Abstract
Spatial orientation is based on a complex cortical network with input from multiple sensory systems. It is affected by training, sex and age as well as cultural and psychological factors, resulting in different individual skill levels in healthy subjects. Various neurological disorders can lead to different patterns or specific deficits of spatial orientation and navigation. Accordingly, numerous tests have been proposed to assess these abilities. Here, we compare the results of (1) a validated questionnaire-based self-estimate of orientation/navigation ability (Santa Barbara Sense of Direction Scale, SBSODS) and (2) a validated pen-and-paper two-dimensional perspective test (Perspective Taking Spatial Orientation Test, SOT) with (3) a newly developed test of finger-arm pointing performance in a 3D real-world (3D-RWPT) paradigm using a recently established pointing device. A heterogeneous group of 121 participants (mean age 56.5 ± 17.7 years, 52 females), including 16 healthy volunteers and 105 patients with different vestibular, ocular motor and degenerative brain disorders, was included in this study. A high correlation was found between 2D perspective task and 3D pointing along the horizontal (azimuth) but not along the vertical (polar) plane. Self-estimated navigation ability (SBSODS) could not reliably predict actual performance in either 2D- or 3D-tests. Clinical assessment of spatial orientation and memory should therefore include measurements of actual performance, based on a 2D pen-and-paper test or a 3D pointing task, rather than memory-based questionnaires, since solely relying on the patient’s history of self-estimated navigation ability results in misjudgments. The 3D finger-arm pointing test (3D-RWPT) reveals additional information on vertical (polar) spatial performance which goes undetected in conventional 2D pen-and-paper tests. Diseases or age-specific changes of spatial orientation in the vertical plane should not be clinically neglected. The major aim of this pilot study was to compare the practicability and capability of the three tests but not yet to prove their use for differential diagnosis. The next step will be to establish a suitable clinical bedside test for spatial memory and orientation.
Keywords: Spatial memory, Spatial orientation, Pointing task, Bedside test, Dementia, Vestibulopathy
Introduction
Deficits in spatial orientation can occur in both cognitive and vestibular disorders [1]. A navigational impairment is an early symptom of cognitive decline such as mild cognitive impairment or Alzheimer’s dementia. This potentially poses the danger of “wandering-off-behavior” [2]. Early diagnosis requires quantitative testing of patients’ performance in spatial orientation, spatial memory and navigation. Different methods have been proposed to assess these abilities, including psychometric questionnaires [3], paper-based 2D-tests [4] and navigation tasks in virtual [5, 6] or standardized real-world environments [7, 8]. Clinical application of complex tests is rare because of the amount of time and work required. Taking the history of the patients and their relatives will only disclose severe deficits, whereas beginning disturbances require more sensitive objective reliable measures.
Since real-world testing of orientation and navigation capabilities appears impracticable, the question arises whether an easier to perform finger-arm 3D real-world pointing task (3D-RWPT, [9]) is suitable to discern impending spatial orientation deficits. In an earlier study, it was shown that the simple 3D-RWPT delivered reliable and reproducible data on spatial memory in azimuth and polar coordinates [9].
In a first step, this test was now compared to a commonly used and validated questionnaire (Santa Barbara Sense of Direction Scale, SBSODS [4]) and to a 2D paper-and-pencil orientation test (Perspective Taking Spatial Orientation Test, SOT [3]). The participants included 16 healthy volunteers and 105 patients with various degenerative cognitive or vestibular and ocular motor disorders. The major goal at this stage was not to establish criteria for differential diagnosis but to compare the practicability and sensitivity of a 3D real-world test for spatial orientation.
