Skip to main content
Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY) logoLink to Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY)
. 2022 Mar 15;33(3):794–810. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhac102

Prediction and action in cortical pain processing

Lina Koppel 1,2,3,, Giovanni Novembre 4,5, Robin Kämpe 6,7, Mattias Savallampi 8, India Morrison 9,10
PMCID: PMC9890457  PMID: 35289367

Abstract

Predicting that a stimulus is painful facilitates action to avoid harm. But how distinct are the neural processes underlying the prediction of upcoming painful events vis-à-vis those taking action to avoid them? Here, we investigated brain activity as a function of current and predicted painful or nonpainful thermal stimulation, as well as the ability of voluntary action to affect the duration of upcoming stimulation. Participants performed a task which involved the administration of a painful or nonpainful stimulus (S1), which predicted an immediately subsequent very painful or nonpainful stimulus (S2). Pressing a response button within a specified time window during S1 either reduced or did not reduce the duration of the upcoming stimulation. Predicted pain increased activation in several regions, including anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), midcingulate cortex (MCC), and insula; however, activation in ACC and MCC depended on whether a meaningful action was performed, with MCC activation showing a direct relationship with motor output. Insula’s responses for predicted pain were also modulated by potential action consequences, albeit without a direct relationship with motor output. These findings suggest that cortical pain processing is not specifically tied to the sensory stimulus, but instead, depends on the consequences of that stimulus for sensorimotor control of behavior.

Keywords: pain, prediction, action, anterior insula, midcingulate cortex

Introduction

The cerebral cortex plays a major role in generating predictions about internal and external events and in comparing and adjusting those predictions against incoming sensory information (Clark 2013). But prediction is of limited usefulness if it does not ultimately lead to changes in behavior which can support the well-being and survival of the organism. The ability to predict a potentially damaging painful event is vital when that pain—and, thus, any ensuing harm—is avoidable through voluntary action. In this study, we investigated how the cortical processing of acute pain is influenced by whether it predicts a second painful stimulation in the near future and whether participants can curtail this upcoming pain via voluntary action.

A growing body of evidence has shown that cortical pain processing and subjective experience are shaped by a variety of cognitive, affective, and contextual factors. A key observation has been that pain-related brain regions are active before a stimulation occurs (Ploghaus et al. 1999; Atlas and Wager 2012; Tu et al. 2020) and can even predict whether or not it is perceived as painful; for example, prestimulus activity in anterior insula (AI) is greater for stimuli rated as painful than for those rated as nonpainful (Ploner et al. 2010; Wiech et al. 2010). Expecting high (vs. low) pain also increases subjective pain reports, and this effect is mediated by activity in anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), AI, and thalamus (Atlas et al. 2010). This and other evidence indicate that the brain’s predictions about the potential consequences of an acute painful event influence the brain’s processing of those events.

Like expectation or prediction, action also has an integral functional link with pain. An essential feature of pain is that it motivates action to avoid harm (Morrison et al. 2013). In keeping with this, acute pain speeds reaction times (RTs) (Perini et al. 2013) and facilitates specific muscle responses (Neige et al. 2018). In other words, pain is in large part an action problem rather than a sensation per se. In this perspective, the brain flexibly adapts its responses to current and anticipated circumstances, taking into account the available options for meaningful behavior, with cortical pain processing modulated according to relevant aspects of the behavioral context. These aspects include voluntary action selection (Perini et al. 2013) and the behavioral relevance of the stimulus as it relates to the current situation (Perini et al. 2020).

Among such flexible cortical processes, the midcingulate cortex (MCC) plays a key role in supporting voluntary action to avoid harm. It is well-situated for this role through its involvement in the cortical control of voluntary movement during nonpainful (Matelli et al. 1991; Vogt and Morecraft 2009; Vogt and Sikes 2009; Hoffstaedter et al. 2013; Amiez and Petrides 2014; Procyk et al. 2016) and painful (Vogt 2005; Pereira et al. 2010; Shackman et al. 2011; Perini et al. 2013, 2020; Misra and Coombes 2014) stimulation in humans and nonhuman primates. In humans, MCC’s response to pain hinges on whether an action (e.g. a button press) is performed. By contrast, AI responses to pain occur regardless of overt action (Perini et al. 2013) but have nevertheless shown a sensitivity to the behavioral relevance of the pain (Perini et al. 2020). Moreover, these two regions work together to generate subjective motivational feelings during pain, with functional connectivity between AI and MCC increasing with higher self-reported urge to move during pain (Perini et al. 2020).

If prediction and meaningful, motivated action are critical component functions of pain, how can they be disentangled for investigation of their underlying functional neuroanatomy? To do so requires experimentally dissociating acute pain processing (what is happening now) from predictive processing (what happens next) and probing the effects of motivated action (what can be done) on these processes. We pursued this aim by using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to measure hemodynamic changes during an experimental paradigm in which participants were administered acute painful or nonpainful thermal stimulation (“current”) which predicted upcoming suprathreshold painful or nonpainful stimulation (“predicted”) later in the trial.

This experimental task required participants to make a timed button-press response during the stimulation in each trial regardless of whether the current stimulus was painful or nonpainful. At the start of each trial, participants were informed of the current stimulus’ (S1) predictive relationship to an upcoming stimulus (S2) as well as whether a successfully timed button-press within a 450-ms window would shorten the upcoming stimulation by 3 s (“effective action” or “ineffective action” trials). This paradigm allowed dissociation of immediate from predicted effects of pain processing (current and predicted pain during S1) as well as any general effects of action from task-related processing (button-presses which would or would not affect S2). It also allowed dissociation of the effects of action execution (button-press) from action consequence (shortening upcoming stimulation).

With this paradigm, we tested for any pain-specific neural modulation of, and any interactions among, 3 major factors during the “current” stimulation in S1: (i) current pain in S1 regardless of whether it predicts upcoming pain in S2, (ii) predicted pain in S2 regardless of current stimulation in S1; and (iii) the ability to affect S2 by making an accurately timed action during S1. In (i), “predicted” pain is held constant, potentially revealing activations selective for current stimulation; a lack of modulation here might indicate that current stimulation has no specific influence on predictive pain processing. Likewise, in (ii), “current” pain is held constant, potentially revealing activations selectively modulated by the predicted stimulation. A lack of modulation here might indicate that current pain processing outweighs predicted pain.

In its operationalization of the factors pain, prediction, and action, this paradigm pulls apart in time functions which may normally be “bundled” in a less attenuated way during real-life acute pain processing. In ecological acute pain, the cortical process that corresponds to the “prediction” factor in this experiment is not necessarily a forecast of a future painful event. Rather, it may reflect a broader apprehension of the immediate consequences of ongoing sensations, handled by the brain in terms of the scope for limiting potential or actual tissue damage via behavioral adjustment. What you sense now depends on what you just did; by the same token, what you will sense soon depends on what you are doing now. In this perspective, such a broadly predictive or apprehensive process would facilitate behavioral responses more likely to aid escape or avoidance of tissue damage in the moment while also adapting to the particular circumstances of the painful event, for example, with regard to prior experience, current goals, and situational context.

We hypothesized that AI would distinguish between predicted painful and nonpainful stimulation regardless of whether or not the upcoming stimulus was controllable by action, whereas MCC would selectively respond to predicted stimulation that was controllable by action regardless of whether or not it was painful. The methods, analysis plan, and specific hypotheses for each contrast of interest were preregistered on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/8p7rq).

Materials and methods

Participants

Forty participants were recruited from a student subject pool at Linköping University, Sweden, using the Online Recruitment System for Experiments in Economics (Greiner 2015). Inclusion criteria were the following: age range = 18–40 years old, right-handed, no magnetic metals in body, no preexisting neurological history (e.g. injury, stroke), not be taking medication related to neurological or psychiatric disorder (e.g. epilepsy or depression), and absence of claustrophobia. Participants gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and were compensated at an hourly rate of 200 SEK (approx. 20 USD). The study was approved by the regional ethics committee (Dnr 2014/340-31). Three participants withdrew from the study before any fMRI data had been collected due to discomfort in the scanner environment, and 7 participants were excluded due to technical problems, leaving 30 participants (17 male, age = 24.33 ± 3.25 years [M ± standard deviation {SD}]) in the final sample. Due to technical issues, 2 participants only have data from 1 of 2 functional runs.

Pain stimuli

Physical pain was delivered using a 3 × 3-cm thermal stimulator probe (Pathway model ATS, Medoc Ltd, Ramat Yishai, Israel) on the dorsal part of the left forearm. Prior to the experiment, thresholds for warmth, cold, and heat pain as well as heat pain limits were determined using a procedure adapted from Perini et al. (2013) in which the thermode had a baseline temperature of 32 °C and increased or decreased at a speed of 1 °C/s until participants pressed a mouse button positioned in their right hand when they felt a difference in temperature (warmth and cold thresholds; 4 trials each), when they started to feel pain (heat pain threshold; 4 trials), and when the temperature reached their maximum tolerable temperature (pain limit; 4 trials). The temperature never exceeded 50 °C. After they had pressed the button, the temperature returned to baseline. The resulting pain thresholds and pain limits were used in the experiment for “painful” and “very painful” stimuli, respectively, with a maximum temperature of 49 °C and at least 2 °C difference between painful and very painful stimuli. Thus, painful stimuli ranged from 42 °C to 47 °C (M = 44.86, SD = 1.77) and very painful stimuli ranged from 45 °C to 49 °C (M = 47.86, SD = 1.41). Nonpainful stimuli corresponded to baseline temperature, 32 °C.

