Abstract
Sharp-wave ripples, prominently in the CA1 region of the hippocampus, are short oscillatory events accompanied by bursts of neural firing. Ripples and associated hippocampal place cell sequences communicate with cortical ensembles during slow-wave sleep, which has been shown to be critical for systems consolidation of episodic memories. This consolidation is not limited to a newly formed memory trace; instead, ripples appear to reactivate and consolidate memories spanning various experiences. Despite this broad spanning influence, ripples remain capable of producing precise memories. The underlying mechanisms that enable ripples to consolidate memories broadly and with specificity across experiences remain unknown. In this review, we discuss data that uncovers circuit-level processes that generate ripples and influence their characteristics during consolidation. Based on current knowledge, we propose that memory emerges from the integration of two parallel consolidation pathways in CA1: the rigid and plastic pathways. The rigid pathway generates ripples stochastically, providing a backbone upon which dynamic plastic pathway inputs carrying novel information are integrated.
Keywords: Hippocampus, Sharp-wave ripples, Slow-wave sleep, Memory consolidation, Rigid pathway, Plastic pathway
1. Introduction
The hippocampus is the center for episodic memory formation. While it performs crucial roles in encoding and consolidation of memories, it arguably has a limited role in long-term storage of memory, particularly the schematic form of memory that lacks contextual details of the original experience (Squire 1992, Squire and Bayley 2007, Winocur, Moscovitch et al. 2010, Preston and Eichenbaum 2013). Instead, long-term schematic memories are thought to be stored in distributed networks of cortical neuronal ensembles – small subsets of neurons that encode components of memory traces (Josselyn, Kohler et al. 2015, Tonegawa, Liu et al. 2015). Hippocampal reactivation is necessary to transform hippocampal dependent memories into cortical neural networks through its active role in a process called systems consolidation (Winocur, Moscovitch et al. 2010, Born and Wilhelm 2012). Systems consolidation involves coordinated, network-wide reactivation of neuronal ensembles during slow-wave sleep (SWS), which strengthens the synaptic connections between neuronal ensembles to allow for long-term storage of memory (Stickgold 2005, Klinzing, Niethard et al. 2019). Despite this understanding, the mechanisms underlying hippocampal reactivation during systems consolidation remain unclear.
The discovery of sharp-wave ripples, neural oscillations most prominent in hippocampal CA1 during SWS and immobility, have begun to uncover these mechanisms (Buzsáki, Horváth et al. 1992, Buzsaki 2015). Sharp-wave ripple is a complex field potential pattern composed of two interdependent and temporally linked events, the sharp wave and ripple. Sharp waves reflect large excitatory inputs to CA1 apical dendrites primarily in the stratum radiatum; and ripples are brief, 110–180 Hz oscillations that reflect alternating perisomatic inhibitions and synchronous spikes in the stratum pyramidale (Liu, Henin et al. 2022). Ripples accompany the reactivation of firing sequences of CA1 pyramidal neurons that occurred during wakefulness, a phenomenon commonly referred to as replay (Lee and Wilson 2002, Foster and Wilson 2006, Diba and Buzsáki 2007, Davidson, Kloosterman et al. 2009). This ripple-associated replay of wakefulness neural sequences is believed to consolidate hippocampal-dependent experiences into long-term storage in the cortex. In support, prolonging ripple duration or enhancing ripple coupling with cortical delta waves and spindles post-learning has been shown to improve memory (Maingret, Girardeau et al. 2016, Fernández-Ruiz, Oliva et al. 2019). Conversely, disruption of ripple and associated activity post-learning leads to memory impairments (Girardeau, Benchenane et al. 2009, Ego-Stengel and Wilson 2010, Jadhav, Kemere et al. 2012, Wang, Yau et al. 2015, Gridchyn, Schoenenberger et al. 2020). These findings, along with other studies that found pronounced hippocampal-cortical communication during ripples, provide compelling evidence supporting the important role of hippocampal ripples in systems consolidation (Siapas and Wilson 1998, Sirota, Csicsvari et al. 2003, Isomura, Sirota et al. 2006, Peyrache, Khamassi et al. 2009).
Ripple content – the specific set of CA1 neurons that fire during a ripple event – varies depending on which memory traces or trajectories are being reactivated (Karlsson and Frank 2009, Gupta, van der Meer et al. 2010, Pfeiffer and Foster 2013, Gillespie, Astudillo Maya et al. 2021). Memory specificity thus appears to emerge from the unique temporal arrangements of CA1 neuronal activity within different ripple content during consolidation. It is still unknown, however, how individual CA1 neurons are selectively recruited and incorporated into ripples and how ripple content is amended following learning. In this review, we aim to elucidate the mechanism by which ripple content evolves following learning and how this contributes to systems consolidation. We propose the existence of a dual-pathway framework wherein ripples are generated through a stochastic rigid pathway that encodes backbone information through preconfigured neural sequence motifs, while novel and salient experiences are incorporated into these motifs through a dynamic plastic pathway.
2. Stochastic generation of sharp-wave ripples
2.1. Sharp-wave ripples are generated within the CA3-CA1 network
A main feature of ripples is their routine generation during SWS and immobility regardless of the presence or absence of immediate prior experiences (Ylinen, Bragin et al. 1995, Dragoi and Tonegawa 2011). Researchers have characterized in vitro spontaneous generation of ripple events in hippocampal CA1 in the absence of extrahippocampal inputs, suggesting that a local circuit within the hippocampus is self-sufficient in generating ripples (Behrens, van den Boom et al. 2005). Excitatory pyramidal neurons in the CA3 of the hippocampus likely drive the generation of ripples, as they send dense projections to CA1 pyramidal neurons and are necessary for inducing ripple events during SWS and immobility (Nakashiba, Buhl et al. 2009, Buzsaki 2015). Importantly, acute optogenetic silencing of CA3 terminals in the CA1 drastically suppresses ripple occurrence in vivo, providing direct evidence supporting CA3’s key role in ripple generation (Davoudi and Foster 2019).