Methods
Participants
Sixteen healthy volunteers and 105 patients presenting with different vestibular pathologies, gait instability, central ocular motor disorders, neurodegenerative syndromes or cognitive disorders underwent a standardized 3D-pointing task sitting on a swivel chair and two well-established psychometric tests, a questionnaire (SBSODS) and a 2D paper-and-pencil orientation test (SOT). Out of these 121 participants (mean age 56.5 ± 17.7 years, minimum 24 years, maximum 86 years, 52 female), 50 had either a unilateral (n = 23) or a bilateral (n = 27) peripheral-vestibular hypofunction, 31 had a cognitive impairment, 25 suffered from polyneuropathy and 9 had ischemic lesions. These groups were not mutually exclusive, i.e., participants with both vestibular and cognitive deficits were included in this analysis, since patient recruitment was part of an ongoing effort to create a large-scale clinical database for future examination of orientation deficits in different neurological disorders. Therefore, the only exclusion criteria were an age below 18 years and the inability to perform the pointing task due to, e.g., joint-neuromuscular disorders, severe visual deficits, pareses or movement disorders. Patients presenting with vertigo or dizziness underwent a complete neuro-otological assessment [10] including caloric irrigation of the semicircular canals, video head impulse test and neuro-orthoptic examination. Healthy participants all had no history of vestibular disorders, vertigo or dizziness and received a clinical neurootological examination including Frenzel’s glasses and head impulse test to rule out a relevant peripheral-vestibular deficit.
The data protection clearance and Institutional Review Board of the Ludwig-Maximilians University Munich, Germany, approved the study (no. 094-10) and all patients gave informed consent. The study was performed in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments.
Self-report of navigation ability: Santa Barbara Sense of Direction Scale (SBSODS)
The SBSODS consists of 15 questions about the self-perceived sense of direction with replies being registered on a Likert scale with seven intervals (1: fully agree, 7: fully disagree). If subjects left a blank space, an intermediate value of 4 (4: I don’t know) was registered. Overall, a higher score means a higher self-reported sense of direction [4].
The test was initially developed and validated in English and had to be translated for the present study. The recommended cross-cultural adaptation process [11] was followed, namely translation of the original SBSODS—and also SOT—version from the English language into German, synthesis of translations, and back translation into English by a native speaker blinded to the original versions. Translations were then evaluated by the initial developers deciding on equivalence; if necessary, the above-mentioned steps were repeated after retranslation and correction. This process is established in the field of vestibular research [12].
Two-dimensional perspective taking: Spatial Orientation Test (SOT)
The tasks from the original English version of the SOT [3] were annotated with German translations and printed on DIN A4 paper. We chose a paper-based version of the SBSODS and SOT to allow for error-correction and to not add a barrier for less technologically inclined elderly patients. While large-scale reviews could prove the overall good comparability of pen-and-paper and computer-based tests [13], the interaction with digital devices can facilitate negative emotional responses in elderly subjects [14] which is detrimental to subject motivation and might artificially skew testing performance. On each trial of the SOT, subjects are shown a two-dimensional bird’s-eye-view of an unchanged map filled with seven objects (Fig. 1). The test requires the subject to imagine being located at one of the objects, facing a second object and then indicating the relative direction toward a third object from said virtual position. No annotations on the test sheet or physical rotation of the test are allowed. To ensure a sufficient understanding of the task, one sample task was explained by the examiner. Thereafter, subjects had 5 min to solve as many of the 12 tasks as possible. Angular errors of the indicated direction were measured to the closest 5° interval on a 360° full circle; lower errors mean a better performance. The maximum error is 180°, i.e., subjects giving a direction completely opposite of the correct solution. Furthermore, the number of tasks solved in the 5-min time test phase was also determined (nSOT).