Experimental design

The experiment had a 2 × 2 × 2 within-subjects design with the following factors: S1 (current stimulation: painful or nonpainful), S2 (predicted stimulation: very painful or nonpainful), and action (effective or ineffective). The trial structure is shown in Fig. 1. Each trial involved the administration of a painful or nonpainful stimulus (S1), followed by a very painful or nonpainful stimulus (S2). An experimenter (M.S.) was positioned beside the scanner and followed sound cues delivered via headphones (not audible to the participant), indicating the timing of thermode onset and offset for manual stimulus delivery (The reason for manual stimulus delivery was technical: the 2 programs running the experiment script and controlling the temperature of the thermode could not be integrated. This, together with the feedback-based design that involved delivering stimuli of varying duration depending on participants’ response, made it necessary to use manual stimulus delivery. Manual stimulus delivery also reduces problems with sensitization because the probe is not always in the exact same location and the thermode reaches target temperature prior to application [rather than ramping up and down while in contact with the participant’s skin].). The thermode was applied to the dorsal part of the left forearm and was preprogrammed to reach the target temperature before it was applied and to remain at target temperature until it was removed. Participants’ task was to press a response button (4-Button Diamond Fiber Optic Response Pad, Current Designs Inc., Philadelphia, USA) using their right index finger as fast as they could upon presentation of a response cue (a dot), which appeared 1 s following onset of S1 (1 s before offset). The cue remained onscreen for 450 ms (determined based on a pilot study; for details, see Supplementary Materials and Supplementary Figs. S1 and S2). On “action-effective” trials (50% of all trials), pressing the response button within the 450-ms time window reduced the duration of S2 from 4 s to 1 s. On “action-ineffective” trials, pressing the response button had no effect on the duration of S2. Participants were instructed to press the response button on all trials regardless of whether doing so would have an effect on the upcoming stimulus. At the start of each trial, participants were presented with a brief (3 s) instruction screen (see example in Fig. 1), informing them about the predictive relationship between S1 and S2 (“painful” or “nonpainful”) and whether pressing the response button would influence the upcoming stimulus (“response button: enabled” or “disabled”) on that trial.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Trial structure. Each trial began with an instruction screen (“cue”; 3 s), informing participants about the predictive relationship between S1 and S2 and whether pressing the response button would affect S2. A fixation cross (2 s) then appeared onscreen, followed by the delivery of a painful or nonpainful stimulus (S1; 2 s). Participants’ task was to press a response button upon the presentation of a cue (a dot), which was displayed 1 s following the onset of S1 for a duration of 450 ms. S1 was followed by a jittered ISI of 4–6 s with a fixation cross display, then by suprathreshold painful or nonpainful stimulation in S2 (1 s or 4 s, depending on action effectiveness and participants’ response). ITI duration varied depending on the duration of the trial and the ramping time of the stimulating thermode (ITI range: 3.56–16.86 s). S1 = stimulation 1 (“current pain”), S2 = stimulation 2 (“predicted pain”).

The experiment was programmed in Matlab and included 96 trials, separated into 2 blocks of 48 trials each. The order of the 2 blocks was counterbalanced between participants. The order of trials within each block was pseudorandomized using a Latin square (and its reverse) balancing the 4 combinations of S1 and S2 to help avoid first-order carryover effects. The action factor was folded in as an additional Latin square (and its reverse). This resulted in “mini-blocks” in which the task changed every four trials. The start of each trial was triggered by the Pathway. The interstimulus interval (ISI; i.e. time from S1 offset to S2 onset) was 4–6 s, randomly jittered by the Matlab script. The intertrial interval (ITI; i.e. time between S2 offset and cue onset) varied depending on trial duration and Pathway ramping time (ITI range 3.56–16.86 s).

fMRI data acquisition

fMRI data were acquired using a 3.0 Tesla Siemens scanner (Prisma; Siemens, Munich, Germany) with a 64-channel head coil (We stated in the preregistration that a 12-channel head coil would be used; however, the 12-channel head coil was replaced with a 64-channel head coil prior to the start of data collection for our study.). We collected data in 2 functional runs (1 for each task block), each lasting for 20 min and 51 s. For each run, 1,389 T2*-weighted echo-planar images (EPIs) containing 45 multiband slices were acquired (repetition time [TR]: 901 ms; echo time [TE]: 30 ms; slice thickness: 3 mm; matrix size: 64 × 64; field of view: 476 mm2 × 476 mm2; in-plane voxel resolution: 3 mm2; flip angle: Ernst angle [59°]). (We stated in the preregistration that 456 EPIs would be collected for each functional run; this number was based on the number reported in a previous publication using a different protocol and is an error. However, because the number of EPIs is directly linked to the duration of the experimental paradigm, this error should not affect the credibility of our findings.) Three dummy volumes were acquired before each run (automatically determined by the system) to ensure that data collection started after the longitudinal magnetization reached steady state. A high-resolution 3D T1-weighted (MP-RAGE) anatomical image was acquired before the first EPI (TR: 2,300 ms; TE: 2.36 ms; flip angle: 8°; field of view: 288 mm × 288 mm; voxel resolution: 0.87 mm × 0.87 mm × 0.90 mm; plane: sagittal; number of slices: 208).

fMRI data preprocessing and analysis

MRI preprocessing and statistical analysis was performed with the Analysis of Functional Neuro Images (AFNI) software v20.0.12. Blood oxygen level–dependent (BOLD) images were despiked and slice-time-corrected. For motion correction and coregistration purposes, each EPI volume was registered to the volume with the minimum outlier fraction (using the AFNI outlier definition). Functional images were then warped to Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template space using a combination of affine and nonlinear transformations. Nuisance effects due to head motion (estimated from the motion correction procedure) were accounted for by adding the motion parameters as regressors of no interest in the main regression. A motion censoring threshold of 0.3 mm per TR was implemented in combination with an outlier fraction threshold of 0.05 (We stated in the preregistration that fMRI data preprocessing and analysis would be performed according to current guidelines from the maintainers of AFNI. At that time, recommendations involved using a motion censoring threshold of 0.2 and an outlier fraction threshold of 0.1, as well as including the motion parameter derivatives as regressors of no interest, as mentioned in the preregistration. However, recommendations have since been updated, and changes were made to our preprocessing script based on the new recommendations. Thus, minor deviations from the preregistered preprocessing plan simply reflect updates in the AFNI guidelines.). Volumes violating either of these thresholds were subsequently ignored in the time-series regression.

A general linear model (GLM) analysis was performed to capture differences across conditions. Whole-brain, voxel-wise GLM statistical analysis was carried out on the BOLD time-series data using 3dmvm in AFNI. We conducted GLM-based analysis of a 1-s time window from the onset of S1. Predictors (convolved with a standard model of the hemodynamic response function) were created for each of the 8 combinations of current stimulation, predicted stimulation, and action effectiveness. Following Perini et al. (2013), we only included trials on which participants responded within 200–800 ms; all other trials were labeled as missed response and were included as a regressor of no interest. Regressors of no interest were also created for the rest of the duration of S1 (all 8 conditions combined into 1 regressor), cue (1 regressor for each of the 8 conditions), the ISI (1 regressor for predicted pain and 1 regressor for predicted nonpain) (The preregistration states ``inter trial interval'' rather than ``interstimulus interval''; this is an unintentional mistake in the preregistration.), and S2 (1 regressor each for the first second of pain, the first second of nonpain, and the rest of the duration of S2 [pain and nonpain combined; this was only relevant if action was ineffective or if the participant responded slower than 450 ms, otherwise the duration of S2 was 1 s]). Again, these regressors only included trials on which participants responded within 200–800 ms; all other trials were captured by an additional regressor of no interest (1 regressor each for S1, cue, ISI, and S2) (We did not specify in the preregistration exactly which regressors of no interest would be included in the analysis; any deviations resulting from the inclusion of regressors of no interest reflect the nonspecificity of the preregistered analysis plan rather than deliberate changes.).

The AFNI program 3dClustSim was used to determine cluster-size thresholds necessary for identifying effects significant at α = 0.05 family-wise-error corrected together with a voxel-wise P-value threshold of P = 0.002, which is in accordance with recommendations from the maintainers of AFNI (Cox et al. 2017). Average spatial smoothness estimates, across all participants, used by 3dClustSim were obtained using the 3dFWHMx function with the ACF flag, as per current recommendations from the maintainers of AFNI. Because AFNI outputs a single peak coordinate for each surviving cluster, a custom script was used to extract the coordinates for the first 10 peaks with the highest t-scores for each cluster.

In addition to the whole-brain analysis, we performed a region-of-interest (ROI) analysis in MCC and left and right insula. For the insula, we used activation clusters from a main effect of pain during S2 (see Table 3 and “Exploratory analyses” section below), excluding any voxels not in left or right insula for each respective ROI (remaining ROI cluster sizes: 257 voxels in left insula and 221 voxels in right insula). For the MCC, because there was no activation in this region in the analysis of activation during S2, we created a sphere with 10 mm radius based on coordinates reported in Perini et al. (2013). Specifically, we used coordinates from a conjunction analysis indicating common activity between motor responses during painful and nonpainful stimulation (MNI Neurosynth coordinates: −1, 8, 40, transformed from Talairach coordinates reported in Perini et al. 2013). Any voxels not in gray matter were removed (remaining ROI cluster size: 146 voxels). For each ROI, we extracted the ß values and performed a repeated-measures 2 × 2 × 2 factorial analysis of variance (ANOVA). We also performed correlational analyses between response times and mean ß values in the MCC and left and right insula. Finally, we extracted ß values in each of the 3 ROIs above, as well as in ACC, during the presentation of the cue at the start of each trial (3 s), during the first second of S1, and during the first second of S2. For the ACC ROI, we used an activation cluster from the main effect of upcoming stimulation during S1 (see Table 1) and removed any voxels not in ACC (remaining ROI cluster size: 84 voxels).