During SWS there is baseline tonic activity within CA3, likely due to a combination of intrinsic properties of CA3 neurons and external inputs such as cortical Up-state inputs (Hahn, McFarland et al. 2012, Schlingloff, Kali et al. 2014). This tonic activity leads to the accumulation of excitatory post-synaptic currents (EPSCs) across reciprocally connected CA3 neurons, and thus gradually raises their membrane potential to a depolarization threshold, which results in stochastic population-level activation (Behrens, van den Boom et al. 2005, de la Prida, Huberfeld et al. 2006, Schlingloff, Kali et al. 2014). At the molecular level, this gradual buildup of depolarization is mediated by the AMPA and kainate glutamate receptors, as blockade of them eliminates or reduces ripple generation in vitro (Maier, Nimmrich et al. 2003, Colgin, Kubota et al. 2004, Behrens, van den Boom et al. 2005). Further supporting CA3’s role in ripple generation, researchers found that EPSCs of CA3 neurons accumulate exponentially ~50 ms before ripple onset in vitro (Behrens, van den Boom et al. 2005, Schlingloff, Kali et al. 2014). In summary, CA3 EPSC accumulation results from the buildup of baseline tonic excitatory inputs. Recurrent excitation in CA3, therefore, allows for the conversion of incremental EPSCs into propagating population-wide excitability of CA3 neurons. Once the depolarization threshold is reached, CA3 population excitability activates CA1 neurons, intermittently triggering the generation of sharp-wave ripples. Notably, this is largely a stochastic process due to the gradual buildup of EPSCs in CA3 neurons.
Although CA3 is thought to be the primary driver of ripple generation, additional hippocampal subregions, such as CA2 and the dentate gyrus, also facilitate ripple generation. It has been shown that optimized optogenetic activation of the CA2 triggers ripple generation and facilitates social memory (Oliva, Fernández-Ruiz et al. 2020). Additionally, the dentate gyrus is necessary for CA3-mediated generation of ripples that support goal-directed behavior and memory (Sasaki, Piatti et al. 2018). More recently, the subiculum has been implicated as another brain region to generate ripple that can backpropagate to the CA1 (Imbrosci, Nitzan et al. 2021). Despite the complexity of neural circuits, CA3 remains the major contributor in sharp-wave ripple generation (Liu, Henin et al. 2022).
2.2. Ripple generation is regulated by interneurons and subcortical inputs
Ripples are not continuous; instead, they occur at variable intervals throughout SWS and immobility. Following a ripple event, there is a short refractory period where additional ripples are not generated due to hyperpolarization (Buzsaki 2015). This refractory period outlasts the refractory period of individual CA1 or CA3 neurons, suggesting that an additional mechanism is responsible for regulating the process of ripple generation (Patel 2015). Interneurons emerge as important candidates due to their known ability to drive oscillatory patterns in the brain (Freund and Buzsaki 1996, Buzsaki 2015). In particular, CA1 parvalbumin (PV) interneurons play a key role in the generation of ripples: selective activation of PV interneurons triggers ripple generation, whereas suppression of PV interneurons perturbs ripple oscillation and impairs memory (Schlingloff, Kali et al. 2014, Gan, Weng et al. 2017, Ognjanovski, Schaeffer et al. 2017). Other types of interneurons, notably the axo-axonic and oriens-lacunosum/moleculare (OLM) neurons, may also regulate ripple generation. These axo-axonic and OLM interneurons turn silent during ripples, raising the possibility that their activation suppresses ripple generation (Klausberger and Somogyi 2008). Together, CA3 recurrent excitation likely triggers coordinated activation of multiple CA1 interneuron subtypes that can initiate, pause, or terminate ripple activity.
Subcortical inputs to the CA1, including projections from the median raphe and medial septum, may also play a role in regulating stochastic ripple generation. In fact, optogenetic activation of the median raphe or septal cholinergic neurons drastically suppresses ripple occurrence, indicating an extra-hippocampal regulation of ripple generation (Vandecasteele, Varga et al. 2014, Wang, Yau et al. 2015). Septal cholinergic neurons exhibit state-dependent activity with maximal activation during active wakefulness and paradoxical sleep, yet almost complete silence during SWS (Lee, Hassani et al. 2005). These cholinergic inputs act on presynaptic cholinergic/muscarinergic receptors, leading to a suppression of CA3 recurrent excitation (Buzsaki 2015). Therefore, septal cholinergic activity may block ripple generation altogether during active wakefulness and paradoxical sleep. During SWS, the median raphe dominates the regulation of ripple generation: its activation drastically decreases ripple occurrence, whereas its suppression increases ripple occurrence (Wang, Yau et al. 2015). This effect on ripple generation is largely mediated by a subset of median raphe neurons (Wang, Yau et al. 2015), which exclusively target hippocampal non-PV interneurons (Miettinen and Freund 1992, Szonyi, Mayer et al. 2016). This subset of raphe neurons was confirmed as Vglut3 neurons in a later study (Huang, Ikemoto et al. 2022). Notably, these Vglut3 neurons decrease their activity prior to ripple occurrence but increase their activity at the termination of ripple events, which likely suppresses the immediate occurrence of another ripple event (Wang, Yau et al. 2015). Therefore, raphe Vglut3 activity appears to separate adjacent ripple events by imposing a brief silent period between events. This potentially prevents distinct ripple contents from intermixing during consolidation. Together, these findings provide direct evidence that the raphe and septal projections to the CA1 regulate the stochastic generation of ripples in a state-dependent manner.
3. Rigid and plastic CA1 neurons
Growing evidence suggests that functional diversity in ripple content arises from specific neuron types in the CA1 (Mizuseki, Diba et al. 2011, Danielson, Zaremba et al. 2016, Geiller, Fattahi et al. 2017, Li, Xu et al. 2017, Masurkar, Srinivas et al. 2017, Sun, Sotayo et al. 2017, Sharif, Tayebi et al. 2021). Two distinct populations of CA1 pyramidal neurons have been identified based on their firing properties during ripples, designated as rigid and plastic neurons. Rigid neurons tend to activate during ripples, and their activity exhibits limited change between pre- and post-learning sleep (Grosmark and Buzsaki 2016, Chenani, Sabariego et al. 2019). Therefore, rigid neurons appear to function as a ripple backbone that is not modified by new experiences. We speculate that the ripple backbone consists of a group of functionally connected neurons that represent either an internally generated sequence or a shared architecture of multiple traversed paths from prior experiences. By contrast, plastic neurons are adaptable and exhibit dynamic changes following novel experiences. These neurons not only alter their firing activity from pre- to post-learning sleep but may also undergo group reorganization, indicating their importance in encoding newly acquired memories (Grosmark and Buzsaki 2016, Liu, Sibille et al. 2018, Chenani, Sabariego et al. 2019). These findings support our theoretical two-module framework of ripple-mediated memory, where rigid neurons represent a stable, unchanging, ripple backbone that serves as a foundation for incorporating adaptive plastic neurons.