3D finger-arm pointing test (3D-RWPT)
The participant was sitting on a swivel chair using a smart phone based pointing device on their dominant hand (testing setup with high spatial discrimination and simple handling; for details see: [9]; Fig. 2). The pointing task consisted of two calibration and five testing paradigms: one calibration for a retinotopic solution where the subject uses an egocentric reference frame and one calibration with a smart phone-fixed laser depicting the pointing trajectory, requiring a world-based allocentric reference frame. During calibration, visual feedback was available. Targets were marked with red 20 mm points on a white wall in a 3 × 3 matrix with a 100 cm distance between points. For each task, a computerized voice from the device gave a command in randomized order, e.g., “top left” in either German or English. The subjects then pointed toward the target with their extended dominant arm and confirmed the final position with a wireless Bluetooth dongle with the other hand. After calibration, the subjects were asked to point to the targets in (newly) randomized order without visual feedback while facing toward them (1), after passive 90° body rotation on the chair to their non-dominant side with visual feedback available during rotation (2), back to the initial body position without visual feedback during rotation (3), after passive 90° body rotation to their dominant side with visual feedback available during rotation (4) and back to the initial target-facing position, without visual feedback during rotation (5). Each test run was separated by a standardized pause of 30 seconds which was signaled in 5 second intervals using a notification sound of the pointing device. Handedness was assessed using the Edinburgh inventory [15].
Data analysis of 3D-RWPT
Applying the technique described [9], the coordinates from the device accelerometer sensors readout for each target measurement were transformed into vectors in a spherical coordinate system centered on the subject’s position by assuming a stable gravitational acceleration on the z-axis. Since the required trigonometric operations for data processing included periodic formulas, deviations larger than π/2 (180°) would potentially give wrong results; large-scale deviations were therefore monitored by test personnel. By standardizing and strictly controlling subject position in 3D-space, these vectors can then be compared to both calibrations, resulting in two azimuth and two polar deviations in degrees from retinotopic calibration and from allocentric/world-based calibration, respectively. These deviations were then averaged, resulting in a mean azimuth deviation (mAD) and a mean polar deviation (mPD) from retinotopic and world-based calibration (mADretinotopic, mADworld-based, mPDretinotopic, mPDworld-based). Furthermore, the tasks which were performed facing the wall (1, 3, 5) were averaged to create the mean reproduction deviations (Reprod. mAD, reprod. mPD) and the tasks where a transformation from a 90° body rotation position relative to the wall was required (2, 4) were averaged to calculate the mean transformation deviations (Transform. mAD, transform. mPD). Again, deviations from retinotopic or world-based calibrations were calculated (Reprod. mADretinotopic, reprod. mADworld-based, reprod. mPDretinotopic, reprod. mPDworld-based, transform. mADretinotopic, transform. mADworld-based, transform. mPDretinotopic, transform. mPDworld-based).
Statistical data analysis
All data were irreversibly anonymized for further analyses and processed using JASP (Jeffreys’s Amazing Statistics Program, Version 0.16.2 [16]). For data description, we used mean values and standard deviation for continuous variables and absolute and relative frequencies for categorical variables. We tested statistical inference using Spearman’s rho (ρ) and used an independent samples Student t test for group comparisons.
Results
Of 121 participants (mean age 56.5 ± 17.7 years, minimum 24 years, maximum 86 years, 52 female), four did not complete the SBSODS but performed the SOT and 3D-RWPT while 11 healthy controls only performed an abbreviated version of SBSODS and SOT. To avoid a lengthy presentation of the numerous data, the results are depicted in Tables 1, 2, 3 and 4. Mean test results and pointing deviations are depicted separately for females and males (Table 1).
Table 1.
Sex | Valid | Missing | Mean | Std. deviation | Minimum | Maximum | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
SOT | M | 63 | 4 | 56.488 | 32.991 | 6.833 | 109.583 |
W | 47 | 7 | 62.972 | 31.406 | 6.25 | 113.917 | |
SBSODS | M | 61 | 6 | 5.016 | 0.99 | 2.867 | 6.867 |
W | 45 | 9 | 4.481 | 0.87 | 2.667 | 6.867 | |
mAD retinotopic | M | 67 | 0 | 9.301 | 4.838 | 4.39 | 25.179 |
W | 54 | 0 | 11.507 | 6.904 | 4.172 | 33.189 | |
mAD world-based | M | 67 | 0 | 10.019 | 5.986 | 4.496 | 40.769 |
W | 54 | 0 | 12.335 | 8.103 | 3.275 | 47.221 | |
mPD retinotopic | M | 67 | 0 | 7.466 | 2.629 | 3.235 | 14.744 |
W | 54 | 0 | 8.155 | 2.649 | 3.407 | 15.153 | |
mPD world-based | M | 67 | 0 | 6.681 | 2.366 | 3.306 | 14.698 |
W | 54 | 0 | 6.363 | 1.472 | 3.858 | 9.796 | |
Reprod. mAD retinotopic | M | 67 | 0 | 7.727 | 4.237 | 3.705 | 23.247 |
W | 54 | 0 | 8.973 | 5.039 | 3.014 | 26.294 | |
Reprod. mAD world-based | M | 67 | 0 | 8.691 | 5.913 | 3.367 | 34.166 |
W | 54 | 0 | 9.743 | 6.951 | 2.707 | 43.674 | |
Reprod. mPD retinotopic | M | 67 | 0 | 6.68 | 2.74 | 2.654 | 14.495 |
W | 54 | 0 | 7.29 | 2.48 | 2.098 | 14.318 | |
Reprod. mPD world-based | M | 67 | 0 | 6.076 | 2.385 | 2.234 | 14.183 |
W | 54 | 0 | 5.745 | 1.658 | 2.617 | 10.049 | |
Transform. mAD retinotopic | M | 67 | 0 | 11.663 | 7.374 | 2.83 | 31.482 |
W | 54 | 0 | 15.31 | 12.326 | 3.574 | 73.739 | |
Transform. mAD world-based | M | 67 | 0 | 12.009 | 7.683 | 3.828 | 50.673 |
W | 54 | 0 | 16.222 | 12.748 | 3.723 | 72.791 | |
Transform. mPD retinotopic | M | 67 | 0 | 8.645 | 3.017 | 3.761 | 17.31 |
W | 54 | 0 | 9.452 | 3.313 | 5.113 | 19.019 | |
Transform. mPD world-based | M | 67 | 0 | 7.589 | 2.73 | 3.935 | 17.145 |
W | 54 | 0 | 7.29 | 1.877 | 3.757 | 12.914 |
SOT: mean angular deviation in ° (lower score means higher performance). SBSODS: mean score of self-reported sense of orientation (higher score better). mAD, mPD: mean pointing deviation in ° in azimuth (mAD) and polar (mPD) direction, further subdivided into reproduction tasks (Reprod.) and transformation tasks (Transform.) and deviation compared to retinotopic calibration and world-based calibration, respectively. All results divided by participant sex (M/W)
Table 2.
Spearman | ||
---|---|---|
ρ | p | |
Age | ||
mAD retinotopic | 0.391*** | < 0.001 |
mAD world-based | 0.4*** | < 0.001 |
mPD retinotopic | 0.075 | 0.42 |
mPD world-based | 0.277** | 0.002 |
Reprod. mAD retinotopic | 0.289** | 0.002 |
Reprod. mAD world-based | 0.302*** | < 0.001 |
Reprod. mPD retinotopic | 0.072 | 0.437 |
Reprod. mPD world-based | 0.319*** | < 0.001 |
Transform. mAD retinotopic | 0.381*** | < 0.001 |
Transform. mAD world-based | 0.401*** | < 0.001 |
Transform. mPD retinotopic | 0.032 | 0.729 |
Transform. mPD world-based | 0.146 | 0.115 |
SOT | 0.544*** | < 0.001 |
nSOT | − 0.475*** | < 0.001 |
SBSODS | 0.019 | 0.845 |
Table 3.
Spearman | |||
---|---|---|---|
ρ | p | ||
SBSODS | SOT | − 0.183 | 0.061 |
mAD retinotopic | − 0.1 | 0.307 | |
mAD world-based | − 0.093 | 0.341 | |
Reprod. mAD retinotopic | − 0.105 | 0.286 | |
Reprod. mAD world-based | − 0.053 | 0.591 | |
Transform. mAD retinotopic | − 0.093 | 0.342 | |
Transform. mAD world-based | − 0.077 | 0.435 | |
SOT | SBSODS | − 0.183 | 0.061 |
mAD retinotopic | 0.292** | 0.002 | |
mAD world-based | 0.375*** | < 0.001 | |
Reprod. mAD retinotopic | 0.181 | 0.059 | |
Reprod. mAD world-based | 0.256** | 0.007 | |
Transform. mAD retinotopic | 0.31*** | < 0.001 | |
Transform. mAD world-based | 0.411*** | < 0.001 |
Table 4.
Spearman | |||
---|---|---|---|
ρ | p | ||
SBSODS | mPD retinotopic | − 0.159 | 0.103 |
mPD world-based | − 0.038 | 0.701 | |
Reprod. mPD retinotopic | − 0.215* | 0.027 | |
Reprod. mPD world-based | − 0.027 | 0.78 | |
Transform. mPD retinotopic | − 0.097 | 0.322 | |
Transform. mPD world-based | − 0.024 | 0.805 | |
SOT | SBSODS | − 0.183 | 0.061 |
mPD retinotopic | 0.044 | 0.651 | |
mPD world-based | 0.103 | 0.284 | |
Reprod. mPD retinotopic | 0.073 | 0.447 | |
Reprod. mPD world-based | 0.138 | 0.149 | |
Transform. mPD retinotopic | 0.022 | 0.817 | |
Transform. mPD world-based | 0.04 | 0.678 |
Objective 2D and 3D performance as well as self-reported sense of direction showed a slight sex bias in favor of male participants. Higher participant age resulted in worse performance (Table 2). Overall, 3D azimuth pointing performance exhibited a sex and age bias (Fig. 3a), while 3D polar performance was more stable across sex and age (Fig. 3b). An independent samples Student t test based on equal variances [t(121) = − 1.994, p < 0.05; Cohen’s d = − 0.365] showed that male participants had a significantly lower mean azimuth deviation (9.660°, SD = 5.122°) than female participants (11.921°, SD = 7.326°), while polar performance did not have a significant sex bias [t(121) = − 0.488, p = 0.626; Cohen’s d = − 0.089, mean male deviation = 7.074°, SD = 2.277°; mean female deviation = 7.259°, SD = 1.792°].
Similarly, 2D pen-and-paper-tests and 3D azimuth pointing correlated considerably (Table 3), whereas 2D and 3D polar performance did not robustly correlate (Table 4).
Complete inability of reference system updating was observed in five patients, who assumed the targets to be still in front of them after active rotation to the side (“egocentric fallback”; Fig. 4).
As explained in the Introduction, this pilot study did not aim to differentiate between specific disorders but rather between the methodological capability of different tests. Therefore, we refrained from presenting quantitative data on specific subgroups of disorders.
Discussion
In this preliminary data analysis, the validated 2D pen-and-paper orientation test and the newly developed 3D real-world pointing task [9] showed significant correlations in a heterogeneous group of participants. Well-known findings of spatial research in a 2D task (e.g., sex bias or age dependence) could be confirmed and further demonstrated for the 3D task. Parametric independent t testing of azimuth pointing performance showed a small, but significant difference in favor of male participants (Cohen’s d: − 0.365), a finding in line with meta-analyses of sex differences in navigation tasks [17]. While further clinical data is required to investigate disease-specific spatial orientation deficits, the aim of this study was to perform a comparison of 2D and 3D participant performance and explore the value of self-reported navigation ability in both healthy subjects and patients. This was motivated by the clinical observation of discrepancies between an individual’s objective test performance and subjective questionnaire results of individual ability. We could show that both 2D and 3D tests give reliable results of a participant’s spatial performance. In contrast, a self-report of these skills is prone to errors, possibly due to, e.g., overestimation of one’s abilities during the lifelong span of performance [18], increasingly affected by unawareness or denial or palliation of one’s cognitive decline [19]. This fits prior research conducted in healthy subjects, e.g., the low predictive value of the SBSODS overall score regarding a cardinal-pointing task (e.g., directional pointing to north, east, south and west) in its validation study [4] or other real-world-navigation experiments with healthy college students [20]. While the overall SBSODS-score could not predict 2D or 3D test performance in the current study population, only some sub-items of this questionnaire were of predictive relevance. Similar effects have already been reported in a large study focused on spatial abilities in an Italian community [21] which introduced a distinction between positive (e.g., pleasure in exploring new environments) and negative self-assessments (e.g., spatial anxiety). Only positive self-assessments explained pointing accuracy in healthy participants, whereas negative self-assessments did not [20]. A neuropsychological and imaging study on patients suffering from bilateral vestibulopathy, on the other hand, had shown a difference in the negative assessments, namely the Spatial Anxiety Scale [22] while other psychometric tests did not differ between healthy subjects and these patients [23]. The SBSODS overall score includes positive and negative questions, while the Spatial Anxiety Scale can differentiate even further sub-aspects of spatial anxiety [24]. Taken together, the importance of a detailed assessment of subjects’ or patients’ orientation-related history is limited as to the expected skill level, be it with detailed questionnaires or thorough history taking. Thus, for actual performance assessment both 2D pen-and-paper and 3D pointing tests are more suitable measures to uncover deficits of spatial orientation.
While SOT and overall azimuth deviation showed a high correlation, SOT and polar deviation often did not coincide. This might be due to the nature of the SOT which tests two-dimensional directionality similar to azimuth pointing. Furthermore, as a ground-based species, human everyday pathfinding is overall confined to the horizontal plane; it might therefore be a more commonly trained task, possibly especially by active young and healthy subjects. The perception and estimation of horizontal and vertical dimensions of imagined buildings, on the other hand, were shown to be error-prone even in familiar environments [25, 26]. It is unclear whether a direct neuroanatomical substrate of the horizontal or vertical mental representation of one’s three-dimensional environment exists.
Overall, the observed mean polar deviations were lower than the mean azimuth deviations. This might partially be due to the yaw-axis-rotation before the transformation tasks constituting a change of body position only on the horizontal but not on the vertical plane. Further research is required to see if polar pointing performance, possibly utilizing an additional pitch-axis transformation task, allows a distinction of orientational deficits that are currently underrepresented in, e.g., 2D testing batteries.
When comparing the reproduction tasks and the transformation tasks, we could show that the transformation tasks (e.g., after rotation to the side) exhibited a higher correlation with 2D-performance than the reproduction tasks. This could be due to participants applying proprioceptive information (e.g., remembered joint position) in the forward-facing reproduction task, while the transformation tasks necessarily involve reference-system adaptation (from a mental map in either retinotopic or world-based coding toward a shoulder-centered motor plan) and therefore require spatial processing. Further research will be required to see if subjects with visuo-spatial cognitive deficits show distinct deficits in these subtasks.
We noticed a pattern of large-scale deviation in exceptional patients (n = 5) who kept applying an egocentric reference frame after rotation to the side, assuming the targets to be still in front of them (Fig. 4), although they had performed the rotation under sensory control by vision (the rotation was performed with open eyes) and hearing (e.g., the command given by the examiner). Further research in a larger group of patients with cognitive deficits will be necessary to improve the understanding of the applied pointing strategies and determine if this “egocentric fallback,” i.e., the faulty direct usage of egocentrically (in our case: retinotopic) coded spatial information without prior transformation to consider changes of the relationship between body and environment, is a disease-specific characteristic.
When analyzing these test results or when rating clinical orientation performance, it is important to keep in mind a subject’s baseline or pre-disease skill level. Multiple factors such as subject age [27] or sex [28] can determine test performance with some developmental studies also showing effects of exercise [29], childhood hobbies [30] or other early-life activities [31]. In studies on targeted locomotion, higher cognitive social inter-personal aspects of interaction with the environment [32, 33] and an individual disposition could be shown to affect results. Wayfinding as both a navigation as well as a decision task is also a social activity and can be influenced by group phenomena, even when other people are not directly present [34]. These factors are well-known in the field of psychological research [20, 35], but are often paid less attention in neuroscientific experiments. Nevertheless, these differences create relevant challenges in the clinical examination of orientation or navigation performance. Simply put, a landscape architect applying three-dimensional navigation and orientation every day comes from a higher level than a desk worker, regardless of the orientational demand. Uncovering disease-specific minor differences outside of overall large-scale performance metrics requires heterogeneous patient groups as well as a simple, yet precise test of spatial navigation. Here, the 3D-real-world pointing task might be able to disclose new disease-specific characteristics in future studies.
Acknowledgements
The present study was supported by the Hertie Foundation (TB), the Deutsche Stiftung Neurologie (DSN) (MD) (project 80766113), and the SyNergy Cluster (German Research Foundation, DFG, under Germany’s Excellence Strategy (EXC 2145 SyNergy – ID 390857198)) (MD).
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
Declarations
Conflicts of interest
The authors declare they have no competing financial interests.
References
- 1.Brandt T, Zwergal A, Glasauer S. 3-D spatial memory and navigation: functions and disorders. Curr Opin Neurol. 2017;30:90–97. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0000000000000415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Byard RW, Langlois NEI. Wandering dementia-a syndrome with forensic implications. J Forensic Sci. 2019;64:443–445. doi: 10.1111/1556-4029.13885. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Kozhevnikov M, Hegarty M. A dissociation between object manipulation spatial ability and spatial orientation ability. Mem Cogn. 2001;29:745–756. doi: 10.3758/BF03200477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Hegarty M. Development of a self-report measure of environmental spatial ability. Intelligence. 2002;30:425–447. doi: 10.1016/S0160-2896(02)00116-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Cogné M, Taillade M, N'Kaoua B, et al. The contribution of virtual reality to the diagnosis of spatial navigation disorders and to the study of the role of navigational aids: a systematic literature review. Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2017;60:164–176. doi: 10.1016/j.rehab.2015.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Diersch N, Wolbers T. The potential of virtual reality for spatial navigation research across the adult lifespan. J Exp Biol. 2019 doi: 10.1242/jeb.187252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Schöberl F, Pradhan C, Grosch M, et al. Bilateral vestibulopathy causes selective deficits in recombining novel routes in real space. Sci Rep. 2021;11:2695. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-82427-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Schöberl F, Zwergal A, Brandt T. Testing navigation in real space: contributions to understanding the physiology and pathology of human navigation control. Front Neural Circuits. 2020;14:6. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2020.00006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Gerb J, Brandt T, Dieterich M. Different strategies in pointing tasks and their impact on clinical bedside tests of spatial orientation. J Neurol. 2022 doi: 10.1007/s00415-022-11015-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Brandt T, Dieterich M, Strupp M. Vertigo and dizziness: common complaints. 3. New York: Springer; 2022. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Beaton DE, Bombardier C, Guillemin F, Ferraz MB. Guidelines for the process of cross-cultural adaptation of self-report measures. Spine. 2000;25(24):3186–3191. doi: 10.1097/00007632-200012150-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Mueller M, Whitney SL, Alghwiri A, et al. Subscales of the vestibular activities and participation questionnaire could be applied across cultures. J Clin Epidemiol. 2015;68:211–219. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2014.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Chan JYC, Kwong JSW, Wong A, et al. Comparison of computerized and paper-and-pencil memory tests in detection of mild cognitive impairment and dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis of diagnostic studies. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2018;19:748–756.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2018.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Urban M. 'This really takes it out of you!' The senses and emotions in digital health practices of the elderly. Digit Health. 2017;3:2055207617701778. doi: 10.1177/2055207617701778. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9:97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.JASP Team (2022). JASP (Version 0.16.2) [Computer software]. https://jasp-stats.org/. Accessed 14 May 2022
- 17.Nazareth A, Huang X, Voyer D, et al. A meta-analysis of sex differences in human navigation skills. Psychon Bull Rev. 2019;26:1503–1528. doi: 10.3758/s13423-019-01633-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kruger J, Dunning D. Unskilled and unaware of it: how difficulties in recognizing one’s own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1999;77:1121–1134. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.77.6.1121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Wilson RS, Sytsma J, Barnes LL, et al. Anosognosia in dementia. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. 2016;16:77. doi: 10.1007/s11910-016-0684-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Weisberg SM, Schinazi VR, Newcombe NS, et al. Variations in cognitive maps: understanding individual differences in navigation. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2014;40(3):669–682. doi: 10.1037/a0035261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Meneghetti C, Borella E, Pastore M, et al. The role of spatial abilities and self-assessments in cardinal point orientation across the lifespan. Learn Individ Differ. 2014;35:113–121. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2014.07.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Lawton CA. Gender differences in way-finding strategies: relationship to spatial ability and spatial anxiety. Sex Roles. 1994;30:765–779. doi: 10.1007/BF01544230. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kremmyda O, Hüfner K, Flanagin VL, et al. Beyond dizziness: virtual navigation, spatial anxiety and hippocampal volume in bilateral vestibulopathy. Front Hum Neurosci. 2016 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Lyons IM, Ramirez G, Maloney EA, et al. Spatial anxiety: a novel questionnaire with subscales for measuring three aspects of spatial anxiety. J Numer Cogn. 2018;4:526–553. doi: 10.5964/jnc.v4i3.154. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Brandt T, Huber M, Schramm H, et al. “Taller and Shorter”: human 3-D spatial memory distorts familiar multilevel buildings. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0141257. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Ertl M, Klaus M, Brandt T, et al. Distorted mental spatial representation of multi-level buildings—humans are biased towards equilateral shapes of height and width. Sci Rep. 2019;9:15046. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-50992-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Wang L, Cohen AS, Carr M. Spatial ability at two scales of representation: a meta-analysis. Learn Individ Differ. 2014;36:140–144. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2014.10.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Tarampi MR, Heydari N, Hegarty M. A tale of two types of perspective taking: sex differences in spatial ability. Psychol Sci. 2016;27:1507–1516. doi: 10.1177/0956797616667459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Moreau D, Clerc J, Mansy-Dannay A, et al. Enhancing spatial ability through sport practice. J Individ Differ. 2012;33:83–88. doi: 10.1027/1614-0001/a000075. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Weckbacher LM, Okamoto Y. Spatial experiences of high academic achievers. J Educ Gift. 2012;35:48–65. doi: 10.1177/0162353211432038. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Doyle RA, Voyer D, Cherney ID. The relation between childhood spatial activities and spatial abilities in adulthood. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2012;33:112–120. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2012.01.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Proulx MJ, Todorov OS, Taylor Aiken A, et al. Where am I? Who am I? The relation between spatial cognition, social cognition and individual differences in the built environment. Front Psychol. 2016;7:64. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Kuehn E, Chen X, Geise P, et al. Social targets improve body-based and environment-based strategies during spatial navigation. Exp Brain Res. 2018;236:755–764. doi: 10.1007/s00221-018-5169-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Dalton RC, Hölscher C, Montello DR. Wayfinding as a social activity. Front Psychol. 2019;10:142. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Hegarty M, Montello DR, Richardson AE, et al. Spatial abilities at different scales: Individual differences in aptitude-test performance and spatial-layout learning. Intelligence. 2006;34:151–176. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2005.09.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]