Table 3.

Exploratory ANOVA of activation during S2.

Cluster number (size) Anatomical region MNI coordinates F
Main effect of pain in S2 (pain > nonpain)
 #1 (575) Right insula lobe 31, 13, 13 36.377
Right putamen 25, 19, −8 35.145
Right pallidum 16, 7, −5 23.514
Right pallidum 16, −5, −5 21.752
Right insula lobe 40, −5, 7 20.041
Right insula lobe 37, −17, 10 17.039
 #2 (442) Left insula lobe −35, 10, 7 41.462
Left insula lobe −41, 1, 7 36.029
Left putamen −20, 10, 1 16.785
 #3 (148) Left thalamus −8, −23, −5 41.128
Midbrain 4, −29, −17 29.306
Main effect of nonpain in S2 (nonpain > pain)
 #1 (83) Left paracentral lobule −5, −32, 70 22.231
 #2 (73) Right postcentral gyrus 49, −8, 31 19.831
Right postcentral gyrus 64, −5, 25 19.411
 #3 (68) Left precuneus −8, −56, 19 18.393
Left precuneus −14, −53, 31 16.714
Left precuneus −8, −56, 43 13.925
 #4 (61) Right postcentral gyrus 52, −20, 61 35.883
Main effect of action in S2 (effective > ineffective)
 #1 (2,605) Right supramarginal gyrus 58, −23, 22 88.722
Right rolandic operculum 55, 13, 1 69.186
Right insula lobe 34, −17, 7 62.138
Right insula lobe 31, 28, −2 58.941
Right rolandic operculum 46, 1, 10 50.999
Right middle frontal gyrus 37, 43, 7 46.613
Right insula lobe 40, −2, −2 40.837
Right inferior frontal gyrus (p. triangularis) 49, 25, 10 39.865
Right superior temporal gyrus 61, −47, 13 39.719
Right supramarginal gyrus 67, −41, 31 37.233
 #2 (1,473) Left supramarginal gyrus −65, −23, 28 62.672
Left rolandic operculum −59, 4, 7 38.273
Left insula lobe −32, 13, 10 34.896
Left insula lobe −38, 1, 13 30.369
Left insula lobe −44, −2, −2 28.076
Left insula lobe −32, 28, 7 27.001
Left middle temporal gyrus −56, −62, 7 24.637
Left insula lobe −38, 7, −5 24.480
Left superior temporal gyrus −62, −50, 16 21.271
Left inferior parietal lobule −59, −35, 55 19.731
 #3 (470) Right SMA 7, 22, 52 54.180
Right SMA 4, 7, 52 32.766
Right superior medial gyrus 10, 40, 46 30.052
 #4 (414) Left cerebellum (VIII) −32, −56, 53 32.579
Left cerebellum (Crus 1) −20, −74, −29 18.702
Left cerebellum (VIII) −17, −77, −44 16.300
Left cerebellum (VI) −32, −53, −29 16.146
Left cerebellum (VIII) −20, −71, −53 15.843
Left cerebellum (crus 1) −47, −59, −32 13.809
Left fusiform gyrus −38, −50, −17 13.479
Left cerebellum (VI) −29, −41, −32 13.148
 #5 (136) Right postcentral gyrus 31, −32, 73 36.210
Right postcentral gyrus 22, −44, 70 33.548
 #6 (97) Left inferior frontal gyrus (p. triangularis) −38, 40, 13 33.992
Left middle frontal gyrus −47, 43, 19 27.496
 #7 (65) Right superior frontal gyrus 16, −2, 76 37.688
3-way interaction in S2 (S1 × S2 × action)
 #1 (286) Right superior medial gyrus 4, 61, 13 26.548
Right superior medial gyrus 7, 58, 34 21.298
Left ACC −8, 31, 13 17.487
Left ACC −11, 43, 13 17.316
Left superior medial gyrus −11, 61, 28 15.476
Left superior medial gyrus −14, 55, 4 14.782
 #2 (264) Right fusiform gyrus 37, −65, −14 23.628
Right lingual gyrus 16, −68, −8 21.726
Right lingual gyrus 19, −77, −14 21.072
Right calcarine gyrus 19, −80, 4 14.153
 #3 (149) Left midorbital gyrus −8, 40, −11 27.273
 #4 (102) Left middle temporal gyrus −62, −11, −14 21.215
Left middle temporal gyrus −62, −29,–5 14.020
 #5 (77) Right medial temporal pole 40, 7, −35 17.395
Right middle temporal gyrus 52, 7, −26 16.801
Right temporal pole 43, 16, −17 16.539
Right inferior temporal gyrus 34, 7, −47 14.856
 #6 (74) Right inferior occipital gyrus −29, −83, −11 24.242
Left inferior occipital gyrus −20, −92, −8 13.703

Notes. Coordinates indicate peak-level activation (MNI Neurosynth coordinates; x, y, z). Cluster size indicates number of voxels. All contrasts thresholded at P < 0.002, cluster-size thresholded at α = 0.05 FWE for n = 30 complete functional datasets.

Table 1.

Activations from ANOVA and t-tests.

Cluster number (size) Anatomical region MNI coordinates t
Main effect: predicted stimulation (pain > nonpain)
 #1 (503) Right insula lobe 34, 10, 7 7.167
Right caudate nucleus 13, 1, 7 5.993
Right thalamus 7, −8, −2 4.808
Right putamen 19, 10, 4 4.681
Right insula lobe 37, 16, −5 4.653
Right rolandic operculum 43, −3, 18 4.536
Right insula lobe 31, 25, 4 4.196
Right caudate nucleus 7, −3, 12 3.656
 #2 (326) Left precentral gyrus −56, 4, 16 5.256
Left insula lobe −38, 13, −2 4.347
Left inferior frontal gyrus (p. trinagularis) −38, 28, 4 4.210
Left putamen −29, −2, 1 4.073
Left putamen −20, 7, −11 3.926
Left insula lobe −35, 1, 16 3.907
Left rolandic operculum −44, −8, 16 3.820
 #3 (161) Right ACC 13, 40, 22 4.728
Right middle cingulate cortex 13, 25, 31 4.568
Right middle cingulate cortex 7, 34, 37 4.351
Left superior frontal gyrus −11, 31, 40 3.758
 #4 (107) Left middle frontal gyrus −47, 33, 39 5.042
Left middle frontal gyrus −26, 40, 22 4.376
Pain predicting pain > pain predicting nonpain
 #1 (492) Right putamen 31, 10, 7 6.645
Right pallidum 16, 1, −5 5.420
Right insula lobe 40, 19, −5 5.023
Right putamen 25, 22, −8 4.998
Right insula lobe 28, 21, 15 4.443
Right inferior frontal gyrus (p. triangularis) 43, 25, 7 4.085
 #2 (363) Left insula lobe −44, 10, −2 5.395
Left putamen −26, −2, −2 5.012
Left insula lobe −38, −8, 13 4.841
Left postcentral gyrus −50, −17, 16 4.118
Left inferior frontal gyrus (p. triangularis) −38, 25, −2 4.090
Left insula lobe −32, 16, −14 3.591
 #3 (172) Right middle cingulate cortex 13, 25, 31 5.258
Right ACC 10, 40, 16 4.802
 #4 (62) Right inferior frontal gyrus (p. opercularis) 55, 10, 19 4.686
Right rolandic operculum 46, 7, 13 3.638

Notes. Coordinates indicate peak-level activation (MNI Neurosynth coordinates; x, y, z). Cluster size indicates number of voxels. All contrasts thresholded at P < 0.002, cluster-size thresholded at α = 0.05 FWE for n = 30 complete functional datasets.

Results

Behavioral findings

On average, participants responded within the 450-ms response window (and thus successfully reduced the duration of the upcoming stimulation when the response button was enabled) on 85.9% of trials (for a distribution of response times, see Supplementary Fig. S3). To investigate whether there were any task-related effects on response times, we performed a 2 × 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA with current stimulation (pain or nonpain), predicted stimulation (pain or nonpain), and action (effective or ineffective) as within-subjects factors and response time as dependent variable. Only responses within 200–800 ms were included (93.6% of all responses, see Supplementary Fig. S3). This analysis revealed a significant main effect of action, F(1, 29) = 19.15, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.398, indicating that participants responded faster when the button-press action was effective (M = 322 ms, standard error [SE] = 11) than when it was ineffective (M = 340 ms, SE = 10; see Fig. 2). There were no main effects of current (F(1, 29) = 0.002, P = 0.963, ηp2 < 0.001) or predicted stimulation (F(1, 29) = 2.06, P = 0.162, ηp2 = 0.066) and no significant interactions (current × predicted: F(1, 29) = 2.38, P = 0.134, ηp2 = 0.076; current × action: F(1, 29) = 0.004, P = 0.949, ηp2 = < 0.001; predicted × action: F(1, 29) = 0.14, P = 0.713, ηp2 = 0.005; current × predicted × action: F(1, 29) = 1.99, P = 0.169, ηp2 = 0.064).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Whole-brain effects of predicted pain and action effectiveness. A) Noxious and innocuous thermal stimulation during S1 (“current pain”) which predicted upcoming pain in S2 (“predicted pain”) gave rise to selective BOLD increases in ACC/MCC, bilateral anterior/midinsula, putamen, and thalamus. Red indicates clusters showing increased signal change for the main effect of predicted pain versus predicted nonpain regardless of current stimulation or the effectiveness of the button-press action in shortening S2 duration; blue indicates clusters showing increased signal change for predicted pain when the current stimulus was painful regardless of action effectiveness; pink indicates overlapping voxels activated in both contrasts. B) whether the button-press action during S1 was effective in shortening S2 duration modulated BOLD activation in ACC/MCC and AI. Yellow indicates clusters showing selective activations for predicted pain when the current stimulus was painful during effective action trials; green indicates activation during trials in which the button-press would have no effect on S2 duration. All contrasts thresholded at P < 0.002, cluster-size thresholded at α = 0.05 FWE for n = 30 complete functional datasets. Images are displayed in radiological convention. S1 = stimulation 1 (“current pain”), S2 = stimulation 2 (“predicted pain”).

fMRI results: whole-brain analyses

We first performed a whole-brain repeated-measures 2 × 2 × 2 ANOVA using the 3dmvm program in AFNI with the following factors: current stimulation (pain or nonpain), predicted stimulation (pain or nonpain), and action (effective or ineffective). Results of any significant main effects or interactions were further explored with general linear tests.

Table 1 shows the resulting brain activations from the ANOVA. First, we expected a main effect of current pain in brain regions typically recruited during pain, including somatosensory cortices SI and SII, AI, ACC, thalamus, and prefrontal cortex (i.e. “pain matrix”). However, this analysis revealed no significant activation for the main effect of current pain during S1. Second, we predicted a main effect of predicted stimulation in AI. Indeed, upcoming pain showed greater activation than upcoming nonpain in a number of regions, including bilateral insula, right caudate nucleus, right thalamus, bilateral putamen, bilateral rolandic operculum, left precentral gyrus, left inferior frontal gyrus, right ACC, right MCC, left superior frontal gyrus, and left middle frontal gyrus (see Table 1 and Fig. 2); the reverse contrast revealed no significant activation. Third, we predicted a main effect of action in MCC. However, there was no main effect of action. There also were no interactions between current stimulation, predicted stimulation, and action in any brain regions.

We also performed 3 preregistered contrasts (t-tests) that were of particular interest. First, we compared activation on trials in which pain predicted pain to trials in which pain predicted nonpain in order to discover any selective activation for pain predicted by congruent current stimulation. Here, we expected increased activation in AI. This analysis revealed greater activation for predicted pain than predicted nonpain in bilateral putamen, right pallidum, bilateral insula, bilateral inferior frontal gyrus, left postcentral gyrus, right MCC, right ACC, and right rolandic operculum (see Table 1 and Fig. 2); the reverse contrast revealed no significant activation. Second, we compared activation for trials in which nonpain predicted pain to trials in which nonpain predicted nonpain in order to discover any selective activation for pain predicted by incongruent current stimulation. Here, we again expected increased AI activation. However, this contrast revealed no significant activation. Finally, we compared trials on which predicted stimulation was painful and action was effective to trials on which predicted stimulation was painful and action was ineffective regardless of current stimulation. Here, we predicted increased MCC activation. However, this analysis revealed no significant activation.

ROI results

Figure 3 shows the results from the ROI analysis. Bilateral insula ROIs were defined by activation clusters from a main effect of pain during S2, while an ROI in the MCC was created as a 10-mm sphere surrounding the peak coordinates for a conjunction analysis in Perini et al. (2013), indicating common activity between motor responses during painful and nonpainful stimulation. The ROIs were thus defined independently from the GLM applied in the main analysis to avoid circularity.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Effects of predicted pain and action effectiveness in cingulate and insula ROIs. A) MCC and insula ROIs: MCC cluster was defined by action selectivity during thermal stimulation in Perini et al. 2013; bilateral insula clusters were defined by the main effect of painful stimulation during S2 (P < 0.002) to avoid circularity with ROIs defined by the main analysis. B) Beta values for each ROI for the factors current pain, predicted pain, and action effectiveness. No above-threshold main effects or interactions in MCC were seen (a 3-way interaction was present at an alpha level of 0.10; see text in Results section). Left insula showed a main effect of upcoming predicted stimulation (P < 0.001) and an interaction between ongoing current stimulation, upcoming predicted stimulation, and action effectiveness, (P = 0.024), with lower relative activation for trials in which predicted pain was nonpainful and action ineffective. Right insula showed a main effect of upcoming predicted stimulation (P < 0.001), indicating greater activation for trials in which predicted stimulation was painful. C) RTs and correlation with ROI beta values. An ANOVA performed on RT values with factors current pain, predicted pain, and action effectiveness revealed a main effect of action (P < 0.001), with faster RTs for trials in which the button-press action was effective in shortening S2 duration. RTs correlated negatively with MCC beta values across action trials regardless of whether predicted stimulation was painful (r = −0.49, P = 0.005, 2-tailed; see also Supplementary Figs. S4 and S5), indicating a general relationship between MCC signal changes and behavioral response speed. No correlations with RTs were seen in insula ROIs (all Ps > 0.05). Images are displayed in radiological convention. S1 = stimulation 1 (“current pain”), S2 = stimulation 2 (“predicted pain”).

In right insula, there was a main effect of predicted stimulation, F(1, 29) = 25.02, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.463, indicating greater activation when predicted stimulation was painful compared to nonpainful. There were no other main effects or interactions (see Supplementary Table S1), although the interaction between current and predicted stimulation as well as the interaction between predicted stimulation and action were both significant at α = 0.10 (current × predicted stimulation: F(1, 29) = 3.06, P = 0.091, ηp2 = 0.095; predicted stimulation × action: F(1, 29) = 2.98, P = 0.095, ηp2 = 0.093).

In left insula, there was a main effect of predicted stimulation, F(1, 29) = 17.69, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.379, and a 3-way interaction between current stimulation, predicted stimulation, and action effectiveness, F(1, 29) = 5.70, P = 0.024, ηp2 = 0.164. Pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni correction indicated that activation was lower when pain predicted nonpain for which action was ineffective than when (i) pain predicted pain for which action was ineffective (t(106) = 3.26, P = 0.042), (ii) pain predicted pain for which action was effective (t(103) = 3.70, P = 0.010), and (iii) nonpain predicted pain for which action was effective (t(105) = 3.64, P = 0.012).

In MCC, there were no statistically significant main effects or interactions (see Supplementary Table S1), although the 3-way interaction between current stimulation, predicted stimulation, and action effectiveness was significant at α = 0.10, F(1, 29) = 3.42, P = 0.075, ηp2 = 0.105.

There was a significant negative correlation between response times and MCC activation on trials on which action was effective in shortening predicted stimulation, both when predicted stimulation was painful (r = −0.49, P = 0.005) and when it was nonpainful (r = −0.45, P = 0.012; see Fig. 3). There was no significant correlation between response times and activation in left or right insula neither when predicted stimulation was painful (left: r = −0.23, P = 0.22; right: r = −0.13, P = 0.49) nor when it was nonpainful (left: r = 0.002, P = 0.99, right: r = −0.10, P = 0.59). This pattern of results in MCC and insula remained regardless of whether current stimulation was painful or nonpainful (i.e. when painful and nonpainful trials were analyzed separately), although the correlation between response times and activation in the MCC when current stimulation was painful and predicted stimulation was nonpainful was only significant at α = 0.10 (see Supplementary Figs. S4 and S5). On trials on which action was not effective in shortening upcoming stimulation, there was a significant correlation between response times and activation in MCC but only when predicted stimulation was painful (r = −0.39, P = 0.034; see Supplementary Fig. S6); there were no other significant correlations.

Exploratory analyses

The t-tests from the whole-brain analysis above revealed activation in both insula and MCC for pain predicted by congruent current stimulation (see Table 1). To further explore this result, we conducted 2 additional t-tests investigating the effect separately for action-effective and action-ineffective trials. That is, we first compared activation on trials on which pain predicted pain to trials on which pain predicted nonpain and action would be effective upon upcoming stimulation (i.e. pain predicting “action-effective” pain vs. pain predicting “action-effective” nonpain). This analysis revealed increased activation in right insula, right ACC, and right MCC (see Table 2); the reverse contrast revealed no significant activation. We then repeated the analysis but for trials on which action was ineffective (i.e. pain predicting “action-ineffective” pain vs. pain predicting “action-ineffective” nonpain). This contrast revealed increased activation in left insula (see Table 2). We also performed corresponding t-tests for current nonpain. That is, we first compared activation on trials on which nonpain predicted “action-effective” pain to those in which nonpain predicted “action-effective” nonpain. This analysis revealed increased activation in right and left thalamus and right caudate (see Table 2). We then compared activation on action-ineffective trials in which nonpain predicted pain to action-ineffective trials in which nonpain predicted nonpain. This analysis revealed no significant activation.

Table 2.

Exploratory t-tests of activation during S1.

Cluster number (size) Anatomical region MNI coordinates t
Effective action: pain predicting pain > pain predicting nonpain
 #1 (67) Right insula lobe 40, 7, 7 4.531
 #2 (61) Right ACC 10, 31, 28 5.771
Right middle cingulate cortex 10, 13, 34 3.945
Ineffective action: pain predicting pain > pain predicting nonpain
 #1 (73) Left insula lobe −41, 16, 1 4.669
Effective action: nonpain predicting pain > nonpain predicting nonpain
 #1 (102) Right thalamus 10, −11, 16 5.469
Left thalamus −2, −8, 4 4.553
 #2 (55) Right caudate 13, 16, 4 5.263

Notes. Coordinates indicate peak-level activation (MNI Neurosynth coordinates; x, y, z). Cluster size indicates number of voxels. All contrasts thresholded at P < 0.002, cluster-size thresholded at α = 0.05 FWE for n = 30 complete functional datasets.

We performed an exploratory ANOVA of activity during a 1-s time interval from the onset of S2 to investigate BOLD changes during delivery of an expected stimulus (S2), preceding the feedback on the outcome of the button press (shortening the stimulation). The resulting activation is shown in Table 3. There was a main effect of S2, indicating greater activation for pain compared to nonpain in bilateral insula, bilateral putamen, right pallidum, left thalamus, and midbrain; the reverse contrast revealed greater activation in left paracentral lobule, right postcentral gyrus, and left precuneus. There was also a main effect of action, indicating greater activation when action was effective than when it was ineffective in a number of regions, including insula but excluding cingulate (see Table 3). The reverse contrast revealed no significant activation. Finally, there was a significant interaction between S1, S2, and action in right superior medial gyrus, left ACC, left superior medial gyrus, right fusiform gyrus, right lingual gyrus, right calcarine gyrus, left midorbital gyrus, bilateral middle temporal gyrus, right medial temporal pole, right inferior temporal gyrus, and left inferior occipital gyrus.

This study was also designed to hold constant the effects of current pain by testing for main effects of painful versus nonpainful stimulation in S2 (regardless of whether S1 was painful or nonpainful), allowing detection of any selective BOLD modulation by the predicted stimulation. This revealed that activation in ACC, MCC, and bilateral anterior and midinsula was influenced by the noxious nature of predicted events over and above that of current stimulation.

These regions were also modulated by the possibility of shortening the duration of upcoming stimulation in S2 by making an accurately timed, effective action during S1. In particular, ACC and MCC activation for predicted pain depended on whether a meaningful action was performed, with signal changes in a MCC ROI showing a direct relationship with motor output, which we had expected (https://osf.io/8p7rq). Contrary to our hypothesis, selective responses for predicted pain in AI were also modulated by potential action consequences, especially in a cluster in the left hemisphere.

Prediction and action in cingulate cortex

A cluster in ACC, extending into the rostral portion of MCC, was selectively engaged by predicted painful compared to predicted nonpainful stimulation (Fig. 2; Table 1), which is consistent with previous reports of anticipatory and ongoing responses to pain in this region (e.g. Ploghaus et al. 1999). If the current stimulation in S1 predicted painful stimulation in S2, these regions showed greater engagement regardless of whether the current stimulation was painful or whether the button-press action would be effective in shortening S2 stimulation. This ACC/MCC cluster also showed more specific selective responses when current pain versus current nonpain predicted upcoming painful stimulation (Fig. 2; Table 1). A subset of voxels in this ACC/MCC cluster was engaged by pain prediction only for trials in which a button-press action could affect future stimulation but not for trials in which action would have no effect (Fig. 2; Table 2). This suggests that pain-predictive activation in this region depended on whether voluntary action had a potential to affect the predicted outcome. However, in this paradigm, no cingulate region was sufficiently action-selective to be activated above a corrected threshold for the whole-brain main effect of meaningful action in S1.

On the whole-brain level, both ACC/MCC and AI showed a main effect for predicted pain during S1, though expected effects of current pain were undetectable in a main effects contrast at the whole-brain level, which was contrary to our expectation. We interpreted these findings as implying that the current stimulation’s meaning as a predictive cue had a stronger and more selective influence on pain processing than its meaning as an acute signal. This interpretation is supported by the observation in bilateral insula ROIs of relative BOLD increases during the condition in which nonpain in S1 predicted pain in S2, particularly during action-effective trials (Fig. 3; a similar increase was observed in the MCC ROI but in the absence of strong main effects or interactions). These changes likely contributed to the main effect of predicted pain while precluding a main effect of current pain since, in some conditions, current nonpain responses were not significantly lower than current pain responses.

During the visual prestimulus cue (which displayed the predictive relationship between S1 and S2), both ACC and MCC ROIs showed nonselective, above-baseline activation, followed by below-baseline responses across conditions in S1 (Fig. 4). This suggests a greater nonspecific sensitivity to cue information relative to the ensuing sensory stimulation, relative to baseline. The across-trial pattern may also reflect the possibility that the relevant information lay in the prediction (cue) and its specific outcomes (S2) rather than the sensory properties of the S1 stimulus per se.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Visualization of mean beta values for thermal stimulation in cingulate and insula ROIs across cue, S1, and S2 trial components. Top row: cue indicating relationship between S1 and S2 elicited nonspecific above-threshold activation across conditions in all ROIs. Middle row: S1 elicited below-threshold activation across conditions in all ROIs (for prediction-selective responses, see text and Fig. 3). Bottom row: S2 elicited preferential activation for painful stimulation in bilateral insula and preferential activation for action effectiveness in cingulate ROIs. S1 = stimulation 1 (“current pain”), S2 = stimulation 2 (“predicted pain”). Error bars represent SEs.

In macaque monkeys and humans, ACC and MCC contain premotor fields (cingulate motor areas/zones), with both output to and input from motoneurons in the spinal cord, well-situated for a role in action selection and control (Matelli et al. 1991; Dum and Strick 1996; Picard and Strick 1996; Koski and Paus 2000; Sewards and Sewards 2003; Vogt and Vogt 2003; Petrides and Pandya 2006; Dum et al. 2009; Shyu et al. 2010; Monosov et al. 2020; Ruehl et al. 2021). The organization of cingulate cortex, including the midcingulate zones, has been called “actotopic” (Caruana et al. 2018). Consistent with this, activation likelihood estimate meta-analyses of human neuroimaging reports have shown overlapping activations for acute cutaneous pain, action execution, and action preparation in MCC (Perini et al. 2013) as well as for pain, negative affect, and control here and in nearby cingulate subregions (Shackman et al. 2011).

The rostral midcingulate region has been specifically implicated in pain-motor relationships, for example, tracking individual variance in motor reflex reactivity, with nearby areas tracking autonomic variance (Piché et al. 2010). Macaque ACC has also shown selective responses to noxious thermal stimuli during voluntary escape response (Iwata et al. 2005). On the perceptual level, intracranial microstimulation of human ACC results in reported feelings of urgency rather than pain sensation (Bancaud et al. 1976; Hsieh et al. 1994). Nomenclature varies, but the predicted-pain cluster here likely corresponds to the human anterior rostral cingulate motor zone (RCZa; Amiez and Petrides 2014; Loh et al. 2017).

Caudal to RCZa lies a posterior rostral cingulate motor zone, corresponding approximately to the predefined MCC ROI (Perini et al. 2013). The motor- and action-related role of MCC is likely intimately linked to motivation to act through overt voluntary behavior (Morrison et al. 2013; Perini et al. 2013). For example, the subjective urge to move the hand away from a painful stimulus is disrupted in a type of congenital indifference to pain (Perini et al. 2020). Our previous work has shown that MCC activation during acute pain is related specifically to motor responses, but not specifically to pain, when motor responses during pain stimulation are controlled for (Perini et al. 2013, 2020). Pain activations in MCC also overlap with motor activations during the exertion of force by the hand (Misra and Coombes 2014). Ongoing pain has been associated with preemptive changes in signaling dynamics in premotor cortex (Misra and Coombes 2014) and in corticospinal tract excitability (Neige et al. 2018), with muscles under continual influence of descending faciliatory and inhibitory cortical interactions (Leis et al. 2000; Millan 2002; Urban et al. 2004; Sambo, Forster, et al. 2012; Sambo, Liang, et al. 2012).

Consistent with these premotor and motor roles and with previous results (Perini et al. 2013), MCC responses in the present study correlated with RTs in a general fashion across pain and nonpain trials (Fig. 3). RTs were faster during “effective action” trials, suggesting an increase in motivated responses in trials for which the button-presses would affect S2 (N.B: interactions [significant at α = 0.10] between action and pain had emerged in behavioral pilot studies outside the scanner, ηp2 = 0.104 and ηp2 = 0.175 in each pilot, respectively, but did not replicate in the main study; see Supplementary Materials). MCC activation also correlates with RTs during mere observation of others’ pain (Morrison et al. 2006). Indeed, a range of effects of observed pain on motor, sensorimotor, and peripheral muscle responses has been demonstrated (Avenanti et al. 2006; Morrison et al. 2007, 2012; Morrison and Downing 2007; Valeriani et al. 2008; Galang and Obhi 2020), suggesting that the brain’s ability to predict and react to the likely sensory outcomes of pain generalizes to visual input about others’ actions and experiences. These findings indicate a readiness in the central nervous system for avoidance action, which can be not only anticipatory but also adaptively tailored to specific parameters of painful stimuli (Farina et al. 2003) and type of available behavioral response (Morrison et al. 2007; though, see Galang et al. 2021) and task instruction (Galang and Obhi 2020).

An exploratory plot of group-level, unsmoothed data (Supplementary Fig. S7) pointed to a caudorostral gradient in the cingulate, going from a more action-specific caudal cluster (pain predicting pain > pain predicting nonpain in effective action but not ineffective action trials) to a more general pain-prediction cluster more rostrally (predicted pain regardless of action or current stimulus). A cluster for current pain predicting upcoming pain (regardless of action) was nested intermediately between them and extended more medially. This observation bolsters the proposal that cingulate premotor subregions work together to integrate stimulus content and current task demands to produce appropriate and timely responses (Vogt 2005; Kouneiher et al. 2009; Wiech and Tracey 2013). Such signaling likely involves recurrent feedback processing in a cingulate control hierarchy (Morrison et al. 2013), with less complex processing occurring more caudally (consistent with the action preference in the caudalmost cluster) and with increasingly more nested, contingent, and abstract processing in the more rostral direction of the dorsal ACC (Morrison et al. 2013; Loh et al. 2017). Here, this rostralmost cluster showed highest responses for predicted pain regardless of task or current stimulation.