In addition to the rigid/plastic classification of CA1 pyramidal neurons, which relies on firing patterns, there is also classification system based on anatomical differences: CA1sup and CA1deep neurons, located in the superficial and deep pyramidal sublayers, respectively (Mizuseki, Diba et al. 2011). CA1sup and CA1deep neurons, although not entirely analogous to rigid and plastic neurons, overlap greatly. CA1sup neurons tend to exhibit rigid-like characteristics, with more stable firing rates and fewer place fields, whereas CA1deep neurons tend to exhibit plastic-like characteristics, with less stable firing rates, more place fields, and stronger modulation by memory demand (Mizuseki, Diba et al. 2011, Danielson, Zaremba et al. 2016, Geiller, Fattahi et al. 2017, Harvey, Robinson et al. 2022). Furthermore, CA1deep neurons are strongly activated by spatial landmarks, enriched cues, and sensory inputs during learning, whereas CA1sup neurons show little response to the same stimuli (Danielson, Zaremba et al. 2016, Geiller, Fattahi et al. 2017, Sharif, Tayebi et al. 2021). Importantly, CA1deep neuronal activity has been shown to correlate with success rate during learning. Researchers found that activity of CA1deep, but not CA1sup neurons, corresponds with reward zones in a goal learning task and predicts task performance, indicating a role of CA1deep neurons in learning (Danielson, Zaremba et al. 2016). These findings suggest that CA1sup neurons demonstrate rigid-like dynamics that are not modified by experiences, while CA1deep neurons exhibit a plastic-like phenotype that is important for new memory formation.
4. Rigid and plastic pathways emerging from CA1 sublayer differences
4.1. Rigid and plastic pathways
At the circuit level, CA1sup and CA1deep neurons receive distinct inputs, potentially contributing to their different characteristics and functions (Valero, Cid et al. 2015, Grosmark and Buzsaki 2016, Valero, Averkin et al. 2017). CA1sup neurons are more responsive to CA3 inputs than CA1deep neurons, whereas CA1deep neurons are more responsive to CA2 inputs (Kohara, Pignatelli et al. 2014, Valero, Cid et al. 2015, Grosmark and Buzsaki 2016, Valero, Averkin et al. 2017, Fernández-Ruiz, Oliva et al. 2019). Moreover, CA3 neurons are most active in response to optogenetic stimulation of the dentate gyrus (DG), while CA2 neurons are active in response to optogenetic stimulation of either the DG or entorhinal cortex (Kohara, Pignatelli et al. 2014, Sun, Sotayo et al. 2017). These findings lead us to characterize two distinct pathways, the DG→CA3→CA1sup (rigid pathway) and entorhinal/CA2→CA1deep (plastic pathway), which influence different sublayers of CA1 neurons (Figure 1).
Figure 1. Two major neural pathways for ripple-mediated memory consolidation.
Red, plastic pathway: Neurons originating from the cortex, mostly through the entorhinal cortex, either directly target CA1deep neurons or indirectly via CA2 neurons. Red dashed line, DG projects to the CA2, which in turn projects to CA1deep neurons, representing another likely plastic pathway. Blue, rigid pathway: Neurons originating from the cortex, through the entorhinal cortex, project to the DG which in turn projects to the CA3. Neurons in the CA3 form Schaffer collaterals and send excitatory projections to CA1 pyramidal and interneurons. PV interneurons preferentially receive CA1sup neuronal inputs and synapse onto CA1deep neurons. As a net effect, CA3 outputs activate CA1sup neurons but suppress CA1deep neurons. Social memory, dorsal CA2 neurons project to ventral CA1 which conveys social information.
The existence of rigid and plastic pathways raises the possibility that hippocampal ripple contents are influenced by their crosstalk. Indeed, a differential CA3 influence on CA1 sublayers leads to opposing effects between the rigid and plastic pathways. Although the CA3 projects comparably to CA1 deep and superficial sublayers, the feedforward inhibition via PV interneurons preferentially targets the deep layer, leading to a greater inhibition of CA1deep neurons compared to their superficial counterparts (Mizuseki, Diba et al. 2011, Kohara, Pignatelli et al. 2014, Lee, Marchionni et al. 2014, Stark, Roux et al. 2014, Valero, Cid et al. 2015). Moreover, CA1deep neurons can be further inhibited by CA1sup neurons which synapse on PV interneurons (Lee, Marchionni et al. 2014). These connectivity patterns manifest in increased hyperpolarization and, thus, decreased activation of CA1deep neurons during ripples. Therefore, CA3-induced ripple generation is predisposed to excite CA1sup neurons but suppress CA1deep neurons. This may reflect a mechanism which, at baseline, facilitates the rigid pathway activation while restricting integration of the plastic pathway into ongoing ripples when memory saliency is low.
4.2. Cortical control of the plastic pathway
CA1deep neurons, therefore, must rely on additional excitatory inputs, external to that of CA3, to overcome PV interneuron-mediated inhibition. Indeed, CA2 drives activation of CA1deep neurons; however, these CA2 neurons are hyperpolarized by CA3 inputs via feedforward inhibition (Kohara, Pignatelli et al. 2014, Valero, Cid et al. 2015, Fernandez-Lamo, Gomez-Dominguez et al. 2019). Therefore, only cases in which strong excitatory input to the CA2 can circumvent CA3-mediated inhibition will CA1deep neurons become activated. Outside of CA2, the medial and lateral entorhinal cortices, which have dense cortical connectivity, act as another mechanism to influence CA1deep neurons. The medial entorhinal cortex not only excites CA2, but also directly synapses onto CA1deep neurons (Kohara, Pignatelli et al. 2014, Masurkar, Srinivas et al. 2017). Lesion of the medial entorhinal cortex affects the activity of plastic but not rigid CA1 neurons, further indicating a selective influence on CA1deep neurons (Chenani, Sabariego et al. 2019). In parallel, the lateral entorhinal cortex projects to the CA2, which in turn targets ventral CA1 neurons, forming a separate circuitry that conveys social information required for the formation of social memories (Kohara, Pignatelli et al. 2014, Meira, Leroy et al. 2018, Lopez-Rojas, de Solis et al. 2022). Although the CA2 projects non-selectively to ventral CA1 sublayers, only the deep sublayer seems to mediate social memory via its projection to the nucleus accumbens and perhaps medial prefrontal cortex (Okuyama, Kitamura et al. 2016, Meira, Leroy et al. 2018, Xing, Mack et al. 2021). Together, CA2 and the entorhinal cortices potentially serve as relays, enabling broad cortical influence to circumvent CA3 baseline inhibition for direct and selective excitation of CA1deep neurons during ripples.
One possible mechanism by which cortical drive is strengthened to initiate plastic pathway activation is through learning-induced long-term potentiation (LTP) and cortical tagging (Teyler and DiScenna 1987, Lesburguères, Gobbo et al. 2011, Girardeau, Cei et al. 2014). In essence, cortical tagging posits that molecular and synaptic changes of cortical and connected neurons during learning form the foundation for future reactivation (Lesburguères, Gobbo et al. 2011, Girardeau, Cei et al. 2014). In support, it has been shown that selective lesion of cortical inputs to the hippocampus disrupts memory consolidation (Remondes and Schuman 2004). Following learning, the rate and length of ripples are increased, indicating a higher frequency of memory reactivation, as well as an incorporation of additional CA1 neural activity into ripple contents (Girardeau, Cei et al. 2014, Fernández-Ruiz, Oliva et al. 2019). These learning-induced ripple changes are reliant upon LTP, as blocking LTP prevents ripple rate alterations (Girardeau, Cei et al. 2014). These alterations most likely reflect specific changes in the recruitment of CA1deep/plastic but not CA1sup/rigid neurons because the latter exhibit little response to learning (Danielson, Zaremba et al. 2016, Grosmark and Buzsaki 2016). Therefore, LTP and cortical tagging during learning likely represent the fundamental elements that can later drive plastic pathway activity during ripples.
Direct evidence of a cortical influence on ripple content comes from a recent study that performed dual-site recording from the CA1 and auditory cortex (Rothschild, Eban et al. 2017). They found that selective auditory cortical neurons, following an auditory association learning paradigm, became reactivated during SWS and that this activation can predict CA1 firing during ripples (sublayer difference in the CA1 was not examined in this study). Although the auditory cortex does not project directly to CA1, it could influence the CA1 plastic pathway indirectly via its connection with the entorhinal cortex. Other sensory cortices, including the olfactory, visual, and somatosensory cortices, likely also influence ripple contents, given their implied roles in memory consolidation (Sirota, Csicsvari et al. 2003, Ji and Wilson 2007, Rasch, Buchel et al. 2007, Rudoy, Voss et al. 2009, Bendor and Wilson 2012). These findings, along with studies that show increased recruitment of CA1deep/plastic neurons during ripples post-learning, suggest that learning-induced cortical tagging is important for integrating plastic pathway activity onto preconfigured rigid frameworks.
5. Integration of rigid and plastic pathways
The rigid pathway, thus far, has been described as a stochastic framework that plastic information is built upon, but what exactly is the rigid pathway encoding? During ripples, hippocampal place cells replay sequences in a manner similar to how they were activated during wakefulness (Lee and Wilson 2002, Foster and Wilson 2006, Diba and Buzsáki 2007, Davidson, Kloosterman et al. 2009). Recently, this concept has been expanded with studies showing that ripple can replay sequences either in forward or reverse direction (Foster and Wilson 2006, Diba and Buzsáki 2007). In studies investigating ripple contents during awake immobility, researchers identified CA1 sequences that may even predict future paths in goal-directed behavioral paradigms (Pfeiffer and Foster 2013, Gillespie, Astudillo Maya et al. 2021). Most unexpectedly, ripples also preplay sequences that have yet to be experienced (Dragoi and Tonegawa 2011, Dragoi and Tonegawa 2013, Farooq and Dragoi 2019). Notably, these CA1 preplay motifs are skeleton-like, such that preplay events often recruit smaller sets of CA1 neurons compared to post-learning memory replays, reflecting a more cursory neural scaffold of information during preplays (Dragoi and Tonegawa 2013, Grosmark and Buzsaki 2016, Farooq, Sibille et al. 2019). These findings reflect a crucial development in the field that ripple contents are, at least in part, composed of preconfigured CA1 sequence motifs (Farooq and Dragoi 2019, McKenzie, Huszar et al. 2021). Based on one estimation, hippocampal CA1 neurons form at least 15 preplay sequence motifs (Dragoi and Tonegawa 2013). And more preplay sequence motifs could be identified as memory demand increases (Rich, Liaw et al. 2014, Liu, Sibille et al. 2021). We speculate that these preconfigured CA1 motifs represent an encoding of ripple backbone by the low-threshold rigid pathway. Functionally, this ripple backbone structure could enable a fast information coding, a notion supported by recent findings that replay of CA1 sequences requires minimal experience (Bittner, Milstein et al. 2017, Berners-Lee, Feng et al. 2022).
We acknowledge that there is controversy surrounding the notion of preplay. Notably, research from the Foster group reported no preplay in multiple settings, and that blocking NMDA receptors during experience disrupts nearly all subsequent replays (Silva, Feng et al. 2015, Foster 2017), which calls into question the original findings and analyses (Dragoi and Tonegawa 2011, Dragoi and Tonegawa 2013). This failed detection of preplay or replay sequences seems to be at least partly explained by the usage of different statistical criteria; nonetheless, additional follow-up studies with modified analyses have since replicated the original preplay findings (Grosmark and Buzsaki 2016, Farooq, Sibille et al. 2019). Additionally, a potential concern of the CA3→CA1sup rigid pathway model stems from a report that CA3 neurons form statistically independent representations of many environments, which indicates a de novo generation of CA3 place cell maps rather than the existence of preconfigured CA3 motifs (Alme, Miao et al. 2014). This finding, in theory, does not necessarily argue against the existence of preconfigured CA1 motifs, because each CA3 mapping may activate only one of a large repertoire of CA1 motifs. In support, a recent study implies the existence of a large repertoire of CA1 network pre-configurations existing across many environments (Liu, Sibille et al. 2021).
Essential for rigid and plastic pathway integration is temporal synchronization of neuronal activity, as synchronized activity within a short time window is critical for information binding and Hebbian plasticity. A number of cortical regions, including the auditory cortex, visual cortex, somatosensory cortex, entorhinal cortex, prelimbic cortex, cingulate cortex, retrosplenial cortex, etc., potentially influence ripple contents due to their pre-ripple activation (Sirota, Csicsvari et al. 2003, Isomura, Sirota et al. 2006, Ji and Wilson 2007, Wang and Ikemoto 2016, Rothschild, Eban et al. 2017, Opalka, Huang et al. 2020, Nitzan, Swanson et al. 2022). Notably, these cortical regions exhibit cyclic Up and Down states that are largely synchronous during SWS (Battaglia, Sutherland et al. 2004, Isomura, Sirota et al. 2006, Narikiyo, Mizuguchi et al. 2020). This synchrony, thus, enables a coordinated cortical influence on ripples that occur predominantly during the Up states (Sirota, Csicsvari et al. 2003, Isomura, Sirota et al. 2006, Nir, Staba et al. 2011, Wang and Ikemoto 2016, Opalka, Huang et al. 2020).
Building on the above findings, we propose a dual-pathway framework that integrates novel information into pre-existing ripple motifs for memory consolidation. We speculate that a global cortical Up state triggers ripple generation and simultaneously influences ripple contents. Specifically, the Up state activates the low-threshold DG→CA3→CA1sup rigid pathway that intermittently triggers stochastic ripple generation. Meanwhile, the same Up state reactivates distinct tagged cortical neuronal ensembles, particularly those embedded within sensory cortices, which are subsequently incorporated into ripple motifs through the entorhinal/CA2→CA1deep plastic pathway (Figure 2).
Figure 2. Theoretical concept of rigid and plastic pathway integration.
Cortical Up states trigger ripple onset and synchronize the timing of each pathway to be activated during Up states and silenced during Down states. Activation of rigid/plastic pathway depends on strength of inputs. Basal cortical activity from low-saliency events (such as exploring a familiar environment) is sent to the hippocampus, where it is sufficient in triggering rigid pathway activation of CA1sup neurons but fails to circumvent PV mediated inhibition of CA1deep neurons. In contrast, cortical tagging stemming from high-saliency events (such as receiving foot-shocks) provides strong excitatory signal that not only triggers rigid pathway activation but is also sufficient in activating plastic CA1deep neurons, which can be incorporated into preconfigured CA1sup motifs. On right, depicts two different ripple events varying in content. Top, rigid pathway ripple which contains skeleton-like motif of CA1sup neuronal spiking (blue). Bottom, plastic pathway ripple which incorporates plastic CA1deep neuronal spiking (red) into preconfigured CA1sup motif.
6. Conclusions
Central to memory is the brain’s ability to encode and consolidate a seemingly limitless range of experiences. Whether it be something as insignificant as breakfast or as momentous as our wedding day, our brain possesses a near effortless ability in consolidating memories of wide-spanning saliency. This requires a robust consolidatory process that can efficiently encode memories from a multitude of experiences without losing much specificity. How exactly does the brain accomplish this task? Here, we have argued that the hippocampus possesses two distinct memory pathways which work in tandem to integrate complex ongoing information onto a rigid preconfigured network of motifs for efficient and specific memory consolidation.
Converging evidence suggests that sharp-wave ripples are a key component in systems consolidation. Ripples have been robustly linked to the reactivation of CA1 place cells, as well as cortical oscillations like delta waves and spindles. In this review, we establish two distinct pathways which simultaneously influence hippocampal ripple contents. The first, is the rigid pathway composed of DG→CA3→CA1 projections that preferentially target the CA1sup neurons, which tend to exhibit stable firing properties across ripples. The second, is the plastic pathway composed of entorhinal/CA2→CA1 projections that preferentially target CA1deep neurons, which tend to exhibit dynamic firing properties between pre- and post-learning ripples. Essentially, we propose that the rigid pathway forms a framework consisting of preconfigured CA1 sequence motifs that are stochastically activated by low-threshold cortical inputs. These preconfigured motifs act as a backbone for plastic pathway integration. Salient and novel experiences strengthen cortical-hippocampal connectivity which drives the activation of plastic-pathway CA1deep neurons. This learning-based novel information becomes incorporated on to rigid motifs through cortical Up/Down state synchronization. We speculate that this dual-framework process is adaptive due to its ability to incorporate new information rapidly and efficiently. The stochastic activity of preexisting ripple motifs establishes ongoing communication with cortical networks which enables efficient and fast integration of plastic activity rather than generation of a new network-communication. As it stands, outstanding questions remain with future investigations needed to provide causal evidence to further establish and uncover these distinct ripple pathways (Box 1).
Box 1.
Outstanding Questions |
---|
What is the mechanism for specific preconfigured CA1 motifs to be selected for information coding by CA3 stochastic activation? |
Do cortical tagging associated neuronal ensembles selectively communicate with CA1 plastic neurons? As a prediction, the post-learning ripple associated CA1deep neuronal activity can be decoded by cortical population spikes. |
How do CA1 rigid and plastic neurons differ in terms of efferent projections? Do they have rigid and plastic counterparts in their targeted brain regions? |
CA1deep and CA1sup neurons express different levels of neuromodulator receptors such as acetylcholine and serotonin. How might these neuromodulators influence the rigid and plastic pathways? |
Highlights.
Sharp-wave ripples are generated within the CA3-CA1 network stochastically
CA1 pyramidal neurons can be separated into rigid and plastic neurons
Rigid and plastic pathways emerge from CA1 sublayer differences
Cortical Up/Down states integrate rigid and plastic pathways
Acknowledgement
This work is supported by the National Institute of Mental Health – National Institutes of Health (R01 MH119102) and the Pennsylvania Commonwealth Universal Research Enhancement Program (CURE 985065). We would like to thank Dr. Wen-Jun Gao, Dr. Rodrigo España, and Ashley Opalka for their thoughtful comments. Figures created with BioRender.com.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
- Alme CB, Miao C, Jezek K, Treves A, Moser EI and Moser MB (2014). “Place cells in the hippocampus: eleven maps for eleven rooms.” Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 111(52): 18428–18435. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Battaglia FP, Sutherland GR and McNaughton BL (2004). “Hippocampal sharp wave bursts coincide with neocortical “up-state” transitions.” Learn Mem 11(6): 697–704. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Behrens CJ, van den Boom LP, de Hoz L, Friedman A and Heinemann U (2005). “Induction of sharp wave–ripple complexes in vitro and reorganization of hippocampal networks.” Nature Neuroscience 8(11): 1560–1567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bendor D and Wilson MA (2012). “Biasing the content of hippocampal replay during sleep.” Nat Neurosci 15(10): 1439–1444. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berners-Lee A, Feng T, Silva D, Wu X, Ambrose ER, Pfeiffer BE and Foster DJ (2022). “Hippocampal replays appear after a single experience and incorporate greater detail with more experience.” Neuron 110(11): 1829–1842 e1825. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bittner KC, Milstein AD, Grienberger C, Romani S and Magee JC (2017). “Behavioral time scale synaptic plasticity underlies CA1 place fields.” Science 357(6355): 1033–1036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Born J and Wilhelm I (2012). “System consolidation of memory during sleep.” Psychological research 76(2): 192–203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsaki G (2015). “Hippocampal sharp wave-ripple: A cognitive biomarker for episodic memory and planning.” Hippocampus 25(10): 1073–1188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsáki G, Horváth Z, Urioste R, Hetke J and Wise K (1992). “High-frequency network oscillation in the hippocampus.” Science 256(5059): 1025–1027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chenani A, Sabariego M, Schlesiger MI, Leutgeb JK, Leutgeb S and Leibold C (2019). “Hippocampal CA1 replay becomes less prominent but more rigid without inputs from medial entorhinal cortex.” Nat Commun 10(1): 1341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colgin LL, Kubota D, Jia Y, Rex CS and Lynch G (2004). “Long-term potentiation is impaired in rat hippocampal slices that produce spontaneous sharp waves.” J Physiol 558(Pt 3): 953–961. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Danielson NB, Zaremba JD, Kaifosh P, Bowler J, Ladow M and Losonczy A (2016). “Sublayer-Specific Coding Dynamics during Spatial Navigation and Learning in Hippocampal Area CA1.” Neuron 91(3): 652–665. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davidson TJ, Kloosterman F and Wilson MA (2009). “Hippocampal replay of extended experience.” Neuron 63(4): 497–507. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davoudi H and Foster DJ (2019). “Acute silencing of hippocampal CA3 reveals a dominant role in place field responses.” Nat Neurosci 22(3): 337–342. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Prida LM, Huberfeld G, Cohen I and Miles R (2006). “Threshold behavior in the initiation of hippocampal population bursts.” Neuron 49(1): 131–142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diba K and Buzsáki G (2007). “Forward and reverse hippocampal place-cell sequences during ripples.” Nat Neurosci 10(10): 1241–1242. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dragoi G and Tonegawa S (2011). “Preplay of future place cell sequences by hippocampal cellular assemblies.” Nature 469(7330): 397–401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dragoi G and Tonegawa S (2013). “Distinct preplay of multiple novel spatial experiences in the rat.” Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110(22): 9100–9105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ego-Stengel V and Wilson MA (2010). “Disruption of ripple-associated hippocampal activity during rest impairs spatial learning in the rat.” Hippocampus 20(1): 1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farooq U and Dragoi G (2019). “Emergence of preconfigured and plastic time-compressed sequences in early postnatal development.” Science 363(6423): 168–173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farooq U, Sibille J, Liu K and Dragoi G (2019). “Strengthened Temporal Coordination within Pre-existing Sequential Cell Assemblies Supports Trajectory Replay.” Neuron 103(4): 719–733 e717. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernandez-Lamo I, Gomez-Dominguez D, Sanchez-Aguilera A, Oliva A, Morales AV, Valero M, Cid E, Berenyi A and de la Prida LM (2019). “Proximodistal organization of the CA2 hippocampal area.” Cell reports 26(7): 1734–1746. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernández-Ruiz A, Oliva A, Fermino de Oliveira E, Rocha-Almeida F, Tingley D and Buzsáki G (2019). “Long-duration hippocampal sharp wave ripples improve memory.” Science 364(6445): 1082–1086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foster DJ (2017). “Replay Comes of Age.” Annu Rev Neurosci 40: 581–602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foster DJ and Wilson MA (2006). “Reverse replay of behavioural sequences in hippocampal place cells during the awake state.” Nature 440(7084): 680–683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freund TF and Buzsaki G (1996). “Interneurons of the hippocampus.” Hippocampus 6(4): 347–470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gan J, Weng SM, Pernia-Andrade AJ, Csicsvari J and Jonas P (2017). “Phase-Locked Inhibition, but Not Excitation, Underlies Hippocampal Ripple Oscillations in Awake Mice In Vivo.” Neuron 93(2): 308–314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geiller T, Fattahi M, Choi JS and Royer S (2017). “Place cells are more strongly tied to landmarks in deep than in superficial CA1.” Nat Commun 8: 14531. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gillespie AK, Astudillo Maya DA, Denovellis EL, Liu DF, Kastner DB, Coulter ME, Roumis DK, Eden UT and Frank LM (2021). “Hippocampal replay reflects specific past experiences rather than a plan for subsequent choice.” Neuron 109(19): 3149–3163 e3146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Girardeau G, Benchenane K, Wiener SI, Buzsáki G and Zugaro MB (2009). “Selective suppression of hippocampal ripples impairs spatial memory.” Nat Neurosci 12(10): 1222–1223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Girardeau G, Cei A and Zugaro M (2014). “Learning-Induced Plasticity Regulates Hippocampal Sharp Wave-Ripple Drive.” The Journal of Neuroscience 34(15): 5176–5183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gridchyn I, Schoenenberger P, O’Neill J and Csicsvari J (2020). “Assembly-Specific Disruption of Hippocampal Replay Leads to Selective Memory Deficit.” Neuron 106(2): 291–300 e296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grosmark AD and Buzsaki G (2016). “Diversity in neural firing dynamics supports both rigid and learned hippocampal sequences.” Science 351(6280): 1440–1443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gupta AS, van der Meer MA, Touretzky DS and Redish AD (2010). “Hippocampal replay is not a simple function of experience.” Neuron 65(5): 695–705. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahn TT, McFarland JM, Berberich S, Sakmann B and Mehta MR (2012). “Spontaneous persistent activity in entorhinal cortex modulates cortico-hippocampal interaction in vivo.” Nat Neurosci 15(11): 1531–1538. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harvey RE, Robinson HL, Liu C, Oliva A and Fernandez-Ruiz A (2022). “Hippocampo-cortical circuits for selective memory encoding, routing, and replay.” bioRxiv: 2022.2009.2025.509420. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang W, Ikemoto S and Wang DV (2022). “Median Raphe Nonserotonergic Neurons Modulate Hippocampal Theta Oscillations.” J Neurosci 42(10): 1987–1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Imbrosci B, Nitzan N, McKenzie S, Donoso JR, Swaminathan A, Bohm C, Maier N and Schmitz D (2021). “Subiculum as a generator of sharp wave-ripples in the rodent hippocampus.” Cell Rep 35(3): 109021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Isomura Y, Sirota A, Ozen S, Montgomery S, Mizuseki K, Henze DA and Buzsaki G (2006). “Integration and segregation of activity in entorhinal-hippocampal subregions by neocortical slow oscillations.” Neuron 52(5): 871–882. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jadhav SP, Kemere C, German PW and Frank LM (2012). “Awake hippocampal sharp-wave ripples support spatial memory.” Science 336(6087): 1454–1458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ji D and Wilson MA (2007). “Coordinated memory replay in the visual cortex and hippocampus during sleep.” Nat Neurosci 10(1): 100–107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Josselyn SA, Kohler S and Frankland PW (2015). “Finding the engram.” Nat Rev Neurosci 16(9): 521–534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karlsson MP and Frank LM (2009). “Awake replay of remote experiences in the hippocampus.” Nat Neurosci 12(7): 913–918. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klausberger T and Somogyi P (2008). “Neuronal diversity and temporal dynamics: the unity of hippocampal circuit operations.” Science 321(5885): 53–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klinzing JG, Niethard N and Born J (2019). “Mechanisms of systems memory consolidation during sleep.” Nat Neurosci 22(10): 1598–1610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kohara K, Pignatelli M, Rivest AJ, Jung HY, Kitamura T, Suh J, Frank D, Kajikawa K, Mise N, Obata Y, Wickersham IR and Tonegawa S (2014). “Cell type-specific genetic and optogenetic tools reveal hippocampal CA2 circuits.” Nat Neurosci 17(2): 269–279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee AK and Wilson MA (2002). “Memory of sequential experience in the hippocampus during slow wave sleep.” Neuron 36(6): 1183–1194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee MG, Hassani OK, Alonso A and Jones BE (2005). “Cholinergic basal forebrain neurons burst with theta during waking and paradoxical sleep.” J Neurosci 25(17): 4365–4369. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee SH, Marchionni I, Bezaire M, Varga C, Danielson N, Lovett-Barron M, Losonczy A and Soltesz I (2014). “Parvalbumin-positive basket cells differentiate among hippocampal pyramidal cells.” Neuron 82(5): 1129–1144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lesburguères E, Gobbo OL, Alaux-Cantin S, Hambucken A, Trifilieff P and Bontempi B (2011). “Early tagging of cortical networks is required for the formation of enduring associative memory.” Science 331(6019): 924–928. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li Y, Xu J, Liu Y, Zhu J, Liu N, Zeng W, Huang N, Rasch MJ, Jiang H, Gu X, Li X, Luo M, Li C, Teng J, Chen J, Zeng S, Lin L and Zhang X (2017). “A distinct entorhinal cortex to hippocampal CA1 direct circuit for olfactory associative learning.” Nature Neuroscience 20(4): 559–570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu AA, Henin S, Abbaspoor S, Bragin A, Buffalo EA, Farrell JS, Foster DJ, Frank LM, Gedankien T, Gotman J, Guidera JA, Hoffman KL, Jacobs J, Kahana MJ, Li L, Liao Z, Lin JJ, Losonczy A, Malach R, van der Meer MA, McClain K, McNaughton BL, Norman Y, Navas-Olive A, de la Prida LM, Rueckemann JW, Sakon JJ, Skelin I, Soltesz I, Staresina BP, Weiss SA, Wilson MA, Zaghloul KA, Zugaro M and Buzsaki G (2022). “A consensus statement on detection of hippocampal sharp wave ripples and differentiation from other fast oscillations.” Nat Commun 13(1): 6000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu K, Sibille J and Dragoi G (2018). “Generative Predictive Codes by Multiplexed Hippocampal Neuronal Tuplets.” Neuron 99(6): 1329–1341 e1326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu K, Sibille J and Dragoi G (2021). “Orientation selectivity enhances context generalization and generative predictive coding in the hippocampus.” Neuron 109(22): 3688–3698.e3686. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lopez-Rojas J, de Solis CA, Leroy F, Kandel ER and Siegelbaum SA (2022). “A direct lateral entorhinal cortex to hippocampal CA2 circuit conveys social information required for social memory.” Neuron 110(9): 1559–1572 e1554. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maier N, Nimmrich V and Draguhn A (2003). “Cellular and network mechanisms underlying spontaneous sharp wave-ripple complexes in mouse hippocampal slices.” J Physiol 550(Pt 3): 873–887. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maingret N, Girardeau G, Todorova R, Goutierre M and Zugaro M (2016). “Hippocampo-cortical coupling mediates memory consolidation during sleep.” Nat Neurosci 19(7): 959–964. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masurkar AV, Srinivas KV, Brann DH, Warren R, Lowes DC and Siegelbaum SA (2017). “Medial and Lateral Entorhinal Cortex Differentially Excite Deep versus Superficial CA1 Pyramidal Neurons.” Cell Rep 18(1): 148–160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKenzie S, Huszar R, English DF, Kim K, Christensen F, Yoon E and Buzsaki G (2021). “Preexisting hippocampal network dynamics constrain optogenetically induced place fields.” Neuron 109(6): 1040–1054 e1047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meira T, Leroy F, Buss EW, Oliva A, Park J and Siegelbaum SA (2018). “A hippocampal circuit linking dorsal CA2 to ventral CA1 critical for social memory dynamics.” Nat Commun 9(1): 4163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miettinen R and Freund TF (1992). “Convergence and segregation of septal and median raphe inputs onto different subsets of hippocampal inhibitory interneurons.” Brain Res 594(2): 263–272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mizuseki K, Diba K, Pastalkova E and Buzsaki G (2011). “Hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells form functionally distinct sublayers.” Nat Neurosci 14(9): 1174–1181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakashiba T, Buhl DL, McHugh TJ and Tonegawa S (2009). “Hippocampal CA3 output is crucial for ripple-associated reactivation and consolidation of memory.” Neuron 62(6): 781–787. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Narikiyo K, Mizuguchi R, Ajima A, Shiozaki M, Hamanaka H, Johansen JP, Mori K and Yoshihara Y (2020). “The claustrum coordinates cortical slow-wave activity.” Nat Neurosci 23(6): 741–753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nir Y, Staba RJ, Andrillon T, Vyazovskiy VV, Cirelli C, Fried I and Tononi G (2011). “Regional slow waves and spindles in human sleep.” Neuron 70(1): 153–169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nitzan N, Swanson R, Schmitz D and Buzsaki G (2022). “Brain-wide interactions during hippocampal sharp wave ripples.” Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 119(20): e2200931119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ognjanovski N, Schaeffer S, Wu J, Mofakham S, Maruyama D, Zochowski M and Aton SJ (2017). “Parvalbumin-expressing interneurons coordinate hippocampal network dynamics required for memory consolidation.” Nat Commun 8: 15039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Okuyama T, Kitamura T, Roy DS, Itohara S and Tonegawa S (2016). “Ventral CA1 neurons store social memory.” Science 353(6307): 1536–1541. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oliva A, Fernández-Ruiz A, Leroy F and Siegelbaum SA (2020). “Hippocampal CA2 sharp-wave ripples reactivate and promote social memory.” Nature 587(7833): 264–269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Opalka AN, Huang WQ, Liu J, Liang H and Wang DV (2020). “Hippocampal Ripple Coordinates Retrosplenial Inhibitory Neurons during Slow-Wave Sleep.” Cell Rep 30(2): 432–441 e433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patel J (2015). “Network Mechanisms Underlying the Initiation and Generation of Sharp-Wave-Associated Ripple Oscillations.” The Journal of Neuroscience 35(6): 2323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peyrache A, Khamassi M, Benchenane K, Wiener SI and Battaglia FP (2009). “Replay of rule-learning related neural patterns in the prefrontal cortex during sleep.” Nat Neurosci 12(7): 919–926. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfeiffer BE and Foster DJ (2013). “Hippocampal place-cell sequences depict future paths to remembered goals.” Nature 497(7447): 74–79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preston AR and Eichenbaum H (2013). “Interplay of hippocampus and prefrontal cortex in memory.” Curr Biol 23(17): R764–773. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rasch B, Buchel C, Gais S and Born J (2007). “Odor cues during slow-wave sleep prompt declarative memory consolidation.” Science 315(5817): 1426–1429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Remondes M and Schuman EM (2004). “Role for a cortical input to hippocampal area CA1 in the consolidation of a long-term memory.” Nature 431(7009): 699–703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rich PD, Liaw HP and Lee AK (2014). “Place cells. Large environments reveal the statistical structure governing hippocampal representations.” Science 345(6198): 814–817. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rothschild G, Eban E and Frank LM (2017). “A cortical–hippocampal–cortical loop of information processing during memory consolidation.” Nature Neuroscience 20(2): 251–259. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rudoy JD, Voss JL, Westerberg CE and Paller KA (2009). “Strengthening individual memories by reactivating them during sleep.” Science 326(5956): 1079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sasaki T, Piatti VC, Hwaun E, Ahmadi S, Lisman JE, Leutgeb S and Leutgeb JK (2018). “Dentate network activity is necessary for spatial working memory by supporting CA3 sharp-wave ripple generation and prospective firing of CA3 neurons.” Nat Neurosci 21(2): 258–269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schlingloff D, Kali S, Freund TF, Hajos N and Gulyas AI (2014). “Mechanisms of sharp wave initiation and ripple generation.” J Neurosci 34(34): 11385–11398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sharif F, Tayebi B, Buzsaki G, Royer S and Fernandez-Ruiz A (2021). “Subcircuits of Deep and Superficial CA1 Place Cells Support Efficient Spatial Coding across Heterogeneous Environments.” Neuron 109(2): 363–376 e366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siapas AG and Wilson MA (1998). “Coordinated interactions between hippocampal ripples and cortical spindles during slow-wave sleep.” Neuron 21(5): 1123–1128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silva D, Feng T and Foster DJ (2015). “Trajectory events across hippocampal place cells require previous experience.” Nat Neurosci 18(12): 1772–1779. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sirota A, Csicsvari J, Buhl D and Buzsaki G (2003). “Communication between neocortex and hippocampus during sleep in rodents.” Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100(4): 2065–2069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Squire LR (1992). “Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans.” Psychol Rev 99(2): 195–231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Squire LR and Bayley PJ (2007). “The neuroscience of remote memory.” Curr Opin Neurobiol 17(2): 185–196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stark E, Roux L, Eichler R, Senzai Y, Royer S and Buzsáki G (2014). “Pyramidal cell-interneuron interactions underlie hippocampal ripple oscillations.” Neuron 83(2): 467–480. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stickgold R (2005). “Sleep-dependent memory consolidation.” Nature 437(7063): 1272–1278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sun Q, Sotayo A, Cazzulino AS, Snyder AM, Denny CA and Siegelbaum SA (2017). “Proximodistal Heterogeneity of Hippocampal CA3 Pyramidal Neuron Intrinsic Properties, Connectivity, and Reactivation during Memory Recall.” Neuron 95(3): 656–672.e653. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szonyi A, Mayer MI, Cserep C, Takacs VT, Watanabe M, Freund TF and Nyiri G (2016). “The ascending median raphe projections are mainly glutamatergic in the mouse forebrain.” Brain Struct Funct 221(2): 735–751. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teyler TJ and DiScenna P (1987). “Long-term potentiation.” Annu Rev Neurosci 10: 131–161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tonegawa S, Liu X, Ramirez S and Redondo R (2015). “Memory Engram Cells Have Come of Age.” Neuron 87(5): 918–931. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valero M, Averkin RG, Fernandez-Lamo I, Aguilar J, Lopez-Pigozzi D, Brotons-Mas JR, Cid E, Tamas G and Menendez L de la Prida (2017). “Mechanisms for Selective Single-Cell Reactivation during Offline Sharp-Wave Ripples and Their Distortion by Fast Ripples.” Neuron 94(6): 1234–1247.e1237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valero M, Cid E, Averkin RG, Aguilar J, Sanchez-Aguilera A, Viney TJ, Gomez-Dominguez D, Bellistri E and de la Prida LM (2015). “Determinants of different deep and superficial CA1 pyramidal cell dynamics during sharp-wave ripples.” Nature Neuroscience 18(9): 1281–1290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vandecasteele M, Varga V, Berényi A, Papp E, Barthó P, Venance L, Freund Tamás F and Buzsáki G (2014). “Optogenetic activation of septal cholinergic neurons suppresses sharp wave ripples and enhances theta oscillations in the hippocampus.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111(37): 13535–13540. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang DV and Ikemoto S (2016). “Coordinated Interaction between Hippocampal Sharp-Wave Ripples and Anterior Cingulate Unit Activity.” J Neurosci 36(41): 10663–10672. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang DV, Yau HJ, Broker CJ, Tsou JH, Bonci A and Ikemoto S (2015). “Mesopontine median raphe regulates hippocampal ripple oscillation and memory consolidation.” Nat Neurosci 18(5): 728–735. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winocur G, Moscovitch M and Bontempi B (2010). “Memory formation and long-term retention in humans and animals: convergence towards a transformation account of hippocampal-neocortical interactions.” Neuropsychologia 48(8): 2339–2356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xing B, Mack NR, Guo KM, Zhang YX, Ramirez B, Yang SS, Lin L, Wang DV, Li YC and Gao WJ (2021). “A Subpopulation of Prefrontal Cortical Neurons Is Required for Social Memory.” Biol Psychiatry 89(5): 521–531. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ylinen A, Bragin A, Nadasdy Z, Jando G, Szabo I, Sik A and Buzsaki G (1995). “Sharp wave-associated high-frequency oscillation (200 Hz) in the intact hippocampus: network and intracellular mechanisms.” J Neurosci 15(1 Pt 1): 30–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]