial attention. Specifically, we found that the direction of auditory spatial attention is monotonically related to parietal alpha lateralization, just as in vision. However, our study did not directly contrast the distribution or extent of lateralized parietal alpha across sensory modalities. Future studies directly contrasting alpha power distributions at the source level are necessary to test whether “a supramodal system interacts with sensory-specific control systems during deployment of spatial attention” (Banerjee et al., 2011).
Recent fMRI work that uses individual definitions of regions of interest (ROIs) based on functional localizers supports the view that auditory spatial processing engages the very same regions that are always engaged during visual processing. Specifically, distinct frontal executive control regions that are biased towards visual processing are differentially more engaged during auditory attention and working memory tasks (Kong et al., 2014; Michalka et al., 2016; Noyce et al., 2017). Importantly, these executive regions, together with retinotopically mapped regions in parietal cortex, form a coherent network that is seen during fMRI resting state in both individual ROI analysis (Michalka et al., 2016) and that emerge at a group level from the large-scale connectome dataset (Tobyne et al., 2018). These results lend further support to the view that the frontoparietal visual spatial attention network is also engaged during auditory spatial processing.
Another piece of evidence for the supramodal nature of parietal representations is the common asymmetry seen in the information representation across modalities. The right hemisphere dominance theory posits that the left hemisphere represents information from right exocentric space, whereas the right hemisphere, while biased towards representing left exocentric space, also represents ipsilateral information (Huang et al., 2014; Mesulam, 1999; Okazaki et al., 2015; Pouget and Driver, 2000; Shulman et al., 2010). This asymmetry helps explain why hemifield neglect is common for sources in left exocentric space (i.e., in patients with right lesions in parietal cortex that destroys the only information about leftward sources), but uncommon for right exocentric space (Heilman and Abell, 1980). This kind of left-right asymmetry is seen not only in past results, but in our current auditory spatial attention data.
Specifically, previous neuroelectric studies in both vision (e.g., Ikkai et al., 2016) and touch (e.g., Haegens et al., 2011) report greater modulation of parietal alpha power when attention is directed to left compared to right exocentric space. We see the same asymmetry. During the preparatory period (following the cue but before the stimuli began), alpha power in the left electrode cluster decreased below baseline when attention was focused on the right, and increased above baseline when attention was focused on the left. In contrast, in the right electrodes, preparatory alpha power never decreased significantly below baseline, even when attention was directed to the far left. Furthermore, attentional modulation of parietal alpha was significant throughout the presentation of the target-distractor stimuli in left parieto-occipital electrodes, but less robust (and not statistically significant) during the stimuli in the right parieto-occipital electrodes.
We included equal numbers of left- and right-handed subjects in the study with the intention of studying effects of atypical hemispheric asymmetry in left-handed subjects. However, we did not find any significant difference between left- and right-handed subjects in any of our analyses. For this reason, we collapsed all of our data across these groups in the presented results. Given the number of subjects we were able to test, combined with the relatively low incidence of atypical hemispheric dominance even in left-handed participants (Knecht, 2000), this failure to find an effect is not particularly surprising. Future studies with a prescreening procedure to test for hemispheric dominance, and separating participants into groups based on this independent measure, would undoubtedly shed more light on how parietal processing is affected when subjects have an atypical spatial representation (e.g., Cai et al., 2013).
5. Conclusions
We studied how individualized parietal alpha power shifts as a function of the lateral direction of auditory spatial attention. We presented auditory targets from one of five azimuth locations by varying ITD from −600 μs to +600 μs. We found unique alpha peak frequencies over central and parieto-occipital electrodes, revealing the presence of at least two distinct generators of alpha oscillations during our task. The parieto-occipital alpha power was modulated by the lateral focus of attention, varying systematically with the focus of auditory attention. The similarity to previous results from other sensory modalities in alpha power lateralization, down to an asymmetry between alpha power changes in left versus right hemisphere, supports the view that the same cognitive processes are engaged during spatial attention across sensory modalities. Past fMRI evidence that the exact brain regions engaged by auditory spatial processing are part of the well-studied frontoparietal visual processing network; together with current results, the current study supports the idea that there is a common, supramodal spatial attention network.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health [NIDCD R01 DC013825].
Footnotes
Declaration of competing interest
The authors declare no financial conflicts of interest.
References
- Alain C, Arnott SR, Hevenor S, Graham S, Grady CL, 2001. “What” and “where” in the human auditory system. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 12301–12306. 10.1073/pnas.211209098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arbogast TL, Mason CR, Kidd G, 2002. The effect of spatial separation on informational and energetic masking of speech. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 112, 2086–2098. 10.1121/1.1510141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnott SR, Binns MA, Grady CL, Alain C, 2004. Assessing the auditory dualpathway model in humans. Neuroimage 22, 401–408. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Banerjee S, Snyder AC, Molholm S, Foxe JJ, 2011. Oscillatory alpha-band mechanisms and the deployment of spatial attention to anticipated auditory and visual target locations: supramodal or sensory-specific control mechanisms? J. Neurosci. 31, 9923–9932. 10.1523/jneurosci.4660-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Basar E, 2012. A review of alpha activity in integrative brain function: Fundamental physiology, sensory coding, cognition and pathology. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 86, 1–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y, 1995. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B 57, 289–300. 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Best V, Ozmeral EJ, Kopco N, Shinn-Cunningham BG, 2008. Object continuity enhances selective auditory attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 105, 13174–13178. 10.1073/pnas.0803718105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bodenmann S, Rusterholz T, Jaggi-Schwarz K, Durr R, Bachmann V, Landolt H-P, Stoll C, Geissler E, 2009. The functional Val158Met polymorphism of COMT predicts interindividual differences in brain oscillations in young men. J. Neurosci. 29, 10855–10862. 10.1523/jneurosci.1427-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brainard DH, 1997. The psychophysics toolbox. Spat. Vis. 10, 433–436. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brungart DS, 2001. Informational and energetic masking effects in the perception of two simultaneous talkers. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 109, 1101–1109. 10.1121/1.1345696. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buschman TJ, Miller EK, 2007. Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and. Science 315, 1860–1863, 80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cai Q, Van der Haegen L, Brysbaert M, 2013. Complementary hemispheric specialization for language production and visuospatial attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 110, E322–E330. 10.1073/pnas.1212956110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Capotosto P, Babiloni C, Romani GL, Corbetta M, 2009. Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human frontal eye field. J. Neurosci. 29, 5863–5872. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0539-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conway ARA, Cowan N, Bunting MF, 2001. The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: the importance of working memory capacity. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 8, 331–335. 10.3758/BF03196169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbetta M, 1998. Frontoparietal cortical networks for directing attention and the eye to visual locations: identical, independent, or overlapping neural systems? Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95, 831–838. 10.1073/pnas.95.3.831. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dai L, Best V, Shinn-Cunningham BG, 2018. Sensorineural hearing loss degrades behavioral and physiological measures of human spatial selective auditory attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 115 (14), E3286–E3295. 10.1073/pnas.1721226115, 201721226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Schotten MT, Dell’Acqua F, Forkel SJ, Simmons A, Vergani F, Murphy DGM, Catani M, 2011. A lateralized brain network for visuospatial attention. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 1245–1246. 10.1038/nn.2905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delorme A, Makeig S, 2004. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delorme A, Sejnowski T, Makeig S, 2007. Enhanced detection of artifacts in EEG data using higher-order statistics and independent component analysis. Neuroimage 34, 1443–1449. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elhilali M, Xiang J, Shamma SA, Simon JZ, 2009. Interaction between attention and bottom-up saliency mediates the representation of foreground and background in an auditory scene. PLoS Biol. 7 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000129 e1000129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feddersen WE, Sandel TT, Teas DC, Jeffress LA, 1957. Localization of High-Frequency Tones. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 29 (9), 988–991. 10.1121/1.1909356. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Foster JJ, Sutterer DW, Serences JT, Vogel EK, Awh E, 2016. The topography of alpha-band activity tracks the content of spatial working memory. J. Neurophysiol. 115, 168–177. 10.1152/jn.00860.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foxe JJ, Snyder AC, 2011. The role of alpha-band brain oscillations as a sensory suppression mechanism during selective attention. Front. Psychol. 2 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frey JN, Mainy N, Lachaux J-P, Muller N, Bertrand O, Weisz N, 2014. Selective modulation of auditory cortical alpha activity in an audiovisual spatial attention task. J. Neurosci. 34, 6634–6639. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4813-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haegens S, Cousijn H, Wallis G, Harrison PJ, Nobre AC, 2014. Inter- and intra-individual variability in alpha peak frequency. Neuroimage 92, 46–55. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haegens S, Handel BF, Jensen O, 2011. Top-down controlled alpha band Activity in somatosensory areas determines behavioral performance in a discrimination task. J. Neurosci. 31, 5197–5204. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5199-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartmann T, Schlee W, Weisz N, 2012. It’s only in your head: expectancy of aversive auditory stimulation modulates stimulus-induced auditory cortical alpha desynchronization. Neuroimage 60, 170–178. 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2011.12.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- He BJ, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Epstein A, Shulman GL, Corbetta M, 2007. Breakdown of functional connectivity in frontoparietal networks underlies behavioral deficits in spatial neglect. Neuron 53, 905–918. 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.02.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heilman KM, Abell TVD, 1980. Right hemisphere dominance for attention: the mechanism underlying hemispheric asymmetries of inattention (neglect). Neurology 30. 10.1212/WNL.30.3.327, 327–327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang S, Chang WT, Belliveau JW, Hämäläinen M, Ahveninen J, 2014. Lateralized parietotemporal oscillatory phase synchronization during auditory selective attention. Neuroimage 86, 461–469. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hülsdünker T, Mierau A, Strüder HK, 2016. Higher balance task demands are associated with an increase in individual alpha peak frequency. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00695. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ikkai A, Dandekar S, Curtis CE, 2016. Lateralization in alpha-band oscillations predicts the locus and spatial distribution of attention. PLoS One 11. 10.1371/journal.pone.0154796 e0154796. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jansen A, Flöel A, Deppe M, van Randenborgh J, Dräger B, Kanowski M, Knecht S, 2004. Determining the hemispheric dominance of spatial attention: a comparison between fTCD and fMRI. Hum. Brain Mapp. 23, 168–180. 10.1002/hbm.20055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jensen O, Mazaheri A, 2010. Shaping functional architecture by oscillatory alpha activity: gating by inhibition. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4, 186. 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaiser J, Lutzenberger W, Preissl H, Ackermann H, Birbaumer N, 2000. Right-Hemisphere Dominance for the Processing of Sound-Source Lateralization. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly SP, Lalor EC, Reilly RB, Foxe JJ, 2006. Increases in alpha oscillatory power reflect an active retinotopic mechanism for distracter suppression during sustained visuospatial attention. J. Neurophysiol. 95, 3844–3851. 10.1152/jn.01234.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klatt LI, Getzmann S, Wascher E, Schneider D, 2018. The contribution of selective spatial attention to sound detection and sound localization: evidence from event-related potentials and lateralized alpha oscillations. Biol. Psychol. 138, 133–145. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.08.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klimesch W, 2012. Alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 606–617. 10.1016/J.TICS.2012.10.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klimesch W, 1999. EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis. Brain Res. Rev. 29, 169–195. 10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00056-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klimesch W, Sauseng P, Hanslmayr S, 2007. EEG alpha oscillations: the inhibition {\textendash}timing hypothesis. Brain Res. Rev. 53, 63–88. 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knecht S, 2000. Handedness and hemispheric language dominance in healthy humans. Brain 123, 2512–2518. 10.1093/brain/123.12.2512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kong L, Michalka SW, Rosen ML, Sheremata SL, Swisher JD, Shinn-Cunningham BG, Somers DC, 2014. Auditory spatial attention representations in the human cerebral cortex. Cerebr. Cortex 24, 773–784. 10.1093/cercor/bhs359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuhn GF, 1977. Model for the interaural time differences in the azimuthal plane. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 62 (1), 157–167. 10.1121/1.381498. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lim S-J, Wöstmann M, Obleser J, 2015. Selective attention to auditory memory neurally enhances perceptual precision. J. Neurosci. 35, 16094–16104. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2674-15.2015 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Llanos C, Rodriguez M, Rodriguez-sabate C, Morales I, Sabate M, 2013. Neuropsychologia Mu-rhythm changes during the planning of motor and motor. Neuropsychologia 1–8. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lopes da Silva FH, van Rotterdam A, Storm van Leeuwen W, Tielen AM, 1970. Dynamic characteristics of visual evoked potentials in the dog. I. Cortical and subcortical potentials evoked by sine wave modulated light. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 29, 246–259. 10.1016/0013-4694(70)90137-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mesulam M-M, 1999. Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal and cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional targeting of salient extrapersonal events. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 354, 1325–1346. 10.1098/rstb.1999.0482. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Michalka SW, Rosen ML, Kong L, Shinn-Cunningham BG, Somers DC, 2016. Auditory spatial coding flexibly recruits anterior, but not posterior, visuotopic parietal cortex. Cerebr. Cortex 26, 1302–1308. 10.1093/cercor/bhv303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mierau A, Klimesch W, Lefebvre J, 2017. State-dependent alpha peak frequency shifts: experimental evidence, potential mechanisms and functional implications. Neuroscience. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.07.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller N, Weisz N, 2012. Lateralized auditory cortical alpha band activity and interregional connectivity pattern reflect anticipation of target sounds. Cerebr. Cortex 22, 1604–1613. 10.1093/cercor/bhr232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray MM, Brunet D, Michel CM, 2008. Topographic erp analyses: a step-by-step tutorial review. Brain Topogr. 20, 249–264. 10.1007/s10548-008-0054-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noyce A, Tobyne S, Michalka S, Osher D, Shinn-Cunningham B, Somers D, 2017. Visual, spatial, or visuospatial? Disentangling sensory modality and task demands in frontal cortex. J. Vis. 17, 1097. 10.1167/17.10.1097. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- O’Sullivan JA, Power AJ, Mesgarani N, Rajaram S, Foxe JJ, Shinn-Cunningham BG, Slaney M, Shamma SA, Lalor EC, 2015. Attentional selection in a cocktail party environment can Be decoded from single-trial EEG. Cerebr. Cortex 25, 1697–1706. 10.1093/cercor/bht355. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Okazaki YO, Horschig JM, Luther L, Oostenveld R, Murakami I, Jensen O, 2015. Real-time MEG neurofeedback training of posterior alpha activity modulates subsequent visual detection performance. Neuroimage 107, 323–332. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oldfield RC, 1971. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oostenveld R, Fries P, Maris E, Schoffelen JM, 2011. FieldTrip: open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 10.1155/2011/156869, 2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfurtscheller G, Brunner C, Schlögl A, Lopes da Silva FH, 2006. Mu rhythm (de) synchronization and EEG single-trial classification of different motor imagery tasks. Neuroimage 31, 153–159. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfurtscheller G, Neuper C, Krausz G, 2000. Functional dissociation of lower and upper frequency mu rhythms in relation to voluntary limb movement. Clin. Neurophysiol. 111, 1873–1879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pouget A, Driver J, 2000. Relating unilateral neglect to the neural coding of space. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00077-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rihs TA, Michel CM, Thut G, 2007. Mechanisms of selective inhibition in visual spatial attention are indexed by ??-band EEG synchronization. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 603–610. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05278.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Romei V, Gross J, Thut G, 2010. On the role of prestimulus alpha rhythms over occipito-parietal areas in visual input regulation: correlation or causation? J. Neurosci. 30, 8692–8697. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0160-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sabate M, Llanos C, Enriquez E, Rodriguez M, 2012. Clinical Neurophysiology Mu rhythm , visual processing and motor control. Clin. Neurophysiol. 123, 550–557. 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.07.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samaha J, Bauer P, Cimaroli S, Postle BR, 2015. Top-down control of the phase of alpha-band oscillations as a mechanism for temporal prediction. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 8439–8444. 10.1073/pnas.1503686112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samaha J, Gosseries O, Postle BR, 2017. Distinct oscillatory frequencies underlie excitability of human occipital and parietal cortex. J. Neurosci. 37, 2824–2833. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3413-16.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samaha J, Sprague TC, Postle BR, 2016. Decoding and reconstructing the focus of spatial attention from the topography of alpha-band oscillations. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 28, 1090–1097. 10.1162/jocn_a_00955. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sauseng P, Klimesch W, Stadler W, Schabus M, Doppelmayr M, Hanslmayr S, Gruber WR, Birbaumer N, 2005. A shift of visual spatial attention is selectively associated with human EEG alpha activity. Eur. J. Neurosci. 22, 2917–2926. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04482.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saygin AP, Sereno MI, 2008. Retinotopy and attention in human occipital, temporal, parietal, and frontal cortex. Cerebr. Cortex 18, 2158–2168. 10.1093/cercor/bhm242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schonwiesner M, Krumbholz K, Rubsamen R, Fink GR, von Cramon DY, 2006. Hemispheric asymmetry for auditory processing in the human auditory brain stem, thalamus, and cortex. Cerebr. Cortex 17, 492–499. 10.1093/cercor/bhj165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sereno MI, Pitzalis S, Martinez A, 2001. Mapping of contralateral space in retinotopic coordinates by a parietal cortical area in humans. Science 294 (80−), 1350–1354. 10.1126/science.1063695. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shinn-Cunningham BG, 2008. Object-based auditory and visual attention. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 182–186. 10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shulman GL, Pope DLW, Astafiev SV, McAvoy MP, Snyder AZ, Corbetta M, 2010. Right hemisphere dominance during spatial selective attention and target detection occurs outside the dorsal frontoparietal network. J. Neurosci. 30, 3640–3651. 10.1523/jneurosci.4085-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silver MA, Ress D, Heeger DJ, 2005. Topographic maps of visual spatial attention in human parietal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 1358–1371. 10.1152/jn.01316.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skrandies W, 1990. Global field power and topographic similarity. Brain Topogr. 3, 137–141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith RCG, Price SR, 2014. Modelling of human low frequency sound localization acuity demonstrates dominance of spatial variation of interaural time difference and suggests uniform just-noticeable differences in interaural time difference. PLoS One 9. 10.1371/journal.pone.0089033 e89033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snyder JS, Large EW, 2005. Gamma-band activity reflects the metric structure of rhythmic tone sequences. Cogn. Brain Res. 24, 117–126. 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.12.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strauß A, Wöstmann M, Obleser J, 2014. Cortical alpha oscillations as a tool for auditory selective inhibition. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 1–7. 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00350. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swisher JD, Halko MA, Merabet LB, McMains SA, Somers DC, 2007. Visual topography of human intraparietal sulcus. J. Neurosci. 27, 5326–5337. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0991-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teshiba TM, Ling J, Ruhl DA, Bedrick BS, Peña A, Mayer AR, 2013. Evoked and intrinsic asymmetries during auditory attention: implications for the contralateral and neglect models of functioning. Cerebr. Cortex 23, 560–569. 10.1093/cercor/bhs039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thut G, 2006. Alpha-band electroencephalographic activity over occipital cortex indexes visuospatial attention bias and predicts visual target detection. J. Neurosci. 26, 9494–9502. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0875-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tobyne SM, Somers DC, Brissenden JA, Michalka SW, Noyce AL, Osher DE, 2018. Prediction of individualized task activation in sensory modality-selective frontal cortex with ‘connectome fingerprinting.’. Neuroimage 183, 173–185. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.08.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tune S, Wöstmann M, Obleser J, 2018. Probing the limits of alpha power lateralisation as a neural marker of selective attention in middle-aged and older listeners. Eur. J. Neurosci. 48, 2537–2550. 10.1111/ejn.13862. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Dijk H, Nieuwenhuis ILC, Jensen O, 2010. Left temporal alpha band activity increases during working memory retention of pitches. Eur. J. Neurosci. 31 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07227.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Gerven M, Jensen O, 2009. Attention modulations of posterior alpha as a control signal for two-dimensional brain–computer interfaces. J. Neurosci. Methods 179, 78–84. 10.1016/J.JNEUMETH.2009.01.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weisz N, Müller N, Jatzev S, Bertrand O, 2014. Oscillatory alpha modulations in right auditory regions reflect the validity of acoustic cues in an auditory spatial attention task. Cerebr. Cortex 24, 2579–2590. 10.1093/cercor/bht113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wightman FL, Kistler DJ, 1992. The dominant role of low-frequency interaural time differences in sound localization. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 91, 1648–1661. 10.1121/1.402445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilsch A, Obleser J, 2016. What works in auditory working memory? A neural oscillations perspective. Brain Res. 1640, 193–207. 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.10.054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolpaw JR, Birbaumer N, McFarland DJ, Pfurtscheller G, Vaughan TM, 2002. Brain–computer interfaces for communication and control. Clin. Neurophysiol. 113, 767–791. 10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00057-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Worden MS, Foxe JJ, Wang N, Simpson GV, 2000. Anticipatory biasing of visuospatial attention indexed by retinotopically specific alpha-band electroencephalography increases over occipital cortex. J. Neurosci. 20, RC63 https://doi.org/Rc63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wöstmann M, Herrmann B, Maess B, Obleser J, 2016. Spatiotemporal dynamics of auditory attention synchronize with speech. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 113, 3873–3878. 10.1073/pnas.1523357113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wostmann M, Herrmann B, Wilsch A, Obleser J, 2015. Neural alpha dynamics in younger and older listeners reflect acoustic challenges and predictive benefits. J. Neurosci. 35, 1458–1467. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3250-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wöstmann M, Lim S-J, Obleser J, 2017. The human neural alpha response to speech is a proxy of attentional control. Cerebr. Cortex 27, 3307–3317. 10.1093/cercor/bhx074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wöstmann M, Waschke L, Obleser J, 2019. Prestimulus neural alpha power predicts confidence in discriminating identical auditory stimuli. Eur. J. Neurosci. 49, 94–105. 10.1111/ejn.14226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]