Such rostral regions, such as the RCZa, may be enlisted when the situation involves higher levels of conditional information like those involved in the present task, such as increased task complexity (Kouneiher et al. 2009), internally generated actions (Mueller et al. 2007), decisions to shift from a default (Procyk et al. 2016), and comparison with current and predicted outcomes (Morrison et al. 2013). Rostral ACC regions are heavily interconnected with dorsomedial and dorsolateral prefrontal networks that also play key roles in executive processing and action selection (Loh et al. 2017) in both current and prospective temporal windows (Kolling et al. 2014, 2016). In the case of pain, the cingulate may thus encode painful events not in sensory terms, but in terms of action consequences, analogously to the goal-level (Michaels et al. 2020) and hierarchical (Koechlin and Summerfield 2007) encoding of actions in lateral premotor areas.

Pain-related processing in AI is modulated by both prediction and action

Alongside the anterior portions of cingulate cortex, AI has been centrally implicated in acute pain processing (Duerden and Albanese 2011; Knudsen et al. 2018). Here, bilateral AI/midinsula was selectively engaged by pain prediction regardless of ongoing stimulation or action as well as when current pain predicted future pain (Fig. 2). This is consistent with evidence that AI activity can predict whether a participant would classify a stimulus as painful, biasing “perceptual decisions” about pain even before the stimulus occurred (Wiech et al. 2010). Among other regions, including the cingulate cortex, the AI mediates cue-related anticipatory effects on pain perception (Atlas et al. 2010). The prior expectation of high or low intensity pain can shift sensory processing and perceptual decision-making biases to the expected outcome, as reflected in speeded or slowed incorrect RTs, respectively (Wiech et al. 2014; Zaman et al. 2018). In conjunction with the periaqueductal gray in the brainstem, AI is also modulated by trait anxiety in influencing pain reports of near-threshold stimuli (Ploner et al. 2010).

Previous findings have indicated that AI responses to pain are independent of overt action when executing a button-press is compared to refraining from a button-press (Perini et al. 2013). Such evidence for pain-selective responses has been further refined by the observation that AI tracks the behavioral relevance of pain as a function of task rather than responding to pain wholly independently of prevailing task requirements (Perini et al. 2020). Right AI was preferentially engaged by pain prediction for trials in which button-press action could affect future stimulation (Fig. 2) alongside ACC/MCC. This whole-brain effect did not emerge for the left AI/midinsula cluster (Fig. 2). This was contrary to our original expectation that AI would be modulated by pain but not action. Further, activation in both left and right insula ROIs (defined by the main effect of pain in S2) was modulated by the action factor (Fig. 3). All 3 factors (current stimulation, predicted stimulation, and action effectiveness) interacted in left insula, although in right insula, this interaction was only statistically significant at α = 0.10.

Despite such influence of the behavioral relevance of S2 during S1, BOLD responses in AI did not correlate with RTs either here or in a previous study (Perini et al. 2013). This suggests that while AI is sensitive to aspects of behavioral relevance in terms of action consequences, it is not directly related to producing the behavioral response. Nonetheless, responses to anticipated and ongoing pain in the AI and the MCC are likely interdependent. When an upcoming stimulus is threatening, the AI increases functional connectivity with MCC as a function of contextual information about the stimulus (Wiech et al. 2010). During painful stimulation, the functional connectivity between AI and MCC also covaries as a function of the subjective motivational urge to escape the painful stimulus through movement (Perini et al. 2020), implying that these regions play network-level roles in stamping pain with a subjective, motivated impetus to engage in escape behavior. The present findings further point to a role for AI in distinguishing whether a painful stimulus is relevant or irrelevant to the current task—and in this case, whether the current task has bearing on future outcomes.

More generally, the relative increases in both AI and ACC/MCC activity for effective action trials might appear contradictory to findings from a study by Salomons et al. (2004), which showed higher general activation in these regions during painful stimulation which was perceived as uncontrollable than when it was perceived as controllable. A possible interpretation of their findings is that increased AI and ACC activity reflects increased unpleasantness, and/or decreased predictability, of current uncontrollable compared to controllable pain. In the present study, the ability to change pain outcomes (in S2) via action (in S1) influenced brain activity before the painful experience occurred (Fig. 3). Since behavioral control over pain may decrease both stimulus unpredictability and subjective distress, a prospective sensitivity of AI and ACC/MCC to these aspects of pain is not necessarily at odds with the findings of Salomons et al. (2004) (see also Brascher et al. 2016).

The proposal that controllability reduces aversiveness is supported by Limbachia et al. (2021), which used a Bayesian analysis to compare the BOLD responses of participants who operated a wheel via button-press, which either would (controllable group) or would not (uncontrollable group) reduce the threat of electric shock coupled with the movements of shapes on a visual display. Controllability was associated with decreased AI and increased PI responses, which is in contrast to the present study, in which both regions showed greater relative activation in the effective-action conditions. It may be relevant that BOLD changes in the insula ROIs, as for the MCC ROI discussed in the previous section, were below baseline for S1 but above baseline for cue; depending on the comparison, AI responses during S1 would appear as deactivations (Fig. 4). It is also possible that the control/action variable is handled differently when a single action alternative is available to participants (effective or ineffective action on the group level) as opposed to two possible outcomes during the experimental session (effective or ineffective on a within-subject level). In addition, the control/action factor in our paradigm was included in a GLM with regressors capturing relative signal changes preceding the effects of the action, whereas Limbachia et al.’s effects were derived from multilevel Bayesian modeling of button-presses alongside state/trait anxiety scores. Thus, the two approaches may have tapped different modulatory aspects related to the stimulation and task context as well as interindividual covariation with self-reported anxiety.

AI has two major subdivisions, ventral and dorsal, though the connectivity profiles of these subregions are not always distinct (e.g. Kurth et al. 2010). The ventral subregion is associated with affective processing and is interconnected with key nodes of the classical limbic system such as the amygdala. The activations in the present study likely correspond to the dorsal subregion, which is associated with action and has anatomical and functional connections with parietal and cingulate networks (Kurth et al. 2010; Wiech et al. 2010; Touroutoglou et al. 2012). The larger activation clusters here also extended into putamen. Like the organization of cingulate cortex discussed in the foregoing section, the processing of nociceptive information may follow a caudo-rostral gradient in the insula (Deen et al. 2011; Cauda et al. 2012; Cerliani et al. 2012), going from more directly somatosensory-related processing in posterior insula to greater degree of integration in the AI, with gradual caudo-rostral shifts in connectivity with other cortical networks (Cerliani et al. 2012).

Cortical processing of predicted outcomes

Whereas the main analysis in this study focused on the effects of current and predicted pain in S1, “predicted” pain becomes “current” pain during S2, with S2 stimulation occurring after the task requirement in S1. Here, the pain factor potentially reflects effects of expectation confirmation based on the immediately preceding stimulus. Similarly, the action factor in S2 reflects a potential effect of feedback expectation based on the action effectiveness and timing accuracy of the button-press performed seconds earlier during S1. To examine the effects of preceding (S1) pain on pain-related BOLD changes in S2, as well as any effects of the action task factor, an exploratory analysis investigated the factors “previous pain” (painful vs. nonpainful stimulation in S1), “predicted pain” (painful vs. nonpainful stimulation in S2), and action effectiveness postresponse (expectation of an action consequence on S2 duration) (Table 3).

For the main effect of pain during S2, whole-brain regional activation was consistent with that expected from painful stimulation (Duerden and Albanese 2011; Knudsen et al. 2018), including bilateral AI extending onto inferior frontal gyrus as well as medial thalamus and midbrain. However, although cingulate clusters emerged for pain prediction in S1, they were absent in main effects of current pain for both S1 and S2. This suggests that the prediction factor can account for cingulate activation in S1, whereas the pain factor is unable to account for it in either S1 or S2. In previous studies (Perini et al. 2013, 2020), the cingulate was sensitive to the action factor when the task was performed during the stimulation, as was the case during S1 in the present study. However, in the present study, the ACC/MCC’s contribution to task performance had already been performed at this point in the trial, and it may not have differentially tracked the action factor postbutton-press.

Instead, a cluster in the pre-SMA/SMA was engaged by the main effect of action at the whole-brain level. The engagement of the pre-SMA/SMA may reflect feedback monitoring (Graziano and Botvinick 2002; Mueller et al. 2007; Kolling et al. 2016). Bilateral insula, bilateral lateral prefrontal and posterior parietal/TPJ regions, and cerebellum were also activated, which may make various contributions to the monitoring of expected task consequences. Further experimentation will be needed to more directly address the interplay of the modulatory influences of pain, action, prediction, and feedback monitoring in cingulate and adjacent medial frontal regions, particularly in light of the observation that temporal processing of reward/choice outcome trajectories in the cingulate can scale to the relevant timeline of trial events (Kolling et al. 2016).

Examination of BOLD responses in ROIs during cue, S1, and S2 trial segments (Fig. 4) suggested that selectivity for ongoing stimulation (or convergence on baseline in the case of ACC) emerges only during S2. This indicates that the realization in S2 of the specific pain-relevant outcomes presented in the prediction (cue) is differentiated with greater selectivity than the predicting stimulation (S1) in itself. More generally, it also suggests that there were neither pain-selective anticipatory effects in S1 which can account for the lack of a main effect of pain during stimulation nor was there a general baseline elevation which could have obscured the detection of relative differences during S1 in the full model. Further experimentation will be needed to rule out the possibility that the subjective level of pain in S1 was simply not sufficient to produce a differential effect, though if the painful stimulus were simply inadequate, one would not expect directional modulation of responses to current nonpainful stimulation that predicts pain.

In experimental pain paradigms in which a pain factor can be either controllable or uncontrollable, AI may be involved in both facilitatory and inhibitory signaling by virtue of its participation in a network that includes insular and prefrontal areas (Bromberg-Martin and Monosov 2020; Jezzini et al. 2021). The findings of Brascher et al. (2016) suggest that medial prefrontal (mPFC) and dorsal medial prefrontal regions may play a facilitating role in uncontrollable pain and an inhibitory role in controllable pain, respectively, via the AI (Brascher et al. 2016). In the present study, BOLD activation in the mPFC, including perigenual anterior cingulate (Brodmann areas 32/24), and orbitofrontal cortex interacted among the factors pain (S1), pain (S2), and action (expectation of an effect of action on S2 duration) (Table 3). This interaction was also seen in bilateral anterior temporal cortex, left inferior frontal gyrus, and bilateral occipital cortex.

Conclusion

This study temporally separated functional processes of sensory pain processing, stimulus relevance, and prediction in terms of action consequences. These processes likely occur in parallel and in a less attenuated fashion during ecological acute pain. BOLD activation in the ACC/MCC and AI/midinsula was preferentially modulated by whether an upcoming, predicted stimulus would be painful but not by whether a current stimulus was painful. ACC/MCC and AI/midinsula were more strongly engaged when the upcoming stimulation could be affected by the possibility that a button-press would shorten it, but this action factor also influenced AI responses to innocuous stimulation that predicted pain. These findings imply that cortical pain processing in these and other cortical regions is not specifically tied to the sensory stimulus but instead is processed in “consequence-level” terms based on what the stimulus implies for sensorimotor control of behavior.

Funding

This study was supported by Distinguished Young Investigator grant FYF-2013-687 from the Swedish Research Council to I.M.

Conflict of interest statement. None declared.

Data availability

Behavioral data and beta values extracted for the ROIs are available on the Open Science Framework and can be accessed at https://osf.io/ghxzy/. Unthresholded statistical maps of the fMRI results are available in NeuroVault at https://neurovault.org, and can be accessed with https://identifiers.org/neurovault.collection:11088.

Authors’ contributions

I.M. designed the research. L.K. and M.S. performed the research. L.K., G.N., and R.K. analyzed the data. I.M. and L.K. wrote the paper.

Supplementary Material

Predict_act_supplementary_revised_220202_bhac102

Contributor Information

Lina Koppel, Department of Management and Engineering, Division of Economics, Linköping University, 581 83 Linköping, Sweden; Center for Social and Affective Neuroscience, Department of Biomedical and Clinical Sciences, Linköping University, 581 83 Linköping, Sweden; Center for Medical Image Science and Visualization (CMIV), Linköping University Hospital, 581 85 Linköping, Sweden.

Giovanni Novembre, Center for Social and Affective Neuroscience, Department of Biomedical and Clinical Sciences, Linköping University, 581 83 Linköping, Sweden; Center for Medical Image Science and Visualization (CMIV), Linköping University Hospital, 581 85 Linköping, Sweden.

Robin Kämpe, Center for Social and Affective Neuroscience, Department of Biomedical and Clinical Sciences, Linköping University, 581 83 Linköping, Sweden; Center for Medical Image Science and Visualization (CMIV), Linköping University Hospital, 581 85 Linköping, Sweden.

Mattias Savallampi, Center for Social and Affective Neuroscience, Department of Biomedical and Clinical Sciences, Linköping University, 581 83 Linköping, Sweden.

India Morrison, Center for Social and Affective Neuroscience, Department of Biomedical and Clinical Sciences, Linköping University, 581 83 Linköping, Sweden; Center for Medical Image Science and Visualization (CMIV), Linköping University Hospital, 581 85 Linköping, Sweden.

References

  1. Amiez  C, Petrides  M. Neuroimaging evidence of the anatomo-functional organization of the human cingulate motor areas. Cereb Cortex. 2014:24:563–578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Atlas  LY, Wager  TD. How expectations shape pain. Neurosci Lett. 2012:520:140–148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Atlas  LY, Bolger  N, Lindquist  MA, Wager  TD. Brain mediators on perceived pain. J Neurosci. 2010:30:12964–12977. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Avenanti  A, Minio-Paluello  I, Bufalari  I, Aglioti  SM. Stimulus-driven modulation of motor-evoked potentials during observation of others’ pain. NeuroImage. 2006:32:316–324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bancaud  J, Talairach  J, Geier  S, Bonis  A, Trottier  S, Manrique  M. Manifestations comportementales induites par la stimulation électrique du gyrus cingulaire antérieur chez l’homme [Behavioral manifestations induced by electric stimulation of the anterior cingulate gyrus in man]. Rev Neurol (Paris). 1976:132:705–724. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brascher  AK, Becker  S, Hoeppli  ME, Schweinhardt  P. Different brain circuitries mediating controllable and uncontrollable pain. J Neurosci. 2016:36:5013–5025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bromberg-Martin  ES, Monosov  IE. Neural circuitry of information seeking. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2020:35:62–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Caruana  F, Gerbella  M, Avanzini  P, Gozzo  F, Pelliccia  V, Mai  R, Rizzolatti  G. Motor and emotional behaviours elicited by electrical stimulation of the human cingulate cortex. Brain. 2018:141:3035–3051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cauda  F, Torta  DM, Sacco  K, Geda  E, D’Agata  F, Costa  T, Amanzio  M. Shared “core” areas between the pain and other task-related networks. PLoS One. 2012:7:41929. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cerliani  L, Thomas  RM, Jbabdi  S, Siero  JC, Nanetti  L, Crippa  A, Keysers  C. Probabilistic tractography recovers a rostrocaudal trajectory of connectivity variability in the human insular cortex. Hum Brain Mapp. 2012:33:2005–2034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Clark  A. Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behav Brain Sci. 2013:36:181–204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cox  RW, Chen  G, Glen  DR, Reynolds  RC, Taylor  PA. FMRI clustering in AFNI: false-positive rates redux. Brain Connect. 2017:7:152–171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Deen  B, Pitskel  NB, Pelphrey  KA. Three systems of insular functional connectivity identified with cluster analysis. Cereb Cortex. 2011:21:1498–1506. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Duerden  EG, Albanese  M-C. Localization of pain-related brain activation: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging data. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011:34:109–149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Dum  RP, Strick  PL. Spinal cord terminations of the medial wall motor areas in macaque monkeys. J Neurosci. 1996:16:6513–6525. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dum  RP, Levinthal  DJ, Strick  PL. The spinothalamic system targets motor and sensory areas in the cerebral cortex of monkeys. J Neurosci. 2009:29:14223–14235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Farina  S, Tinazzi  M, Le  PD, Valeriani  M. Pain-related modulation of the human motor cortex. Neurol Res. 2003:25:130–142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Galang  CM, Obhi  SS. Please empathize! Instructions to empathise strengthen response facilitation after pain observation. Cognit Emot. 2020:34:316–328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Galang  CM, Pichtikova  M, Sanders  T, Obhi  SS. Investigating the effects of pain observation on approach and withdrawal actions. Exp Brain Res. 2021:239:847–856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Graziano  MSA, Botvinick  MM. How the brain represents the body: insights from neurophysiology and psychology. In W. Prinz, B. Hommel (editors). Common Mech Percept action (Attention Perform XIX). Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2002: p 136–157. [Google Scholar]
  21. Greiner  B. Subject pool recruitment procedures: organizing experiments with ORSEE. J Econ Sci Assoc. 2015:1:114–125. [Google Scholar]
  22. Hoffstaedter  F, Grefkes  C, Caspers  S, Roski  C, Palomero-Gallagher  N, Laird  AR, Fox  PT, Eickhoff  SB. The role of anterior midcingulate cortex in cognitive motor control. Hum Brain Mapp. 2013:35:2741–2753. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hsieh  JC, Hagermark  O, Stahle-Backdahl  M, Ericson  K, Eriksson  L, Stone-Elander  S, Ingvar  M. Urge to scratch represented in the human cerebral cortex during itch. J Neurophysiol. 1994:72:3004–3008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Iwata  K, Kamo  H, Ogawa  A, Tsuboi  Y, Noma  N, Mitsuhashi  Y, Taira  M, Koshikawa  N, Kitagawa  J. Anterior cingulate cortical neuronal activity during perception of noxious thermal stimuli in monkeys. J Neurophysiol. 2005:94:1980–1991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Jezzini  A, Bromberg-Martin  ES, Trambaiolli  LR, Haber  SN, Monosov  IE. A prefrontal network integrates preferences for advance information about uncertain rewards and punishments. Neuron. 2021:14:2339–2352.e5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Knudsen  L, Petersen  GL, Norskov  KN, Vase  L, Finnerup  N, Jensen  TS, Svensson  P. Review of neuroimaging studies related to pain modulation. Scand J Pain. 2018:2:108–120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Koechlin  E, Summerfield  C. An information theoretical approach to prefrontal executive function. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007:11:229–235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kolling  N, Wittmann  M, Rushworth  MFS. Multiple neural mechanisms of decision making and their competition under changing risk pressure. Neuron. 2014:81:1190–1202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kolling  N, Wittmann  MK, Behrens  TE, Boorman  ED, Mars  RB, Rushworth  MF. Value, search, persistence and model updating in anterior cingulate cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2016:19:1280–1285. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Koski  L, Paus  T. Functional connectivity of the anterior cingulate cortex within the human frontal lobe: a brain-mapping meta-analysis. Exp Brain Res. 2000:133:55–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kouneiher  F, Charron  S, Koechlin  E. Motivation and cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2009:12:939–945. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kurth  F, Zilles  K, Fox  PT, Laird  AR, Eickhoff  SB. A link between the systems: functional differentiation and integration within the human insula revealed by meta-analysis. Brain Struct Funct. 2010:214:519–534. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Leis  AA, Stokic  DS, Fuhr  P, Kofler  M, Kronenberg  MF, Wissel  J, Glocker  FX, Seifert  C, Stetkarova  I. Nociceptive fingertip stimulation inhibits synergistic motoneuron pools in the human upper limb. Neurology. 2000:55:1305–1309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Limbachia  C, Morrow  K, Khibovska  A, Meyer  C, Padmala  S, Pessoa  L. Controllability over stressor decreases responses in key threat-related brain areas. Commun Biol. 2021:4:42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Loh  KK, Hadj-Bouziane  F, Petrides  M, Procyk  E, Amiez  C. Rostro-caudal organization of connectivity between cingulate motor areas and lateral frontal regions. Front Neurosci. 2017:11:753. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Matelli  M, Luppino  G, Rizzolatti  G. Architecture of superior and mesial area 6 and the adjacent cingulate cortex in the macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol. 1991:311:445–462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Michaels  JA, Schaffelhofer  S, Agudelo-Toro  A, Scherberger  H. A goal-driven modular neural network predicts parietofrontal neural dynamics during grasping. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2020:117:32124–32135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Millan  MJ. Descending control of pain. Prog Neurobiol. 2002:66:355–474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Misra  G, Coombes  SA. Neuroimaging evidence of motor control and pain processing in the human midcingulate cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2014:25:1906–1919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Monosov  IE, Haber  SN, Leuthardt  EC, Jezzini  A. Anterior cingulate cortex and the control of dynamic behavior in primates. Curr Biol. 2020:30:1442–1454. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Morrison  I, Downing  PE. Organization of felt and seen pain responses in anterior cingulate cortex. NeuroImage. 2007:37:642–651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Morrison  I, Peelen  MV, Downing  PE. The sight of others’ pain modulates motor processing in human cingulate cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2006:17:2214–2222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Morrison  I, Poliakoff  E, Gordon  L, Downing  P. Response-specific effects of pain observation on motor behavior. Cognition. 2007:104:407–416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Morrison  I, Tipper  SP, Fenton-Adams  WL, Bach  P. “Feeling” others’ painful actions: the sensorimotor integration of pain and action information. Hum Brain Mapp. 2012:34:1982–1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Morrison  I, Perini  I, Dunham  J. Facets and mechanisms of adaptive pain behavior: predictive regulation and action. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013:7:1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Mueller  VA, Brass  M, Waszak  F, Prinz  W. The role of the preSMA and the rostral cingulate zone in internally selected actions. NeuroImage. 2007:37:1354–1361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Neige  C, Mavromatis  N, Gagné  M, Bouyer  LJ, Mercier  C. Effect of movement-related pain on behaviour and corticospinal excitability changes associated with arm movement preparation. J Physiol. 2018:596:2917–2929. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Pereira  MG, de  Oliveira  L, Erthal  FS, Joffily  M, Mocaiber  IF, Volchan  E, Pessoa  L. Emotion affects action: midcingulate cortex as a pivotal node of interaction between negative emotion and motor signals. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2010:10:94–106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Perini  I, Bergstrand  S, Morrison  I. Where pain meets action in the human brain. J Neurosci. 2013:33:15930–15939. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Perini  I, Ceko  M, Cerliani  L, van  Ettinger-Veenstra  H, Minde  J, Morrison  I. Mutation carriers with reduced C-afferent density reveal cortical dynamics of pain–action relationship during acute pain. Cereb Cortex. 2020:30:4858–4870. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Petrides  M, Pandya  DN. Efferent association pathways originating in the caudal prefrontal cortex in the macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol. 2006:498:227–251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Picard  N, Strick  PL. Motor areas of the medial wall: a review of their location and functional activation. Cereb Cortex. 1996:6:342–353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Piché  M, Arsenault  M, Rainville  P. Dissection of perceptual, motor and autonomic components of brain activity evoked by noxious stimulation. Pain. 2010:149:453–462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Ploghaus  A, Tracey  I, Gati  JS, Clare  S, Menon  RS, Matthews  PM, Rawlins  JNP. Dissociating pain from its anticipation in the human brain. Science (80-). 1999:284:1979–1981. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Ploner  M, Lee  MC, Wiech  K, Bingel  U, Tracey  I. Prestimulus functional connectivity determines pain perception in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010:107:355–360. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Procyk  E, Wilson  CRE, Stoll  FM, Faraut  MCM, Petrides  M, Amiez  C. Midcingulate motor map and feedback detection: converging data from humans and monkeys. Cereb Cortex. 2016:2:467–476. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Ruehl  RM, Ophey  L, Ertl  M, Zu  EP. The cingulate oculomotor cortex. Cortex. 2021:138:341–355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Salomons  TV, Johnstone  T, Backonja  MM, Davidson  RJ. Perceived controllability modulates the neural response to pain. J Neurosci. 2004:24:7199–7203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Sambo  CF, Forster  B, Williams  SC, Iannetti  GD. To blink or not to blink: fine cognitive tuning of the defensive peripersonal space. J Neurosci. 2012:32:12921–12927. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Sambo  CF, Liang  M, Cruccu  G, Iannetti  GD. Defensive peripersonal space: the blink reflex evoked by hand stimulation is increased when the hand is near the face. J Neurophysiol. 2012:107:880–889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Sewards  TV, Sewards  MA. Representations of motivational drives in mesial cortex, medial thalamus, hypothalamus and midbrain. Brain Res Bull. 2003:61:25–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Shackman  AJ, Salomons  TV, Slagter  HA, Fox  AS, Winter  JJ, Davidson  RJ. The integration of negative affect, pain and cognitive control in the cingulate cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011:12:154–167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Shyu  BC, Sikes  RW, Vogt  LJ, Vogt  BA. Nociceptive processing by anterior cingulate pyramidal neurons. J Neurophysiol. 2010:103:3287–3301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Touroutoglou  A, Hollenbeck  M, Dickerson  BC, Feldman  BL. Dissociable large-scale networks anchored in the right anterior insula subserve affective experience and attention. NeuroImage. 2012:60:1947–1958. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Tu  Y, Bi  Y, Zhang  L, Wei  H, Hu  L. Mesocorticolimbic pathways encode cue-based expectancy effects on pain. J Neurosci. 2020:40:382–394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Urban  PP, Solinski  M, Best  C, Rolke  R, Hopf  HC, Dieterich  M. Different short-term modulation of cortical motor output to distal and proximal upper-limb muscles during painful sensory nerve stimulation. Muscle Nerve. 2004:29:663–669. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Valeriani  M, Betti  V, Le  PD, De  AL, Miliucci  R, Restuccia  D, Avenanti  A, Aglioti  SM. Seeing the pain of others while being in pain: a laser-evoked potentials study. NeuroImage. 2008:40:1419–1428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Vogt  BA. Pain and emotion interactions in subregions of the cingulate gyrus. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005:6:533–544. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Vogt  BA, Morecraft  RJ. Cingulate gyrus. In: Binder  MD, Hirokawa  N, editors. editorsEncyclopedia of neuroscience. Berlin: Springer; 2009. pp. 722–726 [Google Scholar]
  70. Vogt  BA, Sikes  RW. Cingulate nociceptive circuitry and roles in pain pro- cessing: the cingulate premotor pain model. In: Vogt  BA, editors. editorCingulate neurobiology and disease. New York: Oxford University Press; 2009. pp. 312–339 [Google Scholar]
  71. Vogt  BA, Vogt  L. Cytology of human dorsal midcingulate and supplementary motor cortices. J Chem Neuroanat. 2003:26:301–309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Wiech  K, Tracey  I. Pain, decisions, and actions: a motivational perspective. Front Neurosci. 2013:7:1–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Wiech  K, Lin  CS, Brodersen  KH, Bingel  U, Ploner  M, Tracey  I. Anterior insula integrates information about salience into perceptual decisions about pain. J Neurosci. 2010:30:16324–16331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Wiech  K, Vandekerckhove  J, Zaman  J, Tuerlinckx  F, Vlaeyen  JW, Tracey  I. Influence of prior information on pain involves biased perceptual decision-making. Curr Biol. 2014:24:679–681. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Zaman  J, Wiech  K, Claes  N, Van Oudenhove  L, Van Diest  I, Vlaeyen  JWS. The influence of pain-related expectations on intensity perception of nonpainful somatosensory stimuli. Psychosom Med. 2018:80:836–844. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Predict_act_supplementary_revised_220202_bhac102

Data Availability Statement

Behavioral data and beta values extracted for the ROIs are available on the Open Science Framework and can be accessed at https://osf.io/ghxzy/. Unthresholded statistical maps of the fMRI results are available in NeuroVault at https://neurovault.org, and can be accessed with https://identifiers.org/neurovault.collection:11088.


Articles from Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY) are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES