Skip to main content
Communications Biology logoLink to Communications Biology
. 2023 Jan 28;6:117. doi: 10.1038/s42003-023-04474-1

Distributed harmonic patterns of structure-function dependence orchestrate human consciousness

Andrea I Luppi 1,2,3,, Jakub Vohryzek 4,5,6, Morten L Kringelbach 4,5, Pedro A M Mediano 7,8, Michael M Craig 1,2, Ram Adapa 1, Robin L Carhart-Harris 9,10, Leor Roseman 9, Ioannis Pappas 1,2,11, Alexander R D Peattie 1,2, Anne E Manktelow 1, Barbara J Sahakian 12,13, Paola Finoia 1,14, Guy B Williams 2,12, Judith Allanson 2,15, John D Pickard 2,12,14, David K Menon 1, Selen Atasoy 4,5, Emmanuel A Stamatakis 1,2
PMCID: PMC9884288  PMID: 36709401

Abstract

A central question in neuroscience is how consciousness arises from the dynamic interplay of brain structure and function. Here we decompose functional MRI signals from pathological and pharmacologically-induced perturbations of consciousness into distributed patterns of structure-function dependence across scales: the harmonic modes of the human structural connectome. We show that structure-function coupling is a generalisable indicator of consciousness that is under bi-directional neuromodulatory control. We find increased structure-function coupling across scales during loss of consciousness, whether due to anaesthesia or brain injury, capable of discriminating between behaviourally indistinguishable sub-categories of brain-injured patients, tracking the presence of covert consciousness. The opposite harmonic signature characterises the altered state induced by LSD or ketamine, reflecting psychedelic-induced decoupling of brain function from structure and correlating with physiological and subjective scores. Overall, connectome harmonic decomposition reveals how neuromodulation and the network architecture of the human connectome jointly shape consciousness and distributed functional activation across scales.

Subject terms: Network models, Dynamical systems, Consciousness


Connectome harmonic decomposition analysis reveals how neuromodulation and the network architecture of the human connectome jointly shape consciousness and distributed functional activation across scales.

Introduction

Understanding the neural underpinnings of human consciousness is a major challenge of contemporary neuroscience1. In addition to being scientifically intriguing, this question also carries clear relevance for clinical practice. On one hand, anaesthesia, the pharmacological induction on a reversible state of unconsciousness, enables millions of life-saving surgeries every year—yet this process remains incompletely understood. On the other hand, patients can suffer from chronic disorders of consciousness following brain injury; our limited understanding of this phenomenon is an obstacle to the identification of better treatment options, and prognosis remains poor. Converging evidence suggests that consciousness is supported by a dynamic repertoire of brain activity213. These discoveries raise the question of how the rich dynamics that support consciousness can arise from a fixed network of anatomical connections—the brain’s structural connectome1421. Initial progress on this question has provided fundamental insights into loss of consciousness and its signatures: when consciousness is lost, the pattern of co-fluctuations between regional BOLD timeseries (functional connectivity) becomes more similar to the pattern of anatomical connections between regions3,5,11,2224.

However, existing investigations of structure-function correspondence during altered states of consciousness have typically relied on correlation or distance metrics, which do not account for the inherently asymmetric relationship between brain structure and function: the organization of anatomical connections guides and constrains the propagation of functional signals, but not vice-versa (a fundamentally different way of relating structure and function—including in different states of consciousness—is by means of whole-brain computational models2528: rather than focusing on the direct quantification of structure-function correspondence in empirical data, these approaches aim to simulate brain activity from structural connectivity). In addition, existing approaches quantify structure-function correspondence at a single scale, even though brain structure and function both exhibit multi-scale, hierarchical network organization29.

Here, we capitalize on the emerging mathematical framework of connectome harmonic decomposition (CHD) to overcome both of these limitations, by generalising the well-known Fourier transform to the network structure of the human brain. The traditional Fourier transform re-represents a signal from the time domain to the domain of temporal harmonic modes (Fig. 1a). The signal is decomposed into a new set of basis functions: temporal harmonics (sinusoidal waves), each associated with a specific temporal frequency (Fig. 1b). Generalising this mathematical principle, CHD uses the harmonic modes of the human structural connectome to perform an analogous change of basis functions (Fig. 1c). Functional brain signals are re-represented from the spatial domain, to the domain of connectome harmonics: distributed patterns of activity, each associated with a specific spatial frequency, from coarse- to fine-grained14,3032 (Fig. 1d).

Fig. 1. Connectome harmonic decomposition generalises the Fourier transform to the network structure of the human brain.

Fig. 1

a In traditional Fourier analysis, a signal in the time domain (represented in terms of sequential time-points) is decomposed into temporal harmonics of different frequency, thereby re-representing it in terms of a new set of basis functions. b High-frequency temporal harmonics correspond to fast-changing signals, such that data-points may have very different values even if they are close in time; in contrast, low-frequency temporal harmonics correspond to signals that vary slowly over time, such that temporally contiguous data-points have similar values, reflecting greater time-dependence of the signal. c In connectome harmonic decomposition, a signal in the space domain (represented in terms of BOLD activation at discrete spatial locations over the cortex) is decomposed into harmonic modes of the human structural connectome, providing a new set of basis functions in terms of whole-brain distributed patterns of activity propagation at different scales, from global patterns of smooth variation along geometrical axes (left–right and anterior–posterior being the most prominent) to increasingly complex and fine-grained patterns. Note that here, frequency is not about time, but about spatial scale (granularity). d Low-frequency (coarse-grained) connectome harmonics indicate that the spatial organisation of the functional signal is closely aligned with the underlying organisation of the structural connectome: nodes that are highly interconnected to one another exhibit similar functional signals to one another (indicated by colour). High-frequency (fine-grained) patterns indicate a divergence between the spatial organisation of the functional signal and the underlying network structure, whereby nodes may exhibit different functional signals even if they are closely connected in the structural network.

CHD is appealing for two reasons. Mathematically, it directly re-expresses functional signals in terms of their dependence on the underlying structural connectome. By direct analogy with the Fourier transform (Fig. 1b), low-frequency (coarse-grained) connectome harmonics indicate that the functional signal is closely constrained by the underlying organisation of the structural connectome: nodes that are highly interconnected to one another exhibit similar functional signals to one another. In turn, high-frequency (fine-grained) connectome harmonics indicate a divergence between the spatial organisation of the functional signal and the underlying network structure: nodes may exhibit different functional signals even if they are closely connected in the structural network (Fig. 1d). Therefore, just like temporal harmonics reflect time-dependence in the signal, so connectome harmonics quantify how brain activity is constrained by the underlying structural network on which it unfolds.

More broadly, CHD provides an alternative approach to conceptualize brain function in terms of distributed activity. The dominant perspective in neuroimaging views brain activity in terms of discrete, spatially localized signals. Operating within this spatially-localised framework, previous studies have sought to identify neural correlates of consciousness in terms of regional changes: whether pertaining to the intrinsic properties of a region, or its relationship with other regions (e.g., connectivity-based approaches). This endeavour has driven major progress in our understanding of consciousness and its neural bases3349.

However, localized and distributed function co-exist in the brain, and its regions are intricately interconnected: local perturbations can have wide-ranging repercussions (diaschisis)50 highlighting the limits of the location-centric view. Conceptually, the mathematical analogy between CHD and the Fourier transform highlights that viewing brain activity in terms of connectome harmonics (distributed patterns of different spatial scale) is just as legitimate as viewing it in terms of discrete spatial locations (Fig. 1c): the two approaches provide perspectives that are neither redundant nor antithetical, but rather complementary.

More broadly, CHD is part of a family of approaches known as Graph Signal Processing, which evaluate how a property of nodes in a graph (in this case, brain activation), treated as a signal, relates to the organisation of the graph itself (in this case, structural connectivity)51. Similarly to the Fourier transform for the time domain, applications include filtering and spectral analysis5254. The approach has found increasing application in neuroscience, albeit with a wide variety of names and mathematical operationalisations53. Graph Signal Processing is also mathematically and conceptually related to Graph Spectral Theory, which is primarily used for dimensionality reduction of graph-based data, including the well-known principal gradient of functional connectivity introduced by Margulies55, and subsequent applications to structural, microstructural, and other forms of connectivity56,57; being nonlinear, this graph-based approach can provide a superior characterisation than what is obtained from linear approaches such as Independent Components Analysis and Principal Components Analysis53,58. This latter approach has found increasing traction as a way to complement traditional analyses based on spatially localised regions-of-interest. In the words of Lioi and colleagues’ authoritative review on this topic: “Some processes may be best characterized in terms of nonoverlapping fixed regions, others in terms of delocalized, overlapping eigenmodes”53.

The spatially distributed perspective on brain function provided by CHD raises a pressing question: what insights are we missing out on as a field, by limiting ourselves to the spatially-localised view of brain function? The central hypothesis of this work is that the connectome harmonic view of brain activity will provide insights about consciousness that are complementary to the spatially-localised perspective, which has dominated neuroimaging research to date.

Crucially, loss of consciousness can occur through different mechanisms, ranging from transient pharmacological interventions to chronic neuroanatomical injuries. To make progress in our understanding of consciousness, it is imperative to identify signatures of consciousness per se, which generalise across different neurophysiological states. Therefore, here we leverage connectome harmonic decomposition of human functional MRI data to investigate the connectome harmonic signatures of loss of consciousness induced by different means: acutely, with the intravenous anaesthetic propofol47; and in brain-injured patients with chronic disorders of consciousness2. In addition, a comprehensive characterisation of human consciousness should account for different kinds of perturbations, whereby consciousness is not lost but rather subjectively altered—such as the states induced by the classic psychedelic LSD and the atypical psychedelic ketamine. Since previous work has shown that CHD can identify a consistent neural signature across the serotonergic psychedelics LSD and psilocybin30,59, extending this signature to ketamine (which is an N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonist60) will provide critical insights into CHD’s ability to identify generalizable links between brain dynamics and alterations of consciousness.

Overall, here we aim to depart from the predominant location-centric view in neuroimaging and provide an alternative, mathematically principled perspective on the neural signatures of consciousness: one that is intrinsically centred on how the distributed network architecture of the human structural connectome shapes neural activation across scales.

Results

Here, we adopt the mathematical framework of connectome harmonic decomposition to undertake an empirical investigation of the similarities and differences between perturbations of consciousness induced by the anaesthetic propofol, severe brain injury, sub-anaesthetic ketamine and LSD.

Connectome harmonic decomposition: relating brain structure and function to characterise states of consciousness

To map the landscape of consciousness, we decompose brain activity (BOLD signals from functional MRI) during each state of consciousness in terms of multi-scale contributions from the harmonic modes of a representative human structural connectome. These harmonic modes are obtained from eigen-decomposition of the graph Laplacian applied to a high-resolution reconstruction of a representative human connectome, and used as a new set of basis functions to re-represent functional brain signals into whole-brain patterns of different spatial scale (frequency): from an entire hemisphere to just a few millimetres31.

In traditional Fourier analysis, the temporal frequency of each temporal harmonic (sinusoid) reflects how much the signal varies over time (Fig. 1a, b); likewise, CHD quantifies to what extent the BOLD signal is constrained by the global network organisation of the connectome, or deviates from it. Low-frequency connectome harmonics correspond to coarse-grained patterns of spatial variation, whereby structurally connected nodes have similar values of the functional signal; in contrast, high-frequency connectome harmonics denote fine-grained patterns of spatial variation, such that nodes can have different values of the functional signal irrespective of whether they are structurally connected (Fig. 1c, d). An overview of our analytic workflow is provided in Fig. 2. For consistency with previous work, for our main results we show connectome harmonics obtained from the same reconstruction of the human structural connectome used by Atasoy and colleagues30,31. However, we replicated our results using two alternative reconstructions of the human connectome at higher resolution (see ‘Methods’), including one obtained from aggregating 985 subjects from the Human Connectome Project (HCP): arguably one of the most representative reconstructions of the human structural connectome available to date.

Fig. 2. Connectome harmonic decomposition: relating brain structure and function to characterise states of consciousness.

Fig. 2

a High-resolution rendering of the representative human connectome is obtained from HCP subjects by combining the surface-based local connections within the grey matter, reconstructed from structural magnetic resonance imaging (sMRI); and the long-range white-matter axonal tracts calculated with diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), thereby taking into account both local and long-range connectivity. b The graph Laplacian of this high-resolution connectome is then decomposed into its eigenvectors φ1…n (harmonic modes) and their associated eigenvalues λ1…n (spatial frequencies with increasing granularity). With an increasing connectome harmonic number k (which we also refer to as wavenumber), we obtain more complex and fine-grained spatial patterns. c For every timepoint ti, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data are projected from volumetric space onto the cortical surface. d Connectome harmonic decomposition (CHD) of the fMRI data estimates the contribution ωk (ti) of each harmonic mode φk to the cortical activity at every timepoint ti. e The connectome harmonic power spectrum is estimated as the absolute magnitude of contribution of each individual harmonic φk to the fMRI data at every timepoint ti: P(φk,ti)=ωk(ti). Similarly, the connectome harmonic energy spectrum is estimated as the square of the absolute contribution ωk(ti) of individual harmonics φk to the fMRI data, weighted by the square of the harmonics’ corresponding eigenvalue λk (intrinsic energy) at every timepoint ti: E(φk,ti)=ωk(ti)2λk2). The overall binned energy spectrum across subjects and timepoints is constructed by discretising the energy of connectome harmonics in 15 logarithmically-spaced frequency-specific bins, here shown for a target state (dark blue) and a reference state (light blue), following previous work showing that this procedure can successfully highlight the connectome harmonic signatures of altered states of consciousness30,31 (Supplementary Fig. 1). f Repertoire entropy is defined as the entropy of the power spectrum across all 18,715 harmonics, for every timepoint ti, computed with the continuous Kozachenko approximation The data-driven energy signature of a target state of consciousness (designated by the term “multivariate signature”, MVS) is obtained from the first principal component of Partial Least Squares-Discriminant Analysis (PLS-DA), which maximally discriminates the target state (dark blue) from the reference state (light blue), based on their respective binned connectome harmonic energy spectra.

Loss of consciousness and the psychedelic state are characterized by specific and opposite connectome harmonic signatures

Based on computational modelling, we had previously predicted that increased global inhibition should lead to a shift in the frequency-specific contribution (energy) of connectome harmonics: from high-frequency (fine-grained), structurally decoupled harmonic patterns to low-frequency (coarse-grained), structurally coupled ones30,31. Here, we began by testing this prediction: as an agonist of the chief inhibitory neurotransmitter GABA, propofol induces globally increased neuronal inhibition61. In accordance with our hypothesis, across N = 15 volunteers undergoing increasing levels of propofol sedation, we observed significantly increased energy of low-frequency harmonics and significantly decreased energy of high-frequency harmonics (Fig. 3a–e, Supplementary Fig. 2), reflecting greater dependence of the signal on the network structure of the human connectome.

Fig. 3. Loss of consciousness and the psychedelic state are characterized by specific and opposite signatures of connectome harmonic energy.

Fig. 3

Pairs of conditions (states of consciousness) were compared with linear mixed effects modelling, by treating condition as a fixed effect and subjects as random effects. Timepoints were also included as random effects, nested within subjects. Plot shows the statistical estimates (fixed effect of condition) for each contrast, and error bars indicate the 95% CIs, from the LME model. Data distributions of connectome harmonic energy for each condition and bin are shown in Supplementary Fig. 2. a Mild propofol sedation (n = 15 subjects with 145 timepoints each) > wakefulness (n = 15 subjects with 145 timepoints each). b Moderate anaesthesia (n = 15 subjects with 145 timepoints each) > wakefulness (n = 15 subjects with 145 timepoints each). c Moderate anaesthesia (n = 15 subjects with 145 timepoints each) > mild sedation (n = 15 subjects with 145 timepoints each). d Moderate anaesthesia (n = 15 subjects with 145 timepoints each) > post-anaesthetic recovery (n = 15 subjects with 145 timepoints each). e Recovery (n = 15 subjects with 145 timepoints each) > wakefulness (n = 15 subjects with 145 timepoints each). f DOC patients (n = 22 subjects with 295 timepoints each) > awake healthy controls (n = 15 subjects with 145 timepoints each). g DOC fMRI+ patients (n = 8 subjects with 295 timepoints each) > awake healthy controls (n = 15 subjects with 145 timepoints each). h DOC fMRI− patients (n = 14 subjects with 295 timepoints each) > awake healthy controls (n = 15 subjects with 145 timepoints each). i fMRI− DOC patients (n = 14 subjects with 295 timepoints each)> fMRI+ DOC patients (n = 8 subjects with 295 timepoints each). j Ketamine (n = 20 subjects with 295 timepoints each) > placebo (n = 20 subjects with 295 timepoints each). A brain surface projection of the connectome harmonic pattern corresponding to each frequency bin, averaged over the constituent spatial frequencies, is shown above each bin. See Supplementary Fig. 3 for our re-derivation of the LSD signature using the same preprocessing and denoising procedures as for our other data to ensure consistency. Source data are provided in Supplementary Data 1. *p < 0.05, FDR-corrected across 15 frequency bins.

Note that this characteristic connectome harmonic signature is not found at just any level of propofol: it is not present for mild sedation, when participants are drowsy but still conscious (Fig. 3a). Instead, it only arises for a dose of propofol sufficient to induce loss of responsiveness (Fig. 3b, c), and it is then reversed upon post-anaesthetic recovery of responsiveness (Fig. 3d) (note that here we follow the relevant literature in considering individuals to be unconscious when they fail to provide evidence of consciousness through either overt or covert responsiveness; we return to this issue in the ‘Discussion’). Therefore, the appearance of this connectome harmonic signature is tied to propofol-induced loss of responsiveness.

Crucially, an analogous connectome harmonic signature is also observed for chronic loss of responsiveness induced by severe brain injury—but only when restricting the comparison to patients who provided no evidence of being conscious (Fig. 3f–i). Specifically, we studied a cohort of N = 22 patients: although all met diagnostic criteria for disorders of consciousness based on overt behaviour, eight patients nevertheless provided evidence of covert consciousness by successfully performing mental imagery tasks in the fMRI scanner (labelled fMRI+), whereas the remaining 14 did not (labelled fMRI−)62,63.

When comparing the entire cohort of DOC patients against awake healthy controls, connectome harmonic decomposition revealed an energy signature with significant similarity to the signature of moderate propofol anaesthesia (Spearman’s ρ = 0.60, CI95% [0.13, 0.85], p = 0.020), although most frequency-specific differences did not reach statistical significance after correction for multiple comparisons (Fig. 3g). Crucially, when the comparison was restricted to fMRI− patients versus controls, the overall signature increased its similarity with the connectome harmonic signature of moderate propofol anaesthesia, both visually and numerically (Spearman’s ρ = 0.87, CI95% [0.64, 0.96], p < 0.001; Fig. 3h). Indeed, in this case we observed both significant increases in the energy of some low-frequency connectome harmonics, and also significant decreases in the energy of some high-frequency harmonics—despite using a smaller sample of patients (Fig. 3h).

Remarkably, this overall harmonic signature also persisted when comparing fMRI− patients against fMRI+ patients, despite the fact that both groups of patients are diagnosed as suffering from disorders of consciousness based on their overt behaviour (Fig. 3i). Although individual frequency-specific differences did not reach statistical significance after correction for multiple comparisons, the overall signature remained strongly and significantly correlated with the signature of moderate propofol anaesthesia (Spearman’s ρ = 0.94, CI95% [0.81, 0.98], p < 0.001). Crucially, however, this putative unconsciousness-specific pattern of connectome harmonic energy was not observed when comparing the subgroup of fMRI+ DOC patients with awake volunteers (Fig. 3f). This is reassuring, being what we should expect from a specific marker of unconsciousness, given that each fMRI+ patient had previously exhibited evidence of being covertly conscious.

Having investigated connectome harmonic signatures across different ways of losing consciousness, we next sought to further expand our investigation of human consciousness by considering the altered state induced by a sub-anaesthetic dose of the NMDA receptor antagonist, ketamine, and comparing it with the previously published connectome harmonic signatures of classic serotonergic psychedelics30,59. At sub-anaesthetic doses, ketamine induces an altered state of consciousness with characteristics including perceptual distortions, vivid imagery and hallucinations, and dissociative symptoms: a set of subjective experiences that are shared by psychedelics, and have led to sub-anaesthetic ketamine being characterised as an atypical psychedelic6466. Connectome harmonic decomposition of fMRI data from N = 20 volunteers revealed that sub-anaesthetic ketamine increased the energy of high-frequency harmonics just like LSD and psilocybin30,59 (Fig. 3j); despite lacking LSD’s pronounced suppression of the low-frequency harmonics, the energy signature of ketamine was strongly correlated with the signature of LSD (Spearman’s ρ = 0.98, CI95% [0.95, 0.99], p < 0.001). Validating previous theoretical predictions14, our findings demonstrate that the common psychoactive effects of ketamine and classic serotonergic psychedelics are reflected in their common increases of high-frequency connectome harmonics, despite occurring through different molecular mechanisms: increased global excitation arising from NMDA receptor antagonism, versus 5HT2A receptor agonism, respectively. Thus, CHD can also identify similar alterations in consciousness induced by different pharmacological interventions.

Intriguingly, elevated energy in high-frequency connectome harmonics, and reduced energy in low-frequency ones, were also observed when comparing post-anaesthetic recovery and pre-anaesthetic wakefulness—resembling the pattern previously observed with classic psychedelics30,59. We elaborate on possible interpretations of this observation in the ‘Discussion’. We also show (Supplementary Figs. 4, 5) that the connectome harmonic energy signatures are preserved if a different number of bins (25 instead of 15) is used (note that binning is logarithmic). Overall, these results suggest that structure-function coupling across scales is under control by neuromodulation, consistently with computational predictions30,59: different pharmacological interventions can either increase it (GABA-ergic agonism induced by propofol) or decrease it (NMDA receptor antagonism from ketamine; 5HT2A receptor agonism from LSD).

Connectome harmonic signatures generalise across states of consciousness and are related to pharmacological and subjective scores

It is readily apparent from Fig. 3 that anaesthesia and disorders of consciousness are characterised by similar patterns of change across the spectrum of connectome harmonic energy; in addition, this signature looks like a mirror-reversed version of the change in energy corresponding to the psychedelic state (whether induced by sub-anaesthetic ketamine or serotonergic psychedelics30,59 (Fig. 3j, Supplementary Fig. 3). These observations suggest that it should be possible to generalise connectome harmonic patterns across datasets, to establish the harmonic signature of (a) unconsciousness and (b) the psychedelic state.

To take into account the full spectrum of connectome harmonic changes at the same time, we turned to Partial Least Squares Discriminant Analysis (PLS-DA)67,68: this data-driven technique allowed us to extract the multivariate patterns of connectome harmonic energy that maximally distinguish between each pair of conditions (multivariate signatures, MVS). The first principal component extracted by PLS-DA represents the single most discriminative pattern present in the data, in terms of distinguishing observations (subjects) belonging to the two different classes (states of consciousness). This approach clearly revealed the existence of two mirror-reversed multivariate patterns characterising loss of consciousness and the psychedelic state (Supplementary Figs. 6, 7).

Next, we aimed to provide an even more compelling demonstration that the signatures of unconsciousness extracted from CHD are generalisable across ways of losing consciousness, by relating them to the underlying neurobiology across subjects. For each stage of sedation in the propofol dataset (mild, moderate, and recovery) we quantified the correspondence between the connectome harmonic energy of each subject (at that stage), and the connectome harmonic signature of unconsciousness, designated as the MVS that best discriminates between fMRI+ and fMRI− DOC patients (Fig. 4). Specifically, we took the dot-product between the two patterns, thereby measuring the alignment between them.

Fig. 4. Neurobiological and subjective relevance of connectome harmonic signatures across states of consciousness.

Fig. 4

Each scatterplot displays individual scores (serum propofol change or subjective intensity) versus the drug-induced change in alignment (dot product) between that subject’s spectrum of connectome harmonic energy, and the multivariate energy signature (MVS). a Change in energy projection onto the DOC energy signature between mild and moderate anaesthesia (moderate minus mild), versus the change of propofol levels in volunteers’ blood serum (n = 15 subjects). b Change in energy projection onto the DOC energy signature between moderate anaesthesia and recovery (moderate minus recovery), versus the change of propofol levels in volunteers’ blood serum (n = 15 subjects). c Change in energy projection onto the ketamine energy signature between placebo and LSD (LSD minus placebo), versus the subjective intensity of the psychedelic experience induced by LSD (n = 15 subjects). Note that this similarity between similar subjective states of consciousness induced by different means is not brought about by generic confounding effects across the datasets, such as head motion: for both the propofol and LSD datasets, the same MVS projections were not significantly correlated with differences in head motion, thereby excluding this potential confound (Supplementary Fig. 8). Shading indicates 95% confidence interval. Source data are provided in Supplementary Data 2.

We show that changes in this alignment across progressive stages of sedation correlate with corresponding changes in propofol concentration in the subjects’ blood serum. We observed this both for the transition from consciousness to unconsciousness, i.e., from mild to moderate propofol anaesthesia (Spearman’s ρ = 0.57, CI95% [0.08, 0.84], p = 0.026; Fig. 4a), and also when transitioning back from moderate anaesthesia to recovery (Spearman’s ρ = 0.56, CI95% [0.07, 0.83], p = 0.030; Fig. 4b). In other words, a greater increase in propofol concentration when transitioning from consciousness to unconsciousness, corresponds to a greater neural alignment with the connectome harmonic signature of unconsciousness (extracted from DOC patients)—and vice-versa when awakening from anaesthesia. These results establish both the generalisability of this connectome harmonic signature of unconsciousness, and also its biological relevance, by correlating with a key pharmacological measure that we know to be causally related to the induction of unconsciousness (propofol concentration).

Furthermore, we demonstrate that the generalisability of connectome harmonic signatures also extends to the psychedelic state (note that from here on we use the term psychedelic to refer to the phenomenology that is shared by classic serotonergic psychedelics and sub-anaesthetic ketamine, including perceptual distortions, vivid imagery and hallucinations, and dissociative symptoms, in line with previous work69,70). Using the LSD data previously used by Atasoy and colleagues30,59 we show that for each individual, the subjective intensity of the psychedelic experience induced by LSD can be predicted by the change in alignment between the subjects’ connectome harmonic energy spectrum, and the connectome harmonic signature derived from ketamine (i.e., the MVS that best discriminates between ketamine and placebo) (Spearman’s ρ = 0.57, CI95% [0.08, 0.84], p = 0.026; Fig. 4c). In other words, the more a subject’s energy pattern becomes similar to the connectome harmonic signature of the psychedelic state (as extracted from the ketamine dataset), the more intense that subject will rate the subjective experience induced by LSD. These results suggest a profound connection between the neural and phenomenological aspects of the psychedelic state, regardless of how induced.

Overall, these findings identify connectome harmonic signatures of conscious-to-unconscious transitions (and vice-versa) induced by both propofol anaesthesia and DOC, as well as signatures of the psychedelic experience induced by the serotonergic psychedelic LSD and the atypical psychedelic ketamine. In addition, we have shown that connectome harmonic signatures can relate brain activity to both pharmacology (correlating with the change in propofol in the bloodstream) and subjective phenomenology (correlating with the intensity of the psychedelic experience induced by LSD).

Diversity of connectome harmonic repertoire tracks level of consciousness from loss of responsiveness to psychedelics

Having demonstrated that connectome harmonic signatures can generalise between specific states of consciousness (different ways of losing consciousness, or different psychedelic drugs), we sought to provide one further level of generalisation, by explicitly bringing all the states of consciousness considered here into the same continuum.

Specifically, recent theoretical efforts seeking to establish a correspondence between the dynamics of mind and brain7173 posit that states of diminished consciousness should be characterized by a more restricted repertoire of brain patterns—whereas the rich mental content and diversity of experiences that characterize the psychedelic state72,74 should correspond to an expanded repertoire of brain patterns. Here, we pursued this hypothesis by quantifying the diversity of the connectome harmonics that are recruited to compose brain activity, across different states of consciousness. Specifically, we expected that anaesthesia and disorders of consciousness should exhibit reduced diversity (quantified in terms of entropy) of connectome harmonics, whereas increased entropy should be observed for ketamine and LSD.

Our results support each of these predictions (Fig. 5 and Supplementary Table 1). Ketamine and LSD exhibited significantly higher diversity of the repertoire of connectome harmonics than placebo, whereas moderate anaesthesia with propofol induced a collapse in the repertoire when compared with wakefulness, recovery, and even mild sedation. Remarkably, our analysis revealed that DOC patients who had previously exhibited evidence of covert consciousness (fMRI+), also exhibited entropy levels comparable to those of awake volunteers—in sharp contrast with fMRI− patients, who had provided no evidence of being conscious, and for whom the repertoire entropy of connectome harmonic signatures was significantly compromised (Fig. 5 and Supplementary Table 1). We also repeated this analysis with a different stratification of DOC patients, combining clinical diagnosis and fMRI-based assessment: on one hand, we grouped together covertly conscious patients (fMRI+) and patients diagnosed with a less severe disorder of consciousness based on their overt behaviour (minimally conscious state) (N = 14); and on the other hand were patients (N = 8) who were both classified as fMRI− based on lack of in-scanner brain responses, and diagnosed with unresponsive wakefulness syndrome based on lack of overt behaviour (Table 1). The binned harmonic energy signature and PLS-derived multivariate signature for this contrast (Any response vs No-response) both showed the expected pattern. Crucially, the two groups also differed significantly in terms of the diversity (entropy) of the full connectome harmonic repertoire, which was significantly diminished for No-response (i.e., fMRI− UWS) patients (Supplementary Fig. 10 and Supplementary Table 1).

Fig. 5. Diversity of connectome harmonic repertoire tracks level of consciousness from loss of responsiveness to psychedelics.

Fig. 5

Pairs of conditions (states of consciousness) were compared with linear mixed effects modelling, by treating condition as a fixed effect and subjects as random effects. Timepoints were also included as random effects, nested within subjects. Plot shows the statistical estimates (fixed effect of condition) for each contrast, and error bars indicate the 95% CIs, from the LME model. At every timepoint ti, the contribution of each connectome harmonic to the overall pattern of brain activity is quantified by the harmonic power: P(φk,ti)=ωk(ti). Note that no binning was used for this analysis. The diversity of the repertoire of harmonic power can then be quantified in terms of the entropy of the power distribution: the higher the “harmonic repertoire diversity”, the wider the range of connectome harmonics that are recruited to compose cortical activity. Data distributions of connectome harmonic repertoire for each condition are shown in Supplementary Fig. 9, and source data are provided in Supplementary Data 3. Wakefulness, mild sedation, moderate anaesthesia, and recovery: n = 15 subjects with 145 timepoints each, for each condition. DOC fMRI+ patients: n = 8 subjects with 295 timepoints each; DOC fMRI− patients: n = 14 subjects with 295 timepoints each. Test-retest scan 1 and scan 2: n = 18 subjects with 155 timepoints each, for each condition. Ketamine and placebo: n = 20 subjects with 295 timepoints each, for each condition. LSD and placebo: n = 15 subjects with 435 timepoints each, for each condition. ***p < 0.001; **p < 0.01; *p < 0.10.

Table 1.

Demographic information for patients with disorders of consciousness.

Sex Age Aetiology Diagnosis CRS-R Score Tennis task Navigation task Classification
M 46 TBI UWS 6 No evidence No evidence fMRI−
M 57 TBI MCS 12 No evidence No evidence fMRI−
M 46 TBI MCS 10 No evidence No evidence fMRI−
M 35 Anoxic UWS 8 No evidence No evidence fMRI−
M 17 Anoxic UWS 8 No evidence Positive fMRI+
F 31 Anoxic MCS 10 No evidence No evidence fMRI−
F 38 TBI MCS 11 Positive No evidence fMRI+
M 29 TBI MCS 10 SMA +ve PPA +ve fMRI+
M 23 TBI MCS 7 SMA +ve No evidence fMRI+
F 70 Cerebral bleed MCS 9 No evidence No evidence fMRI−
F 30 Anoxic MCS 9 PMC +ve No evidence fMRI+
F 36 Anoxic UWS 8 No evidence PPA +ve fMRI+
M 22 Anoxic UWS 7 No evidence No evidence fMRI−
M 40 Anoxic UWS 7 No evidence No evidence fMRI−
F 62 Anoxic UWS 7 No evidence No evidence fMRI−
M 46 Anoxic UWS 5 No evidence No evidence fMRI−
M 21 TBI MCS 11 No evidence No evidence fMRI−
M 67 TBI MCS 11 SMA +ve PPA +ve fMRI+
F 55 Hypoxia UWS 7 No evidence Negative fMRI−
M 28 TBI MCS 8 Positive Positive fMRI+
M 22 TBI MCS 10 No evidence Negative fMRI−
F 28 ADEM UWS 6 No evidence Negative fMRI−

CRS-R Coma Recovery Scale-Revised, UWS Unresponsive Wakefulness Syndrome, MCS Minimally Conscious State, TBI Traumatic Brain Injury, fMRI− negative responders to mental imagery task, fMRI+ positive responders to mental imagery task, SMA supplementary motor area, PPA parahippocampal place area, PMC pre-motor cortex.

Conversely, neither mild sedation nor recovery (during which volunteers were conscious) was significantly different from normal wakefulness in terms of their diversity of harmonic repertoire, despite the presence of propofol in the blood in both cases. This confirms that diversity of the connectome harmonic repertoire is closely associated with the presence or absence of consciousness. As an important validation, no differences in repertoire diversity were observed in our test-retest dataset, when comparing two scans of the same healthy volunteers during normal wakefulness. Taken together, our results demonstrate that the diversity (entropy) of connectome harmonic repertoire can track variations in conscious state on a one-dimensional continuum.

Role and relevance of the human connectome for mapping states of consciousness

To validate our results, we also demonstrate the test-retest reliability of CHD. In a test-retest fMRI dataset of 18 individuals, each scanned twice during resting wakefulness within a timespan of 2–4 weeks, we show that no discernible pattern of differences can be identified when comparing the energy spectra of the first and second scans (Supplementary Fig. 11). In addition, no significant correlations are found between the connectome harmonic signatures of different states of consciousness, and the signature obtained from test-retest scans of the same awake individuals (Supplementary Table 2). This evidence indicates that connectome harmonic patterns remain stable across scans of the same individuals, when they are in the same state of consciousness (resting wakefulness)—providing an important negative control for our previous results.

Finally, we demonstrate that using the human structural connectome for harmonic mode decomposition of fMRI signals is crucial to obtaining a coherent mapping of states of consciousness. The ability of CHD to recover meaningful patterns among the various states of consciousness considered here is critically compromised, if the neuroanatomical distribution of the connectome harmonics is disrupted through spatial rotation58,75, or if the structural connectome is subjected to degree-preserving randomisation, thereby perturbing its topology (Supplementary Figs. 1219 and Supplementary Tables 3, 4).

Crucially, we also show that our results do not depend on our specific operationalisation of the human connectome. Our results are successfully replicated when employing alternative methods of reconstructing a high-resolution representative human connectome—whether from combining a much larger sample of 985 HCP subjects, corresponding to a 100-fold increase in sample size (Supplementary Figs. 2024, Supplementary Table 5); or by using high-quality DSI data (Supplementary Figs. 2529, Supplementary Table 6), thereby demonstrating the robustness of our approach. In fact, using these state-of-the-art connectome reconstructions highlights additional significant frequency-specific differences between DOC patients and healthy controls (Supplementary Figs. 21, 22, 26, 27), as well as a significant difference in repertoire diversity between fMRI+ and fMRI− DOC patients (Supplementary Figs. 24, 29). Taken together, these results indicate that the harmonic modes of the representative human connectome may represent an especially suitable frame of reference for mapping the landscape of consciousness across individuals and datasets.

Discussion

Here, we set out to address a key challenge of contemporary neuroscience: relating different states of consciousness to the underlying brain states. Leveraging the recent framework of connectome harmonic decomposition (CHD) of functional MRI data30,31 we investigated a wide range of perturbations of human consciousness: propofol anaesthesia, disorders of consciousness, and the altered states induced by psychoactive (sub-anaesthetic) doses of ketamine and by the serotonergic psychedelic LSD.

To understand how brain structure supports human consciousness and its alterations, we sought a mathematically principled interpretation of altered states of consciousness in terms of structure-function relationships across scales. Generalizing the Fourier transform to the network structure of the human brain30,31, CHD explicitly re-expresses brain activity in terms of multi-scale contributions from the underlying structural network: each connectome harmonic is a distributed activation pattern characterized by a specific spatial scale (frequency). Just like temporal harmonics from traditional Fourier analysis quantify time-dependence in the signal, so the harmonic modes of the human connectome quantify connectome-dependence in brain activity across scales (Fig. 1). Therefore, CHD provides a principled alternative to go beyond the dominant view of brain activity as consisting of discrete spatial locations, offering complementary insights that are not available from the location-centric perspective.

Complementing extensive previous studies that have sought to implicate specific neuroanatomical regions in supporting consciousness2,3,3349, our results reveal that the range of states of consciousness considered here can be characterised by their specific patterns of connectome harmonics, regardless of how they are induced. Despite their different molecular mechanisms of action, ketamine (at psychedelic-like dosage) and the serotonergic psychedelic LSD increase the contribution of high-frequency (fine-grained) connectome harmonics, whereas propofol- and brain injury-induced unconsciousness lead to reduced high-frequency and increased low-frequency connectome harmonics—demonstrating the value of CHD as a neural marker of conscious state across datasets.

Recall that formally, the progression from low- to high-frequency connectome harmonics reflects increasing decoupling of functional brain activity from the underlying structural connectivity53,76,77 (Fig. 1). Therefore, our results can be interpreted as showing that unconsciousness and the psychedelic state stand in opposite relationships with respect to human structural connectivity (as encoded in the representative high-resolution connectome obtained from HCP data). In unconsciousness, our analysis reveals that functional brain activity becomes more constrained by the structure of the human connectome, as indicated by an increased contribution of low-frequency, large-scale harmonic modes and a more restricted repertoire. In contrast, the psychedelic-induced energy shift to high-frequency harmonics indicates a departure from standard activity patterns encoded in the structural connectome, in favour of increasingly diverse and variable ones78—a plausible neural correlate for the phenomenologically rich state of mind induced by psychedelics30,59,79 according to leading theoretical accounts73,74.

CHD overcomes the main limitations of current structure-function investigations of consciousness, which typically ignore the multi-scale network organization of the connectome, as well as the asymmetric relation between structure and function in the brain. However, it is reassuring that the present results about structure-function relationships of human brain activity are complementary with previous evidence pertaining to functional connectivity, whereby both anaesthesia and disorders of consciousness increase the correlation between the patterns of structural and functional connections3,5,11 whereas LSD reduces structure-function correlation12. Thus, converging evidence implicates a key role of structure-function relationships to understand human consciousness—supporting the explicitly connectome-oriented view that is offered by the connectome harmonics. Future work could proceed to bring the spatially localised and distributed approaches closer together: for instance, by studying functional networks whereby the nodes do not represent distinct spatial locations (regions), but rather they represent connectome harmonics of different frequencies, and the edges would reflect their correlation over time, as a measure of coupling between different spatial frequencies. This is one of many possible future extensions that we envision for CHD-based analysis of functional neuroimaging data, which we hope will continue to provide new insights.

By comparing different states of consciousness in terms of the same spectrum of connectome harmonics (i.e., harmonic modes obtained from the same high-resolution representative human connectome), we have shown that diversity of the connectome harmonic repertoire provides a powerful one-dimensional indicator of level of consciousness, sensitive to differences in anaesthetic dose (mild sedation vs moderate anaesthesia vs recovery) as well as behaviourally indistinguishable sub-categories of patients with disorders of consciousness.

It is important to emphasise that this result is fundamentally distinct from previous evidence of diminished entropy of temporal signals during loss of consciousness2,8085 (but see ref. 86) and increased temporal entropy in the psychedelic state69,70,74,87,88 (see ref. 79 for a recent review). Those previous studies quantified the diversity of brain signals by focusing on the temporal dimension (“Does the brain visit few or many states in a given period of time?”). In contrast, here we quantified diversity in terms of the repertoire of connectome harmonic frequencies that contribute to brain activity (“Do we need a wide or restricted repertoire of connectome harmonics to build the brain activity we observed?”). Therefore, our harmonic-based measure of diversity and the entropy of temporal signals provide complementary rather than redundant perspectives.

Likewise, it is intriguing that loss of consciousness also tends to increase the prevalence of low-frequency (i.e., slow) temporal oscillations, and vice-versa for psychedelics86,8995. However, temporal frequencies and connectome harmonic frequencies are distinct concepts, each providing a unique perspective, and should not be confused or conflated: recent work has begun to investigate the relationship between connectome eigenmodes and M/EEG temporal frequencies9698, opening the door for future multi-modal studies combining fMRI and EEG to elucidate the complex inter-relationships between connectome harmonics, temporal frequencies, and consciousness in the human brain.

We have also shown how connectome harmonics relate brain structure and function with neurophysiology and phenomenology. Computational modelling work has indicated that the relative prevalence of high- vs low-frequency connectome harmonics in brain activity is governed by the global balance between excitation and inhibition30,31. Therefore, changes in the connectome harmonic repertoire reflect the influence of both neuroanatomy (as the source of harmonics) and neurophysiology (governing their relative prevalence) on brain function. This suggests that propofol-induced global inhibition may provide an explanation for the increased structure-function coupling observed during anaesthesia, by restricting the repertoire of connectome harmonic building blocks that are available to contribute to brain activity. Indeed, our results demonstrate that change in propofol concentration correlates with the alignment between an individual’s harmonic spectrum and the connectome harmonic signature of unconsciousness—even when extracted from a different dataset (DOC patients; Fig. 4).

More broadly, the entire spectrum of connectome harmonics may be used to characterise the quality of various states of consciousness, in terms of being unconscious-like or psychedelic-like (or neither, as in the case of our test-retest data). Indeed, our analyses uncovered that the connectome harmonic signature of post-anaesthetic recovery (and, to a lesser extent, mild sedation), resembled the signature of the psychedelic state—even though diversity of the repertoire was near baseline levels. This intriguing observation may reflect the phenomenon whereby individuals emerging from anaesthesia can exhibit symptoms of delirium, cognitive alterations, and even hallucinations99,100. Thus, the similarity that we observed between anaesthetic emergence and psychedelics in terms of connectome harmonics may provide a link between the shared aspects of their phenomenology, demonstrating the usefulness of CHD for generating empirically testable predictions.

Crucially, these results demonstrate that connectome harmonics offer not only an effective one-dimensional indicator, complementing existing complexity measures69,79,81, but also a richer multi-dimensional characterisation of conscious states. Although a deeper understanding of the underlying neurophysiology will be required, it is noteworthy that individual alignment with our connectome harmonic signature of the psychedelic state correlates with the subjective intensity of the psychedelic experience induced by LSD (Fig. 4), suggesting that connectome harmonics may provide a bridge between brain structure, function, and phenomenology (subjective experience). In particular, our results demonstrate that structure-function coupling across scales is under control by neuromodulation, since different pharmacological interventions can either increase it (GABA-ergic agonism induced by propofol) or decrease it (NMDA receptor antagonism and 5HT2A receptor agonism).

It is essential to realize that the general principle of harmonic mode decomposition does not require the harmonic modes to be derived from the same subject who is providing the functional data. In fact, the harmonic modes do not even need to have a biological origin at all. At one extreme, researchers have successfully employed harmonics derived from a sphere to investigate how brain activity depends on the most general geometric properties of the brain and skull101,102. Insights about abnormal processing in DOC patients have also been obtained using graph signal processing on the network of geometrical distances between EEG electrodes, indicating increasingly segregated processing103. At the other extreme, investigators whose focus is subject-specific insight, rather than generalization across datasets, could perform CHD using each individual’s own connectome.

Our choice of using the harmonic modes of a high-resolution representative human connectome (replicated using data from 985 HCP subjects, and processed with a different pipeline) enabled us to strike a balance between these two extremes, combining neurobiological insight with generalizability. On one hand, given our goal of obtaining connectome harmonic signatures of each state of consciousness that can be meaningfully compared across subjects and across datasets, it was imperative for us to use the same set of basis functions (i.e., harmonic modes of the same representative connectome) to decompose different datasets. In this respect, CHD based on a representative connectome is not conceptually different from the traditional spatially-resolved view of brain activity, which to be able to refer to the same localized region across individuals, requires spatial normalization to a standard template (e.g., MNI-152), and use of a standard parcellation, both obtained from aggregating neuroimaging data across healthy individuals104.

On the other hand, in addition to the conceptual advantages of taking into account known physical and anatomical properties of the human brain (e.g. cortical folding, local grey-matter connectivity and long-range white matter projections; see ref. 78 for a detailed discussion), our results provided empirical demonstration that both the specific anatomical distribution of connectome harmonics, and the specific topological organization of the high-resolution human structural connectome, play a crucial role in identifying consistent patterns across different states of consciousness: perturbing either of these two aspects (via spatial rotation or randomization, respectively) obliterated the ability of harmonic decomposition to recover common patterns across the various states of consciousness considered here.

Importantly, our results revealed a prominent role of the fine-grained, high-frequency harmonics to distinguish between states of consciousness. This insight was only possible thanks to the availability of high-quality, high-resolution HCP diffusion data, which we could aggregate across subjects to obtain high-fidelity reconstructions even at the finest scale (up to three orders of magnitude more fine-grained than other approaches to harmonic mode decomposition based on the connectome, which have relied on parcellated data20,21,76,77,98,105). In fact, we successfully replicated our results using a high-resolution connectome obtained by combining 985 HCP subjects: arguably the most representative operationalization of the structural wiring of the human brain that is available to date. The need for high-quality connectome reconstruction may pose a challenge when seeking to perform CHD based on individual subjects’ connectomes, whereupon diffusion data of sufficiently high quality is not always available, and this issue could not be mitigated via aggregation across subjects. Even so, diffusion imaging and tractography are not without limitations: chief among them, their inability to infer fibre directionality, an important feature of brain wiring that will need to be accounted for through both technological and conceptual advances. Likewise, future work could extend our results by taking into account transmission delays based on tract length20.

It is noteworthy that although we stratified our DOC patients based on their performance on mental imagery tasks in the scanner, our connectome harmonic analysis was entirely based on resting-state (i.e., task-free) fMRI data, which imposes no cognitive demands on patients, unlike task-based paradigms106. Connectome harmonics analysis of rs-fMRI may represent a useful screening tool in the clinic to identify patients for more in-depth assessment—contributing to alleviate the high rate of misdiagnoses for DOC patients when relying solely on behavioural criteria106,107. To this end, it will be important to replicate the present results in different, larger samples, both of DOC patients and pertaining to pharmacological perturbations of consciousness, to ensure their robustness.

However, even failure to respond to the fMRI mental imagery tasks cannot conclusively rule out residual consciousness in DOC patients, and loss of behavioural responsiveness during anaesthesia may not always coincide with loss of brain responsiveness and subjective experience91,108,109. More broadly, to further establish the potential clinical value of CHD as a general neural marker of consciousness, it will be essential to obtain a convergence of multi-modal markers of consciousness that bypass overt behaviour, across different neuroimaging modalities91,106,110. For instance, given evidence that EEG slow-wave activity saturation constitutes a marker of loss of brain responsiveness induced by propofol91, a promising avenue for future work will be to investigate whether a corresponding saturation of low-frequency connectome harmonics can be identified in the fMRI signal. Likewise, establishing the susceptibility of connectome harmonics to intervention via Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation will shed light on their suitability as a target for treatment, and enable a potential convergence with the Perturbational Complexity Index: one of the most sensitive indicators of consciousness available to date, which is based on how the brain’s response to TMS pulses spread79,110112.

Overall, the energy spectrum of connectome harmonics and the diversity of their repertoire provide distinct and synergistic insights, to identify meaningful relationships between brain function, its network structure, and subjective experience. Having demonstrated the generalisability of connectome harmonic decomposition across datasets and states of consciousness, and their bi-directional control by neuromodulation, our results lay the groundwork for future harmonic-based quantitative comparison of different mental states in health and disease.

Methods

Generalising the Fourier transform to the network structure of the human connectome: theoretical background

Connectome harmonic decomposition (CHD) generalises the mathematics of the Fourier transform to the network structure of the human brain. The traditional Fourier transform operates in the temporal domain (Fig. 1a): decomposition into temporal harmonics quantifies to what extent the signal varies slowly (low-frequency temporal harmonics) or quickly (high-frequency temporal harmonics) over time (Fig. 1b). Analogously, CHD re-represents a spatial signal in terms of harmonic modes of the human connectome, so that the spatial frequency (granularity) of each connectome harmonic quantifies to what extent the organization of functional brain signals deviates from the organization of the underlying structural network (Fig. 1c, d). Therefore, CHD is fundamentally different from, and complementary to, traditional approaches to functional MRI data analysis. This is because CHD does not view functional brain activity as composed of signals from discrete spatial locations, but rather as composed of contributions from distinct spatial frequencies: each connectome harmonic is a whole-brain pattern with a characteristic spatial scale (granularity)—from an entire hemisphere to just a few millimetres.

On one hand, this means that CHD is unsuitable to address questions pertaining to spatial localisation and the involvement of specific neuroanatomical regions; such questions have been extensively investigated within the traditional framework of viewing brain activity in terms of spatially discrete regions, and several previous studies have implicated specific neuroanatomical regions in supporting consciousness3349. On the other hand, CHD enables us to consider how brain activity across states of consciousness is shaped by the brain’s distributed network of structural connections, reflecting the contribution of global patterns at different spatial scales—each arising from the network topology of the human connectome. We emphasise that neither approach is inherently superior, but rather they each provide a unique perspective on brain function: one localised, the other distributed. The traditional spatially-resolved approach and our frequency-resolved approach are two synergistic sides of the same coin.

Crucially, the use of the word “frequency” for both connectome harmonic decomposition and traditional Fourier analysis should not give the misleading impression that our analyses are redundant with previous literature based on traditional Fourier analysis in the domain of temporal frequencies (just like the connectome harmonic energy defined below is entirely different from metabolic energy). On the contrary, signal decomposition in terms of temporal frequencies (i.e., Fourier analysis) and connectome-based frequencies (i.e., CHD) operate in entirely separate domains and provide very different information about the signal (temporal dependence versus spatial dependence on the connectome network structure). Indeed, this means that even the most suitable neuroimaging modalities for each analysis are different: CHD relies on fMRI data with high spatial resolution, but which have a restricted content of temporal frequencies (the BOLD signals used here were all band-pass filtered in the low-frequency range as part of standard denoising procedures), whereas Fourier investigations of consciousness require high temporal resolution and are therefore typically performed on electro- or magneto-encephalography data. Please note that throughout this article, unless otherwise specified, our use of the word “frequency” refers to the frequency of connectome harmonics (spatial granularity, from fine-grained to coarse-grained).

High-resolution structural connectome

Whereas alternative approaches to harmonic mode decomposition rely exclusively on white-matter connectivity between macroscopic brain regions defined by sub-dividing the brain into discrete parcels21,76,77,113, CHD combines long-range white-matter connections with local connectivity within the grey matter on a continuous cortical surface, thereby achieving a representation of the human structural connectome at the highest resolution available for MR imaging31,78. For consistency with previous work employing connectome harmonic decomposition, for our main results we used connectome harmonics obtained from the same reconstruction of the human structural connectome used by Atasoy and colleagues30. However, note that we also replicated our results using two alternative reconstructions of the human connectome at higher resolution (described below), including one obtained from aggregating 985 subjects from the Human Connectome Project (HCP)114: arguably one of the most representative reconstructions of the human structural connectome available to date, corresponding to a nearly 100-fold increase in sample size with respect to previous CHD studies30.

Connectome reconstruction

The workflow was the same as described in previous work by Atasoy and colleagues30, who derived a high-resolution human structural connectome from derived from DTI and structural MRI data from an independent sample of 10 HCP subjects (six female, age 22–35), preprocessed according to minimal preprocessing guidelines of the HCP protocol. For each of these HCP subjects, Freesurfer (http://freesurfer.net) was used to reconstruct the cortical surfaces of each hemisphere at the interface of white and grey matter, based on the 0.7 mm resolution data from T1-weighted MRI. This resulted in a representation of 18,715 cortical surface vertices for each subject. Subsequently, deterministic tractography was used to reconstruct long-range white matter fibres. After co-registering each subject’s diffusion imaging and cortical surface data, each of the 18,715 vertices of the reconstructed cortical surface was used as a centre to initialise eight seeds for deterministic tractography, implemented with the MrDiffusion tool (http://white.stanford.edu/newlm/index.php/MrDiffusion). Tracking was terminated when fractional anisotropy (FA) was below a threshold of 0.3, with 20 mm minimum tract length, and setting 30 degrees as the maximum allowed angle between consecutive tracking steps30.

The structural connectome of each subject was then represented as a binary adjacency matrix A, treating each cortical surface vertex as a node: for each pair i and j of the n = 18,715 cortical surface grey matter nodes, Aij was set to 1 if there was a white matter tract connecting them, as estimated from the deterministic tractography step described above (in order to account for long-range connections); or if they were adjacent in the grey matter cortical surface representation, thereby accounting for the presence of local (1–6 mm) connections within the grey matter (the importance of accounting for short-range grey-matter connections in addition to long-range white matter tracts was demonstrated in a recent study78). If neither long-range nor short-range connections between i and j existed, Aij was set to 0. This procedure resulted in a symmetric (undirected) binary matrix30.

The individual adjacency matrices were then averaged across the 10 HCP subjects to obtain a group-average matrix Ā, encoding a representative structural conenctome We then define the degree matrix D of the graph as:

Di,i=j=1nĀi,j. 1

Extraction of connectome harmonics

Following ref. 30, we compute the symmetric graph Laplacian ∆G on the group-average adjacency matrix Ā that represents the human connectome, in order to estimate the Laplacian (discrete counterpart of the Laplace operator ∆115) of the human structural connectome:

G=D1/2LD1/2,withL=DĀ. 2

We then calculate the connectome harmonics φk, k ∈ {1, …, 18,715} by solving the following eigenvalue problem:

Gφkvi=λkφkviviV,with0<λ1<λ2<...<λn, 3

where λk, k ∈ {1, …, n} is the corresponding eigenvalue of the eigenfunction φk, V is the set of cortical surface vertices and n represents the number of vertices. In other words, λk and φk are the eigenvalues and eigenvectors of the Laplacian of the human structural connectivity network, respectively. Therefore, if φk is the connectome harmonic pattern of the kth spatial frequency (wavenumber), then the corresponding eigenvalue λk is a term relating to the intrinsic energy of that particular harmonic mode. Crucially, we reiterate that the frequencies associated with each connectome harmonic are in the spatial rather than temporal domain, and should not be confused with the temporal frequencies identified by Fourier transform in the temporal domain (e.g., for denoising of timeseries).

Connectome-harmonic decomposition of fMRI data

At each timepoint t ∈ {1,…,T} (corresponding to one TR), the preprocessed and denoised fMRI data (see below for details of these steps) were projected onto cortical surface coordinates by means of the Human Connectome Project Workbench -volume-to-surface-mapping tool. Then, the spatial pattern of cortical activity over vertices v at time t, denoted as Ft (v), was decomposed as a linear combination of the set of connectome harmonics Ψ={φk}k=1N:

Ft=ω1(t)φ1+ω2(t)φ2+...+ωn(t)φn=k=1nωk(t)φk(v) 4

with the contribution ωk (t) of each connectome harmonic φk at time t being estimated as the projection (dot product) of the fMRI data Ft (v) onto φk:

ωkt=Ft,φk. 5

Power and energy of connectome harmonics

Once the fMRI cortical activation pattern at time t has been decomposed into a linear combination of connectome harmonics, the magnitude of contribution to cortical activity of each harmonic φk, k ∈ {1, …, n} (regardless of sign) at any given timepoint t (P(φk, t)), called its “power” for analogy with the Fourier transform, is computed as the amplitude of its contribution:

P(φk,t)=ωk(t). 6

In turn, the normalized frequency-specific contribution of each harmonic φk, k ∈ {1, …, n} at timepoint t, termed energy, is estimated by combining the strength of activation (power) of a particular connectome harmonic with its own intrinsic energy given by λk2:

E(φk,t)=ωk(t)2λk2. 7

Consequently, total brain energy at time t is given by

Etotalt=k=1nωkt2λk2=∣∣Ftv∣∣2. 8

Since the Laplace operator ∆ represents the amount of activity flow, the latter part of Eq. 8 indicates that the total brain energy at a given point in time can be interpreted as the total cortical flow of neural activity at that time30. A binned energy spectrum across subjects and timepoints is constructed by discretising the energy of connectome harmonics into 15 logarithmically-spaced frequency-specific bins, following previous work showing that this procedure can successfully highlight the connectome harmonic signatures of altered states of consciousness30 (Supplementary Fig. 1).

Data-driven extraction of multivariate connectome harmonic signatures

Partial least squares (PLS, also known as Projection on Latent Spaces) is a multivariate statistical analysis used to identify relationships between one or more targets (Y) and a set of predictor variables X. This method extracts principal components as linear combinations of variables in each set that maximally covary with each other. In the present case, for each pair of states of consciousness under comparison, X was the matrix of 15 binned energy values (see above) for each subject (averaged over timepoints), and Y was the vector of binary classification between the two states (here, target vs baseline state of consciousness, e.g. anaesthetised vs awake, ketamine vs placebo, fMRI− vs fMRI+ DOC, etc.)—making this an application of Partial Least Squares Discriminant Analysis (PLS-DA), owing to the binary nature of Y116. The first principal component extracted by PLS-DA represents the single most discriminative pattern present in the data, in terms of distinguishing observations (subjects) belonging to the two different classes (states of consciousness).

Diversity of connectome harmonic repertoire

To quantify the diversity of the repertoire of connectome harmonics recruited at each point in time, we start by observing that a diverse repertoire is one in which different harmonic modes contribute in different degrees to brain activity—neither one single mode dominating (which would correspond to a periodic oscillation, in analogy with the traditional Fourier transform) nor every mode contributing the same as every other mode (which would correspond to white noise). To capture this intuition, we quantify repertoire diversity in terms of the entropy of the distribution of connectome harmonic power (absolute strength of contribution to the cortical activation pattern) across all 18,715 connectome harmonics (i.e., binning was not used for this analysis). Specifically, to deal with continuous data (as in the present case) we rely on the Kozachenko approximation, as implemented in the Java Information Dynamics Toolbox (JIDT; http://jlizier.github.io/jidt/)117. We note that when dealing with continuous variables, entropy can have negative values118, but its interpretation remains the same: a more entropic distribution (i.e. having a value of entropy closer to positive infinity) will correspond to a more diverse repertoire. We calculate this entropy for each timepoint of each subject.

High-resolution alternative reconstructions of the human connectome

To demonstrate that our results are not fundamentally dependent on this specific operationalisation of the human connectome, we also used two alternative representative human connectomes. The first alternative connectome was constructed from multi-shell diffusion-weighted imaging data from 985 subjects of the HCP 1200 data release (http://www.humanconnectome.org/), each scanned for approximately 59 min. This represents a nearly 100-fold increase in sample size compared with the original connectome used for connectome harmonic decomposition30. We refer to the human connectome constructed from these data as the HCP-985 connectome. The second alternative connectome was constructed from 32 healthy volunteers from the HCP database who were scanned for a full 89 min at Massachusetts General Hospital with high-resolution diffusion spectrum imaging, which can better resolve crossing fibres. We refer to this connectome as the MGH-32 connectome. Acquisition parameters for both groups are described in detail in the relative documentation (http://www.humanconnectome.org/)119, and both dMRI datasets were preprocessed and made available as part of the freely available Lead-DBS software package (http://www.lead-dbs.org/).

For the reconstruction of long-range white matter tracts of each individual, we followed the procedures previously used on these data by Deco and colleagues120: the diffusion data were processed using a generalized q-sampling imaging algorithm implemented in DSI Studio (http://dsi-studio.labsolver.org). A white-matter mask was obtained from segmentation of the T2-weighted anatomical images, which were co-registered to the b0 image of the diffusion data using SPM12. In each HCP participant, 200,000 fibres were sampled within the white-matter mask, using a tracking method that previously achieved the highest (92%) valid connection score among 96 methods submitted from 20 different research groups in a recent open competition121. Finally, the fibres were transformed into standard Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI-152) space using Lead-DBS122. The remaining procedures for obtaining individual connectomes and aggregating them into a group-average representative connectome, and subsequent connectome harmonic decomposition, were the same as described above. To ensure that our results were not unduly influenced by potential aliasing effects introduced by the use of high-resolution diffusion data, for both the HCP-985 and MGH-32 analyses we only used the first 14 logarithmically spaced bins (instead of 15 as for the previous analyses), showing that our results are not critically dependent on the precise number of bins.

Rotated harmonics

To demonstrate the importance of the neuroanatomical distribution of connectome harmonics, we assessed whether our results would replicate when using spherically rotated connectome harmonics, following a recently described approach58, based on freely available code (github.com/spin-test/spin-test)75. After obtaining the connectome harmonics following the procedure described above, the corresponding surface maps were projected onto a spherical surface, and subsequently rotated by a random angle, before mapping back the rotated values onto the nearest vertex (ignoring parts of the corpus callosum that are mapped onto the cortical surface). Since we used multi-dimensional basis functions, we rotated the surface maps corresponding to each dimension by the same angle. Note that the resulting rotated maps are not orthonormal anymore, because each rotated map is symmetrised to preserve this important property. We then proceeded with the normal CHD analysis workflow as described above.

Randomised connectome

To demonstrate the importance of the specific topology of the human connectome, obtained by combining local grey matter connectivity and long-range white matter fibres, we also tested whether our results would replicate when using harmonics obtained from a randomised connectome78. Before performing Laplacian decomposition, the original connectome was therefore turned into a random network using the degree-preserving procedure implemented in the Brain Connectivity Toolbox123. Harmonics were then extracted from Laplacian eigendecomposition, and the full connectome harmonic decomposition pipeline was followed. As for the validation analyses using alternative operationalisations of the human connectome, for this analysis we also only used the first 14 logarithmically spaced bins.

Propofol dataset

The propofol data used in this study have been published before. For clarity and consistency of reporting, where applicable we use the same wording as in our previous publications with these data47,48. Sixteen healthy volunteer subjects were recruited for scanning. In addition to the original 16 volunteers, data were acquired for nine additional participants using the same procedures, bringing the total number of participants in this dataset to 25 (11 males, 14 females; mean age 34.7 years, SD = 9.0 years). Ethical approval for these studies was obtained from the Cambridgeshire 2 Regional Ethics Committee, and all subjects gave informed consent to participate in the study. Volunteers were informed of the risks of propofol administration, such as loss of consciousness, respiratory and cardiovascular depression. They were also informed about more minor effects of propofol such as pain on injection, sedation and amnesia. In addition, standard information about intravenous cannulation, blood sampling and MRI scanning was provided. The GABA-ergic intravenous agent propofol is one of the most commonly used anaesthetic drugs, owing to the stability and predictability of its effects. Three target plasma levels of propofol were used—no drug (Awake), 0.6 mg/ml (Mild sedation) and 1.2 mg/ml (Moderate sedation). Scanning (rs-fMRI) was acquired at each stage, and also at Recovery; anatomical images were also acquired. The level of sedation was assessed verbally immediately before and after each of the scanning runs.

Propofol study: infusion protocol

Propofol was administered intravenously as a “target controlled infusion” (plasma concentration mode), using an Alaris PK infusion pump (Carefusion, Basingstoke, UK)47,48. A period of 10 min was allowed for equilibration of plasma and effect-site propofol concentrations. Blood samples were drawn towards the end of each titration period and before the plasma target was altered, to assess plasma propofol levels. In total, 6 blood samples were drawn during the study. The mean (SD) measured plasma propofol concentration was 304.8 (141.1) ng/ml during mild sedation, 723.3 (320.5) ng/ml during moderate sedation and 275.8 (75.42) ng/ml during recovery. Mean (SD) total mass of propofol administered was 210.15 (33.17) mg, equivalent to 3.0 (0.47) mg/kg. Two senior anaesthetists were present during scanning sessions and observed the subjects throughout the study from the MRI control room and on a video link that showed the subject in the scanner. Electrocardiography and pulse oximetry were performed continuously, and measurements of heart rate, non-invasive blood pressure, and oxygen saturation were recorded at regular intervals47,48.

Propofol study: MRI data acquisition

The acquisition procedures are described in detail in the original study47. Briefly, MRI data were acquired on a Siemens Trio 3T scanner (WBIC, Cambridge). For each level of sedation, 150 rs-fMRI volumes (5 min scanning) were acquired. Each functional BOLD volume consisted of 32 interleaved, descending, oblique axial slices, 3 mm thick with interslice gap of 0.75 mm and in-plane resolution of 3 mm, field of view = 192 × 192 mm, repetition time = 2000 ms, acquisition time = 2 s, time echo = 30 ms, and flip angle 78. We also acquired T1-weighted structural images at 1 mm isotropic resolution in the sagittal plane, using an MPRAGE sequence with TR = 2250 ms, TI = 900 ms, TE = 2.99 ms and flip angle = 9 degrees, for localization purposes. During scanning we instructed volunteers to close their eyes and think about nothing in particular throughout the acquisition of the resting-state BOLD data. Of the 25 healthy subjects, 15 were ultimately retained (7 males, 8 females): 10 were excluded, either because of missing scans (n = 2), or due of excessive motion in the scanner (n = 8, 5 mm maximum motion threshold).

Disorders of consciousness patient dataset

The DOC patient data used in this study have been published before. For clarity and consistency of reporting, where applicable we use the same wording as in our previous publications with these data2,27. Briefly, 71 DOC patients were recruited from specialised long-term care centres from January 2010 to December 2015. Ethical approval for this study was provided by the National Research Ethics Service (National Health Service, UK; LREC reference 99/391). Patients were eligible to be recruited in the study if they had a diagnosis of chronic disorder of consciousness, provided that written informed consent to participation was provided by their legal representative, and provided that the patients could be transported to Addenbrooke’s Hospital (Cambridge, UK). The exclusion criteria included any medical condition that made it unsafe for the patient to participate, according to clinical personnel blinded to the specific aims of the study; or any reason that made a patient unsuitable to enter the MRI scanner environment (e.g. non-MRI-safe implants). Patients were also excluded based on significant pre-existing mental health problems, or insufficient fluency in the English language prior to their injury. After admission to Addenbrooke’s Hospital, each patient underwent clinical and neuroimaging testing (including task-based, resting-state, and anatomical scans), spending a total of five days in the hospital (including arrival and departure days). Neuroimaging scanning took place at the Wolfson Brain Imaging Centre (Addenbrooke’s Hospital, Cambridge, UK), and medication prescribed to each patient was maintained during scanning.

For each day of admission, Coma Recovery Scale-Revised (CRS-R) assessments were recorded at least daily. Patients whose behavioural responses were not indicative of awareness at any time, were classified as UWS. In contrast, patients were classified as being in a minimally conscious state (MCS) if they provided behavioural evidence of simple automatic motor reactions (e.g., scratching, pulling the bed sheet), visual fixation and pursuit, or localisation to noxious stimulation) (note that due to the limited size of our sample of MCS patients, we do not sub-divide these patients into MCS− and MCS+)124,125. Since this study focused on whole-brain properties, coverage of most of the brain was required, and we followed the same criteria as in our previous studies2,27: before analysis took place, patients were systematically excluded if an expert neuroanatomist blinded to diagnosis judged that they displayed excessive focal brain damage (over one-third of one hemisphere), or if brain damage led to suboptimal segmentation and normalisation, or due to excessive head motion in the MRI scanner (exceeding 3 mm translation or 3 degrees rotation)2,27. Forty-one patients were excluded due to excessive brain damage and distortion preventing satisfactory segmentation and normalisation; 8 further patients due to excessive motion. A total of 22 adults (14 males, 8 females; age range 17–70 years; mean time post injury: 13 months) meeting diagnostic criteria for Unresponsive Wakefulness Syndrome/Vegetative State (N = 10) or Minimally Conscious State (N = 12) due to brain injury were included in this study (Table 1)2,27.

Stratification of DOC Patients into fMRI+ and fMRI−

Following our previous work62,63, patients were stratified into two groups based on their ability to perform volitional tasks (mental imagery) in the scanner62,63,126,127. The mental imagery tasks used here have been previously used to assess the presence of covert consciousness in DOC patients62,63,126,127 and their validity has been confirmed in healthy individuals128. Patients were instructed to perform two mental imagery tasks2,27. The first task involved motor imagery (tennis task): each patient was asked to imagine being on a tennis court swinging their arm to hit the ball back and forth with an imagined opponent. The second was a task of spatial imagery (navigation task): the patient was required to imagine walking around the rooms of their house, or the streets of a familiar city, and to visualise what they would see if they were there. Each task comprised five cycles of alternating imagery and rest blocks, each lasting 30 s. The two kinds of mental imagery blocks were cued with the spoken word “tennis” or “navigation”, respectively, whereas the resting blocks were cued with the word “relax”, corresponding to instructions for the patient to just stay still and keep their eyes closed. Univariate fMRI analysis was conducted on all 22 patients for both the motor and spatial mental imagery tasks62. The analyses were performed using FSL version 5.0.9 (https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/). The results of these analyses determined which patients would be placed in each classification condition. For each functional scan, a general linear model consisting of contrasting periods of rest and active imagery was computed. Results were considered significant at a cluster level of z > 2.3 (corrected p < 0.05 at the cluster level)62,63. Patients who exhibited significantly greater brain activation in the appropriate regions (supplementary motor area (SMA) for the tennis task, and parahippocampal place area (PPA) for the navigation task, respectively) during either of the volitional mental imagery tasks than rest (i.e., those who exhibited evidence of being able to respond to the task) were deemed to be covertly conscious (N = 8); for brevity, we refer to these positive responders with the label “fMRI+”62,63. Conversely, we refer to patients who did not respond to either task (negative responders), and who therefore did not exhibit detectable evidence of covert consciousness (N = 14), as “fMRI−”62,63 (Table 1).

DOC patients: MRI data acquisition

Resting-state fMRI was acquired for 10 min (300 volumes, TR = 2000 ms) using a Siemens Trio 3T scanner (Erlangen, Germany). Functional images (32 slices) were acquired using an echo planar sequence, with the following parameters: 3 × 3 × 3.75 mm resolution, TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, 78 degrees FA. Anatomical scanning was also performed, acquiring high-resolution T1-weighted images with an MPRAGE sequence, using the following parameters: TR = 2300 ms, TE = 2.47 ms, 150 slices, resolution 1 × 1 × 1 mm2,27.

Ketamine dataset

The ketamine data used in this study have been published before. For clarity and consistency of reporting, where applicable we use the same wording as in our previous publications with these data64. A total of 21 participants (10 males; mean age 28.7 years, SD = 3.2 years) were recruited via advertisements placed throughout central Cambridge, UK64. All participants underwent a screening interview in which they were asked whether they had previously been diagnosed or treated for any mental health problems and whether they had ever taken any psychotropic medications. Participants reporting a personal history of any mental health problems or a history of any treatment were excluded from the study. All participants were right-handed, were free of current of previous psychiatric or neurological disorder or substance abuse problems, and had no history of cardiovascular illness or family history of psychiatric disorder/substance abuse64. The study was approved by the Cambridge Local Research and Ethics Committee, and all participants provided written informed consent in accordance with ethics committee guidelines. Participants were scanned (resting-state functional MRI and anatomical T1) on two occasions, separated by at least 1 week. On one occasion, they received a continuous computer-controlled intravenous infusion of a racemic ketamine solution (2 mg/ml) until a targeted plasma concentration of 100 ng/ml was reached. This concentration was sustained throughout the protocol. A saline infusion was administered on the other occasion. Infusion order was randomly counterbalanced across participants64.

Ketamine study: infusion protocol

The infusion was performed and monitored by a trained anaesthetist (RA) who was unblinded for safety reasons, but who otherwise had minimal contact with participants64. At all other times, participants were supervised by investigators blinded to the infusion protocol. The participants remained blinded until both assessments were completed. Bilateral intravenous catheters were inserted into volunteers’ forearms, one for infusion, and the other for serial blood sampling. We used a validated and previously implemented three-compartment pharmacokinetic model to achieve a constant plasma concentration of 100 ng/ml using a computerized pump (Graseby 3500, Graseby Medical, UK). The infusion continued for 15 min to allow stabilization of plasma levels. Blood samples were drawn before and after the resting fMRI scan and then placed on ice. Plasma was obtained by centrifugation and stored at −70 °C. Plasma ketamine concentrations were measured by gas chromatography–mass spectrometry.

Ketamine study: MRI data acquisition

All MRI and assessment procedures were identical across assessment occasions64. Scanning was performed using a 3.0T MRI scanner (Siemens Magnetom, Trio Tim, Erlangen, Germany) equipped with a 12-channel array coil located at the Wolfson Brain Imaging Centre, Addenbrooke’s Hospital, Cambridge, UK. T2*-weighted echo-planar images were acquired under eyes-closed resting-state conditions. Participants were instructed to close their eyes and let the minds wander without going to sleep. Subsequent participant debriefing ensured that no participants fell asleep during the scan. Imaging parameters were: 3 × 3 × 3.75 mm voxel size, with a time-to-repetition (TR) of 2000 ms, time-to-echo (TE) of 30 ms, flip angle of 781 in 64 × 64 matrix size, and 240 mm field of view (FOV). A total of 300 volumes comprising 32 slices each were obtained. In addition, high-resolution anatomical T1 images were acquired using a three-dimensional magnetic-prepared rapid gradient echo (MPPRAGE) sequence. In all, 176 contiguous sagittal slices of 1.0 mm thickness using a TR of 2300 ms, TE of 2.98 ms, flip angle of 91, and a FOV of 256 mm in 240 × 256 matrix were acquired with a voxel size of 1.0 mm3. One participant was excluded due to excessive movement, resulting in a final sample of N = 20 subjects.

LSD dataset

The original study129 was approved by the National Research Ethics Service Committee London–West London and was conducted in accordance with the revised declaration of Helsinki (2000), the International Committee on Harmonization Good Clinical Practice guidelines and National Health Service Research Governance Framework. Imperial College London sponsored the research, which was conducted under a Home Office license for research with schedule 1 drugs. All participants were recruited via word of mouth and provided written informed consent to participate after study briefing and screening for physical and mental health. The screening for physical health included electrocardiogram (ECG), routine blood tests, and urine test for recent drug use and pregnancy. A psychiatric interview was conducted and participants provided full disclosure of their drug use history. Key exclusion criteria included: <21 years of age, personal history of diagnosed psychiatric illness, immediate family history of a psychotic disorder, an absence of previous experience with a classic psychedelic drug (e.g. LSD, mescaline, psilocybin/magic mushrooms or DMT/ayahuasca), any psychedelic drug use within 6 weeks of the first scanning day, pregnancy, problematic alcohol use (i.e. >40 units consumed per week), or a medically significant condition rendering the volunteer unsuitable for the study. Twenty healthy volunteers with a previous experience using psychedelic drugs were scanned. Volunteers underwent two scans, 14 days apart. On one day they were given a placebo (10-mL saline) and the other they were given an active dose of LSD (75 μg of LSD in 10-mL saline). The order of the conditions was balanced across participants, and participants were blind to this order but the researchers were not. Participants carried out VAS-style ratings via button-press and a digital display screen presented after each scan, and the 11-factor altered states of consciousness (ASC) questionnaire was completed at the end of each dosing day129. All participants reported marked alterations of consciousness under LSD.

LSD study: infusion protocol

The LSD data used in this study have been published before12,129,130, so we will describe them in brief here. For clarity and consistency of reporting, where applicable we use the same wording as in our previous publications with these data12. The infusion (drug/placebo) was administered over 2 min and occurred 115 min before the resting-state scans were initiated. After infusion, subjects had a brief acclimation period in a mock MRI scanner to prepare them for the experience of being in the real machine. ASL and BOLD scanning consisted of three seven-minute eyes closed resting state scans. The ASL data were not analysed for this study, and will not be discussed further.

LSD study: MRI data acquisition

The first and third scans were eyes-closed, resting state without stimulation, while the second scan involved listening to music; however, this scan was not used in this analysis. The precise length of each of the two BOLD scans included here was 7:20 min. For the present analysis, these two scans were concatenated together in time. Imaging was performed on a 3T GE HDx system. High-resolution anatomical images were acquired with 3D fast spoiled gradient echo scans in an axial orientation, with field of view = 256 × 256 × 192 and matrix = 256 × 256 × 129 to yield 1 mm isotropic voxel resolution. TR/TE = 7.9/3.0 ms; inversion time = 450 ms; flip angle = 20. BOLD-weighted fMRI data were acquired using a gradient echo planer imaging sequence, TR/TE = 2000/35 ms, FoV = 220 mm, 64 × 64 acquisition matrix, parallel acceleration factor = 2, 90 flip angle. Thirty five oblique axial slices were acquired in an interleaved fashion, each 3.4 mm thick with zero slice gap (3.4 mm isotropic voxels). One subject aborted the experiment due to anxiety and four others were excluded for excessive motion (measured in terms of frame-wise displacement), leaving 15 subjects for analysis (11 males, 4 females; mean age 30.5 years, SD = 8.0 years)129.

Test-retest dataset

The test-retest data used in this study have been published before131133, so we will describe them in brief here. For clarity and consistency of reporting, where applicable we use the same wording as in our previous publications with these data131133. Right-handed healthy participants (N = 22, age range, 19–57 years; mean age, 35.0 years; SD 11.2; female-to-male ratio, 9/13) were recruited via advertisements in the Cambridge area and were paid for their participation. Cambridgeshire 2 Research Ethics Committee approved the study (LREC 08/H0308/246) and all volunteers gave written informed consent before participating. Exclusion criteria included National Adult Reading Test (NART) < 70, Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) < 23, left-handedness, history of drug/alcohol abuse, history of psychiatric or neurological disorders, contraindications for MRI scanning, medication that may affect cognitive performance or prescribed for depression, and any physical handicap that could prevent the completion of testing. The study consisted of two scanning visits (separated by 2–4 weeks).

Test-retest dataset: MRI data acquisition

For each visit, resting-state fMRI was acquired for 5:20 min using a Siemens Trio 3T scanner (Erlangen, Germany). Functional imaging data were acquired using an echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence with parameters TR 2000 ms, TE 30 ms, Flip Angle 78◦, FOV 192 × 192 mm2, in-plane resolution 3.0 × 3.0 mm, 32 slices 3.0 mm thick with a gap of 0.75 mm between slices. A 3D high-resolution MPRAGE structural image was also acquired, with the following parameters: TR 2300 ms, TE 2.98 ms, Flip Angle 9◦, FOV 256 × 256 mm2. Task-based data were also collected, and have been analysed before to investigate separate experimental questions131133. A final set of 18 participants had usable data for both resting-state fMRI scans and were included in the present analysis.

The datasets and final number of participants included in each analysis are summarised in Table 2.

Table 2.

Summary of datasets and the number of individuals included in the final analysis.

Dataset Final N for analysis
DOC 22 (10 UWS, 12 MCS)
Propofol 15
Ketamine 21
LSD 15
Test-Retest 18

FMRI preprocessing and denoising

Preprocessing and denoising followed the same pipelines as in our previous publications2,12. For clarity and consistency of reporting, where applicable we use the same wording as in those previous publications. We preprocessed the functional imaging data using a standard pipeline, implemented within the SPM12-based (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) toolbox CONN (http://www.nitrc.org/projects/conn), version 17f134. The pipeline comprised the following steps: removal of the first five scans, to allow magnetisation to reach steady state; functional realignment and motion correction; slice-timing correction to account for differences in time of acquisition between slices; identification of outlier scans for subsequent regression by means of the quality assurance/artefact rejection software art (http://www.nitrc.org/projects/artifact_detect); structure-function coregistration using each volunteer’s high-resolution T1-weighted image; spatial normalisation to Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI-152) standard space with 2 mm isotropic resampling resolution, using the segmented grey matter image, together with an a priori grey matter template.

To reduce noise due to cardiac and motion artefacts, we applied the anatomical CompCor method of denoising the functional data2,12, also implemented within the CONN toolbox135. The anatomical CompCor method involves regressing out of the functional data the following confounding effects: the first five principal components attributable to each individual’s white matter signal, and the first five components attributable to individual cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) signal; six subject-specific realignment parameters (three translations and three rotations) as well as their first-order temporal derivatives; the artefacts identified by art; and main effect of scanning condition135. Linear detrending was also applied, and the subject-specific denoised BOLD signal timeseries were band-pass filtered to eliminate both low-frequency drift effects and high-frequency noise, thus retaining temporal frequencies between 0.008 and 0.09 Hz. Importantly, note that this band-pass filtering pertains to temporal frequencies, which are distinct from the spatial frequencies obtained from connectome harmonic decomposition (as described below).

Due to the presence of deformations caused by brain injury, rather than relying on automated pipelines, DOC patients’ brains were individually preprocessed using SPM12, with visual inspections after each step. In addition, to further reduce potential movement artefacts, data underwent despiking with a hyperbolic tangent squashing function2. The remaining preprocessing and denoising steps were the same as described above for the ketamine and propofol data.

Finally, the preprocessed and denoised functional MRI data were then projected onto the cortical surface and decomposed in terms of connectome harmonics, as described above.

Statistics and reproducibility

Linear Mixed Effects models (implemented as the MATLAB function fitlme) were used to assess the statistical significance of the differences between conditions (states of consciousness), treating condition as a fixed effect, and subjects as random effects. When one measurement was obtained for each timepoint, timepoints were also included as random effects, nested within subjects. Results are reported in terms of the fixed effect of condition, and the upper and lower bounds of its 95% confidence interval, with associated p-value. When comparing different groups, we included age and biological sex as covariates of no interest. For comparison of frequency-specific harmonic energy, the False Discovery Rate for multiple comparisons across 15 frequency bins was controlled by means of the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure136. Correlations were assessed using Spearman’s non-parametric rank-based ρ. All analyses were two-sided, and statistical significance was assessed at the standard alpha threshold of 0.05.

Reporting summary

Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.

Supplementary information

42003_2023_4474_MOESM2_ESM.docx (14.3KB, docx)

Description of Additional Supplementary Data

Supplementary Data 1 (26.2MB, xlsx)
Supplementary Data 2 (12.1KB, xlsx)
Supplementary Data 3 (1.1MB, xlsx)
Reporting summary (5.3MB, pdf)

Acknowledgements

A.I.L., J.V. and P.A.M.M. would like to thank Lena Dorfschmidt for co-organising OxBridge BrainHack 2019, where this work began. We also thank all volunteers and patients who provided data. This work was supported by grants from The Wellcome Trust Research Training Fellowship (grant no. 083660/Z/07/Z), Raymond and Beverly Sackler Studentship, and the Cambridge Commonwealth Trust [R.A.]; the UK Medical Research Council (U.1055.01.002.00001.01) [J.D.P.]; The James S. McDonnell Foundation [J.D.P.]; the Canadian Institute for Advanced Research (CIFAR; grant RCZB/072 RG93193) [to D.K.M. and E.A.S.]; The National Institute for Health Research (NIHR, UK), Cambridge Biomedical Research Centre and NIHR Senior Investigator Awards [J.D.P. and D.K.M.]; The British Oxygen Professorship of the Royal College of Anaesthetists [D.K.M.]; The Stephen Erskine Fellowship, Queens’ College, University of Cambridge [E.A.S.]; The Evelyn Trust, Cambridge and the EoE CLAHRC fellowship [J.A.]; The Gates Cambridge Trust [A.I.L.]; The Cambridge International Trust and the Howard Sidney Sussex Studentship [M.M.C.]; The Oon Khye Beng Ch’Hia Tsio Studentship for Research in Preventive Medicine, Downing College, University of Cambridge [I.P.]; The Wellcome Trust (grant no. 210920/Z/18/Z) [P.A.M.M.]; The European Research Council Consolidator Grant CAREGIVING (615539) [M.L.K. and S.A.]; The Center for Music in the Brain, funded by the Danish National Research Foundation (DNRF117) [M.L.K., S.A. and J.V.]; The Centre for Eudaimonia and Human Flourishing, funded by the Pettit and Carlsberg Foundations [M.L.K.]; The Imperial College President’s Scholarship [L.R.]; The Alex Mosley Charitable Trust [R.L.C.H.]; The ketamine study was funded by the Bernard Wolfe Health Neuroscience Fund and the Wellcome Trust. The original LSD study received support from a Crowd Funding Campaign and the Beckley Foundation, as part of the Beckley-Imperial Research Programme. The research was also supported by the NIHR Brain Injury Healthcare Technology Co-operative based at Cambridge University Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust and University of Cambridge. Data used to obtain the human connectome were provided by the Human Connectome Project, WU-Minn Consortium (Principal Investigators: David Van Essen and Kamil Ugurbil; 1U54MH091657) funded by the 16 NIH Institutes and Centers that support the NIH Blueprint for Neuroscience Research; and by the McDonnell Center for Systems Neuroscience at Washington University. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Author contributions

A.I.L., J.V., E.A.S., S.A. and M.L.K. conceived the study. A.I.L., J.V., P.A.M.M., S.A., M.L.K. and E.A.S. designed the methodology and the analysis. A.I.L. analysed the data. J.V., P.A.M.M., M.M.C., IP. and A.R.D.P. contributed to data analysis. P.F., G.B.W., J.A., J.D.P., R.A., D.K.M., E.A.S., L.R., R.L.C.H. and B.J.S. were involved in designing the original studies for which the present data were collected. P.F., M.M.C., G.B.W., J.A., E.A.S., R.A., A.E.M., R.L.C.H. and L.R. all participated in data collection. A.I.L., E.A.S. and S.A. wrote the manuscript with feedback from all co-authors.

Peer review

Peer review information

Communications Biology thanks Julie Coloigner and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Primary Handling Editors: Camille Maumet and George Inglis.

Data availability

Source data underlying Figs. 35 are presented in Supplementary Data 13, respectively. raw data analysed during the current study are available on request from the following authors. Propofol anaesthesia, Disorders of Consciousness and test-retest datasets: Dr. Emmanuel A. Stamatakis (University of Cambridge, Division of Anaesthesia; email: eas46@cam.ac.uk). LSD dataset: Dr. Robin L. Carhart-Harris (Imperial College London/University of California – San Francisco; email: robin.carhart-harris@ucsf.edu). Ketamine dataset: Dr. Ram Adapa (University of Cambridge, Division of Anaesthesia; email: ra342@cam.ac.uk). The Human Connectome Project datasets are freely available from http://www.humanconnectome.org/.

Code availability

The Java Information Dynamics Toolbox117, together with together with Python and Octave/Matlab interfaces, has been made freely available online: https://github.com/jlizier/jidt. Code for the spherical rotations has been made freely available online: github.com/spin-test/spin-test. The CONN toolbox v17f is freely available online (http://www.nitrc.org/projects/conn). DSI Studio is freely available online: http://dsi-studio.labsolver.org. Lead-DBS is freely available online: http://www.lead-dbs.org. The Brain Connectivity Toolbox is freely available online: https://sites.google.com/site/bctnet/.

Competing interests

The authors declare the following competing interests: RCH reports receiving scientific advisory fees in the last 2 years from: Entheon Biomedical and Beckley Psytech. All other authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s42003-023-04474-1.

References

  • 1.Koch C, Massimini M, Boly M, Tononi G. Neural correlates of consciousness: progress and problems. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2016;17:307–321. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Luppi AI, et al. Consciousness-specific dynamic interactions of brain integration and functional diversity. Nat. Commun. 2019;10:4616. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-12658-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Demertzi A, et al. Human consciousness is supported by dynamic complex patterns of brain signal coordination. Sci. Adv. 2019;5:1–12. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aat7603. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Huang Z, Zhang J, Wu J, Mashour GA, Hudetz AG. Temporal circuit of macroscale dynamic brain activity supports human consciousness. Sci. Adv. 2020;6:87–98. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aaz0087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Barttfeld P, et al. Signature of consciousness in the dynamics of resting-state brain activity. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2015;112:887–892. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1418031112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Golkowski D, et al. Changes in whole brain dynamics and connectivity patterns during sevoflurane- and propofol-induced unconsciousness identified by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Anesthesiology. 2019;130:898–911. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000002704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Campbell JM, et al. Pharmacologically informed machine learning approach for identifying pathological states of unconsciousness via resting-state fMRI. NeuroImage. 2020;206:116316. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Noirhomme Q, et al. Brain connectivity in pathological and pharmacological coma. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2010;4:1–6. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Huang Z, et al. Decoupled temporal variability and signal synchronization of spontaneous brain activity in loss of consciousness: An fMRI study in anesthesia. NeuroImage. 2016;124:693–703. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Tanabe S, et al. Altered global brain signal during physiologic, pharmacologic, and pathologic states of unconsciousness in humans and rats. Anesthesiology. 2020;132:1392–1406. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000003197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Gutierrez-Barragan D, et al. Unique spatiotemporal fMRI dynamics in the awake mouse brain. Curr. Biol.: CB. 2021 doi: 10.1016/J.CUB.2021.12.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Luppi AI, et al. LSD alters dynamic integration and segregation in the human brain. NeuroImage. 2021;227:117653. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117653. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Luppi AI, et al. Brain network integration dynamics are associated with loss and recovery of consciousness induced by sevoflurane. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2021 doi: 10.1002/hbm.25405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Atasoy S, Deco G, Kringelbach ML, Pearson J. Harmonic brain modes: a unifying framework for linking space and time in brain dynamics. Neuroscientist. 2018;24:277–293. doi: 10.1177/1073858417728032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Deco G, Jirsa VK, McIntosh AR. Emerging concepts for the dynamical organization of resting-state activity in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2011;12:43–56. doi: 10.1038/nrn2961. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Cabral J, Kringelbach ML, Deco G. Functional connectivity dynamically evolves on multiple time-scales over a static structural connectome: Models and mechanisms. NeuroImage. 2017;160:84–96. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Honey CJ, et al. Predicting human resting-state functional connectivity from structural connectivity. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2009;106:2035–2040. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811168106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Sporns O, Tononi G, Kötter R. The human connectome: a structural description of the human brain. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2005;1:0245–0251. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.0010042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Fukushima M, et al. Structure–function relationships during segregated and integrated network states of human brain functional connectivity. Brain Struct. Funct. 2018;223:1091–1106. doi: 10.1007/s00429-017-1539-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Xie X, Cai C, Damasceno PF, Nagarajan SS, Raj A. Emergence of canonical functional networks from the structural connectome. NeuroImage. 2021;237:118190. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Abdelnour F, Voss HU, Raj A. Network diffusion accurately models the relationship between structural and functional brain connectivity networks. NeuroImage. 2014;90:335–347. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Tagliazucchi E, et al. Large-scale signatures of unconsciousness are consistent with a departure from critical dynamics. J. R. Soc. Interface. 2016;13:20151027. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2015.1027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Uhrig L, et al. Resting-state dynamics as a cortical signature of anesthesia in monkeys. Anesthesiology. 2018;129:942–958. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000002336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Lee H, et al. Relationship of critical dynamics, functional connectivity, and states of consciousness in large-scale human brain networks. NeuroImage. 2019;188:228–238. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Cofré R, et al. Whole-brain models to explore altered states of consciousness from the bottom up. Brain Sci. 2020;10:1–29. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10090626. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Kringelbach ML, Deco G. Brain states and transitions: insights from computational neuroscience. Cell Rep. 2020;32:108128. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2020.108128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Luppi AI, et al. Whole-brain modelling identifies distinct but convergent paths to unconsciousness in anaesthesia and disorders of consciousness. Commun. Biol. 2022;5:384. doi: 10.1038/s42003-022-03330-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Luppi, A. I. et al. Dynamical models to evaluate structure–function relationships in network neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 1–2 10.1038/s41583-022-00646-w (2022). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 29.Petersen SE, Sporns O. Brain networks and cognitive architectures. Neuron. 2015;88:207–219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Atasoy S, et al. Connectome-harmonic decomposition of human brain activity reveals dynamical repertoire re-organization under LSD. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:1–18. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-17546-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Atasoy S, Donnelly I, Pearson J. Human brain networks function in connectome-specific harmonic waves. Nat. Commun. 2016;7:1–10. doi: 10.1038/ncomms10340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Brahim A, Farrugia N. Graph Fourier transform of fMRI temporal signals based on an averaged structural connectome for the classification of neuroimaging. Artif. Intell. Med. 2020;106:101870. doi: 10.1016/j.artmed.2020.101870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.MacDonald AA, Naci L, MacDonald PA, Owen AM. Anesthesia and neuroimaging: investigating the neural correlates of unconsciousness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2015;19:100–107. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Hannawi Y, Lindquist MA, Caffo BS, Sair HI, Stevens RD. Resting brain activity in disorders of consciousness: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurology. 2015;84:1272–1280. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001404. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Vanhaudenhuyse A, et al. Default network connectivity reflects the level of consciousness in non-communicative brain- damaged patients. Brain. 2010;133:161–171. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Boveroux P, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Phillips C. Breakdown of within- and between-network resting state during propofol-induced loss of consciousness. Anesthesiology. 2010;113:1038–1053. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e3181f697f5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Di Perri C, et al. Multifaceted brain networks reconfiguration in disorders of consciousness uncovered by co-activation patterns. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2018;39:89–103. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23826. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Ranft A, et al. Neural correlates of sevoflurane-induced unconsciousness identified by simultaneous functional magnetic resonance imaging and electroencephalography. Anesthesiology. 2016;125:861–872. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000001322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Spindler LRB, et al. Dopaminergic brainstem disconnection is common to pharmacological and pathological consciousness perturbation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2021;118:e2026289118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2026289118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Di Perri C, et al. Neural correlates of consciousness in patients who have emerged from a minimally conscious state: a cross-sectional multimodal imaging study. Lancet Neurol. 2016;15:830–842. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(16)00111-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Threlkeld, Z. D. et al. Functional networks reemerge during recovery of consciousness after acute severe traumatic brain injury. Cortex 2–11, 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.05.004 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 42.Bonhomme V, et al. Resting-state network-specific breakdown of functional connectivity during ketamine alteration of consciousness in volunteers. Anesthesiology. 2016;125:873–888. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000001275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Guldenmund P, et al. Propofol-induced frontal cortex disconnection: a study of resting-state networks, total brain connectivity, and mean BOLD signal oscillation frequencies. Brain Connect. 2016;6:225–237. doi: 10.1089/brain.2015.0369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Demertzi A, et al. Intrinsic functional connectivity differentiates minimally conscious from unresponsive patients. Brain. 2015;138:2619–2631. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Kafashan M, Ching S, Palanca BJA. Sevoflurane alters spatiotemporal functional connectivity motifs that link resting-state networks during wakefulness. Front. Neural Circuits. 2016;10:107. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2016.00107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Palanca BJA, et al. Resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging correlates of sevoflurane-induced unconsciousness. Anesthesiology. 2015;123:346–356. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000000731. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Stamatakis EA, Adapa RM, Absalom AR, Menon DK. Changes in resting neural connectivity during propofol sedation. PloS one. 2010;5:e14224. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0014224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Adapa RM, Davis MH, Stamatakis EA, Absalom AR, Menon DK. Neural correlates of successful semantic processing during propofol sedation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2013 doi: 10.1002/hbm.22375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Craig MM, et al. Propofol sedation-induced alterations in brain connectivity reflect parvalbumin interneurone distribution in human cerebral cortex. Br. J. Anaesth. 2020 doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2020.11.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Carrera E, Tononi G. Diaschisis: past, present, future. Brain. 2014;137:2408–2422. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Ortega A, Frossard P, Kovačević J, Moura JMF, Vandergheynst P. Graph signal processing: overview, challenges, and applications. Proc. IEEE. 2018;106:808–828. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Shuman DI, Narang SK, Frossard P, Ortega A, Vandergheynst P. The emerging field of signal processing on graphs: Extending high-dimensional data analysis to networks and other irregular domains. IEEE Signal Process. Mag. 2013;30:83–98. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Lioi G, Gripon V, Brahim A, Rousseau F, Farrugia N. Gradients of connectivity as graph Fourier bases of brain activity. Netw. Neurosci. 2021;5:322–336. doi: 10.1162/netn_a_00183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Huang W, et al. A graph signal processing perspective on functional brain imaging. Proc. IEEE. 2018;106:868–885. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Margulies DS, et al. Situating the default-mode network along a principal gradient of macroscale cortical organization. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2016;113:12574–12579. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1608282113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Paquola C, et al. Microstructural and functional gradients are increasingly dissociated in transmodal cortices. PLoS Biol. 2019;17:e3000284. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3000284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Paquola C, et al. A multi-scale cortical wiring space links cellular architecture and functional dynamics in the human brain. PLOS Biol. 2020;18:e3000979. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3000979. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Glomb K, et al. Functional harmonics reveal multi-dimensional basis functions underlying cortical organization. Cell Rep. 2021;36:109554. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2021.109554. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Atasoy S, Vohryzek J, Deco G, Carhart-harris RL, Kringelbach ML. Common neural signatures of psychedelics: Frequency-specific energy changes and repertoire expansion revealed using connectome-harmonic decomposition. Prog. Brain Res. 2018;242:97–120. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Olney JW, Newcomer JW, Farber NB. NMDA receptor hypofunction model of schizophrenia. J. Psychiatr. Res. 1999;33:523–533. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3956(99)00029-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Brown EN, Lydic R, Schiff ND. General anesthesia, sleep, and coma. N. Engl. J. Med. 2010;27:2638–2650. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra0808281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Craig, M. M. et al. Resting-state based prediction of task-related activation in patients with disorders of consciousness. Preprint at bioRxiv10.1101/2021.03.27.436534 (2021).
  • 63.Luppi AI, et al. Preserved fractal character of structural brain networks is associated with covert consciousness after severe brain injury. NeuroImage: Clin. 2021;30:102682. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2021.102682. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Dandash O, et al. Selective augmentation of striatal functional connectivity following NMDA receptor antagonism: implications for psychosis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:622–631. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Corlett PR, Honey GD, Fletcher PC. Prediction error, ketamine and psychosis: an updated model. J. Psychopharmacol. 2016;30:1145–1155. doi: 10.1177/0269881116650087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Moore JW, et al. Ketamine administration in healthy volunteers reproduces aberrant agency experiences associated with schizophrenia. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry. 2011;16:364–381. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2010.546074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Krishnan A, Williams LJ, McIntosh AR, Abdi H. Partial Least Squares (PLS) methods for neuroimaging: a tutorial and review. NeuroImage. 2011;56:455–475. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.McIntosh AR, Mišić B. Multivariate statistical analyses for neuroimaging data. Annu Rev. Psychol. 2013;64:499–525. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Schartner MM, Carhart-Harris RL, Barrett AB, Seth AK, Muthukumaraswamy SD. Increased spontaneous MEG signal diversity for psychoactive doses of ketamine, LSD and psilocybin. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:46421. doi: 10.1038/srep46421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Li D, Mashour GA. Cortical dynamics during psychedelic and anesthetized states induced by ketamine. NeuroImage. 2019;196:32–40. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.03.076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Northoff G, Wainio-Theberge S, Evers K. Is temporo-spatial dynamics the “common currency” of brain and mind? In Quest of “Spatiotemporal Neuroscience”. Phys. Life Rev. 2020;33:34–54. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2019.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Carhart-Harris RL, et al. The entropic brain: a theory of conscious states informed by neuroimaging research with psychedelic drugs. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014;8:20. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Carhart-Harris RL. The entropic brain—revisited. Neuropharmacology. 2018;142:167–178. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2018.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Carhart-Harris RL, Friston KJ. REBUS and the anarchic brain: toward a unified model of the brain action of psychedelics. Pharmacol. Rev. 2019;71:316–344. doi: 10.1124/pr.118.017160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Alexander-Bloch AF, et al. On testing for spatial correspondence between maps of human brain structure and function. NeuroImage. 2018;178:540–551. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.05.070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Preti MG, Van De Ville D. Decoupling of brain function from structure reveals regional behavioral specialization in humans. Nat. Commun. 2019;10:4747. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-12765-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Medaglia JD, et al. Functional alignment with anatomical networks is associated with cognitive flexibility. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2018;2:156–164. doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0260-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Naze S, Proix T, Atasoy S, Kozloski JR. Robustness of connectome harmonics to local gray matter and long-range white matter connectivity changes: sensitivity analysis of Connectome Harmonics. NeuroImage. 2021;224:117364. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117364. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Sarasso S, et al. Consciousness and complexity: a consilience of evidence. Neurosci. Conscious. 2021;2021:1–24. doi: 10.1093/nc/niab023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Burioka N, et al. Approximate entropy in the electroencephalogram during wake and sleep. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 2005;36:21–24. doi: 10.1177/155005940503600106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Schartner M, et al. Complexity of multi-dimensional spontaneous EEG decreases during propofol induced general anaesthesia. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0133532. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0133532. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Olofsen E, Sleigh JW, Dahan A. Permutation entropy of the electroencephalogram: a measure of anaesthetic drug effect. Br. J. Anaesth. 2008;101:810–821. doi: 10.1093/bja/aen290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Liu X, et al. Regional entropy of functional imaging signals varies differently in sensory and cognitive systems during propofol-modulated loss and return of behavioral responsiveness. Brain Imaging Behav. 2019;13:514–525. doi: 10.1007/s11682-018-9886-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Varley TF, et al. Consciousness & brain functional complexity in propofol anaesthesia. Sci. Rep. 2020;10:1–13. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-57695-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Schartner MM, et al. Global and local complexity of intracranial EEG decreases during NREM sleep. Neurosci. Conscious. 2017 doi: 10.1093/nc/niw022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Pal D, et al. Level of consciousness is dissociable from electroencephalographic measures of cortical connectivity, slow oscillations, and complexity. J. Neurosci. 2020;40:605–618. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1910-19.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Lebedev AV, et al. LSD-induced entropic brain activity predicts subsequent personality change. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2016;37:3203–3213. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Tagliazucchi E, Carhart-Harris R, Leech R, Nutt D, Chialvo DR. Enhanced repertoire of brain dynamical states during the psychedelic experience. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2014;35:5442–5456. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Colombo MA, et al. The spectral exponent of the resting EEG indexes the presence of consciousness during unresponsiveness induced by propofol, xenon, and ketamine. NeuroImage. 2019;189:631–644. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.01.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Warnaby CE, Sleigh JW, Hight D, Jbabdi S, Tracey I. Investigation of slow-wave activity saturation during surgical anesthesia reveals a signature of neural inertia in humans. Anesthesiology. 2017;127:645–657. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000001759. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Ní Mhuircheartaigh R, Warnaby C, Rogers R, Jbabdi S, Tracey I. Slow-wave activity saturation and thalamocortical isolation during propofol anesthesia in humans. Sci. Transl. Med. 2013;5:208ra148. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3006007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Ching S, Cimenser A, Purdon PL, Brown EN, Kopell NJ. Thalamocortical model for a propofol-induced α-rhythm associated with loss of consciousness. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2010;107:22665–22670. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1017069108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Donoghue, J. A. et al. Neural signatures of loss of consciousness and its recovery by thalamic stimulation. Preprint at bioRxiv10.1101/806687 (2019).
  • 94.Stephen EP, et al. Broadband slow-wave modulation in posterior and anterior cortex tracks distinct states of propofol-induced unconsciousness. Sci. Rep. 2020;10:13701. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-68756-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Bastos AM, et al. Neural effects of propofol-induced unconsciousness and its reversal using thalamic stimulation. eLife. 2021;10:e60824. doi: 10.7554/eLife.60824. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Raj A, et al. Spectral graph theory of brain oscillations. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2020;41:2980–2998. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Verma P, Nagarajan S, Raj A. Spectral graph theory of brain oscillations—revisited and improved. NeuroImage. 2022;249:118919. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.118919. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Glomb K, et al. Connectome spectral analysis to track EEG task dynamics on a subsecond scale. NeuroImage. 2020;221:117137. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Radtke FM, et al. Risk factors for inadequate emergence after anesthesia: emergence delirium and hypoactive emergence. Minerva Anestesiol. 2010;76:394–404. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Xará D, Silva A, Mendonça J, Abelha F. Inadequate emergence after anesthesia: emergence delirium and hypoactive emergence in the Postanesthesia Care Unit. J. Clin. Anesth. 2013;25:439–446. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinane.2013.02.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Gabay NC, Robinson PA. Cortical geometry as a determinant of brain activity eigenmodes: neural field analysis. Phys. Rev. E. 2017;96:32413. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.96.032413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Mukta KN, Maclaurin JN, Robinson PA. Theory of corticothalamic brain activity in a spherical geometry: Spectra, coherence, and correlation. Phys. Rev. E. 2017;96:52410. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.96.052410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Mortaheb, S. et al. A graph signal processing approach to study high density EEG signals in patients with disorders of consciousness. in Proceedings of the Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society, EMBS 4549–4553 10.1109/EMBC.2019.8856436 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 104.Eickhoff SB, Yeo BTT, Genon S. Imaging-based parcellations of the human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2018;19:672–686. doi: 10.1038/s41583-018-0071-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Wang MB, Owen JP, Mukherjee P, Raj A. Brain network eigenmodes provide a robust and compact representation of the structural connectome in health and disease. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2017;13:e1005550. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005550. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Naci L, Sinai L, Owen AM. Detecting and interpreting conscious experiences in behaviorally non-responsive patients. NeuroImage. 2017;145:304–313. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Luppi AI, et al. Mechanisms underlying disorders of consciousness: bridging gaps to move toward an integrated translational science. Neurocritical Care. 2021;35:37–54. doi: 10.1007/s12028-021-01281-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Huang Z, et al. Brain imaging reveals covert consciousness during behavioral unresponsiveness induced by propofol. Sci. Rep. 2018;8:1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-31436-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Leslie K, et al. Dreaming and electroencephalographic changes during anesthesia maintained with propofol or desflurane. Anesthesiology. 2009;111:547–555. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e3181adf768. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Casali AG, et al. A theoretically based index of consciousness independent of sensory processing and behavior. Sci. Transl. Med. 2013;5:1–10. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3006294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Comolatti, R. et al. A fast and general method to empirically estimate the complexity of brain responses to transcranial and intracranial stimulations. Brain Stimul. 1–10, 10.1016/j.brs.2019.05.013 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 112.Casarotto S, et al. Stratification of unresponsive patients by an independently validated index of brain complexity. Ann. Neurol. 2016;80:718–729. doi: 10.1002/ana.24779. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Kuceyeski A, et al. The application of a mathematical model linking structural and functional connectomes in severe brain injury. NeuroImage: Clin. 2016;11:635–647. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.04.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Van Essen DC, et al. The WU-Minn Human Connectome Project: an overview. NeuroImage. 2013;80:62–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Chung, F. Spectral Graph Theory (American Mathematical Society, 1997).
  • 116.Chuen Lee L, Liong C-Y, Aziz Jemain A. Partial least squares-discriminant analysis (PLS-DA) for classification of high-dimensional (HD) data: a review of contemporary practice strategies and knowledge gaps. Analyst. 2018;143:3526. doi: 10.1039/c8an00599k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Lizier JT. JIDT: an information-theoretic toolkit for studying the dynamics of complex systems. Front. Robot. AI. 2014;1:1–37. [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Cover, T. M. & Thomas, J. A. Elements of Information Theory. Elements of Information Theory. 10.1002/047174882X (Wiley-Interscience, 2005).
  • 119.Setsompop K, et al. Pushing the limits of in vivo diffusion MRI for the Human Connectome Project. NeuroImage. 2013;80:220–233. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Deco G, Vidaurre D, Kringelbach ML. Revisiting the global workspace: orchestration of the functional hierarchical organisation of the human brain. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2021 doi: 10.1101/859579. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Maier-Hein KH, et al. The challenge of mapping the human connectome based on diffusion tractography. Nat. Commun. 2017;8:1–13. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-01285-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Horn A, Blankenburg F. Toward a standardized structural-functional group connectome in MNI space. NeuroImage. 2016;124:310–322. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Rubinov M, Sporns O. Complex network measures of brain connectivity: uses and interpretations. NeuroImage. 2010;52:1059–1069. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Bruno M-A, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Thibaut A, Moonen G, Laureys S. From unresponsive wakefulness to minimally conscious PLUS and functional locked-in syndromes: recent advances in our understanding of disorders of consciousness. J. Neurol. 2011;258:1373–1384. doi: 10.1007/s00415-011-6114-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Wannez S, et al. Prevalence of coma-recovery scale-revised signs of consciousness in patients in minimally conscious state. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 2018;28:1350–1359. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2017.1310656. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Owen AM, et al. Detecting awareness in the vegetative state. Science. 2006;313:1402. doi: 10.1126/science.1130197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Monti MM, et al. Willful modulation of brain activity in disorders of consciousness. N. Engl. J. Med. 2010;362:579–589. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa0905370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Fernández-Espejo D, Norton L, Owen AM. The clinical utility of fMRI for identifying covert awareness in the vegetative state: A comparison of sensitivity between 3T and 1.5T. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e95082. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0095082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Carhart-Harris RL, et al. Neural correlates of the LSD experience revealed by multimodal neuroimaging. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2016;113:201518377. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1518377113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Singleton SP, et al. Receptor-informed network control theory links LSD and psilocybin to a flattening of the brain’s control energy landscape. Nat. Commun. 2022;13:5812. doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-33578-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Vatansever D, Menon XDK, Manktelow AE, Sahakian BJ, Stamatakis EA. Default mode dynamics for global functional integration. J. Neurosci. 2015;35:15254–15262. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2135-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Manktelow AE, Menon DK, Sahakian BJ, Stamatakis EA. Working memory after traumatic brain injury: the neural basis of improved performance with methylphenidate. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2017;11:58. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Moreno-López L, Manktelow AE, Sahakian BJ, Menon DK, Stamatakis EA. Anything goes? Regulation of the neural processes underlying response inhibition in TBI patients. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2017;27:159–169. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2016.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Nieto-Castanon A. Conn: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connect. 2012;2:125–141. doi: 10.1089/brain.2012.0073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Behzadi Y, Restom K, Liau J, Liu TT. A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI. NeuroImage. 2007;37:90–101. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B (Methodol.) 1995;57:289–300. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

42003_2023_4474_MOESM2_ESM.docx (14.3KB, docx)

Description of Additional Supplementary Data

Supplementary Data 1 (26.2MB, xlsx)
Supplementary Data 2 (12.1KB, xlsx)
Supplementary Data 3 (1.1MB, xlsx)
Reporting summary (5.3MB, pdf)

Data Availability Statement

Source data underlying Figs. 35 are presented in Supplementary Data 13, respectively. raw data analysed during the current study are available on request from the following authors. Propofol anaesthesia, Disorders of Consciousness and test-retest datasets: Dr. Emmanuel A. Stamatakis (University of Cambridge, Division of Anaesthesia; email: eas46@cam.ac.uk). LSD dataset: Dr. Robin L. Carhart-Harris (Imperial College London/University of California – San Francisco; email: robin.carhart-harris@ucsf.edu). Ketamine dataset: Dr. Ram Adapa (University of Cambridge, Division of Anaesthesia; email: ra342@cam.ac.uk). The Human Connectome Project datasets are freely available from http://www.humanconnectome.org/.

The Java Information Dynamics Toolbox117, together with together with Python and Octave/Matlab interfaces, has been made freely available online: https://github.com/jlizier/jidt. Code for the spherical rotations has been made freely available online: github.com/spin-test/spin-test. The CONN toolbox v17f is freely available online (http://www.nitrc.org/projects/conn). DSI Studio is freely available online: http://dsi-studio.labsolver.org. Lead-DBS is freely available online: http://www.lead-dbs.org. The Brain Connectivity Toolbox is freely available online: https://sites.google.com/site/bctnet/.


Articles from Communications Biology are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES